Received: 14 August 2021 | Revised: 11 November 2021 | Accepted: 1 December 2021 DOI: 10.1002/jcop.22776 RESEARCH ARTICLE What makes us complete: Hybrid multicultural identity and its social contextual origins Andrea Belgrade | Mari Kira | Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, USA Correspondence Andrea Belgrade, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA. Email: belgrade@umich.edu Shima Sadaghiyani | Fiona Lee Abstract Our research explores the experience of holding a Hybrid Multicultural Identity (a superordinate cultural identity; HMI) and the social contextual experiences hybrid multiculturals describe as influential to the development of an HMI. We conducted a Photovoice study with 10 hybrid multiculturals (age 18–32; 6 women and 4 men) living in a college town in the Midwestern US. The participants valued HMI for the psychological advantages they attributed to this identity. We also found the participants described three broad categories of their social environment that were key to the development of HMI: cultural composition in living environments, perceptions of macro‐level marginalization, and culturally related interpersonal experiences. Our research documents (1) the lived experience of being a hybrid multicultural (2) the importance of cultural mixing for HMI development, and (3) how people with HMI describe primarily negative perceptions of the social environment as instrumental to the development of HMI. KEYWORDS hybrid multicultural identity, identity development, photovoice, qualitative, social contexts 1 | INTRODUCTION [Being multicultural] also gives you lots of access to other things, you know: different communities, different places in the world. That just comes about naturally. You're not even making an effort to go out of your way to go to those places. It's just who you are and what you have access to, and that's just part of what being multicultural gives you. (Yoshito) 2290 | © 2021 Wiley Periodicals LLC wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jcop J Community Psychol. 2022;50:2290–2313. BELGRADE ET AL | 2291 In today's highly interconnected world, people often belong to two or more cultural groups, such as countries of residence, ethnicities, or nationalities. As a result, an increasing number of people define themselves as multicultural (Arasaratnam, 2013; Pekerti & Thomas, 2016; Vora et al., 2018). Leading scholars in the field of multicultural identity describe a superordinate cultural identity that we call “hybrid multicultural identity” (HMI). Individuals with HMIs have two or more separate cultural identities, and they transform those cultural identities into an emergent superordinate hybrid multicultural identity, identifying with other people who are, in various ways, multicultural (West et al., 2017; Yampolsky, et al., 2016). HMI, therefore, captures a multicultural identity that goes beyond the sum of the separate cultural identities and connects the multicultural person to other multicultural individuals across the world. While earlier literature discussed such superordinate cultural identities, further research is needed on how superordinate cultural identities develop in social contexts (Yampolsky et al., 2016). Our aim is to offer empirically grounded insights into the social contextual factors that foster the development of HMI. In doing so, we will be contributing to the understanding of social factors that allow people with different multicultural heritages to experience oneness with each other. Using Photovoice methodology, we explore the contextual origins of HMI, and also examine HMI's meaning, importance, and affective valence to its holders (Ashforth et al., 2008). 1.1 | Multiculturalism Scholars define multiculturalism in many ways (Arasaratnam, 2013; Verkuyten, 2006). We classify this literature into three main groups. First, multiculturalism may describe a specific characteristic of the society such as its poly‐ethnic composition (Verkuyten, 2006). A crucial question here is how people “fit in” in such a society (acculturation; Nguyen & Benet‐Martinez, 2010). Relatedly, postcolonial studies question the very existence of purely monocultural environments, recognizing how cultures have always come together and mixed, creating multicultural spaces (Ackermann, 2012). These bodies of literature also debate how a multicultural society may or may not deteriorate social cohesion (Verkuyten, 2006) and challenge the prevailing dominant power structures (Ackermann, 2012). Second, multiculturalism can refer to individual and shared attitudes towards diversity of cultures (e.g., governmental policies; Nguyen & Benet‐Martinez, 2010; Rosenthal & Levy, 2010). Some countries have adopted multiculturalism as official policy (e.g., The Canadian Multiculturalism Act of 1988), aiming to provide equity for minority groups through the support of multilingual resources, progressive citizenship legislation, and other inclusive practices (Nguyen & Benet‐ Martinez, 2010). Third, multiculturalism can reference individual characteristics – multicultural personality, skills, abilities, cognitions, and identity (Arasaratnam, 2013; Pekerti & Thomas, 2016; Vora et al., 2018). This third category, particularly identity, is the focus of the current study as we explore the development of a hybrid multicultural identity. 1.2 | Hybrid multicultural identity (HMI) The Transformative Theory of Biculturalism (West et al., 2017) suggests that a new type of cultural identity emerges when people simultaneously identify with two or more cultures and transform them into a multicultural superordinate identity, rather than choose between these cultures or seek to integrate them as aspects of their self‐ concept. While someone may still identify with their individual cultural groups, they may also identify more broadly as multicultural. As Yampolsky et al. (2016, p. 168) write: “one may invoke a higher‐order, inclusive identity that encompasses the different cultural identities (i.e., a superordinate identity) as a means for reconciling and uniting them.” 2292 | BELGRADE ET AL Using the analogy of baking a cake, West et al. (2017, p. 967) describe how one can mix and bake ingredients in particular ways to create an outcome that is unrecognizable from its original components. Just like the chemical reactions that occur during the baking process, particular ways of combining cultural identities and experiences can lead to unique identity outcomes (HMI) that require the original components but do not necessarily share the same characteristics as these components. Extending West et al.'s (2017) cake baking metaphor, vastly different sets of ingredients can create cakes. As their ingredients differ, these cakes may have different flavors or textures, but still, they can be identified within a shared “cake” category. Our data demonstrate how this metaphor applies to HMI; while people may hold vastly different cultural experiences and identities, these identities can still lead to the creation of a shared HMI (see also Banks, 2014; Moore & Barker, 2012; Neufeld & Schmitt, 2018). In contrast to diasporic identities that connect the globally dispersed communities formed by people sharing the same national and ancestral origins (Ang, 2003), HMI connects people with different national and ancestral origins but sharing the superordinate identity as a multicultural person. It is important to note that the idea of hybridity has a long history in various fields such as biology, anthropology, sociology, and history (Ackermann, 2012). Most importantly to our study, postcolonial literature approaches hybridity as cultural transformation resulting from the meeting and mixing of cultures. Our study and West et al.’ (2017) recent work on hybridization as an identity negotiation strategy recognizes and explores this process on the level of individual identities. 