Journal of Applied Psychology 2007, Vol. 92, No. 5, 1446 –1455 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0021-9010/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.5.1446 Person–Organization Fit and Work-Related Attitudes and Decisions: Examining Interactive Effects With Job Fit and Conscientiousness Christian J. Resick Boris B. Baltes Drexel University and Florida International University Wayne State University Cynthia Walker Shantz Credit Acceptance Corporation This study examined boundary conditions that surround the importance of perceived person– organization (P-O) fit for work-related attitudes and decisions. The authors hypothesized that P-O fit is more strongly related to satisfaction and job choice decisions when needs–supplies (N-S) job fit or demands–abilities (D-A) job fit is low, and that P-O fit is more strongly related to job choice decisions for highly conscientious individuals. Hypotheses were tested among 299 participants in a 12-week internship program. Results indicated that P-O fit was more strongly related to satisfaction when individuals experienced low N-S job fit. P-O fit was more strongly related to job choice intentions when individuals experienced low D-A job fit or were highly conscientious. Finally, P-O fit was related to job offer acceptance for highly conscientious individuals. Keywords: person– organization fit, person–job fit, perceived fit, attraction, satisfaction The nature of work is changing, and with it the relationship between employers and employees. Person– organization (P-O) fit has become an increasingly important aspect of the employment relationship, as good P-O fit has been linked to organizational attraction and retention, recruiters’ selection decisions, and employees’ work-related attitudes and actions (see Hoffman & Woehr, 2006; Kristof-Brown, Zimmerman, & Johnson, 2005; Verquer, Beehr, & Wagner, 2003). However, relatively little is known about the boundary conditions that surround the importance of perceived P-O fit for positive attitudes and behaviors. We proposed that two personal characteristics serve as boundary conditions (or moderators) of P-O fit to attitude or decision relationships: perceived fit with facets of the job, and conscientious personality traits. We investigated these relationships among participants in a 12-week internship program. Through their experiences, interns form perceptions of fit and attitudes about their work; however, they are not permanent employees and must decide whether pursuing a future employment relationship is desirable. provides what the other needs, or (b) they share similar fundamental characteristics, or (c) both” (p. 6). Compatibility may take the form of supplementary fit, achieved when an individual’s personal characteristics are congruent with the characteristics of the organization and its members, or complementary fit, achieved when an individual’s characteristics fill gaps that are not addressed by others (i.e., demands–abilities [D-A] fit) or an individual’s psychological needs are fulfilled by characteristics of the work environment (i.e., needs–supplies [N-S] fit; Cable & Edwards, 2004; Kristof, 1996; Muchinsky & Monahan, 1987). A second important distinction in the fit literature is between objective fit and perceived (or subjective) forms of fit. Objective fit involves gathering separate information about the person and the organization, then assessing their congruence (e.g., Cable & Parsons, 2001; O’Reilly, Chatman, & Caldwell, 1991). In contrast, perceived fit involves asking people directly whether or not they believe they are a good fit with an organization and its members (e.g., Cable & DeRue, 2002; Lauver & Kristof-Brown, 2001). Edwards, Cable, Williamson, Lambert, and Shipp (2006) have referred to this approach as a molar approach to assessing perceived fit, which focuses on perceptions of the match or the similarity, as opposed to focusing on perceptions of the discrepancy or on perceptions of the environment and person separately. Recent meta-analytic investigations have referred to this conceptualization of fit as subjective fit (e.g., Hoffman & Woehr, 2006; Verquer et al., 2003) and perceived fit (e.g., Kristof-Brown et al., 2005). We use the term perceived fit throughout this article. In addition, larger effect sizes tend to be found for perceived fit rather than objective fit, and Kristof-Brown et al. (2005) suggested that objective congruence between a person and an organization must first be filtered through that person’s perceptions. In turn, these perceptions are likely to be more cognitively accessible (Judge & Cable, 1997) and therefore Fit With an Organization and Job Kristof (1996) defined P-O fit as “the compatibility between people and organizations that occurs when: (a) at least one entity Christian J. Resick, Department of Management, LeBow College of Business, Drexel University, and Department of Psychology, Florida International University; Boris B. Baltes, Department of Psychology, Wayne State University; Cynthia Walker Shantz, Credit Acceptance Corporation, Southfield, Michigan. We thank Amy Kristof-Brown for her helpful comments on drafts of this article. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Christian J. Resick, Department of Management, LeBow College of Business, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA 19104. E-mail: christian.j.resick@drexel.edu 1446 RESEARCH REPORTS more proximally related to attitudes and decisions than objective P-O fit would be (Cable & DeRue, 2002). We examined P-O fit as a person’s perception of his or her compatibility with an organization’s culture and members (i.e., perceived supplementary fit). While congruence with an organization’s values system is often the focus of supplementary P-O fit research (e.g., Chatman, 1989), culture researchers have noted that culture values are held by an organization’s members (e.g., Schein, 2004; Schneider, 1987). Similarly, Cable and Edwards (2004) noted that employees are likely to be comfortable (and thus experience fit) in organizations where “the things that are most important to that employee are also important to other employees” (p. 823). Therefore, consistent with Van Vianen (2000), we suggested that a more holistic picture of P-O fit is captured by addressing a person’s congruence with an organization’s culture and members. Previous research has indicated that perceptions of P-O fit are strongly related to positive work-related