Belison National School Belison, Antique ASSESSMENT ON THE EFFECTS OF LISTENING TO MUSIC TO THE COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE OF SCIENCE, TECHNOLOGY,ENGINEERING, MATHEMATICS (STEM) STUDENTS A Research Proposal Presented To Mr. Albert Morillo Belison National School Belison, Antique By Calixto Popelo John Francis Masabio Zynnette Kaye Ybera Mario Busayong Aldrin Subillaga Fritzel Eljane Mina Shermaine Chavez John Mison Ardonia JULY 2020 Belison National School Belison, Antique JULY 2020 Approval Sheet In Partial Fulfilment Of the Requirements for the Subject Practical Research 1 By: Calixto Popelo John Francis Masabio Zynnette Kaye Ybera Mario Busayong Aldrin Busayong Fritzel Eljane Mina Shermaine Chavez John Mison Ardonia Approved: Mr. Albert Morillo Noel A. Polaron Principal IV Belison National School Belison, Antique RESEARCH ABSTRACT Title: ASSESSMENT ON THE EFFECTS OF LISTENING TO MUSIC TO THE COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE OF SCIENCE, TECHNOLOGY,ENGINEERING, MATHEMATICS (STEM) STUDENTS Authors: Calixto Popelo Aldrin Subillaga John Francis Masabio Fritzel Eljane Mina Zynnette Kaye Ybera Shermaine Chavez Mario Busayong John Mison Ardonia Track: Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics Teacher: Mr. Albert Morillo School: Belison National School Address: Magsaysay St. Poblacion, Belison, Antique Date: July 2020 This descriptive-survey study was concerned with the effects of listening to music to the cognitive performance of STEM students. 30 students were selected to all senior high in Belison National School. Data were obtained through researcher-made questionnaires which have been validated and reliability-tested for utilization in the study. The survey instrument used in this study was designed to assess the effects of listening to music to the cognitive performance of STEM students of Belison National School during the Academic Year 2019-2020. Belison National School Belison, Antique Table of Contents Page Chapter 1: Introduction to the Study …………………………………………………….. 1 Background of the Study ………………………………………………………………. 1 Statement of the Problem …………………………………………………………… 4 Significance of the Study ……………………………………………………………. 4 Definition of Terms ………………………………………………………………….. 5 Delimitation of the Study ……………………………………………………………. 6 Chapter 2: Review of Related Literature ……………………………………………….. 7 The Relationship Between Music Aptitude and Music Performance ……………… 7 Cognitive Performance after Listening to Music …………………………………… 9 Background Music and Cognitive Abilities …………………………………………. 14 The Relationship Between Music Training and Cognitive Performance …………... 17 Theoretical Framework ……………………………………………………………… 19 Conceptual Framework ……………………………………………………………… 21 Chapter 3: Method ………………………………………………………………………… 23 Purpose of the Study and Research Design ………………………………………… 23 Respondents …………………………………………………………………………. 24 Instruments …………………………………………………………………………... 24 Data Collection ………………………………………………………………………. 24 Belison National School Belison, Antique Table of Contents Chapter 4: Result and Discussion ………………………………………………………… Page 25 Table 1: Demographic Profile ……………………………………………………….. 25 Table 2: Music Listening …………………………………………………………….. 26 Table 3: Studying with Music ……………………………………………………….. 26 Table 4: Memory Formation ………………………………………………………... 27 Table 5: Music and Concentration ………………………………………………….. 27 Table 6: Cognitive Performance …………………………………………………….. 28 Table 7: Beating Stress and Anxiety ………………………………………………... 28 Table 8: Effects of Music …………………………………………………………..... 29 Table 9: Mode of Studying ………………………………………………………….. 30 Table 10: Studying with Music ……………………………………………………… 30 Table 11: Music Genre ………………………………………………………………. 31 Chapter 5: Summary, Conclusions, and Recommendations …………………………… 32 Summary …………………………………………………………………………….. 32 Findings ………………………………………………………………………………. 33 Conclusions …………………………………………………………………………... 34 Recommendation ……………………………………………………………………. 35 Acknowledgement …………………………………………………………………… 36 References …………………………………………………………………………37 Belison National School Belison, Antique Chapter I Introduction to the Study Chapter One is divided into five parts: Background of the Study, Statement of the Problem, Significance of the Study, Definition of Terms, and Delimitation of the Study. Part One, Background of the Study, gives the overview or the rationale of the research problem. Part Two, Statement of the Problem, mentions general and specific problems Part Three, Significance of the Study, gives the importance of the result of the study to different persons, organizations, and institutions that directly or indirectly will benefit from it. Part Four, Definition of Terms, discusses the terms used in the study, which are defined conceptually and operationally. Part Five, Delimitation of the Study, cites the coverage and limitation of the study. Background of the Study Music is a very significant part of our daily lives; the image of the quietly-focused student isolating themselves into a personal study zone has led to interest into whether listening to music actually helps studies or not. Research into the field has proven fairly ambiguous, with many studies contradicting each other. However this does provide a useful insight for students who maybe looking into ways to use music to enhance their exam performance. However, research has shown that performance in tasks involving memory and concentration was better in a silent environment, though, studying in place often disturbed by talkers, sneezers, or traffic, few students have access to a silent study space. Subjects tested in environments with background music were found to get better results than those tested against background noise. Therefore, taking along an iPod and a set of headphones may come in handy if you’re looking to avoid being distracted by any ambient sounds. (www.independent.co.uk) 6 Belison National School Belison, Antique Music has become much more readily available to the public in the past decades. One influencing factor was the increasing availability of music: whilst in the past one was in need of CDs or tapes and an according player, nowadays music can be played digitally on many different devices such as computers, mobile phones or iPods. Furthermore, the choice of available songs is almost endless due to music portals. This makes it possible to select suitable songs for different situations, such as relaxing songs for a cozy evening or activating songs before going out. Due to these advances in music technology, learning with background music has received more and more attention over the last decade (e.g., Schwartz et al., 2017). For some situations it seems intuitive to think that music would help to enhance our experience – but how do music and learning fit together? At present the effects of background music while learning and the mechanisms behind this are unclear. On the one side, music seems to have a positive (Mozart effect; Rauscher et al., 1993) and stimulating effect (arousal-mood-hypothesis; Husain et al., 2002), which could improve learning. On the other side, background music could lead to an additional burden on working memory (seductive detail effect; e.g., Rey, 2012), thus hindering learning. To be able to simultaneously deal with the learning material and the background music, the learner’s working memory capacity is a crucial factor to consider. Music listening is one of the most enigmatic of human behaviors. Most common behaviors have a recognizable utility that can be plausibly traced to the practical motives of survival and procreation. Moreover, in the array of seemingly odd behaviors, few behaviors match music for commandeering so much time, energy, and money. Music listening is one of the most popular leisure activities. Music is a ubiquitous companion to people's everyday lives. The enthusiasm for