1.3 | HMI and the importance of social contexts Social contexts are considered a crucial element of identity development (Amiot et al., 2007), but when it comes to the social contexts fostering an HMI, empirical literature mainly focused on the role of early immersive culture mixing (Hong & Khei, 2014; Martin & Shao, 2016; Martin et al., 2019). Through early immersive culture mixing, young people grow up interacting in a significant way with multiple cultures in their home environment. This makes them “innate” multiculturals and increases their likelihood to transform various cultural identities into an HMI, a single cultural schema (Martin & Shao, 2016). This is contrasted to people who became multicultural through exposures to other cultures later in life (so called “achieved” multiculturals); these people tend not to form this superordinate identity and instead switch between multiple cultural frames and identities (Martin & Shao, 2016; Martin et al., 2019). West et al. (2017) suggest, in their turn, that cultural experiences accumulated over a lifetime can also contribute to an HMI. In addition to early immersive culture mixing, positively experienced social contextual factors (i.e., diverse social networks and social support) may foster the transformation of individual cultural identities into a superordinate identity and therefore, could potentially lead to the development of an HMI (Amiot et al., 2007; West et al., 2017). Such contextual experiences would make multiculturals feel more accepted and, consequently, embrace an HMI (Yampolsky et al., 2016). Bhabha's (1994) notion of “third spaces” is relevant here. In third spaces, elements of different cultural systems come together and mix, thus forming new cultural elements, including novel identities. As Chulach and Gagnon (2016, p. 54) write: “Those situated within this ‘in‐between (third) space’ become neither this nor that, but often combine aspects of both cultures to create something unique. … This space… allows emergence of re‐defined or transformed identities.” Therefore, life experiences in spaces where cultures meet and mix both provide ingredients and encouragement for identity exploration and the formation of novel identities, such as HMIs (Ang, 2003; Chulach & Gagnon, 2016). By definition, multicultural individuals belong to many social groups that can be both dominant and subordinate. Negative interpersonal experiences originating from a multicultural individual's dominant group, such as discrimination, may stifle identity hybridization (Yampolsky et al., 2016). When a dominant group discriminates against multicultural individuals’ subordinate identities, multiculturals may react by disengaging from and rejecting that dominant group and its identity and, instead, engage solely with their subordinate cultural ingroups targeted by BELGRADE ET AL | 2293 discrimination (Saleem et al., 2019). However, if these multicultural individuals experience both discrimination from the dominant group and marginalization from their subordinate identity group, they might disengage from each identity group (Durkee & Williams, 2015). West et al. (2017) propose that when a person does not identify strongly with any of their cultural identities, they are more likely to blend these influences to form a single cultural schema that is informed by each cultural influence. Therefore, both validation and rejection may enforce HMI development, but empirically grounded studies are still lacking in this area. Our aim is to offer further light on the positive and negative social contextual factors fostering the formation of HMI. Macro‐level social contextual factors influencing the development of HMI have only been briefly theorized. When it comes to macro‐level factors stifling HMIs, conflicts between one's various cultural identities, playing out in the larger society, may make it more difficult to reconcile the differences between these identities and create a hybrid identity (West et al., 2017). In contrast, living within an individualistic society may favor the development of an HMI, given the emphasis on self‐consistency across contexts and individual creative expression and self‐ affirmation (West et al., 2017). Further, West and colleagues (2017) describe how a multicultural environment centering each cultural component more equally in the mainstream society may elicit hybrid identities. Similarly, Ang (2003) discusses how globalization has resulted in unclear lines between cultural ingroup and outgroup categories; people occupy cultural spaces that do not neatly fall into such categories. In these spaces of complicated entanglements of differences, hybrid identities – that is, identities in between cultural “us” and “they” categories – emerge (Ang, 2003, p. 153). The current study will offer some empirical illustration on the macro‐level social contextual factors that influence HMI development. 1.4 | The current study Although earlier literature recognized the superordinate cultural identity (HMI) emerging as people transform their various cultural identities, further empirical research is needed describing the experience of having an HMI and the social contextual factors fostering the development of an HMI (Yampolsky et al., 2016). Our research questions are: (1) How do people experience being a hybrid multicultural? (2) What are the social contextual factors that participants attribute to the development of their HMI? We examine these questions using open‐ended, exploratory, and qualitative methods. It should be noted that this paper's authors have varying degrees and types of multicultural experiences, and we recognize that our experiences impact how we approach this study. 2 | M E TH O D S Due to the exploratory nature of our research questions, we took an interpretive, qualitative approach (Ponterotto, 2005). In particular, we used Photovoice, a form of participatory action research which prioritizes participants’ personal narratives, thus empowering them and inviting them to take part in the creation of knowledge (Bananuka & John, 2014; Chmielewski & Yost, 2012; Freedman et al., 2012; Johansen & Le, 2014). Photovoice has several key features. First, participants take photographs around a particular theme and, in some cases, they create captions for their photographs (cf. Strack et al., 2018). Second, participants bring their photographs to group sessions where they discuss the photographs. The participants’ photographs, rather than a predetermined set of researchers’ questions, provide the stimuli for discussion. Third, the group discussions are transcribed and analyzed to provide the main source of data (Freedman et al., 2012; Valera et al., 2009). 2294 2.1 | BELGRADE ET AL | Participants The study took place in a large, public university in the Midwestern US. We recruited individuals who self‐identified as “multicultural” by posting fliers and advertising the study in student organizations that catered to students from different cultural backgrounds. We did not define the concept “multicultural,” but left it open for interpretation to include participants with various insights on this construct (cf. West et al., 2017). We asked all potential participants to fill out a pre‐screening form asking what made them identify as multicultural, eligibility questions to ensure all participants were over 18, and logistical questions to be sure they could attend the session dates and had a camera to take photographs with (if a participant did not have one, we provided a disposable camera). Because of the significant time commitment and to reduce attrition, we offered $75 for participants’ participation (an estimated $15 per hour rate). We selected a sample of 10 participants, a typical sample size for Photovoice studies (cf., Cho et al., 2019; Hergenrather et al., 2009; Nitzinger et al., 2019). Table 1 includes cultural background details participants reported on a pre‐screening form. We also asked each participant to tell us their age and gender (included in Table 1) when interacting with them in a secure, private setting during the in‐person data collection. (To protect the anonymity of the participants, we refer to them with pseudonyms throughout the paper). Our sample included four men and six women representing a variety of cultural heritages and experiences. The participants included first‐ and second‐ generation immigrants, as well as sojourners to the United States. All were closely affiliated with the university— eight participants were students, one was a recent graduate who was working, and one was a spouse of a student. Our study was approved by the university's Health Sciences and Behavioral Sciences Institutional Review Board (HUM00126974). 