Introduction to Psychology G AT E WAYS TO M I N D A N D B E H AV I O R TWELFTH EDITION Dennis Coon John O. Mitterer Brock University Australia • Brazil • Canada • Mexico • Singapore • Spain • United Kingdom • United States Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior, Twelfth Edition Dennis Coon / John O. Mitterer Senior Publisher: Linda Schreiber Senior Editors: Jane Potter, Jaime Perkins Managing Development Editor: Jeremy Judson © 2010, 2007 Wadsworth, Cengage Learning ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. No part of this work covered by the copyright herein may be reproduced, transmitted, stored, or used in any form or by any means, graphic, electronic, or mechanical, including but not limited to photocopying, recording, scanning, digitizing, taping, Web distribution, information networks, or information storage and retrieval systems, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without the prior written permission of the publisher. Assistant Editors: Trina Tom, Ileana Shevlin Editorial Assistant: Nicolas Albert For product information and technology assistance, contact us at Cengage Learning Customer & Sales Support, 1-800-354-9706. Associate Media Editor: Rachel Guzman Executive Marketing Manager: Kim Russell Marketing Manager: Tierra Morgan Executive Marketing Communications Manager: Talia Wise Sr. Content Project Manager, Editorial Production: Pat Waldo For permission to use material from this text or product, submit all requests online at www.cengage.com/permissions. Further permissions questions can be e-mailed to permissionrequest@cengage.com. Library of Congress Control Number: 2008938253 Creative Director: Rob Hugel Senior Art Director: Vernon Boes Print Buyer: Karen Hunt Permissions Acquisitions Manager, Images: Amanda Groszko Permissions Acquisitions Manager, Text: Roberta Broyer Production Service: Julie Ninnis, Graphic World Inc. Text and Cover Designer: Liz Harasymczuk Photo Researcher: Kathleen Olson Copy Editor: Graphic World Inc. Student Edition: ISBN-13: 978-0-495-59911-1 ISBN-10: 0-495-59911-5 Loose-leaf Edition: ISBN-13: 978-0-495-59913-5 ISBN-10: 0-495-59913-1 Wadsworth 10 Davis Drive Belmont, CA 94002-3098 USA Cover Image: Nick Wienholt Compositor: Graphic World Inc. Cengage Learning is a leading provider of customized learning solutions with office locations around the globe, including Singapore, the United Kingdom, Australia, Mexico, Brazil, and Japan. Locate your local office at international.cengage.com/region. Cengage Learning products are represented in Canada by Nelson Education, Ltd. For your course and learning solutions, visit www.cengage.com. Purchase any of our products at your local college store or at our preferred online store www.ichapters.com. Printed in Canada 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 12 11 10 09 about the authors After earning a doctorate in psychology from the University of Arizona, Dennis Coon taught for 22 years at Santa Barbara City College, California. Throughout his career, Dr. Coon has especially enjoyed the challenge of teaching introductory psychology. He and his wife, Sevren, have returned to Tucson, where he continues to teach, write, edit, and consult. Dr. Coon is the author of Introduction to Psychology and Psychology: A Journey, as well as Psychology: Modules for Active Learning. Together, these texts have been used by more than 2 million students. Dr. Coon frequently serves as a reviewer and consultant to publishers, and he edited the best-selling trade book Choices. He also helped design modules for PsychNow!, Wadsworth’s interactive CD-ROM. In his leisure hours, Dr. Coon enjoys hiking, photography, painting, woodworking, and music. He also designs, builds, and plays classical and steel string acoustic guitars. He has published articles on guitar design and occasionally offers lectures on this topic, in addition to his more frequent presentations on psychology. John O. Mitterer was awarded his Ph.D. in cognitive psychology from McMaster University. Currently, Dr. Mitterer teaches at Brock University, where he has taught more than 20,000 introductory psychology students. He is the recipient of the 2003 Brock University Distinguished Teaching Award, a 2003 Ontario Confederation of University Faculty Associations (OCUFA) Teaching Award, a 2004 National 3M Teaching Fellowship, and the 2005 Canadian Psychological Association Award for Distinguished Contributions to Education and Training in Psychology. Dr. Mitterer’s primary research focus is on basic cognitive processes in learning and teaching. He consulted for a variety of companies, such as Bell Northern Research, Unisys Corporation, IBM Canada, and computer-game developer Silicon Knights. His professional focus, however, is on applying cognitive principles to the improvement of undergraduate education. In support of his introductory psychology course, he has been involved in the production of textbooks and ancillary materials such as CD-ROMs and websites for both students and instructors. Dr. Mitterer has published and lectured on undergraduate instruction throughout Canada and the United States. In his spare time, Dr. Mitterer strives to become a better golfer and to attain his life goal of seeing all the bird species in the world. To this end he recently traveled to Papua New Guinea, Brazil, Australia, and South Africa. iii This page intentionally left blank brief contents Introduction: The Psychology of Studying 1 1 Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods 11 Psychology in Action: Psychology in the Media — Separating Fact from Fiction 43 2 Brain and Behavior 47 Psychology in Action: Handedness — Are You Dexterous or Sinister? 73 3 Human Development 78 Psychology in Action: Effective Parenting — Raising Healthy Children 112 4 Sensation and Reality 118 Psychology in Action: Controlling Pain — This Won’t Hurt a Bit 145 5 Perceiving the World 149 Psychology in Action: Perception and Objectivity — Believing Is Seeing 176 6 States of Consciousness 181 Psychology in Action: Exploring and Using Dreams 212 7 Conditioning and Learning 218 Psychology in Action: Behavioral Self-Management — A Rewarding Project 247 8 Memory 251 Psychology in Action: Mnemonics — Memory Magic 279 9 Cognition, Language, Creativity, and Intelligence 283 Psychology in Action: Culture, Race, IQ, and You 314 10 Motivation and Emotion 319 Psychology in Action: Emotional Intelligence — The Fine Art of Self-Control 353 v vi Brief Contents 11 Gender and Sexuality 357 Psychology in Action: When Pleasure Fades — Sexual Problems 381 12 Personality 388 Psychology in Action: Barriers and Bridges — Understanding Shyness 420 13 Health, Stress, and Coping 425 Psychology in Action: Stress Management 453 14 Psychological Disorders 459 Psychology in Action: Suicide — Lives on the Brink 490 15 Therapies 495 Psychology in Action: Self-Management and Finding Professional Help 522 16 Social Thinking and Social Influence 529 Psychology in Action: Assertiveness Training — Standing Up for Your Rights 551 17 Prosocial and Antisocial Behavior 556 Psychology in Action: Multiculturalism — Living with Diversity 579 18 Applied Psychology 584 Psychology in Action: Human Factors Psychology — Who’s the Boss Here? 607 Appendix: Behavioral Statistics 612 Glossary G-1 References R-1 Name Index N-1 Subject Index S-1 contents Introduction: The Psychology of Studying 1 The SQ4R Method — How to Tame a Textbook 2 How to Use Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior 3 Effective Note-Taking — Good Students, Take Note! 3 Using and Reviewing Your Notes 4 Study Strategies — Making a Habit of Success 4 Self-Regulated Learning — Academic All-Stars 5 Procrastination — Avoiding the Last-Minute Blues 6 Time Management 6 Goal Setting 6 Make Learning an Adventure 7 Taking Tests — Are You “Test Wise”? 7 General Test-Taking Skills 7 Using Digital Media — Netting New Knowledge 8 Digital Gateways 8 The Book Companion Website 8 CengageNOW 9 Wadsworth’s Psychology Resource Center 9 Psychology Websites 9 A Final Word 10 WEB RESOURCES 10 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 10 1 Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods 11 A Brief History of Psychology — Psychology’s Family Album 22 Structuralism 22 Functionalism 22 Behaviorism 23 Gestalt Psychology 24 Psychoanalytic Psychology 24 Humanistic Psychology 25 The Role of Women in Psychology’s Early Days 26 Psychology Today — Three Complementary Perspectives on Behavior 26 The Biological Perspective 27 The Psychological Perspective 27 The Sociocultural Perspective 28 Psychologists — Guaranteed Not to Shrink 29 Other Mental Health Professionals 31 The Profession of Psychology 31 Specialties in Psychology 32 The Psychology Experiment — Where Cause Meets Effect 32 Variables and Groups 33 Evaluating Results 34 Double Blind — On Placebos and Self-Fulfilling Prophecies 35 Research Participant Bias 35 Researcher Bias 36 Nonexperimental Research Methods — Different Strokes 37 Naturalistic Observation 37 Correlational Studies 38 The Clinical Method — One Case At A Time 40 Survey Method — Here, Have a Sample 40 Preview: Wondering About Human Behavior 12 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Psychology in the Media — Separating Fact from Fiction 43 Psychology — The ABCs of Behavior 12 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 45 Seeking Empirical Evidence 12 Psychological Research 14 Research Specialties 14 Psychology’s Goals 15 Critical Thinking — Take It With a Grain of Salt 16 Thinking About Behavior 16 Pseudopsychologies — Palms, Planets, and Personality 17 Problems in the Stars 18 Scientific Research — How to Think Like a Psychologist 19 The Scientific Method 19 WEB RESOURCES 46 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 46 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • CRITICAL THINKING: Testing Common-Sense Beliefs 13 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Is a Career in Psychology Right for You? 31 • CRITICAL THINKING: That’s Interesting, but Is It Ethical? 34 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Is There a Gender Bias in Psychological Research? 42 vii viii Contents 2 Brain and Behavior The Newborn — More Than Meets the Eye 85 47 Preview: Finding Music in Walnut Grapefruit Tofu 48 Neurons — Building a “Biocomputer” 48 Parts of a Neuron 48 The Nerve Impulse 48 Synapses and Neurotransmitters 51 Neural Networks 52 Perceptual and Cognitive Development 85 Motor Development 87 Emotional Development 87 Social Development — Baby, I’m Stuck on You 89 Attachment 89 Day Care 90 Attachment and Affectional Needs 91 Parental Influences — Life with Mom and Dad 91 The Nervous System — Wired for Action 53 Research Methods — Charting the Brain’s Inner Realms 56 Mapping Brain Structure 56 Exploring Brain Function 57 The Cerebral Cortex — My, What a Big Brain You Have! 59 Cerebral Hemispheres 60 Hemispheric Specialization 60 Lobes of the Cerebral Cortex 63 The Subcortex — At the Core of the (Brain) Matter 67 The Hindbrain 67 The Forebrain 68 The Magnificent Brain 70 The Endocrine System — My Hormones Made Me Do It 70 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Handedness — Are You Dexterous or Sinister? 73 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 76 Parenting Styles 91 Maternal and Paternal Influences 92 Ethnic Differences: Four Flavors of Parenting 93 Language Development — Fast-Talking Babies 94 Language and the Terrible Twos 94 The Roots of Language 95 Cognitive Development — Think Like a Child 97 Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development 97 Piaget Today 99 Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory 101 Adolescence and Young Adulthood — The Best of Times, the Worst of Times 102 Puberty 102 The Search for Identity 103 The Transition to Adulthood 104 Moral Development — Growing a Conscience 104 Levels of Moral Development 105 The Story of a Lifetime — Rocky Road or Garden Path? 106 Erikson’s Psychosocial Theory 106 Later Adulthood: Will You Still Need Me When I’m 64? 108 WEB RESOURCES 77 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 77 A Midlife Crisis? 108 Old Age 109 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • CRITICAL THINKING: You Can Change Your Mind, but Can You Change Your Brain? 53 Death and Dying — The Final Challenge 110 • CRITICAL THINKING: Repairing Your Brain 56 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Effective Parenting — Raising Healthy Children 112 • THE CLINICAL FILE: A Stroke of Bad Luck 61 • CRITICAL THINKING: Mirror, Mirror in the Brain 65 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: His and Hers Brains? 66 3 Human Development CHAPTER IN REVIEW 115 WEB RESOURCES 117 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 117 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) 78 Preview: It’s A Girl! 79 Nature and Nurture — It Takes Two to Tango 79 Heredity 79 Environment 81 Reaction Range 84 Reactions to Impending Death 111 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: What’s Your Attachment Style? 91 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Ethnic Diversity and Identity 103 • CRITICAL THINKING: The Twixters 104 Contents 4 Sensation and Reality 118 5 Perceiving the World Preview: Can’t You Hear the Bats? 119 Preview: Murder! 150 Psychophysics — The Limits of Sensibility 119 Perception: That Extra Step 150 Transduction 119 Absolute Thresholds 119 Difference Thresholds 120 Sensory Analysis and Coding 121 Vision — Catching Some Rays 123 Structure of the Eye 124 Rods and Cones 125 Color Vision — There’s More to It Than Meets the Eye 127 Color Theories 128 Color Blindness and Color Weakness 129 Dark Adaptation — Let There Be Light! 131 Hearing — Good Vibrations 132 How We Hear Sounds 133 Smell and Taste — The Nose Knows When the Tongue Can’t Tell 135 The Sense of Smell 136 Taste and Flavors 137 The Somesthetic Senses — Flying by the Seat of Your Pants 138 The Skin Senses 139 The Vestibular System 140 Adaptation, Attention, and Gating — Tuning In and Tuning Out 142 Sensory Adaptation 142 Selective Attention 142 Sensory Gating 143 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Controlling Pain —This Won’t Hurt a Bit 145 149 Bottom-Up and Top-Down Processing 151 Perception and Attention — May I Have Your . . . Attention! 151 Inattentional Blindness 153 Habituation 153 Motives, Emotions, and Perception 153 Perceptual Organization — Getting It All Together 154 Gestalt Principles 154 Perceptual Constancies — Taming an Unruly World 157 Depth Perception — What If the World Were Flat? 159 Binocular Depth Cues 160 Monocular Depth Cues 162 Perceptual Learning — What If the World Were Upside Down? 165 Perceptual Habits 166 The Context of Perception 168 Illusions 168 Perceptual Expectancies — On Your Mark, Get Set 171 Extrasensory Perception — Do You Believe in Magic? 172 An Appraisal of ESP 173 Stage ESP 174 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Perception and Objectivity — Believing Is Seeing 176 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 178 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 147 WEB RESOURCES 179 WEB RESOURCES 148 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 180 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 148 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • THE CLINICAL FILE: Pay Attention! 152 • CRITICAL THINKING: Subliminal Seduction or Subliminal Myth? 121 • CRITICAL THINKING: The “Boiled Frog Syndrome” 154 • BRAINWAVES: Blindsight: The “What” and the “Where” of Vision 127 • CRITICAL THINKING: A Bird’s-Eye View 158 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Are You ColorBlind? 130 • THE CLINICAL FILE: Staying in Touch with Reality 169 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Going Biosonar 132 • BRAINWAVES: The Matrix: Do Phantoms Live Here? 145 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Do They See What We See? 167 ix x Contents 6 States of Consciousness 181 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Exploring and Using Dreams 212 Preview: A Visit to Several States (of Consciousness) 182 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 215 States of Consciousness — The Many Faces of Awareness 182 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 217 Altered States of Consciousness 182 Sleep — A Nice Place to Visit 182 The Need for Sleep 183 Sleep Patterns 184 Stages of Sleep — The Nightly Roller-Coaster 185 Sleep Stages 186 The Dual Process Hypothesis of Sleep 186 Sleep Disturbances — Showing Nightly: Sleep Wars! 189 Insomnia 189 Sleepwalking, Sleeptalking, and Sleepsex 190 Nightmares and Night Terrors 190 Sleep Apnea 191 Narcolepsy 192 Dreams — A Separate Reality? 192 REM Sleep Revisited 192 Dream Theories 192 Dream Worlds 193 Hypnosis — Look into My Eyes 194 Theories of Hypnosis 194 The Reality of Hypnosis 195 Stage Hypnosis 196 Meditation and Sensory Deprivation — Chilling, the Healthy Way 197 Meditation 197 Sensory Deprivation 198 Positive Psychology: Mindfulness and Well-Being 199 Drug-Altered Consciousness — the High and Low of It 199 Drug Dependence 200 Patterns of Abuse 200 Uppers — Amphetamines, Cocaine, MDMA, Caffeine, Nicotine 201 Cocaine 203 MDMA (“Ecstasy”) 204 Caffeine 204 Nicotine 205 Downers — Sedatives, Tranquilizers, and Alcohol 206 Barbiturates 206 GHB 206 Tranquilizers 207 Alcohol 207 Hallucinogens — Tripping the Light Fantastic 210 LSD and PCP 210 Marijuana 210 WEB RESOURCES 216 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • CRITICAL THINKING: What Is It Like To Be a Bat? 183 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Consciousness and Culture 184 • CRITICAL THINKING: They Came from Outer Space? 188 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Swinging Suggestions 195 • BRAINWAVES: How Psychoactive Drugs Affect the Brain 201 7 Conditioning and Learning 218 Preview: Rats! 219 What Is Learning — Does Practice Make Perfect? 219 Types of Learning 219 Classical Conditioning — Does the Name Pavlov Ring a Bell? 220 Pavlov’s Experiment 220 Principles of Classical Conditioning — Here’s Johnny 222 Acquisition 222 Expectancies 222 Extinction and Spontaneous Recovery 223 Generalization 223 Discrimination 224 Classical Conditioning in Humans — An Emotional Topic 224 Conditioned Emotional Responses 224 Vicarious, or Secondhand, Conditioning 225 Operant Conditioning — Can Pigeons Play PingPong? 226 Positive Reinforcement 226 Acquiring an Operant Response 226 The Timing of Reinforcement 227 Shaping 228 Operant Extinction 228 Negative Reinforcement 229 Punishment 229 Operant Reinforcers — What’s Your Pleasure? 230 Primary Reinforcers 230 Secondary Reinforcers 230 Feedback 232 Learning Aids 232 Contents Partial Reinforcement — Las Vegas, a Human Skinner Box? 234 Schedules of Partial Reinforcement 235 Stimulus Control — Red Light, Green Light 236 Punishment — Putting the Brakes on Behavior 238 Variables Affecting Punishment 238 The Downside of Punishment 239 Using Punishment Wisely 240 Cognitive Learning — Beyond Conditioning 241 Cognitive Maps 242 Latent Learning 242 Modeling — Do as I Do, Not as I Say 243 Observational Learning 243 Modeling and the Media 244 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Behavioral Self-Management — A Rewarding Project 247 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 249 Forgetting in LTM — Why We, uh, Let’s See; Why We, uh . . . Forget! 264 When Encoding Fails 265 When Memory Storage Fails 266 When Retrieval Fails 266 Memory and the Brain — Some “Shocking” Findings 270 Consolidation 270 Long-Term Memory and the Brain 272 Exceptional Memory — Wizards of Recall 273 Eidetic Imagery 273 A Case of Photographic Memory 274 Memory Champions 275 Improving Memory — Keys to the Memory Bank 275 Encoding Strategies 276 Retrieval Strategies 277 A Look Ahead 278 WEB RESOURCES 250 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Mnemonics — Memory Magic 279 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 250 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 281 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) WEB RESOURCES 282 • THE CLINICAL FILE: Coping with Chemo 221 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 282 • BRAINWAVES: Tickling Your Own Fancy 230 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Learning and Conservation 233 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Cows, Memories, and Culture 254 • CRITICAL THINKING: Are Animals Stuck in Time? 236 • CRITICAL THINKING: Do You Like Jam with Your Memory? 257 • CRITICAL THINKING: You Mean Video Games Might Be Bad for Me? 245 8 Memory 251 • CRITICAL THINKING: Telling Wrong from Right in Forensic Memory 258 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Card Magic! 265 Preview: “What the Hell’s Going on Here?” 252 • THE CLINICAL FILE: The Recovered Memory/False Memory Debate 269 Stages of Memory — Do You Have a Mind Like a Steel Trap? Or a Sieve? 252 • BRAINWAVES: The Long-Term Potential of a Memory Pill 273 Sensory Memory 252 Short-Term Memory 253 Long-Term Memory 253 Short-Term Memory — Do You Know the Magic Number? 255 Chunking 255 Rehearsing Information 255 Long-Term Memory — Where the Past Lives 256 Constructing Memories 256 Organizing Memories 259 Skill Memory and Fact Memory 260 Measuring Memory — The Answer Is on the Tip of My Tongue 261 Recalling Information 262 Recognizing Information 262 Relearning Information 263 Implicit and Explicit Memories 263 9 Cognition, Language, Creativity, and Intelligence 283 Preview: Homo Sapiens 284 What Is Thinking? — Brains over Brawn 284 Some Basic Units of Thought 284 Mental Imagery — Does a Frog Have Lips? 284 The Nature of Mental Images 285 Concepts — I’m Positive, It’s a Whatchamacallit 286 Forming Concepts 286 Types of Concepts 287 Language — Don’t Leave Home without It 288 The Structure of Language 290 The Animal Language Debate 291 xi xii Contents Problem Solving — Getting an Answer in Sight 293 Mechanical Solutions 293 Solutions by Understanding 293 Heuristics 293 Insightful Solutions 294 Common Barriers to Problem Solving 297 Creative Thinking — Down Roads Less Traveled 297 Tests of Creativity 298 Stages of Creative Thought 299 Positive Psychology: The Creative Personality 300 Living More Creatively 301 Intuitive Thought — Mental Shortcut? Or Dangerous Detour? 301 Intuition 301 Framing 303 Wisdom 303 Human Intelligence — The IQ and You 304 Defining Intelligence 304 Intelligence Tests 305 Intelligence Quotients 305 The Wechsler Tests 307 Group Tests 307 Variations in Intelligence — Curved Like a Bell 308 The Mentally Gifted 308 Intellectual Disability 309 Questioning Intelligence — How Intelligent Are Intelligence Tests? 310 Multiple Intelligences 310 Artificial Intelligence: I Compute, Therefore I Am 311 Heredity, Environment, and Intelligence 312 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Culture, Race, IQ, and You 314 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 317 WEB RESOURCES 318 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 318 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Bilingualism — Si o No, Oui ou Non, Yes or No? 289 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: How to Weigh an Elephant 296 • CRITICAL THINKING: Have You Ever Thin Sliced Your Teacher? 302 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Intelligence — How Would a Fool Do It? 304 • THE CLINICAL FILE: Meet the Rain Man 309 • CRITICAL THINKING: You Mean Video Games Might Be Good for Me? 313 10 Motivation and Emotion 319 Preview: Moved by the Music of Life 320 Motivation — Forces That Push and Pull 320 A Model of Motivation 320 Biological Motives and Homeostasis 321 Circadian Rhythms 322 Hunger — Pardon Me, My Hypothalamus Is Growling 324 Internal Factors in Hunger 324 Brain Mechanisms 324 External Factors in Hunger and Obesity 326 Dieting 328 Eating Disorders 328 Culture, Ethnicity, and Dieting 331 Biological Motives Revisited — Thirst, Sex, and Pain 331 Thirst 331 Pain 331 The Sex Drive 332 Stimulus Drives — Skydiving, Horror Movies, and the Fun Zone 333 Arousal Theory 333 Levels of Arousal 334 Coping with Test Anxiety 335 Learned Motives — The Pursuit of Excellence 336 Opponent-Process Theory 336 Social Motives 336 The Need for Achievement 336 The Key to Success? 337 Motives in Perspective — A View from the Pyramid 338 Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 339 Turning Play into Work 339 Inside an Emotion — How Do You Feel? 341 Primary Emotions 341 Emotion and the Brain 342 Physiology and Emotion — Arousal, Sudden Death, and Lying 343 Fight or Flight 343 Lie Detectors 344 Expressing Emotions — Making Faces and Talking Bodies 346 Facial Expressions 346 Theories of Emotion — Several Ways to Fear a Bear 348 The James-Lange Theory 348 The Cannon-Bard Theory 349 Schachter’s Cognitive Theory of Emotion 349 Emotional Appraisal 350 The Facial Feedback Hypothesis 350 A Contemporary Model of Emotion 352 Contents PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Emotional Intelligence —The Fine Art of Self-Control 353 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 355 HIV/AIDS 379 Behavioral Risk Factors 379 Risk and Responsibility 380 WEB RESOURCES 356 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 356 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: When Pleasure Fades — Sexual Problems 381 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • BRAINWAVES: Your Brain’s “Fat Point” 326 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: What’s Your BMI? (We’ve Got Your Number) 327 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Behavioral Dieting 329 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Xtreme! 334 • CRITICAL THINKING: To Catch a Terrorist 345 • CRITICAL THINKING: Crow’s-Feet and Smiles Sweet 347 • THE CLINICAL FILE: Suppressing Emotion — Don’t Turn Off the Music 351 11 Gender and Sexuality STDs and Safer Sex — Choice, Risk, and Responsibility 378 357 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 386 WEB RESOURCES 387 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 387 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • THE CLINICAL FILE: Bruce or Brenda — Can Sex Be Assigned? 360 • BRAINWAVES: Genes, the Brain, and Sexual Orientation 362 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: High Test 365 • CRITICAL THINKING: Are We Oversexualizing Young Girls? 376 • CRITICAL THINKING: Gender Role Stereotyping and Rape 377 Preview: Pink and Blue 358 Sexual Development — Circle One: XX or XY? 358 Dimensions of Sex 358 Sexual Orientation — Who Do You Love? 361 Homosexuality 361 Gender Development — Circle One: Masculine or Feminine 363 Gender Identity 364 Gender Roles 364 Gender Role Socialization 366 Androgyny — Are You Masculine, Feminine, or Androgynous? 367 Psychological Androgyny 367 Sexual Behavior — Mapping the Erogenous Zones 369 Sexual Arousal 369 Human Sexual Response — Sexual Interactions 371 Comparing Male and Female Responses 372 Atypical Sexual Behavior — Trench Coats, Whips, Leathers, and Lace 373 Paraphilias 373 Attitudes and Sexual Behavior — The Changing Sexual Landscape 374 Is the Revolution Over? 375 The Crime of Rape 377 12 Personality 388 Preview: The Hidden Essence 389 The Psychology of Personality — Do You Have Personality? 389 Traits 390 Do We Inherit Personality? 390 Types 391 Self-Concept 392 Personality Theories 393 The Trait Approach — Describe Yourself in 18,000 Words or Less 394 Predicting Behavior 394 Describing People 394 Classifying Traits 394 The Big Five 396 Traits, Consistency, and Situations 397 Psychoanalytic Theory — Id Came to Me in a Dream 398 The Structure of Personality 398 The Dynamics of Personality 399 Personality Development 400 The Neo-Freudians 402 Learning Theories of Personality — Habit I Seen You Before? 404 How Situations Affect Behavior 405 Personality Behavior 405 Social Learning Theory 406 Behavioristic View of Development 407 xiii xiv Contents Humanistic Theory — Peak Experiences and Personal Growth 408 Maslow and Self-Actualization 409 Positive Psychology: Positive Personality Traits 410 Carl Rogers’ Self Theory 410 Humanistic View of Development 412 Personality Theories — Overview and Comparison 413 Personality Assessment — Psychological Yardsticks 414 Conflict — Yes, No, Yes, No, Yes, No, Well, Maybe 438 Managing Conflicts 439 Psychological Defense — Mental Karate? 440 Learned Helplessness — Is There Hope? 442 Depression 443 Depression: Why Students Get the Blues 444 Coping with Depression 445 Stress and Health — Unmasking a Hidden Killer 445 The Interview 414 Direct Observation and Rating Scales 415 Personality Questionnaires 416 Projective Tests of Personality — Inkblots and Hidden Plots 418 Sudden Murderers — A Research Example 419 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Barriers and Bridges — Understanding Shyness 420 Life Events and Stress 446 Psychosomatic Disorders 447 Biofeedback 448 The Cardiac Personality 450 Hardy Personality 451 Positive Psychology: Hardiness, Optimism, and Happiness 452 The Value of Social Support 452 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Stress Management 453 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 422 WEB RESOURCES 423 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 457 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 424 WEB RESOURCES 458 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • CRITICAL THINKING: The Amazing Twins 391 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 458 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Self-Esteem and Culture — Hotshot or Team Player? 393 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: What’s Your Musical Personality? 395 • THE CLINICAL FILE: Perfectly Miserable 397 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Unhealthy Birds of a Feather 429 • THE CLINICAL FILE: Burnout — The High Cost of Caring 433 • THE CLINICAL FILE: Coping with Traumatic Stress 435 • THE CLINICAL FILE: Telling Stories About Ourselves 412 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Acculturative Stress — Stranger in a Strange Land 448 13 Health, Stress, and Coping 425 Preview: Jennifer’s Amazing Race 426 Health Psychology — Here’s to Your Good Health 426 Behavioral Risk Factors 426 Health-Promoting Behaviors 427 Early Prevention 429 Community Health 430 Positive Psychology: Wellness 430 Stress — Thrill or Threat? 430 General Adaptation Syndrome 431 Stress, Illness, and Your Immune System 431 When Is Stress a Strain? 432 Appraising Stressors 433 Coping with Threat 434 Frustration — Blind Alleys and Lead Balloons 435 Reactions to Frustration 436 Coping with Frustration 437 • CRITICAL THINKING: It’s All in Your Mind 449 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Feeling Stressed? You’ve Got a Friend 452 14 Psychological Disorders 459 Preview: Beware the Helicopters 460 Normality — What’s Normal? 460 Core Features of Disordered Behavior 462 Insanity 462 Classifying Mental Disorders — Problems by the Book 463 An Overview of Psychological Disorders 464 General Risk Factors 466 Psychotic Disorders — The Dark Side of the Moon 467 The Nature of Psychosis 468 Contents Delusional Disorders — An Enemy Behind Every Tree 470 Paranoid Psychosis 470 Schizophrenia — Shattered Reality 470 Disorganized Schizophrenia 471 Catatonic Schizophrenia 471 Paranoid Schizophrenia 472 Undifferentiated Schizophrenia 472 The Causes of Schizophrenia 472 Implications 476 Mood Disorders — Peaks and Valleys 476 Major Mood Disorders 477 What Causes Mood Disorders? 478 Anxiety-Based Disorders — When Anxiety Rules 480 Adjustment Disorders 480 Anxiety Disorders 481 Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder 482 Stress Disorders 483 Dissociative Disorders 484 Somatoform Disorders 484 Anxiety and Disorder — Four Pathways to Trouble 486 Psychodynamic Approach 486 Humanistic-Existential Approaches 486 Behavioral Approach 486 Cognitive Approach 487 Personality Disorders — Blueprints for Maladjustment 487 Maladaptive Personality Patterns 487 Antisocial Personality 487 Disorders in Perspective — Psychiatric Labeling 489 Social Stigma 489 A Look Ahead 490 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Suicide — Lives on the Brink 490 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 493 WEB RESOURCES 494 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 494 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Crazy for a Day 461 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Running Amok with Cultural Maladies 466 • CRITICAL THINKING: Are the Mentally Ill Prone to Violence? 473 15 Therapies 495 Preview: The Duck Syndrome 496 Psychotherapy — The Talking Cure 496 Dimensions of Therapy 496 Origins of Therapy — Bored Out of Your Skull 497 Psychoanalysis — Expedition into the Unconscious 498 Psychoanalysis Today 499 Humanistic Therapies — Restoring Human Potential 500 Client-Centered Therapy 500 Existential Therapy 501 Gestalt Therapy 501 Therapy at a Distance — Psych Jockeys and Cybertherapy 502 Media Psychologists 502 Telephone Therapists 502 Internet Therapy 503 Behavior Therapy — Healing by Learning 503 Aversion Therapy 504 Desensitization 505 Operant Therapies — All the World Is a Skinner Box? 508 Nonreinforcement and Extinction 508 Reinforcement and Token Economies 509 Cognitive Therapy — Think Positive! 510 Cognitive Therapy for Depression 510 Rational-Emotive Behavior Therapy 510 Group Therapy — People Who Need People 512 Psychodrama 512 Family and Couples Therapy 513 Group Awareness Training 513 Psychotherapy — An Overview 514 Core Features of Psychotherapy 514 The Future of Psychotherapy 515 Basic Counseling Skills 516 Medical Therapies — Psychiatric Care 518 Drug Therapies 518 Electrical Stimulation Therapy 519 Psychosurgery 519 Hospitalization 520 Community Mental Health Programs 521 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Self-Management and Finding Professional Help 522 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 526 • BRAINWAVES: The Schizophrenic Brain 475 WEB RESOURCES 528 • THE CLINICAL FILE: Sick of Being Sick 484 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 528 • CRITICAL THINKING: A Disease Called Freedom 489 xv xvi Contents FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Feeling a Little Tense? Relax! 506 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Assertiveness Training — Standing Up for Your Rights 551 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 553 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Ten Irrational Beliefs — Which Do You Hold? 511 WEB RESOURCES 555 • THE CLINICAL FILE: Overcoming the Gambler’s Fallacy 512 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • CRITICAL THINKING: Touch and Status 532 • CRITICAL THINKING: How Do We Know Therapy Actually Works? 514 • THE CLINICAL FILE: Self-Handicapping — Smoke Screen for Failure 534 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Therapy and Culture — A Bad Case of “Ifufunyane” 517 • CRITICAL THINKING: Groupthink — Agreement at Any Cost 543 16 Social Thinking and Social Influence • CRITICAL THINKING: How to Drive a Hard Bargain 546 529 Preview: Six Degrees of Separation 530 Humans in a Social Context — People, People, Everywhere 530 Roles 530 Group Structure and Cohesion 531 Social Cognition — Behind the Mask 533 Attribution Theory 533 Actor and Observer 534 Attitudes — Belief Emotion Action 535 Forming Attitudes 536 Attitudes and Behavior 536 Attitude Measurement 537 Attitude Change — Why the Seekers Went Public 537 Persuasion 538 Cognitive Dissonance Theory 538 Social Influence — Follow the Leader 540 Social Power 540 Mere Presence — Just Because You Are There 541 Social Facilitation and Loafing 541 Personal Space 541 Spatial Norms 542 Conformity — Don’t Stand Out 542 The Asch Experiment 543 Group Factors in Conformity 544 Compliance — A Foot in the Door 544 Passive Compliance 545 Obedience — Would You Electrocute a Stranger? 547 Milgram’s Obedience Studies 547 Coercion — Brainwashing and Cults 549 Brainwashing 549 Cults 549 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 555 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Quack Like a Duck 548 17 Prosocial and Antisocial Behavior 556 Preview: Love and Hate 557 The Need for Affiliation — Come Together 557 Social Comparison Theory 557 Interpersonal Attraction — Social Magnetism? 558 Physical Proximity 558 Physical Attractiveness 559 Competence 559 Similarity 559 Self-Disclosure 560 Social Exchange Theory 560 Liking and Loving — Dating, Rating, Mating 561 Love and Attachment 562 Evolution and Mate Selection 562 Helping Others — The Good Samaritan 564 Bystander Intervention 564 Who Will Help Whom? 565 Positive Psychology: Everyday Heroes 566 Aggression — The World’s Most Dangerous Animal 566 Instincts 567 Biology 567 Frustration 567 Social Learning 568 The World According to TV 568 Preventing Aggression 570 Prejudice — Attitudes That Injure 571 Becoming Prejudiced 572 The Prejudiced Personality 573 Intergroup Conflict — The Roots of Prejudice 573 Experiments in Prejudice 576 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Multiculturalism — Living with Diversity 579 Contents CHAPTER IN REVIEW 581 WEB RESOURCES 582 PSYCHOLOGY IN ACTION: Human Factors Psychology —Who’s the Boss Here? 607 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 583 CHAPTER IN REVIEW 610 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) WEB RESOURCES 611 • CRITICAL THINKING: Pornography and Aggression Against Women — Is There a Link? 569 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 611 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: I’m Not Prejudiced, Right? 572 • CRITICAL THINKING: From Glass Ceiling to Labyrinth 587 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Choking on Stereotypes 574 • THE CLINICAL FILE: Desk Rage and Healthy Organizations 590 • CRITICAL THINKING: Terrorists, Enemies, and Infidels 575 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Is America Purple? 576 FEATURE BOXES (HIGHLIGHTS) • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Surviving Your Job Interview 591 • CRITICAL THINKING: Territoriality 594 18 Applied Psychology 584 Preview: Insanely Great 585 Industrial/Organizational Psychology — Psychology at Work 585 Theories of Leadership 585 Theory X and Theory Y Leadership 586 Job Satisfaction 588 Job Enrichment 589 Organizational Culture 589 Personnel Psychology 590 Job Analysis 590 Selection Procedures 590 Environmental Psychology — Life on Spaceship Earth 593 Environmental Influences 595 Stressful Environments 595 Toxic Environments 597 Sustainable Lifestyles 597 Social Dilemmas 598 Environmental Problem Solving 600 Conclusion 600 Educational Psychology — An Instructive Topic 601 Elements of a Teaching Strategy 601 Psychology and Law — Judging Juries 602 Jury Behavior 602 Jury Selection 603 Sports Psychology — The Athletic Mind 604 Motor Skills 606 Positive Psychology: Peak Performance 606 • DISCOVERING PSYCHOLOGY: Reuse and Recycle 599 • HUMAN DIVERSITY: Peanut Butter for the Mind: Designing Education for Everyone 602 • CRITICAL THINKING: Death-Qualified Juries 604 Appendix: Behavioral Statistics 612 Preview: Statistics from “Heads” to “Tails” 613 Descriptive Statistics — Psychology by the Numbers 613 Graphical Statistics 613 Measures of Central Tendency 614 Measures of Variability 615 Standard Scores 616 The Normal Curve 616 Correlation — Rating Relationships 618 Relationships 618 The Correlation Coefficient 618 Inferential Statistics — Significant Numbers 620 Samples and Populations 621 Significant Differences 621 APPENDIX IN REVIEW 622 WEB RESOURCES 622 INTERACTIVE LEARNING 622 Glossary G-1 References R-1 Name Index N-1 Subject Index S-1 xvii This page intentionally left blank preface to the twelfth edition An Invitation to the Student Greetings from your authors. We are delighted to be your guides as you explore the exciting field of psychology. We hope you will find that psychology is at once familiar, exotic, surprising, and challenging. What, really, could be more intriguing than our evolving understanding of human behavior? Psychology is about each of us. It invites us to adopt a reflective attitude as we ask, “How can we step outside ourselves to look objectively at how we live, think, feel, and act?” Psychologists believe the answer is through intelligent thought, observation, and inquiry. As simple as that might seem, careful reflection takes practice to develop. It is the guiding light for all that follows. Reading Gateways to Mind and Behavior We trust you will find much that interests you in this book. To make your reading enjoyable, we tried to write as if we were talking with you. And to add to your interest, we will often invite you to relate psychology to your own experiences. But make no mistake, Gateways to Mind and Behavior is a sophisticated textbook that offers you an up-to-date introduction to psychology. At the beginning of each chapter you will find a list of Gateway Questions to guide your reading. As you read a chapter, try to see if you can discover the answers to these questions. Then compare your answers with the ones listed in the chapter summary. The answers are what we think of as Gateway concepts. In other words, they open intellectual pathways and summarize psychology’s “big ideas.” Although you don’t need to memorize the Gateway concepts, you can use them to review the most important points in each chapter. Ultimately, the Gateway concepts will provide a good summary of what you learned in this course. If you remember most of them 10 years after you finish reading this book, you will make us very happy, indeed. Studying Gateways to Mind and Behavior None of us likes to start a new adventure by reading a manual. We want to get right into a new computer game, step off the airplane and begin our vacation, or just start using our new camera or cell phone. You might be similarly tempted to just start reading this textbook. Please be patient. Successfully learning psychology depends on how you study this book, as well as how you read it. Gateways to Mind and Behavior is your passport to an active adventure in learning, not just passive reading. To help you get off to a good start, we strongly encourage you to read our short Introduction, which precedes Chapter 1. The Introduction describes study skills, including the SQ4R method, that you can use to get the most out of this text and your psychology course. It also tells how you can explore psychology through the Internet, electronic databases, and interactive CDs. Each chapter of this book will take you into a different realm of psychology, such as personality, abnormal behavior, memory, consciousness, and human development. Each realm is complex and fascinating in its own right. Gateways to Mind and Behavior is your passport to an adventure in learning. In a very real sense, we wrote it about you, for you, and to you. An Invitation to the Instructor Thank you for choosing this book for your students and your course. Marcel Proust wrote, “The real voyage of discovery consists not in seeing new landscapes but in having new eyes.” It is in this spirit that we encourage you to use this book’s special features to help change the way your students see human behavior. Accordingly, we have written this book to promote an interest in human behavior, including an appreciation of the practical applications of psychology, the richness of human diversity, and the field of positive psychology. At the same time, we have structured this book to help students learn efficiently and apply criticalthinking skills. Without such skills, students cannot easily go, as Jerome Bruner put it, “beyond the information given” (Bruner, 1973). To help students read more effectively, we open every chapter with a list of Gateway Questions that students can use as powerful advance organizers for digesting new information (e.g., Ausubel, 1978). These questions are addressed throughout the chapter and are explicitly answered in the chapter summaries. Featured in these summaries are psychology’s Gateway concepts — the “take home” ideas every student should remember 10 years after reading this text. As a whole, they are capable of transforming the way students view human behavior. Now widely emulated, earlier editions of Gateways to Mind and Behavior revolutionized textbooks by using psychology to help students learn more effectively. We continue that tradition of innovation in this edition. We have again updated our presentation of the SQ4R method to better promote active learning, longterm retention of ideas, and the reflective attitude that lies at the heart of critical thinking. In the previous edition we replaced the “Relate” step of the SQ4R method with “Reflect,” so that SQ4R now refers to Survey, Question, Read, Recite, Reflect, and Review. In every chapter, we have refined the Reflect step of the SQ4R method to strengthen connections among learning, elaborative processing, and critical thinking (Gadzella, 1995). For example, we have redesigned the chapter pedagogy to make it even clearer to students why it is valuable to engage in reflection while reading. xix xx Preface to the Twelfth Edition Readability and Narrative Emphasis Selecting a textbook is half the battle in teaching a successful course. When a book overwhelms students or cools their interest, teaching and learning suffer. A good text does much of the work of imparting information to your students. This frees class time for your discussion, extra topics, or media presentations. It also leaves students asking for more. Many introductory psychology students are reluctant readers. No matter how interesting a text may be, its value is lost if students fail to read it. That’s why we’ve worked hard to make this a clear, readable, and engaging text. We want students to read this book with genuine interest and enthusiasm, not merely as an obligation. To encourage students to read, we made a special effort to weave narrative threads through every chapter. Everyone loves a good story, and the story of psychology is among the most compelling to be told. Throughout Gateways to Mind and Behavior, we have used intriguing anecdotes and examples to propel reading and sustain interest. As students explore concepts, they are encouraged to think about ideas and relate them to current events and their own experiences. Practical Applications Gateways to Mind and Behavior is designed to give students a clear grasp of major concepts without burying them in details. At the same time, it offers a broad overview that reflects psychology’s rich heritage of ideas. We think students will find this book informative and intellectually stimulating. Moreover, we have emphasized the many ways that psychology relates to practical problems in daily life. A major feature of this book is the Psychology in Action section found at the end of each chapter. These high-interest discussions bridge the gap between theory and practical application. We believe it is fair for students to ask, “Does this mean anything to me? Can I use it? Why should I learn it if I can’t?” The Psychology in Action features show students how to solve problems and manage their own behavior. This allows them to see the benefits of adopting new ideas, and it breathes life into psychology’s concepts. An Integrated Study Guide The chapters of this text are divided into short segments by special sections called Knowledge Builders. These “mini study guides” challenge students to relate concepts to their own experiences, to quiz themselves, and to think critically about the principles they are learning. For this edition, we have reorganized the Knowledge Builders into Recite and Reflect sections, to better mirror the SQ4R method. Recite questions are somewhat easier than inclass test questions and are designed provide immediate feedback to students. Reflect questions come in two “flavors.” Critical Thinking questions encourage critical reflection and come with answers. Relate questions are open-ended invitations to students to elaborate on just-read material by relating it to their personal experiences. If students would like even more feedback and practice, Chapter Quizzes are available in a free booklet, Gateways to Mind and Behavior: Concept Maps and Concept Reviews; a traditional Study Guide is available; and students can use a web-based course-management tool called WebTutor™ to take online quizzes or to practice with electronic flash cards. Gateways to Mind and Behavior: Concept Maps and Concept Reviews accompanies every new copy of the text and also includes Gateway concepts for every chapter, concept maps of key concepts, and concept reviews (consisting of a 30-item multiple-choice quiz for each chapter). This useful booklet is available to qualified adopters; please consult your local sales representative for details. Electronic Resources To encourage further exploration, students will find a section called Web Resources at the end of each chapter. The websites described there offer a wealth of information on topics related to psychology. All chapters include a list of relevant modules in PsychNow! 