A Review and Conceptual Analysis of Emotions in Financial Markets Tommy Gärling and Amelie Gamble Department of Psychology and Center for Finance, School of Business, Economics, and Law, University of Gothenburg P.O. Box 500, SE-405 30 Göteborg, Sweden Darren Duxbury Newcastle University Business School, Newcastle University, 5 Barrack Road, Newcastle upon Tyne, NE1 4SE, UK Abstract Emotions have played a negligible role in decision-making research until about two decades ago. In financial economics a lagged interest in emotions is observed. In the paper we review the relatively scant research on emotions in financial markets including studies of effects of mood proxies on performance of financial markets, field studies of financial investors´ emotions, and laboratory tests of emotion influences on financial investors. Several shortcomings are identified. One is lack of clarity in defining different emotion constructs. Our contribution is a classification of emotion-related phenomena drawing on basic emotion research. We then follow up on this by demonstrating how emotions may explain buy and sell decisions in asset markets. We argue that this is the level that needs to be examined although it should not detract from the importance of analyses of market consequences. It is also necessary to taking into account that investors comprise a heterogeneous group varying in several characteristics that are likely moderators of emotion influences. Key words: Emotion; Financial market; Investor; Measurement Ever since the seminal formal analysis of decision making published by von Neumann and Morgernstern (1947), emotions have played a negligible role in decision-making research. About two decades ago this started to change (Peters, Västfjäll, Gärling, & Slovic, 2006; Weber & Johnson, 2009), importantly due to influences from the neuropsychological research by Damasio and collaborators (Damasio, 1994). In financial economics we currently observe a lagged interest in emotions. We consider this to be a desirable development. As argued by among others Forgas (1995), Loewenstein (1999), and Schwarz (2000), the influences of emotions on judgment and decision making are straight-forward, ubiquitous, and not necessarily irrational. Our stance elaborated on later is that emotions are input to the decisionmaking process engaging investors in asset markets moderated by their mandate, liquidity, investment horizon, experience, and sophistication. All-in-all, the complex interaction of emotion, cognition, and decision making at play in the financial markets make such a setting a rich, but challenging environment in which to examine further the influence of emotions. In this paper we first review the relatively scant research on emotions in financial markets. Since this research tends to lack appropriate conceptualizations of emotions, in a following section we present a classification of emotion-related phenomena drawing on basic emotion research. A section then follows that presents an emotion account of buy and sell preferences in asset markets. Emotions in Financial Markets Mood proxies and performance of financial markets Although few would deny that emotions have some influence on judgment and decision making in financial markets, the market impacts may not be substantial (Mehra & Sah, 2002). With the aim of investigating this, several studies have empirically attempted to determine effects of mood proxies, that is, factors that are known or believed to induce positive or negative mood in people. In some early studies below-average returns in equity markets were shown to correlate with (1) cloudy weather possibly leading to a negative mood (Saunders, 1993; Hishleifer & Shumway, 2003), (2) geomagnetic storms presumably causing depression (Krivelyova & Robotti, 2003), (3) the full moon phase of the lunar cycle associated with depressed mood (Dichev & Janes, 2003; Yuan, Zheng, & Zhu, 2006)1, and (4) lower temperatures believed to increase aggressive risk-taking that results in above-average returns (Cao & Wei, 2005). Below-average returns due to season have also been observed (Kamstra, Kramer, & Levi, 2003), presumably due to shorter daylight hours (“winter blues”). Other studies (Kamstra, Kramer, & Levi, 2000) have attempted to show negative effects of disruption in sleep patterns dependent on daylight savings time changes. Two studies in the UK (Dowling & Lucey, 2005, 2008) including several mood proxies related to weather and bio-rhythm replicated some of the previous results. In reviewing these and other findings published before 2005, Lucey and Dowling (2005) note that mood misattribution (Schwartz & Strack, 1999) is a possible causal mechanism. A criticism is that only mood proxies have been studied, with little or no attempt to measure investors’ moods directly. This leaves such empirical studies open to critique that claims the reported results are explained by other factors. For example, Jacobsen and Masquering (2008, 2009) suggest that the findings reported in Kamstra et al. (2003) can be explained by a range of other factors related to the seasons (they illustrate with ice cream consumption and airline travel), thus calling into question their conclusion that changes in investors’ moods associated with the Seasonal 1 While Keef and Khaled (2011) confirm the findings in Dichev and Janes (2003) and Yuan et al .