Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Get Complete eBook Download link Below for Instant Download: https://browsegrades.net/documents/286751/ebook-payment-link-forinstant-download-after-payment Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com EIGHTH EDITION Research Methods in Physical Activity Jerry R. Thomas, EdD Philip E. Martin, PhD Jennifer L. Etnier, PhD Stephen J. Silverman, EdD Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Names: Thomas, Jerry R., author. | Martin, Philip E. (Philip Edward), 1955-author. | Etnier, Jennifer L., author. | Silverman, Stephen J., author. Title: Research methods in physical activity / Jerry R. Thomas, Philip E. Martin, Jenny L. Etnier, Stephen J. Silverman . Description: Eighth edition. | Champaign, IL : Human Kinetics, Inc., 2023. | Includes bibliographical references and indexes. Identifiers: LCCN 2021042222 (print) | LCCN 2021042223 (ebook) | ISBN 9781718201026 (paperback) | ISBN 9781718201033 (epub) | ISBN 9781718201040 (pdf) Subjects: LCSH: Physical education and training--Research. | Health--Research. | Recreation--Research. Classification: LCC GV361 .T47 2023 (print) | LCC GV361 (ebook) | DDC 613.7/1072--dc23 LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2021042222 LC ebook record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2021042223 ISBN: 978-1-7182-0102-6 (paperback) ISBN: 978-1-7182-1304-3 (loose-leaf) Copyright © 2023 by Jerry R. Thomas, Philip E. Martin, Jennifer L. Etnier, and Stephen J. Silverman Copyright © 2015, 2011, 2005 by Jerry R. Thomas, Jack K. Nelson, and Stephen J. Silverman Copyright © 2001, 1996, 1990, 1985 by Jerry R. Thomas and Jack K. Nelson Human Kinetics supports copyright. Copyright fuels scientific and artistic endeavor, encourages authors to create new works, and promotes free speech. Thank you for buying an authorized edition of this work and for complying with copyright laws by not reproducing, scanning, or distributing any part of it in any form without written permission from the publisher. You are supporting authors and allowing Human Kinetics to continue to publish works that increase the knowledge, enhance the performance, and improve the lives of people all over the world. To report suspected copyright infringement of content published by Human Kinetics, contact us at permissions@hkusa.com. To request permission to legally reuse content published by Human Kinetics, please refer to the information at https://US.HumanKinetics.com/pages /permissions-information. The web addresses cited in this text were current as of October 2021, unless otherwise noted. Acquisitions Editor: Diana Vincer Developmental Editor: Melissa J. Zavala Copyeditor: Amy Pavelich Proofreader: Leigh Keylock Indexer: Nan N. Badgett Permissions Manager: Dalene Reeder Senior Graphic Designer: Nancy Rasmus Cover Designer: Keri Evans Cover Design Specialist: Susan Rothermel Allen Photographs (interior): © Human Kinetics, unless otherwise noted Photo Asset Manager: Laura Fitch Photo Production Manager: Jason Allen Senior Art Manager: Kelly Hendren Illustrations: © Human Kinetics, unless otherwise noted Printer: Walsworth Printed in the United States of America 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 The paper in this book was manufactured using responsible forestry methods. Human Kinetics 1607 N. Market Street Champaign, IL 61820 USA United States and International Website: US.HumanKinetics.com Email: info@hkusa.com Phone: 1-800-747-4457 Canada Website: Canada.HumanKinetics.com Email: info@hkcanada.com E8222 (paperback) / E8605 (loose-leaf) Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Jack Kimberly Nelson (September 14, 1932-January 12, 2018) Dr. Jack K. Nelson grew up in Valier, Montana, where he worked on a ranch, survived polio, and enlisted in the Air Force, where he was a pilot. He graduated from the University of Montana at Missoula, then matriculated to the University of Oregon, where he earned his PhD working with H. Harrison Clark. He was a professor at Louisiana State University (1962-1990) and University of Idaho (1990-1996). Jack Nelson and Jerry Thomas taught, as a team, research methods at Louisiana State University and, as a result, wrote the first edition of this textbook. This was the beginning of a long, professional partnership and a deep, personal friendship. Jack had a quick and dry wit, often offered a helping hand, and was always a trusted friend. Among his numerous accomplishments and contributions to the field of research methods, he taught research methods for 35 years; conducted research; had over 80 publications, including authoring multiple textbooks; was an adviser on more than 100 doctoral dissertations and master’s theses combined; and served as editor of research publications. A fellow in the Research Consortium, he was also a member of American Alliance for Health, Physical Education, Recreation and Dance (now SHAPE America), American Educational Research Association (AERA), and American College of Sports Medicine (ACSM). He also served as president of the Association for Research, Administration, Professional Councils and Societies (now AAALF) and as vice president of AAHPERD. Jack was a remarkable kind of friend who was easygoing and could make you laugh until it hurt. He was very proud of his children and grandchildren and was loved by his family, friends, colleagues, and students. Jack had a spirit of adventure and was keen to taking a spur-of-the-moment trip on a houseboat or train and yet perfectly content with sitting on the screened-in porch, completing a New York Times crossword. Professionally, he brought to each project a combination of superior intellect, strong work ethic, integrity, abundant kindness, and humor. As our careers diverged, we found ourselves looking forward to subsequent editions of this book because the work brought us together again. It was an honor to have him in our lives; we are so thankful for all the wonderful and comforting memories of him. And so, nearly 40 years after its original publication, we’ve developed this latest edition without Jack. It is our sincerest hope that he would be proud of our work. Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com CONTENTS Preface Study Tips Acknowledgments PART I Overview of the Research Process 1 Introduction to Research in Physical Activity The Nature of Research Unscientific Versus Scientific Methods of Problem-Solving Alternative Models of Research Types of Research Overview of the Research Process Parts of a Thesis: A Reflection on the Steps in the Research Process Summary 2 Developing the Problem and Using the Literature Identifying the Research Problem Purpose of the Literature Review Basic Literature Search Strategies Steps in the Literature Search Summary 3 Presenting the Problem Choosing the Title Developing the Introduction: Background and Justification Stating the Research Purpose Presenting the Research Hypothesis Operationally Defining Terms Basic Assumptions, Delimitations, and Limitations Presenting the Significance of the Study in a Thesis or Dissertation Differences Between the Thesis and the Journal Article Summary 4 Formulating the Method How to Present Methods Why Planning the Methods Is Important Two Principles for Planning Experiments Describing Participants Selecting and Describing Instruments Describing Procedures Describing Design and Analysis Establishing Cause and Effect Interaction of Participants, Measurements, and Treatments Summary 5 Ethical Issues in Research and Scholarship Seven Areas of Research Misconduct Ethical Issues Regarding Copyright Model for Considering Scientific Misconduct Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Working With Faculty Protecting Human Participants Protecting Animal Subjects Summary PART II 6 Statistical and Measurement Concepts in Research Becoming Acquainted With Statistical Concepts Why We Need Statistics Use of Computers in Statistical Analysis Description and Inference Are Not Statistical Techniques Ways to Select a Sample Ways to Assign Participants to Groups Post Hoc Justifications Difficulty of Random Sampling and Assignment: Good Enough? Measures of Central Tendency and Variability Basic Concepts of Statistical Techniques Data for Use in the Remaining Statistical Chapters Summary 7 Statistical Issues in Research Planning and Evaluation Probability Hypothesis Testing Meaningfulness (Effect Size) Power Using Information in the Context of the Study Summary 8 Relationships of Variables What Correlational Research Investigates Understanding the Nature of Correlation What the Coefficient of Correlation Means Using Correlation for Prediction Partial Correlation Semipartial Correlation Procedures for Multiple Regression Logistic Regression Discriminant Function Analysis Moderators and Mediators Multivariate Forms of Correlation Summary 9 Differences Between Groups How Statistics Test Differences Types of t Tests Interpreting t Relationship of t and r Analysis of Variance Analysis of Covariance Experiment-Wise Error Rate Understanding Multivariate Techniques Summary 10 Nonparametric Techniques Chi Square: Testing the Observed Versus the Expected Procedures for Rank-Order Data Correlation Differences Between Groups Summary Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com 11 Measuring Research Variables Validity Reliability Methods of Establishing Reliability Intertester Reliability (Objectivity) Standard Error of Measurement Using Standard Scores to Compare Performance Measuring Movement Measuring Written Responses Measuring Affective Behavior Scales for Measurement Measuring Knowledge Item Response Theory Summary PART III Types of Research 12 Sociohistorical Process in Sport Studies Development of the Discipline Theory and Sport History Relationship Between Theory and Method Research Sources Research Topics Research Design Data Analysis and Interpretation Research Findings Exemplary Studies in Sport History Summary 13 Philosophical Research in Physical Activity Identifying the Purposes of Philosophical Research Philosophical Inquiry Continuum Locating a Research Problem Analyzing a Research Problem Summary 14 Research Synthesis Purpose of Research Synthesis Using Systematic Review for Research Synthesis Summary 15 Surveys Questionnaires Additional Considerations for Online Surveys Delphi Method Personal Interviews Normative Surveys Summary 16 Other Descriptive Research Methods Developmental Research Case Studies Observational Research Unobtrusive Research Techniques Correlational Research Summary 17 Physical Activity Epidemiology Research U.S. National Physical Activity Guidelines and Plan Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Observational Versus Experimental Research What Is Physical Activity Epidemiology? Definitions of Physical Activity and Its Components Assessment of Physical Activity Epidemiological Study Designs Reading and Interpreting a Physical Activity Epidemiological Study Summary 18 Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Research Sources of Invalidity Threats to Internal Validity Threats to External Validity Controlling Threats to Internal Validity Controlling Threats to External Validity Types of Designs Summary 19 Qualitative Research Procedures in Qualitative Research Data Analysis Concluding Remarks Summary 20 Mixed-Methods Research Combining Quantitative and Qualitative Methods Designing Mixed-Methods Research Issues in Mixed-Methods Research Examples of Mixed-Methods Research Summary PART IV Writing the Research Report 21 Completing the Research Process Research Proposal Thesis and Dissertation Proposals Advisor and Dissertation Committee The Good Scholar Must Research and Write Scientific Writing First Things Are Sometimes Best Done Last Developing a Good Introduction Describing the Methods The Proposal Process Preparing and Presenting Qualitative Research Proposals Writing Proposals for Granting Agencies Submitting Internal Proposals Completing Your Thesis or Dissertation Results and Discussion Handling Multiple Experiments in a Single Report Using Tables and Figures Summary 22 Ways of Reporting Research Basic Writing Guidelines A Brief Word About Acknowledgments Thesis and Dissertation Format: Traditional Versus Journal Helpful Hints for Successful Journal Writing Revising Research Papers Writing Abstracts Making Oral and Poster Presentations Summary Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Appendix References Author Index Subject Index About the Authors Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com PREFACE The first edition was published in 1985 and was titled Introduction to Research in Health, Physical Education, Recreation and Dance. Publishing the eighth edition is rewarding and surprising. In 1985, Human Kinetics was a new publisher. We could not have guessed that we would do this many editions nor did we expect the field to evolve so dramatically. The second edition recognized the evolving field with a title to represent its breadth, Research in Physical Activity. The title was inclusive of the field: sport science, exercise science, kinesiology, physical education, and so forth. In 1985, we could not have predicted that physical activity would become a key factor in public health. By 1990, however, it was clear that physical activity was becoming important and of interest to those outside sports, exercise, dance, and physical education. Research in physical activity was published in the parent journals—for example, Developmental Psychology, Physiology—and in other highly regarded journals such as Journal of the American Medical Association. Scholars in physical activity were recognized for their high-quality work. Further, Human Kinetics was growing. We take this opportunity to thank all the people who have used this book over the years. Once again, we hope that you have learned about research methods in the study of physical activity such that you will be an informed consumer of research and a knowledgeable scholar. Maybe you have even enjoyed the humorous stories, jokes, and pictures that we have included to enliven the reading. We also thank the reviewers for their helpful comments and suggestions, which we have tried to address in this edition. When we read reviews, we feel as Day (1983, p. xi) did when he read that a reviewer described his book as both good and original, but then went on to say that “the part that is good is not original, and the part that is original is not good.” We are also delighted that many of you in other English-speaking countries have also used this book. In addition, we appreciate that earlier editions have been translated into Chinese (twice), Greek, Korean, Italian, Japanese, Spanish, and