2.2 | Procedure Given that most of the participants were full‐time students or workers, we designed this study to minimize the time required to participate. We conducted the study in two sessions over the course of two weeks (see Strack et al., 2018 for a similar design). The first session lasted approximately an hour and served to obtain informed consent, to introduce the participants to the basics of Photovoice methodology, and to explain their task to take photographs inspired by two questions: “How would you describe your multicultural identity?” and “How was your multicultural identity formed?”1 We also asked the participants to produce photograph captions to encourage their awareness of what their photographs actually mean to them (cf. Strack et al., 2018; Valera et al., 2009). The participants co‐created several ground rules to facilitate the respectful discussion of ideas and to create an environment where participants had the space and safety to self‐define as multicultural as they saw the best. Some of these rules were: agreeing to respond to ideas rather than people, being mindful about not dominating the discussion, and focusing on their personal experiences rather than making broad generalizations about specific social groups (Krueger & Casey, 2015). Participants came together two weeks later for a group discussion about their photographs. We first held two parallel, simultaneous subgroup discussions, with five participants in each subgroup, lasting one hour. Both subgroups were equal in gender representation. To ensure maximum cultural diversity, no single country or ethnicity was represented twice in one subgroup. Friends were divided into separate subgroups. Two members of the research team, each representing different cultural heritages, facilitated each subgroup. First, each participant took turns describing all of their photographs until everyone shared their photographs (as in, e.g., Valera et al., 2009). 1 While in psychological literature, there is an important distinction placed on identity formation versus identity development, we explained this question in a way that allowed for either answer, and we did not distinguish between the two with our participants. 32 22 18 23 18 19 20 25 19 ~20s Ling Zara Tesfu Germaine Nima Ian Yoshito Ashwini João Isabella Woman Man Woman Man Man Man Woman Woman Woman Woman Gender Puerto Rico Brazil United States France Brazil United States Malaysia United States Malaysia Taiwan Place of birth ~20s 6 Birth 6–11, 14 18 Birth 18 Birth 18 29 Age arrived in the United States Puerto Rican (Black, European, and Jibaro) Paraguayan, Ukrainian Indian (Tamil) Japanese Chinese, Taiwanese Persian, Muslim Chinese Eritrean Malay and Javanese Taiwanese, Chinese origins Self‐described heritage cultures Note: Isabella's audio recording was corrupted, and the age reported is from memory. Age Demographics of participants (N = 10) Participant pseudonym TABLE 1 Moved to the United States for graduate education Moved to the United States with family Lived in diverse communities in the United States, a primarily white community in high school, and a primarily African American community in Atlanta, visited family in India Japan, Ghana and United States residency with family Moved to United States for undergraduate education Self‐described American Multicultural Malaysian community, moved to the United States for undergraduate education Eritrean church, primarily white United States high school Moved to the United States for undergraduate education Japanese and Western influences, moved to the United States to accompany husband Other significant cultural experiences in life BELGRADE ET AL | 2295 2296 | BELGRADE ET AL Finally, the participants were invited to comment on each other's photographs and stories. Facilitators were trained to intervene minimally. After this, the subgroups came together for a full group (10 persons), 30‐minute session to share topics that emerged from the subgroup discussion. All of these sessions were audio‐recorded and transcribed. As typical in Photovoice studies (Nykiforuk et al., 2011; Wachs, 1999), the transcriptions of the discussions provide the main source of data. | Data analysis 2.3 Our aim was to capture both the meaning of HMI to the participants and the social contextual factors that they attributed to the development of their HMI. Following Braun and Clarke's (2006) model of thematic analysis, we used an iterative process of coding using Nvivo software and thematic mapping. The first round of coding was open‐ended and data‐driven. Two of the study's authors (each representing different cultural backgrounds) coded the data independently, and together created thematic maps that, in the second round of coding, helped organize the codes into a data structure of second‐order categories and aggregated theoretical dimensions (Caza et al., 2017; Gioia et al., 2013). The two coders regularly checked in with other members of the research team to discuss the codes, categories, and theoretical dimensions (Barbour, 2001; Richards & Hemphill, 2018). After these discussions, the coders made adjustments as needed and repeated the process of coding and restructuring their thematic maps. 3 | RESULTS Figure 1 outlines the data structure and its theoretical dimensions, second‐order categories, and first‐order codes (cf., Gioia et al., 2013). The subsequent figures present the participants’ photographs cited in the text, the captions they chose for those photographs, and transcript quotes relating to each photograph (Belon et al., 2016; Wang & Burris, 1997). Consistent with previous findings (West et al., 2017; Yampolsky et al., 2016), we found that all the participants described a shared, superordinate, emergent identity (i.e., HMI) that went beyond each of their cultural identities, valued for both its psychological and interpersonal benefits. We also found three broad categories of social contextual factors described by the participants as essential experiences that influenced the development of their identity: cultural composition in living environments, perceptions of macro‐level marginalization, and culturally related interpersonal experiences. 3.1 3.1.1 | Hybrid multicultural identity | Common multicultural identity The participants recognized a superordinate cultural identity that incorporated and built on their cultural influences. As Germaine described, “It's just a blend of different things… we need every different culture, that's what makes us complete, as a whole thing.” They talked about this common identity explicitly as something they all experienced. “We're talking about something that's common to all of us, the way we interpret [being multicultural] in our head, the way we perceive it as a concept.” (Yoshito). The participants also expressed this shared identity implicitly. For example, instead of using words like “I,” the participants used the word “we” to refer to this identity shared by BELGRADE ET AL | 2297 F I G U R E 1 First‐order codes and second‐order categories within their respective aggregated theoretical dimensions. The participants often referred to the non‐multicultural dominant or mainstream society members as the “Americans.” participants in the Photovoice group and its associated experiences. “Because we have so many different experiences with so many different people. So I think multicultural make people more open‐minded and more tolerant or inclusive towards others” (Ling). 3.1.2 | Benefits of HMI As Ling alluded to in the previous section, the participants evaluated their HMI positively. First, they perceived psychological benefits as their HMI enabled higher levels of flexible thinking. The participants felt they were not bound by their own experiences and could more easily shift their thinking to understand alternative perspectives. Zara said: “We can see things from multiple perspectives, so we are not boxed to just one perspective.” The participants also described interpersonal benefits associated with their HMI and described being more able to 2298 | BELGRADE ET AL empathize with others; Ashwini described herself as “an ambassador for empathy” during difficult intergroup interactions in class. One of my white‐American friends is getting frustrated with how different race, or, different ethnicity students in class have reacted to things in our group project…. Um, so being able to empathize with my classmates and share that reflection with my White‐American classmates. The participants also recounted having enhanced appreciation of differences across people. They described the “beauty” in diversity (see Figure 2 caption) and also the need to leave one's “bubble” and explore different cultures (see Figure 3 caption). 