(2006), that mean returns during the full moon phase are significantly lower than those during the new moon phase, they find that the mean return on new moon days is higher than that observed on lunar control days, but the mean return on full moon days is not lower than observed on such control days. Thus, they conclude that while there is evidence of an enhanced new moon effect, there is little evidence of a depressed or negative full moon effect. Affective Disorder directly influence stock market returns. The findings in Kamstra et al. (2000, 2003) are also challenged by Gerlach (2010), who provides empirical evidence to suggest that their results may be driven by seasonal patterns in market-related information and not by changes in investor mood, while Gregory-Allen, Jacobsen, and Marquering (2010). (2010), in a comprehensive study of daylight saving time changes, fail to find evidence to support the hypothesis that investor mood changes induced by disrupted sleeping patterns impact on stock returns. Furthermore, other measures of market performance than returns are needed to increase understanding of the role of emotions. A notable exception is the study by Symeonidis, Daskalakis, and Markellos (2010) examining the association between stock market volatility and mood-proxies related to the weather conditions (cloudiness, temperature and precipitation) and night-time length. They report that cloudiness and length of night-time are inversely related to various measures of volatility. Another emotion influence on prices in financial markets is investor sentiment (Baker & Wurgler, 2007; DeLong, Shleifer, Summers, & Waldman, 1990) depending on widespread moods in a society what Nofsinger (2005) and others refer to as “social moods”. Social moods are the outcomes of interpersonal communication and would in general be adaptive. Examples include optimism or hope about the future or pessimism about and fear for the future. In particular investors’ buying and selling decisions in asset markets may easily be influenced by such optimism or pessimism, as suggested by excessive price volatility (Shiller, 2003). Furthermore, speculative bubbles may be driven by high optimism (Shiller, 2002). Similar to the caveats noted above, this research suffers from lack of measures of social moods. A possible solution is demonstrated in a study of Bollen, Mao, and Zeng (2011) that developed and cross-validated a measure of social mood based on classified recordings of daily twitter feeds. A high degree of predictability of changes in stock prices was observed. Another approach is exemplified by Bialkowski, Etebari, and Wisniewski (2012) who in 14 predominantly Muslim countries found that stock returns were higher and less volatile during the month of Ramadan which is a significant social event when fasting is believed to have positive mood effects. Field studies of financial investors´ emotions Other research has investigated emotion effects by directly targeting investors´ responses. In semi-structured interviews of investors employed by four London investment banks, Fenton-O´Creevy, Soane, Nicholson, and Willman (2011) found emotion and cognition to be inextricably linked in the investors´ investment decisions. The question they asked was therefore whether there are more or less effective strategies of managing emotions. The answer was that high-performing investors regulated emotions better than low-performing investors, in particular by avoiding being influenced by negative emotions. It was also found that experience improved efficient emotion regulation. Another observation was that highperforming experienced investors were less influenced by (e.g.,weather-related) mood not being relevant for their decisions. In a pioneering study by Lo and Repin (2002), a broad battery of physiological emotion markers was applied to monitor emotions in ten professional investors during their regular trading activities. The measurements were correlated with real market data including several types of price changes identified in advance by the investors themselves to being in need of their attention. Emotion effects related to the price changes were observed compared to controls, larger in less than in more experienced investors but still present in the latter. An acknowledged problem is clearly to infer which role the emotions played for the trading decisions. In a subsequent study by Lo, Repin, and Steenbarger (2005) asking trader trainees to provide self-reports of how they felt after each trading day, modest correlations were found between trading performance and emotion. In this study possible emotion effects on decisions would be observed on subsequent trading days but any such effects were not reported. Fenton- O´Creevy, Lins, Vohra, Richards, Davies, and Schaaf (2012) obtained physiological emotion marker data from 28 professional traders who were market makers. The results supported their previous interview data (Fenton-O´Creevy et al., 2011) in showing that experienced traders were less affected. Consistent with the results of Lo and Repin (2002), high price volatility had large effects. This effect was consistent with observed higher cortisol levels related to fear responses observed by Coates and Herbert (2008). The reported studies are steps towards an increased understanding of the role of emotion effects in financial markets. Yet, the studies are only empirical demonstrations. Hypotheses are needed to be derived from theory and tested. Laboratory tests of emotion influences on financial investors Some laboratory studies complement the research on effects of mood proxies in measuring or inducing mood in participants who are asked to perform an investment task. A general finding is that mood may have both positive and negative effects on investment performance. Seo and Feldman Barret (2007) distinguished between feeling-as-bias-inducers implying that feelings induce various biases in the decision-making process, for instance, that people in a positive mood make judgments congruent with their mood (Isen, 2000), and feeling-asdecision-facilitators implying that mood improves decision making, for instance, that people in a negative mood invest more effort in information processing (Schwartz, 2000). In an empirical study, investment performance was related to