Portuguese. Dr. Stephen Silverman joined us as a coauthor on the fifth edition and, in spite of our sense of humor, agreed to continue on the subsequent editions. Dr. Silverman is a well-known scholar and methodologist in physical education pedagogy and is a former editor-in-chief of Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. Joining the team on this edition are Dr. Philip E. Martin and Dr. Jennifer L. Etnier. Dr. Martin is a professor and the chair emeritus at Iowa State University and a biomechanist with an impressive scholarly record. Dr. Etnier is a distinguished professor and department chair at the University of North Carolina–Greensboro. She is an exercise psychologist who has led research on Alzheimer’s disease as well as national professional organizations in sport psychology and kinesiology, all while producing a stellar scholarly record. All members of this team have taught research methods. The main use of this text still appears to be in the first graduate-level research methods courses, although it is also being used in undergraduate research methods courses and as a resource for those engaged in research planning and analysis. Our use of the term physical activity in the book title is meant to convey the broadly conceived field of study often labeled kinesiology, exercise science, exercise and sport science, human movement, sport studies, or physical education, as well as related fields such as physical therapy, rehabilitation, and occupational therapy. We hope that everyone who reads, understands, plans, carries out, writes, or presents research will find the book a useful tool to enhance their efforts. This eighth edition retains the basic organization of the seventh edition, as follows: • Part I is an overview of the research process, including developing the problem, using the literature, preparing a research plan, and understanding ethical issues in research and writing. • Part II introduces statistical and measurement issues in research, including statistical descriptions, power, interrelationships of variables, differences between groups, nonparametric procedures, and measurement issues in research. • Part III presents the types of, or approaches to, research, including historical, philosophical, research synthesis, survey, descriptive, epidemiological, experimental, qualitative, and mixed methods. • Part IV will help you complete the research process, which includes writing the results and discussion, organizing the research paper, developing good figures and tables, and presenting research in written and oral forms. • The appendix includes statistical tables. Instructors using this text in their courses will find an instructor guide, test package, chapter quizzes, presentation package, and image bank in HKPropel. The image bank includes most of the art, tables, and example elements from the text, which can be used to create custom presentations. The instructor guide includes chapter overviews, sample course syllabuses, supplemental class activities, and student handouts. The test bank includes over 600 questions. The chapter Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com quizzes contain ready-made quizzes, with about 10 questions per chapter drawn from the test package, to assess student comprehension of the most important concepts in each chapter. Although the format of the book remains similar to that of the seventh edition, we have made a number of changes that we hope improve and update the text. Following is a short review of the changes in this eighth edition: • Part I: Overview of the Research Process. Each chapter includes minor revisions that reflect updated information and more recent reports. Chapter 1 includes examples that more broadly represent physical activity research and the inclusion of cases studies. We have again made a significant revision to chapter 2 about using library techniques by adding much more on electronic searches. In addition, chapter 5 on ethical issues has been updated with particular attention to procedures for the use of human and animal subjects with expanded focus on security. • Part II: Statistical and Measurement Concepts in Research. We strive in each edition to increase the relevance of the examples and provide easy-to-understand calculations for basic statistics. We have reduced the examples of hand calculations and formulas and replaced them with sample output from the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences. We have included 2019 player performance data for outcome and skill variables from the Professional Golfers Association website as examples for analysis in the statistical chapters. In chapter 6, we have added a more in-depth introduction to the need for and types and uses of statistics. More information on sampling, as well as greater detail on the stem-and-leaf technique, has been included. Chapters 7, 8, and 9 have been reorganized with some information shifted among the chapters in this section. Along with the chapter examples, the learning activities in the instructor guide should help students grasp the fundamentals of statistical techniques. We continue to use a unified approach to parametric and nonparametric techniques. • Part III: Types of Research. We have continued our use of expert authors to present coherent views of sociohistorical research (David Wiggins and Daniel Mason), philosophical research (Tim Elcombe and R. Scott Kretchmar), and epidemiological research in physical activity (Duck-chul Lee and Angelique Brellenthin). These three types of research are outside our expertise, and we wanted them presented by expert scholars. Chapter 14 has been expanded to include systematic reviews. In addition, we have made minor revisions and updates to all the other chapters in this part. • Part IV: Writing the Research Report. The two chapters in this section remain essentially the same, with changes and updates. Chapter 21 includes these headings: Thesis and Dissertation Proposals, Advisor and Dissertation Committee, The Good Scholar Must Research and Write, Scientific Writing, and First Things Are Sometimes Best Done Last. Chapter 22 has greater focus on all aspects of presenting research results. As we have said in each edition, we are grateful for the help of our friends, both for help that we acknowledge in various places in the book and for help in other places where we have inadvertently taken an idea without giving credit. After the passage of time, one can no longer remember who originated what idea. After the passage of even more time, it seems to me that all of the really good ideas originated with me, a proposition which I know is indefensible. (Day, 1983, p. xv) We believe that this book provides the necessary information for both the consumer and the producer of research. Although no amount of knowledge about the tools of research can replace expertise in the content area, good scholars of physical activity cannot function apart from the effective use of research tools. Researchers, teachers, clinicians, technicians, health workers, exercise leaders, sport managers, athletic counselors, and coaches need to understand the research process. If they do not, they are forced to accept information at face value or on the recommendation of others. Neither is necessarily bad, but the ability to evaluate and reach a valid conclusion based on data, method, and logic is the mark of a professional. Inserted into some chapters are humorous stories, anecdotes, sketches, laws, and corollaries. These are intended to make a point and enliven the reading without distracting from the content. Research processes are not mysterious events that graduate students should fear. To the contrary, they are useful tools that every academic and professional should have access to; they are, in fact, the very basis on which we make competent decisions. Jerry R. Thomas Philip E. Martin Jennifer L. Etnier Stephen J. Silverman Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com STUDY TIPS Dear Student of Research Methods: We want you to learn the material here, and most of you are learning it in a classroom setting as well as by reading the textbook. From many years of teaching research methods, we have arrived at the following recommendations: 1. Attend and participate in class—90% of life is showing up! 2. Take notes in class—writing it down is an effective way to learn. 3. Read the assigned materials before class—duh! 4. Plan for and ask at least one question in every class. 5. At the conclusion of class, recall everything you can about the class content—research shows this enhances learning. 6. Develop and work with a study group. 7. Prepare for exams and tests—do not cram; study over several days. 8. Use campus resources to improve learning—library, computer, the Internet. 9. Visit often with your professor—those of us teaching research methods are likable folks! The following list will help you determine your readiness to be a student of research methods. Score one point for each of the following statements that describes you: Your library carrel is better decorated than is your apartment. You have taken a scholarly article to a bar or coffee shop. You rate coffee shops on the availability of Wi-Fi and outlets for your electronic devices. You have discussed academic matters at a sport event. You actually have a preference between microfilm and microfiche. You always read the reference lists in research articles. You think that the sorority sweatshirt Greek letters are a statistical formula. You need to explain to children why you are in the 20th grade. You refer to stories as “Snow White et al.” You wonder how to cite talking to yourself in APA style. Scoring Scale 5 or 6—definitely ready to be a student in research methods 7 or 8—probably a master’s student 9 or 10—probably a doctoral student Humorously yours, Professors of Research Methods Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com ACKNOWLEDGMENTS As with any work, numerous people contributed to this book, and we want to recognize them. Many are former students and colleagues who have said or done things that better developed our ideas as expressed in these pages. Also, a number of faculty members who have used previous editions have either written reviews or made suggestions that have improved the book. Although we cannot list or even recall all these contributions, we do know that you made them, and we thank all of you. In particular, Scott Kretchmar, Tim Elcombe, David Wiggins, Daniel Mason, Duck-chul Lee and Angelique G. Brellenthin made invaluable contributions with their chapters on research methods in the areas of philosophy, history, and exercise epidemiology, which are areas we simply could not write about effectively. Finally, we thank the staff at Human Kinetics—in particular Diana Vincer and Melissa Zavala, for their support and contributions. They have sharpened our thinking and improved our writing. Jerry R. Thomas Philip E. Martin Jennifer L. Etnier Stephen J. Silverman Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com PART I Overview of the Research Process The researches of many have thrown much darkness on the subject and if they continue, soon we shall know nothing at all about it. —Attributed to Mark Twain Part I provides an overall perspective of the research process. The introductory chapter defines and reviews the types of research done in physical activity and provides some examples. We define science as “systematic inquiry,” and we discuss the steps in the scientific method. This logical method answers the following four questions (Day, 1983, p. 4), which constitute the parts of a typical thesis, dissertation, or research report: 1. What was the problem? Your answer is the introduction. 2. How did you study the problem? Your answer is the materials and methods. 3. What did you find? Your answer is the results. 4. What do these findings mean? Your answer is the discussion. We also present alternative approaches for doing research relative to a more philosophical discussion of science and ways of knowing. In particular, we address qualitative research, the use of field studies, and methods of introspection as strategies for answering research questions instead of relying on the traditional scientific paradigm as the only approach to research problems. Chapter 2 suggests ways of developing a problem and using the literature to clarify the research problem, specify hypotheses, and develop the methodology. In particular, we emphasize the use of new electronic technology for searching, reading, analyzing, synthesizing, organizing, and writing literature reviews. The next two chapters in part I present the format of the research proposal with examples. This information is typically required of the master’s or doctoral student before collecting data for the thesis or dissertation. Chapter 3 addresses defining and delimiting the research problem, including the introduction, statement of the problem, research hypotheses, operational definitions, assumptions and limitations, and significance. Information is provided for both quantitative and qualitative approaches. Chapter 4 covers methodology, or how to do the research, using either quantitative or qualitative methods. Included are the topics of participant selection, instrumentation or apparatuses, procedures, and design and analysis. We emphasize the value of pilot work conducted before the research and how cause and effect may be established. Chapter 5 discusses ethical issues in research and scholarship. We include information on misconduct in science; security of data, ethical considerations in research writing, collaborative work with advisors, and copyright; and the use of humans and animals in research. When you have completed part I, you should have a better understanding of the research process. Then comes the tricky part: learning all the details. We consider these details in part II (Statistical and Measurement Concepts in Research), part III (Types of Research), and part IV (Writing the Research Report). Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com 1 Introduction to Research in Physical Activity Everything that can be invented has been invented. —Charles H. Duell, Commissioner, U.S. Office of Patents, 1899 To each person, the word research conjures up a different picture. One might think of searching the Internet or going to the library; another might visualize a lab filled with test tubes, vials, and perhaps little, white rats. Therefore, as we begin a text on the subject, we must establish a common understanding of research. In this chapter, we introduce you to the nature of research. We do this by discussing methods of problem-solving and types of research. We explain the research process and relate it to the parts of a thesis. By the time you reach the end of chapter 1, you should understand what research really involves. The Nature of Research The object of research is to determine how things are as compared to how they might be. To achieve this, research implies a careful and systematic means of solving problems and involves the following five characteristics (Tuckman, 1978): • Systematic. Problem-solving begins with and is accomplished by identifying and labeling variables. Research is then designed to test the relationships of these variables. Data are collected that, when related to the variables, allow the evaluation of the problem and hypotheses. • Logical. Examination of the procedures used in the research process allows researchers to evaluate the conclusions they’ve drawn. • Empirical. Researchers collect data on which to base decisions. • Reductive. Researchers take individual events (data) and use them to establish general relationships. • Replicable. The research process is recorded, enabling others to test the findings by repeating the research or to build future research on previous results. Problems to be solved come from many sources and can entail resolving controversial issues, testing theories, and trying to improve present practices. For example, a popular topic of concern is obesity and the methods for losing weight. Suppose we want to investigate this issue by comparing the effectiveness of two exercise programs in reducing body fat in people who are overweight. Since we know that caloric expenditure can contribute to a reduction in body fat, we will try to find out which program does this better under specified conditions. Note: Our approach here is to give a simple, concise overview of a research study. We do not intend it to be a model of originality or sophistication. This study is an example of applied research. Rather than try to measure the calories expended and so on, we approach it strictly from a programmatic standpoint. Let’s say we operate a health club and offer aerobic dance and jogging classes for people who want to lose weight. Our research question would be: Which program is more effective in reducing fat? applied research—A type of research that has direct value to practitioners but limits researchers’ control over the research setting. Suppose we have a pool of participants to draw from and can randomly assign two-thirds of them to the two exercise programs and one-third to a control group. We have their scout’s honor that no one is drastically dieting or engaging in any other strenuous activities for the duration of the study. Both classes are one hour long and held five times a week for 10 weeks. The same enthusiastic and immensely qualified instructor teaches both classes. Our method for measuring body composition is by using a portable bioelectric impedance system. Of course, we could use other measures, such as hydrostatic weighing or DXA scanning (or some other estimate of adiposity). In any case, we can defend our measures as valid and reliable indicators of adiposity, and bioelectric impedance is a functional field measure. We measure all the participants, including those in the control group, at the beginning and the end of the 10-week period. During the study, we try to ensure that the two programs are similar in procedural aspects, such as motivational techniques and the aesthetics of the surroundings. In other words, we do not favor one group by cheering them on and not encouraging the other, nor do we have one group exercise in an air-conditioned, cheerful, and healthful facility while the other has to sweat it out in a dingy room or a parking lot. We make the programs as similar as possible in every respect except the experimental treatments: The control group does not engage in any regular exercise. bioelectric impedance—Bioelectric impedance analysis is a non-invasive technique in which a very small current is passed through the body between electrodes attached to the skin at two distant locations (e.g., right wrist and left ankle). Because the body’s tissues impede or resist current flow, a measure of the impedance allows for the estimation of total body water, fat mass, and fat-free mass. hydrostatic weighing—A technique for measuring body composition in which body density is estimated from the ratio of a person’s weight in air and the loss of weight underwater. Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com DXA scanning—Dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry (DXA, or DEXA) uses X-ray beams at two different energies and measures the differential attenuation of the beams by fat, muscle, and bone tissues in scanned regions to predict body composition. After we have measured all the participants on our criterion of adiposity at the end of the 10-week program, we are ready to analyze our data. We want to see how much change in fat mass has occurred and whether differences have occurred between the two types of exercise. Because we are dealing with samples of people (from a whole universe of similar people), we need to use some type of statistics to establish how confident we can be in our results. In other words, we need to determine the significance of our results. Suppose the mean (average) scores for the groups are as follows: Aerobic dance: −3.1% Jogging: −3.7% Control: +1.1% These hypothetical values represent the average changes in percent body fat for each group. The two experimental groups lost fat, but the control group actually showed an increase over the 10-week period. Basic research and applied research can be thought of as two ends of a continuum. Basic research addresses theoretical problems, often under highly controlled conditions in laboratory settings, and has limited direct application. Applied research addresses immediate problems, often in less-controlled, real-world settings, and is more closely linked to application than basic research. We decide to use the statistical technique of analysis of variance. We find a significant F ratio, indicating that significant differences exist between the three groups. Using a follow-up test procedure, we discover that both exercise groups significantly differ from the control group. But we find only a slight difference between the aerobic dance and the jogging groups. (Many of you may not have the foggiest idea what we are talking about with the statistical terms F ratio and significance, but do not worry about it! All that is explained later. This book is directly concerned with those kinds of concepts.) We conclude from our study that over a course of 10 weeks, both aerobic dance and jogging are effective (and, apparently, equally so) in reducing adiposity in people (such as those in our study). Although these results are reasonable, remember that this is a hypothetical scenario. We could also pretend that this study was published in a prestigious journal and that we won the Nobel Prize. Research Continuum Research in our field can be placed on a continuum that has basic research at one extreme and applied research at the opposite extreme. The research extremes are generally associated with certain characteristics. Basic research usually deals with theoretical problems. It uses the laboratory as the setting, sometimes uses animals as subjects, carefully controls conditions, and produces results that have limited direct application. At the other extreme, applied research tends to address immediate problems. It is conducted in real-world settings, uses human participants, and involves limited control over the research setting. Applied research provides practitioners with results that have direct value. basic research—A type of research that may have limited direct application but allows researchers to have careful control of the conditions. Christina (1989) suggested that basic and applied forms of research are useful in informing each other as to future research directions. Table 1.1 demonstrates how research problems in motor learning might vary along a continuum from basic to applied depending on their goals and approaches. To some extent, the strengths of applied research are the weaknesses of basic research and vice versa. Considerable controversy exists in the literature on social science (e.g., Creswell, 2009; Jewczyn, 2013) and physical activity (e.g., Christina, 1989) about whether research should be more basic or more applied. This issue, labeled ecological validity, deals with two concerns: Is the research setting perceived by the research participant in the way intended by the experimenter? Does the setting have enough of the real-world characteristics to allow generalizing to reality? ecological validity—The extent to which research emulates the real world. TABLE 1.1 Levels of Relevance of Motor Learning Research for Finding Solutions to Practical Problems in Sport Ultimate goal Level 1 Least direct relevance Basic research Level 2 Moderate direct relevance Applied research Level 3 Most direct relevance Applied research Develop theory-based knowledge appropriate for understanding motor learning in general with no requirement to demonstrate its relevance for solving practical problems Develop theory-based knowledge appropriate for understanding the learning of sport skills in sport settings with no requirement to find immediate solutions to learning problems in sport Find immediate solutions to learning problems in sport with no requirement to demonstrate, or develop theory-based knowledge at either level 1 or level 2 Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Get Complete eBook Download link Below for Instant Download: https://browsegrades.net/documents/286751/ebook-payment-link-forinstant-download-after-payment Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Level 1 Least direct relevance Basic research Level 2 Moderate direct relevance Applied research Level 3 Most direct relevance Applied research Test hypotheses in a laboratory Test hypotheses in a sport setting or in Test solutions to specific learning setting using experimenter-designed an applied laboratory setting using sport problems in sport in the settings Main approach motor tasks skills or motor tasks that have properties of described under the applied those skills research at level 2 From R.W. Christina, “Whatever happened to applied research in motor learning?” in Future Directions in Exercise and Sport Science Research, edited by J.S. Skinner et al. (Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1989). By permission of Robert W. Christina. Most research incorporates elements of both basic and applied research to some degree. We believe that systematic efforts are needed in the study of physical activity to produce research that moves back and forth across Christina’s (1989) levels of research (table 1.1). Excellent summaries of this type of research and the accumulated knowledge are provided in three edited volumes representing exercise physiology, exercise and sport psychology, and motor control: Physical Activity and Health (Bouchard, Blair, & Haskell, 2012), Psychobiology of Physical Activity (Acevedo & Ekkekakis, 2006), and Motor Control: Theories, Experiments, and Applications (Danion & Latash, 2011). Experts prepared each chapter in these books to summarize theories as well as to present basic and applied research about areas related to exercise physiology, exercise and sport psychology, and motor control. The novice researcher would do well to read several of these chapters as examples of how knowledge is developed and accumulated in the study of physical activity. We need more efforts to produce a related body of knowledge in the study of physical activity. Although the research base has grown tremendously in our field over the past 40 years, much remains to be done. There is a great need to prepare proficient consumers and producers of research in kinesiology. To be proficient, people must thoroughly understand the appropriate knowledge base (biomechanics, exercise physiology, exercise psychology, motor control, pedagogy, and the social and biological sciences) as well as research methods (qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods). In this book, we attempt to explain the tools necessary for consuming and producing research. Many of the same methods are used in the various areas of kinesiology (as well as in the fields of psychology, sociology, education, and physiology). Quality research efforts always involve some or all of the following actions: Identifying and delimiting a problem Searching, reviewing, critically analyzing, integrating, and effectively summarizing relevant literature Specifying and defining testable hypotheses Designing the research to test the hypotheses Selecting, describing, testing, and treating the participants Analyzing and reporting the results Discussing the meaning and implications of the findings Practicality and Accessibility We recognize that not everyone is a researcher. Many people in kinesiology have little interest in research per se. In fact, some have a decided aversion to it. The public at large sometimes may view researchers as people with eccentricities who deal with “insignificant” problems and are out of touch with the real world. In an informative yet entertaining book on writing scientific papers, Gastel and Day (2016, pp. 213) related the story about two men who, while riding in a hot-air balloon, encountered some cloud coverage and lost their way. When they finally descended, they did not recognize the terrain and had not the faintest idea where they were. It so happened that they were drifting over the grounds of one of our more famous scientific research institutes. When the balloonists saw a man walking alongside a road, one of them called out, “Hey, mister, where are we?” The man looked up, took in the situation, and after a few moments of reflection said, “You’re in a hot-air balloon.” One balloonist turned to the other and said, “I’ll bet that man is a researcher.” The other balloonist asked, “What makes you think so?” The first replied, “His answer is perfectly accurate— and totally useless.” All kidding aside, the need for research in any profession cannot be denied. After all, one of the primary distinctions between a discipline or profession and a trade is that the trade deals only with how to do something, whereas the discipline or profession concerns itself not only with how but also with why something should be done in a certain manner (and why it should even be done at all). But although most people in a discipline or profession recognize the