2.0. This excellent CD-ROM from Wadsworth provides students with a rich assortment of interactive learning experiences, animations, and simulations. On the web, students can visit this text’s Book Companion Website, where they will find quizzes, a final exam, chapter-bychapter web links, flash cards, an audio glossary, and more (www. cengage.com/psychology/coon). Students can also make use of CengageNOW for Coon and Mitterer’s Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior, Twelfth Edition, a web-based, personalized study system that provides a pretest and a posttest for each chapter and separate chapter quizzes. CengageNOW for Coon and Mitterer’s Introduction to Psychology, Twelfth Edition, can also create personalized study plans — which include rich media such as videos, animations, and learning modules — that point students to areas in the text that will help them master course content. An additional set of integrative questions helps students pull all the material together. Human Diversity Today’s students reflect the multicultural, multifaceted nature of contemporary society. In Gateways to Mind and Behavior, students will find numerous discussions of human diversity, including differences in race, ethnicity, culture, gender, abilities, sexual orientation, and age. Too often, such differences needlessly divide people into opposing groups. Our aim throughout this text is to discourage stereotyping, prejudice, discrimination, and intolerance. We’ve tried to make this book gender neutral and sensitive to diversity issues. All pronouns and examples involving females and males are equally divided by gender. In artwork, photographs, and examples, we have tried to portray the rich diversity of humanity. In addition, a boxed feature, Human Diversity, appears throughout the book, providing students with examples of how to be more reflective about human diversity. In short, many topics and examples in this book encourage students to appreciate social, physical, and cultural differences and to accept them as a natural part of being human. Preface to the Twelfth Edition Positive Psychology In January 2000, Martin Seligman and Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi co-edited a special issue of American Psychologist devoted to optimal functioning, happiness, and “positive psychology.” Over the past 100 years, psychologists have paid ample attention to the negative side of human behavior. This is easy to understand because we urgently need to find remedies for human problems. However, Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi have urged us to also study positive psychology. What do we know, for instance, about love, happiness, creativity, well-being, self-confidence, and achievement? Throughout this book, we have attempted to answer such questions for students. Our hope is that students who read this book will gain an appreciation for the potential we all have for optimal functioning. Also, of course, we hope that they will leave introductory psychology with emotional and intellectual tools they can use to enhance their lives. How Chapter Features Support the SQ4R Method Introduction to Psycholog y was the first college text with an SQ4R, active-learning format. Through Dennis Coon’s pioneering efforts, this book has made learning psychology a rewarding experience for more than 2 million students. With their feedback and generous help from many professors, we have continued to refine the unique features of Gateways to Mind and Behavior. Notice how the steps of the SQ4R method — survey, question, read, recite, reflect, and review — are incorporated into the chapter design. Survey Features at the beginning of each chapter help students build cognitive maps of upcoming topics, thus serving as advance organizers. Students begin with a chapter Theme and a list of Gateway Questions that identify the main points students should search for as they read. Next, a short Preview arouses interest, gives an overview of the chapter, and focuses attention on the task at hand. These chapter-opening features encourage students to read with a purpose and actively process information. Question Throughout each chapter, frequent italicized Guide Questions also serve as advance organizers. That is, Guide Questions prompt students to look for important ideas as they read and thus promote active learning. They also establish a dialogue in which the questions and reactions of students are anticipated. This clarifies difficult points — in a lively give-and-take between questions and responses. Read We’ve made every effort to make this a clear, readable text. To further aid comprehension, we’ve used a full array of traditional learning aids. These include boldface terms (with phonetic pronunciations), bullet summaries, a robust illustration program, summary tables, a name index, a subject index, and a detailed glossary. As an additional aid, figure and table references in the text are marked with small geometric shapes. These “placeholders” make it easier for students to return to reading after they have paused to view a table or figure. xxi An Integrated Glossary aids reading comprehension by providing precise definitions directly in context. When important terms first appear, they are immediately defined. In this way, students get clear definitions when and where they need them — in the general text itself. In addition, a parallel Running Glossary defines key terms in the margins of pages. The Running Glossary makes it easier for students to find, study, and review important terms. Recite Every few pages, a Knowledge Builder gives students a chance to test their recall and understanding of preceding topics. As mentioned earlier, the Knowledge Builders are small, built-in study guides that include a Recite section (a short, noncomprehensive quiz), which helps students actively process information and assess their progress. Recite questions are not as difficult as in-class tests, and they are just a sample of what students could be asked about various topics. Students who miss any items are asked to backtrack and clarify their understanding before reading more. Completing Recite questions serves as a form of recitation to enhance learning. Reflect Cognitive psychology tells us that elaboration, the reflective processing of new information, is one of the best ways to foster understanding and form lasting memories (Anderson, 2005; Gadzella, 1995; Goldstein, 2008). The more students elaborate ideas as they read and study, the richer their understanding will be and the better they will remember new concepts. Self-reference, a particularly powerful form of elaboration, makes new information more meaningful by relating it to what is already known (Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986). Discovering Psychology boxes in this edition are “try-it” demonstrations that allow students to observe interesting facets of their own behavior or do self-assessment exercises. In this way, students can link new information to concrete experiences. To help students further elaborate their new understanding, each Knowledge Builder includes a series of Relate questions that encourage students to associate new concepts with personal experiences and prior knowledge. A course in psychology also naturally contributes to deeper forms of reflection, such as the development of critical-thinking abilities. As we described earlier, to further facilitate reflection, each Knowledge Builder also includes one or more Critical Thinking Questions to further facilitate reflection. These stimulating questions challenge students to think critically and analytically about psychology. Each is followed by a brief answer with which students can compare their own thoughts. Many of these answers are based on research and are informative in their own right. In addition, several boxed highlights encourage other forms of reflective thought. The Critical Thinking boxes model a reflective approach to critical thinking in psychology. In addition, Human Diversity boxes encourage reflection on the variability of human experience, The Clinical File boxes help students think about clinical applications of psychology, and Brainwaves boxes, which are new to this edition, encourage reflection on brain mechanisms that underlie psychological phenomena. Finally, Bridges appear throughout the text. Each Bridge links a topic under discussion to related information elsewhere in the xxii Preface to the Twelfth Edition book. This feature helps students reflect on the rich interconnection of ideas in contemporary psychology. Review As noted previously, all important terms appear in a Running Glossary throughout the book, which aids review. As also noted, a Psychology in Action section shows students how psychological concepts relate to practical problems, including problems in their own lives. The information found in Psychology in Action helps reinforce learning by illustrating psychology’s practicality. Next, a point-by-point summary provides a concise synopsis of all major concepts. The Chapter in Review summary is organized around answers to the Gateway Questions found at the beginning of the chapter. This brings the SQ4R process full circle and provides closure with respect to the learning objectives of each chapter. Critical Thinking The active, questioning nature of the SQ4R method is, in itself, an inducement to think critically. Many of the Guide Questions that introduce topics in the text act as models of critical thinking. So do the Critical Thinking, Human Diversity, Discovering Psychology, The Clinical File, and Brainwaves boxes. Further, Chapter 1 contains a discussion of critical-thinking skills and a rational appraisal of pseudo-psychologies. In addition, the research methods portion of Chapter 1 is basically a short course on how to think clearly about behavior. It is augmented by tips about how to critically evaluate claims in the popular media. Chapter 9, which covers intelligence, cognition, language, and creativity, includes many topics related to critical thinking. Taken together, these features will help students gain thinking skills of lasting value. Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior — What’s New? Thanks to psychology’s vitality, this text is improved in many ways. Most importantly, the book has been shortened to 18 chapters. Two developmental psychology chapters have been combined into one, as have two chapters on intelligence and cognition, language, and creativity. This major change is based on feedback suggesting that a 20-chapter book presents too much material to cover in most introductory courses. This edition also features an updated SQ4R system, outlined elsewhere in this Preface. The refinements strengthen the link between remembering new information and thinking reflectively and critically about psychology. Finally, the Twelfth Edition of Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior features some of the most recent and interesting information in psychology. Research references have been extensively updated, with fully two thirds of citations dated 2000 or newer. The following annotations highlight some of the new topics and features that appear in this edition. Learning Check questions are now Recite questions. A new section titled Reflect now includes Relate questions and Critical Thinking questions. This change is designed to make it clearer to students that relating new information to personal experiences and thinking critically about new ideas are both forms of reflective cognition. In addition, the Introduction shows students how to read effectively, study more efficiently, take good notes, prepare for tests, take tests, create study schedules, and avoid procrastination. Chapter 1: Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods • • • • • • • Chapter 2: Brain and Behavior • • • • • Introduction: The Psychology of Studying We have once again updated our SQ4R framework. The Knowledge Builders have been restructured so that students can more easily see how they relate to the SQ4R method. Specifically, Chapter 1 has been extensively reorganized and updated and begins with a more engaging chapter-opening vignette. Critical thinking is now covered early in the chapter and is immediately followed by a discussion of the scientific method. This placement permits students to more tightly link critical thinking and scientific thought. The history of psychology is now presented after a discussion of critical thinking and the scientific method. This arrangement gives students a fuller context for understanding historical changes in psychology. Modern views on behavior have been reorganized into three major perspectives: biological, behavioral, and sociocultural. A new Critical Thinking highlight, “That’s Interesting, but Is It Ethical?” provides increased coverage of research ethics in psychology. The material on placebo effects and the experimenter effect has been rewritten to include both research participant bias and experimenter bias. Placebo effects are treated as a form of research participant bias. The experimental method is now explored first, before other research methods. Nonexperimental methods (case studies, surveys, correlations, and natural observation) are then addressed as alternative research strategies. • Chapter 2 has been enlarged to reflect the growing importance of neuroscience in contemporary psychology. Additional neuroscience content can also be found in subsequent chapters (most noticeably in the new Brainwaves boxes). The section on neural transmission now includes information about myelin and saltatory conduction. The explanation of neural networks has been expanded and includes a new explanatory figure. Artwork illustrating the structure of the nervous system has been unified for greater simplicity and clarity. Coverage of the spinal cord, which concludes with an appraisal of the possibility of spinal cord repair, is now followed by a Critical Thinking box, “Brain Repair.” The discussion of neuroscience research methods has been rewritten to more clearly distinguish ablation from electrical stimulation. Preface to the Twelfth Edition • • The lobes of the cerebral cortex are covered in a new order, and the entire topic has been revised, including an updated figure. A new Critical Thinking box, “Mirror, Mirror, in the Brain,” discusses the discovery of mirror neurons and their possible significance. Chapter 3: Human Development • • • • Chapter 3 combines major topics from Child Development (Chapter 3 of the previous edition) and Human Development (Chapter 4 of the previous edition). The resulting chapter moves from infancy through childhood to adulthood. Material on heredity and environmental influences has been reorganized and streamlined. Most notably, maturation is now treated in the heredity section, along with readiness. The section on the newborn has been reorganized for greater clarity. The discussion of Piaget’s theory has also been reorganized. Transformations are now introduced in a subsection about the preoperational stage. Simple exercises have been added to make it easier for students to assimilate the concepts of internal representations and transformations. Chapter 4: Sensation and Reality • • • • • • • Chapter 4 begins with a new vignette about echolocation by bats that illustrates the limits of our senses. For greater clarity, the first section has been restructured to combine the previously separated topics of sensory limits and psychophysics. A more helpful metaphorical comparison is now drawn between the visual system and digital cameras. Material on blindsight is now presented in a Brainwaves box, “Blindsight: The ‘What’ and the ‘Where’ of Vision,” which has been expanded to include a new figure illustrating the dorsal and ventral visual pathways. A new Discovering Psychology box titled “Going Biosonar” harks back to the chapter-opening vignette about bat echolocation by describing a blind human teenager who echolocates. Students are given instructions about how to navigate by echolocation. The discussion of hearing loss has been revised to follow current terminology (i.e., sensorineural hearing loss and conductive hearing loss). The material on gustation and olfaction has been rewritten. In particular, anosmia is now referred to as dysosmia. • • • xxiii Because attention is an early stage of the perceptual process, we now discuss it earlier in the chapter. Student feedback suggests that it is easier to understand perceptual organization before tackling perceptual constancies. Accordingly, Gestalt principles are now covered before perceptual constancies are introduced. The section on depth perception has also been reorganized and streamlined. In particular, depth cues are now separated into binocular and monocular cues. Muscular depth cues have been omitted as a separate category because the heading mixed a binocular cue (convergence) with a monocular one (accommodation). Chapter 6: States of Consciousness • • • • • • • Chapter 6 features revised coverage of sleep and dreaming. The dual process hypothesis, which assigns different functions to NREM and REM sleep, is now discussed. The treatment of dreaming has been updated to include Domhoff ’s neurocognitive theory as a complement to psychodynamic and activation-synthesis theories. The section on hypnosis has also been rewritten for greater clarity. Meditation is now treated in a section along with REST (restricted environmental stimulation therapy), and both are related to the relaxation response. A new Brainwaves box, “How Drugs Affect the Brain,” was fashioned from existing material and includes a new figure. Statistics have been updated throughout the chapter, such as those concerning rates of drug abuse and the percentage of accidents related to sleepiness. Chapter 7: Conditioning and Learning • • • • Chapter 7 opens with an engaging new vignette about acquiring a phobia. This example better highlights differences between associative and cognitive learning, a distinction that is also more clearly reflected throughout the chapter. The section on classical conditioning has been streamlined for greater clarity. A new Brainwaves box, “Tickling Your Own Fancy,” updates coverage of the self-stimulation phenomenon. Are animals aware of the past? A new Critical Thinking box, “Are Animals Stuck in Time?” explores this interesting question and also shows how operant conditioning can be used as a research technique. Chapter 8: Memory Chapter 5: Perceiving the World • • Chapter 5 has been extensively reorganized to allow students to draw sharper distinctions between sensation and perception. The first section in the chapter explains in more detail why perception is a necessary extra step beyond sensation, and it includes a discussion of bottom-up and top-down processing. • • • The three-store memory model is now clearly labeled as the Atkinson-Schiffrin model. The term “dual memory” has been eliminated to avoid confusing students. The term “elaborative rehearsal” has been changed to “elaborative encoding” to better tie elaboration to encoding, one of the three fundamental memory processes. xxiv • • • • • Preface to the Twelfth Edition A new Critical Thinking box, “Do You Like Jam with Your Memory?” shows students how advertisers try to manipulate memory. It also serves as an interesting example of (re)constructive processing. Discussions of exceptional memory and strategies for improving memory have been extensively reorganized and rewritten. To help students remember why we forget, theories of forgetting are now organized into three categories: encoding failure, storage failure, and retrieval failure. Material on the neuroscience of memory has been reorganized. In particular, flashbulb memories are now included in a section on memory and emotion and linked to the role the amygdala plays in processing emotional memories. The section on improving memory has been reorganized into two subsections that tell students how they can improve encoding and retrieval. Chapter 9: Intelligence, Cognition, Language, and Creativity • • • • This chapter is a condensation of Cognition, Language, and Creativity (Chapter 10 of the previous edition) and Intelligence (Chapter 11 of the previous edition). The chapter has also been streamlined for greater interest and readability. The distinction between multiple intelligences and intelligence as a g-factor is given more prominence. The coverage of artificial intelligence has been rewritten and is now included in the section on intelligence. • • Chapter 12: Personality • • • • • • • • • Chapter 10 opens with a new vignette featuring an interesting case study of alexithymia. The topic of hunger has been restructured into major sections that discuss internal and external factors affecting eating. A new Brainwaves box, “Your Brain’s ‘Fat Point,’” discusses setpoints and the role of leptin in hunger and feeding. The material on eating disorders has been reworked and now includes information about eating disorders in men. A new Discovering Psychology box, “Behavioral Dieting,” more clearly invites students to apply behavioral principles to control their own eating behavior. To help students better organize their understanding, the Psychology in Action section on emotional intelligence has been reorganized around a revised definition of emotional intelligence. Hereditary influences on personality have been moved forward into the first section of this chapter. A new The Clinical File box, “Telling Stories about Ourselves,” explores narrative psychology and personality change. Information about when personality “hardens” has been rewritten. Direct and indirect aggression are clearly differentiated in the discussion of personality and gender. Chapter 13: Health, Stress, and Coping • • • • A new table updates information about the top ten stressors. Material on the general adaptation syndrome and psychoneuroimmunology now appears at the beginning of this chapter. A new Discovering Psychology box, “Unhealthy Birds of a Feather,” presents some new data on the social transmission of healthy and unhealthy behaviors. The Critical Thinking box, “It’s All in Your Mind,” has been rewritten. Chapter 14: Psychological Disorders • Chapter 10: Motivation and Emotion • Material on gender development is now presented in a single section of the chapter. A new Human Diversity box, “High Test,” discusses how testosterone affects men. • • • • • Chapter 14 has been reorganized for greater clarity. Specifically, the presentation of disorders has been rearranged. We now begin with psychotic disorders (to lead with a malady that is dramatic and highly interesting to students), and follow with mood disorders, anxiety disorders, and personality disorders. This ordering is easier for students to understand, and it matches the order of topics provided at the beginning of the chapter. A new Brainwaves box, “The Schizophrenic Brain,” better highlights for students some differences between schizophrenic and normal brains. The Human Diversity box, “Running Amok with Cultural Maladies,” has been updated. A new The Clinical File box, titled “Sick of Being Sick,” introduces students to the fascinating topic of Munchausen syndrome by proxy. The discussion of insanity now occurs near the front of the chapter, where it makes better connections with related topics. The Critical Thinking box, “The Politics of Madness,” has been repositioned to the end of the chapter. Chapter 11: Gender and Sexuality • • • The flow of topics in this chapter has been reorganized. Information about sexual development has been moved to the beginning of the chapter, which helps clarify the sequence of sexual development. Sexual orientation is discussed earlier in the chapter and is characterized as an intrinsic biological component of sexual development. Chapter 15: Therapies • • • • Chapter 15 has received a relatively light update. The section on cybertherapy has been rewritten and updated. A new description of interpersonal psychotherapy (IPT) has been added. The coverage of somatic therapies includes a discussion of electrical stimulation of the brain (other than ECT). Preface to the Twelfth Edition • A new The Clinical File box, “Overcoming the Gambler’s Fallacy,” introduces students to the problem of compulsive gambling and relates it to cognitive therapies. xxv The Psychology in Action section has been rewritten to introduce students to human factors, with a focus on the study of human–computer interaction. Chapter 16: Social Thinking and Social Influence • • • • • • The two social psychology chapters (Chapters 16 and 17) have been extensively reorganized. Chapter titles have changed accordingly. Attribution (from the previous Chapter 16), attitudes, and attitude change (from the old Chapter 17) are discussed together, as forms of social thinking. The remaining topics from the former Chapter 16 now appear as an extended discussion of social influence, presented in order from mild to strong forms of influence. The effect of mere presence is now discussed, as are social facilitation and social loafing. Invasions of personal space are reinterpreted as a form of mere presence. The presentation of social influence then moves from conformity to compliance and obedience. Coercion, which is the final topic in this series, is composed mainly of material on brainwashing and cults from the old Chapter 17. Chapter 17: Prosocial and Antisocial Behavior • • • • • As the new chapter title implies, Chapter 17 now focuses on prosocial and antisocial behavior. The presentation of prosocial behavior has been broadened to include affiliation, liking, and loving, as well as a willingness to help strangers. An exploration of affiliation, liking, and loving now opens this chapter. Robert Sternberg’s triangular theory of love is now included because it is highly interesting to students. A new Human Diversity box, “Purple America,” invites students to think beyond stereotyping American citizens as either “red” or “blue” (Republican or Democrat). Chapter 18: Applied Psychology • • • • • • The chapter-opening vignette now discusses how human factors have contributed to the iPod’s success, a topic of great interest to students. The section on industrial/organizational psychology has been rewritten and restructured and now begins with the topic of leadership. A new Critical Thinking box, “From Glass Ceiling to Labyrinth,” describes the challenges women face as business leaders. Material on flextime now includes a discussion of compressed workweeks and telecommuting. A new Discovering Psychology box, “Surviving Your Job Interview,” offers concrete advice to students as they look for jobs. The discussion of environmental psychology has been revised and updated to include information about carbon footprints. A Complete Course — Teaching and Learning Supplements A rich array of supplements accompanies Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior, including several that make use of the latest technologies. These supplements are designed to make teaching and learning more effective. Many are available free to professors or students. Others can be packaged with this text at a discount. Student Support Materials Introductory students must learn a multitude of abstract concepts, which can make a first course in psychology difficult. The materials listed here will greatly improve students’ chances for success. Gateways to Psychology: Concept Maps and Concept Reviews Concept Maps created by Christine M. Vanchella and quiz items updated by Shawn Talbot, Kellogg Community College. For each chapter of the text, this booklet includes the Gateway concepts, a concept map, and a 35-item multiple-choice practice exam (ISBN: 0-49582929-3). Study Guide The Study Guide, written by Thuy Karafa of Ferris State University and Dennis Coon, is an invaluable student resource. It contains a variety of study tools, including: Chapter Overviews, Recite and Review (fill-in-the-blank), Connections (matching), Check Your Memory (true/false), Final Survey and Review (fill-inthe-blank), and Mastery Test (ISBN: 0-495-80429-0). Careers in Psychology: Opportunities in a Changing World, Second Edition This informative booklet, written by Tara L. Kuther, is a Wadsworth exclusive. The pamphlet describes the field of psychology, as well as how to prepare for a career in psychology. Career options and resources are also discussed. Careers in Psychology can be packaged with this text at no additional cost to students (ISBN: 0-495-09078-6). Multimedia CD-ROMs Interactive CD-ROMs make it possible for students to directly experience some of the phenomena they are studying. The following CDs from Wadsworth provide a wealth of engaging modules and exercises. PsychNow!™ Interactive Experiences in Psychology 2.0 This exciting CD-ROM was created by Joel Morgovsky, Lonnie Yandell, Elizabeth Lynch, and project consultant Dennis Coon. At the end of each chapter of this text, students will find a list of PsychNow! modules they can access for additional, “hands-on” learning. PsychNow! provides stunning graphics and animations, interesting video clips, interactive exercises, and web links, bringing psychology to life. With PsychNow!, students can do more than just read xxvi Preface to the Twelfth Edition about a topic — they can read, watch, listen, react, and reflect on the meaning of their own responses. PsychNow!, which is available for Macintosh and Windows, contains 39 fully interactive modules that will enhance their understanding, 8 “Interact Now” Collaborative Labs, and “Quiz Game Now” quizzes. Students can also conduct 15 different “Interactive Research Experiments” in areas such as neurocognition, perception, memory, concepts, and imagery. PsychNow! 2.0 can be packaged with this text for a discount; contact your sales representative for details (ISBN: 0-53459046-2). Sniffy™ the Virtual Rat, Lite Version 2.0 There’s no better way to master the basic principles of learning than working with a real laboratory rat. However, this is usually impractical in introductory psychology courses. Sniffy the Virtual Rat offers a fun, interactive alternative to working with lab animals. This innovative and entertaining software teaches students about operant and classical conditioning by allowing them to condition a virtual rat. Users begin by training Sniffy to press a bar to obtain food. Then they progress to studying the effects of reinforcement schedules and simple classical conditioning. In addition, special “Mind Windows” enable students to visualize how Sniffy’s experiences in the Skinner Box produce learning. The Sniffy CD-ROM includes a Lab Manual that shows students how to set up various operant and classical conditioning experiments. Sniffy™ the Virtual Rat, Lite Version 2.0 may be packaged with this text for a discount (ISBN: 0-534-63357-9). Online Resources The Internet is providing new ways to exchange information and enhance education. In psychology, Wadsworth is at the forefront in making use of this exciting technology. Book Companion Website As users of this text, you and your students will have access to the Book Companion Website for Introduction to Psychology at www.cengage.com/psychology/coon. Access is free and no pin is required. This outstanding site features chapterby-chapter online tutorial quizzes, a final exam, chapter-by-chapter web links, flash cards, and more! Th is web -ba se d study a id , by Christopher B. Mayhorn, North Carolina State University, helps your students discover the areas of text where they need to focus their efforts through a series of diagnostic pretests and posttests, personalized study plans — which include rich media such as videos, animations, and learning modules — that parallel the modules in the book, eBook files, and other integrated media elements. New to this edition, students have access to the Psychology Resource Center included with CengageNow. The new Psychology Resource Center features powerful teaching and learning tools, bringing psychology to life with a full library of original and classic video clips plus interactive learning modules tied to all the topics covered in an introductory psychology course. Organized by topic, the Psychology Resource Center is easy to navigate by students to find learning resources, and instructors will also find it an amazing lecture resource to easily stream multimedia into their classrooms (ISBN: 0-495-80682-X). WebTutor This online supplement helps students succeed by taking them into an environment rich with study and mastery tools, communication aids, and additional course content. For students, WebTutor offers real-time access to a full array of study tools, including videos, animations, flash cards (with audio), practice quizzes and tests, online tutorials, exercises, asynchronous discussion, a whiteboard, and an integrated e-mail system. Students will also have integrated access to the Newbury House Online Dictionary, an interactive dictionary that gives users instant access to definitions (including audio pronunciations). Professors can use WebTutor to offer virtual office hours, post syllabi, set up threaded discussions, track student progress on quizzes, and more. You can customize the content of WebTutor in any way you choose, including uploading images and other resources, adding web links, and creating course-specific practice materials (WebTutor on WebCT, ISBN: 0-495-80690-0; WebTutor on Blackboard, ISBN: 0-495-80691-9). Essential Teaching Resources As every professor knows, teaching an introductory psychology course is a tremendous amount of work. The supplements listed here should not only make life easier for you, they should also make it possible for you to concentrate on the more creative and rewarding facets of teaching. Instructor’s Resource Manual The Instructor’s Resource Manual, by Wanda McCarthy, Clermont College, University of Cincinnati, contains resources designed to streamline and maximize the effectiveness of your course preparation. In a three-ring binder format, this IRM is a treasure trove — from the introduction section, which includes a Resource Integration Guide, to a full array of chapter resources. Each chapter includes learning objectives, discussion questions, lecture enhancements, role-playing scenarios, “oneminute motivators,” broadening-our-cultural-horizons exercises, journal questions, suggestions for further reading, media suggestions, and web links (ISBN: 0-495-60314-7). Test Bank The Test Bank was prepared by Jeannette Murphey of Meridian Community College. It includes over 4,500 multiplechoice questions organized by chapter and by learning objectives. All items, which are classified as factual, conceptual, or applied, include correct answers and page references from the text. All questions new to this edition are identified by an asterisk (ISBN: 0-495-59916-6). PowerLecture with JoinIn and ExamView This one-stop digital library and presentation tool includes preassembled Microsoft® PowerPoint® lecture slides by Andrew Getzfeld, New York University. In addition to a full Instructor’s Manual and Test Bank, PowerLecture® also includes all videos from Wadsworth’s Psychology Digital Video Library 3.0, which can be easily integrated into PowerPoint® for more interactive presentations. PowerLecture® brings together all your media resources in one place, including an image library with graphics from the book itself, video clips, and more. PowerLecture® also includes ExamView® testing software with all the test items from the printed Test Bank in electronic format, enabling you to create customized tests in Preface to the Twelfth Edition print or online, as well as JoinIn™ Student Response System, offering instant assessment and better student results (ISBN: 0-49580676-5). Videotapes and Films Wadsworth offers a variety of videotapes and films to enhance classroom presentations. Many video segments in the Wadsworth collection pertain directly to major topics in this text, making them excellent lecture supplements. ABC Video for Introductory Psychology, Volume 1 and 2 Available to adopters, these ABC videos feature short, high-interest clips about current studies and research in psychology. These videos are perfect to start discussions or to enrich lectures. Topics include brain damage, measuring IQ, sleep patterns, obsessivecompulsive disorder, obedience to authority, rules of attraction and much more (Volume 1 ISBN: 0-495-50306-1; Volume 2 ISBN: 0-495-59637-X). Wadsworth Film and Video Library for Introductory Psychology Adopters can select from a variety of continually updated film and video options. The Wadsworth Film and Video Library includes selections from the Discovering Psychology series, the Annenberg series, and Films for Humanities. Contact your local sales representative or Wadsworth Marketing at 1-877-999-2350 for details. Psychology Digital Video Library Version 3.0 CD-ROM This CD-ROM contains a diverse selection of classic and contemporary clips, including “Little Albert,” the “Action Potential of a Neuron,” “Parts of the Brain,” and many more! The digital library offers a convenient way to access an appropriate clip for every lecture. An accompanying Digital Video Handbook offers a detailed description, approximate running time, and references to related media clips. It also offers objective quizzing and critical-thinking questions for each clip, as well as instructions on how to embed clips into your PowerPoint presentations. Available exclusively to instructors who adopt Wadsworth psychology texts (ISBN: 0495-09063-8). Wadsworth Media Guide for Introductory Psychology This essential instructor resource, edited by Russell J. Watson, contains hundreds of video and feature film recommendations for all major topics in introductory psychology (ISBN: 0-534-17585-6). Supplementary Books No text can cover all the topics that might be included in an introductory psychology course. If you would like to enrich your course, or make it more challenging, the Wadsworth titles listed here may be of interest. Challenging Your Preconceptions: Thinking Critically about Psychology, Second Edition This paperbound book (ISBN: 0-534-267394), written by Randolph Smith, helps students strengthen their critical-thinking skills. Psychological issues such as hypnosis and repressed memory, statistical seduction, the validity of pop psy- xxvii chology, and other topics arc used to illustrate the principles of critical thinking. Writing Papers in Psychology: A Student Guide The Sixth Edition of Writing Papers in Psychology (ISBN: 0-534-52395-1), by Ralph L. Rosnow and Mimi Rosnow, is a valuable “how to” manual for writing term papers and research reports. This new edition has been updated to reflect the latest APA guidelines. The book covers each task with examples, hints, and two complete writing samples. Citation ethics, how to locate information, and new research technologies are also covered. Cross-Cultural Perspectives in Psychology How well do the concepts of psychology apply to various cultures? What can we learn about human behavior from cultures different from our own? These questions lie behind a collection of original articles written by William F. Price and Rich Crapo. The Fourth Edition of CrossCultural Perspectives in Psychology (ISBN: 0-534-54653-6) contains articles on North American ethnic groups as well as cultures from around the world. Summary We sincerely hope that both teachers and students will consider this book and its supporting materials a refreshing change from the ordinary. Creating them has been quite an adventure. In the pages that follow, we believe students will find an attractive blend of the theoretical and the practical, plus many of the most exciting ideas in psychology. Most of all, we hope that students using this book will discover that reading a college textbook can be entertaining and enjoyable. Acknowledgments Psychology is a cooperative effort requiring the talents and energies of a large community of scholars, teachers, researchers, and students. Like most endeavors in psychology, this book reflects the efforts of many people. We deeply appreciate the contributions of the following professors who have, over the years, supported this text’s evolution: Faren R. Akins University of Arizona Nancy L. Ashton R. Stockton College of New Jersey Avis Donna Alexander John Tyler Community College Scott A. Bailey Texas Lutheran University Clark E. Alexander Arapahoe Community College Frank Barbehenn Bucks County Community College Tricia Alexander Long Beach City College Michael Bardo University of Kentucky Dennis Anderson Butler Community College Larry W. Barron Grand Canyon University Lynn Anderson Wayne State University Linda M. Bastone Purchase College, SUNY xxviii Preface to the Twelfth Edition Brian R. Bate Cuyahoga Community College Daniel B. Cruse University of Miami Hugh E. Bateman Jones Junior College Betty J. Daughenbaugh Wor-Wic Community College Evelyn Blanch-Payne Oakwood College Keith E. Davis University of South Carolina– Columbia Cheryl Bluestone Queensborough Community College–CUNY Diane DeArmond University of Missouri, Kansas City Galen V. Bodenhausen Michigan State University Patrick T. DeBoll St. John’s University Aaron U. Bolin Arkansas State University Dawn Delaney University of Wisconsin– Whitewater Tom Bond Thomas Nelson Community College John Boswell University of Missouri, St. Louis Anne Bright Jackson State Community College Soheila T. Brouk Gateway Technical College Derek Cadman El Camino Community College James F. Calhoun University of Georgia Jane Marie Cirillo Houston Community CollegeSoutheast Dennis Cogan Texas Tech University Lorry Cology Owens College Jack Demick Suffolk University Lorraine P. Dieudonne Foothill College H. Mitzi Doane University of Minnesota–Duluth Wendy Domjan University of Texas at Austin Roger A. Drake Western State College of Colorado John Dworetzky Glendale Community College Bill Dwyer Memphis State University Thomas Eckle Modesto Community College David Edwards Iowa State University Darlene Colson Norfolk State University Raymond Elish Cuyahoga Community College William N. Colson Norfolk State College Diane Feibel University of Cincinnati–Raymond Walters College Chris Cozby California State University, Fullerton Corinne Crandell Broome County Community College Thomas L. Crandell Broome County Community College Charles Croll Broome Community College Paul W. Fenton University of Wisconsin, Stout Dave Filak Joliet Junior College Gloria Fisher, Ph.D. Mississippi College Oney D. Fitzpatrick, Jr. Lamar University Linda E. Flickinger Saint Clair County Community College Callina Henson Oakland Community College– Auburn Hills William F. Ford Bucks County Community College Anne Hester Pennsylvania State University– Hazleton Campus Marie Fox Metropolitan State College of Denver Gregory P. Hickman The Pennsylvania State University– Fayette Chris Fraser Gippsland Institute of Advanced Education Don Hockenbury Tulsa Junior College Christopher Frost Southwest Texas State University Sidney Hockman Nassau Community College Eugenio J. Galindro El Paso Community College Barbara Honhart Lansing Community College Irby J. Gaudet University of Southwestern Louisiana John C. Johanson Winona State University David Gersh Houston Community College David A. Gershaw Arizona Western College Andrew R. Getzfeld New Jersey City University Carolyn A. Gingrich South Dakota State University Perilou Goddard Northern Kentucky University Michael E. Gorman Michigan Technological University Peter Gram Pensacola Junior College David A. Gries State University of New York, Farmingdale R.J. Grisham Indian River Community College John Grivas Monash University Anne Groves Montgomery College Michael B. Guyer John Carroll University Janice Hartgrove-Freile North Harris College Raquel Henry Kingwood College James A. Johnson Sam Houston State University Myles E. Johnson Normandale Community College Pat Jones Brevard Community College Richard Kandus Menifee Valley Campus Bruno M. Kappes University of Alaska–Anchorage Charles Karis Northeastern University John P. Keating University of Washington Patricia Kemerer Ivy Tech Community College Cindy Kennedy Sinclair Community College Shaila Khan Tougaloo College Richard R. Klene University of Cincinnati Ronald J. Kopcho Mercer Community College Mary Kulish Thomas Nelson Community College Billie Laney Central Texas College Preface to the Twelfth Edition xxix Phil Lau DeAnza College Beth Moore Madisonville Community College Steven J. Pollock Moorpark College Glenda Smith North Harris Community College Robert Lawyer Delgado Community College Feleccia R. Moore-Davis Houston Community College System Jack Powell University of Hartford Steven M. Smith Texas A&M University Ravi Prasad Texas Tech University Francine Smolucha Moraine Valley Community College Derrick L. Proctor Andrews University Michael C. Sosulski College of DuPage Douglas Pruitt West Kentucky Community College Lynn M. Sprott Jefferson Community College Robin Raygor Anoka-Ramsey Community College Donald M. Stanley North Harris County College Richard Rees Glendale Community College Julie E. Stokes California State University– Fullerton Walter Leach College of San Mateo Christopher Legrow Marshall University Lindette I. Lent Arizona Western College Elizabeth Levin Laurentian University Julie Lewis Georgian College Elise B. Lindenmuth York College of Pennsylvania Linda Lockwood Metropolitan State College of Denver Philip Lom West Connecticut State University Cheryl S. Lynch University of Louisiana–Lafayette Edward J. Morris Owensboro Community College Edward Mosley Pasiac County Community College James Murray San Jacinto University Gary Nallan University of North Carolina– Ashville Andrew Neher Cabrillo College Don Nelson Indiana State University Steve Nida Franklin University Peggy Norwood Tidewater Community College James P. B. O’Brien Tidewater Community College Salvador Macias, III University of South Carolina, Sumter Frances O’Keefe Tidewater Community College Abe Marrero Rogers State University Steve G. Ornelas Central Arizona College Al Mayer Portland Community College Laura Overstreet Tarrant County College Michael Jason McCoy Cape Fear Community College Darlene Pacheco Moorpark College Edward R. McCrary III El Camino College Lisa K. Paler College of New Rochelle Yancy B. McDougal University of South Carolina, Spartanburg Debra Parish Tomball College Paul A. Rhoads Williams College Harvey Richman Columbus State University Marcia Rossi Tuskegee University Kim Royster Tidewater Community College Jeffrey Rudski Mulhenberg College James J. Ryan University of Wisconsin, La Crosse John D. Sanders Butler County Community College Nancy Sauerman Kirkwood Community College Michael Schuller Fresno City College Pamela E. Scott-Johnson Spelman College Carol F. Shoptaugh Southwest Missouri State University Catherine Grady Strathern University of Cincinnati Harvey Taub Staten Island Community College Christopher Taylor University of Arizona Carol Terry University of Oklahoma Laura Thompson New Mexico State University Richard Townsend Miami-Dade Community College– Kendall Campus Bruce Trotter Santa Barbara City College Susan Troy Northeast Iowa Community College Pat Tuntland Pima College Paul E. Turner David Lipscomb University Mark McGee Texas A&M University Cora F. Patterson University of Southwestern Louisiana Harold I. Siegel Rutgers University Angela McGlynn Mercer County Community College Leon Peek North Texas State University Richard Siegel University of Massachusetts, Lowell Mark McKinley Lorain County Community College John Pennachio Adirondack Community College Nancy Simpson Trident Technical College Charles Verschoor Miami-Dade Community College– Kendall Campus Chelley Merrill Tidewater Community College Peter Phipps Sullivan County Community College Madhu Singh Tougaloo College Frank Vitro Texas Women’s University A.D. VanDeventer Thomas Nelson Community College Mark Vernoy Palomar College xxx Preface to the Twelfth Edition John Vojtisek Castleton State College Paul J. Wellman Texas A&M University Carl D. Williams University of Miami Michael Zeller Mankato State University Francis Volking Saint Leo University Sharon Whelan University of Kentucky Don Windham Roane State Community College Margaret C. Zimmerman Virginia Wesleyan College Margaret L. Walker Georgia State University Robert Wiley Montgomery College Kaye D. Young North Iowa Area Community College Otto Zinser East Tennessee State College David W. Ward Arkansas Tech University Thomas Wilke University of Wisconsin, Parkside Producing Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior and its supplements was a formidable task. We are especially indebted to each of the following individuals for supporting this book: At Cengage Learning, Susan Badger, Sean Wakely, Eve Howard, Vicki Knight, and Michele Sordi. We are also grateful to Jaime Perkins for his insight and support. Jaime has unmistakably contributed to this book. At home, Heather Mitterer, Kayleigh Hagerman, and Barbara Kushmier helped in oh so many ways. We also wish to thank the individuals at Cengage Learning who have so generously shared their knowledge and talents over the past year. These are the people who made it happen: Jeremy Judson, Vernon Boes, Pat Waldo, Rachel Guzman, Ileana Shevlin, and Kim Russell. It has been a pleasure to work with such a gifted group of professionals and many others at Cengage Learning. We especially want to thank Jeremy Judson. As our developmental editor, Jeremy has gracefully kept us on track and offered many deeply appreciated suggestions. Dennis Coon John O. Mitterer INTRODUCTION The Psychology of Studying Eric Audras/PhotoAlto/Jupiterimages Well Hello There! You’re actually reading this! As your authors, we’re impressed. Too often, students just jump in and read a textbook from the first assigned chapter to the last. That’s a shame because a textbook needs to be studied, not just read. Think about it: How much do you typically remember after you’ve read straight through a whole textbook chapter? If the answer is “Nada,” “Zilch,” or simply “Not enough,” it may be because reading a chapter is not really the same as studying it. Even if you’re an excellent student, you may be able to improve your study skills. Students who get good grades tend to work smarter, not just longer or harder (Santrock & Halonen, 2007). To help you get a good start, let’s look at several ways to improve studying. 