feelings measured by means of checklists of adjectives sampled from the affect grid (Russell, 1980, 2003; Yik, Russell, & Steiger, 2011) to vary in both valence and activation (arousal). Participants were on each of 20 trading days asked to make simulated investment decisions and report their feelings, broadly defined as mood or discrete emotions directed towards an object. Higher affective reactivity was hypothesized and found to have positive effects on investment performance. Implying again that experience is important, regulation of affect-influences mediated the positive effect on performance. A similar procedure was used by Au, Chan, Wang, and Vertinsky (2003) to study foreign exchange trading. An important difference was that positive and negative moods were induced in different experimental groups. The results showed that in a positive mood performance was worse and in a negative mood better than in a neutral mood. Similar results were found by Kuhnen and Knutson (2011) in a task designed to measure probability beliefs and risk-taking in repeated choices between a risky security and a risk-less bond. Before each of 90 choices a picture was shown to induce a highly arousing positive, a highly arousing negative or a neutral mood. Less suboptimal risk-taking was observed following negative pictures compared to positive or neutral pictures. Laboratory studies have also attempted to investigate investor sentiment. Bosman and Van Winden (2010) designed an experiment to simulate influences of social moods, for instance, fear due to international terrorism or political risks that may nourish aversion to risky investments. In repeated trials participants either choose or did not choose a risky option. After each choice they rated how they felt on scales defined by emotion words. In a “global risk” condition subsequent to the base-line condition, participants were told that they with some probability at the end would loose what they had earned. In this condition compared to the base-line condition choices of the risky option were on average lower. It was also found that amount invested was related to rated fear but that there was no difference between the “global risk” and base-line conditions. Social moods may also in a society or subgroups (e.g., investors) induce positive or negative attitudes towards industrial sectors such as, for instance, information technology or even specific companies listed on stock markets. Although attitudes have both informational and affective determinants (Eagley & Chaiken, 1993), in several finance studies the focus has been on affective determinants. Thus, an investment option associated with a positive or negative affect-laden evaluation may when other information is unavailable or inaccessible influence choice of the option. Slovic, Finucane, Peters, and MacGregor (2002) referred to this as the affect heuristic. In a pioneering study (MacGregor, Slovic, Dreman, & Berry, 2000) it was shown that positive affect images associated with industrial sectors influenced simulated investments made by banking students that would have lead to worse outcomes. In a similar study of a sample of business and economics undergraduates, Kempf, Merkle, and Niessen-Ruenzi (2013) found that if attitudes towards 30 German companies rated on adjective scales were classified as positive, returns on stocks in the companies were rated high and risk low. In contrast, if the attitudes were classified as negative, returns were rated low and risk high. Financial literacy reduced the difference, which is consistent with that returns and risk are generally negatively instead of positively correlated. But also experts may conceivably under some circumstances make the same mistake. Finucane, Alhakami, Slovic, and Johnson (2000) found that a negative correlation between rated benefits and risk was less negative under time pressure than under no time pressure. A study attempting to show individual differences in response to positive and negative price shocks were reported by Muehlfield, Weitzel, and van Wittelosstuijn (2013). Their hypothesis was that investors would differ depending on the relative impact of two different motivational systems (Gray, 1987), the Behavioral Approach System (BAS) and the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS). Investors high in BAS (as measured by the BIS/BAS self-report scale developed by Carver and White, 1994) should emphasize the upside of price movements, people high in BIS should in contrast emphasize the downside. Shocks were expected to exaggerate these differences. An asset–market experiment employing undergraduates found that irrespectively of shock, high BAS compared to high BIS participants traded more actively, were more risk taking and, except when the shock was negative, generated higher profits. Positive shocks “unfreezed” participants high in BIS who started to trade more and take more risk. In order to further increase the understanding of the role of emotions, still another approach (see Dowling & Lucey, 2005; Gärling, 2011) would be to identify the role emotions may play in observed anomalies in financial markets. One of the most well-documented and robust anomalies is the disposition effect referring to the observation that winners are hold too short and losers too long (Shefrin & Statman, 1985). In explaining the disposition effect, Odean (1998), Shefrin and Statman (1985), and Weber and Camerer (1998) all draw on prospect theory (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Tversky & Kahneman, 1992). A necessary auxiliary hypothesis is that of realization utility (Barberis & Xiong, 2012), that is, that the utility of gains and disutility of potential (paper) losses are derived from realizing the outcome. Frydman et al. (2014) conducted a test of this hypothesis in an experimental market with