Psychological Reports, 1984, 55, 115-118. @ Psychological Reports 1984 EMPATHY AND BIRTH ORDER MIR J A KALLIOPUSKA U?tiversity of Helsink? Summary.-Previous conflicting results have been found - i n studying the relation between empathy and birth order. 194 school students aged 9 to 12 yr. completed an adapted version of the Mehrabian and Epstein scale of emotional empathy. The social background data including birth order were gathered during home interviews. The mean differences in empathy for ordinal positions were not statistically significant, although the difference between third- and fourth-borns approached significance. It appears that middle-born children seem more prone to empathize than firstborns. Considerable data concerning birth order have been gathered, but conflicting results have been reported (Schooler, 1972). It is generally accepted that firstborns enjoy a more favorable position in the family than later-borns. They represent the center of attraction for parents and receive extensive time. Rashba (1975) reported that ordinal positions of 8-yr.-old girls made a significant difference in perceived maternal warmth. Last-borns received significantly more maternal warmth as infants and as latency-age children than did first- and only-borns. Altus (1966) reported that firstborns tend to be more conscientious and have higher levels of self-esteem than later-borns, McArthur (1956) noted that firstborns are more sensitive and studious than second-borns who are more placid, more easy-going, and less studious than their older siblings. Thompson (1974) pointed out that second-borns may also be more fun-loving, humorous, and less introverted than firstborns. Some researchers maintain that family size and altruistic behaviour are unrelated (Handlon & Gross, 1959; Dreman & Greenbaum, 1973), while others state that growing u p in a large family promotes generosity (Sawyer, 1966). Other investigators have stated that children from small families may have a great deal of self-assurance and initiative and are more willing to act spontaneously to help another child in emergencies (Staub, 1971a, 1971b). Staub (1971a) noticed that middle or younger children seemed to help a peer in distress and to share or donate generously. Whiting and Whiting (1975) observed that only children and the youngest children in a family tend to be more egoistic than others, seeking more help and attention. They offer less help and support to their fellow peers. Mussen and Eisenberg-Berg (1977) stated in terms of social learning theory, that older children are expected to help with their younger siblings and are frequently rewarded for being helpful and nurrurant to them. Hamrnersla (1974) in studying college 'Department of Psychology, Division of Applied Psychology, Fabianinkatu 33, 00170 Helsinki 17, Finland. 116 ' M. KALLIOPUSKA freshmen confirmed that firstborns and only children are prone to help another person in distress if the required action is quite easy or simple. Bizman with coworkers (1978) showed that children 5 yr. of age and older in ageheterogeneous kindergartens in Israeli cities and kibbutzim were more helpful than same-age children with age-homogeneous kindergartens. Stotland, et al. (1971) hypothesized that birth order in the family is one determinant of social schemas. Firstborn and only children tended to respond empathetically to people who differed from them on the dimension of status, while later-borns related to the similarity dimension by showing more emotional arousal when the model was a peer or was perceived as similar to the self. Stotland, et dl. (1978) indicated that on hospital wards, firstborn nursing students showed a greater tendency to spend less time in the patients' rooms and more time interacting with fellow workers, in the hallways and around the nursing station. Later-borns interacted more actively with patients and stayed longer in the patients' rooms. These data suggest that later-borns may be more empathetic than firstborns with persons in a dependent situation. Ernst and Angst (1983) have, however, summarized study reports and have drawn a conclusion that chere is no evidence of superior empathy in later-borns. METHOD Subjects A group of 194 school children ranging from 9 to 12 yr. of age took part in the study. They were tested in a group to which each question was presented orally. Sufficient time for comprehension and answering were allowed. Social