1 INTRODUCTION © 1996 William Haefeli from cartoonbank.com. All Rights Reserved. 2 The SQ4R Method — How to Tame a Textbook looking at topic headings, figure captions, and summaries. Try to get an overall picture of what lies ahead. Because this book is organized into short sections, you can survey just one section at a time if you prefer. Q Question. As you read, turn each topic heading into one or more questions. For example, when you read the heading “Stages of Sleep” you might ask, “Is there more than one stage of sleep?” “What are the stages of sleep?” “How do they differ?” Asking questions helps you read with a purpose. R1 Read. The first R in SQ4R stands for read. As you read, look for answers to the questions you asked. Read in short “bites,” from one topic heading to the next, then stop. For difficult material you may want to read only a paragraph or two at a time. R2 Recite. After reading a small amount, you should pause and recite or rehearse. Try to mentally answer your questions. Better yet, summarize what you just read in brief notes. Making notes will show you what you know and don’t know, so you can fill gaps in your knowledge (Peverly et al., 2003). If you can’t summarize the main ideas, skim over each section again. Until you can remember what you just read, there’s little point to reading more. After you’ve studied a short “bite” of text, turn the next topic heading into questions. Then read to the following heading. Remember to look for answers as you read and to recite or take notes before moving on. Ask yourself repeatedly, “What is the main idea here?” Repeat the question–read–recite cycle until you’ve finished an entire chapter (or just from one Knowledge Builder to the next, if you want to read shorter units). What’s the difference between reading a textbook and studying it? You have probably occasionally spent an evening just vegging out in front of a TV set. According to psychologist Donald Norman (1993), you were engaging in experiential cognition. This type of thinking occurs when you passively let an experience happen to you. There is nothing wrong with merely experiencing entertainment. But have you noticed that the next morning you often have trouble remembering what you watched the night before? In contrast, suppose one of the programs was about, say, global R3 Reflect. As you read, try to reflect on what you are reading. warming, and it really got you thinking. You might have wondered One powerful way to do this is to relate new facts, terms, and how global warming will affect your own future plans. You might concepts to information you already know well or to your have questioned some of the program’s more dire projections for own experiences. You’ve probably noticed that it is especially the future. You might have thought back to another program on easy to remember ideas that are personally meaningful, so global warming and remembered some different perspectives. try to relate the ideas you encountered to your own life. This Now you are “going beyond the information given” (Bruner, may be the most important step in the SQ4R method. The 1973). For Norman (1993), this is reflective cognition: Rather than more genuine interest you can bring to your reading, the just experiencing something, you also actively think about what more you will learn (Hartlep & Forsyth, 2000). you experienced. The next morning, you may forget most of what R4 Review. When you’re done reading, skim back over a secyou watched the night before, but the odds are you will remember tion or the entire chapter, or read your notes. Then check the program on global warming. Thus, studying a textbook ideally your memory by reciting and quizzing yourself again. Try to involves not just experiencing it, but also actively reflecting on make frequent, active review a standard part of your study what you read. habits ( Figure I.1). One way to be more reflective while reading a textbook is to use the SQ4R method. SQ4R stands for survey, question, Question Question Question Question read, recite, reflect, and review. These six Read Read Read Read Survey Review steps can help you learn as you read and Recite Recite Recite Recite Reflect Reflect Reflect Reflect reflect, remember more, and review effectively: • S Survey. Skim through a chapter before you begin reading it. Start by Figure I.1 The SQ4R method promotes active learning and information processing. You should begin with a •survey of the chapter or section, depending on how much you plan to read. Then you should proceed through cycles of questioning, reading, reciting, and reflecting, and conclude with a review of the section or the entire chapter. The Psychology of Studying Does this really work? Yes. Using a reflective reading strategy improves learning and course grades (Taraban, Rynearson, & Kerr, 2000). Simply reading straight through a chapter can give you “intellectual indigestion.” That’s why it’s better to stop often to think, question, recite, reflect, review, and “digest” information as you read. How to Use Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior You can apply the SQ4R method to any text. However, we have specifically designed this textbook to help you actively learn psychology. Survey Each chapter opens with a chapter survey that includes a Gateway Theme and a list of Gateway Questions as well as a Preview of what will be covered. You can use these features to identify important ideas as you begin reading. The Preview should help you get interested in the topics you will be reading about. The Gateway Theme and Gateway Questions are a good guide to the kinds of information to look for as you read. In fact, answers to the Gateway Questions are a good summary of the core concepts in each chapter. If, years from now, you still remember those Gateway concepts, your authors will be very happy indeed. Anyway, after you’ve studied the Gateway Questions, take a few minutes to do your own survey of the chapter. You should notice that each major chapter heading is accompanied by one of the Gateway Questions. This will help you build a “mental map” of upcoming topics. Question How can I use the SQ4R method to make reading more interesting and effective? One of the key steps is to ask yourself a lot of questions while you read. Dialogue Questions like the one that began this paragraph will help you focus on seeking information as you read. These questions are very much like those running through the minds of students like you as they read this book. Try to anticipate these questions. Even better, be sure to ask your own questions. Try to actively interact with your textbooks as you read. Read As an aid to reading, important terms are printed in boldface type and defined where they first appear. (Some are followed by pronunciations — capital letters show which syllables are accented.) You’ll also find a running glossary in the lower righthand corner of pages you are reading, so you never have to guess about the meaning of technical terms. If you want to look up a term from a lecture or another chapter, check the main Glossary. This “mini-dictionary” is located near the end of the book. Perhaps you should take a moment to find it now. In addition, many figures and tables help you quickly grasp important concepts. 3 Recite and Reflect To help you study in smaller “bites,” this book is divided into short sections that end with Knowledge Builders, which make good stopping points. Knowledge Builders provide opportunities to check your memory for what you just read. They also invite you to think more deeply by presenting critical thinking questions and questions designed to help you relate material to your own life. (Don’t forget to also take notes or recite and reflect on your own.) This book also provides other opportunities for you to reflect more deeply about what you are reading. Each chapter ends with a Psychology in Action section. These discussions are filled with practical ideas you can relate to your own life. In many chapters, Discovering Psychology boxes also invite you to relate psychology to your own behavior. Critical Thinking boxes present intriguing questions you can use to sharpen your critical thinking skills. In addition, Human Diversity boxes encourage you to reflect on the rich variety of human experience; Brainwaves boxes show how the brain relates to psychology; and in The Clinical File boxes we will see how psychology can be applied to treat clinical problems. Review Each chapter concludes with a detailed review. There you will find a list of Gateways to psychology. These are summaries of psychology’s “big ideas” and enduring principles. The first time you finish a chapter, don’t feel obligated to memorize the Gateways concepts. However, be sure to take a moment to think about each statement. Ultimately, the Gateways will provide a good high-level summary of what you learned in this course. By making these ideas your own, you will gain something of lasting value: You will learn to see human behavior as psychologists do. Following each Gateways concept, you will find a more detailed, point-by-point summary of ideas presented in the chapter. These points will help you identify important ideas to remember. For further review, you can use the running glossary in the margin, as well as boldface terms, figures, and tables. Table I.1 summarizes how this text helps you apply the SQ4R method. Even with all this help, there is still much more you can do on your own. • Effective Note-Taking — Good Students, Take Note! Reading strategies may be good for studying, but what about taking notes in class? Sometimes it’s hard to know what’s important. Just as studying a textbook is best done reflectively, so too is attending class (Norman, 1993). Like effective reading, good notes come from actively seeking information. People who are active listeners avoid distractions and skillfully gather ideas. Here’s a listening/note-taking SQ4R method An active study-reading technique based on these steps: survey, question, read, recite, reflect, and review. Active listener A person who knows how to maintain attention, avoid distractions, and actively gather information from lectures. 4 INTRODUCTION Table I.1 • Using the SQ4R Method Survey • Preview • Gateway Theme • Gateway Questions • Chapter Preview • Topic Headings • Figure Captions Question • Topic Headings • In-Text Dialogue Questions Read • Topic Headings • Boldface Terms • Running Glossary • Figures and Tables Recite • Recite Questions (in Knowledge Builders) • Practice Quizzes (online) • Notes (make them while reading) Reflect • Critical Thinking Questions (in Knowledge Builders) • Relate Questions (in Knowledge Builders) • Psychology in Action Sections (throughout the text) • Boxed Highlights (throughout the text) Review • Italicized Gateways Concepts • Boldface Terms • Running Glossary • Figure and Tables • Practice Quizzes (online) • Study Guide plan that works for many students. The letters LISAN, pronounced like the word listen, will help you remember the steps: L Lead. Don’t follow. Read assigned materials before coming to class. Try to anticipate what your teacher will say by asking yourself questions. If your teacher provides course notes or PowerPoint overheads before lectures, review them before coming to class. Reflective questions can come from those materials or from study guides, reading assignments, or your own curiosity. I Ideas. Every lecture is based on a core of ideas. Usually, an idea is followed by examples or explanations. Ask yourself often, “What is the main idea now? What ideas support it?” S Signal words. Listen for words that tell you what direction the instructor is taking. For instance, here are some signal words: There are three reasons why . . . Here come ideas Most important is . . . Main idea On the contrary . . . Opposite idea As an example . . . Support for main idea Therefore . . . Conclusion A Actively listen. Sit where you can get involved and ask questions. Bring questions you want answered from the last lecture or from your text. Raise your hand at the beginning of class or approach your professor before the lecture. Do anything that helps you stay active, alert, and engaged. N Note taking. Students who take accurate lecture notes tend to do well on tests (Williams & Eggert, 2002). However, don’t try to be a tape recorder. Listen to everything, but be selective and write down only key points. If you are too busy writing, you may not grasp what your professor is saying. When you’re taking notes, it might help to think of yourself as a reporter who is trying to get a good story (Ryan, 2001). Actually, most students take reasonably good notes — and then don’t use them! Many students wait until just before exams to review. By then, their notes have lost much of their meaning. If you don’t want your notes to seem like “chicken scratches,” it pays to review them every day (Rowe, 2007). Using and Reviewing Your Notes When you review, you will learn more if you take the extra steps listed here (Knaus & Ellis, 2002; Rowe, 2007; Santrock & Halonen, 2007): • As soon as you can, reflect on and improve your notes by filling in gaps, completing thoughts, and looking for connections among ideas. • Remember to link new ideas to what you already know. • Summarize your notes. Boil them down and organize them. • After each class session, write down at least seven major ideas, definitions, or details that are likely to become test questions. Then make up questions from your notes and be sure you can answer them. Summary The letters LISAN are a guide to active listening, but listening and good note taking are not enough. You must also review, organize, reflect, extend, and think about new ideas. Use active listening to get involved in your classes and you will undoubtedly learn more (Rowe, 2007). Study Strategies — Making a Habit of Success Grades depend as much on effort as they do on “intelligence.” However, don’t forget that good students work more efficiently, not just harder. Many study practices are notoriously poor, such as recopying lecture notes, studying class notes but not the textbook (or the textbook but not class notes), outlining chapters, answering study questions with the book open, and “group study” (which often becomes a party). The best students emphasize quality: They study their books and notes in depth and attend classes regularly. It’s a mistake to blame poor grades on events “beyond your control.” Students who are motivated to succeed usually get better grades (Perry et al., 2001). Let’s consider a few more things you can do to improve your study habits. The Psychology of Studying 5 Study in a Specific Place Test Yourself Ideally, you should study in a quiet, well-lit area free of distractions. If possible, you should also have at least one place where you only study. Do nothing else at that spot: Keep magazines, MP3 players, friends, cell phones, pets, posters, video games, puzzles, food, lovers, sports cars, elephants, pianos, televisions, YouTube, kazoos, and other distractions out of the area. In this way, the habit of studying will become strongly linked with one specific place. Then, rather than trying to force yourself to study, all you have to do is go to your study area. Once there, you’ll find it is relatively easy to get started. A great way to improve grades is to take practice tests before the real one in class. In other words, studying should include self-testing, in which you pose questions to yourself. You can use flash cards, Learning Check questions, online quizzes, a study guide, or other means. As you study, ask many questions and be sure you can answer them. Studying without self-testing is like practicing for a basketball game without shooting any baskets. For more convenient self-testing, your professor may make a Study Guide or a separate booklet of Practice Quizzes available. You can use either to review for tests. Practice quizzes are also available on the book companion website, as described later. However, don’t use practice quizzes as a substitute for studying your textbook and lecture notes. Trying to learn from quizzes alone will probably lower your grades. It is best to use quizzes to find out what topics you need to study more (Brothen & Wambach, 2001). Use Spaced Study Sessions It is reasonable to review intensely before an exam. However, you’re taking a big risk if you are only “cramming” (learning new information at the last minute). Spaced practice is much more efficient (Anderson, 2005). Spaced practice consists of a large number of relatively short study sessions. Long, uninterrupted study sessions are called massed practice. (If you “massed up” your studying, you probably messed it up too.) Cramming places a big burden on memory. Usually, you shouldn’t try to learn anything new about a subject during the last day before a test. It is far better to learn small amounts every day and review frequently (Anderson, 2005). Overlearn Many students underprepare for exams, and most overestimate how well they will do. A solution to both problems is overlearning, in which you continue studying beyond your initial mastery of a topic. In other words, plan to do extra study and review after you think you are prepared for a test. One way to overlearn is approach all tests as if they will be essays. That way, you will learn more completely, so you really “know your stuff.” Try Mnemonics Learning has to start somewhere, and memorizing is often the first step. Many of the best ways to improve memory are covered in Chapter 8. Let’s consider just one technique here. A mnemonic (nee-MON-ik) is a memory aid. There are many ways to create mnemonics. Most mnemonics link new information to ideas or images that are easy to remember. For example, what if you want to remember that the Spanish word for duck is pato (pronounced POT-oh)? To use a mnemonic, you could picture a duck in a pot or a duck wearing a pot for a hat. Likewise, to remember that the cerebellum controls coordination, you might picture someone named “Sarah Bellum” who is very coordinated. For best results, make your mnemonic images exaggerated or bizarre, vivid, and interactive (Macklin & McDaniel, 2005). Self-Regulated Learning — Academic All-Stars Think of a topic you are highly interested in, such as music, sports, fashion, cars, cooking, politics, or movies. Whatever the topic, you have probably learned a lot about it — painlessly. How could you make your college work more like voluntary learning? An approach called self-regulated learning might be a good start. Selfregulated learning is deliberately reflective and active, self-guided study (Hofer & Yu, 2003). Here’s how you can change passive studying into goal-oriented learning: 1. Set specific, objective learning goals. Try to begin each learning session with specific goals in mind. What knowledge or skills are you trying to master? What do you hope to accomplish (Knaus & Ellis, 2002)? Spaced practice Practice spread over many relatively short study sessions. Massed practice Practice done in a long, uninterrupted study session. Mnemonic A memory aid or strategy. Self-testing Evaluating learning by posing questions to yourself. Mnemonics make new information more familiar and memorable. Forming an image of a duck wearing a pot for a hat might help you remember that pato is the Spanish word for duck. Overlearning Continuing to study and learn after you think you’ve mastered a topic. Self-regulated learning Deliberately reflective and active, self-guided study. 6 INTRODUCTION 2. Plan a learning strategy. How will you accomplish your goals? Make daily, weekly, and monthly plans for learning. Then put them into action. 3. Be your own teacher. Effective learners silently give themselves guidance and ask themselves questions. For example, as you are learning, you might ask yourself, “What are the important ideas here? What do I remember? What don’t I understand? What do I need to review? What should I do next?” 4. Monitor your progress. Self-regulated learning depends on self-monitoring. Exceptional learners keep records of their progress toward learning goals (pages read, hours of studying, assignments completed, and so forth). They quiz themselves, use study guides, make sure they follow the SQ4R system, and find other ways to check their understanding while learning. 5. Reward yourself. When you meet your daily, weekly, or monthly goals, reward your efforts in some way, such as going to a movie or downloading some new music. Be aware that self-praise also rewards learning. Being able to say, “Hey, I did it!” or “Good work!” and knowing that you deserve it can be very rewarding. In the long run, success, self-improvement, and personal satisfaction are the real payoffs for learning. 6. Evaluate your progress and goals. It is a good idea to frequently evaluate your performance records and goals. Are there specific areas of your work that need improvement? If you are not making good progress toward long-range goals, do you need to revise your short-term targets? 7. Take corrective action. If you fall short of your goals you may need to adjust how you budget your time. You may also need to change your learning environment to deal with distractions such as watching TV, daydreaming, talking to friends, or testing the structural integrity of the walls with your stereo system. If you discover that you lack necessary knowledge or skills, ask for help, take advantage of tutoring programs, or look for information beyond your courses and textbooks. Knowing how to regulate and control learning can be a key to lifelong enrichment and personal empowerment. Procrastination — Avoiding the Last-Minute Blues All of these study techniques are fine. But what can I do about procrastination? A tendency to procrastinate is almost universal. (When campus workshops on procrastination are offered, many students never get around to signing up!) Even when procrastination doesn’t lead to failure, it can cause much suffering. Procrastinators work only under pressure, skip classes, give false reasons for late work, and feel ashamed of their last-minute efforts. They also tend to feel frustrated, bored, and guilty more often (Blunt & Pychyl, 2005). Why do so many students procrastinate? Many students equate grades with their personal worth. That is, they act as if grades tell whether they are good, smart people who will succeed in life. By procrastinating they can blame poor work on a late start, rather than a lack of ability (Beck, Koons, & Milgrim, 2000). After all, it wasn’t their best effort, was it? Perfectionism is a related problem. If you expect the impossible, it’s hard to start an assignment. Students with high standards often end up with all-or-nothing work habits (Onwuegbuzie, 2000). Time Management Most procrastinators must eventually face the self-worth issue. Nevertheless, most can improve by learning study skills and better time management. We have already discussed general study skills, so let’s consider time management in a little more detail. A weekly time schedule is a written plan that allocates time for study, work, and leisure activities. To prepare your schedule, make a chart showing all the hours in each day of the week. Then fill in times that are already committed: sleep, meals, classes, work, team practices, lessons, appointments, and so forth. Next, fill in times when you will study for various classes. Finally, label the remaining hours as open or free times. Each day, you can use your schedule as a checklist. That way you’ll know at a glance which tasks are done and which still need attention (Knaus & Ellis, 2002). You may also find it valuable to make a term schedule that lists the dates of all quizzes, tests, reports, papers, and other major assignments for each class. The beauty of sticking to a schedule is that you know you are making an honest effort. It will also help you avoid feeling bored while you are working or guilty when you play. Be sure to treat your study times as serious commitments, but respect your free times, too. And remember, students who study hard and practice time management do get better grades (Rau & Durand, 2000). Goal Setting As mentioned earlier, students who are active learners set specific goals for studying. Such goals should be clear-cut and measurable (Knaus & Ellis, 2002). If you find it hard to stay motivated, try setting goals for the semester, the week, the day, and even for single study sessions. Also, be aware that more effort early in a course can greatly reduce the “pain” and stress you will experience later. If your professors don’t give frequent assignments, set your own day-by-day goals. That way, you can turn big assignments into a series of smaller tasks that you can actually complete (Ariely & Wertenbroch, 2002). An example would be reading, studying, and reviewing 8 pages a day to complete a 40-page chapter in 5 days. For this textbook, reading from one Knowledge Builder to the next each day might be a good pace. Remember, many small steps can add up to an impressive achievement. The Psychology of Studying Study Skills Checklist Time Management Make formal schedule Set specific goals Study Habits Study in specific area Pace study and review Create memory aids Test yourself Overlearn Reading Use SQ4R method Study while reading Review frequently Note Taking Listen actively Use LISAN method Review notes frequently • Figure I.2 Study skills checklist. Make Learning an Adventure A final point to remember is that you are most likely to procrastinate if you think a task will be unpleasant (Pychyl et al., 2000). Learning can be hard work. Nevertheless, many students find ways to make schoolwork interesting and enjoyable. Try to approach your schoolwork as if it were a game, a sport, an adventure, or simply a way to become a better person. The best educational experiences are challenging yet fun (Ferrari & Scher, 2000). Virtually every topic is interesting to someone, somewhere. You may not be particularly interested in the sex life of South American tree frogs. However, a biologist might be fascinated. (Another tree frog might be, too.) If you wait for teachers to “make” their courses interesting, you are missing the point. Interest is a matter of your attitude. (See Figure I.2 for a summary of study skills.) • Taking Tests — Are You “Test Wise”? If I read and study effectively, is there anything else I can do to improve my grades? You must also be able to show what you know on tests. Here are some suggestions for improving your test-taking skills: General Test-Taking Skills 4. Be sure to answer all questions. 5. Use your time wisely. 6. Ask for clarification when necessary. Objective Tests Several additional strategies can help you do better on objective tests. Objective tests (multiple-choice and true–false items) require you to recognize a correct answer among wrong ones or a true statement versus a false one. Here are some strategies for taking objective tests: 1. First, relate the question to what you know about the topic. Then, read the alternatives. Does one match the answer you expected to find? If none match, reexamine the choices and look for a partial match. 2. Read all the choices for each question before you make a decision. Here’s why: If you immediately think that a is correct and stop reading, you might miss seeing a better answer like “both a and d.” 3. Read rapidly and skip items you are unsure about. You may find “free information” in later questions that will help you answer difficult items. 4. Eliminate certain alternatives. With a four-choice multiplechoice test, you have one chance in four of guessing right. If you can eliminate two alternatives, your guessing odds improve to 50-50. 5. Unless there is a penalty for guessing, be sure to answer any skipped items. Even if you are not sure of the answer, you may be right. If you leave a question blank, it is automatically wrong. When you are forced to guess, don’t choose the longest answer or the letter you’ve used the least. Both strategies lower scores more than random guessing does. 6. There is a bit of folk wisdom that says “Don’t change your answers on a multiple-choice test. Your first choice is usually right.” This is false. If you change answers, you are more likely to gain points than to lose them. This is especially true if you are uncertain of your first choice or it was a hunch, and if your second choice is more reflective (Higham & Gerrard, 2005). 7. Remember, you are searching for the one best answer to each question. Some answers may be partly true, yet flawed in some way. If you are uncertain, try rating each multiplechoice alternative on a 1–10 scale. The answer with the highest rating is the one you are looking for. 8. Few circumstances are always or never present. Answers that include superlatives such as most, least, best, worst, largest, or smallest are often false. You’ll do better on all types of tests if you observe the following guidelines (Wood & Willoughby, 1995): 1. Read all directions and questions carefully. They may give you good advice or clues. 2. Quickly survey the test before you begin. 3. Answer easy questions before spending time on more difficult ones. 7 Weekly time schedule A written plan that allocates time for study, work, and leisure activities during a 1-week period. Term schedule A written plan that lists the dates of all major assignments for each of your classes for an entire semester or quarter. Specific goal A goal with a clearly defined and measurable outcome. 8 INTRODUCTION Essay Tests Essay questions are a weak spot for students who lack organization, don’t support their ideas, or don’t directly answer the question (Rowe, 2007). When you take an essay exam try the following: 1. Read the question carefully. Be sure to note key words, such as compare, contrast, discuss, evaluate, analyze, and describe. These words all demand a certain emphasis in your answer. 2. Answer the question. If the question asks for a definition and an example, make sure you provide both. Providing just a definition or just an example will get you half marks. Giving three examples instead of the one asked for will not earn you any extra marks. 3. Think about your answer for a few minutes and list the main points you want to make. Just write them as they come to mind. Then rearrange the ideas in a logical order and begin writing. Elaborate plans or outlines are not necessary. 4. Don’t beat around the bush or pad your answer. Be direct. Make a point and support it. Get your list of ideas into words. 5. Look over your essay for errors in spelling and grammar. Save this for last. Your ideas are of first importance. You can work on spelling and grammar separately if they affect your grades. Short-Answer Tests Tests that ask you to fill in a blank, define a term, or list specific items can be difficult. Usually, the questions themselves contain little information. If you don’t know the answer, you won’t get much help from the questions. The best way to prepare for short-answer tests is to overlearn the details of the course. As you study, pay special attention to lists of related terms. Again, it is best to start with the questions you’re sure you know. Follow that by completing items you think you probably know. Questions you have no idea about can be left blank. Again, for your convenience, Figure I.2 provides a checklist summary of the main study skills we have covered. “pages.” If you know the URL (or “address”) of a website, you can view the information it contains. Almost all web pages also have links to other websites. These links let you “jump” from one site to the next to find more information. Google It To find psychological information on the Internet, you’ll need a computer and an Internet connection. If you don’t own a computer, you can usually use one on campus. Various software browsers make it easier to navigate around the web. A browser allows you to see text, images, sounds, and video clips stored on other computers. Browsers also keep lists of your favorite URLs so that you can return to them. The Book Companion Website How would I find information about psychology on the Internet? Your first stop on the Internet should be the book companion website. Here’s what you’ll find there: Online Quizzes. You can use these chapter-by-chapter multiple-choice and true–false quizzes to practice for tests and check your understanding. Web Links. This area is a “launching pad” that will take you to other psychology-related sites on the Internet. If a site sounds interesting, a click of the mouse will take you to it. Online Flash Cards. These online flash cards allow you to practice terms and concepts interactively. Crossword Puzzles. Crosswords are a fun way to check your knowledge of key terms and their definitions. The book companion website is located at www.cengage.com/ psychology/coon. Be sure to visit this site for valuable information about how to improve your grades and enhance your appreciation of psychology. • Using Digital Media — Netting New Knowledge Google any psychological term ranging from amnesia to zoophobia and you will find a vast library of information, from serious websites, like that maintained by the American Psychological Association, to Wikipedia entries and personal blogs. Even if you don’t own a computer, you can usually use one on campus to learn more about psychology. However, be aware that information on the Internet is not always accurate. It is wise to approach all websites with a healthy dose of skepticism. Digital Gateways The Internet is a network of interlinked computers. An important subpart of the Internet is the World Wide Web (WWW) or just plain “web,” an interlinked system of information “sites” or The book companion website gives you online access to a variety of valuable learning aids and interesting materials. The Psychology of Studying 9 • Figure I.3 A sample screen from CengageNOW. CengageNOW Students can also make use of CengageNOW for Coon/Mitterer’s Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior, twelfth edition, a web-based, personalized study system that provides a pre-test and a post-test for each chapter. CengageNOW, located at academic .cengage.com/login, also creates personalized study plans — which include rich media such as videos, animations, and learning modules and links to the eBook that help you study the specific topics in the text where you need to study most ( Figure I.3). • Wadsworth’s Psychology Resource Center Do you like videos, simulations, and animations? Do you learn best when you get actively involved in psychology? The Wadsworth Psychology Resource Center brings psychology to life with a full library of original and classic video clips plus interactive learning modules tied to all of the topics covered in your introductory psychology course. Log in at www.cengage.com/login to visit the resource center. • Figure I.4 This is a sample abstract from the PsycINFO database. If you search for the term study skills, you will find this article and many more in PsycINFO. (Reproduced with permission of the American Psychological Association, publisher of the PsycINFO database. PsycINFO Database Record © 2007 APA, all rights reserved. No further reproduction or distribution is permitted without written permission from the American Psychological Association.) ics by entering words such as drug abuse, postpartum depression, or creativity. You can gain access to PsycINFO in several ways. Almost every college and university subscribes to PsycINFO. If this is the case, you can usually search PsycINFO from a terminal in your college library or computer center for free. PsycINFO can also be directly accessed (for a fee) through the Internet via APA’s PsycINFO Direct service. For more information on how to gain access to PsycINFO, check this website: www.apa.org/psycinfo. The APA Website Psychology Websites You’ll find the titles of interesting websites you may want to explore at the end of each chapter in this book, including this one. The best way to reach these sites is through the book companion website. We have not included website addresses in the book because they often change or may become inactive. At the website you’ll find up-to-date links for websites listed in this book. The sites we’ve listed are generally of high quality. However, be aware that information on the Internet is not always accurate. It is wise to approach all websites with a healthy dose of skepticism. The APA also maintains an online library of general interest articles on aging, anger, children and families, depression, divorce, emotional health, kids and the media, sexuality, stress, testing issues, women and men, and other topics. They are well worth consulting when you have questions about psychological issues. You’ll find them at www.apa.org. For links to recent articles in newspapers and magazines, be sure to check the APA’s PsycPORT page at www.psycport.com. Internet An electronic network of interlinked computers. PsycINFO Psychological knowledge can also be found through specialized online databases. One of the best is PsycINFO, offered by the American Psychological Association. PsycINFO provides summaries of the scientific and scholarly literature in psychology. Each record in PsycINFO consists of an abstract (short summary), plus notes about the author, title, source, and other details ( Figure I.4). All entries are indexed using key terms. Thus, you can search for various top- • World Wide Web (WWW) A system of information sites accessible through the Internet. Links Connections built into websites that let you “jump” from one site to the next. Browser Software that facilitates access to text, images, sounds, video, and other information stored in formats used on the Internet. PsycINFO A searchable, online database that provides brief summaries of the scientific and scholarly literature in psychology. INTRODUCTION Please do take some of the “digital gateways” described here. You might be surprised by the fascinating information that awaits you. Investigating psychology on your own is one of the best ways to enrich an already valuable course. A Final Word There is a distinction in Zen between “live words” and “dead words.” Live words come from personal experience; dead words are “about” a subject. This book can only be a collection of dead words unless you accept the challenge of taking an intellectual step through this gateway to psychology. You will find many helpful, useful, and exciting ideas in the pages that follow. To make them yours, you must set out to actively learn as much as you can. The ideas presented here should get you off to a good start. Good luck! For more information, consult any of the following books: Hettich, P. I. (2005). Connect college to career: Student guide to work and life transition. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Knaus, W. J., & Ellis, A. (2002). The procrastination workbook: Your personalized program for breaking free from the patterns that hold you back. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Press. Rosnow, R. L. (2006). Writing papers in psychology: A student guide to research papers, essays, proposals, posters, and handouts (7th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Rowe, B. (2007). College awareness guide: What students need to know to succeed in college. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Santrock, J. W., & Halonen, J. S. (2007). Connections to college success. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Relate Which study skills do you think would help you the most? Which techniques do you already use? Which do you think you should try? To what extent do you already engage in self-regulated learning? What additional steps could you take to become a more active, goal-oriented learner? Answers: 1. T 2. F 3. T 4. F 5. T 6. self-regulated 7. T 8. Both encourage people to actively seek information as a way of learning more effectively. 10 Web Resources For an up-to-date list of direct links to interesting sites, including those listed here, visit the student companion site for this book at www.cengage.com/psychology/coon How to Succeed as a Student Advice on how to be a college student. Topics from studying to housing to preparation for work are included. Library Research in Psychology Hints on how to do library research in psychology. Psychology Glossary You can use this glossary to get additional definitions for common psychological terms. Study Skills More information on SQ4R, taking tests, note taking, and time management. Interactive Learning Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior Book Companion Website www.cengage.com/psychology/coon K NOW LED G E B U I L D E R Study Skills Visit your book companion website, where you will find flash cards, practice quizzes, web links, and more to help you study. RECITE 1. The four Rs in SQ4R stand for “read, recite, reflect, and review.” T or F? 2. When using the LISAN method, students try to write down as much of a lecture as possible so that their notes are complete. T or F? 3. Spaced study sessions are usually superior to massed practice. T or F? 4. According to research, you should almost always stick with your first answer on multiple-choice tests. T or F? 5. To use the technique known as overlearning, you should continue to study after you feel you have begun to master a topic. T or F? 6. Setting learning goals and monitoring your progress are important parts of ______________________________ learning. 7. Procrastination is related to seeking perfection and equating selfworth with grades. T or F? REFLECT Critical Thinking 8. How are the SQ4R method and the LISAN method related? Just what you need to know NOW! Spend time on what you need to master rather than on information you already have learned. Take a pre-test for this chapter, and CengageNOW will generate a personalized study plan based on your results. The study plan will identify the topics you need to review and direct you to online resources to help you master those topics. You can then take a post-test to help you determine the concepts you have mastered and what you will need to work on. Try it out! Go to www.cengage.com/login to sign in with an access code or to purchase access to this product. CHAPTER 1 Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods Gateway Theme Psychology is a science and a profession. Scientific observation is the most powerful way to answer questions about behavior. Mike Nelson/AFP/Getty Images Gateway Questions • What is psychology, and what are its goals? • What is critical thinking? • How does psychology differ from false explanations of behavior? • How is the scientific method applied in psychological research? • • How did the field of psychology emerge? • • • • What are the major specialties in psychology? • How good is the psychological information found in the popular media? How is an experiment performed? What is a double-blind experiment? What nonexperimental research methods do psychologists use? What are the contemporary perspectives in psychology? 