attitudes and organizational attraction (e.g., Chapman, Uggerslev, Carroll, Piasentin, & Jones, 2005; Kristof-Brown et al., 2005; Verquer et al., 2003), and moderately related to task and citizenship performance (Hoffman & Woehr, 2006). However, Kristof-Brown et al. (2005) also noted that there are critical gaps in the fit literature and suggested that there is a “need for future research on personal and situational characteristics that moderate fit-outcome relationships” (p. 322). We now examine fit with aspects of the job and conscientious personality traits likely provide two such moderating personal characteristics. Researchers have contended that employees’ well-being and success hinge on their fit with multiple components of the work environment, particularly with the job and organization (e.g., Bowen, Ledford, & Nathan, 1991; Dawis & Lofquist, 1984; KristofBrown, 2000). Fit with the task demands and psychological characteristics of a job likely serve as salient cues used in the development of job-related attitudes, and as input for work-related decisions. We therefore suggest that fit with aspects of the job are likely to moderate relationships between P-O fit and work-related decisions and attitudes. Fit with coworkers and team members ( person–work group [P-G] fit) also likely provides important cues about the environment that people use to form fit perceptions and attitudes. However, people often spend the vast majority of the workday involved in job-related activities, and jobs tend to be a reflection of the career choices that are the result of years of education and training. Moreover, Jansen and Kristof-Brown (2006) suggested that forms of person–job (P-J) fit are salient cues at all stages of the employment process, while P-G fit becomes salient only posthire. They also suggested that P-O fit is likely most salient during the selection process and for long-term tenure decisions, which would inevitability involve satisfaction with work. Finally, perceived P-J fit has been shown to have stronger relationships with attitudes such as job satisfaction ( ⫽ .58) than does P-G fit ( ⫽ .31; see Kristof-Brown et al., 2005). Therefore, in this study we focused on the two types of fit most likely to be related to job choice decisions and attitudes: P-O fit and P-J fit. P-J fit focuses on the match between personal characteristics and job characteristics, and takes two distinct forms: D-A fit and N-S fit (Edwards, 1991). D-A job fit represents the degree to which a person possesses the knowledge, skills, and abilities (KSAs) needed to perform the task-related demands of a job, while N-S job fit reflects the extent to which a job’s characteristics and 1447 rewards fulfill a person’s psychological needs or preferences. Several studies have simultaneously examined both P-O and P-J fit, finding that each form of fit explains unique variance in work-related attitudes (e.g., Lauver & Kristof-Brown, 2001; Saks & Ashforth, 1997). However, these studies have focused only on the main effects of each type of fit, representing simple additive effects (see Kristof, 1996). To date, only Kristof-Brown, Jansen, and Colbert (2002) have examined the interactive effects of multiple types of fit. Using a policy capturing study, they found that P-O, P-J, and P-G fit perceptions were each related to work satisfaction, and that P-O fit demonstrated a two-way interaction with P-J fit and a three-way interaction with P-J and P-G fit. Their findings suggest that people combine information about their environments in more complex ways than would be identified using simple additive models, and that high fit with one aspect of the work environment may compensate for low fit in another area. Jansen and Kristof-Brown (2006) further elaborated on this compensatory process, suggesting that when people experience good fit with one facet of the environment and poor fit with another facet, they may downplay the lack of fit to reduce the dissonance that might arise from conflicting perceptions of fit. As a result, good fit with one aspect of the work environment becomes a dominant fit cue that is relied upon when forming work-related attitudes or making decisions. Regarding fit and satisfaction, Cable and DeRue (2002) suggested that the N-S aspect of job fit should be more strongly associated with job satisfaction than are P-O and D-A job fit, because individuals should experience greater satisfaction with their jobs when their jobs’ characteristics fulfill their psychological needs. Kristof-Brown et al.’s (2005) meta-analysis also identified larger effect sizes for N-S job fit ( ⫽ .61) than for D-A job fit ( ⫽ .41) or perceived P-O fit ( ⫽ .56). Moreover, Shore and Martin (1989) suggested that job-related attitudes should be most strongly associated with job-related constructs. Based on these works, we suggest that when people experience good fit with aspects of the job, P-O fit may be less salient. That is, good N-S job fit or good D-A job fit becomes a more dominant fit cue, and P-O fit has a weaker relationship with satisfaction. In contrast, P-O fit becomes a more salient fit cue for people who experience a lack of fit with aspects of their jobs, and P-O fit is in these cases more strongly related to satisfaction. Hypothesis 1a: Perceived P-O fit is positively related to satisfaction with the internship, and this relationship is moderated by perceived N-S job fit such that the relationship is stronger when N-S job fit is low and weaker when N-S job fit is high. Hypothesis 1b: Perceived P-O fit is positively related to satisfaction with the internship, and this relationship is moderated by perceived D-A job fit such that the relationship is stronger when D-A job fit is low and weaker when D-A job fit is high. P-J fit has also been linked to organizational attraction and job choice intentions (Chapman et al., 2005; Kristof-Brown et al., 2005). When people perceive a potential job to be a good fit, this may become a salient and dominant fit cue. Therefore, P-O fit may be less salient and have a weaker relationship with job choice RESEARCH REPORTS 1448 Hypothesis 2a: Perceived P-O fit is positively related to (a) intent to accept a job offer, and (b) job offer acceptance, and these relationships are moderated by perceived N-S job fit such that the relationships are stronger when N-S job fit is low and weaker when N-S job fit is high. carefully before acting (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Moreover, conscientious