need for research, most do not read research results. This situation is not unique to our field. It has been reported that only 1% of chemists read research publications, that fewer than 7% of psychologists read psychological research journals, and so on. The big question is why? Our best guess is that most professionals believe that the findings either are not practical enough or do not directly pertain to their work, rendering the act of reading such publications unnecessary. Another reason practitioners give for not reading research publications is the literature is indecipherable: The language is too technical, and the terminology is unfamiliar and confusing. This complaint is valid, but we could Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com argue that if the professional preparation programs were more scientifically oriented, the problem would diminish. Nevertheless, the research literature is extremely difficult for most people to understand. We must continue efforts to decrease this communication gap. Reading Research Someone once said (facetiously) that scientific papers are meant not to be read but to be published. Unfortunately, we find considerable truth in this observation. We writers are often guilty of trying to use language to dazzle the reader and perhaps to give the impression that our subject matter is more esoteric than it really is. We tend to write for the benefit of a rather small audience of readers—that is, other researchers in our field. We have the problem of jargon, of course (Plaven-Sigray et al., 2017). In any field, whether it is physics, football, or cake baking, jargon confounds the outsider. The use of jargon serves as a kind of shorthand. It provides meaning to the people within the field because everyone uses those truncations in the same context. Research literature is famous for using a three-dollar word when a nickel word would do. As Gastel and Day (2016) asked, “who would use the three-letter word now instead of the elegant expression at this point in time” (p. 209)? Researchers never do anything, they perform it; they never start, they initiate; and they terminate instead of end. Gastel and Day further remarked that “an occasional author will slip and use the word drug, but most will salivate like Pavlov’s dogs in anticipation of using chemotherapeutic agent” (p. 209). The need to bridge the gap between the researcher and the practitioner has been recognized for years. For example, the Translational Journal of the American College of Sports Medicine was created to communicate implications of basic, clinical, and policy research to practitioners. The website for the American Kinesiology Association (www .americankinesiology.org) regularly has a section on applied research. Yet despite these and other attempts to bridge the gap between researchers and practitioners, the gap is still imposing. It goes without saying that if you are not knowledgeable about the subject matter, you cannot read the research literature. Conversely, if you know the subject matter, you can probably wade through the researcher’s jargon more effectively. For example, if you know baseball and the researcher is recommending that by shortening the radius, the hitter can increase the angular velocity, you can figure out that the researcher means to choke up on the bat. One of the big stumbling blocks is the statistical analysis part of research reports. Even the most ardent seeker of knowledge can be turned off by such descriptions as this: “The tetrachoric correlations among the test variables were subjected to a centroid factor analysis, and orthogonal rotations of the primary axes were accomplished by Zimmerman’s graphical method until simple structure and positive manifold were closely approximated.” Please note that we are not criticizing the authors for such descriptions, because reviewers and editors usually require them. We are just acknowledging that statistical analysis is frightening to someone who is trying to read a research article and does not know a factor analysis from a plank exercise. How to Read Research Despite all the hurdles that loom in a practitioner’s path when reading research, we contend that you can read and profit from the research literature even if you are not well grounded in research techniques and statistical analysis. We offer the following suggestions on reading the research literature: • Become familiar with a few publications that contain pertinent research in your field. You might get some help on choosing the publications from a professor or librarian. • Learn to use search tools (e.g., Web of Science, Pubmed, Scopus) for identifying research literature relevant to your interests. • Read only studies that are of interest to you. This point may sound too trite to mention, but some people feel obligated to wade through every article. • Read as a practitioner would. Do not look for eternal truths. Look for ideas and indications. No study is proof of anything. Only when it has been verified repeatedly does it constitute knowledge. • Read the abstract first. This saves time by helping you determine whether you wish to read the whole thing. If you are still interested, then you can read the study to gain a better understanding of the objectives, hypotheses, methods, and interpretations, but do not get bogged down with details. • Do not be too concerned about statistical significance. Understanding the concept of significance certainly helps, but a little common sense serves you about as well as knowing the difference between the 0.02 and the 0.01 levels, or a one-tailed test versus a two-tailed test. Think in terms of meaningfulness. For example, if two regimens of resistance training result in an average difference in strength improvement of 0.5%, does it matter whether the difference is significant? On the other hand, if a big difference is present but not significant, further investigation is warranted, especially if the study involved a small number of participants. Knowing the concepts of the types of statistical analysis is certainly helpful, but it is not crucial to being able to read a study. Just skip that part. • Be critical but objective. You can usually assume that a national research journal selects studies for publication by the jury method. Two or three qualified people read and judge the relevance of the problem, the validity and reliability of the procedures, the efficacy of the experimental design, and the appropriateness of the statistical analysis. Certainly, Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com some studies are published that should not be. Yet if you are not an expert in research, you do not need to be suspicious about the scientific worth of a study that appears in a recognized journal. If it is too far removed from any practical application to your situation, do not read it. You will find that the more you read, the more you will understand, simply because you enhance your familiarity with the language and the methodology, like the person who was thrilled to learn they had been speaking prose all their life. An Example of Practical Research To illustrate our research consumer suggestions, consider the following fictional account of Sonjia Roundball, a newly trained physical education teacher and coach (Nelson, 1988). In a moment of curiosity, Coach Roundball began browsing the Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, which had been left in her car by a graduate student friend. An article titled “The Effects of a Season of Basketball on the Cardiorespiratory Responses of High School Girls” immediately caught her attention. In its introductory passages, the article stated that only a negligible amount of information was available on the specific physiological changes in girls that result from sport participation. The article cited a few studies on swimmers and other sport participants, and the rather broad takeaway of these studies was that the female athletes possessed higher levels of cardiorespiratory fitness than nonathletes. The author emphasized that no studies had tried to detect changes in girls’ fitness during a season of basketball. The article’s next section described methods used in one particular study as well as noting the length of the season, numbers of games and practices (including their lengths), and a breakdown of time devoted to drills, scrimmages, and individual practice. Participants were placed into two groups. The first group comprised 12 girls who played on a high school basketball team. The control group was made up of 14 nonplayers who took physical education classes and had academic and activity schedules similar to the basketball players. All participants in the study were tested at the start and end of the season for maximal oxygen consumption and various other physiological measurements dealing with ventilation, heart rate, and blood pressure. Since these were concepts Coach Roundball remembered from her exercise physiology course several years earlier, she was willing to accept them as appropriate indicators of cardiorespiratory fitness. Coach Roundball was inexperienced with interpreting the kinds of results that were presented in tables, so she was inclined to trust the author’s claims. No significant increases in any of the cardiorespiratory measures from the pre- and postseason tests for either group were detected, which raised the first red flag. Surely, a strenuous sport such as basketball should produce improvements in fitness. The coach continued to read with more skepticism. The author reported that the basketball players had higher values of maximal oxygen consumption than the control group did at both the beginning and the end of the season. The discussion mentioned that the values were actually higher than similar values in other studies. So what? Coach Roundball thought. Additionally, the author stated that the number of participants was small and some changes may not have been detected. Despite these and other potentially flawed aspects of the study, the author still concluded that the training program used in this study was not strenuous enough to induce significant improvement in cardiorespiratory fitness. discussion—The chapter or section of a research report that explains what the results mean. Coach Roundball understood the limitations of the single study. Nevertheless, the practice schedule and general practice routines used in the study were similar to her own. The article’s references section listed three studies from a journal she had never read called Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. The following weekend, she visited the university library and found the journal’s latest issue, which happened to have an article on the conditioning effects of swimming on college-age women. Although a different sport and age group, Coach Roundball reasoned that this article could contain useful information that might resolve some questions about the earlier article she had read. The Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise article cited, of all things, a recent study on aerobic capacity, heart rate, and energy cost during a season of girls’ basketball. The coach quickly located that study and was pleasantly surprised at how she could read this study with ease now that she was more familiar with the terminology and general organization of the research literature. Coincidentally, this study also reported no improvement in aerobic capacity during the season. While monitoring heart rates during games by telemetry, researchers rarely observed heart rates above 170 beats per minute (bpm). They concluded that the practice sessions were apparently too moderate in intensity and that the training should be structured to meet both the skill and the fitness demands of the sport. Coach Roundball returned to her school determined to take a more scientific approach to her basketball program. To start, she had one of her managers chart the number of minutes players were actually engaged in movement in the practice sessions. She also had the players take their pulses at various intervals throughout the sessions. She was surprised to find that the heart rates rarely surpassed 130 bpm. As an outgrowth of her recent literature search, she recalled that an intensity threshold would be necessary to bring about improvement in cardiorespiratory fitness. She knew that for this age group, a heart rate of about 160 bpm was needed to provide a significant training effect. By adjusting practice sessions to include more conditioning drills and make the scrimmages more intensive and game-like, Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Coach Roundball’s team would go on to have an overall stronger competitive advantage in district and state championships. Summarizing the Nature of Research Thomas Huxley, the famous British scientist who promoted Darwin’s theory of evolution, wrote that science is simply common sense at its best. However, the status science holds is based on findings being correct most of the time as well as finding the instances in which reported findings are not correct. Science is systematic; if you and I do the same experiment at the same time or two years apart, we should get the same answer. Unfortunately, several recently reported attempts to replicate earlier studies have not been successful. Of the more than two million scholarly papers published in journals each year, an important question might be, How many reported findings are wrong? science—A process of careful and systematic inquiry. Having said that about science, discovery can be rewarding, whether that discovery is research that applies to and can improve your situation or is simply new knowledge obtained while researching your thesis or dissertation. We must work against the common misperception that research is some dark and mysterious realm inhabited by impractical people who speak and write in baffling terms. In general, research should be viewed for what it is: a methodic approach for solving problems. We firmly believe that practitioners can read research literature. Our intent with this book is to help facilitate that process of turning our readers into research consumers. Unscientific Versus Scientific Methods of Problem-Solving Although the term research has many definitions, nearly all characterize research activity as some type of structured problem-solving. The word structured refers to the fact that a number of research techniques can be used as long as they are considered acceptable by scholars in the field. Thus, research is concerned with problem-solving, which then may lead to new knowledge. The problem-solving process