music is not a recent development. Recognizably musical activities appear to have been present in every known culture on earth, with ancient roots extending back 250,000 years or more (see Zatorre and Peretz, 2001). The ubiquity and antiquity of music has inspired considerable speculation regarding its origin and function. (www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov) 7 Belison National School Belison, Antique Musical training has recently gained additional interest in education as increasing neuroscientific research demonstrates its positive effects on brain development. Neuroimaging revealed plastic changes in the brains of adult musicians but it is still unclear to what extent they are the product of intensive music training rather than of other factors, such as preexisting biological markers of musicality. In this review, we synthesize a large body of studies demonstrating that benefits of musical training extend beyond the skills it directly aims to train and last well into adulthood. For example, children who undergo musical training have better verbal memory, second language pronunciation accuracy, reading ability and executive functions. Learning to play an instrument as a child may even predict academic performance and IQ in young adulthood. The degree of observed structural and functional adaptation in the brain correlates with intensity and duration of practice. Importantly, the effects on cognitive development depend on the timing of musical initiation due to sensitive periods during development, as well as on several other modulating variables. Notably, we point to motivation, reward and social context of musical education, which are important yet neglected factors affecting the long-term benefits of musical training. Further, we introduce the notion of rhythmic entrainment and suggest that it may represent a mechanism supporting learning and development of executive functions. It also hones temporal processing and orienting of attention in time that may underlie enhancements observed in reading and verbal memory. We conclude that musical training uniquely engenders near and far transfer effects, preparing a foundation for a range of skills, and thus fostering cognitive development. (www.frontiersin.org) People intuitively use music to support their thinking at work. Spotify is full of playlists to help gear your cognitive state towards concentration and improved memory: you might even be using one while you are reading this very article! In part due to the popularity of this productivity hack, research on the topic of enhancing cognitive function with music is abundant. However, the wealth of investigation has focused on what happens to cognitive processing after listening to music; there is much less investigation of what occurs during music listening. According to studies, music exerts its effects on cognition (www.google.com/amp/syncproject.co/blog) 8 through its power on emotions. Belison National School Belison, Antique Statement of the Problem Generally this study is conducted to Assess the Effectiveness of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of STEM students of Belison National School (BNS) during the Academic Year 2019-2020. Specifically, this sought to answer the following questions: 1. What are the Effects of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of Students? 2. What is/are the perception to the Effects of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of Students ? Significance of the Study This study aimed to assess the effectiveness of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of STEM students of Belison National School (BNS). The results of this study will be benefited by the following: 1. Students The students will recognize the effects of listening to music in relation to their cognitive performance. 2. Teachers The teachers will be able to understand the effects of listening to music to the cognitive performance of students and encourage them to listen to different genres of music. 3. Administrators The administrators will support the school in implementing some of the activities where students can widen their knowledge through listening to music. 4. Parents Parents would be aware for the importance of listening to music in the learning of their children. 9 Belison National School Belison, Antique 5. Future Researchers Future researchers might use the result of this study in their own research. Definition of Terms Assessment Assessment is the process of gathering and discussing information from multiple and diverse sources in order to develop a deep understanding of what students know, understand and can do with their knowledge as a result of their educational experiences. (https://www.westminster.edu) In this study, the same definition was applied. Music Music is an art concerned with combining vocal or instrumental sounds for beauty of form or emotional expression, usually according to cultural standards of rhythm, melody, and, in most western music, harmony. (https://www.britanica.com) In this study, music refers to music that may improve focus on a task by providing motivation and improving mood. Cognitive Performance Cognitive Performance refers to multiple mental abilities, including learning, thinking, reasoning, remembering, problem solving, decision making and attention. (http://www.sciencedirect.com) In this study, the same definition was applied. 10 Belison National School Belison, Antique Delimitation of the Study This study aimed to Assess the Effects of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of STEM students. This study was limited to all Science, Technology, Engineering, Mathematics (STEM) students. The correspondents were randomly selected and interviewed as to their perception on the effects of listening to the music to their cognitive performance A set of questionnaires was used to determine the effects of Listening to Music to the Cognitive performance of Science, Technology, Engineering, Mathematics (STEM) students during the Academic year 2019-2020. 11 Belison National School Belison, Antique Chapter II Review of Related Literature Chapter Two presents literatures relevant to the cognitive performance of students when it comes to music. This includes four parts: The Relationship Between Music Aptitude and Cognitive Performance, Cognitive Performance after Listening to Music, Background Music and Cognitive Abilities, The Relationship Between Music Training and Cognitive Performance The Relationship Between Music Aptitude and Cognitive Performance Music aptitude refers to natural music abilities or the innate potential to succeed as a musician. One school of thought (e.g., Ericsson, Krampe, & Tesch-Ro¨mer, 1993; Howe, Davidson, & Sloboda, 1998) contends that innate music talent (i.e., aptitude plus a demonstrated ability to perform music) does not account for variations in levels of musicality. Rather, expert levels can be achieved by anyone who starts early enough and works hard enough. In short, practice makes perfect (cf. Meinz & Hambrick, 2010). The debate about the existence of music talent or aptitude is beyond the scope of the present chapter. We assume that music aptitude exists, that it varies among individuals, and that aptitude is something that tests of music aptitude measure. Although this definition is circular, our principal focus is on whether tests of music aptitude measure an ability that is independent of or associated with other cognitive abilities. The issue of music aptitude as an ability distinct from other cognitive abilities is closely related to concepts of modularity (Fodor, 1983; Peretz, 2009; Peretz & Coltheart, 2003) and multiple intelligences (Gardner, 1983, 1999). The notion of modularity proposes that (1) the brain has specialized modules for processing different kinds of information, (2) domain-specific information (re: language, faces, music, and so on) is processed automatically by the appropriate module, and (3) each module functions independently of other modules (Fodor, 1983). Gardner (1983, 1999) posits similarly that intelligence is a multidimensional construct. In the original formulation of his theory of multiple intelligences, he specified seven distinct intelligences: 12 Belison National School Belison, Antique bodily-kinesthetic, interpersonal, intrapersonal, linguistic, logical-mathematical, spatial, and most importantly, musical intelligence. From either the modularity or multiple intelligences perspective, music aptitude should be distinct from other abilities. The typical task on tests of music aptitude involves presenting two short melodies (or two short rhythms) on each trial. Listeners are asked whether the second melody (or rhythm) is the same as or different from the first. After several trials, a score is calculated separately for each test. An aggregate score can also be calculated by averaging across tests. The origin of music-aptitude testing is often attributed to Seashore (1919, 1960). Seashore’s test has six subtests, including pitch and rhythm tasks as well as subtests of loudness, meter, timbre, tonal memory, and an aggregate measure of general music aptitude. In North America, one of the most well-known measures is Gordon’s (1965) Music Aptitude Profile (MAP), which has seven subtests. Gordon later simplified his approach, forming three different tests based on grade level: the Primary Measures of Music Audiation (PMMA, kindergarten to third grade; Gordon, 1979), the Intermediate Measures of Music Audiation (IMMA, first to sixth grade; Gordon, 1982), and the Advanced Measures of Music Audiation (AMMA, seventh grade and higher; Gordon, 1989). Each test has only two subtests (pitch and rhythm). In the United Kingdom, Wing’s (1962) Musical Aptitude Test has been used frequently, as has Bentley’s (1966) Measures of Musical Abilities. All of the aptitude tests and their corresponding subtests tend to be correlated at moderate to high levels (e.g., Gordon, 1969; McLeish, 1968; Vispoel, 1992; Young, 1972, 1973). Unfortunately, there is no test of music aptitude that is considered to be the “gold standard.” Consequently, the particular test varies from study to study, which undoubtedly contributes to inconsistent findings. In tests of the validity of aptitude measures, criterion variables (i.e., those that should be correlated with aptitude) also vary across studies. Often, construct validity is tested by examining associations between aptitude scores and a teacher’s or parent’s subjective rating of the participant’s “musicality” (Davies, 1971; Drake, 1954; Harrington, 1969; Tarrell, 1965; Young, 1972, 1976). These correlations are typically positive but small to moderate in size, which is not surprising because aptitude tests present participants with short auditory sequences13that are a far cry from actual musical pieces. Music training is seldom used as a criterion variable precisely because aptitude tests are supposed to 13 Belison National School Belison, Antique measure natural music ability independent of training. Nevertheless, when musically trained and untrained participants are compared, the trained group typically has higher aptitude scores (Bentley, 1970; Davies, 1971; Drake, 1954; Flohr, 1981; Forgeard, Winner, Norton, & Schlaug, 2008; Gordon, 1969, 1980, 2001; Hassler, 1992; Hassler, Birbaumer, & Feil, 1985; Isaacs & Trofimovich, 2011; Milovanov, Pietila¨, Tervaniemi, & Esquef, 2010; Milovanov, Tervaniemi, Takio, & Ha¨ma¨la¨inen, 2007; Posedel, Emery, Souza, & Fountain, 2011; Tsang & Conrad, 2011; Wallentin, Nielsen, Friis-Olivarius, Vuust, & Vuust, 2010). The major question discussed here is whether music aptitude is related to nonmusical cognitive abilities. The relevant studies have examined associations between music aptitude and language skills, mathematical abilities, and general intelligence. Links between music aptitude and nonmusical abilities are problematic for theorists who posit modularity for music (Peretz, 2009; Peretz & Coltheart, 2003) or that music ability represents an intelligence distinct from other abilities (Gardner, 1983, 1999).(Retrieved from www.researchgate.net, February 28, 2020) Cognitive Performance after Listening to Music Schellenberg and his colleagues (Husain, Thompson, & Schellenberg, 2002; Nantais & Schellenberg, 1999; Schellenberg & Hallam, 2005; Schellenberg, Nakata, Hunter, & Tamoto, 2007; Thompson, Schellenberg, & Husain, 2001) sought to determine whether the Mozart effect, when evident, was a consequence of arousal and mood. The basic idea was that any stimulus that improves how a person feels can in turn improve how they perform on a cognitive task. The advantage of this hypothesis is that it explains the Mozart effect with two links that are well established in the psychological literature. Consider links between music listening and emotional responding. People choose to listen to music because of the way it makes them feel (e.g., Juslin & Va¨stfja¨ll, 2008; Lonsdale & North, 2011; Sloboda, 1992). Calming music changes cortisol levels (Flaten, Asli, & Simonsen, 2006) and blood pressure (Triller, Erzen, Dub, Petrinic-Primozic & Kosnik, 2006), and listening to music reduces anxiety in medical contexts (Bare & Dundes, 2004; Cooke, Chaboyer, Schluter, & Hiratos, 2005; Pelletier, 2004; Weeks & Nilsson, 2010). Music listening also facilitates falling asleep (Field, 1999), and it increases levels of sedation for patients in 14 Belison National School Belison, Antique intensive care (Dijkstra, Gamel, van der Bijl, Bots, & Kesecioglu, 2010). Listening to one’s preferred music can even reduce perceived pain levels after surgery (Ebneshahidi & Mohseni, 2008). Music’s ability to calm the listener is one reason why music therapy often yields positive effects (Gold, Voracek & Wigram, 2004). Thus, it is clear that music listening can change one’s emotional state. There is also an abundance of evidence that feelings influence cognitive performance (e.g., Cassady, Mohammed, & Mathieu, 2004; Isen & Labroo, 2003, O’Hanlon, 1981). Isen (2009) illustrated how positive affect enhances cognitive abilities, including decision making, problem solving, social interaction, and thought processes in general. Positive affect is associated with increases in dopamine levels, which may improve cognitive flexibility (Ashby, Isen, & Turken, 1999). Effects of emotional state on cognition are evident even with small increases in positive affect, such as those that occur when receiving a small gift or watching a comic film. For example, in a problem-solving task that required participants to15solve Duncker’s (1945) candle problem, participants who saw a brief comic film just before performed better than control participants (Isen, Daubman, & Nowicki, 1987). Similarly, participants who were given a small bag of candy performed better than controls on a test of remote associates (Isen et al., 1987). Moreover, negative affect (e.g., boredom) impairs cognitive performance (Cassady et al., 2004; O’Hanlon, 1981). Thus, links between arousal and/or mood and cognitive performance are also well established. In the first of a series of studies, Nantais and Schellenberg (1999) had participants complete one of two sets of PF&C items on two different visits to the laboratory, once after listening to Mozart (K. 448) for 10 minutes and once after sitting in silence. The order of the conditions (Mozart then silence or silence then Mozart) and the two sets of PF&C items was counterbalanced, with an equal number of participants in each of the four cells. PF&C performance was better after listening to Mozart than after sitting in silence, thus replicating the Mozart effect. This result was not surprising because the control condition involved sitting in silence