3.2 | Social contextual factors: Origins of HMI The second aim of this study is to explore the social contextual factors that the participants attributed to the development of their HMIs. The participants described three factors influencing their HMI development: the cultural composition of the environments in which they lived, their perceptions of macro‐level marginalization in the society, and their culturally related interpersonal experiences. Each of these three factors influenced each other, with the cultural composition in living environments not only directly influencing the development of HMI, but also shaping the participants’ perceptions of macro‐level marginalization in the society as well as their culturally related interpersonal experiences. A summary of these observed relationships is illustrated in Figure 4. 3.2.1 | Cultural composition in living environments The cultural composition in the participants’ communities (i.e., by the represented cultural groups in the community and the degree of its homogeneity or heterogeneity) was cited as a major factor in their stories of how they developed their identities. The participants also emphasized the degree to which they were exposed to diverse social contexts and experienced cultural mixing as important, identity shaping aspects of the living environment. F I G U R E 2 Beautiful. Black sand, or white sand. Both are equally beautiful. Beauty after all, is in the eye of beholder. Transcript quote: “Whether it is black sand or white sand both are equally beautiful because beauty is in the eye of the beholder. So there's no such thing as like, white is beautiful or black is beautiful or brown is beautiful. For me, everyone is beautiful. And beauty can be perceived by different people, in a different manner. Everything is just beautiful.” (Zara) BELGRADE | ET AL 2299 F I G U R E 3 Life in a bubble has shallow roots. Participant quote: “I love terrariums, but what I'm trying to bring out here is like, life in a bubble has shallow roots. […] I feel like it's really important for us to, you know, not stay in our own bubble. And know what other cultures has going on around the world. So… yeah that's what I've been doing for the past… my life.” (Germaine) FIGURE 4 A summary of the developmental relationships of each social contextual factor These factors directly related to the development of an HMI, and also influenced their culturally related interpersonal interactions and perceptions of macro‐level marginalization. First, the participants who grew up in the United States, experienced various cultural representations in their communities: from a primarily white American community to a homogenous cultural ingroup community, or a mix of 2300 | BELGRADE ET AL different cultures where there was no clear majority. All these representations contributed to the development of an HMI, each in its unique way. Heterogeneous environments tended to validate multicultural identities or even instill a multicultural identity among otherwise monocultural participants. In monocultural environments, the “innate” multicultural participants tended to feel isolated, encouraging them to identify with other (similarly isolated) multiculturals and, thus, form a superordinate HMI. Ashwini, when asked to describe the factors that led to the development of her HMI, described the stark differences in cultural representation throughout her life, where she experienced the heterogeneous college town as accepting, while the largely white town left her feeling isolated. So the elementary school I went to was in [a name of a city], a college town, so my classmates were from all over the world which was a really unique experience. So I vividly remember having teachers, like around the holidays, not just talking about Christmas, like we talked about different religions or cultural holidays, and I don't think I realized how unique that was until much later in life. Where it wasn't just like reading about it in civilizations textbooks, but very much like students were encouraged to share what they celebrated at home. Um, so for me that was cool, but then I moved to [a name of a state] in a town that was 94% white, so needless to say there wasn't that kind of conversation. So I think your point of some communities being very homogenous definitely like centers whiteness and centers the idea of whiteness um… I don't know how that can happen on an institutional level where like if you look around the room and there's like one person who doesn't look like the others, if that could really be a constructive conversation? Across the participants, cultural representation was often described as contextual information to explain the participants’ experiences with cultural misunderstandings, devaluation of multiculturalism, and also intragroup marginalization, factors they all cited as important for the development of their HMI. For example, Ashwini described how living in predominantly white spaces (see Figure 5 caption) and having friends who did not speak her ethnic group's language, made her feel marginalized in her own cultural group. This strengthened her sense as a hybrid multicultural, rather than as a representative of only her own ethnic group. My parents tried teaching me Tamil as a kid but I thought it was weird and none of my friends spoke it so why would I speak it? Um… but yeah, they really wanted me to learn and now I have the vocabulary of a two‐year old. It has definitely impacted my relationship with family in India. Whenever we go back I can't have a conversation with a lot of people. So everything they know about me is through others speaking about me. The degree to which the participants engaged in cultural mixing, or moved between social contexts over time, also influenced whether they formed an HMI. Ashwini (see Figure 5 caption) and Zara, in alignment with “achieved” multiculturals, described how moving to different cities/countries allowed them to experience cultural mixing throughout their lives, even into adulthood, which in turn contributed to their HMI. I've been living in Malaysia my whole life and when I'm here [US], I'm like, ‘Whoa. There's something else here.’ So yeah. This totally changed my mind about a lot of things. I would definitely say I'm multicultural because I've been exposed to a lot of different things when I'm studying here compared to when I was back in Malaysia. Ian exemplified how a person can experience cultural mixing without moving between cities, and how this can occur even within the home. BELGRADE ET AL | 2301 F I G U R E 5 The journey. Participant quote: “It really symbolized for me this solo journey that I feel like I've been going on for the past 25 years. I grew up in predominantly white spaces at some chapters [of my story]. At other chapters, I lived in really diverse communities. And in telling my story I feel like I have a very unique experience. Which I know is something that a lot of people who identify as multicultural would say. But really thinking through especially being in college, living outside of my home and interacting with different people and places, and then moving to Atlanta, and then moving to Phoenix, and then moving back to the Midwest. Um I just feel like life is a giant roadtrip that has taken me to different people and places and I have found myself along the way and come to grips and really accepted what my unique identities are.” (Ashwini) I speak English with my dad, I speak Portuguese with my mom, and Mandarin with my Grandparents. And with my sister I switch between English and Portuguese all the time. I just keep switching. I've lived in one place my whole life, and I consider myself to be multicultural because at home there's always been a clash between cultures‐being my dad was Chinese, and my mom was Taiwanese, and in the context of Brazilian society, and going to international school. I interacted all the time with different people from different places in different contexts. Like Ashwini and Yoshito, many participants framed their hybrid multicultural identity as a lifelong accumulation of changing circumstances and shifting experiences (see Figures 6 and 7 captions). They constantly navigated different social contexts, trying to understand different worldviews and operate within a changing set of norms. 