an unspecified group of 28 participants. For the average participant the results conclusively demonstrated the disposition effect. Evidence in support of the realization utility hypothesis was secured from brain-scanning (functional magnetic resonance imaging or fMRI) data showing that at the moment of making a sell decision, neural activity in the brain was as expected proportional to the capital gain. Similar results for losses were however not found. It may also be asked whether the observed neural activity corresponds to emotions. Related studies by Kuhnen and Knutson (2005) and Knutson, Wimmer, Kuhnen, and Winkielman (2008) suggest this. Another unresolved question is whether the neural activity corresponds to anticipated or experienced emotion. In the affect account of the disposition effect proposed by Gärling, Blomman, and Carle (2014), only the former (as well as anticipatory emotions, see next section) would have an influence on the sell decisions. Summers and Duxbury (2012) similarly investigated the role of emotion in accounting for the disposition effect. They did that in experiments examining whether an outcome experienced in one period (at that point a potential gain or loss) and its associated selfreported emotional response had an impact on a sell or hold decision in the next period. No responsibility for the decision to hold the risky asset in the first period lead to disappointment due to a loss outcome and elation due to a gain outcome, while responsibility additionally lead to regret due to the loss and rejoicing due to the gain outcome. It is concluded that regret is necessary to drive investors to continue to holding losing shares, while elation is necessary to cause investors to sell winning shares. A Classification of Emotions Evaluations versus emotions An outcome of a choice is normally perceived to have an affective quality (e.g., good, bad or neutral) (Russell, 2003). We refer to this as an evaluation of the choice outcome. An emotion is a response to an impact of an object (e.g. the choice outcome) (Russell, 2003). An evaluation as bad or good does not normally have an emotion impact. According to several emotion theories (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1990; Lazarus, 1991; Oatley, 2009), this will occur if and only if the outcome has personal relevance, for instance if it is perceived to facilitate attainment of a positive personal goal or prevent attainment of a negative personal goal. An issue is to identify conditions that are personally relevant. One approach is to consider how much an investor has at stake, for instance, as in Summers and Duxbury (2012), whether being responsible for holding a risky asset. There are presumably several other conditions yet to be identified. Russell (2003) posits that core affects are elemental building blocks involved in all emotion responses or states. More precisely, a core affect is a “neurophysiological state consciously accessible as the simplest raw (nonreflective) feelings evident in moods and emotions” (p. 148). Corroboration comes from brain-imaging research (e.g., WilsonMendenhall, Feldman Barret, & Barsalou, 2013). Core affects are always accessible, either being neutral or having any other value in a dimensional system defined by the axis pleasuredispleasure and activation-deactivation. Several different methods to measure affect (selfreports, peripheral physiology, startle responses, EEG, neuro-imaging with fMRI or PET, face expressions measured with EMG) support a dimensional description although all the methods do not converge on the two dimensions of pleasure and activation (arousal) (Mauss & Robinson, 2009). Approach-avoidance has been proposed to be a third dimension. Yet, this dimension has been found to be a function of valence and activation (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974; Västfjäll, Gärling, & Kleiner, 2001). Another criticism (e.g. Lazarus, 1991) is that emotions are discrete. A counter-argument is that discrete states may be conceptualized as combinations of multiple dimensions. The two-dimensional system of pleasure and activation is illustrated in Figure 1 (Russell, 1980, 2003; Yik et al., 2011), also showing discrete emotion states located in the dimensional system. Incidental versus integral emotions An emotion impact of a choice outcome is an example of an integral emotion. Another example is that a choice is influenced by an anticipated emotion associated with its outcome. An unrelated emotion state such as mood affecting the choice is considered to be incidental. The bulk of emotion research in financial markets (e.g., Lucey & Dowling, 2005) appears to target incidental emotions or mood effects. Because emotion and mood are not experienced to be very different, their distinction is less clear to both lay people and researchers (Beedie, Terry, & Lane, 2005). Language is also a fallible source for making or not making the distinction. The similarity of the experience of emotion and mood is consistent with Russell´s (2003) claim that mood is a prolonged core affect, also emphasizing that moods are less transient than emotions. Emotions are furthermore frequently stronger, thus would more likely occupy the conscious focus with mood residing in the background (Lazarus, 1991). Emotion impacts may trigger several transient feeling, physiological, and behavioral changes. The conscious substrate is changes in core affect resulting in discrete emotion states. Gärling et al. (2014) make the connection between emotion and mood even stronger by proposing that emotion impacts result in changes in mood (prolonged core affect) that linger after the transient changes caused by the emotion impact have dissipated. A good-bad evaluation of a personally relevant outcome is likely to have an emotion impact and thus also to some degree change