11 preview Wondering About Human Behavior Near the end of every summer, a remote stretch of Nevada desert hosts Burning Man, an eclectic weeklong festival attended by tens of thousands of, well, unusual folks. It’s hard to explain just what this unique event is about except to say that attendees are encouraged to openly express themselves in any way they see fit. (Feel free to unleash your imagination on this one.) Revelers participate in a riotous cultural experience that climaxes with the burning of a giant wicker man. Old hippies mix with self-described rednecks, performance artists, musicians, sun worshippers, and individualists of every stripe. Why, you might wonder, do people go to Burning Man, some of them year after year? But then again, you might equally wonder why people join the Army, or get married, or travel to different countries, or become suicide bombers, or live out their lives in monasteries, or go to school. You might even wonder, at least sometimes, why you do what you do. In other words, just like your authors, the odds are you are curious about human behavior. That may even be a part of the reason you are taking a course in psychology and reading this book. Look around you: The Internet, television, newspapers, radio, and magazines are brimming with psychological topics. Psychology is an ever-changing panorama of people and ideas. You really can’t call yourself educated without knowing something about it. And, although we might envy those who have walked on the moon or explored the ocean’s depths, the ultimate frontier lies much closer to home. Psychology can help you better understand yourself and others. This book is a guided tour of human behavior. We hope you enjoy the adventure. Gateway Question: What is psychology, and what are its goals? People have always been curious about human behavior. The word psychology itself is thousands of years old, coming from the ancient Greek roots psyche, which means “mind,” and logos, meaning “knowledge or study.” However, have you ever actually seen or touched a “mind”? Because the mind can’t be studied directly, psychology is now defined as the scientific study of behavior and mental processes. What does behavior refer to in the definition of psychology? Anything you do — eating, hanging out, sleeping, talking, or sneezing — is a behavior. So are studying, snowboarding, gambling, watching television, tying your shoelaces, wrapping presents, learning Spanish, and reading this book. Naturally, we are interested in overt behaviors (directly observable actions and responses). But psychologists also study covert behaviors. These are private, internal activities, such as thinking, dreaming, remembering, and other mental events ( Jackson, 2008). All in all, then, psychology studies behaviors from A to Z: aging, burnout, conformity, death, emotion, flexibility, groupthink, hypnosis, intelligence, joy, kinesthesis, love, memory, noise pollution, originality, personality, quantitative reasoning, repression, sexuality, therapy, unconscious, wisdom, vision, xenophobia, youth, zeitgeist, and much more. Today, psychology is both a science and a profession. As scientists, some psychologists do research to discover new knowledge. Others are teachers who share their knowledge with students. Still others apply psychology to solve problems in mental health, education, business, sports, law, and medicine (Coolican et al., 2007). Later we will return to the profession of psychology. For now, let’s focus on how knowledge is created. Whether they work in a class12 Picture Partners/Alamy Psychology — The ABCs of Behavior Psychologists are highly trained professionals. In addition to the psychological knowledge they possess, psychologists learn specialized skills in counseling and therapy, measurement and testing, research and experimentation, statistics, diagnosis, treatment, and many other areas. room, a lab, or a clinic, all psychologists rely on critical thinking and information gained from scientific research. Seeking Empirical Evidence Many people regard themselves as expert “people watchers” and form their own “common-sense” theories of behavior. However, you may be surprised to learn how often self-appointed authorities and common-sense beliefs are wrong. Check your own expertise about human behavior by reading “Testing Common-Sense Beliefs.” Because of the limitations of common sense, psychologists have a special respect for scientific observation, which is based Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods 13 CRIT ICA L T H I N KI N G Testing Common-Sense Beliefs • • • Babies love their mothers because mothers fulfill their babies’ physiological need for food. True or False? Most humans use only 10 percent of their potential brainpower. True or False? Blind people have unusually sensitive organs of touch. True or False? • • • • • The more motivated you are, the better you will do at solving a complex problem. True or False? The major cause of forgetting is that memory traces decay or fade as time passes. True or False? Psychotherapy has its greatest success in the treatment of psychotic patients who have lost touch with reality. True or False? Personality tests reveal your basic motives, including those you may not be aware of having. True or False? To change people’s behavior toward members of ethnic minority groups, we must first change their attitudes. True or False? on gathering empirical evidence (information gained from direct observation). Unlike personal observation, scientific observation is systematic and intersubjective. In other words, observations are planned and they can be confirmed by more than one observer. Basically, the empirical approach says, “Let’s take a more objective look” (Stanovich, 2007). We study behavior directly and collect data (observed facts) so that we can draw valid conclusions. Would you say it’s true, for instance, that “Absence makes the heart grow fonder”? Why argue about it? As psychologists, we would simply get some people who are separated (“absent hearts”) and some who see each other every day (“present hearts”) and find out who is fonder of their loved ones! Here’s an example of gathering empirical evidence: Have you ever wondered if people become more hostile when it’s boiling hot outside? John Simister and Cary Cooper (2005) decided to find out. They obtained data on temperatures and criminal activity in • 9% 8% 7% Jul. 1996 Jan. 1996 Jul. 1995 Jan. 1995 Jul. 1994 Jan. 1994 Jul. 1993 Aggravated assault, % of year (left-hand scale) Temperature in LA (right-hand scale) Jan. 1993 Aggravated assault, % of year 10% 6% Actually research has shown that all these beliefs are false. Yet in a survey, all the beliefs were accepted as common sense by many college students (Landau & Bavaria, 2003). How did you do? We can all benefit from being more reflective as we evaluate our beliefs. It’s valuable to think critically and ask whether a belief makes logical sense. Do any of the concepts in this book apply to the belief? Can you imagine how you could collect evidence that might get you closer to the truth? Critical Thinking boxes like this one will help you be more reflective as you think critically about human behavior. Los Angeles over a 4-year period. When they graphed air temperature and the frequency of aggravated assaults, a clear relationship emerged ( Figure 1.1). Assaults and temperatures rise and fall more or less in parallel (so there may be something to the phrase “hot under the collar”). Isn’t the outcome of this study fairly predictable? Not if you started out believing otherwise. Sometimes the results of studies match our personal observations and common-sense beliefs and sometimes they come as a surprise. In this instance, you may have guessed the outcome. Your suspicions were confirmed by scientific observation. However, hostile actions that require more extreme physical exertion, such as fistfights, might become less likely at very high temperatures. Without systematically gathering data, we wouldn’t know for sure if overheated Angelenos become more lethargic or more aggressive when it gets hot. Thus, the study tells us something interesting about frustration, discomfort, and aggression. 43 40 37 34 31 28 25 22 19 16 13 10 7 4 1 2 5 8 11 14 17 1.1 Results of an empirical study. The graph shows •thatFigure aggravated assaults in Los Angeles become more likely as Temperature in Los Angeles It may appear that psychological research “discovers” what we already know from everyday experience. Why waste time and money confirming the obvious? Actually, commonsense beliefs are often wrong. See if you can tell which of the following common-sense beliefs are wrong (Landau & Bavaria, 2003): air temperature increases. This suggests that physical discomfort is associated with interpersonal hostility. (Data from Simister & Cooper, 2005.) Psychology The scientific study of behavior and mental processes. Scientific observation An empirical investigation structured to answers questions about the world in a systematic and intersubjective fashion (observations can be reliably confirmed by multiple observers). 14 CHAPTER 1 The scientific study of dreaming was made possible by use of the EEG, a device that records the tiny electrical signals generated by the brain as a person sleeps. The EEG converts these electrical signals into a written record of brain activity. Certain shifts in brain activity, coupled with the presence of rapid eye movements, are strongly related to dreaming. (See Chapter 6, pages 186–188, for more information.) Psychological Research Many fields, such as history, law, art, and business, are also interested in human behavior. How is psychology different? Psychology’s great strength is that it uses scientific observation to systematically answer questions about behavior (Stanovich, 2007). Of course, it may be impractical or unethical to study some topics. More often, questions go unanswered for lack of a suitable research method (a systematic process for answering scientific questions). In the past, for example, we had to take the word of people who say they never dream. Then the EEG (electroencephalograph, or brain-wave machine) was invented. Certain EEG patterns, and the presence of eye movements, can reveal that a person is dreaming. People who “never dream,” it turns out, dream frequently. If they are awakened during a dream, they vividly remember it. Thus, the EEG helped make the study of dreaming more scientific. Research Specialties What kinds of topics do psychologists study? Here’s a sample of what various psychologists might say about their work: “I’m a personality theorist. I study personality traits, motivation, and individual differences. I am especially interested in the personality profiles of highly creative college students.” “In general, developmental psychologists study the course of human growth and development, from conception until death. I’m especially interested in the transition from the teenage years to early adulthood.” “Like other learning theorists, I study how and why learning occurs in humans and animals. Right now I’m investigating how patterns of punishment affect learning.” “As a sensation and perception psychologist, I investigate how we discern the world through our senses. I am using a perceptual theory to study how we are able to recognize faces in a crowd.” “Comparative psychologists study and compare the behavior of different species, especially animals. Personally, I’m fascinated by the communication abilities of porpoises.” “Cognitive psychologists are primarily interested in thinking. I want to know how reasoning, problem solving, memory, and other mental processes relate to computer game playing.” “Biopsychologists are interested in how behavior relates to biological processes, especially activities in the nervous system. I’ve been doing some exciting research on how the brain controls hunger.” “Gender psychologists study differences between females and males. I want to understand how gender differences are influenced by biology, child rearing, education, and stereotypes.” “Social psychologists explore human social behavior, such as attitudes, persuasion, riots, conformity, leadership, racism, and friendship. My own interest is interpersonal attraction. I place two strangers in a room and analyze how strongly they are attracted to each other.” “Evolutionary psychologists are interested in how our behavior is guided by patterns that evolved during the long history of humankind. I am studying some interesting trends in male and female mating choices.” “Cultural psychologists study the ways in which culture affects human behavior. The language you speak, the foods you eat, how your parents disciplined you, what laws you obey, who you regard as ‘family,’ whether you eat with a spoon or your fingers — these and countless other details of behavior are strongly influenced by culture.” “Forensic psychologists apply psychological principles to legal issues. I am interested in improving the reliability of eyewitness testimony during trials.” This small sample should give you an idea of the diversity of psychological research. It also hints at some of the information we will explore in this book. Animals and Psychology Research involving animals was mentioned in some of the preceding examples. Why is that? You may be surprised to learn that psychologists are interested in the behavior of any living creature — from flatworms to humans. Indeed, some comparative psychologists spend their entire careers studying rats, cats, dogs, parrots, or chimpanzees. Although only a small percentage of psychological studies involve animals, they include many different types of research (Ord et al., 2005). Some psychologists use animal models to discover principles that apply to humans. For instance, animal studies have helped us understand stress, learning, obesity, aging, sleep, and many other topics. Psychology also benefits animals. For example, caring for domestic animals, as well as endangered species in zoos, relies on behavioral studies. Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods 15 bridges Bystander apathy and conditions that influence whether people will help in an emergency are of great interest to social psychologists. See Chapter 17, pages 564–566 for details. Ron Cohn/The Gorilla Foundation Prediction Some of the most interesting research with animals has focused on attempts to teach primates to communicate with sign language. Psychologist Penny Patterson has spent 35 years teaching Koko more than 1,000 signs. One of Koko’s favorite signs (“stink”) is shown here. (To learn more, visit www.koko.org.) Such research has helped illuminate the origins of human language and has even suggested better methods for teaching language to children with serious language impairment. (See Chapter 9, pages 291–292, for more information.) Psychology’s third goal, prediction, is the ability to forecast behavior accurately. Notice that our explanation of bystander apathy makes a prediction about the chances of getting help. If you’ve ever been stranded on a busy freeway with car trouble, you’ll recognize the accuracy of this prediction: Having many potential helpers nearby is no guarantee that anyone will stop to help. Control Description, explanation, and prediction seem reasonable, but is control a valid goal? “Control” may seem like a threat to personal freedom. However, to a psychologist, control simply refers to altering conditions that affect behavior. If a clinical psychologist helps a person overcome a terrible fear of spiders, control is involved. If Psychology’s Goals Charles Gupton/Stock Boston What do psychologists hope to achieve? Psychology’s ultimate goal is to benefit humanity (O’Neill, 2005). Specifically, the goals of the science of psychology are to describe, understand, predict, and control behavior. What do psychology’s goals mean in practice? Let’s see. Description Answering psychological questions often begins with a careful description of behavior. Description, or naming and classifying, is typically based on making a detailed record of scientific observations. But a description doesn’t explain anything, does it? Right. Useful knowledge begins with accurate description, but descriptions fail to answer the important “why” questions. Why do more women attempt suicide, and why do more men complete it? Why are people more aggressive when they are uncomfortable? Why are bystanders often unwilling to help in an emergency? Understanding We have met psychology’s second goal when we can explain an event. That is, understanding usually means we can state the causes of a behavior. For example, research on “bystander apathy” reveals that people often fail to help when other possible helpers are nearby. Why? Because a “diffusion of responsibility” occurs. Basically, no one feels personally obligated to pitch in. As a result, the more potential helpers there are, the less likely it is that anyone will help (Darley, 2000; Darley & Latané, 1968). Now we can explain a perplexing problem. Some psychologists specialize in administering, scoring, and interpreting psychological tests, such as tests of intelligence, creativity, personality, or aptitude. This specialty, which is called psychometrics, is an example of using psychology to predict future behavior. Research method A systematic approach to answering scientific questions. Animal model In research, an animal whose behavior is used to derive principles that may apply to human behavior. Description In scientific research, the process of naming and classifying. Understanding In psychology, understanding is achieved when the causes of a behavior can be stated. Prediction An ability to accurately forecast behavior. Control Altering conditions that influence behavior. 16 CHAPTER 1 you suggest changes in a classroom that help students learn better, you have exerted control. Control is also involved in designing automobiles to keep drivers from making fatal errors. Clearly, psychological control must be used wisely and humanely. In summary, psychology’s goals are a natural outgrowth of our desire to understand behavior. Basically, they boil down to asking the following questions: What is the nature of this behavior? (description) Why does it occur? (understanding and explanation) Can we forecast when it will occur? (prediction) What conditions affect it? (control) K NOW LED G E B U I L D E R The Science of Psychology RECITE To check your memory, see if you can answer these questions. If you miss any, skim over the preceding material before continuing to make sure you understand what you just read. 1. Psychology is the ___________________ study of _______________ and ____________ processes. 2. The best psychological information is typically based on a. proven theories b. opinions of experts and authorities c. anthropomorphic measurements d. empirical evidence 3. In psychological research, animal _________________ may be used to discover principles that apply to human behavior. 4. Which of the following questions relates most directly to the goal of understanding behavior? a. Do the scores of men and women differ on tests of thinking abilities? b. Why does a blow to the head cause memory loss? c. Will productivity in a business office increase if room temperature is raised or lowered? d. What percentage of college students suffer from test anxiety? Match the following research areas with the topics they cover. _____ 5. Developmental psychology A. Attitudes, groups, leadership _____ 6. Learning B. Conditioning, memory _____ 7. Personality C. The psychology of law _____ 8. Sensation and perception D. Brain and nervous system _____ 9. Biopsychology E. Child psychology _____ 10. Social psychology F. Individual differences, _____ 11. Comparative psychology motivation G. Animal behavior H. Processing sensory information REFLECT Critical Thinking 12. All sciences are interested in controlling the phenomena they study. T or F? Relate At first, many students think that psychology is primarily about abnormal behavior and psychotherapy. Did you? How would you describe the field now? Answers: 1. scientific, behavior, mental 2. d 3. models 4. b 5. E 6. B 7. F 8. H 9. D 10. A 11. G 12. F Astronomy and archaeology are examples of sciences that do not share psychology’s fourth goal. • • • • Critical Thinking — Take It with a Grain of Salt Gateway Question: What is critical thinking? How does critical thinking play a role in psychology? Most of us would be skeptical when offered a “genuine” Rolex watch or expensive designer sunglasses for just a few dollars on eBay. Likewise, most of us easily accept our ignorance of subatomic physics. But because we deal with human behavior every day, we tend to think that we already know what is true in psychology. All too often, we are tempted to “buy” common-sense beliefs and even outrageous claims about psychics “channeling” dead people, the powers of “healing” crystals, “miraculous” herbal remedies, astrology, and so forth. For these and many more reasons, learning to think critically is one of the lasting benefits of a college education. Critical thinking refers to an ability to reflect on, evaluate, compare, analyze, critique, and synthesize information. Critical thinkers are willing to challenge conventional wisdom by asking the hard questions. For example, everyone knows that women are more talkative than men, right? Critical thinkers might immediately ask: “How do we know that women talk more than men? Is there any empirical evidence that supports this “wisdom”? (Be on the lookout later in this chapter for some evidence concerning this belief.) What could we do to find out for ourselves? Thinking About Behavior The heart of critical thinking is a willingness to actively reflect on ideas. Critical thinkers evaluate ideas by analyzing the evidence supporting their beliefs and probe for weaknesses in their reasoning. They question assumptions and look for alternate conclusions. True knowledge, they recognize, comes from constantly revising our understanding of the world. They are not afraid to admit they were wrong. As Susan Blackmore (2001) said when her studies caused her to abandon some long-held beliefs, “Admitting you are wrong is always hard — even though it’s a skill that every psychologist has to learn.” Critical thinking relies on the following basic principles (Elder, 2006; Kida, 2006): 1. Few “truths” transcend the need for empirical testing. While religious beliefs and personal values may be held as matters of faith, without supporting evidence, most other ideas can be evaluated by applying the rules of logic, evidence, and the scientific method. 2. Judging the quality of evidence is crucial. Imagine that you are a juror in a courtroom, judging claims made by two battling lawyers. To decide correctly, you can’t just weigh the amount of evidence. You must also critically evaluate the quality of the evidence. Then you can give greater weight to the most credible facts. 3. Authority or claimed expertise does not automatically make an idea true. Just because a teacher, guru, celebrity, or authority Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods 17 To put these principles into action, here are some questions to ask over and over again as you evaluate new information (Browne & Keeley, 2007; Elder, 2006): 1. What claims are being made? What are their implications? 2. What tests (if any) of these claims has been made? What was the nature and quality of the tests? Are they credible? Can they be repeated? 3. How good is the evidence? (In general, scientific observations provide the highest-quality evidence.) 4. Who did the tests? How reliable and trustworthy were the investigators? Do they have conflicts of interest? Do their findings appear to be objective? Has any other independent researcher duplicated the findings? 5. Finally, how much credibility can the claim be given? High, medium, low, provisional? A course in psychology naturally enriches thinking skills. In this book, all upcoming chapters include Critical Thinking questions like the ones you have seen here. Take the time to tackle these questions. The effort will sharpen your thinking abilities and make learning more lively. For an immediate thinking challenge, let’s take a critical look at several nonscientific systems that claim to explain behavior. Pseudopsychologies — Palms, Planets, and Personality Gateway Question: How does psychology differ from false explanations of behavior? A pseudopsychology (SUE-doe-psychology) is any unfounded system that resembles psychology. Many pseudopsychologies give the appearance of science but are actually false. (Pseudo means “false.”) Pseudopsychologies change little over time because followers seek evidence that appears to confirm their beliefs and avoid evidence that contradicts their beliefs. Scientists, in contrast, actively look for contradictions as a way to advance knowledge. They are skeptical critics of their own theories (Schick & Vaughn, 2004). Can you give some examples of false psychologies? One early pseudopsychology, known as phrenology, was popularized in the nine- Bettmann/Corbis is convinced or sincere doesn’t mean you should automatically believe them. It is unscientific and self-demeaning to just take the word of an “expert” without asking, “What evidence convinced her or him? How good is it? Is there a better explanation?” 4. Critical thinking requires an open mind. Be prepared to consider daring departures and go wherever the evidence leads. However, don’t become so “open-minded” that you are simply gullible. As astronomer Carl Sagan once noted, “It seems to me that what is called for is an exquisite balance between two conflicting needs: the most skeptical scrutiny of all hypotheses that are served up to us and at the same time a great openness to new ideas” (Kida, 2006, p. 51). Phrenology was an attempt to assess personality characteristics by examining various areas of the skull. Phrenologists used charts such as the one shown here as guides. Like other pseudopsychologists, phrenologists made no attempt to empirically verify their concepts. teenth century by Franz Gall, a German anatomy teacher. Phrenology claimed that personality traits are revealed by the shape of the skull. Modern research has long since shown that bumps on the head have nothing to do with talents or abilities. In fact, the phrenologists were so far off that they listed the part of the brain that controls hearing as a center for “combativeness”! Palmistry is a similar false system that claims lines on the hand reveal personality traits and predict the future. Despite the overwhelming evidence against this, palmists can still be found separating the gullible from their money in many cities. At first glance, a pseudopsychology called graphology might seem more reasonable. Some graphologists claim that personality traits are revealed by handwriting. Based on such claims, some companies use graphologists to select job candidates. This is troubling because graphologists score close to zero on tests of accuracy in rating personality (Furnham, Chamorro-Premuzic, & Callahan, 2003). In fact, graphologists do no better than untrained college students in rating personality and job performance (Neter & Ben-Shakhar, 1989). Even a graphological society recently concluded that handwriting analysis should not be used to select people for jobs (Simner & Goffin, 2003). (By the way, graphology’s failure at revealing personality should be separated from its value for detecting forgeries.) Graphology might seem harmless enough. However, imagine being denied a job because a graphologist didn’t like your handwriting. This false system has been used to determine who is hired, given bank credit, or selected for juries. In these and similar situations, pseudopsychologies do, in fact, harm people. Critical thinking An ability to reflect on, evaluate, compare, analyze, critique, and synthesize information. Pseudopsychology Any false and unscientific system of beliefs and practices that is offered as an explanation of behavior. 18 CHAPTER 1 Uncritical Acceptance Even daily horoscopes printed in newspapers can seem uncannily accurate. However, such perceptions are typically based on uncritical acceptance (the tendency to believe positive or flattering descriptions of yourself ). Horoscopes are generally made up of mostly flattering traits. Naturally, when your personality is described in desirable terms, it is hard to deny that the description has the “ring of truth.” How much acceptance would astrology receive if a birth sign read like this: Virgo: You are the logical type and hate disorder. Your nitpicking is unbearable to your friends. You are cold, unemotional, and usually fall asleep while making love. Virgos make good doorstops. If pseudopsychologies have no scientific basis, how do they survive and why are they popular? There are several reasons, all of which can be illustrated by a critique of astrology. Problems in the Stars Astrology is arguably the most popular pseudopsychology. Astrology holds that the positions of the stars and planets at the time of one’s birth determine personality traits and affect behavior. Like other pseudopsychologies, astrology has repeatedly been shown to have no scientific validity. The objections to astrology are numerous and devastating (Kelly, 1999): 1. A study of more than 3,000 predictions by famous astrologers found that only a small percentage were fulfilled. These “successful” predictions tended to be vague (“There will be a tragedy somewhere in the east in the spring”) or easily guessed from current events (Culver & Ianna, 1988). 2. If astrologers are asked to match people with their horoscopes, they do no better than would be expected by chance. In one famous test, astrologers could not even use horoscopes to distinguish murderers from law-abiding people (Gauquelin, 1970). 3. There is no connection between people’s astrological signs and their intelligence or personality traits (Hartmann, Reuter, & Nyborg, 2006). There is also no connection between the “compatibility” of couples’ astrological signs and their marriage and divorce rates or between astrological signs and leadership, physical characteristics, or career choices (Martens & Trachet, 1998). 4. Astrologers have failed to explain why the moment of birth should be more important than, say, the moment of conception. (Perhaps it is because it is relatively easy to figure out the moment of birth and much trickier and touchy to determine the moment of conception.) Besides, the zodiac has shifted in the sky by one full constellation since astrology was first set up. (In other words, if astrology calls you a Scorpio you are really a Libra, and so forth.) However, most astrologers simply ignore this shift (Martens & Trachet, 1998). In short, astrology doesn’t work. Then why does astrology often seem to work? The following discussion explains why. Positive Instances Even when an astrological description contains a mixture of good and bad traits it may seem accurate. To find out why, read the following personality description. Your Personality Profile You have a strong need for other people to like you and for them to admire you. You have a tendency to be critical of yourself. You have a great deal of unused energy which you have not turned to your advantage. While you have some personality weaknesses, you are generally able to compensate for them. Your sexual adjustment has presented some problems for you. Disciplined and controlled on the outside, you tend to be worrisome and insecure inside. At times you have serious doubts as to whether you have made the right decision or done the right thing. You prefer a certain amount of change and variety and become dissatisfied when hemmed in by restrictions and limitations. You pride yourself on being an independent thinker and do not accept other opinions without satisfactory proof. You have found it unwise to be too frank in revealing yourself to others. At times you are extroverted, affable, sociable, while at other times you are introverted, wary, and reserved. Some of your aspirations tend to be pretty unrealistic.* Does this describe your personality? A psychologist read this summary individually to college students who had taken a personality test. Only 5 students out of 79 felt that the description was inaccurate. Another classic study found that people rated this “personality profile” as more accurate than their actual horoscopes (French et al., 1991). Reread the description and you will see that it contains both sides of several personality dimensions (“At times you are extroverted . . . while at other times you are introverted”). Its apparent accuracy is an illusion based on the fallacy of positive instances, in which we remember or notice things that confirm our expectations and forget the rest. The pseudopsychologies thrive on this effect. For example, you can always find “Aquarius characteristics” in an Aquarius. If you looked, however, you could also find “Gemini characteristics,” “Scorpio characteristics,” or whatever. Perhaps this explains why, in an ironic twist, 94% of those sent the full 10-page horoscope of a famous mass murderer accepted it as their own (Gauquelin, 1970). *Reprinted with permission of author and publisher from: R. E. Ulrich, T. J. Stachnik, and N. R. Stainton, “Student acceptance of generalized personality interpretations,” Psychological Reports, 13, 1963, 831–834. 19 Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods The Barnum Effect Pseudopsychologies also take advantage of the Barnum effect, which is a tendency to consider personal descriptions accurate if they are stated in general terms (Kida, 2006). P. T. Barnum, the famed circus showman, had a formula for success: “Always have a little something for everybody.” Like the all-purpose personality profile, palm readings, fortunes, horoscopes, and other products of pseudopsychology are stated in such general terms that they can hardly miss. There is always “a little something for everybody.” To observe the Barnum effect, read all 12 of the daily horoscopes found in newspapers for several days. You will find that predictions for other signs fit events as well as those for your own sign do. Try giving a friend the wrong horoscope sometime. Your friend may still be quite impressed with the “accuracy” of the horoscope. Astrology’s popularity shows that many people have difficulty separating valid psychology from systems that seem valid but are not. The goal of this discussion, then, has been to make you a more critical observer of human behavior and to clarify what is, and what is not, psychology. Here is what the “stars” say about your future: Emphasis now on education and personal improvement. A learning experience of lasting value awaits you. Take care of scholastic responsibilities before engaging in recreation. The word psychology figures prominently in your future. Pseudopsychologies may seem like no more than a nuisance, but they can do harm. For instance, people seeking treatment for psychological disorders may become the victims of self-appointed “experts” who offer ineffective, pseudoscientific “therapies” (Kida, 2006; Lilienfeld et al., 2005). Valid psychological principles are based on scientific observation and evidence, not fads, opinions, or wishful thinking. Non Sequitur © 1993. Reprinted by permission of Universal Press Syndicate. The fallacy of positive instances is also used by various “psychic mediums” who pretend to communicate with the deceased friends and relatives of audience members. An analysis shows that the number of “hits” (correct statements) made by these fakes tends to be very low. Nevertheless, many viewers are impressed because of the natural tendency to remember apparent hits and ignore misses. Of course, embarrassing misses are often edited out before the shows appear on television (Nickell, 2001). a silver lining.” With this in mind, you use your lover’s worrisome absences as a chance to spend more time with your family. You take comfort in this “silver lining” until a family member comments, “Where there’s smoke, there’s fire!” Much of what passes for common sense is equally vague and inconsistent. Notice also that most of these B.S. statements work best after the fact. (B.S., of course, stands for Before Science.) Systematically recording facts and events is the heart of all sciences. To be scientific, our observations must be systematic, so that they reveal something about behavior (Stanovich, 2007). To use an earlier example, if you are interested in heat and aggression, you will learn little by driving around and making haphazard observations of aggressive behavior. To be of value, your observations must be planned and systematic. The Scientific Method The scientific method is a form of critical thinking based on careful collection of evidence, accurate description and measurement, precise definition, controlled observation, and repeatable results ( Jackson, 2008). In its ideal form the scientific method has six elements: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. Making observations Defining a problem Proposing a hypothesis Gathering evidence/testing the hypothesis Publishing results Theory building Let’s take a closer look at some elements of the scientific method. All the basic elements of the scientific method are found in the example that follows. Scientific Research — How to Think Like a Psychologist Gateway Question: How is the scientific method applied in psychological research? Suppose that your friend marries someone just like her. What do people say? “Ah . . . birds of a feather flock together.” And what do they say if she gets divorced soon after? “Well, she should have known that opposites attract.” Let’s examine another common-sense statement. It is frequently said that “every cloud has Uncritical acceptance The tendency to believe generally positive or flattering descriptions of oneself. Fallacy of positive instances The tendency to remember or notice information that fits one’s expectations while forgetting discrepancies. Barnum effect The tendency to consider a personal description accurate if it is stated in very general terms. Scientific method A form of critical thinking based on careful measurement and controlled observation. 20 CHAPTER 1 Conceptual Level Hypothesized relationship Concepts Frustration Aggression Interrupted finishing a puzzle Number of times person insults interrupter Concrete Level © Dan McCoy/Rainbow Operational definitions Observed relationship Figure 1.2 Operational definitions are used to link concepts with concrete •observations. Do you think the examples given are reasonable operational definitions of frustration and aggression? Operational definitions vary in how well they represent concepts. For this reason, many different experiments may be necessary to draw clear conclusions about hypothesized relationships in psychology. Applying the scientific method to the study of behavior requires careful observation. Here, a psychologist videotapes a session in which a child’s thinking abilities are being tested. Observation Many people believe that women are more chatty than men. Is there any truth to this belief ? University of Arizona psychologist Mathias Mehl and his colleagues observed that the results of a few published reports do seem to support this stereotype. trated individual insults the person who prevented work on the puzzle.” In other words, covert behaviors are operationally defined in terms of overt behavior so they can be observed and studied scientifically. Gathering Evidence/Testing the Hypothesis Defining a Problem However, the researchers noticed that none of the studies had actually recorded men’s and women’s normal conversations over long periods. Thus, they defined their problem as, “How can we record natural conversations without bothering people and perhaps biasing our observations?” Proposing a Hypothesis What exactly is a “hypothesis”? A hypothesis (hi-POTH-eh-sis) is a tentative statement about, or explanation of, an event or relationship. In common terms, a hypothesis is a testable hunch or educated guess about behavior. For example, you might hypothesize that “Frustration encourages aggression.” How could you test this hypothesis? First you would have to decide how you are going to frustrate people. (This part might be fun.) Then you will need to find a way to measure whether or not they become more aggressive. (Not so much fun if you plan to be nearby.) Your observations would then provide evidence to confirm or disconfirm the hypothesis. Because we cannot see or touch frustration, we must define it operationally. An operational definition states the exact procedures used to represent a concept. Operational definitions allow unobservable ideas, such as covert behaviors, to be tested in realworld terms ( Figure 1.2). For example, since you can’t measure frustration directly, you might define frustration as “interrupting an adult before he or she can finish a puzzle and win an iPhone.” And aggression might be defined as “the number of times a frus- • Now let’s return to the question of whether women talk more than men do. To gather data, the researchers used an electronically activated recorder (EAR) to track people’s conversations. This device recorded sounds for 30 seconds every 12.5 minutes. Participants could not tell when they were being recorded, so they acted and spoke normally. Researchers counted the number of words spoken when the recorder was on and used that to estimate the total number of words spoken each day. On average, women spoke 16,215 words a day, with men close behind at 15,699. This difference is too small to be meaningful, so we can conclude, as Mehl did, that there is no evidence that women talk more than men (Mehl et al., 2007). Publishing Results Scientific information must always be publicly available. That way, other researchers can read about the results and make their own observations if they doubt the study’s findings. If others are able to replicate (repeat) the results of a study, those results become more credible. The results of psychological studies are usually published in professional journals ( Table 1.1). That way, anyone willing to make appropriate observations can see whether or not a claim is true ( Jackson, 2008). In a scholarly article, Mehl and his colleagues carefully describe the question they investigated, the methods they used, and the results of their study on male and female talkativeness. The article was published in the journal Science (Mehl et al., 2007). • Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods Table 1.1 • Outline of a Research Report • Abstract Research reports begin with a very brief summary of the study and its findings. The abstract allows you to get an overview without reading the entire article. • Introduction The introduction describes the question to be investigated. It also provides background information by reviewing prior studies on the same or related topics. • Method This section tells how and why observations were made. It also describes the specific procedures used to gather data. That way, other researchers can repeat the study to see if they get the same results. • Results The outcome of the investigation is presented. Data may be graphed, summarized in tables, or statistically analyzed. • Discussion The results of the study are discussed in relation to the original question. Implications of the study are explored and further studies may be proposed. 21 bridges One of the major limitations of Freudian personality theory is that many of its concepts are not testable or falsifiable. See Chapter 12, page 402. While Mehl and his colleagues did not present a theory of why humans talk more or less, they did discuss how their findings might affect such a theory. For instance, they pointed out that they only studied university students, and that older men and women might differ in how much they talk each day. Such findings invite others to study talkativeness in other age groups and to propose theories to explain any differences that might be observed. Theory Building What about theory building? In research, a theory acts as a map of knowledge. Good theories summarize observations, explain them, and guide further research ( Figure 1.3). Without theories of forgetting, personality, stress, mental illness, and the like, psychologists would drown in a sea of disconnected facts (Stanovich, 2007). • Observation Define problem Propose hypothesis Gather evidence Test hypothesis Reject hypothesis Retain hypothesis KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R Critical Thinking and the Scientific Method in Psychology RECITE 1. The fallacy of positive instances refers to graphology’s accepted value for the detection of forgeries. T or F? 2. Personality descriptions provided by pseudopsychologies are stated in general terms, which provide “a little something for everybody.” This fact is the basis of the a. palmist’s fallacy b. uncritical acceptance pattern c. fallacy of positive instances d. Barnum effect 3. Most of psychology can rightfully be called common sense because psychologists prefer informal observation to systematic observation. T or F? 4. A psychologist does a study to see if exercising increases sense of well-being. In the study he will be testing an a. experimental hypothesis b. operational definition c. empirical definition d. anthropomorphic theory 5. ____________ behaviors are operationally defined in terms of ____________ behavior a. overt, covert b. observable, overt c. covert, overt d. covert, abstract Continued Publish results Theory building Figure 1.3 Psychologists use the logic of science to answer questions about •behavior. Specific hypotheses can be tested in a variety of ways, including controlled experiments, naturalistic observation, correlational studies, clinical studies, and the survey method. Psychologists revise their theories to reflect the evidence they gather. New or revised theories then lead to new observations, problems, and hypotheses. Hypothesis The predicted outcome of an experiment or an educated guess about the relationship between variables. Operational definition Defining a scientific concept by stating the specific actions or procedures used to measure it. For example, “hunger” might be defined as “the number of hours of food deprivation.” Theory A system of ideas designed to interrelate concepts and facts in a way that summarizes existing data and predicts future observations. CHAPTER 1 REFLECT Critical Thinking 6. Can you think of some “common-sense” statements that contradict each other? 7. Try constructing a few “Barnum statements,” personality statements so general that virtually everyone will think they apply to themselves. Can you string them together to make a “Barnum personality profile”? Can you adapt the same statements to construct a “Barnum horoscope”? 