3.3 | Perceptions of macro‐level marginalization In the previous section, we focused on the composition of the participants’ cultural social environments; here we shift towards their perceptions of macro‐level environments. The participants constantly experienced and witnessed marginalization, and this fostered their identification as uniquely hybrid multiculturals. When responding to our question regarding experiences that led to the development of their HMI, participants reported instances of cultural misunderstandings because of their memberships in nondominant cultural groups. 2302 | BELGRADE ET AL F I G U R E 6 Studying abroad. Participant quote: “This represents our journey so far just riding in. People coming in, people going out, different interactions all the time. It's like, not only different people but different cultures interacting all the time.” (Ian) Speaking about his photo (see Figure 8), João describes how the Brazilian greeting of a kiss on the cheek was misconstrued by his American schoolmates (see Figure 8 caption). Many participants who grew up in the United States described frequent cultural misunderstandings in mainstream American contexts, such as public primary and secondary schools. Yoshito recognized this pattern among his peers and described his contrasting experience in international school. When studying at the United Nations International School, he experienced very little cultural misunderstanding. Being in an environment that had a deep understanding of diverse cultures facilitated “accepting‐ness.” In international school you have kids from all over the world. That creates a lot of understanding and accepting‐ness in each one of us towards the other, and we have lots of cultural awareness because I attended the United Nations international school. My school really valued that diversity. (Yoshito) Then he contrasted this experience to a typical public school in the United States, where he saw cultural misunderstandings likely stemming from the lack of representation. “Who is there to help them in their education to be multicultural?” While the participants did not hold the same cultural backgrounds, each of them described instances of cultural misunderstanding, and this joint experience facilitated the sense of group identity as hybrid multiculturals. Many participants assessed that communities in the US devalued multiculturalism. For example, Isabella described the differences she saw in the society's value of multiculturalism when she moved from Puerto Rico to the United States, with the United States generally showing a lack of respect for diverse cultures (see Figure 9 caption). João mirrored Isabella's views, echoing how in the United States many people do not value multiculturalism and actively pressure those outside the dominant majority culture to assimilate. For example, João shared a photograph of the American flag on campus and described how this symbol represents a pressure to assimilate to the dominant, monocultural society. (João's photograph is not included to protect his anonymity). BELGRADE ET AL | 2303 F I G U R E 7 This is a photo of a busy highway and intersection. I just happened to take it for no specific reason, but this represents the trajectory of my life. So far, I've lived in 4 countries around the world, which is symbolized by the road signs and the multiple ways I can go. Time is moving fast and my next move will come soon, as symbolized by the fact that it is a highway. (Yoshito) My thought here […] focuses on the government and by extension the culture within that government and the caption says, “The pressure to assimilate sometimes feels like it's just looming over our heads.” And, so sometimes it can feel like there's, everybody wants you to do a certain thing a certain way, and if you do it any other way, it's wrong. So, it's like, it's just always there, looming, and no‐one understands how it feels other than people that have our same kind of multicultural background. Given the participants’ HMI, it is unsurprising that they valued environments that celebrated and reflected that multiculturalism. However, when reflecting on the mainstream US culture, many participants described hostility and disinterestedness towards multiculturalism. Rather paradoxically, living in a society that devalued multiculturalism made the participants acutely aware how central multiculturalism is to their sense of self, and this was cited as a primary driver in their identity development. Some participants viewed their contexts through a lens that was very acute at recognizing societal inequities (see Figure 10). For instance, Tesfu recognized that even when there were conversations including diverse groups, society continued to center whiteness. “One way to try and focus on multiculturalness and also focus on them more deeply instead of talking about the stereotypes or like the very basic level of a country or region is to stop talking about white people so much.” For Tesfu, consequently, feeling the oneness and identifying with the mainstream culture was not possible because she could see the structural inequities in the society that disadvantaged her as a woman of color. 2304 | BELGRADE ET AL F I G U R E 8 It's very tough when the things taught here conflict with the things taught at home. Participant quote: “And then I was at elementary school at that point, and I would go up to some other girl that I just met, kiss her, and it would be like, ‘oh my god… ahhh’ and like, I just said, ‘hi.’ And they would be like ‘oh you like her.’ I don't know, things like that. Just because I said, ‘hi,’ and that's just how I say hi. But they're like ‘ahhhh you love her.’ I'm like, ‘no.’” (João) F I G U R E 9 Wired. Participant quote: “I had to study every single religion and see what they have in common. I had to study, as well, all the cultures in the world and try and understand how they work. And I come over here so you can respect and so on. And I don't see that happening ever here.” (Isabella) The participants also recognized inequities among minority groups, and mistreatment exercised even by the groups to which they belong. Ashwini, who has heritage from southern India, reflected: I actually spent one summer in India and another in Atlanta, Georgia, in predominately Black communities. So that was my first time being in not predominantly white spaces so it was like I don't fit here either and everybody around me knows that too. So learning to adapt in other ways. Not just being conscious of Black and Asian BELGRADE ET AL | 2305 F I G U R E 10 “Feminist: A person who believes in the social, political, and economic equality of the sexes” ‐ Chimamanda Ngozi Adichie (Tesfu) tension‐ like the history of that. Like, not pretending that that doesn't exist, like anti‐Blackness in a lot of Asian communities is really real and so recognizing that and feeling the need to adapt to be like, “I care about you and I see you as a human. I don't have these kinds of… that kind of attitude that other Asians you might have encountered have had.” (Ashwini) As a result of their awareness of the structural inequities that disadvantage some groups more than others, the participants felt more distance towards the larger society and also their own ethnic group, when its members discriminated against other ethnic groups. They were, therefore, more likely to identify as a hybrid multicultural, an identity that embraced their western influences but also had room for their social criticism. The recognition of societal inequity also showed in their empathy towards various discriminated cultural groups; this strengthened their sense of oneness with other multicultural people. 