background data including birth order were gachered during the home interview. Measwe The Mehrabian and Epstein's (1972) emotional empathy scale was modified for children to include only a three-step scale, the alternatives being completely disagree, disagree and agree to some extent, and completely agree. The maximum number of points is 99. The internal consistency of the test for 363 children is indicated by a moderate alpha-coefficient of .68. In this study scores on the scale of empathy correlated slightly, i.e., .ll,with scores on the scale of social desirability by Crandall, Crandall, and Katkovsky (1965). In general, results show that mean empathy scores by birth order did not differ significantly. Findings are somewhat different from the results of former investigations in that middle-born children (second- and third-borns) were prone to empathize more.than firstborns. Fourth- and fifth-borns were 117 EMPATHY AND BIRTH ORDER TABLE I EMPATHY:MEANSAND STANDARD DEVIATIONSACCORDING TO BIRTHORDER Birth Order First Second Third Fourth Fifth Sixth Total M 69.4 70.7 72.4 67.7 66.0 72.0 69.9 SD 8.3 6.5 6.8 6.0 8.4 0.0 7.6 Comparison n I06 52 20 11 4 1 194 df 12 t 1,3 1,4 1,5 2,3 24 23 3,4 335 4,5 generally less empathic than firstborns, a n d second- a n d third-borns. It seems obvious that in large families (4 or more children) t h e youngest children receive less special parental attention t h a n t h e oldest siblings, because parents transfer responsibility for caretaking o n t o t h e older siblings' shoulders. In principle this is a good way t o teach e m p a t h y a n d responsibility t o t h e older children i n t h e family, b u t within large families parents m a y need to devote m o r e special t i m e t o t h e younger children. REFERENCES ALTUS,N. C. Birth order and its sequelae. Science, 1966, 151, 44-49. BIZMAN, A., YINON,Y., MIVTZARI,E., & SHAVIT, R. Effects of the a e structure of the kindergarten on altruistic behavior. J o u m l o f School Psycho&ogy, 1978, IG, 154-160 CRANDALL, V. C., GRANDALL, V. J., & KATKOVSKY, W. A children's social desirability questlonnalre. Joursal of Consulting Psychology, 1965, 29, 27-36. DREMAN,S. B., & GREENBAUM,C. W. Altruism or sharing behavior in Israeli kindergarten children. Child Development, 1973, 44, 61-68. ERNST, C.,& ANGST, J. Birth order: its influence on personality. Berlin: Springer, 1983. H~MERSLA E. , J. The effects of participation in a laboratory bystander intervention study on subsequent attitudes and intervention behavior. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Univer. of Washington, Seattle, 1974. HANDLON, B. J., & GROSS, P. The development of sharing behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1953, 59, 425-428. MCARTHUR,C. Personalities of first and second children. Psychiatry, 1956, 19, 47-54. MEHRABIAN,A., & EPSTEIN,N. A measure of emotional empathy. Jourrrl o f Perronaliry, 1972, 40, 525-543. MUSSEN, P., & EISENBERG-BERG, N. Roots o f caring, sharing, and helping: the development of prosociaL behavior in children. San Francisco, CA: Freeman, 1977. RASHBA, S. J. M. Antecedent conditions in the development of empathy and style of approach in eight-year-old girls. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Univer. of Florida, 1975. 118 M. KALLIOPUSKA SAWYER,J. The altruism scale: a measure of cooperative, individualistic and competitive interpersonal orientation. American Journal o f Sociology, 1966, 71, 407-416. SCHOOLER,C Birch order effects: not here, not now. Psychological Bulletin, 1972, 78, 161-175. E. A child in distress, the influence of modeling and nurmrance on children's attempts to help. Developmental Psychology, 1971, 5 , 124-133. (a) STAUB, E. The use of role playing and induction in children's learning of helping and sharing behavior. Child Development, 1971, 42, 805-816. ( b ) STOTLAND,E.. MATHEWS,K. E., JR., SHERMAN.S. F., HANSSON, R. O., & RICHARDSON, B. Z. Empathy, fantasy and helping. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage, 1978. STOTLAND, E., SHERMAN, S. E., & SHAVER, K G. Empathy a d b h h order. Lincoln, NE: Univer. of Nebraska Press, 1971. THOMPSON, V. D. Family size: implicit policies and assumed psychological outcomes. Journal o f Social Issues, 1974, 30, 93-124. WHITING, B. B., & WHITING. J. W. M. Children in six cultures: a psychocultural analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer. Press, 1975. STAUB, Accepted June 8, 1984.