while staring at a computer monitor for 10 minutes, which was unlikely to put participants in an optimal state of mind for any sort of task. A separate group of participants was tested identically but the Mozart music was replaced with a piece composed by Schubert from 15 Belison National School Belison, Antique the same CD, performed by the same pianists with the same production values. For these participants, a Schubert effect (i.e., better PF&C performance after listening to Schubert than sitting in silence) was evident and similar in magnitude to the Mozart effect. Again, adverse aspects of the control condition virtually guaranteed the outcome. For a third group of participants, Nantais and Schellenberg (1999) used the same experimental design but they compared PF&C performance after listening to Mozart or a narrated story written by Stephen King. The story was chosen because it was an auditory stimulus that changed over time (like the Mozart piece) and it was likely to be as enjoyable as listening to Mozart for a sample of college freshmen. Accordingly, the authors predicted that both Mozart and King should enhance cognitive performance equally, and this prediction was confirmed. After completing the experiment, participants were asked which listening experience they preferred. Approximately half preferred the music; the other half preferred the story. When the data were reanalyzed as a function of preference and condition, performance was better in the preferred than in the nonpreferred conditions. In other words, there was a Mozart effect for participants who preferred the music, but a Stephen King effect for participants who preferred the story. Thompson et al. (2001) used the same basic design. Each participant completed a version of the PF&C task after listening to music and sitting in silence. The music was the same Mozart piece used earlier (K. 448) for half of the participants, and Albinoni’s Adagio for the other half. The Adagio is a classic example of sadsounding music, written in a minor key with a slow tempo. Measures of arousal and mood were administered before and after the listening experiences. The hypotheses were that (1) there would be no Albinoni effect because the piece sounds so somber, (2) a Mozart effect would once again be evident (the first movement is happysounding with a fast tempo and in a major key), and (3) the advantage for Mozart over silence would be eliminated after controlling for changes in arousal or mood. The results were consistent with these predictions. Because the Albinoni piece is relatively well known and considered to be a quintessential piece of sad-sounding music, however, it could have evoked particular associations with sad events, such that these associations rather than the music were the source of the null effect. 16 Belison National School Belison, Antique Accordingly, Husain et al. (2002) conducted an additional experiment in which they manipulated the tempo and mode of the same piece (Mozart K. 448) using MIDI. As noted, happy-sounding music tends to be fast-tempo and in a major key, whereas sad-sounding music tends to be slow and minor (Hunter & Schellenberg, 2010). Each participant completed the PF&C task after listening to one version of the Mozart piece: fast/major, slow/major, fast/minor, or slow/minor. Arousal and mood were measured before and after listening. PF&C performance was better after the fast and major-key versions. Arousal levels were improved after hearing the fast versions, whereas moods were improved after hearing the majorkey versions. As predicted, changes in arousal and mood accounted for the bulk of the variance in PF&C scores. If effects of music on cognition are determined by music’s emotional impact, then the type of music that is the most effective in this regard should depend critically on the particular sample of listeners. In the next study (Schellenberg & Hallam, 2005), more than 8,000 10- and 11-year-olds living in the United Kingdom were recruited from approximately 200 schools to participate in a study of the Mozart effect, which was coordinated by the British Broadcasting Corporation (BBC). At each school, students were assigned to one of three rooms at exactly the same time. In one room, they heard pop music on BBC Radio 1, including a song by the band Blur. In a second room, they heard a Mozart piece over BBC Radio 3. In the third room, they listened to Susan Hallam discuss the experiment on BBC Radio 5. After listening to the radio, they completed two tests of spatial abilities. According to the arousal and mood hypothesis, the pop songs should be the most likely to put the children in an optimal emotional state for the cognitive tests. On the simpler of the two spatial tests, no differences among groups emerged. On the more difficult test, however, performance was indeed best for the children who heard pop songs. In other words, a Blur effect was evident for 10- and 11-year-olds living in the United Kingdom. Taking this approach even further, Schellenberg et al. (2007) tested the creative abilities of 5-year-old Japanese children. Recall that the arousal and mood hypothesis does not give special status to music listening. Any experience that changes the participant’s emotional state 17 Belison National School Belison, Antique can influence cognitive performance. Moreover, the hypothesis extends beyond spatial abilities to cognitive performance construed broadly, including creativity, which is considered to be one aspect of cognition (e.g., Sternberg, 2009). In an initial (baseline) session, children were given a piece of paper and 18 crayons and asked to draw whatever they liked. They subsequently made another drawing after a musical experience: listening to Mozart (K. 448), Albinoni’s Adagio, or children’s playsongs, or singing children’s playsongs. The hypothesis was that creativity would be enhanced after singing or listening to the18playsongs. The dependent measures were drawing times and ratings made by adults about each child’s pair of drawings in terms of creativity, energy, and technical proficiency. In line with predictions, drawing times increased more from baseline in both playsong groups than in the Mozart and Albinoni groups. Ratings of creativity, energy, and technical proficiency also increased relative to baseline for children who heard or sang playsongs, but these ratings decreased for children who heard Mozart or Albinoni. In short, a playsong effect was evident for Japanese 5-year-olds. In another experiment that examined whether the Mozart effect extends beyond measures of spatial ability, college freshmen were tested on measures of processing speed or working memory after they heard Mozart or Albinoni (Schellenberg et al., 2007). Each student came to the lab twice, both times completing one test after one listening experience. The order of the tests and the musical pieces was counterbalanced. Arousal and mood were measured before and after the music listening. At the first testing session, music listening led to inconsistent changes in arousal and mood and there were no differences between conditions on either cognitive test. At the second session, the data were consistent with predictions. Arousal increased after listening to Mozart but decreased after listening to Albinoni; similarly, mood improved after listening to Mozart but declined after listening to Albinoni. Although performance on both cognitive tests was better after listening to Mozart, the comparison was significant only for the test of processing speed. When these data are considered jointly with those from the British children (Schellenberg & Hallam, 2005), it appears that effects of emotional state on cognition may indeed be greater on some tasks (e.g., more difficult tests, tests of visuospatial abilities or processing speed) than on others (e.g., easier tests, tests of working memory; see also Rauscher et al., 1995; Steele, Ball, & Runk, 1997). In any event, there is no compelling evidence of a special link between listening to Mozart (or to any Classical music) and visuospatial (or spatial18 Belison National School Belison, Antique temporal) abilities. Rather, listening to music is an effective way to improve one’s emotional state, and how one feels can influence cognitive performance quite generally. As an aside, very few published studies have examined mathematical performance after listening to music. Although there was improvement in one study from pretest to posttest on a measure of mathematical ability, such improvement was similar whether participants listened to Mozart, Bach, or ocean sounds in the interim (Bridgett & Cuevas, 2000). In another study (Jausovec & Habe, 2003 ˇ), performance on a mathematical task was slightly (but not significantly) worse after listening to Mozart than after sitting in silence.