3.4 | Culturally related interpersonal experiences In addition to macro‐level marginalization, the participants’ personal encounters with members of dominant and minority cultures similarly entailed experiences of marginalization. Less inclusive social settings bred negative interpersonal interactions (see Figure 4 for a graphical depiction of this relationship). Many participants, especially those who spent most of their lives in the United States, described a tension that resulted from having multiple, conflicting cultural influences. I do consider myself multicultural because… my parents immigrated here. […] I have my culture at home but then I was born and raised in America so there's the Western culture in me, sometimes, that takes away from Eritrean culture. (Tesfu) 2306 | BELGRADE ET AL F I G U R E 11 Why should we blend in when we can stand out? Participant quote: “This is a picture I took in the past. It's actually in Spain. And the caption is, ‘Why would we blend in when we could stand out?’ And I guess I was just trying to say that something as simple as, you know, when people want us to be like them, to assimilate to their culture. Why should we do that when we can shine in our own way and have our own, like, “I'm different and that's okay and that's good.” You know? So there's different ways we can all… you don't have to be part of a crowd that nobody can distinguish. We can be our own thing and people. I think the best kind of people appreciate that for who you are.” (João) Internalizing influences from the dominant culture exposed the participants to marginalization from their ethnic culture. Their nondominant cultural affiliations, in turn, exposed them to assimilation pressures and discrimination. This made the participants question their belongingness to either culture and pushed them to recognize their belongingness to a multicultural subculture instead. Not being able to completely fit into any singular cultural group, the participants formed an HMI that could accommodate and encompass all their cultural influences in the way the monocultural categories could not (see Figure 11 caption). The participants discussed how outgroup interactions, specifically implicit and explicit forms of discrimination and marginalization from members in the dominant culture, shaped their behaviors and perceptions of their own multiculturalism. For example, Tesfu described how struggles with her hair reflected standards of beauty that are shaped in the form of whiteness. “Straightening my hair is horrible. But it's what's considered pretty so I kind of have always had to do it. Just so I felt pretty enough. So that I kind of fit in.” Ashwini, whose first language is English, also described discrimination when she was complimented on her English by members of the dominant culture: “People who are like, ‘You don't have an accent!’ Well, of course not. I was born and raised here.” Referring to the photograph in Figure 12, João described mainstream society members’ negative judgment of not only one, but all of his cultural practices. To him, this reflected a generalized rejection of his cultural heritage. The participants also described ingroup interactions largely as ambivalent relationships with their family members and other ethnic ingroup members. In particular, the participants found that family members aggravated the felt differences between their multiple cultural identities. Speaking about the chasm between the buildings in the photograph in Figure 13, and herself positioned in that chasm between, Ashwini described how she was not fully accepted as Indian by her extended family (see Figure 13 caption). BELGRADE ET AL | 2307 F I G U R E 12 When you choose a path, how do you know if it's the right one? Participant quote: “Sometimes things that feel super natural to us and have been natural for the longest time suddenly is totally switched and is weird and different to other people and it's wrong what you're doing and you've been doing it for your whole life and then you have to switch how you do everything.” (João) F I G U R E 13 Not part of your world. Participant quote: “I'm up in my own headspace sometimes. Feeling detached from everything that's around me. Not understanding different objects or different people. But very much still being in it, and attached to it. But still feeling not grounded in that, but still having a front row seat to it.” (Ashwini) 2308 | BELGRADE ET AL F I G U R E 14 Is this what I'm supposed to? Participant quote: “I just thought the dog looks like… Piña looks so funny like he's just like, ‘Is this what I'm supposed to be doing?’ It looks so unnatural for this little dog to be sitting out on a bench in the middle of the city and that's sometimes how I feel sometimes. I'm just like ‘aaaahhhh is this right?’ Like somebody's kind of holding me at an angle and I'm trying to smile, and it all just feels a little strange. And… yeah! That's something I experience often. Whether it's like, ‘I wanna wear this outfit today…. Oh, but I'm going to my parent's in 20 min so I'm going to change and wear this thing that I don't actually like.’ But that's the position I need to be held in at this moment in time. And I'm just gonna pose. I'm just gonna pose a little bit.” (Ashwini) Describing her photograph in Figure 14, Ashwini described how interactions with her family felt like a constant “façade” (see Figure 14 caption). Participants described how their experience of the world did not fit within the parameters of their family member's expectations or others’ that were close to them, leading to a sense of isolation from their ingroup. For example, João noted: A lot of times, through our multicultural background, we feel like we have all these identities and experiences and even though people seem like they're near us and wanna help, it's not always that easy. It's not always that simple to just join a group and be part of it. So even though sometimes you are in a close proximity, you can be far away still. 4 | DISC US SION This study, to our knowledge, is the first to qualitatively describe how being “more than the sum of the parts,” (West et al., 2017) or having an HMI, is experienced. The participants identified with one another, recognizing their belongingness to the same group, even though they individually represented different cultural groups. The participants also unanimously described their HMI in a positive way and recognized both psychological and interpersonal benefits resulting from this identity. BELGRADE ET AL | 2309 While the participants described HMI as a positive identity, their evaluations of their social contexts (i.e., macro‐level social environments and culturally related interpersonal experiences) were largely negative, aligning with previous work describing both the struggles and opportunities of hybridity (Ang, 2003; Verbaan & Cox, 2014). This paradox may have been present in our sample because the study participants viewed the United States, their country of residence, as largely unsupportive of multiculturalism. When residing in culturally homogeneous environments (particularly in those made up of the dominant society members), participants encountered negative perceptions towards multiculturalism. This highlighted participants’ awareness of their difference as hybrid multiculturals and, in a countermeasure against the homogenous culture, they learned to appreciate and increasingly define themselves through their HMI. Nevertheless, participants also had experiences of culturally heterogeneous environments. These were described much like Bhabha's (1994) “third spaces” or Ang's (2003) “hybridizing contexts”; that is, spaces where different cultural systems interact and intermix to foster novel cultural elements and identities. Participants generally recognized positive perceptions regarding multiculturalism in these spaces, and this validated and strengthened their HMIs. Therefore, participants cited both negative and positive social experiences that played a role as they bridged beyond their own cultural identities and identified with multicultural people more broadly, though their emphasis was often placed more strongly on negative social experiences. 