8. Each New Year’s Day, phony “psychics” make predictions about events that will occur during the coming year. The vast majority of these predictions are wrong, but the practice continues each year. Can you explain why? Relate It is nearly impossible to get through a day without encountering people who believe in pseudopsychologies or who make unscientific or unfounded statements. How stringently do you evaluate your own beliefs and the claims made by others? How might you scientifically test the old saw that you can’t teach an old dog new tricks? Follow the steps of the scientific method to propose a testable hypothesis and decide how you would gather evidence. (Well, OK, you don’t have to publish your results.) Answers: 1. F 2. d 3. F 4. a 5. c 6. There are many examples. Here are a few more to add to the ones you thought of: “He (or she) who hesitates is lost” versus “Haste makes waste.” “Never too old to learn” versus “You can’t teach an old dog new tricks.” 7. The term “Barnum statement” comes from Levy (2003), who offers the following examples: You are afraid of being hurt. You are trying to find a balance between autonomy and closeness. You don’t like being overly dependent. You just want to be understood. 8. Because of the fallacy of positive instances, people only remember predictions that seemed to come true and forget all the errors. Incidentally, “predictions” that appear to be accurate are usually easily deduced from current events or are stated in very general terms to take advantage of the Barnum effect. A Brief History of Psychology — Psychology’s Family Album Gateway Question: How did the field of psychology emerge? As we noted previously, people have been informally observing human behavior and philosophizing about it for thousands of years. In contrast, psychology’s history as a science dates back only about 130 years to Leipzig, Germany. There, Wilhelm Wundt (VILL-helm Voont), the “father of psychology,” set up a laboratory in 1879 to study conscious experience. What happens, he wondered, when we experience sensations, images, and feelings? To find out, Wundt systematically observed and measured stimuli of various kinds (lights, sounds, weights). A stimulus is any physical energy that affects a person and evokes a response (stimulus: singular; stimuli [STIM-you-lie]: plural). Wundt then used introspection, or “looking inward,” to probe his reactions to various stimuli. (Stop reading, close your eyes, carefully examine your thoughts, feelings, and sensations, and you will be introspecting.) Wilhelm Wundt, 1832–1920. Wundt is credited with making psychology an independent science, separate from philosophy. Wundt’s original training was in medicine, but he became deeply interested in psychology. In his laboratory, Wundt investigated how sensations, images, and feelings combine to make up personal experience. Hulton Archive/Getty Images 22 Over the years, Wundt studied vision, hearing, taste, touch, memory, time perception, and many other topics. By insisting on systematic observation and measurement, he asked some interesting questions and got psychology off to a good start (Schultz & Schultz, 2008). Structuralism Wundt’s ideas were carried to the United States by Edward Titchener (TICH-in-er). Titchener called Wundt’s ideas structuralism and tried to analyze the structure of mental life into basic “elements” or “building blocks.” How could he do that? You can’t analyze experience like a chemical compound, can you? Perhaps not, but the structuralists tried, mostly by using introspection. For instance, an observer might heft an apple and decide that she had experienced the elements “hue” (color), “roundness,” and “weight.” Another example of a question that might have interested a structuralist is, What basic tastes mix together to create complex flavors as different as broccoli, lime, bacon, and strawberry cheesecake? Introspection proved to be a poor way to answer most questions (Benjafield, 2004). Why? Because no matter how systematic the observations, the structuralists frequently disagreed. And when they did, there was no way to settle differences. Think about it. If you and a friend both introspect on your perceptions of an apple and end up listing different basic elements, who would be right? Despite such limitations, “looking inward” is still used as one source of insight in studies of hypnosis, meditation, problem solving, moods, and many other topics. Functionalism American scholar William James broadened psychology to include animal behavior, religious experience, abnormal behavior, and other interesting topics. James’s brilliant first book, Principles of Psychology (1890), helped establish the field as a separate discipline (Hergenhahn, 2005). The term functionalism comes from James’s interest in how the mind functions to help us adapt to the environment. James regarded consciousness as an ever-changing stream or flow of images and sensations — not a set of lifeless building blocks, as the structuralists claimed. The functionalists admired Charles Darwin, who deduced that creatures evolve in ways that favor survival. According to Darwin’s principle of natural selection, physical features that help animals adapt to their environments are retained in evolution. Similarly, the functionalists wanted to find out how the mind, perception, habits, and emotions help us adapt and survive. What effect did functionalism have on modern psychology? Functionalism brought the study of animals into psychology. It also promoted educational psychology (the study of learning, teaching, classroom dynamics, and related topics). Learning makes us more adaptable, so the functionalists tried to find ways to improve education. For similar reasons, functionalism gave rise to industrial psychology, the study of people at work. bridges Today, educational psychology and industrial psychology remain two major applied specialties. See Chapter 18 for more information about applied psychology. Behaviorism Functionalism and structuralism were soon challenged by behaviorism, the study of observable behavior. Behaviorist John B. Watson objected strongly to the study of the “mind” or “conscious experience.” He believed that introspection is unscientific because there is no way to settle disagreements between observers. Watson realized that he could study the behavior of animals even though he couldn’t ask them questions or know what they were thinking (Watson, 1913/1994). He simply observed the relationship between stimuli (events in the environment) and an animal’s responses (any muscular action, glandular activity, or other identifiable behavior). These observations were objective because they did not involve introspecting on subjective experience. Why not, he asked, apply the same objectivity to human behavior? Watson soon adopted Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov’s (eeVAHN PAV-lahv) concept of conditioning to explain most behavior. (A conditioned response is a learned reaction to a particular stimulus.) Watson claimed, “Give me a dozen healthy infants, well-formed, and my own special world to bring them up in and I’ll 23 John B. Watson, 1878–1958. Watson’s intense interest in observable behavior began with his doctoral studies in biology and neurology. Watson became a psychology professor at Johns Hopkins University in 1908 and advanced his theory of behaviorism. He remained at Johns Hopkins until 1920 when he left for a career in the advertising industry! guarantee to take any one at random and train him to become any type of specialist I might select — doctor, lawyer, artist, merchantchief, and yes, beggarman and thief ” (Watson, 1913/1994). Would most psychologists agree with Watson’s claim? No, today it is regarded as an overstatement. Just the same, behaviorism helped make psychology a natural science, rather than a branch of philosophy (Benjafield, 2004). Radical Behaviorism The best-known behaviorist, B. F. Skinner (1904–1990), believed that our actions are controlled by rewards and punishments. To study learning, Skinner created his famous conditioning chamber, or “Skinner box.” With it, he could present stimuli to animals and record their responses. bridges See Chapter 7, pages 226–241, for more information about operant conditioning. Many of Skinner’s ideas about learning grew out of work with rats and pigeons. Nevertheless, he believed that the same laws of behavior apply to humans. As a “radical behaviorist,” Skinner also believed that mental events, such as thinking, are not needed to explain behavior (Schultz & Schultz, 2008). Stimulus Any physical energy sensed by an organism. Introspection To look within; to examine one’s own thoughts, feelings, or sensations. Structuralism The school of thought concerned with analyzing sensations and personal experience into basic elements. Functionalism The school of psychology concerned with how behavior and mental abilities help people adapt to their environments. Natural selection Darwin’s theory that evolution favors those plants and animals best suited to their living conditions. Behaviorism The school of psychology that emphasizes the study of overt, observable behavior. Response Any muscular action, glandular activity, or other identifiable aspect of behavior. Archives of the History of American Psychology, University of Akron William James, 1842–1910. William James was the son of philosopher Henry James, Sr., and the brother of novelist Henry James. During his long academic career, James taught anatomy, physiology, psychology, and philosophy at Harvard University. James believed strongly that ideas should be judged in terms of their practical consequences for human conduct. Archives of the History of American Psychology, University of Akron Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods 24 CHAPTER 1 Cognitive Behaviorism Radical behaviorists have been criticized for ignoring the role that thinking plays in our lives. One critic even charged that Skinnerian psychology had “lost consciousness!” However, many criticisms have been answered by cognitive behaviorism, a view that combines cognition (thinking) and conditioning to explain behavior (Zentall, 2002). As an example, let’s say you frequently visit a particular website because it offers free streaming videos. A behaviorist would say that you visit the site because you are rewarded by the pleasure of watching interesting videos each time you go there. A cognitive behaviorist would add that, in addition, you expect to find good videos at the site. This is the cognitive part of your behavior. Behaviorists deserve credit for much of what we know about learning, conditioning, and the proper use of reward and punishment. Behaviorism is also the source of behavior therapy, which uses learning principles to change problem behaviors such as overeating, unrealistic fears, or temper tantrums. (See Chapter 15, pages 503–509, for more information.) Gestalt Psychology Imagine that you just played “Happy Birthday” on a low-pitched tuba. Next, you play it on a high-pitched flute. The flute duplicates none of the tuba’s sounds. Yet we notice something interesting: The melody is still recognizable — as long as the relationship between notes remains the same. Now, what would happen if you played the notes of “Happy Birthday” in the correct order, but at a rate of one per hour? What would we have? Nothing! The separate notes would no longer be a melody. Perceptually, the melody is somehow more than the individual notes that define it. It was observations like these that launched the Gestalt school of thought. German psychologist Max Wertheimer (VERT-hi-mer) was the first to advance the Gestalt viewpoint. It is inaccurate, he said, to analyze psychological events into pieces, or “elements,” as the structuralists did. Accordingly, Gestalt psychologists studied thinking, learning, and perception as whole units, not by analyz- Figure 1.4 The design you see here is entirely made up of broken circles. •However, as the Gestalt psychologists discovered, our perceptions have a powerful tendency to form meaningful patterns. Because of this tendency, you will probably see a triangle in this design, even though it is only an illusion. Your whole perceptual experience exceeds the sum of its parts. Max Wertheimer, 1880–1941. Wertheimer first proposed the Gestalt viewpoint to help explain perceptual illusions. He later promoted Gestalt psychology as a way to understand not only perception, problem solving, thinking, and social behavior, but also art, logic, philosophy, and politics. ing experiences into parts. Their slogan was, “The whole is greater than the sum of its parts” ( Figure 1.4). In fact the German word Gestalt means “form, pattern, or whole.” Like a melody, many experiences cannot be broken into smaller units, as the Structuralists proposed. For this reason, studies of perception and personality have been especially influenced by the Gestalt viewpoint. Gestalt psychology also inspired a type of psychotherapy. • bridges If you are curious about what Gestalt therapy is like, look ahead to Chapter 15, pages 501–502. Psychoanalytic Psychology As American psychology grew more scientific, an Austrian doctor named Sigmund Freud was developing radically different ideas which opened new horizons in art, literature, and history, as well as psychology (Jacobs, 2003). Freud believed that mental life is like an iceberg: Only a small part is exposed to view. He called the area of the mind that lies outside of personal awareness the unconscious. According to Freud, our behavior is deeply influenced by unconscious thoughts, Archives of the History of American Psychology, University of Akron Skinner was convinced that a “designed culture” based on positive reinforcement could encourage desirable behavior. (Skinner opposed the use of punishment because it doesn’t teach correct responses.) Too often, he believed, misguided rewards lead to destructive actions that create problems such as overpopulation, pollution, and war. Yvonne Hemsey/Getty Images B. F. Skinner, 1904–1990. Skinner studied simple behaviors under carefully controlled conditions. The “Skinner box” has been widely used to study learning in simplified animal experiments. In addition to advancing psychology, Skinner hoped that his radical brand of behaviorism would improve human life. Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods Humanistic Psychology Humanism is a view that focuses on subjective human experience. Humanistic psychologists are interested in human potentials, ideals, and problems. How is the humanistic approach different from others? Carl Rogers, Abraham Maslow, and other humanists rejected the Freudian idea that we are ruled by unconscious forces. They were also uncomfortable with the behaviorist emphasis on conditioning. Both views have a strong undercurrent of determinism (the idea that behavior is determined by forces beyond our control). In contrast, the humanists stress free will, our ability to make voluntary choices. Of course, past experiences do affect us. Nevertheless, humanists believe that people can freely choose to live more creative, meaningful, and satisfying lives. Humanists are interested in psychological needs for love, self-esteem, belonging, self-expression, creativity, and spirituality. Such needs, they believe, are as important as our biological urges for food and water. For example, newborn infants deprived of human love may die just as surely as they would if deprived of food. How scientific is the humanistic approach? Initially, humanists were less interested in treating psychology as a science. They stressed subjective factors, such as one’s self-image, self-evaluation, and frame of reference. (Self-image is your perception of your own body, personality, and capabilities. Self-evaluation refers to appraising yourself as good or bad. A frame of reference is a mental perspective used to interpret events.) Today, humanists still seek to understand how we perceive ourselves and experience the world. However, most now do research to test their ideas, just as other psychologists do (Schneider, Bugental, & Pierson, 2001). Maslow’s concept of self-actualization is a key feature of humanism. Self-actualization refers to developing one’s poten- Cognitive behaviorism An approach that combines behavioral principles with cognition (perception, thinking, anticipation) to explain behavior. Gestalt psychology A school of psychology emphasizing the study of thinking, learning, and perception in whole units, not by analysis into parts. Unconscious Contents of the mind that are beyond awareness, especially impulses and desires not directly known to a person. Repression The unconscious process by which memories, thoughts, or impulses are held out of awareness. Psychoanalysis A Freudian approach to psychotherapy emphasizing the exploration of unconscious conflicts. Neo-Freudian A psychologist who accepts the broad features of Freud’s theory but has revised the theory to fit his or her own concepts. Psychodynamic theory Any theory of behavior that emphasizes internal conflicts, motives, and unconscious forces. Humanism An approach to psychology that focuses on human experience, problems, potentials, and ideals. Determinism The idea that all behavior has prior causes that would completely explain one’s choices and actions if all such causes were known. Free will The idea that human beings are capable of freely making choices or decisions. Self-actualization The ongoing process of fully developing one’s personal potential. Bettmann/Corbis impulses, and desires—especially those concerning sex and aggression. Freud theorized that many unconscious thoughts are repressed (held out of awareness) because they are threatening. But sometimes, he said, they are revealed by dreams, emotions, or slips of the tongue. (“Freudian slips” are often humorous, as when a student who is late for class says, “I’m sorry I couldn’t get here any later.”) Freud believed that all thoughts, emotions, and actions are determined. In other words, nothing is an accident: If we probe deeply enough, we will find the causes of every thought or action. Freud was also among the first to appreciate that childhood affects adult personality (“The child is father to the man”). Most of all, perhaps, Freud is known for creating psychoanalysis, the first fully developed psychotherapy, or “talking cure.” Freudian psychotherapy explores unconscious conflicts and emotional problems. (See Chapter 15, pages 498–499, for details.) It wasn’t very long before some of Freud’s students began to promote their own theories. Several who modified Freud’s ideas became known as neo-Freudians (neo means “new” or “recent”). Neo-Freudians accepted much of Freud’s theory but revised parts of it. Many, for instance, placed less emphasis on sex and aggression and more on social motives and relationships. Some well-known neo-Freudians are Alfred Adler, Anna Freud (Freud’s daughter), Karen Horney (HORN-eye), Carl Jung (yoong), Otto Rank (rahnk), and Erik Erikson. Today, Freud’s ideas have been altered so much that few strictly psychoanalytic psychologists are left. However, his legacy is still evident in various psychodynamic theories, which continue to emphasize internal motives, conflicts, and unconscious forces (Gedo, 2002). Abraham Maslow, 1908–1970. As a founder of humanistic psychology, Maslow was interested in studying people of exceptional mental health. Such self-actualized people, he believed, make full use of their talents and abilities. Maslow offered his positive view of human potential as an alternative to the schools of behaviorism and psychoanalysis. Hulton Archive/Getty Images Sigmund Freud, 1856–1939. For more than 50 years, Freud probed the unconscious mind. In doing so, he altered modern views of human nature. His early experimentation with a “talking cure” for hysteria is regarded as the beginning of psychoanalysis. Through psychoanalysis, Freud added psychological treatment methods to psychiatry. 25 • The Early Development of Psychology Perspective Date Notable Events Experimental psychology 1875 First psychology course offered by William James First American Ph.D. in psychology awarded Wilhelm Wundt opens first psychology laboratory in Germany First American psychology lab founded at Johns Hopkins University First American psychology textbook written by John Dewey 1878 1879 1883 1886 Structuralism 1898 Functionalism 1890 1892 Edward Titchener advances psychology based on introspection William James publishes Principles of Psychology American Psychological Association founded Psychodynamic psychology 1895 1900 Sigmund Freud publishes first studies Freud publishes The Interpretation of Dreams Behaviorism 1906 Ivan Pavlov reports his research on conditioned reflexes John Watson presents behavioristic view 1913 Gestalt psychology 1912 Max Wertheimer and others advance Gestalt viewpoint Humanistic psychology 1942 Carl Rogers publishes Counseling and Psychotherapy Abraham Maslow publishes “A Theory of Human Motivation” 1943 tial fully and becoming the best person possible. According to humanists, everyone has this potential. Humanists seek ways to help it emerge. Table 1.2 presents a summary of psychology’s early development. • bridges Behaviorism, Gestalt psychology, psychoanalytic theory, and humanism have all given rise to various forms of psychotherapy. See Chapter 15 for more information about how psychological disorders are treated. The Role of Women in Psychology’s Early Days Were all the early psychologists men? Even though most of the early psychologists were men, women have contributed to psychology from the beginning (Minton, 2000). By 1906, in the United States about 1 psychologist in 10 was a woman. Who were these “foremothers” of psychology? Three who became well known are Mary Calkins, Christine Ladd-Franklin, and Margaret Washburn. Mary Calkins did valuable research on memory and was also the first woman president of the American Psychological Asso- Mary Calkins, 1863–1930. Christine Ladd-Franklin, 1847–1930. ciation, in 1905. Christine Ladd-Franklin studied color vision. In 1906 she was ranked among the 50 most important psychologists in America. In 1908 Margaret Washburn published an influential textbook on animal behavior, titled The Animal Mind. The first woman to be awarded a Ph.D. in psychology was Margaret Washburn, in 1894. Over the next 15 years many more women followed her pioneering lead. Today, more than half the members of the American Psychologi- Margaret Washburn, 1871–1939. cal Association are women, two out of three graduate students in psychology are women, and in recent years nearly 75 percent of all college graduates with a major in psychology have been women. Clearly, psychology has become fully open to both men and women (Hyde, 2004). Psychology Today — Three Complementary Perspectives on Behavior Gateway Question: What are the contemporary perspectives in psychology? At one time, loyalty to each school of thought was fierce, and clashes were common. Now, some early systems, such as structuralism, have disappeared entirely while new ones have gained prominence. Also, viewpoints such as functionalism and Gestalt psychology have blended into newer, broader perspectives. Certainly, loyalties and specialties still exist. But today, many psychologists are eclectic (ek-LEK-tik), because they realize that a single perspective is unlikely to fully explain complex human behavior. As a result, psychologists commonly draw insight from a variety of perspectives, with insights from one often complementing insights from the other. The three broad views that shape modern psychology are the biological, psychological, and sociocultural perspectives ( Table 1.3). • Archives of the History of American Psychology, University of Akron Table 1.2 Archives of the History of American Psychology, University of Akron CHAPTER 1 Archives of the History of American Psychology, University of Akron 26 Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods Table 1.3 27 • Contemporary Ways to Look at Behavior Biological Perspective Biopsychological View Key Idea: Human and animal behavior is the result of internal physical, chemical, and biological processes. Seeks to explain behavior through activity of the brain and nervous system, physiology, genetics, the endocrine system, and biochemistry; neutral, reductionistic, mechanistic view of human nature. Evolutionary View Key Idea: Human and animal behavior is the result of the process of evolution. Seeks to explain behavior through evolutionary principles based on natural selection; neutral, reductionistic, mechanistic view of human nature. S R INPUT Processing OUTPUT CONSCIOUS UNCONSCIOUS Selfimage Self Selfevaluation Social Self Psychological Perspective Behavioristic View Key Idea: Behavior is shaped and controlled by one’s environment. Emphasizes the study of observable behavior and the effects of learning; stresses the influence of external rewards and punishments; neutral, scientific, somewhat mechanistic view of human nature. Cognitive View Key Idea: Much human behavior can be understood in terms of the mental processing of information. Concerned with thinking, knowing, perception, understanding, memory, decision making, and judgment; explains behavior in terms of information processing; neutral, somewhat computer-like view of human nature. Psychodynamic View Key Idea: Behavior is directed by forces within one’s personality that are often hidden or unconscious. Emphasizes internal impulses, desires, and conflicts — especially those that are unconscious; views behavior as the result of clashing forces within personality; somewhat negative, pessimistic view of human nature. Humanistic View Key Idea: Behavior is guided by one’s self-image, by subjective perceptions of the world, and by needs for personal growth. Focuses on subjective, conscious experience, human problems, potentials, and ideals; emphasizes self-image and self-actualization to explain behavior; positive, philosophical view of human nature. Sociocultural Perspective Sociocultural View Key Idea: Behavior is influenced by one’s social and cultural context. Emphasizes that behavior is related to the social and cultural environment within which a person is born, grows up, and lives from day to day; neutral, interactionist view of human nature. Cultural The Biological Perspective The biological perspective seeks to explain our behavior in terms of biological principles such as brain processes, evolution, and genetics. By using new techniques, biopsychologists are producing exciting insights about how the brain relates to thinking, feelings, perception, abnormal behavior, and other topics. Biopsychologists and others who study the brain and nervous system, such as biologists and biochemists, are part of the broader field of neuroscience. As mentioned earlier, evolutionary psychologists look at how human evolution and genetics might explain our current behavior. acknowledge that mental processes underlie much of our behavior. Cognitive psychology has gained greater prominence in recent years as researchers have devised ways to objectively study covert behaviors, such as thinking, memory, language, perception, problem solving, consciousness, and creativity. Cognitive psychologists and other researchers interested in cognition, such as computer scientists and linguists, form the broader field of cognitive science. With a renewed interest in thinking, it can be said that psychology has finally “regained consciousness” (Robins, Gosling, & Craik, 1998). The Psychological Perspective The psychological perspective takes the view that behavior is shaped by psychological processes within each person. Although it continues to emphasize objective observation, just as the early behaviorists did, the psychological perspective now includes cognitive behaviorism and cognitive psychology. These perspectives Biological perspective The attempt to explain behavior in terms of underlying biological principles. Psychological perspective The traditional view that behavior is shaped by psychological processes occurring at the level of the individual. 28 CHAPTER 1 Patrick Giardino/Corbis Freudian psychoanalysis continues to evolve into the broader psychodynamic view. Although many of Freud’s ideas have been challenged, psychodynamic psychologists continue to trace human behavior to unconscious processes. They also seek to develop therapies to help people lead happier, fuller lives. Humanistic psychologists do, too, although they stress subjective, conscious experience and the positive side of human nature, rather than unconscious processes. Positive Psychology Psychologists have always paid attention to the negative side of human behavior. This is easy to understand because of the pressing need to solve human problems. However, inspired by the humanists, more and more psychologists have recently begun to ask, What do we know about love, happiness, creativity, well-being, self-confidence, and achievement? Together, such topics make up positive psychology, the study of human strengths, virtues, and optimal behavior (Compton, 2005; Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Many topics from positive psychology can be found in this book. Ideally, they will help make your own life more positive and fulfilling (Simonton & Baumeister, 2005). The Sociocultural Perspective As you can see, it is helpful to view human behavior from more than one perspective. This is also true in another sense. The sociocultural perspective stresses the impact that social and cultural contexts have on our behavior. We are rapidly becoming a multicultural society, made up of people from many different nations. How has this affected psychology? Let us introduce you to Jerry, who is Japanese American and is married to an Irish-Catholic American. Here is what Jerry, his wife, and their children did one New Year’s Day: We woke up in the morning and went to Mass at St. Brigid’s, which has a black gospel choir. . . . Then we went to the Japanese-American Community Center for the Oshogatsu New Year’s program and saw Buddhist archers shoot arrows to ward off evil spirits for the year. Next, we ate traditional rice cakes as part of the New Year’s service and listened to a young JapaneseAmerican storyteller. On the way home, we stopped in Chinatown and after that we ate Mexican food at a taco stand. (Njeri, 1991) Jerry and his family reflect a new social reality: Cultural diversity is becoming the norm. More than 100 million Americans are now African American, Hispanic, Asian American, Native American, or Pacific Islander (United States Bureau of Census, 2007). In some large cities “minority” groups are already the majority. In the past, psychology was based mostly on the cultures of North America and Europe. Now, we must ask, do the principles of Western psychology apply to people in all cultures? Are some psychological concepts invalid in other cultures? Are any universal? As psychologists have probed such questions, one thing has become clear: Most of what we think, feel, and do is influenced in one way or another by the social and cultural worlds in which we live (Lehman, Chiu, & Schaller, 2004). To fully understand human behavior, personal differences based on age, race, culture, ethnicity, gender, and sexual orientation must be taken into account. Cultural Relativity Imagine that you are a psychologist. Your client, Linda, who is a Native American, tells you that spirits live in the trees near her home. Is Linda suffering from a delusion? Is she abnormal? Obviously, you will misjudge Linda’s mental health if you fail to take her cultural beliefs into account. Cultural relativity (the idea that behavior must be judged relative to the values of the culture in which it occurs) can greatly affect the diagnosis and treatment of mental disorders (Draguns, Gielen, & Fish, 2004). Cases like Linda’s teach us to be wary of using narrow standards when judging others or comparing groups. bridges Psychotherapy can be less effective if a therapist and client come from different cultures. See Chapter 15, page 517, for a discussion of the impact of culture on therapy. A Broader View of Diversity In addition to cultural differences, age, ethnicity, gender, religion, disability, and sexual orientation all affect the social norms that guide behavior. Social norms are rules that define acceptable and expected behavior for members of various groups. All too often, the unstated standard for judging what is “average,” “normal,” or “correct” has been the behavior of white, middle-class males (Reid, 2002). To fully understand human behavior, psychologists need to know how people differ, as well as the ways in which we are all alike. To be effective, psychologists must be sensitive to people who are ethnically and culturally different from themselves (APA, 2003). For the same reason, an appreciation of human diversity can enrich your life, as well as your understanding of psychology (Denmark, Rabinowitz, & Sechzer, 2005). In a moment we will further explore what psychologists do. First, here are some questions to enhance your learning: Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods K N O W L E D GE B U I L D E R History and Contemporary Perspectives RECITE Match: _____ 1. _____ 2. _____ 3. _____ 4. _____ 5. _____ 6. _____ 7. _____ 8. _____ 9. _____ 10. _____ 11. Philosophy Wundt Structuralism Functionalism Behaviorism Gestalt Psychodynamic Humanistic Cognitive Washburn Biopsychology A. Against analysis; studied whole experiences B. “Mental chemistry” and introspection C. Emphasizes self-actualization and personal growth D. Interested in unconscious causes of behavior E. Interested in how the mind aids survival F. First woman Ph.D. in psychology G. Studied stimuli and responses, conditioning H. Part of psychology’s “long past” I. Concerned with thinking, language, problem solving J. Used introspection and careful measurement K. Relates behavior to the brain, physiology, and genetics L. Also known as engineering psychology 12. Who among the following was not a historic woman psychologist? a. Calkins b. Ladd-Franklin c. Washburn d. Watson 13. A psychotherapist is working with a person from an ethnic group other than her own. She should be aware of how cultural relativity and _____________________ affect behavior. a. the anthropomorphic error b. operational definitions c. biased sampling d. social norms 29 Psychologists — Guaranteed Not to Shrink Gateway Question: What are the major specialties in psychology? Psychologists are all shrinks, right? Nope. “Shrinks” (a slang term derived from “head shrinkers”) are psychiatrists, a different type of mental health professional. A psychologist is highly trained in the methods, knowledge, and theories of psychology. Psychologists usually have a master’s degree or a doctorate. These degrees typically require several years of postgraduate training. Psychologists are often inaccurately portrayed in the media. Some films have featured psychologists who are more disturbed than their patients (such as Jack Nicholson’s character in Anger Management) or psychologists who are bumbling buffoons (such as Billy Crystal’s character in Analyze This). In the comedy Prime REFLECT Relate Which school of thought most closely matches your own view of behavior? Do you think any of the early schools offers a complete explanation of why we behave as we do? What about the three broad contemporary perspectives? Can you explain why so many psychologists are eclectic? A group of psychologists were asked to answer this question: “Why did the chicken cross the road?” Their answers are listed next. Can you identify their theoretical orientations? The chicken had been rewarded for crossing road in the past. The chicken had an unconscious wish to become a pancake. The chicken was trying to solve the problem of how to reach the other side of the road. The chicken felt a need to explore new possibilities as a way to actualize its potentials. The chicken’s motor cortex was activated by messages from its hypothalamus. © Mike Baldwin/CartoonStock Critical Thinking 14. Modern sciences such as psychology are built on observations that can be verified by two or more independent observers. Did structuralism meet this standard? Why or why not? Positive psychology The study of human strengths, virtues, and effective functioning. Sociocultural perspective The focus on the importance of social and cultural contexts in influencing the behavior of individuals. Cultural relativity The idea that behavior must be judged relative to the values of the culture in which it occurs. Social norms Rules that define acceptable and expected behavior for members of a group. Psychologist A person highly trained in the methods, factual knowledge, and theories of psychology. Answers: 1. H 2. J 3. B 4. E 5. G 6. A 7. D 8. C 9. I 10. F 11. K 12. d 13. d 14. No, it did not. The downfall of structuralism was that each observer examined the contents of his or her own mind, which is something that no other person can observe. 30 CHAPTER 1 a therapist listens to a patient describe intimate details of her relationship with a man but fails to tell the patient that the man is her son. Such characters may be dramatic and entertaining, but they seriously distort public perceptions of responsible and hardworking psychologists (Schultz, 2004). Real psychologists follow an ethical code that stresses respect for people’s privacy, dignity, confidentiality, and welfare (APA, 2002). Even without media distortions, misconceptions about psychologists are common. For instance, most psychologists are not therapists in private practice. Instead, they are employed by schools, businesses, and social agencies. Likewise, only 16 percent work in clinics or hospitals. Contrary to common belief, clinical psychology is not the only specialty in the field. Only a little over half of all psychologists study mental disorders or do therapy. Others may teach, do research, give psychological tests, or serve as consultants to business, industry, government, or the military. Table 1.4 One perception of psychologists is accurate: Most do help people in one way or another. Psychologists interested in emotional problems specialize in clinical or counseling psychology ( Table 1.4). Clinical psychologists treat psychological problems or do research on therapies and mental disorders. In contrast, counseling psychologists tend to treat milder problems, such as troubles at work or school. However, such differences are fading, and many counseling psychologists now work full time as therapists. To enter the profession of psychology, it is best to have a doctorate (Ph.D., Psy.D., or Ed.D.). Most clinical psychologists have a Ph.D. degree and follow a scientist-practitioner model. That is, they are trained to do either research or therapy. Many do both. Other clinicians earn the Psy.D. (Doctor of Psychology) degree, which emphasizes therapy skills rather than research (Peterson, 2001). Have you ever wondered what it takes to become a psychologist? See “Is a Career in Psychology Right for You?” • • Kinds of Psychologists and What They Do Specialty Typical Activities Biopsychology B* Does research on the brain, nervous system, and other physical origins of behavior Clinical A Does psychotherapy; investigates clinical problems; develops methods of treatment Cognitive B Studies human thinking and information processing abilities Community A Promotes community-wide mental health through research, prevention, education, and consultation Comparative B Studies and compares the behavior of different species, especially animals Consumer A Researches packaging, advertising, marketing methods, and characteristics of consumers Counseling A Does psychotherapy and personal counseling; researches emotional disturbances and counseling methods Cultural B Studies the ways in which culture, subculture, and ethnic group membership affect behavior Developmental A, B Conducts research on infant, child, adolescent, and adult development; does clinical work with disturbed children; acts as consultant to parents and schools Educational A Investigates classroom dynamics, teaching styles, and learning; develops educational tests, evaluates educational programs Engineering A Does applied research on the design of machinery, computers, airlines, automobiles, and so on, for business, industry, and the military Environmental A, B Studies the effects of urban noise, crowding, attitudes toward the environment, and human use of space; acts as a consultant on environmental issues Forensic A Studies problems of crime and crime prevention, rehabilitation programs, prisons, courtroom dynamics; selects candidates for police work Gender B Does research on differences between males and females, the acquisition of gender identity, and the role of gender throughout life Health A, B Studies the relationship between behavior and health; uses psychological principles to promote health and prevent illness Industrialorganizational A Selects job applicants, does skills analysis, evaluates on-the-job training, improves work environments and human relations in organizations and work settings Learning B Studies how and why learning occurs; develops theories of learning Medical A Applies psychology to manage medical problems, such as the emotional impact of illness, self-screening for cancer, compliance in taking medicine Personality B Studies personality traits and dynamics; develops theories of personality and tests for assessing personality traits School A Does psychological testing, referrals, emotional and vocational counseling of students; detects and treats learning disabilities; improves classroom learning Sensation and perception B Studies the sense organs and the process of perception; investigates the mechanisms of sensation and develops theories about how perception occurs Social B Investigates human social behavior, including attitudes, conformity, persuasion, prejudice, friendship, aggression, helping, and so forth *Research in this area is typically applied (A), basic (B), or both (A, B). Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods 31 D ISCOVERI N G P S Y CH OL O G Y Is a Career in Psychology Right for You? As you read this book we encourage you to frequently reflect on new ideas by relating them to your own life to better understand and remember them. Discovering Psychology boxes like this one are designed to help you be more reflective about how psychology relates to your own life. Answer the following questions to explore whether you would enjoy becoming a psychologist: 1. I have a strong interest in human behavior. True or False? 2. I am good at recognizing patterns, evaluating evidence, and drawing conclusions. True or False? 3. I am emotionally stable. True or False? 4. I have good communication skills. True or False? 5. I find theories and ideas challenging and stimulating. True or False? 6. My friends regard me as especially sensitive to the feelings of others. True or False? 7. I enjoy planning and carrying out complex projects and activities. True or False? 8. Programs and popular books about psychology interest me. True or False? 9. I enjoy working with other people. True or False? 10. Clear thinking, objectivity, and keen observation appeal to me. True or False? If you answered “True” to most of these questions, a career in psychology might be a good choice. And remember that many psychology majors also succeed in occupations such as management, public affairs, social services, business, sales, and education. Other Mental Health Professionals The Profession of Psychology Clinical psychologists are not the only people who work in the field of mental health. Often they coordinate their efforts with other specially trained professionals. What are the differences among psychologists, psychiatrists, psychoanalysts, counselors, and other mental health professionals? Certainly, they’re not all “shrinks.” Each has a specific blend of training and skills. A psychiatrist is a medical doctor who treats mental disorders, usually by doing psychotherapy. Psychiatrists can also prescribe drugs, which is something psychologists usually cannot do. However, this is changing. Psychologists in New Mexico and Louisiana can now legally prescribe drugs. It will be interesting to see whether other states grant similar privileges (Munsey, 2006). To be a psychoanalyst, you must have a moustache and goatee, spectacles, a German accent, and a well-padded couch — or so the media stereotype goes. Actually, to become a psychoanalyst you must have an M.D. or Ph.D. degree plus further training in Freudian psychoanalysis. In other words, either a physician or a psychologist may become an analyst by learning a specific type of psychotherapy. In many states, counselors also do mental health work. A counselor is an adviser who helps solve problems with marriage, career, school, work, or the like. To be a licensed counselor (such as a marriage and family counselor, a child counselor, or a school counselor) typically requires a master’s degree plus 1 or 2 years of full-time supervised counseling experience. Counselors learn practical helping skills and do not treat serious mental disorders. Psychiatric social workers play an important role in many mental health programs, where they apply social science principles to help patients in clinics and hospitals. Most hold an M.S.W. (Master of Social Work) degree. Often, they assist psychologists and psychiatrists as part of a team. Their typical duties include evaluating patients and families; conducting group therapy; or visiting a patient’s home, school, or job to alleviate problems. Does a person have to have a license to practice psychology? At one time it was possible in many states for anyone to “hang out a shingle” as a “psychologist.” Now psychologists must meet rigorous educational and legal requirements. To work as a clinical or counseling psychologist you must have a license issued by a state examining board. However, the law does not prevent you from calling yourself anything else you choose — therapist, rebirther, primal feeling facilitator, cosmic aura balancer, or Rolfer — or from selling your “services” to anyone willing to pay. Beware of people with self-proclaimed titles. Even if their intentions are honorable, they may have little training. A licensed psychologist who chooses to use a particular type of therapy is not the same as someone “trained” solely in that technique. Ethics Most psychologists take pride in following a professional code that stresses (1) high levels of competence, integrity, and responsibility; (2) respect for people’s rights to privacy, dignity, confidentiality, Clinical psychologist A psychologist who specializes in the treatment of psychological and behavioral disturbances or who does research on such disturbances. Counseling psychologist A psychologist who specializes in the treatment of milder emotional and behavioral disturbances. Psychiatrist A medical doctor with additional training in the diagnosis and treatment of mental and emotional disorders. Psychoanalyst A mental health professional (usually a medical doctor) trained to practice psychoanalysis. Counselor A mental health professional who specializes in helping people with problems not involving serious mental disorder; for example, marriage counselors, career counselors, or school counselors. Psychiatric social worker A mental health professional trained to apply social science principles to help patients in clinics and hospitals. 