people appear to be more selective in their choice of jobs (Wanberg, Watt, & Rumsey, 1996), perhaps because they desire to work in organizations where they fit. In summary, conscientious people have a desire to achieve (Digman, 1989), are proactive and dutiful (Roberts et al., 2005), and are cautious and deliberate (McCrae & Costa, 1987). Because of these qualities, a highly conscientious person may actively seek information about his or her fit with a potential employing organization. Such individuals may be more likely to accept job offers from organizations where they perceive good fit, because they want to join organizations where they will adjust smoothly and be successful. A highly conscientious intern may use an internship as an opportunity to gather and evaluate information and make decisions about a potential employer. In the current study, we focused on conscientiousness instead of other personality traits because of the behavioral tendencies associated with conscientiousness and the link with job search behaviors. Hypothesis 2b: Perceived P-O fit is positively related to (a) intent to accept a job offer, and (b) job offer acceptance, and these relationships are moderated by perceived D-A job fit such that the relationships are stronger when D-A job fit is low and weaker when D-A job fit is high. Hypothesis 3: Perceived P-O fit is positively related to (a) intent to accept a job offer, and (b) job offer acceptance, and this relationship is moderated by conscientiousness such that the relationships are stronger when conscientiousness is high and weaker when conscientiousness is low. intentions or decisions. However, P-O fit likely becomes an increasingly salient and dominant fit cue for people who perceive a lack of N-S or D-A job fit. P-O fit should then be more strongly related to job choice intentions or decisions when job fit is low. For example, a person may view a company as a great place to work but believe that a job’s responsibilities do not fulfill his or her psychological needs. That person may accept a job offer with the hope of moving to a new position within the firm after getting a “foot in the door,” and under those circumstances the individual may downplay lack of fit with the accepted job. This could also apply to individuals who feel that their KSAs are not a good match for a job’s requirements. Method Conscientiousness Kristof (1996) proposed that personality differences may affect the importance that people attach to P-O fit when seeking and choosing an organization, and in particular that “conscientious and self-aware job seekers will weight anticipated P-O fit more heavily in their job/organization choice decisions than those who are less conscientious and self-aware” (p. 37). Highly conscientious individuals tend to be ambitious, dutiful, and cautious in their actions (Costa, McCrae, & Dye, 1991; Roberts, Chernyshenko, Stark, & Goldberg, 2005). Because of these tendencies, highly conscientious people should be likely to seek a thorough understanding of potential employers and to exercise caution in accepting job offers, to ensure that they join firms where they will fit in and be successful. That is, we suggest that P-O fit is likely to be more strongly associated with job choice decisions for highly conscientious individuals. Conscientiousness is a broad trait that is characterized by two primary dimensions, dependability and achievement (Mount & Barrick, 1995). From a dependability perspective, conscientiousness encompasses thoroughness, self-discipline, order, and dutifulness; while from an achievement perspective, conscientiousness reflects perseverance, industriousness, achievement striving, and deliberation (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Goldberg, 1990; McCrae & Costa, 1987). Previous research has linked conscientiousness to proactive job search behaviors (Schmit, Amel, & Ryan, 1993). According to Ashford and Black (1996), proactive behaviors involve gathering information and feedback to make sense of new situations, and building relationships and networks with potential employers. These behaviors would likely be used to gain a thorough understanding of the expectations of potential employers, based on which the individual would then join an organization where the fit was good. At the same time, conscientious individuals are cautious and deliberate, and have a tendency to think Participants Data were collected from participants in a 12-week internship program at a large manufacturing company in the Midwestern U.S. during the summer of 2000. All 974 interns hired that summer were invited to participate, of whom 756 (77.6%) agreed to participate. Of these, 299 (30.7% of the overall sample) completed all sets of surveys. Using t tests, we found that responding and nonresponding participants did not differ in terms of gender or conscientiousness, but that they did differ significantly in terms of age (t ⫽ ⫺2.10, p ⬍ .05); respondents were slightly older (M ⫽ 22.7 years) than nonrespondents (M ⫽ 22.1 years). Because the groups did not differ in terms of gender or conscientiousness and age differences were minimal, we concluded that there was little evidence of systematic nonresponse bias. The 299 participants ranged in age from 18 to 41 years (M ⫽ 22.7), had an average of 21.5 months of full-time work experience, and were predominantly male (61% male, 39% female). Interns worked full time in one of seven job function areas: product development (n ⫽ 102), manufacturing (n ⫽ 81), finance (n ⫽ 42), marketing (n ⫽ 28), information technology (n ⫽ 20), purchasing (n ⫽ 18), and human resources (n ⫽ 7). Area was unknown for 1 participant. Measures Perceived fit. Perceived P-O fit was measured with a 5-item scale derived by combining items from Cable and Judge (1996) and Saks and Ashforth (1997) that addressed supplementary P-O fit. Internal consistency reliability was ␣ ⫽ .94. Perceived D-A fit and N-S fit scales were also derived from perceived P-J fit scales developed by Cable and Judge and by Saks and Ashforth. For D-A job fit, 4 items addressing the match between a job’s requirements and a person’s KSAs and/or training were used. For N-S job fit, 4 RESEARCH REPORTS items assessing perceptions that job characteristics fulfill a person’s psychological needs were retained. Internal consistency reliability was ␣ ⫽ .72 for the D-A job fit and ␣ ⫽ .92 for the N-S job fit scales. Confirmatory factor analysis, conducted using