4.1 | Valence of experiences and perceptions The negative social experiences (relations with both ingroup and outgroup members), as well as negative perceptions of the society (cultural misunderstandings, devalued multiculturalism, and recognizing social inequities), were instrumental for the multicultural participants to develop a superordinate HMI that both encompassed their cultural influences and was also separated from these monocultural influences. The experience of building one's HMI was described as an isolating journey; the participants experienced distance and marginalization from both their own cultural groups and cultural outsiders. This finding is in contrast to the literature that connects social identity development to positive social experiences that signal social acceptance of one's identity (Ashforth & Schinoff, 2016; Bradford, 2006; Darr & Scarselletta, 2002; Smith et al., 2013). This finding also departs from West et al.'s (2017) suggestion that HMIs are not likely to emerge when the surrounding society perceives various cultural identities as conflictual. We propose that negative social experiences fostered the development of HMI because HMI, as a superordinate identity, is not threatened by the rejection from the majority cultural group or one's ethnic groups. Quite the contrary, HMI is strengthened when such negative cultural experiences weaken one's ties to individual cultural groups. While the negative perceptions of the society and interpersonal relations were described as instrumental in the development of HMI, we propose that positive social experiences may also reinforce an existing HMI (Meeus et al., 2002; Postmes et al., 2005). For example, when multicultural individuals can meet socially, their commitment to and positive assessment of that identity likely becomes stronger. This was implied when participants such as Yoshito, Ashwini, and Isabella described the affirmative multicultural contexts they were a part of (i.e., international school, Puerto Rico, and a multicultural college town) and described these as places where multicultural individuals can flourish and feel supported. The participants did not directly state this, but in our estimation, having the opportunity to discuss their common experiences with the other study participants who also had an HMI appeared to make them feel more positive and validated in their own identity. This finding aligns with West et al.'s (2017) proposal on multicultural contexts eliciting hybrid identities, and also the postcolonial work viewing ‘third spaces’ where cultures meet and mix as fertile grounds for the growth of hybrid identities (Bhabha, 1994; Chulach & Gagnon, 2016; Verbaan & Cox, 2014). Moreover, past work suggested that negatively appraised identity‐relevant experiences (identity questioning) are transformational and push people to create their identities anew, while positive (identity validating) experiences actually reinforce and strengthen the pre‐existing identity (Kira & Balkin, 2014). Further studies should 2310 | BELGRADE ET AL empirically examine the potentially differential roles of positive and negative identity‐relevant experiences in the development of HMI. 4.2 | Cultural mixing across a lifetime Our findings both align and diverge from existing research describing HMI and cultural mixing. The participants described a lifelong journey of accumulating cultural experiences that layered on top of their often‐multicultural home environments. This on‐going cultural mixing corresponds to “achieved” multiculturalism as defined by Martin and Shao (2016) which was not, however, linked to HMI in their studies. Therefore, our findings offer empirical evidence that aligns with the proposition of the Transformative Theory of Biculturalism. West et al. (2017, p. 973) theorized that to form an HMI one must: “draw on cultural ingredients beyond those from the source cultures; a multitude of cultures could come into play in forming one's hybrid culture.” Our findings support this idea of people developing HMIs through adolescence and adult experiences, not only when being immersed into a multicultural context early in their lives. 4.3 | Limitations Our study provided some of the first empirical evidence addressing the nature of HMI and how perceptions and experiences of one's social contexts contribute to the development of HMI. However, the study has its limitations and raises important questions for additional research and exploration. Our findings were based on the experiences and perspectives of 10 participants, most of whom were relatively young and highly educated, and all participants were affiliated with a large university. It is important to examine whether our findings can be observed in a larger, more representative sample of hybrid multiculturals. Further, our exploration of HMI development relies on the participants’ own reflection and does not provide longitudinal measures necessary to truly observe development. Finally, Photovoice may introduce confounds to our findings. In particular, a group discussion format can create a spurious shared identity among our multicultural participants, raising questions about whether HMI is indeed a shared social identity that transcended the multicultural participants’ different cultural backgrounds. 4.4 | Conclusion Using qualitative Photovoice methodology, we offer a window into the lived experiences of people with HMI and their perceptions of how their social contexts influenced the development of their HMI. Our results highlight how experiencing diverse cultures both at home and over the course of one's life are essential aspects of multicultural people's social contexts, as theorized by West et al. (2017) in their Transformative Theory of Biculturalism. We also found that people with an HMI tended to describe many of their social contexts through a negative lens, describing negative interpersonal experiences with both their ingroups and outgroups, and describing the dominant US society as inequitable, devaluing multiculturalism, and rife with cultural misunderstandings. While most of the contexts crucial to the development of HMI were described as negative, some participants were able to find positive, validating social environments outside the dominant US cultural influence. The paradox lies in that while the participants’ developmental journeys were largely described in terms of invalidation originating from their various social influences, they still viewed HMI as a positive identity, and strongly valued it for both its perceived psychological and interpersonal benefits. BELGRADE | ET AL 2311 A C KN O W L E D G M E N T Thank you to the University of Michigan Library Mini Grant for funding this project. D A TA A V A I L A B I L I T Y S T A T E M E N T The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. ORCID Andrea Belgrade http://orcid.org/0000-0002-9566-2351 PE ER RE VIEW The peer review history for this article is available at https://publons.com/publon/10.1002/jcop.22776. REFERENCES Ackermann, A. (2012). Cultural hybridity: Between metaphor and empiricism, Conceptualizing Cultural Hybridization (pp. 5–25). Springer. Amiot, C. E., De la Sablonniere, R., Terry, D. J., & Smith, J. R. (2007). Integration of social identities in the self: Toward a cognitive‐developmental model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(4), 364–388. Ang, I. (2003). Together‐in‐difference: beyond diaspora, into hybridity. Asian Studies Review, 27(2), 141–154. Arasaratnam, L. A. (2013). A review of articles on multiculturalism in 35 years of IJIR. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 37(6), 676–685. Ashforth, B. E., Harrison, S. H., & Corley, K. G. (2008). Identification in organizations: An examination of four fundamental questions. Journal of Management, 34(3), 325–374. Ashforth, B. E., & Schinoff, B. S. (2016). Identity under construction: How individuals come to define themselves in organizations. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 3, 111–137. https://doi.org/10. 