mood in a positive or negative direction. Note however that the mood effect is weaker such that only a very strong negative emotion impact is likely to change the valence of current mood. Since people generally are in a positive mood (Biswar-Diener, Vittersø, & Diener, 2005), a change from a positive to a negative mood facing a negative emotion impact seems less likely than a change in the reverse direction facing a positive emotion impact (Erber, Erber, & Poe, 2004). Anticipatory versus anticipated emotions Anticipatory emotions are experienced when thinking about what may happen in the future. In the context of financial outcomes hope of earning and fear of losing would qualify. Investor sentiment of optimism and pessimism is a similar phenomenon in financial markets (Nofsinger, 2005). In the neuropsychological research reviewed by Bechara, Damasio, and Damasio (2000), the Iowa Gambling Task is used to investigate choices between decks of cards (sequences of choice outcomes) that either are associated with a higher volatility such that if chosen the probabilities of losing and gaining are both higher than if other decks associated with less volatility are chosen. Anticipatory negative feelings as indexed by a physiological marker such as the skin conductance response are more influenced by choices of the former than of the latter decks. At a conscious level anticipatory fear may be experienced to a larger extent than anticipatory hope when the volatility is higher. In reality the distinction between anticipatory emotion and mood is not perfectly clear. An anticipatory emotion is associated with a future unspecific event or series of event. In asset markets anticipatory emotions of hope and fear may be associated with expectations of gaining or losing but not with earning or losing any specific amounts. A mood is not associated with any specific future event or series of events although still influenced by events in the past. Anticipatory emotions may be shaped by mood, for instance a positive mood may strengthen a brighter outlook of the future, a negative mood the reverse. Or the casual influence may go in the reverse direction. The degree of influence in this direction may partly depend on individuals´ emotional responsiveness (Rusting, 1998). Another less clear distinction is that between anticipatory and anticipated emotions. In contrast to the former, anticipated emotions are associated with specific choice outcomes (Mellers, 2000), for instance the degree of anticipated elation that may vary with the amount of gain or the degree of anticipated disappointment that may vary with the amount of loss. Anticipatory and anticipated emotion may furthermore differ in quality, in our example hopefear versus elation-disappointment. Another distinction is that anticipatory emotions are felt, whereas anticipated emotions are imagined. Both would still have conscious elements of core affects (Västfjäll, Gärling, & Kleiner, 2004). A large body of research investigating affective forecasting (e.g., Loewenstein & Schkade, 1999) has nevertheless demonstrated that anticipated emotions tend to be underestimation of the corresponding experienced emotions whether they are positive or negative. Emotion Account of Buy and Sell Preferences Influences of emotions in asset markets are documented by the studies we reviewed above. At the same time we have noted that these studies have largely failed to specify the targeted type of emotion. In the preceding section we made distinctions between different emotion constructs that we draw on in the following, that is, the distinctions between incidental (mood) and integral (anticipatory and anticipated) emotions and between anticipatory and anticipated emotions. We also note that our focus is solely on emotions, that is, evaluations that have personal relevance. In our analysis presented in this section we propose that price movement in an asset market is a primary covariate of emotion. As illustrated in Figure 2, we distinguish between (1) changes in a (pleasant-displeasant) mood that, despite being influenced by circumstances incidental to price movements, still may influence investor decision making (Isen, 2000; Schwarz, 2000); (2) changes in anticipatory emotions of hope of earning and fear of losing due to the price movements (Lopes, 1987; Nofsinger, 2005; Shefrin and Statman, 2000), and; (3) changes in anticipated emotions of elation and disappointment (or pride and regret) associated with decisions to realize gains and losses (Mellers, 2000). We further suggest that whereas current mood may have a direct effect on choices, anticipatory emotions have effects on choices through anticipated emotions. Anticipatory hope is strengthened by price increases such that anticipated elation associated with buying or selling winners are triggered. Conversely, decreasing prices strengthen anticipatory fear that triggers anticipated disappointment associated with selling losers. Our claim is that investors2 with anticipatory emotions of hope dominating anticipatory emotions of fear are attracted to purchase stocks in upmarkets (Baker & Wurgler, 2007; Kubinska et al., 2012). They may alternatively be attracted to purchase a particular popular stock. This is illustrated in Figure 3 where we conjecture that some lag is needed for the hopefear balance to stabilize before preferences to buy are formed. A positive mood due to other influences (a sunny weather, a windfall salary) may sometimes strengthen the hope-fear balance such that preferences to buy increases. More precisely, we propose that a preference to sell is formed when anticipated elation is equally strong as anticipatory hope or anticipated disappointment is equally strong as anticipatory fear. We assume that hope-fear varies along an axis oblique to the main axes in the affect grid (see Figure 