LISREL 8.7, indicated that the 13 items (see the Appendix) were associated with separate perceived fit factors (comparative fit index ⫽ .98, goodness of fit index ⫽ .93, nonnormed fit index ⫽ .97, and root-mean-square error of approximation ⫽ .07). For all measures, participants responded along a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Conscientiousness. Conscientiousness was measured using the 12-item scale from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Internal consistency reliability for the current study was ␣ ⫽ .78. Control variables. Individuals with previous work experience may have preconceived ideas about the types of organizations they enjoy working for, which could affect fit perceptions, work-related attitudes, and employment decisions. Therefore, we controlled for months of full-time work experience. We also controlled for intern age to minimize any potential effects. Satisfaction. Satisfaction with the internship was assessed with two items: “Overall, I am satisfied with my internship” and “I am likely to recommend this organization to others as a favorable company to work for.” Internal consistency reliability was ␣ ⫽ .78. Job choice intentions and decisions. Intent to accept a job offer was measured with a single item: “I am likely to accept a full-time job offer from this organization if I am offered one.” Actual job choice decision data were obtained from the organization’s employment records. Of the 299 interns, 248 (82.9%) were offered positions, and 128 (51.6%) accepted. 1449 informed that if they excelled in the internship program, they would be offered positions in the firm’s graduate training program (GTP) upon graduation. According to company HR representatives, the general nature of work in the two programs was similar. A participant in the GTP would be hired into a job function and would perform assignments in several different areas of that function over a 2-year period. Of the 248 interns who were offered GTP positions, 214 (86.3%) were offered positions in the same job function, and 25 (10.1%) were offered positions in a different job function. For the final 9 (3.6%), either the internship or GTP job function was unknown. Based on this analysis, we concluded that comparability existed between positions. Analyses We performed hierarchical multiple regression analyses to test the hypotheses. The work experience and age control variables were entered into the first step of the regression model, followed by the block of main effect variables in the second step and the block of interaction terms in the third step. We centered all variables to minimize problems of multicollinearity (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). Following Baron and Kenney’s (1986) approach, we examined the significance of the incremental variance explained by the addition of the interaction terms, along with the significance of the regression coefficients, to determine if the hypotheses were supported and moderated relationships were found. Because job choice is a dichotomous variable (i.e., accept or decline an offer), we used logistic regression to test these relationships. Results Data Collection The interns were surveyed on the 1st day of the internship; they received a description of the study and an assurance of confidentiality. Those who agreed to participate completed several questionnaires, including the Conscientiousness Scale and a demographics questionnaire. Data were collected a second time near the end of the summer internship. Participants completed the Perceived Fit, Internship Satisfaction, and Intent to Accept an Offer scales at this time. During the following spring, human resource (HR) department officials reviewed employment records and indicated whether each intern had been offered a full-time position and whether the offer had been accepted or declined. Comparability of Positions For perceived N-S and D-A job fit with the internship position to be a meaningful gauge of fit with a future position, the responsibilities of each role needed to be comparable. We analyzed role comparability by (a) interviewing company HR representatives, (b) examining descriptions of the internship and entry-level, fulltime positions, and (c) comparing job function areas for the internship and full-time positions. An intern was hired into a job function (e.g., product development), where he or she gained experience performing assignments in a variety of areas within that function. These assignments were designed to be measurable and realistic and to stretch the interns’ abilities. New interns were Table 1 summarizes the correlations among variables. Hypothesis 1a examined the interactive effects of P-O and N-S job fit with satisfaction, while Hypothesis 1b examined the interactive effects of P-O and D-A job fit. At Step 1 of the regression model, previous work experience and age explained a small but nonsignificant amount of variance in satisfaction (R2 ⫽ .02), F(2, 294) ⫽ 2.72, ns. The addition of the main effect variables at Step 2 explained a sizeable amount of incremental variance in satisfaction (⌬R2 ⫽ .63), F(3, 291) ⫽ 179.70, p ⬍ .01. The strongest relationships were found for N-S job fit (sr ⫽ .46,  ⫽ .56, p ⬍ .01), followed by P-O fit (sr ⫽ .29,  ⫽ .33, p ⬍ .01) and D-A job fit (sr ⫽ .08,  ⫽ .09, p ⬍ .05). The interaction terms entered at Step 3 explained a small but significant amount of the remaining incremental variance (⌬R2 ⫽ .02), F(2, 289) ⫽ 6.79, p ⬍ .01, and this finding was due to the P-O Fit ⫻ N-S Job Fit interaction term (sr ⫽ ⫺.11,  ⫽ ⫺.14, p ⬍ .01). Results are summarized in Table 2. Next, the P-O Fit ⫻ N-S Job Fit interaction was graphed, following procedures suggested by Cohen, Cohen, West, and Aiken (2003). This graph is depicted in Figure 1. Results indicated that when N-S job fit was high, P-O fit had only a slight positive relationship to satisfaction. However, when N-S job fit was low, interns who experienced greater fit with the organization reported substantially greater satisfaction than those interns who fit the organization less well. That is, P-O fit became increasingly important for interns who perceived less of a match between job characteristics and their psychological needs. Therefore, we con- RESEARCH REPORTS 1450 Table 1 Zero-Order Correlations Among Variables Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. Work experience Age Person–organization fit Needs–supplies job fit Demands–abilities job fit