1146/annurevn-orgpsych-041015-062322 Bananuka, T., & John, V. M. (2014). Picturing community development work in Uganda: fostering dialogue through Photovoice. Community Development Journal, 50(2), 196–212. Banks, J. A. (2014). Multicultural education and global citizens. In Benet‐Martínez, V. & Hong, Y. Y., (Eds.) The oxford handbook of multicultural identity (pages of chapter). Oxford Library of Psychology Barbour, R. S. (2001). Checklists for improving rigour in qualitative research: a case of the tail wagging the dog? BMJ, 322(7294), 1115–1117. Belon, A. P., Nieuwendyk, L. M., Vallianatos, H., & Nykiforuk, C. I. (2016). Perceived community environmental influences on eating behaviors: A Photovoice analysis. Social Science & Medicine, 171, 18–29. Bhabha, H. K. (1994). The Location of Culture. Routledge. Bradford, M. (2006). Affirmative Psychotherapy with Bisexual Women. Journal of Bisexuality, 6(1–2), 13–25. https://doi. org/10.1300/J159v06n01_02 Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77–101. https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa Caza, B. B., Moss, S., & Vough, H. (2017). From synchronizing to harmonizing: The process of authenticating multiple work identities. Administrative Science Quarterly000183921773 3972. Chmielewski, J. F., & Yost, M. R. (2012). Psychosocial influences on bisexual women's body image. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37(2), 224–241. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684311426126 Cho, S., Kim, M. A., & Kwon, S. I. (2019). Using the photovoice method to understand experiences of people with physical disabilities working in social enterprises. Disability and Health Journal, 12, 685–693. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dhjo. 2019.03.011 Chulach, T., & Gagnon, M. (2016). Working in a ‘third space’: A closer look at the hybridity, identity and agency of nurse practitioners. Nursing Inquiry, 23(1), 52–63. Darr, A., & Scarselletta, M. (2002). Technicians, clients, and professional authority: structured interactions and identity formation in technical work. New Technology, Work and Employment, 17(1), 61–73. Durkee, M. I., & Williams, J. L. (2015). Accusations of acting White: Links to Black students’ racial identity and mental health. Journal of Black Psychology, 41(1), 26–48. 2312 | BELGRADE ET AL Freedman, D. A., Pitner, R. O., Powers, M. C., & Anderson, T. P. (2012). Using photovoice to develop a grounded theory of socio‐environmental attributes influencing the health of community environments. British Journal of Social Work, 44(5), 1301–1321. Gioia, D. A., Corley, K. G., & Hamilton, A. L. (2013). Seeking qualitative rigor in inductive research: Notes on the Gioia methodology. Organizational Research Methods, 16(1), 15–31. Hergenrather, K. C., Rhodes, S. D., Cowan, C. A., Bardhoshi, G., & Pula, S. (2009). Photovoice as community‐based participatory research: A qualitative review. American Journal of Health Behavior, 33(6), 686–698. Hong, Y. Y., & Khei, M. (2014). Dynamic multiculturalism: the interplay of socio‐cognitive, neural and genetic mechanisms. The Oxford Handbook of Multicultural Identity, 11–34. Johansen, S., & Le, T. N. (2014). Youth perspective on multiculturalism using photovoice methodology. Youth & Society, 46(4), 548–565. Kira, M., & Balkin, D. B. (2014). Interactions between work and identities: Thriving, withering, or redefining the self? Human Resource Management Review, 24(2), 131–143. Krueger, R. A., & Casey, M. A. (2015). Focus groups a practical guide for applied research. Sage. Martin, L., & Shao, B. (2016). Early immersive culture mixing: The key to understanding cognitive and identity differences among multiculturals. Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology, 47(10), 1409–1429. Martin, L., Shao, B., & Thomas, D. C. (2019). The role of early immersive culture mixing in cultural identifications of multiculturals. Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology, 50, 508–523. Meeus, W. I. M., Oosterwegel, A., & Vollebergh, W. (2002). Parental and peer attachment and identity development in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 25(1), 93–106. Moore, A. M., & Barker, G. G. (2012). Confused or multicultural: Third culture individuals’ cultural identity. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 36, 553–562. Neufeld, S. D., & Schmitt, M. T. (2018). Solidarity Not Homogeneity: Constructing a Superordinate Aboriginal Identity That Protects Subgroup Identities. Political Psychology Nguyen, A. M. D., & Benet‐Martinez, V. (2010). Multicultural identity: What it is and why it matters. The Psychology of Social and Cultural Diversity, 6, 87–114. Nitzinger, V., Held, S., Kevane, B., & Eudave, Y. (2019). Latino health perceptions in Rural Montana. Family & Community Health, 42(2), 150–160. https://doi.org/10.1097/FCH.0000000000000213 Nykiforuk, C. I., Vallianatos, H., & Nieuwendyk, L. M. (2011). Photovoice as a method for revealing community perceptions of the built and social environment. International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 10(2), 103–124. Pekerti, A. A., & Thomas, D. C. (2016). n‐Culturals: modeling the multicultural identity. Cross Cultural & Strategic Management, 23(1), 101–127. Ponterotto, J. G. (2005). Qualitative research in counseling psychology: A primer on research paradigms and philosophy of science. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(2), 126–136. Postmes, T., Haslam, S. A., & Swaab, R. I. (2005). Social influence in small groups: An interactive model of social identity formation. European Review of Social Psychology, 16(1), 1–42. Richards, K. A. R., & Hemphill, M. A. (2018). A practical guide to collaborative qualitative data analysis. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 37(2), 225–231. Rosenthal, L., & Levy, S. R. (2010). The colorblind, multicultural, and polycultural ideologicalapproaches to improving intergroup attitudes and relations. Social Issues and Policy Review, 4(1), 215–246. Saleem, M., Wojcieszak, M. E., Hawkins, I., Li, M., & Ramasubramanian, S. (2019). Social identity threats: How media and discrimination affect Muslim Americans’ identification as Americans and trust in the US government. Journal of Communication, 69(2), 214–236. Smith, L. G. E., Amiot, C. E., Smith, J. R., Callan, V. J., & Terry, D. J. (2013). The social validation and coping model of organizational identity development: A longitudinal test. Journal of Management, 39(7), 1952–1978. https://doi.org/ 10.1177/0149206313488212 Strack, R. W., Aronson, R. E., Orsini, M. M., Seitz, C. M., & Mccoy, R. (2018). Using photovoice to uncover campus issues and advocate change for Black males. Journal of College Student Development, 59(4), 491–498. Valera, P., Gallin, J., Schuk, D., & Davis, N. (2009). “Trying to Eat Healthy”: A photovoice study about women's access to healthy food in New York City. Affilia, 24(3), 300–314. Verbaan, E., & Cox, A. M. (2014). Occupational sub‐cultures, jurisdictional struggle and third space: Theorising professional service responses to research data management. The. Journal of Academic Librarianship, 40(3‐4), 211–219. Verkuyten, M. (2006). Multicultural recognition and ethnic minority rights: A social identity perspective. European Review of Social Psychology, 17(1), 148–184. Vora, D., Martin, L., Fitzsimmons, S. R., Pekerti, A. A., Lakshman, C., & Raheem, S. (2018). Multiculturalism within individuals: A review, critique, and agenda for future research. Journal of International Business Studies, 1–26. BELGRADE ET AL | 2313 Wachs, T. (1999). Celebrating complexity. Conceptualization and assessment of the environment. In S. Freidman, & T. Wachs (Eds.), Measuring environment across the life span: Emerging Methods and Concepts (pp. 357–392). American Psychological Association. Wang, C., & Burris, M. A. (1997). Photovoice: Concept, methodology, and use for participatory needs assessment. Health Education & Behavior, 24(3), 369–387. West, A. L., Zhang, R., Yampolsky, M., & Sasaki, J. Y. (2017). More than the sum of its parts: A transformative theory of biculturalism. Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology, 48(7), 963–990. Yampolsky, M. A., Amiot, C. E., & de la Sablonnière, R. (2016). The Multicultural Identity Integration Scale (MULTIIS): Developing a comprehensive measure for configuring one's multiple cultural identities within the self. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 22(2), 166–184. https://doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000043 How to cite this article: Belgrade, A., Kira, M., Sadaghiyani, S., & Lee, F. (2022). What makes us complete: Hybrid multicultural identity and its social contextual origins. Journal of Community Psychology, 50, 2290–2313. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.22776 Copyright of Journal of Community Psychology is the property of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.