1; Yik et al., 2011), whereas elation-disappointment varies along an axis at an angle to hope-fear. When moving from one end-point of the continua to the other end-point, hope3 or elation decreases at the same time as fear or disappointment increases. This is what we assume happens when prices change. We further assume the preference order elation > hope > disappointment > fear (Västfjäll & Gärling, 2006). Elation is thus preferred to hope and disappointment to fear when being of equal strength, We further propose that a preference to sell winners is determined by the price reached when anticipated elation associated with the outcome of a choice to sell is equally strong as anticipatory hope. Volatility of prices may increase fear and thus decrease the hope-fear balance such that the preference to sell is reached at a lower price. An asset price may still start to decrease before this happens. A downward price trend then elicits anticipatory fear of 2 Investors are known to be a heterogeneous group differing in mandate, liquidity, investment horizon, experience, and sophistication. Even though the consequences for rational investment behavior differ (e.g., Feng & Seasholes, 2005), we see no compelling reason to not claim that all investors are influenced by emotions. Yet, emotions determining preferences to buy or sell are only one input to a decision process resulting in a buy or sell choice. The characteristics of this decision process are beyond our scope. In less experienced and sophisticated investors with a short investment horizon and possibly limited liquidity, the decision process is likely to be shortcut such that emotions play a larger role (Finucane, Alhakami, Slovic, & Johnson, 2000). Note also that we use the term preference to refer to what in consumer research would be called intention to buy (or sell). In addition to the investor characteristics listed above, whether the preference is realized as a choice depends of course also on the availability of other market actors willing to sell or buy. 3 In analogy with some neurophysiological research (LeDoux, 1996) hope may alternatively be conceived of as reflecting cognitive control of emotion (fear). Why hope gives way for elation may conversely be conceived of as loss of cognitive control of temptation. Then, the prediction that sell preferences are formed when hope and elation is equally strong needs to be justified in some other way. losing. We propose that preferences to sell are deferred until anticipated disappointment is equally strong as anticipatory fear. Anticipatory fear may still for some time be balanced by anticipatory hope of price reversion or high volatility of price decreases. Preferences for selling may also be deferred because investors shield themselves from negative market information (Karlsson, Loewenstein, & Seppi, 2009). Conclusion The review of emotion research in financial markets reveals a number of short-comings. One is the lack of clarity in defining different emotion constructs. Our contribution is a discussion of several useful distinctions. The most fundamental one, almost always overlooked, is that between evaluation and emotion impact. It cannot thus be taken for granted that investors respond emotionally unless they have some personal involvement. The counter-argument is that gains or losses of money always have personal relevance. Yet, there are other conditions (e.g. responsibility, reputation) which for at least some are more personally relevant and therefore result in larger emotion impacts. We then follow up our distinctions (i.e., between anticipatory and anticipated emotions) by demonstrating how they may be used to explain buy and sell decisions in asset markets. We argue that this is the level that needs to be examined although it should not detract from the importance of analyses of market consequences. It is also necessary to take into account that investors comprise a heterogeneous group varying in several characteristics that are likely moderators of emotion influences. Another observation is the over-reliance in finance research on physiological markers. Even though their validity has improved, self-reports of emotion are still essential, at least to provide converging evidence (Mauss & Robinson, 2009; Wilson-Mendenhall et al., 2013). It is hard to otherwise claim that emotion is measured. Ending on a positive note, we praise the use of asset-market experiments, which should improve generalizability of the results. This method could and should also be used to test changes to existing markets and actors in these markets, not only to simulate the existing conditions. References Au, K., Chan, F., Wang, D., & Vertinsky, I. (2003). Mood in foreign exchange trading: Cognitive processes and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91, 322–338. Baker, M., & Wurgler, J. (2007). Investor sentiment in the stock market. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21(2), 129-152. Barberis, N., & Xiong, W. (2012). Realization utility. Journal of Financial Economics, 104(2), 251-271. Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 215-225. Beedie, C. J., Terry, P. C., & Lane, A. M. (2005). Distinctions between emotion and mood.Cognition and Emotion, 19, 847-878. Bialkowski, J., Etebari, A., & Wisniewski, T. P. (2012). Fast profits: Investor sentiment and stock returns during Ramadan. Journal of Banking and Finance, 36, 835-845. Biswar-Diener, R., Vittersø, J., & Diener, E. (2005). Most people are pretty happy, but there is cultural variation: The Inughuit, the Amish, and the Maasai. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6, 205-226 Bollen, J., Mao, H., & Zeng, X. (2011). Twitter mood predicts the stock market. Journal of Computational Science, 2, 1-8. Bosman, R., & Van Winden, F. (2010). Global risk, investment and emotions. Economica, 77, 451-471. Cao, M., & Wei, J. (2005). Stock market returns: A note on temperature anomaly. Journal of Banking and Finance, 29, 1559-1573. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19 – 35. Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319-333. Coates, J. M., & Herbert, J. (2008). Endogenous steroids and financial risk taking on a London trading floor. PANAS, 105, 6167-6172. Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Avon. DeLong, J. B., Shleifer, A., Summers, L. H., & Waldman, R. J. (1990). Noise trader risk in financial markets. Journal of Political Economy, 98, 703-738. Dichev, I. D., & Janes, T. D. (2003). Lunar cycle effects in stock returns. The Journal of Private Equity, 6(4), 8-29. Dowling, M., & Lucey, B. M. (2005). Weather, biorhythms, beliefs and stock returns—Some preliminary Irish evidence. International Review of Financial Analysis, 14(3), 337-355. Dowling, M., & Lucey, B. M. (2008). Mood and UK equity pricing. Applied Financial Economics Letters, 4(4), 233-240. Eagley, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Hartcourt. Erber, R., Erber, M. W., & Poe, J. (2004). Mood regulation and decision-making. Is irrational exuberance really a problem? In I. Brocas, & J. D. Carillo (Eds.), The psychology of economic decisions (Vol 2, pp. 197-210). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Feng, L., & Seasholes, M. S. (2005). Do investor sophistication and trading experience eliminate behavioral biases in financial markets? Review of Finance, 9, 305-351. Fenton- O´Creevy, M., Lins, J. T., Vohra, S., Richards, D. W., Davies, G., and Schaaf, K. (2012). Emotion regulation and trader expertise: Heart rate variability on the trading floor. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 5(4), 227-237. Fenton-O´Creevy, M., Soane, E., Nicholson, N., & Willman, P. (2011) Thinking, feeling and deciding: The influence of emotions on the decision making and performance of traders. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32, 1044-1061. Finucane, M. L., Alhakami, A., Slovic, P., Johnson, S. M. (2000). The affect heuristic in judgments of risk and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13, 1-17. Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39-66. Frydman, C., Barberis, N., Camerer, C., Bossaerts, P., & Rangel, A. (2014). Using neural data to test a theory of investor behavior: An application to realization utility. Journal of Finance, 69, 907-946. Gärling, T. (2011). Heuristics and biases making people rich or poor: A psychological account of stock market anomalies. In W. Brun, G. Keren, G. Kirkeboen & H. Montgomery (Eds.), Perspectives on thinking, judging, and decision making (pp. 146-156). Oslo: Universitetsforlaget. Gärling, T., Blomman, M., & Carle, T. A. Affect account of the disposition effect and consequences for stock prices. Manuscript. Gärling, T., Ettema, D., Friman, M., & Olsson, L. E. (2014). Experienced utility conceptualized as updated current mood, and remembered utility conceptualized as reconstructed current moods. Manuscript. Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Gerlach, J. R. (2010). Daylight and investor sentiment: A second look at two stock market behavioral anomalies. Journal of Financial Research, 33(4), 429-462. Gregory‐Allen, R., Jacobsen, B., & Marquering, W. (2010). The Daylight Saving Time Anomaly in Stock Returns: Fact or Fiction? Journal of Financial Research, 33(4), 403-427. Hirshleifer, D., & Shumway, T. (2003). Good day sunshine: Stock returns and the weather. Journal of Finance, 56, 1533-1597. Isen, A. M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis & J. M. Havieland (Eds.). Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 417-435). London: Guilford. Jacobsen, B., & Marquering, W. (2008). Is it the weather? Journal of Banking & Finance, 32(4), 526-540. Jacobsen, B., & Marquering, W. (2009). Is it the weather? Response. Journal of Banking & Finance, 33(3), 583-587. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. Kamstra, M. J., Kramer, L. A., & Levi, M. D. (2000). Losing sleep at the market: The daylight-savings anomaly. American Economic Review, 90, 1005-1011. Kamstra, M. J., Kramer, L. A., & Levi, M. D. (2003). Winter Blues: A sad stock market cycle. American Economic Review, 93, 324-343. Karlsson, N., Loewenstein, G. F., and Seppi, D. J. (2009). The Ostrich effect: Selective attention to information. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 38(2), 95-115. Keef, S. P., & Khaled, M. S. (2011). Are investors moonstruck? Further international evidence on lunar phases and stock returns. Journal of Empirical Finance, 18(1), 56-63. Kempf, A., Merkle, C., & Niessen-Ruenzi, A. (2013). Low risk and high return – affective attitudes and stock market expectations. European Financial Management. doi: 10.1111/eufm.12001 Knutson, B., Wimmer, G. E., Kuhnen, C. M., and Winkielman, P. (2008). Nucleus accumbens activation mediates the influence of reward cues on financial risk taking. NeuroReport, 19(5), 509-513. Krivelyova, A., & Robotti, C. (2003). Playing the field. Geomagnetic storms and intrnational stock markets. Retrieved from https:// Kubinska, E., Markiewics, L., & Tyszka, T. (2012). Disposition effect among contrarian and momentum investors. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 13(3), 214-225. Kuhnen, C. M., & Knutson, B. (2005). The neural basis of financial risk taking. Neuron, 47, 763-770. Kuhnen, C. M., & Knutson, B. (2011). The impact of affect on beliefs, preferences and financial decisions. Journal of Financial and Quantative Analysis, 46, 605-626. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press. LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster. Lo, A., and Repin, D. V. (2002). The psychophysiology of real-time financial risk processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 323-339. Lo, A., Repin, D. V., & Steenbarger, B. N. (2005). Fear and greed in financial markets: A clinical study of day-traders. American Economic Review, 95, 352-359. Loewenstein, G. F. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 272-292. Loewenstein, G. F., & Schkade, D. (1999). “Wouldn´t it be nice?” Predicting future feelings. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 85-108). New York: Russell Sage. Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, E. S. (1996). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267-286. Lopes, L. (1987). Between hope and fear: The psychology of risk. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20(3), 255-295. Lucey, B. M., and Dowling, M. (2005). The role of feelings in investor decision-making. Journal of Economic Surveys, 19(2), 211-237. MacGregor, D. G., Slovic, P., Dreman, D., and Berry, M. (2000). Imagery, affect, and financial judgment. Journal of Psychology and Financial Markets, 1(2), 104-110. Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 209-237. Mehra, R., & Sah, R. (2002). Mood fluctuations, projection bias and volatility of equity prices. Journal of Economic Dynamics and Control, 26, 869-887. Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. (1974). An approach to environmental psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Mellers, B. A. (2000). Choice and the relative pleasures of consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 910-924. Muehlfeld, K., Weitzel, U., & Van Witteoostuijn, A. (2013). Fight or freeze? Individual differences in investors´ motivational systems and trading in experimental asset markets. Journal of Economic Psychology, 34, 195-209. Nofsinger, J. R. (2005). Social mood and financial economics. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 6, 144-160. Oatley, K. (2009). Communication to self and others: Emotional experience and its skill. Emotion Review, 1, 206-213. Odean, T. (1998). Are investors reluctant to realize their losses? Journal of Finance, 53(5), 1775-1798. Peters, E., Västfjäll, D., Gärling, T., & Slovic, P. (2006). Affect and decision making: A ”hot” topic (introduction to special issue). Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 79-85. Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178. Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145-172. Rusting, C. L. (1998). Personality, mood, and cognitive processing of emotional information: Three conceptual frameworks. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 165-196. Saunders, E. M. (1993). Stock prices and Wall Street weather. American Economic Review, 83, 1337-1345. Schwarz, N. (2000). Emotion, cognition, and decision making. Cognition and Emotion, 14(4), 433-440. Seo, M.-G., & Feldman Barret, L. (2007). Being emotional during decision making: Good or bad? An empirical investigation. Academy of Management Journal, 50, 923-940. Shefrin, H. M., & Statman, M. (1985). The disposition to sell winners too early and ride losers too long. Journal of Finance, 40(3), 777-790. Shefrin, H. M., & Statman, M. (2000). Behavioral portfolio theory. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, 32(2), 127-151. Shiller, R. J. (2002). Bubbles, human judgment, and expert opinion. Financial Analyst Journal, 58(3), 18-26. Shiller, R. J. (2003). From efficient market theory to behavioral finance. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 17, 83-104. Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). The affect heuristic. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Intuitive judgment: Heuristics and biases (pp. 397-420). New York: Cambridge University Press. Staw, B. M. (1981). The escalation of commitment to a course of action. Academy of Management Review, 6(4), 577-587. Summers, B., & Duxbury, D. (2012). Decision-dependent emotions and behavioral anomalies. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 118(2), 226–238. Symeonidis, L., Daskalakis, G., & Markellos, R. N. (2010). Does the weather affect stock market volatility? Finance Research Letters, 7(4), 214-223. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5(4), 297-323. Västfjäll, D., & Gärling, T. (2006). Preferences for negative emotions. Emotion, 6(2), 326329. Västfjäll, D., Gärling, T., & Kleiner, M. (2001). Does it make you happy feeling this way? A core affect account of preference for current mood. Journal of Happiness Studies, 2, 337-354. Västfjäll, D., Gärling, T., & Kleiner, M. (2004). Preference for mood, emotional reaction, and anticipated emotional reaction. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 45, 25-34. Von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1947). Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Weber, M., & Camerer, C. F. (1998). The disposition effect in securities trading: An experimental analysis. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 33(2), 167-184. Weber, E. U., & Johnson, E. J. (2009). Mindful judgment and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 53-85. Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Feldman Barret, L., & Barsalou, L. W. (2013). Neural evidence that human emotions share core affective properties. Psychological Science, 25, 947-956. Yik, M., Russell, J. A., & Steiger, J. H. (2011). A 12-point circumplex structure of core affect. Emotion, 11, 705-731. Yuan, K., Zheng, L., & Zhu, Q. (2006). Are investors moonstruck? Lunar phases and stock returns. Journal of Empirical Finance, 13(1), 1-23. Activation Fear Elation Displeasure Pleasure Disappointment Hope Deactivation Figure 1 Anticipatory emotion: Hope-fear Mood: Pleasantdispleasant Buy-sell preference Anticipated emotion: Elation – disappointment Figure 2 Balance Downward price trend Upward price trend Anticipatory hope – fear Sell Price change Buy Sell Anticipated elation disappointment Figure 3