32 CHAPTER 1 and personal freedom; and, above all, (3) protection of the client’s welfare. Psychologists are also expected to use their knowledge to contribute to society. Many do volunteer work in the communities in which they live (Barnett et al., 2007; APA, 2002). Specialties in Psychology Do all psychologists do therapy and treat abnormal behavior? No. About 58 percent are clinical and counseling psychologists. The rest are found in other specialties. At present, the American Psychological Association (APA) consists of more than 50 divisions, each reflecting special skills or areas of interest. Some of the major specialties are listed in Table 1.4. (Also see Figure 1.5.) Nearly 30 percent of all psychologists are employed full time at colleges • • or universities, where they teach and do research, consulting, or therapy. Some do basic research, in which they seek knowledge for its own sake. For example, a psychologist might study memory simply to understand how it works. Others do applied research to solve immediate practical problems, such as finding ways to improve athletic performance. Some do both types of research. In a moment we’ll take a closer look at how research is done. Before that, here’s a chance to do a little research on how much you’ve learned: KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R Psychologists and Their Specialties RECITE (a) Specialties in Psychology 48% Clinical 11% Counseling 18% Other 5% Experimental and other research areas 1% Health 2% Educational 4% School 4% Industrial/organizational 3% Developmental 4% Social and personality 1. Which of the following is a medical doctor? a. a psychologist b. a psychiatrist c. a psychotherapist d. a counselor 2. A psychologist who specializes in treating human emotional difficulties is called a ____________________ psychologist. 3. Roughly 40 percent of psychologists specialize in counseling psychology. T or F? 4. Who among the following would most likely be involved in the detection of learning disabilities? a. a consumer psychologist b. a forensic psychologist c. an experimental psychologist d. a school psychologist REFLECT 34% Private practice 28% Colleges and universities 14% Hospital/clinic 8% Other Relate Which specialty in psychology is most interesting to you? What is it about that specialty that most attracts you? Answers: 1. b 2. clinical or counseling 3. F 4. d 5. Because practitioners benefit from basic psychological research in the same way that physicians benefit from basic research in biology. Discoveries in basic science form the knowledge base that leads to useful applications. (b) Where Psychologists Work Critical Thinking 5. If most psychologists work in applied settings, why is basic research still of great importance? 4% Schools 6% Human services 6% Business, industry, government (c) What Psychologists Do (Primary Activity) 50% Mental health services 9% Other 4% Applied psychology 10% Research 18% Education and educational services 9% Management/administration • Figure 1.5 Specialties in psychology (APA, 2005). Percentages are approximate. (b) Where psychologists work (APA, 2000). (c) This chart shows the main activi(a) ties psychologists engage in at work (APA, 2000). Any particular psychologist might do several of these activities during a work week. As you can see, most psychologists specialize in applied areas and work in applied settings. The Psychology Experiment — Where Cause Meets Effect Gateway Question: How is an experiment performed? To get beyond description and fully understand behavior, psychologists must to be able explain why we act the way we do. To discover the causes of behavior, we must usually conduct an experiment. An experiment is a formal trial undertaken to confirm or disconfirm a hypothesis about the causes of behavior (although causes are sometimes revealed by naturalistic observation or correlations). Experiments allow psychologists to carefully control conditions and bring cause-and-effect relationships into sharp focus. Hence, they are generally accepted as the most powerful Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods scientific research tool. To perform an experiment you would do the following: 1. Directly vary a condition you think might affect behavior. 2. Create two or more groups of subjects. These groups should be alike in all ways except the condition you are varying. 3. Record whether varying the condition has any effect on behavior. Suppose you want to find out if using cell phones while driving a car affects the likelihood of having an accident. First, you would form two groups. Then you could give the members of one group a test of driving ability while they are using a cell phone. The second group would take the same test without using a cell phone. By comparing average driving ability scores for the two groups, you could tell if cell phone use affects driving ability. As you can see, the simplest psychological experiment is based on two groups of experimental subjects (animals or people whose behavior is investigated; human subjects are also called participants). One group is called the experimental group; the other becomes the control group. The control group and the experimental group are treated exactly alike except for the condition you intentionally vary. This condition is called the independent variable. Variables and Groups A variable is any condition that can change and that might affect the outcome of the experiment. Identifying causes and effects in an experiment involves three types of variables: 1. Independent variables are conditions altered or varied by the experimenter, who sets their size, amount, or value. Independent variables are suspected causes for differences in behavior. 2. Dependent variables measure the results of the experiment. That is, they reveal the effects that independent variables have on behavior. Such effects are often revealed by measures of performance, such as test scores. 3. Extraneous variables are conditions that a researcher wishes to prevent from affecting the outcome of the experiment. We can apply these terms to our cell phone/driving experiment in this way: Cell phone use is the independent variable — we want to know if cell phone use affects driving ability. Driving ability (defined by scores achieved on a test of driving ability) is the dependent variable — we want to know if the ability to drive well depends on whether a person is using a cell phone. All other variables that could affect driving ability are extraneous. Examples of extraneous variables are the number of hours slept the night before the test, driving experience, or familiarity with the car used in the experiment. (You might be interested to know that people using cell phones drive no better than people who are legally drunk [Strayer, Drews, & Crouch, 2006].) As you can see, an experimental group consists of participants exposed to the independent variable (cell phone use in the preced- 33 ing example). Members of the control group are exposed to all conditions except the independent variable. Let’s examine another simple experiment. Suppose you notice that you seem to study better while listening to your iPod. This suggests the hypothesis that music improves learning. We could test this idea by forming an experimental group that studies with music. A control group would study without music. Then we could compare their scores on a test. Is a control group really needed? Can’t people just study while listening to their iPods to see if they do better? Better than what? The control group provides a point of reference for comparison with the scores in the experimental group. Without a control group it would be impossible to tell if music had any effect on learning. If the average test score of the experimental group is higher than the average of the control group, we can conclude that music improves learning. If there is no difference, it’s obvious that the independent variable had no effect on learning. In this experiment, the amount learned (indicated by scores on the test) is the dependent variable. We are asking, Does the independent variable affect the dependent variable? (Does music affect or influence learning?) Experimental Control How do we know that the people in one group aren’t more intelligent than those in the other group? It’s true that personal differences might affect the experiment. However, they can be controlled by randomly assigning people to groups. Random assignment means that a participant has an equal chance of being in either the experimental group or the control group. Randomization evenly balances personal differences in the two groups. In our musical experiment, this could be done by simply flipping a coin for each participant: Heads, and the participant is in the experimental Experiment A formal trial undertaken to confirm or disconfirm a hypothesis about cause and effect. Experimental subjects Humans (also referred to as participants) or animals whose behavior is investigated in an experiment. Variable Any condition that changes or can be made to change; a measure, event, or state that may vary. Independent variable In an experiment, the condition being investigated as a possible cause of some change in behavior. The values that this variable takes are chosen by the experimenter. Dependent variable In an experiment, the condition (usually a behavior) that is affected by the independent variable. Extraneous variables Conditions or factors excluded from influencing the outcome of an experiment. Experimental group In a controlled experiment, the group of subjects exposed to the independent variable or experimental condition. Control group In a controlled experiment, the group of subjects exposed to all experimental conditions or variables except the independent variable. Random assignment The use of chance (for example, flipping a coin) to assign subjects to experimental and control groups. 34 CHAPTER 1 C R IT ICAL T H I N KI N G That’s Interesting, but Is It Ethical? In one classic study, participants were ordered to give what they thought were painful electric shocks to another person (no shocks were actually given) (Milgram, 1963). Believing that they had hurt someone, many people left the experiment shaken and upset. A few suffered guilt and distress for some time afterward. bridges Stanley Milgram undertook his infamous studies to better understand just how willing people are to obey an authority. See Chapter 16, pages 547–549. Such experiments raise serious ethical questions about behavioral research. Did the information gained justify the emotional costs? Was deception really necessary? As a reply to such questions, American Psychological Association guidelines state that “Psychologists must carry out investigations with respect for the people who participate and with concern for their dignity and welfare.” Similar guidelines apply to animals, where investigators are expected to “ensure the welfare of animals and treat them humanely” (APA, 2002) ( Table 1.5). To assure this, most university psychology departments have ethics committees that oversee research. Nevertheless, no easy answers exist for the ethical questions raised by psychology, and debate about specific experiments is likely to continue. • Possible subjects Random assignment controls for subject differences Control group Experimental group Study and testing conditions Music included Behavior (test scores) • Identical conditions to control extraneous variables Independent variable (Cause) Dependent variable (Effect) Study and testing conditions No music Behavior (test scores) Is there a difference? Figure 1.6 Elements of a simple psychological experiment to assess the effects on test scores of music during study. • Table 1.5 Basic Ethical Guidelines for Psychological Researchers Do no harm. Accurately describe risks to potential participants. Ensure that participation is voluntary. Minimize any discomfort to participants. Maintain confidentiality. Do not unnecessarily invade privacy. Use deception only when absolutely necessary. Remove any misconceptions caused by deception (debrief ). Provide results and interpretations to participants. Treat participants with dignity and respect. amount of light, and so forth — must also be prevented from affecting the outcome of an experiment. But how? Usually this is done by making all conditions (except the independent variable) exactly alike for both groups. When all conditions are the same for both groups — except the presence or absence of music — then any difference in the amount learned must be caused by the music ( Figure 1.6). (Psychology experiments sometimes raise ethical questions. See “That’s Interesting, but Is It Ethical” for more information.) • Cause and Effect Now let’s summarize. In an experiment, two or more groups of subjects are treated differently with respect to the independent variable. In all other ways they are treated the same. That is, extraneous variables are equated for all groups. The effect of the independent variable (or variables) on some behavior (the dependent variable) is then measured. In a carefully controlled experiment, the independent variable is the only possible cause for any effect noted in the dependent variable. This allows clear cause-andeffect connections to be identified ( Figure 1.7). • Evaluating Results group; tails, it’s the control group. This would result in few average differences in the number of people in each group who are women or men, geniuses or dunces, hungry, hungover, tall, music lovers, or whatever. Other extraneous, or outside, variables — such as the amount of study time, the temperature in the room, the time of day, the How can we tell if the independent variable really made a difference? This problem is handled statistically. Reports in psychology journals almost always include the statement, “Results were statistically significant.” What this means is that the obtained results would occur very rarely by chance alone. To be statistically significant, a difference must be large enough so that it would occur by Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods Dependent Variable A B C A B C 35 nothing. Later, she assesses how much each participant learned. Does this experiment seem valid? Actually, it is seriously flawed for several reasons. Research Participant Bias Experimental Group Control Group Extraneous Variables Independent Variable 1.7 Experimental control is achieved by balancing extraneous vari•ablesFigure for the experimental group and the control group. For example, the average age (A), education (B), and intelligence (C) of group members could be made the same for both groups. Then we could apply the independent variable to the experimental group. If their behavior (the dependent variable) changes (in comparison with the control group), the change must be caused by the independent variable. chance in less than 5 experiments out of 100. (See the Statistics Appendix for more information.) Of course, findings also become more convincing when they can be replicated (repeated) by other researchers. Meta-Analysis As you might guess, numerous studies are done on important topics in psychology. Although each study adds to our understanding, the results of various studies don’t always agree. Let’s say we are interested in whether males or females tend to be greater risk takers. A computer search would reveal that more than 100 studies have investigated various types of risk-taking (for example, smoking, fast driving, or unprotected sex). Is there a way to combine the results of the studies? Yes, a statistical technique called meta-analysis can be used to combine the results of many studies as if they were all part of one big study (Rosenthal & DiMatteo, 2001). In other words, a meta-analysis is a study of the results of other studies. In recent years, meta-analysis has been used to summarize and synthesize mountains of psychological research. This allows us to see the big picture and draw conclusions that might be missed in a single, small-scale study. Oh, and about that risk-taking question: A meta-analysis showed that males do tend to take more risks than females (Byrnes, Miller, & Schafer, 1999). (The most frequent last words uttered by deceased young males is rumored to be, “Hey, watch this!”) Double Blind — On Placebos and Self-Fulfilling Prophecies Gateway Question: What is a double-blind experiment? Suppose a researcher hypothesizes that the drug amphetamine (a stimulant) improves learning. She explains her hypothesis to her participants and gives experimental group participants an amphetamine pill before they begin studying. Control group members get Why? The experimental group took the drug and the control group didn’t. Differences in the amount they learned must have been caused by the drug, right? No, because the drug wasn’t the only difference between the groups. For a start, because of what they were told, participants in the experimental group likely expected to learn more. Any observed differences between groups then may reflect differences in expectation, not the actual effect of the drug. In a well-designed experiment, you must be careful about what you tell participants. Small bits of information might create research participant bias, or changes in participants’ behavior caused by the influence of their expectations. Notice also that experimental group participants swallowed a pill, and control participants did not. This is another form of research participant bias. It could be that those who swallowed a pill unconsciously expected to do better. After all, pills are medicine, aren’t they? This alone might have created a placebo effect (changes in behavior caused by belief that one has taken a drug). Suppose the researcher had not given the experimental group an amphetamine pill and instead had given them a placebo (plahSEE-bo) or fake drug. Inactive substances such as sugar pills and saline (saltwater) injections are commonly used as placebos. If a placebo has any effect, it must be based on suggestion, rather than chemistry (Thompson, 2005). Placebo effects can be quite powerful. For instance, a saline injection is 70 percent as effective as morphine in reducing pain. That’s why doctors sometimes prescribe placebos — especially for complaints that seem to have no physical basis. Placebos have been shown to affect pain, anxiety, depression, alertness, tension, sexual arousal, cravings for alcohol, and many other processes (Wampold et al., 2005). How could an inert substance have any effect? Placebos alter our expectations, both conscious and unconscious, about our own emotional and physical reactions. Because we associate taking medicine with feeling better, we expect placebos to make us feel better, too (Stewart-Williams, 2004). After a person takes a pla- Statistical significance Experimental results that would rarely occur by chance alone. Meta-analysis A statistical technique for combining the results of many studies on the same subject. Research participant bias Changes in the behavior of research participants caused by the unintended influence of their own expectations. Placebo effect Changes in behavior due to participant’s expectations that a drug (or other treatment) will have some effect. Placebo An inactive substance given in the place of a drug in psychological research or by physicians who wish to treat a complaint by suggestion. CHAPTER 1 Royalty-Free/Corbis 36 The placebo effect is a major factor in medical treatments. Would you also expect the placebo effect to occur in psychotherapy? (It does, which complicates studies on the effectiveness of new psychotherapies.) cebo, there is a reduction in brain activity linked with pain, so the effect is not imaginary (Wager et al., 2004). Controlling Research Participant Bias How can you avoid research participant bias? To control for research participant bias, we could use a single-blind experiment. In this case, participants do not know if they are in the experimental or the control group or if they are receiving a real drug or a placebo. All participants are given the same instructions and everyone gets a pill or injection. People in the experimental group get a real drug, and those in the control group get a placebo. Because participants are blind as to the hypothesis under investigation and whether they received the drug, their expectations (conscious and unconscious) are the same. Any difference in their behavior must be caused by the drug. However, even this arrangement is not enough, because researchers themselves sometimes affect experiments by influencing participants. Let’s see how this occurs. Although the teachers were not conscious of any bias, apparently they subtly communicated their expectations to students. Most likely, they did this through tone of voice, body language, and by giving encouragement or criticism. Their “hints,” in turn, created a self-fulfilling prophecy that affected the students. A selffulfilling prophecy is a prediction that prompts people to act in ways that make the prediction come true. For instance, many teachers underestimate the abilities of ethnic minority children, which hurts the students’ chances for success (Weinstein, Gregory, & Strambler, 2004). In short, people sometimes become what we prophesy for them. It is wise to remember that others tend to live up or down to our expectations for them ( Jussim & Harber, 2005). The Double-Blind Experiment Because of research participant bias and researcher bias, it is common to keep both participants and researchers “blind.” In a double-blind experiment neither subjects nor researchers know who is in the experimental group or the control group, including who received a drug and who took a placebo. Typically, someone else conducts the experiment, including preparing the pills or injections so that actual researchers don’t know until after testing who got what. This not only controls for research participant bias, it also keeps researchers from unconsciously influencing participants. Double-blind testing has shown that about 50 percent of the effectiveness of antidepressant drugs, such as the “wonder drug” Prozac, is due to the placebo effect (Kirsch & Sapirstein, 1998). Much of the popularity of herbal health remedies is also based on the placebo effect (Seidman, 2001). bridges For more information about how psychologists study placebos, see Chapter 13, page 449. Researcher Bias How could a researcher influence subjects?As we saw earlier, when the experimenter explained her hypothesis to the participants, she likely biased the results of the study. But even if a researcher uses a single-blind procedure to avoid deliberately biasing participants, researcher bias (changes in behavior caused by the unintended influence of a researcher) remains a problem. In essence, experimenters run the risk of finding what they expect to find. This occurs because humans are very sensitive to hints about what is expected of them (Rosenthal, 1994). Researcher bias even applies outside the laboratory. Psychologist Robert Rosenthal (1973) reports an example of how expectations influence people: At the U.S. Air Force Academy Preparatory School, 100 airmen were randomly assigned to five different math classes. Their teachers did not know about this random placement. Instead, each teacher was told that his or her students had unusually high or low ability. Students in the classes labeled “high ability” improved much more in math scores than those in “low-ability” classes. Yet initially all the classes had students of equal ability. KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R The Psychology Experiment RECITE 1. To understand cause and effect, a simple psychological experiment is based on creating two groups: the ______________________________ group and the _____________________________ group. 2. There are three types of variables to consider in an experiment: ______________________ variables (which are manipulated by the experimenter), _____________________ variables (which measure the outcome of the experiment), and _________________________ variables (factors to be excluded in a particular experiment). 3. A researcher performs an experiment to learn if room temperature affects the amount of aggression displayed by college students under crowded conditions in a simulated prison environment. In this experiment, the independent variable is which of the following? a. room temperature b. the amount of aggression c. crowding d. the simulated prison environment Continued Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods 4. A procedure used to control both research participant bias and researcher bias in drug experiments is the a. correlation method b. extraneous prophecy c. double-blind technique d. random assignment of subjects REFLECT Critical Thinking 5. There is a loophole in the statement, “I’ve been taking vitamin C tablets, and I haven’t had a cold all year. Vitamin C is great!” What is the loophole? 6. People who believe strongly in astrology have personality characteristics that actually match, to a degree, those predicted by their astrological signs. Can you explain why this occurs? Relate In a sense, we all conduct little experiments to detect cause-andeffect connections. If you are interested in cooking, for example, you might try adding a particular spice to a meal on one occasion but not another. The question then becomes, “Does the use of the spice (the independent variable) affect the appeal of the meal (the dependent variable)?” By comparing a spiced meal (the control group) with an unspiced meal (the experimental group) you could find out if that spice is worth using. Can you think of at least one informal experiment you’ve run in the last month? What were the variables? What was the outcome? 37 Naturalistic Observation Psychologists sometimes actively observe behavior in a natural setting (the typical environment in which a person or animal lives). For example, in 1960 Jane Goodall observed a chimpanzee use a grass stem as a tool to remove termites from a termite mound (Van Lawick-Goodall, 1971). Notice that naturalistic observation only provides descriptions of behavior. In order to explain observations we may need information from other research methods. Just the same, Goodall’s discovery showed that humans are not the only tool-making animals (Nakamichi, 2004). Chimpanzees in zoos use objects as tools. Doesn’t that demonstrate the same thing? Not necessarily. Naturalistic observation allows us to study behavior that hasn’t been tampered with or altered by outside influences. Only by observing chimps in their natural environment can we tell if they use tools without human interference. Limitations Doesn’t the presence of human observers affect the animals’ behavior? Yes. The observer effect is a major problem. The observer effect refers to changes in a subject’s behavior caused by an awareness of being observed. Naturalists must be very careful to keep their distance and avoid “making friends” with the animals they are watching. Likewise, if you are interested in why automobile drivers have traffic accidents, you can’t simply get in people’s cars and start taking notes. As a stranger, your presence would probably change the driver’s behaviors. When possible, the observer effect can be minimized by concealing the observer. Another solution is to use hidden recorders. One naturalistic study of traffic accidents was done with video cameras installed in Answers: 1. experimental, control 2. independent, dependent, extraneous 3. a 4. c 5. The statement implies that vitamin C prevents colds. However, not getting a cold could just be a coincidence. A controlled experiment with a group given vitamin C and a control group not taking vitamin C would be needed to learn if vitamin C actually has any effect on susceptibility to colds. 6. Belief in astrology can create a selffulfilling prophecy in which people alter their behaviors and selfconcepts to match their astrological signs (van Rooij, 1994). Single-blind experiment An arrangement in which participants remain unaware of whether they are in the experimental group or the control group. Nonexperimental Research Methods — Different Strokes Gateway Question: What nonexperimental research methods do psychologists use? Determining cause-and-effect relationships between variables lies at the heart of explaining not just what we do, but why we do it. For this reason, psychologists place a special emphasis on controlled experimentation (experimental method). However, because it is not always possible to conduct experiments, psychologists gather evidence and test hypotheses in many others ways ( Jackson, 2008). They observe behavior as it unfolds in natural settings (naturalistic observation); they make measurements to discover relationships between events (correlational method); they study psychological problems and therapies in clinical settings (clinical method); and they use questionnaires to poll large groups of people (survey method). Let’s see how each of these is used to advance psychological knowledge. Researcher bias Changes in subjects’ behavior caused by the unintended influence of a researcher’s actions. Self-fulfilling prophecy A prediction that prompts people to act in ways that make the prediction come true. Double-blind experiment An arrangement in which both participants and experimenters are unaware of whether participants are in the experimental group or the control group, including who might have been administered a drug or a placebo. Experimental method Investigating causes of behavior through controlled experimentation. Naturalistic observation Observing behavior as it unfolds in natural settings. Correlational method Making measurements to discover relationships between events. Clinical method Studying psychological problems and therapies in clinical settings. Survey method Using questionnaires and surveys to poll large groups of people. Observer effect Changes in a person’s behavior brought about by an awareness of being observed. 38 CHAPTER 1 100 cars (Dingus et al., 2006). It turns out that most accidents are caused by failing to look at the traffic in front of the car (eyes forward!). Hidden stationary video cameras have also provided valuable observations of many animal species. As recording devices have become miniaturized, it has even become possible to attach “critter cams” directly to many species, allowing observations to be in a wide range of natural environments ( Figure 1.8). For example, zoologist Christian Rutz and his colleagues outfitted shy New Caledonian crows with “crow cams” to better understand their use of tools to forage for food (Rutz et al., 2007). Apparently humans and other primates are not the only tool-using species. Observer bias is a related problem in which observers see what they expect to see or record only selected details ( Jackson, 2008). For instance, teachers in one classic study were told to watch normal elementary school children who had been labeled (for the study) as “learning disabled,” “mentally retarded,” “emotionally disturbed,” or “normal.” Sadly, teachers gave the children very different ratings, depending on the labels used (Foster & Ysseldyke, 1976). In some situations, observer bias can have serious consequences (Spano, 2005). For example, a police officer expecting criminal behavior might shoot a person who is reaching for his wallet because he appears to be reaching for a gun. A special mistake to avoid while observing animals is the anthropomorphic (AN-thro-po-MORE-fik) error. This is the error of attributing human thoughts, feelings, or motives to animals— especially as a way of explaining their behavior (Wynne, 2004). The temptation to assume that an animal is “angry,” “jealous,” “bored,” • or “guilty” can be strong. If you have pets at home, you probably already know how difficult it is to avoid anthropomorphizing. But it can lead to false conclusions. For example, if your dog growls at your girlfriend every time she visits, you might assume the dog doesn’t like her. But maybe she wears a perfume that irritates the dog’s nose. Psychologists doing naturalistic studies make a special effort to minimize bias by keeping an observational record, or detailed summary of data and observations. As suggested by the study of traffic accidents and the use of “critter cams,” video recording often provides the most objective record of all. Despite its problems, naturalistic observation can supply a wealth of information and raise many interesting questions. In most scientific research it is an excellent starting point. Correlational Studies Let’s say a psychologist notes an association between the IQs of parents and their children, or between beauty and social popularity, or between anxiety and test performance, or even between crime and the weather. In each case, two observations or events are correlated (linked together in an orderly way). The Los Angeles study of crime and temperature mentioned earlier in this chapter is an example of a correlational study. Such studies evaluate the degree of relationship, or correlation, between two existing traits, behaviors, or events. First, two factors are measured. Then a statistical technique is used to find their degree of correlation. For example, we could find the correlation between the number of hours spent practicing and sports performance during competitions. If the correlation is large, knowing how much a person practices would allow us to predict his or her success in competition. Likewise, success in competition could be used to predict the amount of practice. Psychologists seek to identify relationships concerning memory, perception, stress, aging, therapy, and a host of similar topics. Much of this book is a summary of such relationships. Correlation Coefficients How is the degree of correlation expressed? The strength and direction of a relationship can be expressed as a coefficient of correlation. This can be calculated as a number falling somewhere between 1.00 and 1.00. Drawing graphs of relationships can also help clarify their nature ( Figure 1.9). If the number is zero or close to zero, the association between two measures is weak or nonexistent ( see Figure 1.9c). For example, the correlation between shoe size and intelligence is zero. (Sorry, size 12 readers.) If the correlation is 1.00, a perfect positive relationship exists ( see Figure 1.9e); if it is –1.00, a perfect negative relationship has been discovered ( see Figure 1.9a). • • © Jolyon Troscianko 2006 • Figure 1.8 New Caledonian crows wearing tiny “crow cams” barely half the •weight of a silver dollar have been recorded using twigs to forage for food (Rutz et al., 2007). bridges For more details about calculating and graphing correlations, see the Statistics Appendix. • 39 Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods Correlations in psychology are rarely perfect. But the closer the coefficient is to 1.00 or –1.00, the stronger the relationship. For example, identical twins tend to have almost identical IQs. In contrast, the IQs of parents and their children are only generally similar. The correlation between the IQs of parents and children is .35; between identical twins it’s .86. Correlation Strength of relationship Negative Perfect –1.00 Medium Small –.30 –.10 Small Medium Large Very large +.10 +.30 +.50 +.75 Perfect +1.00 Y Perfect negative relationship X (a) Moderate negative relationship No relationship Moderate positive relationship Perfect positive relationship (b) (c) (d) (e) X Figure 1.9 The correlation coefficient tells how strongly two measures are related. These graphs show a range of rela•tionships between two measures, X and Y. If a correlation is negative, increases in one measure are associated with decreases in the other. (As Y gets larger, X gets smaller.) In a positive correlation, increases in one measure are associated with increases in the other. (As Y gets larger, X gets larger.) Graph b (“moderate negative relationship”) might result from comparing grades in college (Y) with hours spent playing computer games (X): Higher anxiety is associated with lower scores. Graph c (“no relationship”) would result from plotting a person’s shoe size (Y) and his or her IQ (X). Graph d (“moderate positive relationship”) could be a plot of grades in high school (Y) and grades in college (X) for a group of students: Higher grades in high school are associated with higher grades in college. What do the terms “positive” and “negative” correlation mean? A positive correlation shows that higher scores in one measure are matched by higher scores in the other. For example, there is a moderate positive correlation between high school grades and college grades; students who do well in high school tend to do well in college (and the reverse) (see Figure 1.9d). In a negative correlation, higher scores in the first measure are associated with lower scores in the second. We might observe, for instance, a moderate negative correlation between the number of hours that students play computer games and grades. That is, more play is associated with lower grades. (This is the wellknown computer-game-zombie effect; see Figure 1.9b). Wouldn’t that show that playing computer games too much causes lower grades?It might seem so, but as we saw previously, the best way to be confident that a cause-and-effect relationship exists is to perform a controlled experiment. Z Typical diet in mental hospitals • • Correlation and Causation Correlational studies help us discover relationships and make predictions. However, correlation does not demonstrate causation (a cause-effect relationship) (Elder, 2006). It could be, for instance, that students who aren’t interested in their classes have more time for computer games. If so, then their lack of study and lower grades would both result from disinterest, not from excess game playing. Just because one thing appears to be related to another does not mean that a cause-and-effect connection exists. Here is another example of mistaking correlation for causation: What if a psychologist discovers that the blood of patients with schizophrenia contains a certain chemical not found in the general population? Does this show that the chemical causes schizophrenia? It may seem so, but schizophrenia could cause the chemical to form. Or both schizophrenia and the chemical might be caused by some unknown third factor, such as the typical diet in mental hospitals ( Figure 1.10). To reiterate, just because one thing appears • Zero 0.0 Y bridges Correlations between the IQs of family members are used to estimate the degree to which intelligence is affected by heredity and environment. See Chapter 9, pages 312–313. Positive Very large Large –.75 –.50 Figure 1.10 A correlation •between two variables might mean that X causes Y, that Y causes X, or that a third variable, Z, causes both X and Y. Schizophrenia Chemical in blood X Y Observer bias The tendency of an observer to distort observations or perceptions to match his or her expectations. Anthropomorphic error The error of attributing human thoughts, feelings, or motives to animals, especially as a way of explaining their behavior. Observational record A detailed summary of observed events or a videotape of observed behavior. Correlation The existence of a consistent, systematic relationship between two events, measures, or variables. Correlational study A nonexperimental study designed to measure the degree of relationship (if any) between two or more events, measures, or variables. Coefficient of correlation A statistical index ranging from 1.00 to 1.00 that indicates the direction and degree of correlation. Positive correlation A statistical relationship in which increases in one measure are matched by increases in the other (or decreases correspond with decreases). Negative correlation A statistical relationship in which increases in one measure are matched by decreases in the other. Causation The act of causing some effect. 40 CHAPTER 1 to cause another does not confirm that it does. The best way to be confident that a cause-and-effect relationship exists is to perform a controlled experiment. The Clinical Method — One Case at a Time It may be impractical, unethical, or impossible to use the experimental method to study rare events, such as unusual mental disorders, childhood “geniuses,” or “rampage” school shootings (Harding, Fox, & Mehta, 2002). In such instances, a case study (an in-depth focus on a single subject) may be the best source of information. Clinical psychologists rely heavily on case studies, especially as a way to investigate mental disorders, such as depression or psychosis. Also, case studies of psychotherapy have provided many useful ideas about how to treat emotional problems (Wedding & Corsini, 2005). Case studies may sometimes be thought of as natural clinical tests (accidents or other natural events that provide psychological data). Gunshot wounds, brain tumors, accidental poisonings, and similar disasters have provided much information about the human brain. One remarkable case from the history of psychology is reported by Dr. J. M. Harlow (1868). Phineas Gage, a young foreman on a work crew, had a 13-pound steel rod blown through the front of his brain by a dynamite explosion ( Figure 1.11). Amazingly, he survived the accident. Within 2 months Gage could walk, talk, and move normally, but the injury forever changed his personality. Instead of the honest and dependable worker he had been before, Gage became a surly, foul-mouthed liar. Dr. Harlow carefully recorded all details of what was perhaps the first in-depth case study of an accidental frontal lobotomy (the destruction of front brain matter). • • Figure 1.11 Some of the earliest information on the effects of damage to frontal areas of the brain came from a case study of the accidental injury of Phineas Gage. When a Los Angeles carpenter named Michael Melnick suffered a similar injury, he recovered completely, with no lasting ill effects. Melnick’s very different reaction to a similar injury shows why psychologists prefer controlled experiments and often use lab animals for studies of the brain. Case studies lack formal control groups. This, of course, limits the conclusions that can be drawn from clinical observations. Nevertheless, case studies can provide special opportunities to answer interesting questions. For instance, a classic case study in psychology concerns identical quadruplets, known as the Genain sisters. In addition to having identical genes, all four women became schizophrenic before age 25 (Rosenthal & Quinn, 1977). The chances of identical quadruplets all becoming schizophrenic are about 1 in 1.5 billion. The Genains, who have been studied for more than 40 years, were in and out of mental hospitals most of their lives. The fact that they share identical genes suggests that mental disorders are influenced by heredity. The fact that some of the sisters are more disturbed than others suggests that environmental conditions also affect mental illness. Indeed, Myra, the least ill of the four, was the only sister who was able to avoid her father, an alcoholic who terrorized, spied on, and sexually molested the girls. Thus, cases like theirs provide insights that can’t be obtained by any other means (Mirsky et al., 2000). bridges See Chapter 14, pages 472–476, for more information about the causes of schizophrenia. Survey Method — Here, Have a Sample Sometimes psychologists would like to ask everyone in the world a few well-chosen questions: “Do you drink coffee? How often per week?” “What form of discipline did your parents use when you were a child?” “What is the most dishonest thing you’ve done?” Honest answers to such questions can reveal much about people’s behavior. But, because it is impossible to question everyone, doing a survey is often more practical. In the survey method, public polling techniques are used to answer psychological questions (Tourangeau, 2004). Typically, people in a representative sample are asked a series of carefully worded questions. A representative sample is a small group that accurately reflects a larger population. A good sample must include the same proportion of men, women, young, old, professionals, blue-collar workers, Republicans, Democrats, whites, African Americans, Native Americans, Latinos, Asians, and so on as found in the population as a whole. A population is an entire group of animals or people belonging to a particular category (for example, all college students or all single women). Ultimately, we are interested in entire populations. But by selecting a smaller sample we can draw conclusions about the larger group without polling each and every person. Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods 41 Representative samples are often obtained by randomly selecting who will be included ( Figure 1.12). (Notice that this is similar to randomly assigning participants to groups in an experiment.) How accurate is the survey method? Modern surveys such as the Gallup and Harris polls are quite accurate. The Gallup poll has erred in its election predictions by only 1.5 percent since 1954. However, if a survey is based on a biased sample, it may paint a false picture. A biased sample does not accurately reflect the population from which it was drawn. Surveys done by magazines, websites, and online information services can be quite biased. Surveys on the Figure 1.12 If you were conducting a survey in which a person’s height might be an important variable, use of guns done by O: The Oprah Magazine the upper, nonrandom sample would be very unrepresentative. The lower sample, selected using a table of ranand Guns and Ammo magazine would prob- dom numbers, better represents the group as a whole. ably produce very different results — neither of rassed? Replies to survey questions are not always accurate or which would represent the general population. That’s why psycholtruthful. Many people show a distinct courtesy bias (a tendency to ogists using the survey method go to great lengths to ensure that give “polite” or socially desirable answers). For example, answers their samples are representative. Fortunately, people can often be to questions concerning sex, drinking or drug use, income, and polled by telephone, which makes it easier to obtain large samples. church attendance tend to be less than truthful. Likewise, the Even if one person out of three refuses to answer survey questions, week after an election, more people will say they voted than actuthe results are still likely to be valid (Hutchinson, 2004). ally did (Hutchinson, 2004). In recent years, 93 percent of human subjects in psychology experiments have been recruited from introductory psychology Summary courses (Sieber & Saks, 1989). The majority of these subjects have been white members of the middle class, and most of the Despite their limitations, surveys frequently produce valuable researchers themselves have been white males (Guthrie, 2004). information. For instance, the survey method has been used to None of this automatically invalidates the results of psycholfind out about the attitudes of Muslims in the Middle East toward ogy experiments. However, it may place some limitations on U.S. policies in the region and to clarify the debate about Islam their meanings. (See “Is There a Gender Bias in Psychologiand violence that has arisen since the terrorist attacks of 9/11 cal Research?”) The distinguished psychologist Edward Tolman (Haddad, 2003). To sum up, the survey method can be a powerful once noted that much of psychology is based on two sets of research tool. Like other methods, it has limitations, but new techsubjects: rats and college sophomores. Tolman urged scientists niques and strategies are providing valuable information about our to remember that rats are certainly not people and that college behavior (Kahneman et al., 2004). sophomores may not be! Is so much emphasis on science really necessary in psychology? In a word, yes. As we have seen, science is a powerful way of asking quesInternet Surveys tions about the world and getting trustworthy answers. ( Table 1.6 Recently, psychologists have started doing surveys and experisummarizes many of the important ideas we have covered.) ments on the Internet. Web-based research can be a cost-effective way to reach very large groups of people. Internet studies have provided interesting information about topics such as anger, decision making, racial prejudice, what disgusts people, religion, sexual Case study An in-depth focus on all aspects of a single person. attitudes, and much more. Biased samples can limit web-based Survey method The use of public polling techniques to answer research (because it isn’t easy to control who actually answers your psychological questions. online questionnaire), but psychologists are finding ways to gather Representative sample A small, randomly selected part of a larger valid information with it (Birnbaum, 2004; Whitaker, 2007). population that accurately reflects characteristics of the whole • • • Social Desirability Even well-designed surveys may be limited by another problem. If a psychologist were to ask you detailed questions about your sexual history and current sexual behavior, how accurate would your replies be? Would you exaggerate? Would you be embar- population. Population An entire group of animals or people belonging to a particular category (for example, all college students or all married women). Biased sample A subpart of a larger population that does not accurately reflect characteristics of the whole population. 