(Retrieved from www.researchgate.net, February 28, 2020) Background Music and Cognitive Abilities The question of whether background music is able to enhance cognitive task performance is of interest to scholars, educators, and stakeholders in business alike. Studies have shown that background music can have beneficial, detrimental or no effects on cognitive task performance. Extraversion—and its postulated underlying cause, cortical arousal—is regarded as an important factor influencing the outcome of such studies. According to Eysenck's theory of personality, extraverts' cortical arousal at rest is lower compared to that of introverts. Scholars have thus hypothesized that extraverts should benefit from background music in cognitive tasks, whereas introverts' performance should decline with music in the background. Reviewing studies that have considered extraversion as a mediator of the effect of background music on cognitive task performance, it is demonstrated that there is as much evidence in favor as there is against Eysenck's theory of personality. Further, revisiting Eysenck's concept of cortical arousal—which has traditionally been assessed by activity in the EEG alpha band—and reviewing literature on the link between extraversion and cortical arousal, it is revealed that there is conflicting evidence. Due to Eysenck's focus on alpha power, scholars have largely neglected higher frequency bands in the EEG signal as indicators of cortical arousal. Based on recent findings, it is suggested that beta power might not only be an indicator of alertness and attention but also a predictor of cognitive task performance. In conclusion, it is proposed that focused music listening prior to cognitive tasks might be a more efficient way to boost performance than listening to background music during cognitive tasks. 19 Belison National School Belison, Antique There has been a surge in commercial applications promising to improve their users' concentration and focus by playing specifically designed music in the background. The basic idea is simple: playing background music activates your brain and leads to better performance in cognitive tasks. However, this idea comes with several issues. For example, even if music is specifically designed to set free cognitive resources it is implausible that every person benefits in the same manner in a cognitive task. A piece of music that has beneficial effects on cognitive task performance for one individual may well have no effect, or even detrimental effects, for another. Is searching for background music that enhances cognitive task performance therefore chasing a red herring? Not necessarily, if—as a first step—we are able to identify and better understand the neural underpinnings that enhance cognitive task performance in general. As a second step, we may begin to ask what are the characteristics of the music needed to change an individual's neural activation in a specific way. Since inter-individual differences play an important role in this endeavor, the evidence in favor of, and against, Eysenck's theory of personality will be reviewed before a new perspective is provided. Although the effects of background music on cognitive task performance have been studied by psychologists and educators for more than seventy years (Fendrick, 1937), no clear pattern of results has emerged thus far. On the one hand, background music, in comparison with silence, has been found to be beneficial for reading comprehension (Kiger, 1989), foreign vocabulary learning (de Groot, 2006; Kang and Williamson, 2014), spatial and linguistic processing (Angel et al., 2010), IQ tests (Cockerton et al., 1997), spatial and numerical reasoning (Miller and Schyb, 1989), visual search tasks (Crust et al., 2004), and students' achievement in a psychology class (Schreiber, 1988). On the other hand, background music, compared to silence, has been found to impair cognitive performance, showing detrimental effects for reading comprehension (Fendrick, 1937; Henderson et al., 1945; Etaugh and Ptasnik, 1982; Furnham and Bradley, 1997; Avila et al., 2012; Thompson et al., 2012), verbal memory (Iwanaga and Ito, 2002; Woo and Kanachi, 2005; Cassidy and MacDonald, 2007), visual memory (Furnham and Bradley, 1997), serial recall of digits (Nittono, 1997; Alley and Greene, 2008), Stroop tasks (Parente, 1976; Cassidy and MacDonald, 2007), writing fluency (Ransdell and Gilroy, 2001), and logical reasoning and associative learning (Crawford and Strapp, 1994). Yet other investigations revealed that background music had no significant impact on cognitive task 20 Belison National School Belison, Antique performance whatsoever (Henderson et al., 1945; Freeburne and Fleischer, 1952; Furnham and Allass, 1999; Pool et al., 2003; Alley and Greene, 2008; Schlittmeier and Hellbrück, 2009; Thompson et al., 2012). A recent meta-analysis on the effects of background music on adults' cognitive, affective and behavioral responses seems to support the trend toward an overall null effect (Kämpfe et al., 2011). Without theory-driven research focused on inter-individual differences, these contradictory findings are not surprising. To address this issue, many scholars have used Eysenck's theory of personality (Eysenck, 1967) as a theoretical framework for their studies. While there are several inter-individual differences that influence the effects of background music on cognitive task performance—ranging from personality traits to musical taste and age— one inter-individual difference that has been studied extensively is extraversion. According to a particular aspect of Eysenck's theory of personality, extraversion can be described and explained by the underlying cortical arousal. Extraverts are reported to have a lower level of cortical arousal compared to introverts. Eysenck's theory therefore predicts that introverts require little or no external stimulation to reach an optimal level of cognitive performance, whereas extraverts require comparatively more external stimulation. External stimulation exceeding the optimal threshold should lead to a decline in cognitive performance, following the Yerkes-Dodson Law (Yerkes and Dodson, 1908). Thus, presented with moderate to high levels of external stimulation should lead to a decline in introverts', but not extraverts', cognitive performance. Using background music as a source of external stimulation—which has been shown to increase arousal in participants in several studies (Thompson et al., 2001; Jones et al., 2006; Schellenberg et al., 2007)—researchers have empirically tested Eysenck's theory by investigating intro- and extraverts' performance in different www.researchgate.net, February 28, 2020) 21 cognitive tasks.