involves several steps whereby the problem is developed, defined, and delimited; hypotheses are formulated; methods are planned and employed to gather and analyze data; and the results are interpreted with regard to the acceptance or rejection of the hypotheses. These steps are often referred to as the scientific method of problem-solving. The steps also constitute the chapters, or sections, of the research paper, thesis, or dissertation. Consequently, we devote much of this text to the specific ways these steps are accomplished. scientific method of problem-solving—A method of solving problems that uses the following steps in this order: (a) define and delimit the problem; (b) form a hypothesis; (c) gather data; (d) analyze the data; and (e) interpret the results. Some Unscientific Methods of Problem-Solving Before we go into more detail concerning the scientific method of problem-solving, we should recognize some other ways by which humankind has acquired knowledge. All of us have used these methods, so they are recognizable. Helmstadter (1970) labeled the methods tenacity, intuition, authority, the rationalistic method, and the empirical method. Tenacity People are prone to clinging to certain beliefs despite a lack of supporting evidence. Our superstitions are good examples of the method called tenacity. Coaches and athletes are notoriously superstitious. A coach may wear a particular sport coat, hat, tie, or pair of shoes because the team won the last time he wore it. Athletes frequently have a set pattern that they consider lucky for dressing, warming up, or entering the stadium. Although they acknowledge no logical relationship between the game’s outcome and their particular routine, they are afraid to break the pattern. tenacity—An unscientific method of problem-solving in which people cling to certain beliefs regardless of a lack of supporting evidence. For example, take the man who believed that black cats bring bad luck. One night while he was returning to his ranch, a black cat started to cross the road. The man swerved off the road to keep the cat from crossing in front of him and hit a hard bump that caused the headlights to turn off. Unable to see the black cat in the dark, he sped frantically over rocks, mounds, and holes until he came to a sudden stop in a ravine and wrecked his car. Of course, this episode just confirmed his staunch belief that black cats do indeed bring bad luck. Obviously, tenacity has no place in science. It is the least reliable source of knowledge. Intuition Intuitive knowledge is sometimes considered common sense or self-evident. Many self-evident truths, however, often turn out to be false. That the earth is flat is a classic example of the intuitively obvious; that the sun revolves around the earth was once self-evident; that no one could run a mile (1.6 km) in less than four minutes once was self-evident. Furthermore, for anyone to shot-put more than 70 ft (21 m) or pole-vault more than 18 ft (5.5 m), or for a woman to run distances over 0.5 mi (0.8 km), such feats were impossible. One fundamental tenet of science is that we must be ever cognizant of the importance of substantiating our convictions with factual evidence. Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Authority Reference to some authority has long been used as a source of knowledge. Although this approach is not necessarily invalid, it does depend on the authority and the rigidity of adherence. But the appeal to authority has been carried to absurd lengths. Even personal observations and experiences have been deemed unacceptable when they dispute authority. For example, people purportedly refused to look through Galileo’s telescope when he disputed Ptolemy’s explanation of the world and the heavens. Galileo was later jailed and forced to recant his beliefs. Bruno also rejected Ptolemy’s theory and was burned at the stake. (Scholars read and believed Ptolemy’s book on astrology and astronomy for 1,200 years after his death!) In 1543, Vesalius wrote a book on anatomy, much of which is still considered correct today. But because his work clashed with Galen’s theories, he met with such ridicule that he gave up his study of anatomy. Perhaps the most crucial aspect of the appeal to authority as a means of obtaining knowledge is the right to question and to accept or reject the information. Furthermore, the authority’s qualifications and the methods by which the authority acquired the knowledge also determine the validity of this source of information. Rationalistic Method In the rationalistic method, we derive knowledge through reasoning. A good example is the following classic syllogism: All men are mortal (major premise). The emperor is a man (minor premise). Therefore, the emperor is mortal (conclusion). Although you probably would not argue with this reasoning, the key to this method is the truth of the premises and their relationship to each other, as shown in the following example: Basketball players are tall. Tom Thumb is a basketball player. Therefore, Tom Thumb is tall. In this case, however, Tom is very short. The conclusion is trustworthy only if it is derived from premises (assumptions) that are true. Also, the premises may not in fact be premises but rather descriptions of events or statements of fact. The statements are not connected in a cause-and-effect manner. Consider the following example: There is a positive correlation between shoe size and mathematics performance among elementary school children. In other words, children with large shoe sizes do well in math. Herman is in elementary school and wears large shoes. Therefore, Herman is good in mathematics. Of course, in the first statement, the factor common to both mathematics achievement and shoe size is age. Older children tend to be bigger and thus have bigger feet than younger children. Older children also have higher achievement scores in mathematics, but there is no cause-and-effect relationship. You must always be aware of this when dealing with correlation. Reasoning is fundamental in the scientific method of problem-solving, but it cannot be used by itself to arrive at knowledge. Empirical Method The word empirical denotes experience and the gathering of data. Certainly, data gathering is part of the scientific method of solving problems. But relying too much on your own experience (or data) has drawbacks. First, your own experience is limited. Furthermore, your retention depends substantially on how the events agree with your experience and beliefs, on whether things “make sense,” and on what your state of motivation to remember is. Nevertheless, the use of data (and the empirical method) is high on the continuum of methods of obtaining knowledge as long as you are aware of the limitations of relying too heavily on this method. empirical—A description of data or a study that is based on objective observations. Scientific Method of Problem-Solving The methods of acquiring knowledge previously discussed lack the objectivity and control that characterize the scientific approach to problem-solving. The scientific method involves several basic steps. Some authors list seven or eight steps, and others condense these steps into three or four. Regardless, all the authors are in general agreement as to the sequence and processes involved. The steps are briefly described next. The basic processes are covered in detail in other chapters. Step 1: Developing the Problem (Defining and Delimiting It) This step may sound contradictory, because how could the development of the problem be part of solving it? Actually, the discussion here is not about finding a problem to study (ways of identifying a problem are discussed in chapter 2); Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com the assumption is that the researcher has already selected a topic. But to design and execute a sound investigation, the researcher must be specific about what is to be studied and to what extent it will be studied. Many ramifications constitute this step, an important one being the identification of the independent and dependent variables. The independent variable is what the researcher is manipulating. If, for example, two training programs for improving balance in older adults are being compared, then type of training program is the independent variable; this item is sometimes called the experimental, or treatment, variable. independent variable—The part of the experiment that the researcher is manipulating; also called the experimental variable or treatment variable. The dependent variable is the effect of the independent variable. In the comparison of balance training programs, the measure of balance is the dependent variable. If you think of an experiment as a cause-and-effect proposition, the cause is the independent variable and the effect is the dependent variable. The latter is sometimes referred to as the yield. Thus, the researcher must define exactly what will be studied and what will be the measured effect. When this question is resolved, the experimental design can be determined. dependent variable—The effect of the independent variable; also called the yield. Step 2: Formulating the Hypothesis The hypothesis is the expected result. A person setting out to conduct a study generally has an idea as to what the outcome will be. This anticipated solution to the problem may be based on some theoretical construct, on the results of previous studies, or perhaps on the experimenter’s own experiences and observations. (Remember: The last source is least likely and least defensible because of the weaknesses of the unscientific methods of acquiring knowledge discussed previously.) Regardless, the research should have some experimental hypothesis about each subproblem in the study. hypothesis—The anticipated outcome of a study or experiment. If a hypothesis is testable, a study will either support or refute it. Testability is a necessary feature of a hypothesis. One of the essential features about the hypothesis is that it be testable. The study must be designed in such a way that the hypothesis can be either supported or refuted. Obviously, then, the hypothesis cannot be a type of value judgment or an abstract phenomenon that cannot be observed. For example, you might hypothesize that success in athletics depends solely on fate. In other words, if a team wins, it is because it was meant to be; similarly, if a team loses, a victory was just not meant to be. Refuting this hypothesis is impossible because no evidence could be obtained to test it. Step 3: Gathering the Data Next, the researcher must decide on the proper methods of acquiring the necessary data to be used in testing the research hypothesis. The reliability of the measuring instruments, the controls that are employed, and the overall objectivity and precision of the data-gathering process are crucial to solving the problem. In terms of difficulty, gathering data may be the easiest step because in many cases, it is routine. Planning the method, however, is one of the most difficult steps. Good methods attempt to maximize both the internal validity and the external validity of the study. internal validity—The extent to which the results of a study can be attributed to the treatments used in the study. external validity—The generalizability of the results of a study. Internal validity and external validity relate to the research design and controls that are used. Internal validity refers to the extent to which the results can be attributed to the treatments used in the study. In other words, the researcher must try to control all other variables that could influence the results. For example, Jim Nasium wants to assess the effectiveness of his exercise program in developing physical fitness in young boys. He tests his participants first at the beginning and then at the end of a nine-month training program and concludes that the program brought about significant improvement in fitness. What is wrong with Jim’s conclusion? His study contains several flaws. The first is that Jim did not consider maturity. Nine months of maturation produced significant changes in size and in accompanying strength and endurance. Also, what else were the participants doing during this time? How do we know that other activities were not responsible, or partly so, for the changes in their fitness levels? Chapter 18 deals with these threats to internal validity. External validity pertains to the generalizability of the results. To what extent can the results apply to the real world? A paradox often occurs for research in the behavioral sciences because of the controls required for internal validity. In motor-learning studies, for example, the task is often something novel so that it provides a control for experience. Furthermore, being able to measure the performance objectively and reliably is desirable. Consequently, the learning task is frequently a maze, a rotary pursuit meter, or a linear position task, all of which may meet the demands for control with regard to internal validity. But then you face the question of external validity: How does performance in a laboratory setting with a novel, irrelevant task apply to learning a real-world motor skill? These questions are important and sometimes vexing, but they are not insurmountable. (They are discussed later.) Possible Misinterpretations of Results We will never run out of math professors because they always multiply. Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com When the body is fully immersed in water, the telephone rings. If there are only two people in a locker room, they will have adjacent lockers. The ocean would be much deeper without sponges. Step 4: Analyzing and Interpreting Results Any researcher new to the field finds this step to be the most formidable for several reasons. First, this step usually involves some statistical analysis, and the novice researcher (particularly a graduate student) often has a limited background in and a fear of statistics. Second, analysis and interpretation require considerable knowledge, experience, and insight, which the novice may lack. It goes without question that analyzing and interpreting results is the most crucial and challenging of all the steps in the scientific method of problem-solving. It is here that the researcher must provide evidence for the support or rejection of the research hypothesis. In doing this, the researcher also compares the results with those of others (the related literature) and perhaps attempts to relate and integrate the results into some theoretical model. Inductive reasoning is employed in this step (whereas deductive reasoning is primarily used in the statement of the problem; we’ll more thoroughly address inductive and deductive reasoning in chapter 2). The researcher attempts to synthesize the data from their study along with the results of other studies to contribute to the development or substantiation of a theory. Alternative Models of Research In the preceding section, we summarized the basic steps in the scientific method of problem-solving. Science is a way of knowing and is often defined as structured inquiry. One basic goal of science is to explain things or to generalize and build a theory. When a scientist develops a useful model to explain behavior, scholars often test predictions from this model using the steps of the scientific method. The model and the approaches used to test the model are called a paradigm. Normal Science For centuries, the scientific approaches used in studying problems in both the natural and the social sciences have been what Thomas Kuhn (1970), a noted science historian, termed normal science. This manner of study is characterized by the elements we listed at the beginning of this chapter: systematic, logical, empirical, reductive, and replicable. Its basic doctrine is objectivity. It is quantitative in nature; that is, phenomena are described or measured numerically. Normal science is grounded in the natural sciences, which have long adhered to the idea of the orderliness and reality of matter—that is, that nature’s laws are absolute and discoverable by objective, systematic observations and investigations that are not influenced by (i.e., independent of) humans. The experiments are theory driven and have testable hypotheses. normal science—An objective manner of study grounded in the natural sciences that is systematic, logical, empirical, reductive, and replicable. Normal science received a terrific jolt with Einstein’s theory of relativity and the quantum theory, which indicated that nature’s laws could be influenced by humans (that is, that reality depends to a great extent on how one perceives it). Moreover, some things, such as the decay of a radioactive nucleus, happen for no reason at all. The fundamental laws that had been believed to be absolute were now considered statistical rather than deterministic. Phenomena could be predicted statistically but not explained deterministically (Jones, 1988). Challenges to Normal Science Relatively recently (since about 1960), serious challenges have arisen regarding normal science’s concept of objectivity (i.e., that the researcher can be detached from the instruments and conduct of the experiment). Two of the most powerful challengers to the idea of objective knowledge were Michael Polanyi (1958) and Thomas Kuhn (1970). They contended that objectivity is a myth and has no basis in reality. From the first inception of the idea for the hypothesis through the selection of apparatus to the analysis of the results, the observer is involved. The conduct of the experiment and the results can be considered expressions of the researcher’s point of view. Polanyi was especially opposed to the adoption of normal science for the study of human behavior. Kuhn (1970) maintained that normal science does not really evolve in systematic steps the way that scientific writers describe it. Kuhn discussed the paradigm crisis phenomenon, in which researchers who have been following a particular paradigm begin to find discrepancies in it. The findings no longer agree with the predictions, and a new paradigm is advanced. Interestingly, the old paradigm does not die completely; rather, it, metaphorically speaking, just develops varicose veins and fades away. Many researchers with a great deal of time and effort invested in the old paradigm are reluctant to change, so it is usually a new group of researchers who propose the new paradigm. Thus, normal science progresses by revolution, with a new group of scientists breaking away and replacing the old. Nevertheless, normal science has been and will continue to be successful in the natural sciences and in certain aspects of the study of humans. Martens (1987), however, contended that it has failed miserably in the study of human behavior, especially in human behavior’s more complex functions. Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com paradigm crisis phenomenon—The development of discrepancies in a paradigm leading to proposals of a new paradigm that better explains the data. As a sport psychologist, Martens asserted that laboratory experiments have limited use in answering questions about complex human behavior in sport. He considered his role as a practicing sport psychologist to have been far more productive in gaining knowledge about athletes and coaches and the solutions to their problems. Other workers in the so-called helping professions have made similar observations about both the limitations of normal science and the importance of alternative sources of knowledge in forming and shaping professional beliefs. Schein (1987), a noted scholar of social psychology, related an interesting (some might call it shocking) revelation concerning the relative influence of published research results as opposed to practical experience. At a conference, he and a number of his colleagues were discussing what they relied on most for their classroom teaching. These professors seemed to agree that the data they really believed in and used in the classroom came from personal experience and information learned in the field. Schein was making the point that different categories of knowledge can be obtained by different methods. In effect, some people are more influenced by sociological and anthropological research models than by the normal science approach. For some time, many scholars in education, psychology, sociology, anthropology, sport psychology, physical education, and other disciplines have proposed methods of studying human behavior other than those of conventional normal science. Anthropologists, sociologists, and clinical psychologists have used in-depth observation, description, and analysis of human behavior for nearly three-quarters of a century. For over 60 years, researchers in education have used participant and nonparticipant observation to obtain comprehensive, firsthand accounts of teacher and student behaviors as they occur in real-world settings. More recently, physical educators, sport psychologists, and exercise specialists have also become engaged in this type of field research. This general form of research is referred to by several names: ethnographic, qualitative, grounded, naturalistic, and participant observational research. Regardless of the names and the commitments to and beliefs of the researchers, this type of research was not well received initially by the adherents of normal science and the scientific method. In fact, this form of research (we include all its forms under the name qualitative research) has often been labeled by normal scientists as superficial, lacking in rigor, and just plain unscientific. As qualitative research methods have evolved, so has the thinking of many of these people. As you will see in chapters 19 and 20, many of the research tenets listed by Kuhn (1970) are found in contemporary qualitative research. qualitative research—A research method that often involves intensive, long-term observation in a natural setting; precise and detailed recording of what happens in the setting; and the interpretation and analysis of the data using description, narratives, quotes, charts, and tables. Also called ethnographic, naturalistic, interpretive, grounded, phenomenological, subjective, and participant observational research. Martens (1987) referred to such adherents of normal science as the gatekeepers of knowledge because they are the research journal editors and reviewers who decide which research gets published, who serves on the editorial boards, and whose papers are presented at conferences. Studies without internal validity are not published, yet studies without external validity lack practical significance. Martens charged that normal science (in psychology) prefers publication to practical significance. The debates over qualitative and normal (often classified as quantitative) research methods have been heated and prolonged. The qualitative proponents have gained momentum as well as more researchers’ confidence in recent years and qualitative research is now recognized as a viable method of addressing problems in the behavioral sciences. Credibility is established by systematically categorizing and analyzing causal and consequential factors. The naturalistic setting of qualitative research both facilitates analysis and precludes precise control of so-called extraneous factors, as does much other research occurring in field settings. The holistic interrelationship of observations and the complexity and dynamic processes of human interaction make it impossible to limit the study of human behavior to the sterile, reductionistic approach of normal science. Reductionism, a characteristic of normal science, assumes that complex behavior can be reduced, analyzed, and explained as parts that can then be put back together as a whole and understood. Critics of the conventional approach to research believe that the central issue is the unjustified belief that normal science is the only source of true knowledge. quantitative research—Research involving measurement of phenomena or outcomes numerically using reliable and objective data gathering and analysis methods consistent with normal science and the scientific method of problem solving. reductionism—A characteristic of normal science that assumes that complex behavior can be reduced, analyzed, and explained as parts that can then be put back together to understand the whole. Implications of Challenges to Normal Science The challenges to normal science involve many implications. For example, when we study simple movements, such as linear positioning in a laboratory to reflect cognitive processing of information, do we learn anything about movements and performance of sport skills in real-world settings? When we evaluate EMG activity in specific muscle groups during a simple movement, does the result really tell us anything about the way the nervous system controls complex movements in athletics in natural settings? Can we study the association of psychological processes related to movement in laboratory settings and expect the results to apply in sport and exercise situations? When we conduct these types of experiments, are we studying nature’s phenomena or laboratory phenomena? Do not misinterpret the intent of these questions. They do not mean that nothing important can be discovered about physical activity from laboratory research. What they suggest is that these findings do not necessarily accurately model Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com the way humans plan, control, and execute movements in natural settings associated with exercise and sport. Kuhn’s (1970) explanations of how science advances and of the limitations of applying normal science to natural settings demonstrate that scientists need to consider the various ways of obtaining knowledge and that the strict application of the normal scientific method of problem-solving may sometimes hinder rather than advance science. If the reductionistic approach of the scientific method has not well served the natural scientists who developed it, then certainly human behavior researchers need to assess the relative strengths and weaknesses of conventional and alternative research paradigms for their particular research questions. Alternative Forms of Scientific Inquiry Martens (1987, p. 52) suggested that we view knowledge not as being either scientific or unscientific or as being either reliable or unreliable but rather as existing on a continuum, as illustrated in figure 1.1. This continuum, labeled DK for degrees of knowledge, ranges from “don’t know” to “damn konfident.” Considered in this way, varying approaches to disciplined inquiry are useful in accumulating knowledge. As examples, Martens (1979, 1987) urged sport psychologists to consider the idiographic approach, introspective methods, and field studies instead of relying on the paradigm of normal science as the only answer to research questions in sport psychology. Thomas, French, and Humphries (1986) detailed how to study children’s sport knowledge and skills in games and sports. Costill (1985) discussed the study of physiological responses in practical exercise and sport settings. Locke (1989) presented a tutorial on the use of qualitative research in physical education and sport. In later chapters, we provide greater detail about some of these alternative strategies for research, particularly the historical, philosophical, qualitative, and mixed methods. Figure 1.1 The degrees of knowledge theory with examples of methods varying in degree of reliability. Reprinted by permission from R. Martens, “Science, Knowledge, and Sport Psychology,” The Sport Psychologist 1, no. 1 (1987): 46. Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com What we hope you gain from this section is that science is disciplined inquiry, not a set of specific procedures. Although advocates of alternative methods of research are often persuasive, we do not want you to conclude that the study of physical activity should abandon the traditional methods of normal science. We have learned much from these techniques and will continue to do so. Furthermore, we certainly do not want you to toss away this book as being pointless. We have not even begun to tell you all the fascinating things that we have learned over the years—it is hard to tell whether some of these things should be classified as normal or abnormal science. In addition, we have many stories of the abnormal humor variety yet to tell. Aside from these compelling reasons for continuing with the book, we want you to realize and appreciate that so-called normal science is not the solution to all questions raised in our field. Furthermore, none of the alternative methods of research denounces the scientific method of problem-solving. The bottom line is that different problems require different solutions. As we said before, science is disciplined inquiry, not a set of specific procedures. We need to embrace all systematic forms of inquiry. Rather than argue about the differences, we should capitalize on the strengths of all scholarly methods to provide useful knowledge about human movement. The nature of the research questions and setting should drive the selection of approaches to acquiring knowledge. In fact, just as Christina (1989) suggested, researchers might move among levels of research (basic to applied), and so researchers might move among paradigms (quantitative to qualitative to mixed methods) to acquire knowledge. In addressing this issue, we highlight qualitative and mixed-method research in chapters 19 and 20, which focus on using varying types of research approaches. Of course, we do not want to be perceived like Danae in Wiley’s comic strip Non Sequitur. Danae, a young girl, says to her horse that she wants to grow up to be a preconceptual scientist. Her horse asks, “What is that?” to which Danae responds “The new science of reaching a conclusion before doing any research and then simply dismissing anything contrary to your preconceived notions.” Types of Research Research is a structured way of solving problems. Different kinds of problems attend the study of physical activity; thus, different types of research are used to solve these problems. This text concentrates on five types of research: analytical, descriptive, experimental, qualitative, and mixed methods. A brief description of each follows. Analytical Research As the name implies, analytical research involves in-depth study and the evaluation of available information in an attempt to explain complex phenomena. The types of analytical research are historical, philosophical, reviews, and research synthesis. analytical research—A type of research that involves in-depth study and the evaluation of available information in an attempt to explain complex phenomena; can be categorized in the following way: historical, philosophical, reviews, and research synthesis. Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com My research methods teacher will love this idea. Historical Research As its name indicates, historical research deals with events that have already occurred. Historical research focuses on events, organizations, institutions, and people. In some studies, the researcher is interested mostly in preserving the record of events and accomplishments. In other investigations, the researcher attempts to discover facts that will provide more meaning and understanding of past events to explain the present state of affairs. Some historians have even attempted to use information from the past to predict the future. The research procedures associated with historical studies are addressed in considerable detail in chapter 12. Philosophical Research Critical inquiry characterizes philosophical research. The researcher establishes hypotheses, examines and analyzes facts, and synthesizes the evidence into a workable theoretical model. Many of the most important problem areas must be dealt with by the philosophical method. Problems involving objectives, curricula, course content, requirements, and methodology are but a few of the important issues that can be resolved only through the philosophical method of problem-solving. Although some authors emphasize the differences between science and philosophy, the philosophical method of research follows essentially the same steps as other methods of scientific problem-solving. The philosophical approach uses scientific facts as the basis for formulating and testing research hypotheses. An example of such philosophical research is Morland’s 1958 study in which he analyzed the educational views held by leaders in American physical education and categorized them into educational philosophies of reconstructionism, progressivism, essentialism, and perennialism. Having an opinion is not the same as having a philosophy. In philosophical research, beliefs must be subjected to rigorous criticism in light of the fundamental assumptions. Academic preparation in philosophy and a solid background in the fields from which the facts are derived are necessary. Other examples and a more detailed explanation of philosophical research are given in chapter 13. Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Reviews A review is a critical evaluation of recent research on a particular topic. The author must be extremely knowledgeable about the available literature as well as the research topic and procedures. A review involves an analysis, evaluation, and integration of the published literature, often leading to important conclusions concerning the research findings up to that time. For good examples of reviews, see Blair (1993) and Silverman and Subramaniam (1999). review—A critical evaluation of research on a particular topic. Certain publications consist entirely of reviews, such as Psychological Review, Annual Review of Physiology, Exercise and Sports Sciences Reviews, Review of Educational Research, Sports Medicine, and Kinesiology Review. A number of journals publish reviews periodically, and some occasionally devote entire issues to reviews. For example, the 75th anniversary issue of Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport (Silverman, 2005) contains some excellent reviews on various topics. Research Synthesis Reviews of literature are difficult to write because they require the synthesis of a large number of studies to determine common underlying findings, agreements, or disagreements. To some extent, this is like trying to make sense of data collected on a large number of participants by simply looking at the data. Glass (1977) and Glass, McGaw, and Smith (1981) proposed a quantitative means of analyzing the findings from numerous studies; this method is called metaanalysis. Findings between studies are compared by changing results within studies to a common metric called effect size. Over the years, many meta-analyses have been reported in the physical activity literature (e.g., Lee, Folsum & Blair, 2003; Rawdon, Sharp, Shelley, & Thomas, 2012; Sibley & Etnier, 2003; Schieffer & Thomas, 2012; Vazou, Pesce, Lakes, & Smiley-Oyen, 2019). This technique is discussed in more detail in chapter 14. Descriptive Research Descriptive research is concerned with status. The most prevalent descriptive research technique is the survey, most notably the questionnaire. Other forms of surveys include the personal interview, online polling, and the normative survey. Chapter 15 provides detailed coverage of these techniques. The following sections briefly describe three types of survey research techniques. descriptive research—A type of research that attempts to describe the status of the study’s focus. Common techniques are questionnaires, interviews, normative surveys, case studies, job analyses, observational research, developmental studies, and correlational studies. Questionnaire The main justification for using a questionnaire is the need to obtain responses from people, often from a wide geographical area. The questionnaire usually strives to secure information about present practices, conditions, and demographic data. Occasionally, a questionnaire asks for opinions or knowledge. Online polling has become an increasingly common approach for questionnaire research in recent years. Interview The interview and the questionnaire are essentially the same technique insofar as planning and procedures are concerned. Obviously, the interview has certain advantages over the questionnaire. The researcher can rephrase questions and ask additional ones to clarify responses and secure results that are more valid. Becoming a skilled interviewer requires training and experience. Telephone interviewing has been used for decades. It costs half as much as face-to-face interviews and can cover a wide geographical area, which is generally a limitation in personal interviews. We discuss some other advantages of the telephone interview technique in chapter 15. Normative Survey A number of notable normative surveys have been conducted in the fields of physical activity and health. The normative survey generally seeks to gather performance or knowledge data on a large sample from a population and to present the results in the form of comparative standards, or norms. The AAHPER Youth Fitness Test Manual (American Association for Health, Physical Education and Recreation, 1958) is an outstanding example of a normative survey. Thousands of boys and girls ages 10 to 18 throughout the United States were tested on a battery of motor fitness items. Percentiles were then established for comparative performances to provide information for students, teachers, administrators, and parents. The AAHPER youth fitness test was developed in response to another survey, the KrausWeber test (Kraus & Hirschland, 1954), which revealed that American children scored dramatically lower on a test battery of minimum muscular fitness when compared with European children. Other Descriptive Research Techniques Among the other forms of descriptive research are the case study, the job analysis, observational research, developmental studies, and correlational studies. Chapter 16 provides detailed coverage of these descriptive research Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com procedures. Case Study The case study is used to provide detailed information about an individual person, institution, or community, and so on. It aims to determine unique characteristics about the subject or condition. This descriptive research technique is used widely in such fields as medicine, psychology, counseling, and sociology. The case study is also a technique used in qualitative research. Job Analysis This type of research is a special form of case study. It is done to describe the nature of a particular job, including the duties, responsibilities, and preparation required for success in the job. Observational Research Observational research is a descriptive technique in which behaviors are observed in the participants’ natural setting, such as the classroom or play environment. The observations are frequently coded, and then their frequency and duration are analyzed. Developmental Studies In developmental research, the investigator is usually concerned with the interaction of learning or performance with maturation. For example, a researcher may wish to assess the extent to which the ability to process information about movement can be attributed to maturation as opposed to strategy, or to determine the effects of growth on a physical parameter such as aerobic capacity. Developmental studies can be undertaken by what is called the longitudinal method, whereby the same participants are studied over a period of years. Obvious logistical problems are associated with longitudinal studies, so an alternative is to select samples of participants from different age groups to assess the effects of maturation. This is called the crosssectional approach. Correlational Studies The purpose of correlational research is to examine the relationship between performance variables, such as heart rate and ratings of perceived exertion; the relationship between traits, such as anxiety and pain tolerance; or the correlation between attitudes and behavior, as in the attitude toward fitness and the amount of participation in fitness activities. Sometimes correlation is employed to predict performance. For example, a researcher may wish to predict the percentage of body fat from skinfold measurements. Correlational research is descriptive in that a cause-and-effect relationship cannot be presumed. All that can be established is that an association is (or is not) present between two or more traits or performances. Epidemiological Research Another form of descriptive research that has become a viable approach to studying problems concerning health, fitness, and safety is the epidemiological research method. This type of research pertains to the frequencies and distributions of health and disease conditions among populations. Rate of occurrence is the basic concept in epidemiological studies. The size of the population being studied is an important consideration in examining the prevalence of such things as injuries, illnesses, or health conditions in a specified at-risk population. Although cause and effect cannot be established by incidence and prevalence data, a strong inference of causation can often be made through association. Chapter 17 is devoted to epidemiological research. Experimental Research Experimental research has a major advantage over other types of research in that the researcher can manipulate treatments to cause things to happen (i.e., establish a cause-and-effect situation). As an example of an experimental study, assume that Virginia Reel, a dance teacher, hypothesizes that students would learn more effectively through the use of video. First, she randomly assigns students to two sections. One section is taught by the so-called traditional method (explanation, demonstration, practice, and critique). The other section is taught in a similar manner except that the students are filmed while practicing and can thus observe themselves as the teacher critiques their performances. After nine weeks, a panel of dance teachers evaluates both sections. In this study, the teaching method is the independent variable, and dance performance (skill) is the dependent variable. After the groups’ scores are compared statistically, Virginia can conclude whether her hypothesis can be supported. experimental research—A type of research that involves the manipulation of treatments in an attempt to establish cause-and-effect relationships. In experimental research, the researcher attempts to control all factors except the experimental (or treatment) variable. If the extraneous factors can be controlled, then the researcher can presume that the changes in the dependent Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com variable are due to the independent variable. Chapter 18 is devoted to experimental and quasi-experimental research. Qualitative Research In the study of physical activity, qualitative research is the relatively new kid on the block, although it has been used for many years in other fields, such as anthropology and sociology. Researchers in education have been engaged in qualitative methods longer than researchers in our field have. As previously mentioned, several names are given to this type of research (ethnographic, naturalistic, interpretive, grounded, phenomenological, subjective, and participant observational). Some are simply name differences, whereas some have different approaches and points of focus. We have lumped them all together under qualitative research because that term seems to be the most commonly used in our field. Qualitative research is different from other research methods. It is a systematic method of inquiry, and it follows the scientific method of problem-solving to a considerable degree, although it deviates in certain dimensions. Qualitative research rarely establishes hypotheses at the beginning of the study; instead, it uses more general questions to guide the study. It proceeds in an inductive process in developing hypotheses and theory as the data unfold. The researcher is the primary instrument in data collection and analysis. Qualitative research is characterized by intensive, firsthand presence. The tools of data collection are observation, interviews, and researcher-designed instruments (Creswell, 2009). Qualitative research is described in chapters 19 and 20. Mixed Methods of Research In mixed methods of research, both quantitative and qualitative approaches are included (or mixed) within a research effort. This approach, often viewed as a pragmatic one, suggests that both qualitative and quantitative techniques are useful when studying real-world phenomena. For capturing behavioral data, the notion is that a researcher should use the best approach; in this case, it is the mixed method in which quantitative techniques are integrated within a single study. In other words, the whole study comprises two smaller studies—one that is quantitative and the other qualitative. Figure 1.2 The total research setting. Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Overview of the Research Process A good overview of the research methods course, which serves well as an introduction to this book, is provided in figure 1.2. This flowchart provides a linear way to think about planning a research study. After the problem area is identified, reading and thinking about relevant theories and concepts, as well as a careful search of the literature for relevant findings, lead to the specification of hypotheses or questions. Operational definitions are needed in a research study so that the reader knows exactly what the researcher means by certain terms. Operational definitions describe observable phenomena that enable the researcher to examine empirically whether the predictions can be supported. The study is designed, and the methods are made operational. The data are then collected and analyzed, and the findings are identified. Finally, the results are related back to the original hypotheses or questions and discussed in relation to theories, concepts, and previous research findings. Parts of a Thesis: A Reflection on the Steps in the Research Process This chapter has introduced the research process. The theme has been the scientific method of problem-solving. Generally, a thesis or research article has a standard format. This feature is for expediency, so that the reader knows where to find the pieces of information, such as purpose, methods, and results. The format also reflects the steps in the scientific method of problem-solving. We now look at a typical thesis format and see how the parts correspond to the steps in the scientific method. Sometimes, theses and dissertations are done in a chapter format in which each chapter represents a separate part of the research report (e.g., introduction). This model has been commonly used over the years. We believe that it is more appropriate for graduate students to prepare their theses or dissertations in a form suitable for journal publication because that is an important part of the research process. Writing the theses or dissertation in a journal format also prepares students for future writing. In chapter 22, we provide considerable detail about how to use a journal format for a thesis or dissertation and the value of doing so. Throughout this book, we refer to the typical parts of a research report. These can be considered either as parts of a journal paper or as chapters, depending on the format selected. Introduction In the introduction, the problem is defined and delimited. The researcher specifically identifies the problem and often states the research hypotheses. Certain terms critical to the study are operationally defined for the reader, and limitations and perhaps some basic assumptions are acknowledged. The literature review may be in the introduction, or it may warrant a separate section. When it is part of the introduction, the literature review more closely adheres to the steps in the scientific method of problem-solving; that is, the literature review is instrumental in the formulation of hypotheses and the deductive reasoning leading to the problem statement. Methods The purpose of the methods section is to make the thesis format parallel to the data-gathering steps of the scientific method. First, the researcher explains how the data were gathered. The participants are identified, the measuring instruments are described, the measurement and treatment procedures are presented, the experimental design is explained, and the methods of analyzing the data are summarized. The major purpose of the methods section is to describe the study in such detail and with such clarity that a reader could replicate it. The introduction and methods section often comprise the research proposal and are presented to the student’s thesis committee before the research is undertaken. For the proposal, methods are often written in future tense and then changed to past tense when the final version of the thesis is completed. Of course, presenting methods in past tense in the proposal eliminates the need to make this conversion later in the research process. Discuss with your advisor which tense is preferred for the proposal. The research proposal also often contains some preliminary data demonstrating that the student has the required expertise to collect the data using the instrumentation needed. research proposal—A formal preparation that includes the introduction, literature review, and proposed method for conducting a study. Results The results present the pertinent findings from the data analysis and represent the contribution to new knowledge. The results section corresponds to the step in the scientific method in which the meaningfulness and reliability of the results are scrutinized. Discussion and Conclusions In this last step in the scientific method, the researcher employs inductive reasoning to analyze the findings, interpret the findings relative to those of previous studies, and integrate them into a theoretical model. In this part of a research Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com paper, the acceptability of the research hypotheses is judged. Then, based on the analysis and discussion, conclusions are usually made. The conclusions should address the purpose and secondary purposes that were specified in the introduction. Qualitative and Mixed-Method Studies When the qualitative or mixed-method approach is used, the format for the thesis or dissertation often varies from the preceding (i.e., introduction, method, results, and discussion). However, the general notion of explaining the problem, describing how data were collected, presenting the results, and providing a discussion remain the same. Chapters 19, 20, 21, and 22 provide a more in-depth discussion of this. Summary Research is simply a way of solving problems. Questions are raised, and methods are devised to try to answer them. There are various ways of approaching problems (research methods). Sometimes the nature of the problem dictates the method of research. For example, a researcher who wants to discover the origins of a sport would use the historical method of research. A researcher who wants to look at a problem from a particular angle may select a research method that can best answer the question from that angle. Research on the topic of teaching effectiveness, for example, can be approached in several ways. An experimental study could be conducted in which the effectiveness of teaching methods in bringing about measurable achievement is compared. Or a study could be designed in which teachers’ behaviors are coded and evaluated using some observational instrument. Or another form of descriptive research could be used that employs the questionnaire or the interview technique to examine teachers’ responses to questions concerning their beliefs or practices. Or perhaps a qualitative study could be undertaken to observe and interview one teacher in one school systematically over an extended period to portray the teacher’s experiences and perceptions in the natural setting. And of course, qualitative and quantitative approaches can be combined in a mixed-method format. The point is that there is not just one way to do research. Some people do only one type of research. Some are critical of the methods used by others. However, anyone who believes that their type of research is the only “scientific” way to solve problems has a narrow-minded and downright foolish understanding of how to conduct research. Science is disciplined inquiry, not a set of specific procedures. Basic research deals primarily with theoretical problems, and the results are not intended to have immediate application. Applied research, on the other hand, strives to answer questions that have direct value to the practitioner. There is a need to prepare proficient consumers of research as well as researchers. Thus, one purpose of a book on research methods is to help the reader understand the tools necessary both to consume and to produce research. We presented here an overview of the nature of research. The scientific method of problem-solving was contrasted with “unscientific” methods by which people acquire information. Multiple research models were discussed to emphasize that there is not just one way to approach problems in our discipline and profession of kinesiology. We identified the five major types of research used in the study of physical activity: analytical, descriptive, experimental, qualitative, and mixed methods. These categories and the techniques they encompass are covered in detail in later chapters. Check Your Understanding 1. Look through some recent issues of Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. Find and read a research article of interest that is quantitative in nature and another that you believe is qualitative. Which of these did you find easier to understand? Why? 2. Find an article that describes a study that you would classify as an applied research study and another that you believe describes a basic research study. Defend your choices. 3. Think of two problems that need research in your field. From the descriptions of the types of research in this chapter, suggest how each problem might be researched. Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com 2 Developing the Problem and Using the Literature The library banned drinks after someone poured milk on the serials. —Early Bird Books Getting started is the hardest part of almost any new venture, and research is no exception. You cannot do any meaningful research until you have identified the area that you want to investigate, learned what has been published in that area, and figured out how you are going to conduct the investigation. In this chapter, we discuss ways to identify researchable problems, search for literature, and write the literature review. Identifying the Research Problem Of the many major issues facing the graduate student, a primary one is identifying a research problem. Research ideas may arise from a student’s curiosity about some aspect of human performance, be stimulated by real-world settings, or be generated from theoretical frameworks. Regardless, a fundamental requirement for developing and appropriately limiting a good research problem is in-depth knowledge about the area of interest. But sometimes, as students become more knowledgeable about a content area, everything seems to be already known. Thus, although you want to become an expert, do not focus too narrowly, because doing so can limit topics. Relating your knowledge base to other areas often provides insight into significant areas for research. Ironically, we ask students to start thinking about possible research topics in their research methods course, typically a course taken in the first semester (or quarter) of graduate school before students have had the opportunity to acquire in-depth knowledge. As a result, many of their research problems are ill-conceived, infeasible, unattainable, or superficial; lack a theoretical base, or are replications of earlier research. Although this shortcoming is considerable, the advantages of taking the research methods course early in the program are substantial in terms of success in other graduate courses. In this course, students learn the following: To approach and solve problems in a scientific way To search the literature To write in a clear, concise, scientific fashion To understand basic measurement and statistical issues To use an appropriate writing style To be intelligent consumers of research To appreciate the wide variety of research strategies and techniques used in an area of study Problems That Have Not Been Resolved by Humankind 9. Is there ever a day when mattresses are not on sale? 8. If people evolved from apes, why are there still apes? 7. Why does someone believe you when you say there are four billion stars but checks when you say the paint is wet? 6. Why do you never hear father-in-law jokes? 5. If swimming is so good for your physique, how do you explain whales? 4. How do those dead bugs get into enclosed light fixtures? 3. Why does Superman stop bullets with his chest but duck when you throw a revolver at him? 2. Why do banks charge a fee on “insufficient funds” when they know there is not enough money in the account? … drum roll … 1. Why doesn’t Tarzan have a beard? How, then, does a student without much background select a problem? As you devote ever-greater effort to thinking of a topic, you may become increasingly inclined to think that all the problems in the field have already been solved. Adding to this frustration is the pressure of time. To assure you that important questions have yet to be addressed, we have provided the Problems That Have Not Been Resolved by Humankind sidebar. Guidelines for Finding a Topic To help alleviate the problem of finding a topic, we offer the following suggestions. First, be aware of the research being done at your institution, because research spawns other research ideas. Often a researcher has a series of studies planned. Second, be alert for any controversial issues in some area of interest. Lively controversy prompts research in efforts to resolve the issue. In any case, be sure to talk to professors and advanced graduate students in your area of Get Complete eBook Download by Email at discountsmtb@hotmail.com Get Complete eBook Download link Below for Instant Download: https://browsegrades.net/documents/286751/ebook-payment-link-forinstant-download-after-payment