Conscientiousness Satisfaction Intent to accept job offer Job offer acceptancea M SD 21.46 22.68 3.95 3.68 4.24 3.21 4.11 3.70 0.52 28.81 3.67 0.69 0.94 0.61 0.42 0.70 0.91 0.50 1 — .68** .07 .10 .17** .06 .11 .17** ⫺.02 2 — .04 .19** .17** ⫺.01 .13* .09 ⫺.05 3 4 — .46** .28** .23** .62** .55** .10 — .38** .04 .74** .39** .15* 5 — .18** .40** .26** ⫺.01 6 7 8 9 — .08 .06 ⫺.04 — .57** .16** — .38** — Note. Work experience ⫽ months of prior full-time work experience. Age ⫽ age in years. N ⫽ 290 –299 (except with Job Offer Acceptance: N ⫽ 248). Decline ⫽ 0; accept ⫽ 1. * p ⬍ .05. **p ⬍ .01. a cluded that Hypothesis 1a was supported and Hypothesis 1b was not supported. Hypothesis 2a examined the interactive effects of P-O and N-S job fit with job choice intentions and decisions; Hypothesis 2b examined the interactive effects of P-O and D-A job fit; and Hypothesis 3 examined the interactive effects of P-O fit and conscientiousness. Regarding job choice intentions, previous work experience (sr ⫽ .15,  ⫽ .21, p ⬍ .01) and age (sr ⫽ ⫺.04,  ⫽ ⫺.06, ns) entered into Step 1 explained a small but significant amount of variance (R2 ⫽ .03), F(2, 287) ⫽ 4.65, p ⬍ .05. The addition of the main effect variables at Step 2 explained approximately 32% of the incremental variance in job choice intentions (⌬R2 ⫽ .32), F(4, 283) ⫽ 34.67, p ⬍ .01. However, only P-O fit (sr ⫽ .40,  ⫽ .46, p ⬍ .01) and N-S job fit (sr ⫽ .13,  ⫽ .16, p ⬍ .01) had significant main effects. Finally, the interaction terms entered at Step 3 explained a small but significant amount of incremental variance (⌬R2 ⫽ .03), F(3, 280) ⫽ 5.12, p ⬍ .01. A review of the regression coefficients indicated that both the P-O Fit ⫻ D-A Job Fit (sr ⫽ ⫺.11,  ⫽ ⫺.14, p ⬍ .05) and P-O Fit ⫻ Conscientiousness (sr ⫽ .13,  ⫽ .13, p ⬍ .01) interaction terms were significantly related to job choice intentions, while the P-O Fit ⫻ N-S Job Fit interaction was not significant (sr ⫽ ⫺.02,  ⫽ ⫺.02, ns). Results are summarized in Table 2. The P-O Fit ⫻ D-A Job Fit interaction is depicted graphically in Figure 2. Results indicated that the relationship between P-O fit and job choice intentions was generally positive, and that the relationship was slightly stronger for interns who perceived lower D-A job fit and slightly weaker for interns who experienced higher D-A job fit. This finding was consistent with Hypothesis 2b. Figure 3 depicts a graph of the interactive relationship between P-O fit and conscientiousness. Once again, results indicated that the relationship between P-O fit and intentions was generally positive; however, the relationship was stronger for highly conscientious interns and weaker for less conscientious interns, which was consistent with Hypothesis 3. Logistic regression was used to test the relationships with job choice decisions. These results are summarized in Table 3. In Step 1, previous work experience and age were unrelated to job choice decisions (R2 ⫽ .00), 2(2, N ⫽ 248) ⫽ 0.69, ns. With the addition of the main effect variables at Step 2, the model accounted for a small but nonsignificant amount of explainable variance (R2 ⫽ .05), 2(6, N ⫽ 248) ⫽ 9.92, ns. The addition of the interaction terms at Step 3 resulted in a model that explained a moderate amount of explainable variance (R2 ⫽ .12), 2(9, N ⫽ 248) ⫽ 23.69, p ⬍ .01. A review of the regression coefficients indicated that job choice decisions were significantly related only to the P-O Fit ⫻ Conscientiousness interaction term (B ⫽ 1.91, p ⬍ .01), and not to the P-O Fit ⫻ N-S Job Fit (B ⫽ .11, ns) or P-O Fit ⫻ D-A Job Fit (B ⫽ ⫺.65, ns) interactions. Results are depicted in Figure 4. As hypothesized, P-O fit was strongly and positively related to job offer acceptance for highly conscientious interns. Unexpectedly, however, P-O fit was negatively related to job offer acceptance for less conscientious interns. Based on these findings, we concluded that partial support was found for Hypotheses 2b and 3, while Hypothesis 2a was not supported. Discussion This study provided evidence that perceived fit with aspects of a job and conscientious personality traits serve as boundary conditions on the relationship between perceived P-O fit and both satisfaction and job choice decisions. Regarding satisfaction, the findings indicated that P-O fit was more strongly linked to satisfaction for people who experienced a lack of N-S job fit. In contrast, P-O fit was less important for satisfaction for people who experienced good N-S job fit. D-A job fit did not interact with P-O fit in relation to satisfaction. Perhaps experiencing fit with a job’s psychological characteristics becomes a dominant cue regarding satisfaction, decreasing the salience of fit with the organization and thus weakening the relationship between P-O fit and satisfaction. However, P-O fit may become a more dominant cue for people who experience poor N-S job fit. As a result, P-O fit is more strongly related to satisfaction under these conditions. Regarding job choice intentions and decisions, D-A job fit moderated the relationships between P-O fit and intentions but not decisions, while N-S job fit did not interact with P-O fit in relation to either intentions or decisions. Through their experiences, interns had the opportunity to determine their fit with the job’s demands and the organization’s environment. As a result, fit with the organization likely became more salient, and thus more strongly related to job choice intentions, if they experienced poor fit with a job’s demands. However, in the time between reporting their intentions and making their actual decisions, some people may have considered the implications of accepting jobs for which they perceived poor fit and RESEARCH REPORTS 1451 Table 2 Regression Analyses of the Relationships Between Fit Perceptions and Conscientiousness With Satisfaction and Intent to Accept a Job Offer 95% CI R2 F ⌬R2 Step 1 Work experience Age Step 2 Work experience Age P-O fit N-S job fit D-A job fit Step 3 Work experience Age P-O fit N-S job fit D-A job fit P-O Fit ⫻ N-S Job Fit P-O Fit ⫻ D-A Job Fit .02 2.72 .02 Step 1 Work experience Age Step 2 Work experience Age P-O fit N-S job fit D-A job fit Conscientiousness Step 3 Work experience Age P-O fit N-S job fit D-A job fit Conscientiousness P-O Fit ⫻ N-S Job Fit P-O Fit ⫻ D-A Job Fit P-O Fit ⫻ Conscientiousness .03 Variable Finc dfsinc sr  .02 .08 .03 .11 .02 ⫺.01 .29 .46 .08 B SE Lower Upper .00 .02 .00 .02 .00 ⫺.01 .01 .05 .03 ⫺.02 .33 .56 .09 .00 ⫺.01 .34** .42** .11* .00 .01 .04 .03 .04 .00 ⫺.02 .26 .36 .02 .00 .01 .42 .48 .19 .01 ⫺.01 .25 .45 .08 ⫺.11 .02 .02 ⫺.02 .30 .55 .10 ⫺.14 .03 .00 .00 .31** .41** .11* ⫺.13** .04 .00 .01 .04 .03 .04 .04 .06 .00 ⫺.02 .23 .35 .02 ⫺.21 ⫺.08 .00 .02 .39 .47 .20 ⫺.06 .16 .15 ⫺.04 .21 ⫺.06 .01** ⫺.01 .00 .02 .00 ⫺.05 .01 .02 .13 ⫺.07 .40 .13 .05 ⫺.07 .18 ⫺.10 .46 .16 .06 ⫺.07 .01* ⫺.02 .61** .16* .09 ⫺.15 .00 .02 .07 .06 .08 .11 .00 ⫺.06 .46 .05 ⫺.07 ⫺.36 .01 .01 .75 .27 .25 .06 .12 ⫺.06 .38 .11 .02 ⫺.04 ⫺.02 ⫺.11 .13 .16 ⫺.08 .47 .13 .02 ⫺.04 ⫺.02 ⫺.14 .13 .01* ⫺.02 .62** .13* .03 ⫺.08 ⫺.03 ⫺.27* .36** .00 .02 .08 .06 .08 .11 .07 .11 .13 .00 ⫺.05 .47 .02 ⫺.13 ⫺.30 ⫺.17 ⫺.49 .10 .01 .01 .76 .24 .19 .13 .11 ⫺.04 .62 Satisfaction .65 .67 ** 110.90 84.30** .63 .02 2.72 ** 179.70 6.79** 2, 294 3, 291 2, 289 Intent to accept a job offer .35 .38 4.65* 25.39** 19.37** .03 .32 .03 4.65* 34.67** 5.12** 2, 287 4, 283 3, 280 Note. N ⫽ 290 –299. Finc ⫽ F test associated with the ⌬R2. dfsinc ⫽ degrees of freedom associated with the Finc test. 95% CI ⫽ 95% confidence interval around unstandardized regression coefficient. P-O fit ⫽ person– organization fit; N-S job fit ⫽ needs–supplies job fit; D-A job fit ⫽ demands–abilities job fit. * p ⬍ .05. **p ⬍ .01. decided to decline the offers, and some people may have received offers for jobs for which they were a better fit. As a result, D-A job fit did not moderate P-O fit and acceptance relationships. Together, these findings suggest that D-A job fit is an important boundary condition on relationships between P-O fit and employment intentions, while N-S job fit is an important boundary condition on relationships between P-O fit and work-related attitudes. As expected, conscientiousness moderated the relationship between P-O fit and both job choice intentions and decisions. Regarding intentions, perceived P-O fit was positively related to the intention of accepting an employment offer for both high- and low-conscientiousness individuals; however, the relationship was stronger for highly conscientious people. In addition, perceived P-O fit was strongly and positively related to actual job offer acceptance for more highly conscientious individuals. Perhaps highly conscientious people are cautious about joining organizations that do not provide environments congruent with their values and personalities, and thus perceived fit with an organization is highly salient. As a result, these individuals are likely to thoroughly investigate potential employers, and to form intentions toward and accept job offers from organizations where they perceive good fit. At the same time, several meta-analytic studies have shown that conscientiousness is linked to performance across job categories (see Barrick & Mount, 1991; Hurtz & Donovan, 1452 RESEARCH REPORTS Figure 1. Interaction of person– organization (P-O) fit and needs– supplies (N-S) job fit on satisfaction with the internship. High N-S job fit ⫽ 1 standard deviation above the mean; low N-S job fit ⫽ 1 standard deviation below the mean. 2000). Highly conscientious individuals may have had more job offers, affording them the luxury of declining offers where they experienced low P-O fit. Surprisingly, for less conscientious individuals P-O fit was negatively related to job offer acceptance. For less conscientious individuals, P-O fit may be a less salient cue in job search behaviors; or they may be more reckless and tend to join organizations where they perceive a lack of fit. Alternatively, less conscientious individuals may have had few job opportunities and might have accepted any job offer they received, regardless of how well they fit the organization. Finally, on a methodological note, the findings of the current study also support the argument that P-O is a moderator of conscientiousness and job choice relationships, N-S job fit and satisfaction relationships, or D-A job fit and job choice intention relationships. For example, the P-O Fit ⫻ N-S Job Fit interaction could be interpreted as perceived P-O fit moderating the relation- Figure 2. Interaction of person– organization (P-O) fit and demands– abilities (D-A) job fit with intent to accept a job offer. High D-A job fit ⫽ 1 standard deviation above the mean; low D-A job fit ⫽ 1 standard deviation below the mean. Figure 3. Interaction of person– organization (P-O) fit and conscientiousness with intent to accept a job offer. High conscientiousness ⫽ 1 standard deviation above the mean; low conscientiousness ⫽ 1 standard deviation below the mean. ship between N-S job fit and satisfaction. For this reason, we stress the importance of relying on an a priori theoretical rationale both for proposing and for interpreting the main and moderator effects. Several limitations of this study exist. First, the study focuses exclusively on fit perceptions and neglects the implications of actual P-O, N-S, or D-A congruence. Moreover, molar approaches to assessing fit perceptions, such as those used in the current study, may indicate more of a general affective reaction to the workplace than an assessment of one’s actual fit (Edwards et al., 2006). Future research should include both objective and perceived measures of fit to more fully examine these relationships. Second, participants were from a single organization, which likely has members with similar types of personalities and values throughout (Giberson, Resick, & Dickson, 2005; Schneider, Smith, Taylor, & Fleenor, 1998). Moreover, participants were both a self-selected and organization-selected sample, and there is likely to be restriction in the range of variability regarding perceptions of P-O fit, N-S job fit, and D-A job fit, as well as regarding satisfaction scores—all of which would attenuate the magnitude of the relationships for both the main and interactive effects. This may, in part, explain the small effect sizes for the interaction terms. Future research should examine these relationships in a crossorganization sample. Third, the sample of interns is another potential limitation of this study. Interns are temporary employees and are limited in their exposure to the organization’s culture. As a result, interns may have a less accurate understanding of the organization’s culture (Cable, Aiman-Smith, Mulvey, & Edwards, 2000) and form less accurate P-O fit perceptions. Future research should examine these relationships using a sample of more experienced workers. However, interns also have an informed view of an