42 CHAPTER 1 HUM AN D IVERS I T Y Is There a Gender Bias in Psychological Research? As you read through this book you may find yourself wondering whether or not a particular concept, theory, or research finding applies equally well to women and men, to members of various races or ethnic groups, or to people of different ages or sexual orientations. Human Diversity boxes like this one will help you be more reflective about our multicultural, multifaceted society. Here, let’s begin with a basic question: Is there is a gender bias in the research process itself? Many doctors continue to recommend that adults take an aspirin a day to help prevent a heart attack. Both men and women are given this advice. The problem? Not a single woman was included in the original sample on which the advice is based. Although females make up more than half the population, they continue to be neglected in psychological and medical research (Hyde, 2004). This oversight is just one form of gender bias in research. This term refers to the tendency for females to be underrepresented as research subjects and female Table 1.6 topics to be ignored by many investigators. Consequently, the investigators assumed that conclusions based on men also apply to women. But without directly studying women it is impossible to know how often this assumption is wrong. A related problem occurs when researchers combine results from men and women. Doing so can hide important male–female differences. An additional problem is that unequal numbers of men and women may volunteer for some kinds of research. For example, in studies of sexuality, more male college students volunteer to participate than females (Wiederman, 1999). What a surprise! Another form of gender bias in research occurs when women are underrepresented among the researchers themselves. In one example, Laurence Kohlberg (1969) proposed a theory about how we develop moral values. His studies suggested that women were morally “immature” because they were not as concerned with justice as men were. However, few women were involved in doing the studies and Kohlberg merely assumed that theories based on men also apply to women. In response, Carol Gilligan (1982) provided evidence that women were more likely to make moral choices based on caring, rather than justice. From this point of view, it was men who were morally immature. Today, we recognize that both justice and caring perspectives may be essential to adult wisdom. (See Chapter 3, pages 104–106, for more details.) Similar biases exist concerning the race, ethnicity, age, and sexual orientation of researchers and participants in psychological research (Denmark, Rabinowitz, & Sechzer, 2005; Guthrie, 2004). Far too many conclusions are created by and/or based on small groups of people who do not represent the rich tapestry of humanity. However, the solution to such problems is straightforward: We need to encourage a wider array of people to become researchers and, when possible, researchers need to include a wider array of people in their studies. In recognition of human diversity, many researchers are doing just that (Reid, 2002). • Comparison of Psychological Research Methods Method Advantages Disadvantages Experimental Method Clear cause-and-effect relationships can be identified; powerful controlled observations can be staged; no need to wait for natural event May be somewhat artificial; some natural behavior not easily studied in laboratory (field experiments may avoid these objections) Naturalistic Observation Behavior is observed in a natural setting; much information is obtained, and hypotheses and questions for additional research are formed Little or no control is possible; observed behavior may be altered by the presence of the observer; observations may be biased; causes cannot be conclusively identified Correlational Method Demonstrates the existence of relationships; allows prediction; can be used in lab, clinic, or natural setting Little or no control is possible; relationships may be coincidental; cause-and-effect relationships cannot be confirmed Clinical Method Takes advantage of “natural clinical trials” and allows investigation of rare or unusual problems or events Little or no control is possible; does not provide a control group for comparison; subjective interpretation is often necessary; a single case may be misleading or unrepresentative Survey Method Allows information about large numbers of people to be gathered; can address questions not answered by other approaches Obtaining a representative sample is critical and can be difficult to do; answers may be inaccurate; people may not do what they say or say what they do A Look Ahead To help you get the most out of psychology, each chapter of this text includes a Psychology in Action section like the one that follows. There you will find ideas you can actually use, now or in the future. To complete the topics we have been discussing, let’s take a critical look at information reported in the popular press. You should find this an interesting way to conclude our opening tour of psychology and its methods. Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods Nonexperimental Research Methods RECITE 1. Two major problems in naturalistic observation are the effects of the observer and observer bias. T or F? 2. The ____________________________ fallacy involves attributing human feelings and motives to animals. 3. Correlation typically does not demonstrate causation. T or F? 4. Which correlation coefficient represents the strongest relationship? a. 0.86 b. 0.66 c. 0.10 d. 0.09 5. Case studies can often be thought of as natural tests and are frequently used by clinical psychologists. T or F? 6. For the survey method to be valid, a representative sample of people must be polled. T or F? 7. A problem with the survey method is that answers to questions may not always be _______________________________ or _______________________________. REFLECT Critical Thinking 8. A psychologist conducting a survey at a shopping mall (The Gallery of Wretched Excess) flips a coin before stopping passersby. If the coin shows heads, he interviews the person; if it shows tails, he skips that person. Has the psychologist obtained a random sample? 9. Is attributing mischievous motives to a car that is not working properly a thinking error similar to anthropomorphizing? Relate Google “critter cam” and find one you can watch. What species are you watching? What behaviors might you observe and record? See if you can identify at least one positive relationship and one negative relationship that involves human behavior. Have you ever known someone who suffered a brain injury or disease? How did his or her behavior change? Was the change clear-cut enough to serve as a natural clinical test? Have you ever been asked to complete a survey? Did you do it or did you refuse? If you refused, do you think your refusal influenced the final results of the survey? What would it say about accuracy if lots of people refused to complete the survey? If you completed the survey, were you honest about your answers? What would it say about accuracy if lots of people refused to answer accurately? Answers: 1. T 2. anthropomorphic 3. T 4. a 5. T 6. T 7. accurate, truthful 8. The psychologist’s coin flips might produce a reasonably good sample of people at the mall. The real problem is that people who go to the mall may be mostly from one part of town, from upper income groups, or from some other nonrepresentative group. The psychologist’s sample is likely to be seriously flawed. 9. Yes. It appears to be difficult for humans to resist thinking of other species and even machines in human terms. K N O WL E D GE B U I L D E R 43 P S YC H O L OGY IN A CT ION Psychology in the Media — Separating Fact from Fiction Gateway Question: How good is psychological information found in the popular media? Psychology is a popular topic in contemporary media. Unfortunately, much of what you will encounter is based on entertainment value rather than critical thinking or science. Here are some suggestions for separating highquality information from misleading fiction: Suggestion 1: Be skeptical. Have you ever played the game called “telephone” or “pass it down”? You know, one person whispers a sentence to someone else who, in turn, whispers it on down the line. Usually, when the person at the end of the line repeats the message it has been humorously distorted. Similarly, modern media, and especially the Internet, function as a giant “echo chamber” awash with rumors, hoaxes, half-truths, and urban legends like the one about giant alligators living in New York sewers. One of our all-time favorites was a story about the health department in Oregon seeking a Klingon interpreter for mental health patients who only speak in the fictional language used on the Star Trek TV series. This tale started when a newspaper reported that Klingon was on a list of languages that some psychiatric patients claimed they could speak. The article specifically noted that “in reality, no patient has yet tried to communicate in Klingon.” Nevertheless, as the story echoed around the web, the idea that Oregon was looking for someone fluent in Klingon had become a “fact” (O’Neill, 2003). Reports in the popular media tend to be made uncritically and with a definite bias toward reporting “astonishing” findings and telling interesting stories. Remember, saying “That’s incredible” means “That’s not believable” — which is often true. Suggestion 2: Consider the source of information. It should come as no surprise that information used to sell a product often reflects a desire for profit rather than the objective truth. Here is a typical advertising claim: “Government tests prove that no sleep medicine is stronger or more effective than Coma.” A statement like this usually means that there was no difference between Coma and the other products tested. No other sleep aid was stronger or more effective. But none was weaker either. Keep the source in mind when reading the claims of makers of home biofeedback machines, sleep-learning devices, subliminal CDs, and the like. Remember that psychological services may be merchandised as well. Be wary of expensive courses that promise instant mental health and happiness, increased efficiency, memory, extrasensory perception (ESP) or psychic ability, control of the unconscious mind, an end to smoking, and so on. Usually they are promoted with a few testimonials and many unsupported claims (Lilienfeld, 2005). Psychic claims should be viewed with special caution. Stage mentalists make a living by deceiving the public. Understandably, they are highly interested in promoting belief in their nonexistent powers. The same is true of the so-called psychic advisers promoted in TV commercials. These charlatans make use of the Barnum effect (the tendency to consider personal descriptions accurate if they are stated in general terms) to create an illusion Gender bias (in research) A tendency for females and female issues to be underrepresented in research, psychological or otherwise. CHAPTER 1 that they know private information about the people who call them (Nickell, 2001). Google magician James Randi’s Million Dollar Challenge. Randi is offering $1,000,000 to anyone demonstrating such abilities under controlled conditions. Are you surprised to learn that no one has even passed the preliminary tests yet? Suggestion 3: Beware of oversimplifications, especially those motivated by monetary gain. Courses or programs that offer a “new personality in three sessions,” “six steps to love and fulfillment in marriage,” or some newly discovered “secret for unlocking the powers of the mind and the universe” should be immediately suspect. An excellent example of oversimplification is provided by websites devoted to a video that promises to reveal “the secret to unlimited joy, health, money, relationships, love, youth: everything you have ever wanted.” According to these sites, all you need to do is put your desires out to the universe and the universe must respond by granting your wishes. And all it will cost you is the price of the video. (It’s no secret that the promoters are the real winners in this game.) Suggestion 4: Remember, “for example” is no proof. After reading this chapter you should be sensitive to the danger of selecting single examples. If you read, “Law student passes state bar exam using sleep-learning device,” don’t rush out to buy one. Systematic research showed long ago that these devices are of little or no value (Druckman & Bjork, 1994). A corollary to this suggestion is to ask if the reported observations are important or widely applicable. Examples, anecdotes, single cases, and testimonials are all potentially deceptive. According to numerous testimonials, believers in the power of the “secret” described earlier have been showered with money, success, and happiness immediately after viewing the video. Unfortunately, such individual cases (or even several) tell nothing about what is true in general (Stanovich, 2007). How many people didn’t win the lottery after buying the video? Similarly, studies of large groups of people show that smoking increases the likelihood of lung cancer. It doesn’t matter if you know a lifelong heavy smoker who is 95 years old. The general finding is the one to remember. Suggestion 5: Ask yourself if there was a control group. The key importance of a control group in any experiment is frequently overlooked by the unsophisticated — an error to which you are no longer susceptible. The popular media are full of reports of “experiments” performed without control groups: “Talking to Plants Speeds Growth”; “Special Diet Controls Hyperactivity in Children”; “Graduates of Firewalking Seminar Risk Their Soles.” Consider the last example for a moment. Expensive commercial courses have long been promoted to teach people to walk barefoot on hot coals. (Why anyone would want to do this is itself an interesting question.) Firewalkers supposedly protect their feet with a technique called “neurolinguistic programming.” Many people have paid good money to learn the technique, and most do manage a quick walk on the coals. But is the technique necessary? And is anything remarkable happening? We need a comparison group. Fortunately, physicist Bernard Leikind has provided one. Leikind showed with volunteers that anyone (with reasonably callused feet) can walk over a bed of coals without being burned. The reason is that the coals, which are light, fluffy carbon, transmit little heat when touched. The principle involved is similar to briefly putting your hand in a hot oven. If you touch a pan, you will be burned because metal transfers heat efficiently. But if your hand stays in the heated air you’ll be fine because air transmits little heat (Kida, 2006; Mitchell, 1987). Mystery solved. thing caused another just because they are correlated. In spite of this, you will see many claims based on questionable correlations. Here’s an example of mistaking correlation for causation: Jeanne Dixon, a well-known astrologer, once answered a group of prominent scientists — who had declared that there is no scientific foundation for astrology — by saying, “They would do well to check the records at their local police stations, where they will learn that the rate of violent crime rises and falls with lunar cycles.” Dixon, of course, believes that the moon affects human behavior. If it is true that violent crime is more frequent at certain times of the month, doesn’t that prove her point? Far from it. Increased crime could be due to darker nights, the fact that many people expect others to act crazier, or any number of similar factors. Besides, direct studies of the alleged “lunar effect” have shown that it doesn’t occur (Dowling, 2005). Moonstruck criminals, influenced by “a bad moon rising,” are a fiction (Iosif & Ballon, 2005). Suggestion 7: Be sure to distinguish between observation and inference. If you see a person crying, is it correct to assume that she or he is sad? Although it seems reasonable to make this assumption, it is actually quite risky. We can observe objectively that the person is crying, but to infer sadness may be in error. It could be that the individual has just peeled 5 pounds of onions. Or maybe he or she just won a million-dollar lottery or is trying contact lenses for the first time. Psychologists, politicians, physicians, scientists, and other experts often go far beyond the available facts in their claims. This does not mean that their inferences, opinions, and interpretations have no value; the opinion of an expert on the causes of mental illness, criminal behavior, learning problems, or whatever can be revealing. But be careful to distinguish between fact and opinion. Suggestion 6: Look for errors in distinguishing between correlation and causation. As you now know, it is dangerous to presume that one © John Nordell/The Image Works 44 Firewalking is based on simple physics, not on any form of supernatural psychological control. The temperature of the coals may be as high as 1,200º F. However, coals are like the air in a hot oven: They are very inefficient at transferring heat during brief contact. Summary We are all bombarded daily with such a mass of new information that it is difficult to absorb it. The available knowledge, even in a limited area like psychology, biology, medicine, or contemporary hip-hop music, is so vast that no single person can completely know and comprehend it. With this situation in mind, it becomes increasingly important that you become a critical, selective, and informed consumer of information. Introduction to Psychology and Research Methods Psychology in the Media RECITE 1. Popular media reports usually stress objective accuracy. T or F? 2. Stage mentalists and psychics often use deception in their “acts.” T or F? 3. Blaming the lunar cycle for variations in the rate of violent crime is an example of mistaking correlation for causation. T or F? 4. If a psychology student uses a sleep-learning device to pass a midterm exam, it proves that the device works. T or F? REFLECT Critical Thinking 5. Mystics have shown that fresh eggs can be balanced on their large ends during the vernal equinox when the sun is directly over the equator, day and night are equal in length, and the world is in perfect balance. What is wrong with their observation? chapter in review Psychology is the science of behavior and mental processes. Psychologists gather scientific data in order to describe, understand, predict, and control behavior. 6. Many parents believe that children become “hyperactive” when they eat too much sugar, and some early studies seemed to confirm this connection. However, we now know that eating sugar rarely has any effect on children. Why do you think that sugar appears to cause hyperactivity? Relate Do you tend to assume that a statement must be true if it is in print, on television, or made by an authority? How actively do you evaluate and question claims found in the media? Could you be a more critical consumer of information? Should you be a more critical consumer of information? Answers: 1. F 2. T 3. T 4. F 5. The mystics have neglected to ask if eggs can be balanced at other times. They can be balanced any time you like. The lack of a control group gives the illusion that something amazing is happening, but the equinox has nothing to do with egg balancing (Halpern, 2003). 6. This is another case of mistaking correlation for causation. Children who are hyperactive may eat more sugar (and other foods) to fuel their frenetic activity levels. K N O WL E D GE B U I L D E R 45 Gateways to Psychology publish results. Scientific debate and theories suggest new hypotheses, which lead to further research. • Psychologists are professionals who create and apply psychological knowledge. Psychologists engage in critical thinking as they systemically gather and analyze • Psychology spans a wide variety of specialty areas of research. • Some psychologists are directly interested in animal behavior. Others study animals as models of human behavior. • As a science, psychology’s goals are to describe, understand, predict, and control behavior. • Because the scientific method is a powerful way to observe the natural world and draw valid conclusions, scientific research provides the highest quality information about behavior. • In the scientific method, systematic observation is used to test hypotheses about behavior and mental events. • Concepts must be defined operationally before they can be studied empirically. • The results of scientific studies are made public so that others can evaluate them, learn from them, and use them to produce further knowledge. Critical thinking is the ability to reflect on, evaluate, compare, analyze, critique, and synthesize information. Critical thinking is central to the scientific method, to psychology, and to effective behavior in general. The field of psychology emerged 130 years ago when researchers began to directly study and observe psychological events. • To judge the validity of a claim, it is important to gather evidence for and against the claim and to evaluate the quality of the evidence. Pseudopsychologies are unfounded systems that resemble psychology. Unlike psychology, pseudopsychologies change little over time because followers seek evidence that appears to confirm their beliefs and avoid evidence that contradicts their beliefs. • Numerous pseudopsychologies are frequently confused with valid psychology. Belief in pseudopsychologies is based in part on uncritical acceptance, the fallacy of positive instances, and the Barnum effect. Psychological research begins by defining problems and proposing hypotheses. Next, researchers gather evidence, test hypotheses, and • The first psychological laboratory was established in Germany by Wilhelm Wundt, who studied conscious experience. • The first school of thought in psychology was structuralism, a kind of “mental chemistry” based on introspection. • Structuralism was followed by functionalism, behaviorism, and Gestalt psychology. • Psychodynamic approaches, such as Freud’s psychoanalytic theory, emphasize the unconscious origins of behavior. • Humanistic psychology accentuates subjective experience, human potentials, and personal growth. • Even though most of the early psychologists were men, women have contributed to psychology from the beginning. Three complementary streams of thought in modern psychology are the biological perspective, including biopsychology and evolutionary psychology; the psychological perspective, including behaviorism, 46 CHAPTER 1 cognitive psychology, the psychodynamic approach, and humanism; and the sociocultural perspective. • There is an eclectic blending of sviewpoints within psychology. • Psychologists have recently begun to formally study positive aspects of human behavior, or positive psychology. • Most of what we think, feel, and do is influenced by the social and cultural worlds in which we live. There are dozens of specialties in psychology including clinical, counseling, industrial, educational, consumer, school, developmental, engineering, medical, environmental, forensic, community, psychometric, and experimental psychology. • Although psychologists, psychiatrists, psychoanalysts, and counselors all work in the field of mental health, their training and methods differ considerably. • Psychological research may be basic or applied. Experiments involve two or more groups of subjects that differ only with regard to the independent variable. Effects on the dependent variable are then measured. All other conditions (extraneous variables) are held constant. • Since the independent variable is the only difference between the experimental group and the control group, it is the only possible cause of a change in the dependent variable. • The design of experiments allows cause-and-effect connections to be clearly identified. • Psychological research must be done ethically, in order to protect the rights, dignity, and welfare of participants. • To be taken seriously, the results of an experiment must be statistically significant (they would occur very rarely by chance alone). In a double-blind experiment, neither the research participants nor the researchers collecting data know who was in the experimental group or the control group, allowing valid conclusions to be drawn. • Research participant bias is a problem in some studies. The placebo effect is also a factor, especially in experiments involving drugs. • A related problem is researcher bias. Researcher expectations can create a self-fulfilling prophecy, in which a participant changes in the direction of the expectation. Psychologists also rely on naturalistic observation, the correlational method, case studies, and the survey method. • Unlike controlled experiments, nonexperimental methods usually cannot demonstrate cause-and-effect relationships. • Naturalistic observation is a starting place in many investigations. • Two problems with naturalistic observation are the effects of the observer on the observed and observer bias. • In the correlational method, relationships between two traits, responses, or events are measured and a correlation coefficient is computed to gauge the strength of the relationship. • Correlations allow prediction but do not show cause and effect. • Relationships in psychology may be positive or negative. • Case studies provide insights into human behavior that can’t be gained by other methods. • In the survey method, people in a representative sample are asked a series of carefully worded questions. • Obtaining a representative sample of people is crucial when the survey method is used to study large populations. Information in the mass media varies greatly in quality and accuracy and should be approached with skepticism and caution. • It is essential to critically evaluate information from popular sources (or from any source, for that matter) in order to separate facts from fallacies. • Problems in media reports are often related to biased or unreliable sources of information, uncontrolled observation, misleading correlations, false inferences, oversimplification, use of single examples, and unrepeatable results. Web Resources For an up-to-date list of direct links to interesting sites, including those listed here, visit the student companion site for this book at www.cengage.com/psychology/coon What Is Psychology? Discusses psychology as a science, with links to other articles about various branches of psychology. Critical Thinking in Everyday Life: 9 Strategies Some useful critical thinking strategies, along with many other articles on aspects of critical thinking. Today in the History of Psychology Events in the history of psychology by the date, including podcasts. Careers in Psychology Marky Lloyd’s Careers in Psychology Page. The Experimental Method An introduction to experimental research methods. Psychological Research on the Net Find and complete a survey study. That’s Infotainment! Article about sensationalism in the news media. Interactive Learning Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior Book Companion Website www.cengage.com/psychology/coon Visit your book companion website, where you will find flash cards, practice quizzes, web links, and more to help you study. Just what you need to know NOW! Spend time on what you need to master rather than on information you already have learned. Take a pre-test for this chapter, and CengageNOW will generate a personalized study plan based on your results. The study plan will identify the topics you need to review and direct you to online resources to help you master those topics. You can then take a post-test to help you determine the concepts you have mastered and what you will need to work on. Try it out! Go to www.cengage.com/login to sign in with an access code or to purchase access to this product. CHAPTER 2 Brain and Behavior Gateway Theme Brain activity is the source of human consciousness, intelligence, and behavior. Hill Street Studios/Photolibrary Gateway Questions • How do nerve cells operate and communicate? • What are the major parts of the nervous system? • How is the brain studied? • Why is the human cerebral cortex so important, and what • • • What are the major parts of the subcortex? Does the glandular system affect behavior? In what ways do right- and left-handed individuals differ? are its parts? 47 preview Finding Music in Walnut Grapefruit Tofu Your authors recently shared an evening together in Tucson, Arizona, watching some fiery Spanish flamenco. We both had brains on our minds. (Really!) The guitarist and dancer were passionate and brilliant. If they had been athletes, you would say they were “in the zone.” Of course, in everything from classical to jazz to hip-hop, musicians regularly make music that no machine could duplicate. A virtual Bruce Springsteen? A mechanical Beyoncé? We doubt it. That’s why music is such a good example of the central role the brain plays in all that is human. Your 3-pound brain is wrinkled like a walnut, the size of a grapefruit, and the texture of tofu. The next time you are in a market that sells beef brains, stop and have a look. What you will see is similar to your own brain, only smaller. How could Neurons — Building a “Biocomputer” Gateway Question: How do nerve cells operate and communicate? While they may seem far removed from your daily life, 100 billion tiny neurons (NOOR-ons: individual nerve cells) make up your brain. Neurons carry information from the senses to the brain, where they process it. They also activate muscles and glands. Yet, a single neuron is not very smart — it would take many just to make you blink. Millions of neurons must send messages at the same time to produce even the most fleeting thought. When a musician such as Eric Clapton plays a guitar riff, literally billions of neurons may be involved (Kalat, 2007). Your brainpower arises because individual neurons link to one another in tight clumps and long “chains.” Each neuron receives messages from many others and sends its own message on. Everything you think, feel, or do can be traced back to electrical impulses flashing through spidery networks of neurons. When neurons form vast networks, they produce intelligence and consciousness. Let’s see how neurons operate and how the nervous system is “wired.” Like miniature cables, axons carry messages through the brain and nervous system. Altogether, your brain contains about 3 million miles of axons (Rosenzweig, Breedlove, & Watson, 2004). Axons “branch out” into smaller fibers ending in bulb-shaped axon terminals. By forming connections with the dendrites and somas of other neurons, axon terminals allow information to pass from neuron to neuron. Now let’s summarize with a metaphor. Imagine that you are standing in a long line of people who are holding hands. A person on the far right end of the line wants to silently send a message to the person on the left end. She does this by pressing the hand of the person to her left, who presses the hand of the person to his left, and so on. The message arrives at your right hand (your dendrites). You decide whether to pass it on. (You are the soma.) The message goes out through your left arm (the axon). With your left hand (the axon terminals), you squeeze the hand of the person to your left, and the message moves on. The Nerve Impulse Electrically charged molecules called ions (EYE-ons) are found inside each neuron ( Figure 2.2). Other ions lie outside the cell. Some ions have a positive electrical charge, and some are negative. When a neuron is inactive, more of these “plus” charges exist outside the neuron and more “minus” charges exist inside. As a result, the inside of each neuron in your brain has an electrical charge of about minus 70 millivolts. (A millivolt is one thousandth of a volt.) This charge allows each neuron in your brain to act like a tiny biological battery. The electrical charge of an inactive neuron is called its resting potential. But neurons seldom get much rest: Messages arriving from other neurons raise and lower the resting potential. If the electrical charge rises to about minus 50 millivolts, the neuron • Parts of a Neuron What does a neuron look like? What are its main parts? No two neurons are exactly alike, but most have four basic parts ( Figure 2.1). The dendrites (DEN-drytes), which look like tree roots, receive messages from other neurons. The soma (SOH-mah: cell body) does the same. In addition, the soma sends messages of its own (nerve impulses) down a thin fiber called the axon (AK-sahn). Some axons are only .1 millimeter long. (That’s about the width of a pencil line.) Others stretch up to a meter through the nervous system. (From the base of your spine to your big toe, for instance.) • 48 such a squishy little blob of tissue allow us to make music of exquisite beauty? To seek a cure for cancer? To fall in love? Or to read a book like this one? Each of the billions of nerve cells in your brain is linked to thousands of others. The resulting network allows you to process immense amounts of information. In fact, there may be more possible pathways between the neurons in your brain than there are stars in the visible universe! Undeniably, the human brain is the most amazing of all computers. Scientists use the power of the brain to study the brain. Yet, even now we must wonder if the brain will ever completely understand itself. Nevertheless, answers to many age-old questions about the mind, consciousness, and knowledge lie buried within the brain. Let’s visit this fascinating realm. Brain and Behavior 49 2.1 A neuron, or nerve •cell.Figure In the right foreground you can Synapse (see Figure 2.6 for an enlarged view) Other neuron Axon terminals see a nerve cell fiber in cross section. The upper left photo gives a more realistic picture of the shape of neurons. Nerve impulses usually travel from the dendrites and soma to the branching ends of the axon. The nerve cell shown here is a motor neuron. The axons of motor neurons stretch from the brain and spinal cord to muscles or glands of the body. Myelin Nerve impulse Neurilemma Soma (cell body) Nerve impulse Axon collateral (branch) Axon Nerve cell fiber Myelin sheath Axon Dendrites • will reach its threshold, or trigger point for firing. (See Figure 2.2.) It’s as if the neuron says, “Ah ha! It’s time to send a message to my neighbors.” When a neuron reaches about 50 millivolts, an action potential, or nerve impulse, sweeps down the axon at up to 200 miles per hour ( Figure 2.3). That may seem fast, but it still takes at least a split second to react. That’s one reason why hitting a 95-mile-per-hour major league fastball is so difficult. What happens during an action potential? The axon membrane is pierced by tiny tunnels or “holes,” called ion channels. Normally, these tiny openings are blocked by molecules that act like “gates” or “doors.” During an action potential, the gates pop open. This allows sodium ions (Na) to rush into the axon (Carlson, 2007). The channels first open near the soma. Then gate after gate opens down the length of the axon as the action potential zips along ( Figure 2.4). Each action potential is an all-or-nothing event (a nerve impulse occurs completely or not at all). You might find it helpful to picture the axon as a row of dominoes set on end. Tipping over the dominoes is an all-or-nothing act. Once the first domino drops, a wave of falling blocks will zip rapidly to the end of the line. Similarly, when a nerve impulse is triggered near the soma, a wave of activity (the action potential) travels down the length of the axon. This is what happens in long chains of neurons as a flamenco dancer’s brain tells her feet what to do next, beat after beat. • • After each nerve impulse, the cell briefly dips below its resting level and it becomes less willing to fire. This negative after-potential occurs because potassium ions (K) flow out of the neuron while the membrane gates are open. (See Figure 2.4.) After a nerve impulse, ions flow both into and out of the axon, recharging it for more action. In our model, it takes an instant for the row of dominoes to be set up again. Soon, however, the axon is ready for another wave of activity. • Neuron An individual nerve cell. Dendrites Neuron fibers that receive incoming messages. Soma The main body of a neuron or other cell. Axon Fiber that carries information away from the cell body of a neuron. Axon terminals Bulb-shaped structures at the ends of axons that form synapses with the dendrites and somas other neurons. Resting potential The electrical charge of a neuron at rest. Threshold The point at which a nerve impulse is triggered. Action potential The nerve impulse. Ion channels Tiny openings through the axon membrane. Negative after-potential A drop in electrical charge below the resting potential. 50 CHAPTER 2 Action potential Membrane potential (in millivolts) +30 • 0 Resting potential Negative after-potential Threshold –50 –70 Time + – – + Figure 2.2 Electrical probes placed inside and outside an axon measure its activity. (The scale is exaggerated here. Such measurements require ultra-small electrodes, as described in this chapter.) The inside of an axon at rest is about –60 to –70 millivolts, compared with the outside. Electrochemical changes in a neuron generate an action potential. When sodium ions (Na) that have a positive charge rush into the cell, its interior briefly becomes positive. This is the action potential. After the action potential, positive potassium ions (K) flow out of the axon and restore its negative charge. (See Figure 2.3 for further explanation.) + – – + + – – + + – – + + + – – – – + + Axon • Figure 2.3 The inside of an axon normally has a negative electrical charge. The fluid surrounding an axon is normally positive. As an action potential passes along the axon, these charges reverse so that the interior of the axon briefly becomes positive. This process is described in more detail in Figure 2.4. Axon 1. In its resting state, the axon has a negatively charged interior. – + 2. During an action potential, positively charged atoms (ions) rush into the axon. This briefly changes the electrical charge inside the axon from negative to positive. Simultaneously, the charge outside the axon becomes negative. – – + – + + + + + + – – – + + + + – – – – + + + + + + + + – – – – + + + + – + – + – + – – – – – + – + – + – + – – + – + + – – – – – + Action potential – + • – Action potential – The axons of some neurons (such as the one pictured in Figure 2.1) are coated with a fatty layer called myelin (MY-eh-lin). Small gaps in the myelin help nerve impulses move faster. Instead of passing down the entire length of the axon, the action potential leaps from gap to gap, a process called saltatory conduction. Without the added speed this allows, it would probably be – + 3. The action potential advances as positive and negative charges reverse in a moving zone of electrical activity that sweeps down the axon. 4. After an action potential passes, positive ions rapidly flow out of the axon to quickly restore its negative charge. An outward flow of additional positive ions returns the axon to its resting state. – Action potential – Saltatory Conduction + – – + – + – – + – + – + – + – + + + + + – – – – – + impossible to brake in time to avoid many automobile accidents (or hit that major league fastball). When the myelin layer is damaged, a person may suffer from numbness, weakness, or paralysis. That, in fact, is what happens in multiple sclerosis, a disease that occurs when the immune system attacks and destroys the myelin in a person’s body. Brain and Behavior 51 Action potential – – – – Ion channels + + + + + – – – – Na+ – Na+ Na+ Presynaptic axon terminal Resting potential Na+ – Axon + + + + + Synaptic gap + + + + + Synaptic vesicle Neurotransmitter Na+ Action potential + + K+ + + K+– K+ + + + + K+ + + – – – – – + + + + + – – – – – Axon repolarizes 2.4 The interior of an axon. The right end of the top axon is at rest. •Thus,Figure it has a negative charge inside. An action potential begins when ion channels open and sodium ions (Na) rush into the axon. In this drawing, the action potential would travel from left to right along the axon. In the lower axon, the action potential has moved to the right. After it passes, potassium ions (K) flow out of the axon. This quickly renews the negative charge inside the axon so that it can fire again. Sodium ions that enter the axon during an action potential are pumped out more slowly. Removing them restores the original resting potential. Synapses and Neurotransmitters How does information move from one neuron to another? The nerve impulse is primarily electrical. That’s why electrically stimulating the brain affects behavior. To prove the point, researcher José Delgado once entered a bullring with a cape and a radio transmitter. The bull charged. Delgado retreated. At the last instant the speeding bull stopped short. Why? Because Delgado had placed radio-activated electrodes (metal wires) deep within the bull’s brain. These, in turn, stimulated “control centers” that brought the bull to a halt (Horgan, 2005). In contrast to the nerve impulse, communication between neurons is chemical. The microscopic space between two neurons, over which messages pass, is called a synapse (SIN-aps) ( Figure 2.5). When an action potential reaches the tips of the axon terminals, neurotransmitters (NOOR-oh-TRANS-miters) are released into the synaptic gap. Neurotransmitters are chemicals that alter activity in neurons. Let’s return to our metaphor of people standing in a line. To be more accurate, you and the others shouldn’t be holding hands. Instead, each person should have a squirt gun in his or her left hand. To pass along a message, you would squirt the right hand of the person to your left. When that person notices this “message,” he or she would squirt the right hand of the person to the left, and so on. When chemical molecules cross over a synapse, they attach to special receiving areas on the next neuron. (See Figure 2.5.) These tiny receptor sites on the cell membrane are sensitive to neu- • • Receptor site Postsynaptic dendrite Figure 2.5 A highly magnified view of a synapse. Neurotransmitters are stored •in tiny sacs called synaptic vesicles (VES-ih-kels). When a nerve impulse reaches the end of an axon, the vesicles move to the surface and release neurotransmitters. These molecules cross the synaptic gap to affect the next neuron. The size of the gap is exaggerated here; it is actually only about one millionth of an inch. Some transmitter molecules excite the next neuron and some inhibit its activity. rotransmitters. The sites are found in large numbers on nerve cell bodies and dendrites. Muscles and glands have receptor sites, too. Do neurotransmitters always trigger an action potential in the next neuron? No, but they do change the likelihood of an action potential in the next neuron. Some transmitters excite the next neuron (move it closer to firing). Others inhibit it (make firing less likely). More than 100 transmitter chemicals are found in the brain. Some examples are acetylcholine, epinephrine, norepinephrine, serotonin, dopamine, histamine, and various amino acids. Disturbances of any of these substances can have serious consequences. For example, too little dopamine can cause the shaking and muscle tremors of Parkinson’s disease. Too much dopamine may cause the severe mental disorder known as schizophrenia (Di Forti, Lappin, & Murray, 2007). Many drugs imitate, duplicate, or block neurotransmitters. For example, acetylcholine (ah-SEET-ul-KOH-leen) normally activates muscles. Without acetylcholine, our flamenco musicians couldn’t even move, much less perform. That’s exactly how the Myelin A fatty layer coating some axons. Saltatory conduction The process by which nerve impulses conducted down the axons of neurons coated with myelin jump from gap to gap in the myelin layer. Synapse The microscopic space between two neurons, over which messages pass. Neurotransmitter Any chemical released by a neuron that alters activity in other neurons. Receptor sites Areas on the surface of neurons and other cells that are sensitive to neurotransmitters or hormones. Acetylcholine The neurotransmitter released by neurons to activate muscles. 