(Retrieved from Belison National School Belison, Antique The Relationship Between Music Training and Cognitive Performance Associations between music training and general cognitive ability extend beyond intelligence testing to grades in school. For example, Wetter, Koerner, and Schwaninger (2009) examined the academic records of third- to sixth-grade Swiss children who either did or did not take music lessons outside of school. Children with lessons had higher average grades even when SES was held constant, and the advantage was evident across all school subjects except for sports. Similarly general associations are evident in samples of students from the US. Fitzpatrick (2006) examined performance on standardized tests of academic proficiency for more than 15,000 American high-school students, more than 900 of whom were registered in an instrumental music course. The music and control groups were further subdivided into low- and normal-SES groups. Fitzpatrick looked at performance in fourth, sixth, and ninth grades, before the students opted to take a music course in high school. With differences in SES held constant, the future instrumentalists outperformed the control group in every subject at each grade level. These results confirm that high-functioning children are more likely than other children to take music lessons, at least in high school. Other evidence implies that (1) positive associations between taking music classes in school and performance on standardized tests are more likely when the instruction is of particularly high quality (Johnson & Memmott, 2006), and (2) replacing some standard academic classes with instrumental music lessons does not have a negative impact on average grades in elementary school (Kvet, 1985). Gouzouasis, Guhn, and Kishor (2007) examined performance on standardized tests of academic achievement among 150,000 Canadian 12th-grade students. The tests provided separate scores for mathematics, English, and biology. Compared with other students, those who took music classes in 11th grade had higher scores in mathematics and biology but not in English. Among the music students, grades in 11th-year music courses were correlated positively with 12th-year standardized cores for mathematics and biology, and weaker but still evident for English. By contrast, participation in 11th-grade visual arts courses was not associated with standardized test scores in 12th grade. These results confirm that high-functioning students are more likely than other students to take high-school courses in music but not in visual arts, that taking music courses in high school does not interfere with achievement in core academic subjects, and that students who do 22 Belison National School Belison, Antique well in music classes tend to do well in other subjects. Similar findings in Schellenberg’s (2006b) study revealed that among elementary-school children, as duration of training increased, so did academic performance whether it was measured with actual grades on report cards or performance on a standardized test, and even when SES and duration of nonmusical activities were held constant. Even more provocative was the finding that school performance was associated positively with duration of training when IQ was controlled. In other words, children who took music lessons for years on end tended to be particularly good students. Duration of playing music regularly in childhood also predicted high-school average when SES was controlled. Finally, there is experimental evidence that 1 year of music training causes small improvements in academic achievement (Schellenberg, 2004). The association between duration of music training and general cognitive abilities suggests that professional musicians should be geniuses. When highly trained individuals or professional musicians are compared with similarly professional individuals without music training, however, the association often disappears. In other words, although professional musicians may be above average in intelligence, people who are highly trained in other disciplines perform at similar levels. For example, when members of a symphony orchestra or students from university music departments were compared with students from other disciplines (e.g., psychology, business) with a similar amount of education, the IQ advantage for the music students vanished (Franklin et al., 2008; Helmbold et al., 2005) or favored the students from the nonmusic disciplines (Brandler & Rammsayer, 2003). Similarly, when the comparison involved highly trained versus untrained participants, differences in intelligence fell short of statistical significance (Sluming et al., 2002) or disappeared altogether (Bialystok & DePape, 2009; Patston & Tippett, 2011; Schellenberg & Moreno, 2010). Thus, it appears that music training is associated positively with intelligence when training is added as an activity on top of regular schooling. One problem with this interpretation is that many of the null findings involved tests of fluid intelligence (Bialystok & DePape, 2009; Brandler & Rammsayer, 2003; Franklin et al., 2008; Helmbold et al., 2005; Patston, Hogg, et al., 2007; Patston & Tippett, 2011; Schellenberg & Moreno, 2010)—such as the Cattell Culture Fair Test, Raven’s Progressive Matrices, or the Matrices subtest from the Wechsler tests—rather than more comprehensive measures that include subtests of learned abilities (e.g., vocabulary). Although the null findings might therefore be interpreted as showing that associations between 23 Belison National School Belison, Antique music training and cognitive abilities do not extend to pure measures of fluid intelligence, other researchers have reported such an association (Dege´, Kubicek, & Schwarzer, 2011; Forgeard, Winner, et al., 2008; Hille et al., 2011; Portowitz et al., 2009; Thompson et al.,242004; Trimmer & Cuddy, 2008). In short, the distinction between music training in childhood and whether one is a “real musician” appears to be an important one, such that associations with general cognitive ability are much more likely in the former case.(Retrieved from www.researchgate.net, February 28, 2020) Theoretical Framework The theoretical framework of this study discusses the effects of listening to music to the cognitive performance of STEM students. There is a wide range of evidences concerning associations between music and cognitive abilities. These are the Music Aptitude, Music Training, Listening to Music and Background Music. Under the Music Aptitude are the concepts of modularity and multiple intelligences theory. While under the Music Training, Listening to Music and Background Music are the Music Learning Theory, Mozart effect and Eysenck’s Theory, respectively. The effects of listening to music to the cognitive performance of STEM students of Belison National School can be classified into the four evidences they are under. They can identify their aspects of cognition all related to music with this evidences. 24 Belison National School Belison, Antique Figure 1. Theoretical Framework Music Participating in Music Classes Music Aptitude Concepts of Modularity Melomaniac Music Training Multiple Intelligences Theory Listening to Music Music Training Theory Mozart Effect Cognitive Performance of STEM Students 25 Background Music Eysenck’s Theory Belison National School Belison, Antique Conceptual Framework The conceptual framework of this study discusses the factors involved in the effects of listening to music to the cognitive performance of STEM students of Belison National School. In knowing the effects of listening to music, four factors are most notably interconnected with each other that lead to enhance performance of students cognitive. These are the Environment, Learner’s Working Memory Capacity, Personality of the Student and Exposure Effects. The first, Environment, open environment sometimes is disturbed by talkers of traffics. Therefore, set of headphones may come in handy if you are looking to avoid being distracted by any ambient sounds. Learner’s Working Memory Capacity has a crucial influence on learning with seductive details. The higher the learner’s working memory capacity, the better they learn with background music. Personality of the students is also a factor because students prefer styles of music are consistent with their personalities. For instance, people who have a need for creative and intellectual stimulation prefer unconventional and complex music styles. Exposure Effects, on the other hand, shape our musical preferences. Repeated exposures can be considered as a form of classical conditioning that can increase liking of stimuli through a process of conditioning. All this factors are important, and with each individual, they vary very significantly. 