organization and of potential jobs. Although interns may be somewhat less representative of traditional job candidates, these results may be particularly useful for recruiting candidates from internal applicant pools. Fourth, because the assessments of fit, satisfaction, and intent to accept a job offer were gathered at the same time and by the same source, there was the potential for the magnitude of the relationships to be inflated by common method variance. In their meta- RESEARCH REPORTS 1453 Table 3 Logistic Regression Analyses of the Relationships Between Fit Perceptions and Conscientiousness With Actual Job Offer Acceptance Step coefficients Model coefficients Variable 2(N ⫽ 248) df 2(N ⫽ 248) df R2a Step 1 Work experience Age Step 2 Work experience Age P-O fit N-S job fit D-A job fit Conscientiousness Step 3 Work experience Age P-O fit N-S job fit D-A job fit Conscientiousness P-O Fit ⫻ N-S Job Fit P-O Fit ⫻ D-A Job Fit P-O Fit ⫻ Conscientiousness 0.69 2 0.69 2 .00 9.23 13.77** 4 3 9.92 23.69** 6 9 B SE .00 ⫺.04 .01 .05 .00 ⫺.06 .22 .36* ⫺.27 ⫺.28 .01 .05 .23 .17 .24 .33 .00 ⫺.04 .35 .33 ⫺.39 ⫺.22 .11 ⫺.65 1.91** .01 .05 .27 .17 .26 .36 .29 .49 .61 .05 .12 Note. P-O fit ⫽ person– organization fit; N-S job fit ⫽ needs–supplies job fit; D-A job fit ⫽ demands–abilities job fit. a 2 R ⫽ Nagelkerke estimated R2. * p ⬍ .05. **p ⬍ .01. analysis of fit studies, Kristof-Brown et al. (2005) noted that relationships tended to be high when fit and outcome variables were measured at the same time, and they suggested that this may be the result of common method bias. Drawing on the arguments of interactional psychology (e.g., Caplan, 1987; Endler & Magnusson, 1976; French, Caplan, & Harrison, 1982), that people are primarily influenced by their perceptions of fit as opposed to their objective fit with the environment, Kristof-Brown et al. (2005) suggested that the strong link between fit perceptions and outcomes may reflect the reality of subjective internal states and not merely an artifact of bias. Thus, some findings of the current study should be interpreted in light of the potential for inflation due to common method bias. However, an important strength of this study is that job choice decision data were provided by the company’s HR department, not via self-reports. Therefore, relationships with job choice decisions should not have been affected by common method bias. To disentangle the true effects from the bias-driven effects, future research could use multiple methods of assessing fit and attitudes or decisions. In conclusion, our findings provide evidence that N-S job fit moderates relationships between P-O fit and work-related attitudes, while D-A job fit and conscientiousness traits moderate relationships between P-O fit and employment intentions and decisions. Future research should examine a wider range of workrelated attitudes, decisions, and behaviors. Future research should also examine the moderating role of other traits, such as emotional stability. A better understanding of the importance of P-O fit in the workplace will be uncovered by examining P-O fit in conjunction with other forms of environmental fit and/or the characteristics of the individual, rather than by examining P-O fit in isolation. References Figure 4. Interaction of person– organization (P-O) fit and conscientiousness with job offer acceptance. High conscientiousness ⫽ 1 standard deviation above the mean; low conscientiousness ⫽ 1 standard deviation below the mean. Ashford, S. J., & Black, S. J. (1996). Proactivity during organizational entry: The role of desire for control. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 199 –214. Baron, R. M., & Kenney, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. 1454 RESEARCH REPORTS Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26. Bowen, D. E., Ledford, G. E., & Nathan, B. R. (1991). Hiring for the organization, not the job. Academy of Management Executive, 4, 35–51. Cable, D. M., Aiman-Smith, L., Mulvey, P. W., & Edwards, J. R. (2000). The sources and accuracy of job applicant’s’ beliefs about organizational culture. Academy of Management Journal, 43, 1076 –1085. Cable, D. M., & DeRue, D. S. (2002). The convergent and discriminant validity of subjective fit perceptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 875– 884. Cable, D. M., & Edwards, J. R. (2004). Complementary and supplementary fit: A theoretical and empirical integration. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 822– 834. Cable, D. M., & Judge, T. A. (1996). Person-organization fit, job choice decisions, and organizational entry. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67(3), 294 –311. Cable, D. M., & Parsons, C. K. (2001). Socialization tactics and personorganization fit. Personnel Psychology, 54, 1–23. Caplan, R. D. (1987). Person-environment fit theory: Commensurate dimensions, time perspectives, and mechanisms. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 31, 248 –267. Chapman, D. S., Uggerslev, K. L., Carroll, S. A., Piasentin, K. A., & Jones, D. A. (2005). Applicant attraction to organizations and job choice: A meta-analytic review of the correlates of recruiting outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 928 –944. Chatman, J. A. (1989). Improving interactional organizational research: A model of person-organization fit. Academy of Management Review, 14, 333–349. Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO Five-Factor Inventory professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Costa, P. T., McCrae, R. R., & Dye, D. A. (1991). Facet scales for agreeableness and conscientiousness: A revision of the NEO personality inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 887– 898. Dawis, R. V., & Lofquist, L. H. (1984). A psychological theory of work adjustment. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. Digman, J. M. (1989). Five robust trait dimensions: Development, stability, and utility. Journal of Personality, 57, 195–214. Edwards, J. E., Cable, D. M., Williamson, I. O., Lambert, L. S., & Shipp, A. J. (2006). The phenomenology of fit: Linking the person and environment to the subjective experience of person-environment fit. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 802– 827. Edwards, J. R. (1991). Person-job fit: A conceptual integration, literature review, and methodological critique. In C. L. Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International review of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 283–357). Chichester, West Sussex, England: Wiley. Endler, N. S., & Magnusson, D. (1976). Interactional psychology and personality. New York: Wiley. French, J. R. P., Caplan, R. D., & Harrison, R. V. (1982). The mechanisms of stress and strain. London: Wiley. Giberson, T. R., Resick, C. J., & Dickson, M. W. (2005). Embedding leader characteristics: An examination of homogeneity of personality and values in organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 1002–1010. Goldberg, L. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: The Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1216 –1229. Hoffman, B. J., & Woehr, D. J. (2006). A quantitative review of the relationship between person-organization fit and behavioral outcomes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 68, 389 –399. Hurtz, G. M., & Donovan, J. J. (2000). Personality and job performance: The Big Five revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 869 – 879. Jansen, K., & Kristof-Brown, A. (2006). Toward a multi-dimensional theory of person-environment fit. Journal of Managerial Issues, 28, 193–212. Judge, T. A., & Cable, D. M. (1997). Applicant personality, organizational culture, and organizational attraction. Personnel Psychology, 50, 359 –393. Kristof, A. L. (1996). Person-organization fit: An integrative review of its conceptualizations, measurement, and implications. Personnel Psychology, 49, 1– 49. Kristof-Brown, A. L. (2000). Perceived applicant fit: Distinguishing between recruiters’ perceptions of person-job and person-organization fit. Personnel Psychology, 53, 643– 671. Kristof-Brown, A. L., Jansen, K. J., & Colbert, A. E. (2002). A policy capturing study of the simultaneous effects of fit with jobs, groups, and organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 985–993. Kristof-Brown, A. L., Zimmerman, R. D., & Johnson, E. C. (2005). Consequences of individuals’ fit at work: A meta-analysis of person-job, person-organization, person-group, and person-supervisor fit. Personnel Psychology, 58, 281–342. Lauver, K. J., & Kristof-Brown, A. (2001). Distinguishing between employees’ perceptions of person-job and person-organization fit. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 59, 454 – 470. McCrae, R., & Costa, P. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 81–90. Mount, M. K., & Barrick, M. R. (1995). The Big Five personality dimensions: Implications for research and practice in human resources management. In K. M. Rowland and G. Ferris (Eds.), Research in personnel and human resource management (Vol. 13, pp. 153–200). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Muchinsky, P. M., & Monahan, C. J. (1987). What is person-environment congruence? Supplementary versus complementary models of fit. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 31, 268 –277. O’Reilly, C. A., Chatman, J., & Caldwell, D. F. (1991). People and organizational culture: A profile comparison approach to assessing person-organization fit. Academy of Management Journal, 34, 487–516. Roberts, B. R., Chernyshenko, O. S., Stark, S., & Goldberg, L. R. (2005). The structure of conscientiousness: An empirical investigation based on seven major personality questionnaires. Personnel Psychology, 58, 103–139. Saks, A. M., & Ashforth, B. E. (1997). A longitudinal investigation of the relationships between job information sources, applicant perceptions of fit, and work outcomes. Personnel Psychology, 50, 395– 426. Schein, E. (2004). Organizational culture and leadership (3rd ed.). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Schmit, M. J., Amel, E. L., & Ryan, A. M. (1993). Self-reported assertive job-seeking behaviors of minimally-educated job hunters. Personnel Psychology, 46, 105–124. Schneider, B. (1987). The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, 40, 437– 453. Schneider, B., Smith, D. B., Taylor, S., & Fleenor, J. (1998). Personality and organizations: A test of the homogeneity of personality hypothesis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 462– 470. Shore, L. M., & Martin, H. J. (1989). Job satisfaction and organizational commitment in relation to work performance and turnover intentions. Human Relations, 42, 625– 638. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Van Vianen, A. E. M. (2000). Person-organization fit: The match between newcomers’ and recruiters’ preferences for organizational culture. Personnel Psychology, 53, 113–149. Verquer, M. L., Beehr, T. A., & Wagner, S. H. (2003). A meta-analysis of relations between person-organization fit and work attitudes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 63, 473– 489. Wanberg, C. R., Watt, J. D., & Rumsey, D. J. (1996). Individuals without jobs: An empirical study of job-seeking behavior and reemployment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 76 – 87. RESEARCH REPORTS 1455 Appendix Person–Environment Fit Scales Items Contained in the Person–Organization Fit Scale 1. I feel my values “match” or fit this organization and the current employees in this organization (from Cable & Judge, 1996). 2. I think the values and personality of this organization reflect my own values and personality (from Cable & Judge, 1996). 3. The values of this organization are similar to my own values (from Saks & Ashforth, 1997). 4. My values match those of current employees in this organization (from Cable & Judge, 1996). 5. I feel my personality matches the “personality” or image of this organization (from Saks & Ashforth, 1997). Items Contained in the Demands–Abilities Fit Scale 1. I believe my skills and abilities match those required by the internship (from Cable & Judge, 1996). 2. My job performance is hurt by a lack of expertise on the job (from Cable & Judge, 1996). 3. My knowledge, skills and abilities match the requirements of the internship (from Saks & Ashforth, 1997). 4. I possess the skills and abilities to perform this job (from Cable & Judge, 1996). Items Contained in the Needs–Supplies Fit Scale 1. I feel that this internship enables me to do the kind of work I want to do (from Saks & Ashforth, 1997). 2. This internship measures up to the kind of internship I was seeking (from Saks & Ashforth, 1997). 3. This internship is a good match for me (from Saks & Ashforth, 1997). 4. This internship fulfills my needs (from Saks & Ashforth, 1997). Received January 27, 2006 Revision received September 1, 2006 Accepted October 6, 2006 䡲