26 Belison National School Belison, Antique Figure 2. Conceptual Framework Listening to Music Factors Environment Personality of the Students Exposure Effects Learner’s Working Memory Capacity Cognitive Performance of STEM Students 27 Belison National School Belison, Antique CHAPTER III Method Chapter 3 includes four parts: Purpose of the Study and Research Design, Respondents, Instrument, and Data Collection. Part One, Purpose of the Study and Research Design, presents the reasons on why how the study was conducted. Part Two, Respondents, presents the participants in the study and how they were chosen. Part Three, Instruments, describes the number of items and structure of questions. Part Four, Data Collection, presents the procedures or steps in gathering the data needed in this study. Purpose of the Study This study aims to Assess the Effects of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of Science, Technology, Engineering, Mathematics (STEM) Students. Specifically, this sought to answer the following questions: 1. What are the Effects of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of Students? 2. What is/are the students perceptions on the Effects of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of Students? The descriptive analysis of this research was used in this study because the researchers gave researcher-made questionnaires among the STEM students to answer for the study. 28 Belison National School Belison, Antique Respondents Respondents of this study were the STEM students of Belison National School. Table 1 Entire Group Category F % A. Entire Group 30 100 Males 15 50 Females 15 50 B. Gender Instrument One set of questionnaire was used in gathering the data. These data were then used in Assessing the Effects of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of STEM students of Belison National School. Data Collection The survey instrument used in this study was designed to Assess the Effects of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of STEM students of Belison National School. A personal data sheet requested demographic data in addition to the responses to the survey questions. The survey instruments were distributed to 30 STEM students. 29 Belison National School Belison, Antique Chapter IV Result and Discussion This chapter deals with the results and discussion of the gathered data through the distributed questionnaire. The aim of this research is to Assess the Effectiveness of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of STEM students of Belison National School (BNS). Table 1 shows the number and percentage of the 30 male and female respondents of the G12 STEM class. Table 1 Demographic Profile Entire Group Respondents 30 Male Female n % n % 15 50 15 50 As shown in table 1, out of 30 respondent both male and female has the same number and percentage. 30 Belison National School Belison, Antique Table 2 shows the result of the answer to Question No. 1. Table 2 Music Listening Question Always Often Sometimes Rarely Never How often do you listen n % n % n % n % n % to music? 17 56.67 8 26.67 5 16.67 0 0 0 0 As shown in Table 2, 17 or 56.67% of all the respondents answered always when asked on how often they listen to music, 8 or 26.67% answered often and 5 or 16.67% answered sometimes. There are no answers for the rest of responses. This means that majority of the respondents always listen to music. Table 3 shows the result of the answer to Question No. 2. Table 3 Studying with Music Question Very Frequently Occasionally Rarely Never Frequently Do you listen to music n % n % n % n % n % when you study? 4 13.33 13 43.33 6 20 6 20 1 3.33 As shown in Table 3, 4 out of 30 or 13.33% of the respondents answered very frequently when asked if they study while listening to music, 13 or 43.33% answered frequently, 6 or 20% answered occasionally and rarely and 1 or 3.33% answered very rarely which means that STEM students frequently listen to music while studying. 31 Belison National School Belison, Antique Table 4 shows the result of the answer to Question No. 3. Table 4 Memory Formation Question Do you agree that music SA A U D SD n % n % n % n % n % 12 40 17 56.67 0 0 1 3.33 0 0 creates a positive mood which indirectly boost memory formation? As shown in Table 4, 12 or 40% of the respondents strongly agrees that music creates a positive mood which indirectly boost memory formation, 17 or 56.67% agrees, 1 or 3.33% disagrees and none is undecided or strongly disagrees. This means that most of the respondents agrees with the implication of the question. Table 5 shows the result of the answer to Question No. 4. Table 5 Music and Concentration Question Does listening to music improve concentration? SA A U D SD n % n % n % n % n % 4 13.33 12 40 11 36.67 3 10 0 0 As shown in Table 5, 4 or 13.33% of the respondents strongly agrees that music improves concentration. Those who agrees are 12 or 40% , those who are undecided are 11 or 36.67% and those who disagrees are 3 or 10%, and there is none who strongly disagrees. 32 Belison National School Belison, Antique Table 6 shows the result of the answer to Question No. 5. Table 6 Cognitive Performance Question Does listening to music improve your cognitive performance? SA A U D SD n % n % n % n % n % 5 16.67 18 60 3 10 4 13.33 0 0 As shown in Table 6, 5 or 16.67% of the respondents strongly agrees that listening to music improves your cognitive performance, 18 or 60% agrees, those who are undecided are 3 or 10%, 4 or 13.33% disagrees and none of them strongly disagrees. Table 7 shows the result of the answer to Question No. 6. Table 7 Beating Stress and Anxiety Question Can music satisfy your inner mind to beat stress and anxiety? SA A U D SD n % n % n % n % n % 12 40 17 56.67 1 3.33 0 0 0 0 As shown in Table 7, 12 or 40% of the respondents strongly agrees that music can satisfy inner mind to beat stress and anxiety, those who agrees are 17 or 56.67%, only 1 or 3.33% is undecided and there is none who disagrees or strongly disagrees. Which means that most of the respondents agrees to the implication of the question. 33 Belison National School Belison, Antique Table 8 shows the result of the answer to Question No. 7. Table 8 Effects of Music Question Do you agree that the SA A U D SD n % n % n % n % n % 7 23.33 18 60 3 10 2 6.67 0 0 effects of listening to music to the cognitive performance of STEM students vary significantly with each individual? As shown in Table 8, 7 or 23.33% of the respondents strongly agrees that the effects of listening to music to the cognitive performance of STEM students vary significantly with each individual, 18 or 60% agrees, those who are undecided are 3 or 10%, only 2 or 6.67% disagrees and none of the respondents strongly disagrees. 34 Belison National School Belison, Antique Table 9 shows the result of the answer to Question No. 8. Table 9 Mode of Studying Question Which do you prefer more? Studying with Studying music without music Undecided n % n % n % 18 60 7 23.33 5 16.67 As shown in Table 9, 18 or 60% of the respondents prefer studying with music, those who prefer studying without music are 7 or 23.33% and those who are undecided are 5 or 16.67%. Which means that majority of STEM students prefer studying with music. Table 10 shows the result of the answer to Question No. 9. Table 10 Studying with Music Question Soothing and Loud and relaxing music rhythmic Undecided music If you like studying with music, which do n % n % n % you prefer to listen? 18 60 0 0 12 40 As shown in Table 10, 18 or 60% of the respondents prefer soothing and relaxing music, none of the respondents prefer loud and rhythmic music and those who are undecided are 12 or 40%. Which means that majority of STEM students prefer soothing music. 35 Belison National School Belison, Antique Table 11 shows the result of the answer to Question No. 10. Table 11 Music Genre Question Classical K-pop Country Rap Pop What genre of music n % n % n % n % n % do you listen the most? 3 10 4 13.33 0 0 0 0 11 36.67 Rock EDM Techno Hip-hop R&B Jazz n % n % n % n % n % n % 0 0 0 0 2 6.67 3 10 6 20 1 3.33 As shown in Table 11, 3 or 10% of the respondents listen to classical music the most, 4 or 13.33% listen to Kpop music the most, 11 or 36.67% listen to pop music the most, 2 or 6.67% listen to techno music the most, 3 or 10% listen to hiphop music the most, 6 or 20% listen to R&B music the most and only 1 or 3.33% listen to jazz music the most. There are no answers for the rest of the other genres with implication to the question. 36 Belison National School Belison, Antique Chapter V Summary, Conclusions, and Recommendations This chapter presents the summary of the research work undertaken, the conclusions drawn, and the recommendation made as an outgrowth of this study entitled, Assessment on the Effects of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics (STEM) Students of Belison National School. Summary This study utilized the descriptive research design through survey with the use of questionnaires. The respondents of the study was the G11 and G12 STEM students of Belison National School of the school year 2020-2021. The researchers used questionnaire method to analyze the results of the study. More specifically, the researchers sought to answer the following: 1. What are the Effects of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of STEM students? 2. What is/are the student's perception to the Effects of Listening to Music to the Cognitive Performance of STEM students? 37 Belison National School Belison, Antique Findings The findings of the study were the following: 1. In Table 2, 17 or 56.66% of all the respondents answered always when asked on how often they listen to music, 8 or 26.66% answered often and 5 or 16.66% answered sometimes. There are no answers for the rest of responses. This means that majority of the respondents always listen to music. 2. In Table 3, 4 out of 30 or 13.33% of the respondents answered very frequently when asked if they study while listening to music, 13 or 43.33% answered frequently, 6 or 20% answered occasionally and rarely and 1 or 3.33% answered very rarely which means that STEM students frequently listen to music while studying. 3. In Table 4, 12 or 40% of the respondents strongly agrees that music creates a positive mood which indirectly boost memory formation, 17 or 56.66% agrees, 1 or 3.33% disagrees and none is undecided or strongly disagrees. This means that most of the respondents agrees with the implication of the question. 4. In Table 5, 4 or 13.33% of the respondents strongly agrees that music improves concentration. Those who agrees are 12 or 40% , those who are undecided are 11 or 36.66% and those who disagrees are 3 or 10%, and there is none who strongly disagrees. 5. In Table 6, 5 or 16.66% of the respondents strongly agrees that listening to music improves your cognitive performance, 18 or 60% agrees, those who are undecided are 3 or 10%, 4 or 13.33% disagrees and none of them strongly disagrees. 6. In Table 7, 12 or 40% of the respondents strongly agrees that music can satisfy inner mind to beat stress and anxiety, those who agrees are 17 or 56.66%, only 1 or 3.33% is undecided and there is none who disagrees or strongly disagrees. Which means that most of the respondents agrees to the implication of the question. 7. In Table 8, 7 or 23.33% of the respondents strongly agrees that the effects of listening to music to the cognitive performance of STEM students vary significantly with each individual, 18 or 60% agrees, those who are undecided are 3 or 10%, only 2 or 6.66% disagrees and none of the respondents strongly disagrees. 38 Belison National School Belison, Antique 8. In Table 9, 18 or 60% of the respondents prefer studying with music, those who prefer studying without music are 7 or 23.33% and those who are undecided are 5 or 16.66%. Which means that majority of STEM students prefer studying with music. 9. In Table 10, 18 or 60% of the respondents prefer soothing and relaxing music, none of the respondents prefer loud and rhythmic music and those who are undecided are 12 or 40%. Which means that majority of STEM students prefer soothing music. 10. In Table 11, 3 or 10% of the respondents listen to classical music the most, 4 or 13.33% listen to Kpop music the most, 11 or 36.66% listen to pop music the most, 2 or 6.66% listen to techno music the most, 3 or 10% listen to hiphop music the most, 6 or 20% listen to R&B music the most and only 1 or 3.33% listen to jazz music the most. There are no answers for the rest of the other genres with implication to the question. Conclusion Music is a very significant part of our daily lives: the image of the quietly-focused student isolating themselves into a personal study zone has led interest into whether listening to music actually improves cognitive performance or not. In the STEM students 4 out of 30 or 13.33% of the respondents answered very frequently when asked if they study while listening to music, 13 or 43.33% answered frequently, 6 or 20% answered occasionally and rarely and 1 or 3.33% answered very frequently. Through this study, there is assessment in the effects of listening to music to the cognitive performance of STEM students of Belison National School. In the end, these preferences will serve as guides to students, and it is up to them to decide whether to listen to music while studying or not. 39 Belison National School Belison, Antique Recommendation 1. The students should recognize the effects of listening to music in relation to their cognitive performance. 2. The teachers should be able to understand the effects of listening to music to the cognitive performance of students and encourage them to listen to different genres of music. 3. The administrators should support the school in implementing some of the activities where students can widen their knowledge through listening to music. 4. Parents should be aware of the importance of listening to music in the learning of their children. 5. For future researchers, the results of this study can be a source of reference should they decide to conduct their research of the same topic. 40 Belison National School Belison, Antique ACKNOWLEDGEMENT The researchers would like to extend their profound and genuine gratitude to the following persons who have shared their knowledge, assistance, time, resources for the completion of the study. To the researcher’s parents. To Mr. Albert Morillo, for being an understanding adviser for the subject Research Report and for helping in this study and sharing his time, intelligent advises, suggestions, recommendations, and keeping us on the right track until the study is finished. And above all, to God Almighty, who gave the wisdom, strength and perseverance to complete this study. The Researchers 41 Belison National School Belison, Antique REFERENCES www.independent.co.uk www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov www.frontiersin.org www.google.com/amp/syncproject.co/blog https://www.britanica.com http://www.sciencedirect.com www.researchgate.net 42