52 CHAPTER 2 drug curare (cue-RAH-ree) causes paralysis. By attaching to receptor sites on muscles, curare competes with acetylcholine. This prevents acetylcholine from activating muscle cells. As a result, a person or animal given curare cannot move — a fact known to South American Indians of the Amazon River Basin, who use curare as an arrow poison for hunting. + A + Neural Regulators More subtle brain activities are affected by chemicals called neuropeptides (NOOR-oh-PEP-tides). Neuropeptides do not carry messages directly. Instead, they regulate the activity of other neurons. By doing so, they affect memory, pain, emotion, pleasure, moods, hunger, sexual behavior, and other basic processes. For example, when you touch something hot, you jerk your hand away. The messages for this action are carried by neurotransmitters. At the same time, pain may cause the brain to release enkephalins (enKEF-ah-lins). These opiate-like neural regulators relieve pain and stress. Related chemicals called endorphins (en-DORF-ins) are released by the pituitary gland. Together, these chemicals reduce the pain so that it is not too disabling (Drolet et al., 2001). Such discoveries help explain the painkilling effect of placebos (fake pills or injections), which raise endorphin levels (StewartWilliams, 2004). A release of endorphins also seems to underlie “runner’s high,” masochism, acupuncture, and the euphoria sometimes associated with childbirth, painful initiation rites, and even sport parachuting ( Janssen & Arntz, 2001). In each case, pain and stress cause the release of endorphins. These in turn induce feelings of pleasure or euphoria similar to being “high” on morphine. People who say they are “addicted” to running may be closer to the truth than they realize. bridges To learn more about how pain can sometimes produce feelings of relaxation or euphoria, see Chapter 4, pages 143–144. Ultimately, neural regulators may help explain depression, schizophrenia, drug addiction, and other puzzling topics. For example, women who suffer from severe premenstrual pain and distress have unusually low endorphin levels (Straneva et al., 2002). Neural Networks Let’s put together what we now know about the nerve impulse and synaptic transmission to see how neural networks process information in our brains. Figure 2.6 shows a small part of a neural network. Five neurons synapse with a single neuron that, in turn, connects with three more neurons. At the point in time depicted in the diagram, the single neuron is receiving one stronger and two weaker excitatory messages () as well as two inhibitory ones (). Does it fire an impulse? It depends: If enough “exciting” messages • – + + – Figure 2.6 A small neural network. Neuron A receives inputs from two •weaker and one stronger excitatory connections () and two inhibitory connections () and combines the inputs into a “decision” to launch an action potential, which may help trigger further synaptic transmissions in other neurons. arrive close in time, the neuron will fire — but only if it doesn’t get too many “inhibiting” messages that push it away from its trigger point. In this way, messages are combined before a neuron “decides” to fire its all-or-nothing action potential. Let’s try another metaphor. You are out shopping for new jeans with five friends. Three of them think you should buy the jeans (your best friend is especially positive) and two think you shouldn’t. Because, on balance, their input is positive, you go ahead and buy the jeans. Maybe you even tell some other friends they should buy those jeans as well. Similarly, any single neuron in a neural network “listens” to the neurons that synapse with it and combines that input into an output. At any instant, a single neuron may weigh hundreds or thousands of inputs to produce an outgoing message. After the neuron recovers from the resulting action potential, it again combines the inputs, which may have changed in the meantime, into another output, and another, and another. In this way, each neuron in your brain functions as a tiny computer. Compared with the average laptop computer, a neuron is terribly simple and slow. But multiply these events by 100 billion neurons and 100 trillion synapses, all operating at the same time, and you have an amazing computer — one that could easily fit inside a shoebox. Neuroplasticity The neural networks in your brain constantly change. The term neuroplasticity refers to the capacity of our brains to change in response to experience. New synapses may form between neurons or synaptic connections may grow stronger. ( Figure 2.6 shows one particularly strong synapse — the large .) Other synaptic connections may weaken and might even die. Every new experience you have is reflected in changes in your brain. For example, rats raised in a complex environment have more synapses and longer dendrites in their brains than rats raised in a simpler environment (Kolb, Gibb, & Gorny, 2003). (See “You Can Change Your Mind, but Can You Change Your Brain?”) • Brain and Behavior 53 CRIT ICA L T H I N KI N G You Can Change Your Mind, but Can You Change Your Brain? You can always change your mind. But does that have anything to do with your brain? Philosophers have debated the relationship of the mind to the brain (and the rest of the body) for centuries. Biopsychologists argue that every mental event involves a brain event. In one study, people suffering from spider phobias were treated with cognitive behavior therapy. (For more information, see Chapter 15, pages 505–507.) After therapy, they could actually touch spiders (although they might not have been The Nervous System — Wired for Action ready to appear on Fear Factor). Images of their brains revealed reduced activity in the brain areas involved in the phobia (Paquette et al., 2003). Not only did they change their minds about spiders, they literally changed their brains. Another study focused on patients with language difficulties caused by damage to the left sides of their brains. To aid their recovery, the patients were given training in language comprehension. Not only did the training help improve their ability to under- stand language, but brain images revealed that the right sides of their brains had become more active to compensate for their left-brain damage (Musso et al., 1999). Again, a learning experience changed their brains. Every time you learn something, you are reshaping your living brain. There is even a fancy phrase to describe what you are doing: self-directed neuroplasticity. Just think. As you study this psychology textbook you are changing your mind, and your brain, about psychology. Central nervous system Peripheral nervous system Gateway Question: What are the major parts of the nervous system? Spinal Somatic Autonomic Brain cord system system Harry and Maya are playing catch with a football. This may look fairly simple. However, to merely toss the foot(a) ball or catch it, a huge amount of information must be sensed, interSympathetic Parasympathetic preted, and directed to countless muscle fibers. As they play, Harry system system and Maya’s neural circuits are ablaze with activity. Let’s explore the (b) “wiring diagram” that makes their game of catch possible. As you can see in Figure 2.7, the central nervous system Figure 2.7 Subparts of the nervous (CNS) consists of the brain and spinal cord. The brain carries out system. (a) Central nervous system. (b) Peripheral nervous system. most of the “computing” in the nervous system. Harry must use his brain to anticipate when and where the football will arrive. Harry’s brain communicates with the rest of his body through general, it controls voluntary behavior, such as when Maya tosses a large “cable” called the spinal cord. From there, messages flow the football or Tiger Woods hits a golf ball. In contrast, the autothrough the peripheral nervous system (PNS). This intricate nomic nervous system (ANS) serves the internal organs and network of nerves carries information to and from the CNS. glands. The word autonomic means “self-governing.” Activities Are nerves the same as neurons? No. Neurons are tiny cells. You would need a microscope to see one. Nerves are large bundles of Neuropeptides Brain chemicals that regulate the activity of neurons, neuron axons. You can easily see nerves without magnification. such as enkephalins and endorphins. Nerves in the peripheral nervous system can regrow if they are Neuroplasticity The capacity of our brains to change in response to damaged. The axons of most neurons in nerves outside the brain experience. and spinal cord are covered by a thin layer of cells called the neuriCentral nervous system (CNS) The brain and spinal cord. lemma (NOOR-rih-LEM-ah). (Return to Figure 2.1.) The neurilemma forms a “tunnel” that damaged fibers can follow as they Peripheral nervous system (PNS) All parts of the nervous system outside the brain and spinal cord. repair themselves. Because of this, patients can expect to regain some control over severed limbs once they have been reattached. Nerve A bundle of neuron fibers. • • • Neurilemma A layer of cells that encases many axons. The Peripheral Nervous System The peripheral system can be divided into two major parts (See Figure 2.7). The somatic nervous system (SNS) carries messages to and from the sense organs and skeletal muscles. In • Somatic nervous system (SNS) The system of nerves linking the spinal cord with the body and sense organs. Autonomic nervous system (ANS) The system of nerves carrying information to and from the internal organs and glands. 54 CHAPTER 2 governed by the autonomic nervous system are mostly “vegetative” or automatic, such as heart rate, digestion, and perspiration. Thus, messages carried by the somatic system can make your hand move, but they cannot cause your eyes to dilate. Likewise, messages carried by the ANS can stimulate digestion, but they cannot help you carry out a voluntary action, such as writing a letter. If Harry feels a flash of anger when he misses a catch, a brief burst of activity will spread through his autonomic system. for “fight or flight” during times of danger or high emotion. In essence, it arouses the body for action. The parasympathetic branch quiets the body and returns it to a lower level of arousal. It is most active soon after an emotional event. The parasympathetic branch also helps keep vital processes such as heart rate, breathing, and digestion at moderate levels. Of course, both branches of the ANS are always active. At any given moment, their combined activity determines if your body is more or less relaxed or aroused. bridges The Spinal Cord The ANS plays a central role in our emotional lives. In fact, without the ANS a person would feel little emotion. See Chapter 10, pages 343–345 for more information about the ANS and emotion. As mentioned earlier, the spinal cord connects the brain to other parts of the body. If you were to cut through this “cable,” you would see columns of white matter (bundles of axons covered with myelin). This tissue is made up of axons that eventually leave the spinal cord to form the peripheral nervous system nerves. Thirtyone spinal nerves carry sensory and motor messages to and from the spinal cord. In addition, 12 pairs of cranial nerves leave the brain directly. Together, these nerves keep your entire body in communication with your brain. Is the spinal cord’s only function to connect the brain to the peripheral nervous system? Actually, the spinal cord can do some simple “computing” of its own. Reflex arcs, which occur when a stimulus provokes an automatic response, arise within the spinal cord, without any help from the brain ( Figure 2.9). Imagine that Maya steps on a thorn. (Yes, they’re still playing catch.) Pain is detected in her foot by a sensory neuron (a nerve cell that carries messages from the senses toward the CNS). Instantly, the sensory neuron fires off a message to Maya’s spinal cord. The ANS and SNS work together to coordinate the body’s internal reactions to events in the world outside. For example, if a snarling dog lunges at you, the somatic system will control your leg muscles so that you can run. At the same time, the autonomic system will raise your blood pressure, quicken your heart, and so forth. The ANS can be divided into the sympathetic and parasympathetic branches. Both are related to emotional responses, such as crying, sweating, heart rate, and other involuntary behavior ( Figure 2.8). How do the branches of the autonomic system differ? The sympathetic branch is an “emergency” system. It prepares the body • Parasympathetic • Sympathetic Cell body of sensory neuron Sensory nerve Constricts pupil Stimulates tears Stimulates salivation Inhibits heart rate Constricts respiration Constricts blood vessels Stimulates digestion Contracts bladder Stimulates elimination Stimulates genitals Dilates pupil Inhibits tears Inhibits salivation Activates sweat glands Increases heart rate Increases respiration Inhibits digestion Release of adrenaline Release of sugar from liver Relaxes bladder Inhibits elimination Inhibits genitals Ejaculation in males Figure 2.8 Sympathetic and parasympathetic branches of the autonomic •nervous system. Both branches control involuntary actions. The sympathetic system generally activates the body. The parasympathetic system generally quiets it. The sympathetic branch relays its messages through clusters of nerve cells outside the spinal cord. Sensory neuron Connector neuron Spinal cord (cross section) Motor neuron Muscle cell responds by contracting 2.9 A sensory-motor •arc,Figure or reflex, is set in motion by a stimulus to the skin (or other part of the body). The nerve impulse travels to the spinal cord and then back out to a muscle, which contracts. Such reflexes provide an “automatic” protective device for the body. Sensory receptor in skin Stimulus to skin Brain and Behavior Inside the spinal cord, the sensory neuron synapses with a connector neuron (a nerve cell that links two others). The connector neuron activates a motor neuron (a cell that carries commands from the CNS to muscles and glands). The muscle fibers are made up of effector cells (cells capable of producing a response). The muscle cells contract and cause Maya’s foot to withdraw. Note that no brain activity is required for a reflex arc to occur. Maya’s body will react automatically to protect itself. In reality, even a simple reflex usually triggers more complex activity. For example, muscles of Maya’s other leg must contract to support her as she shifts her weight. Even this can be done by the spinal cord, but it involves many more cells and several spinal nerves. Also, the spinal cord normally informs the brain of its actions. As her foot pulls away from the thorn, Maya will feel the pain and think, “Ouch, what was that?” Perhaps you have realized how adaptive it is to have a spinal cord capable of responding on its own. Such automatic responses leave the brains of our football stars free to deal with more important information — such as the location of trees, lampposts, and attractive onlookers — as they take turns making grandstand catches. 55 While peripheral nerves can regrow, a serious injury to the brain or spinal cord is usually permanent. However, scientists are starting to make progress repairing damaged neurons in the CNS. For instance, they have partially repaired cut spinal cords in rats. First they close the gap with nerve fibers from outside the spinal cord. Then they biochemically coax the severed spinal nerve fibers to grow through the “tunnels” (neurilemma) provided by the implanted fibers. Within months, rats treated this way regain some use of their hind legs (Cheng, Cao, & Olson, 1996). Similarly, medical researchers have already begun the first human trials in which nerve grafts will be used to repair damaged spinal cords (Féron et al., 2005). Imagine what that could mean to a person confined to a wheelchair. Although it is unwise to raise false hopes, solutions to such problems are beginning to emerge. Nevertheless, it is wise to take good care of your own CNS. That means using seat belts when you drive, wearing a helmet if you ride a motorcycle or bicycle, wearing protective gear for sports, and avoiding activities that pose a risk to your head or spinal cord. Can brain damage also be repaired? While we will be exploring the brain itself in more detail later on in the chapter, we can, for now, answer with an optimistic but cautious yes. (See “Repairing Your Brain.”) Before we go on to explore some of the research tools biopsychologists use, take some time to check out how much you’ve learned. KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R Neurons and the Nervous System RECITE Blend Images/Jupiter images 1. The _______________ and ____________ are the receiving areas of a neuron where information from other neurons is accepted. 2. Nerve impulses are carried down the __________________ to the ______________ _______________________. 3. The ______________ potential becomes a(n) ______________ potential when a neuron passes the threshold for firing. 4. Neuropeptides are transmitter substances that help regulate the activity of neurons. T or F? 5. The somatic and autonomic systems are part of the ______________ nervous system. 6. Sodium and potassium ions flow across the synapse to trigger a nerve impulse in the receiving neuron. T or F? 7. The simplest behavior sequence is a _____________ ____________. 8. The parasympathetic nervous system is most active during times of high emotion. T or F? Continued Each year spinal cord injuries rob many thousands of people of the ability to move. Yet there is growing hope that nerve-grafting techniques may someday make it possible for some of these people to walk again. Sympathetic branch A branch of the ANS that arouses the body. Parasympathetic branch A branch of the ANS that quiets the body. Reflex arc The simplest behavior, in which a stimulus provokes an automatic response. Sensory neuron A nerve cell that carries information from the senses toward the CNS. 56 CHAPTER 2 C R IT ICAL T H I N KI N G Repairing Your Brain Until only a few years ago, it was widely believed that we are born with all the brain cells we will ever have. This led to the depressing idea that we all slowly go downhill, as the brain loses thousands of neurons every day. It also led to limited options, such as nerve cell transplants, for the treatment of brain damage (Wong, Hodges, & Horsburgh, 2005). Imagine that Bobby M. suffered partial paralysis in his left arm due to a stroke. (A stroke occurs when an artery in the brain becomes blocked or bursts open. This interrupts blood flow and causes some brain tissue to die.) What could be done to help Bobby recover? One remedy involves injecting immature nerve cells into his damaged brain areas. This would allow the new cells to link up with existing neurons in order to repair Bobby’s stroke damage (Borlongan, Sanberg, & Freeman, 1999; Zhang, Zhang, & Chopp, 2005). Rather than facing a steady decline, we now know that a healthy 75-year-old brain has just as many neurons as it did when it was careening through life in the body of a 25-year-old. Although it is true that the brain loses cells daily, it simultaneously grows new neurons to replace them. This process is called neurogenesis (noor-oh-JEN-uh-sis; the production of new brain cells) (Kempermann, 2005). Each day, thousands of new cells originate deep within the brain, move to the surface, and link up with other neurons to become part of the brain’s circuitry. This is stunning news to brain scientists, who must now figure out what the new cells do. Most likely they are involved in learning, memory, and our ability to adapt to changing circumstances (Gould & Gross, 2002). The discovery of neurogenesis in adult brains has raised hope that new treatments can be found for some types of brain damage. For example, an approach called constraint-induced movement therapy could be used to speed Bobby M.’s recovery. In this case, Bobby’s good right arm would be restrained, forcing his impaired left arm to be more active, which would increase neurogenesis in the damaged part of his brain (Taub, 2004). In another approach, drugs that speed up neurogenesis could be injected into REFLECT Critical Thinking 9. What effect would you expect a drug to have if it blocked passage of neurotransmitters across the synapse? Relate To cope with all the technical terms in this section, think of neurons as strange little creatures. How do they act? What excites them? How do they communicate? To remember the functions of major branches of the nervous system, think about what you couldn’t do if each part were missing. the damaged area of Bobby’s brain (Zhang, Zhang, & Chopp, 2005). Such techniques are beginning to offer new hope for people suffering from a variety of other disabilities, such as blindness and Parkinson’s disease (Brinton & Wang, 2006; Burke et al., 2007). But don’t these treatments assume that Bobby’s brain is still capable of neurogenesis? What if it isn’t? Brilliant! Although a stroke most likely doesn’t damage the brain’s ability to repair itself, it is quite possible that other brain disorders do arise from impaired neurogenesis (Thompson et al., 2008). In fact, that is exactly the theory proposed by neuroscientists Carla Toro and Bill Deakin to explain the serious mental disorder schizophrenia (Toro & Deakin, 2007). The brains of schizophrenic persons are usually smaller than normal, indicating that they have fewer neurons. Toro and Deakin’s idea is that the schizophrenic brain may be unable to continually create new neurons to replace old ones that have died. If they are right, new therapies to promote neurogenesis may hold the key to treating schizophrenia, one of the most devastating mental illnesses. brain to the control of particular cognitive or behavioral functions, such as being able to recognize faces or move your hands. That is, they try to learn where functions are localized (located) in the brain. Many techniques have been developed to help identify brain structures and the functions they control. Mapping Brain Structure Anatomists have learned much about brain structure by dissecting (cutting apart) autopsied human and animal brains and examining them under a microscope. Dissection reveals that the brain is made up of many anatomically distinct areas or “parts.” Less intrusive newer methods, such as the CT scan and the MRI scan, can be used to map brain structures in living brains. Answers: 1. dendrites, soma 2. axon, axon terminals 3. resting, action 4. T 5. peripheral 6. F 7. reflex arc 8. F 9. Such a drug could have wideranging effects. If the drug blocked excitatory synapses, it would depress brain activity. If it blocked inhibitory messages, it would act as a powerful stimulant. CT Scan Research Methods — Charting the Brain’s Inner Realms Gateway Question: How is the brain studied? Biopsychology is the study of how biological processes, and especially those of the nervous system, relate to behavior. In their research, many biopsychologists try to relate specific parts of the Computerized scanning equipment has revolutionized the study of brain structures and made it easier to identify brain diseases and injuries. At best, conventional X-rays produce only shadowy images of the brain. Computed tomographic (CT) scanning is a specialized type of X-ray that does a much better job of making the brain visible. In a CT scan, X-rays taken from a number of different angles are collected by a computer and formed into an image of the brain. A CT scan can reveal the location of strokes, injuries, tumors, and other brain disorders. 57 Brain and Behavior recovery that has been miraculous by any measure, she even went on to appear before the U. S. Congress. Instead of relying on clinical studies, researchers have learned much from electrical stimulation of the brain (ESB) ( Figure 2.11). For example, the surface of the brain can be “turned on” by stimulating it with a mild electrical current delivered through a thin insulated wire called an electrode. When this is done during brain surgery, the patient can describe what effect the stimulation had. (The brain has no pain receptors, so surgery can be done while a patient is awake. Only local painkillers are used for the scalp and skull.) (Any volunteers?) Even structures below the surface of the brain can be activated by lowering a stimulating electrode, insulated except at the tip, into a target area inside the brain. ESB can call forth behavior with astonishing power. Instantly, it can bring about aggression, alertness, escape, eating, drinking, sleeping, movement, euphoria, memories, speech, tears, and more. Could ESB be used to control a person against his or her will? It might seem that ESB could be used to control a person like a robot. But the details of emotions and behaviors elicited by ESB are modified by personality and circumstances. Sci-fi movies to the contrary, it would be impossible for a ruthless dictator to enslave people by “radio controlling” their brains. An alternate approach is ablation (ab-LAY-shun: surgical removal) of parts of the brain. (See Figure 2.11.) When ablation CNRI/Photo Researchers, Inc. • Figure 2.10 A colored MRI scan of the brain reveals many details. Can you •identify any brain regions? MRI Scan Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) uses a very strong magnetic field, rather than X-rays, to produce an image of the body’s interior. During an MRI scan, the body is placed inside a magnetic field. Processing by a computer then creates a three-dimensional model of the brain or body. Any two-dimensional plane, or slice, of the body can be selected and displayed as an image on a computer screen. MRI scans produce more detailed images than are possible with CT scans, allowing us to peer into the living brain almost as if it were transparent ( Figure 2.10). • Stimulation electrode • Deep-lesioning electrode Surgical ablation Exploring Brain Function How does the brain allow us to think, feel, perceive, or act? To answer questions like these, we must localize function by linking these psychological or behavioral capacities with particular brain structures. In many instances, this has been done through clinical case studies. Such studies examine changes in personality, behavior, or sensory capacity caused by brain diseases or injuries. If damage to a particular part of the brain consistently leads to a particular loss of function, then we say the function is localized in that structure. Presumably, that part of the brain controls the same function in all of us. Consider, for example, the story of Kate Adamson (Adamson, 2004). At the age of 33, she had a stroke that caused catastrophic damage to her brainstem. This event left her with locked-in syndrome: One moment she was fine, and the next she was totally paralyzed, trapped in her own body and barely able to breathe. As you can see, this clinical case suggests that our brainstems play a role in the control of vital life functions, such as movement and breathing. But what happened to Kate? Oh, yes. Unable to move a muscle, but still fully awake and aware, Kate thought she was going to die. Her doctors, who thought she was brain dead (Laureys & Boly, 2007), did not administer painkillers as they inserted breathing and feeding tubes down her throat. However, in time Kate discovered that she could communicate by blinking her eyes. After a 2.11 The functions of •brainFigure structures are explored by selectively activating or removing them. Brain research is often based on electrical stimulation, but chemical stimulation is also used at times. Neurogenesis The production of new brain cells. Computed tomographic (CT) scanning A computer-enhanced X-ray image of the brain or body. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) A three-dimensional image of the brain or body, based on its response to a magnetic field. Localization of function The research strategy of linking specific structures in the brain with specific psychological or behavioral functions. Clinical case study A detailed investigation of a single person, especially one suffering from some injury or disease. Electrical stimulation of the brain (ESB) Direct electrical stimulation and activation of brain tissue. Electrode Any device (such as a wire, needle, or metal plate) used to electrically stimulate or destroy nerve tissue or to record its activity. Ablation Surgical removal of tissue. CHAPTER 2 causes changes in behavior or sensory capacity, we also gain insight into the purpose of the missing “part.” By using deep lesioning (LEE-zhun-ing), structures below the surface of the brain can also be removed. A strong electric current can be used to destroy a small amount of brain tissue when delivered via an electrode lowered into a target area inside the brain. (See Figure 2.11.) Again, changes in behavior give clues to the function of the affected area. By using ESB, ablation, and deep lesioning, researchers are creating a three-dimensional brain map. This “atlas” shows the sensory, motor, and emotional responses that can be elicited from various parts of the brain. It promises to be a valuable guide for medical treatment, as well as for exploring the brain (Kalat, 2007; Yoshida, 1993). To find out what individual neurons are doing, we need to do a microelectrode recording. A microelectrode is an extremely thin glass tube filled with a salty fluid. The tip of a microelectrode is small enough to detect the electrical activity of a single neuron. Watching the action potentials of just one neuron provides a fascinating glimpse into the true origins of behavior. (The action potential shown in Figure 2.2 was recorded with a microelectrode.) Are any less invasive techniques available for studying brain function? Yes, several techniques allow us to observe the activity of parts of the brain without doing any damage at all. These include the EEG, PET scan, and fMRI. Such techniques allow biopsychologists to pinpoint areas in the brain responsible for thoughts, feelings, and actions. example, Chapter 6, page 186, explains how changes in brain waves help define various stages of sleep.) PET Scan A newer technology, called positron emission tomography (PET), provides much more detailed images of activity both near the surface and below the surface of the brain. A PET scan detects positrons (subatomic particles) emitted by weakly radioactive glucose (sugar) as it is consumed by the brain. Because the brain runs on glucose, a PET scan shows which areas are using more energy. Higher energy use corresponds with higher activity. Thus, by placing positron detectors around the head and sending data to a computer, it is possible to create a moving, color picture of changes in brain activity. As you can see in Figure 2.13, PET scans reveal that very specific brain areas are active when you see, hear, speak, or think. • • bridges PET scans suggest that different patterns of brain activity accompany major psychological disorders, such as depression or schizophrenia. See Chapter 14, pages 473–475. • More active brains are good, right? Surprisingly, although we might assume that smart brains are hardworking brains, the reverse appears to be true. Using PET scans, psychologist Richard Haier and his colleagues found that the brains of people who perform well on a difficult reasoning test consume less energy than those of poor performers (Haier et al., 1988) ( Figure 2.14). Haier believes this shows that intelligence is related to brain efficiency: Less efficient brains work harder and still accomplish less (Haier, White, & Alkire, 2003). We’ve all had days like that! Is it true that most people use only 10 percent of their brain capacity? This is one of the lasting myths about the brain. Brain scans show that all parts of the brain are active during waking hours. Obviously, some people make better use of their innate brainpower than others do. Nevertheless, there are no great hidden or untapped reserves of mental capacity in a normally functioning brain. EEG • Electroencephalography (ee-LEK-tro-in-SEF-ah-LOG-ruh-fee) measures the waves of electrical activity produced near the surface of the brain. Small disk-shaped metal plates are placed on a person’s scalp. Electrical impulses from the brain are detected by these electrodes and sent to an electroencephalograph (EEG). The EEG amplifies these very weak signals (brain waves) and records them on a moving sheet of paper or a computer screen ( Figure 2.12). Various brain-wave patterns can identify the presence of tumors, epilepsy, and other diseases. The EEG also reveals changes in brain activity during sleep, daydreaming, hypnosis, and other mental states. (For AJPhoto/Photo Researchers, Inc. • • Figure 2.12 An EEG recording. Seeing Hearing Speaking Thinking WDCN/Univ. College London/Photo Researchers, Inc. 58 Figure 2.13 Colored PET scans reveal different patterns of brain activation •when we engage in different tasks. 59 Brain and Behavior A functional MRI (f MRI) uses MRI technology to make brain activity visible. Like PET scans, functional MRIs also provide images of activity throughout the brain. For example, if we scanned Carlos Santana while he played his guitar, areas of his brain that control his hands would be highlighted in an fMRI image. Psychiatrist Daniel Langleben and his colleagues have even used fMRI images to tell if a person is lying (Langleben et al., 2005). As Figure 2.15 shows, the front of the brain is more active when a person is lying, rather than telling the truth. This Figure 2.14 In the images you see here, red, orange, and yellow indicate high consumption may occur because it takes extra effort to lie and of glucose; green, blue, and pink show areas of low glucose use. The PET scan of the brain on the left the resulting extra brain activity is detected with shows that a man who solved 11 out of 36 reasoning problems burned more glucose than the man on fMRI. Eventually, fMRI may help us distinguish the right, who solved 33. between lies, false statements made with the intention to deceive, and confabulations, which are false claims made by persons who believe they are telling the truth (Hirstein, 2005; Langleben, Dattilio, & Gutheil, 2006). Clearly, it is just a matter of time until even brighter beacons are flashed into the shadowy inner world of thought. • • Left side K N O WL E D GE B U I L D E R Brain Research Lie Activation Truth Activation RECITE 1. Which of the following research techniques has the most in common with clinical studies of the effects of brain injuries? a. EEG recording b. deep lesioning c. microelectrode recording d. PET scan 2. CT scans cannot determine which part of your brain plays a role in speech because CT scans a. use X-rays b. reveal brain structure, not brain activity c. reveal brain activity, not brain structure d. use magnetic fields 3. _________________ links brain structures to brain functions. 4. People only use 10 percent of their brain capacity. T or F? REFLECT Critical Thinking 5. Deep lesioning is used to ablate an area in the hypothalamus of a rat. After the operation, the rat seems to lose interest in food and eating. Why would it be a mistake to automatically conclude that the ablated area is a “hunger center”? Relate You suspect that a certain part of the brain is related to risk-taking. How could you use clinical studies, ablation, deep lesioning, and ESB to study the structure? You want to know which areas of the brain’s surface are most active when a person sees a face. What methods will you use? Anterior • Daniel Langleben, University of Pennsylvania Right side Figure 2.15 Participants were asked to tell the truth or to lie while fMRI images of their brains were taken. When compared with telling the truth (shown in blue), areas toward the front of the brain were active during lying (shown in red). (Adapted from Langleben et al., 2005.) The Cerebral Cortex — My, What a Big Brain You Have! Gateway Question: Why is the human cerebral cortex so important, and what are its parts? In many ways we are pretty unimpressive creatures. Animals surpass humans in almost every category of strength, speed, and sensory sensitivity. However, we do excel in intelligence. Deep lesioning Removal of tissue within the brain by use of an electrode. Electroencephalograph (EEG) A device that detects, amplifies, and records electrical activity in the brain. PET scan Positron emission tomography; a computer-generated image of brain activity based on glucose consumption in the brain. Functional MRI (fMRI) Functional magnetic resonance imaging that records brain activity. Richard Haier, University of California, Irvine fMRI Answers: 1. b 2. b 3. Localization of function 4. F 5. Because other factors might explain the apparent loss of appetite. For example, the taste or smell of food might be affected, or the rat might simply have difficulty swallowing. It is also possible that hunger originates elsewhere in the brain and the ablated area merely relays messages that cause the rat to eat. 60 CHAPTER 2 Cortex Cerebrum Cerebellum Neocortex Olfactory lobe Although the cortex is only 3 millimeters thick (one tenth of an inch), it contains 70 percent of the neurons in the central nervous system. It is largely responsible for our ability to use language, make tools, acquire complex skills, and live in complex social groups (Gibson, 2002). Without the cortex, we humans wouldn’t be much smarter than toads. Cerebral Hemispheres Fish Brain The cortex is composed of two sides, or cerebral hemispheres (half-globes), conCerebrum nected by a thick band of fibers called the corpus callosum (KORE-pus kah-LOHsum) ( Figure 2.17). The left side of the brain mainly controls the right side of the Cerebellum body. Likewise, the right brain mainly Human Brain Olfactory lobe Cerebrum controls left body areas. When our friend Marge had a stroke, her right hemisphere Reptile Brain suffered damage. In Marge’s case, the Figure 2.16 stroke caused some paralysis and loss of sensation on the left side of her body. Damage to one hemisphere may also cause a curious problem Does that mean humans have the largest brains? Surpriscalled spatial neglect. A patient with right hemisphere damage may ingly, no. Elephant brains weigh 13 pounds, and whale brains, pay no attention to the left side of visual space ( Figure 2.18). 19 pounds. At 3 pounds, the human brain seems puny — until Often, the patient will not eat food on the left side of a plate. we compare brain weight to body weight. We then find that Some even refuse to acknowledge a paralyzed left arm as their an elephant’s brain is 1/1,000 of its weight; the ratio for sperm own (Hirstein, 2005). If you point to the “alien” arm, the patient whales is 1 to 10,000. The ratio for humans is 1 to 60. If someone is likely to say, “Oh, that’s not my arm. It must belong to someone tells you that you have a “whale of a brain” be sure to find out if else.” (To learn more about strokes, see “A Stroke of Bad Luck.”) they mean size or ratio! So having a larger brain doesn’t necessarily make a person smarter? That’s right. While a small positive correlation exists between Hemispheric Specialization intelligence and brain size, overall size alone does not determine In 1981, Roger Sperry (1914–1994) won a Nobel Prize for his human intelligence ( Johnson et al., 2008; Witelson, Beresh, & remarkable discovery that the right and left brain hemispheres Kigar, 2006). In fact, many parts of your brain are surprisingly perform differently on tests of language, perception, music, and similar to corresponding brain areas in lower animals, such as other capabilities. lizards. It is your larger cerebral (seh-REE-brel or ser-EH-brel) Corpus Cerebral cortex that sets you apart. callosum cortex The cerebral cortex, which looks a little like a giant, wrinkled walnut, consists of the two large hemispheres that cover the upper part of the brain. The two hemispheres are divided into smaller areas known as lobes. Parts of various lobes are responsible for the ability to see, hear, move, think, and speak. Thus, a map of the cerebral cortex is in some ways like a map of human behavior, as we shall see. The cerebral cortex covers most of the brain with a mantle of gray matter (spongy tissue made up mostly of cell bodies). The cortex in lower animals is small and smooth. In humans it is twisted and folded, and it is the largest brain structure ( Figure 2.16). The fact that humans are more intelligent than other animals is related to this corticalization (KORE-tih-kal-ih-ZAY-shun), or Figure 2.17 increase in the size and wrinkling of the cortex. Neocortex Cerebellum • • • • • Brain and Behavior 61 T HE CLI N I CA L FI LE A Stroke of Bad Luck One morning Bryan Kolb lost his left hand. Up early to feed his cat, he could not see his hand, or anything else to his upper left side. Kolb, a Canadian neuroscientist, instantly realized that he had suffered a right hemisphere stroke. (Remember, a stroke occurs when an artery carrying blood in the brain bleeds or becomes blocked, causing some brain tissue to die.) He drove to the hospital where he argued with the doctors about his own diagnosis. He was right, of course! He eventually resumed his career and even wrote a fascinating account of his own case (Kolb, 1990). Model Neurological soft signs, as they are called, include clumsiness, an awkward gait, poor hand–eye coordination, and other problems with perception or fine muscle control (Stuss & Levine, 2002). These telltale signs are “soft” in the sense that they aren’t direct tests of the brain, like a CT scan or MRI scan. Bryan Kolb initially diagnosed himself entirely with soft signs. Likewise, soft signs help psychologists diagnose problems ranging from childhood learning disorders to full-blown psychosis (Ward, 2006). Patient’s copy 1 f Le 2 10 9 tv iel d 11 12 Strokes and other brain injuries can hit like a thunderbolt. Almost instantly, victims realize that something is wrong. You would, too, if you suddenly found that you couldn’t move, or feel parts of your body, or see, or speak. However, some brain injuries are not so obvious. Many involve less dramatic, but equally disabling, changes in personality, thinking, judgment, or emotions (Banich, 2004; Borod et al., 2002). Although major brain injuries are easy enough to spot, psychologists also look for more subtle signs that the brain is not working properly. 3 f al l field Right visu isu a 4 8 7 6 5 Left eye Right eye Optic nerve Corpus callosum (cut) Optic chiasm (crossover) Lateral geniculate body of thalamus Optic radiation Occipital lobe Figure 2.18 Spatial neglect. A patient with right-hemisphere damage was •asked to copy three model drawings. Notice the obvious neglect of the left side 2.19 Basic nerve pathways of vision. Notice that the left portion of •eachFigure eye connects only to the left half of the brain; likewise, the right portion of in his drawings. Similar instances of neglect occur in many patients with righthemisphere damage. (From Left Brain, Right Brain, Fifth Edition by Sally P. Springer & Georg each eye connects to the right brain. When the corpus callosum is cut, a “split brain” results. Then visual information can be sent to just one hemisphere by flashing it in the right or left visual field as the person stares straight ahead. Deutsch. © 1981, 1985, 1989, 1993, 1998 by Sally P. Springer and Georg Deutsch. Used with permission of W. H. Freeman and Company.) How is it possible to test only one side of the brain? One way is to work with people who’ve had a “split-brain” operation. In this rare type of surgery, the corpus callosum is cut to control severe epilepsy. The result is essentially a person with two brains in one body. After the surgery it is possible to send information to one hemisphere or the other ( Figure 2.19). • Cerebral cortex The outer layer of the brain. Corticalization An increase in the relative size of the cerebral cortex. Neurological soft signs Subtle behavioral signs of brain dysfunction, including clumsiness, an awkward gait, poor hand–eye coordination, and other perceptual and motor problems. “Split-brain” operation Cutting the corpus callosum. 62 CHAPTER 2 • Figure 2.20 A circle is flashed to the left brain of a split-brain patient and he is asked what he saw. He easily replies, “A circle.” He can also pick out the circle by merely touching shapes with his right hand, out of sight behind a screen. However, his left hand can’t identify the circle. If a triangle is flashed to the patient’s right brain, he can’t say what he saw (speech is controlled by the left hemisphere). He also can’t identify the triangle by touch with the right hand. Now, however, the left hand has no difficulty picking out the triangle. In other tests, the hemispheres reveal distinct skills, as listed above the drawing. I see nothing. I see a circle. Left Hemisphere Right Hemisphere “Split Brains” After the right and left brain are separated, each hemisphere will have its own separate perceptions, concepts, and impulses to act. How does a split-brain person act after the operation? Having two “brains” in one body can create some interesting dilemmas. When one split-brain patient dressed himself, he sometimes pulled his pants up with one hand (that side of his brain wanted to get dressed . . .) and down with the other (. . . while this side didn’t). Once, he grabbed his wife with his left hand and shook her violently. Gallantly, his right hand came to her aid and grabbed the aggressive left hand (Gazzaniga, 1970). However, such conflicts are actually rare. That’s because both halves of the brain normally have about the same experience at the same time. Also, if a conflict arises, one hemisphere usually overrides the other. Split-brain effects are easiest to see in specialized testing. For example, we could flash a dollar sign to the right brain and a question mark to the left brain of a patient named Tom. ( Figure 2.19 shows how this is possible.) Next, Tom is asked to draw what he saw, using his left hand, out of sight. Tom’s left hand draws a dollar sign. If Tom is then asked to point with his right hand to a picture of what his hidden left hand drew, he will point to a question mark (Sperry, 1968). In short, for the split-brain person, one hemisphere may not know what is happening in the other. This has to be the ultimate case of the “right hand not knowing what the left hand is doing”! Figure 2.20 provides another example of split-brain testing. • tions, the right hemisphere must use nonverbal responses, such as pointing at objects ( Figure 2.21). Although it is poor at producing language, the right brain is especially good at perceptual skills, such as recognizing patterns, faces, and melodies; putting together a puzzle; or drawing a picture. It is also helps you express emotions and detect the emotions that other people are feeling (Borod et al., 2002; Stuss & Alexander, 2000). Even though the right hemisphere is nearly “speechless,” it is superior at some aspects of understanding language. If the right side of the brain is damaged, people lose their ability to understand jokes, irony, sarcasm, implications, and other nuances of language. Basically, the right hemisphere helps us see the overall context in which something is said (Beeman & Chiarello, 1998). • Left hemisphere Right hemisphere DETAILS OVERALL PATTERN “A bunch of Ds” D D D D D DDDDD “The letter L” A stitch in time saves nine. “A small effort now saves time later.” “It’s about sewing.” Right Brain/Left Brain Earlier it was stated that the hemispheres differ in abilities; in what ways are they different? The brain divides its work in interesting ways. Roughly 95 percent of us use our left brain for language (speaking, writing, and understanding). In addition, the left hemisphere is superior at math, judging time and rhythm, and coordinating the order of complex movements, such as those needed for speech. In contrast, the right hemisphere can produce only the simplest language and numbers. Working with the right brain is like talking to a child who can say only a dozen words or so. To answer ques- “Dots and blobs” “A dog” Figure 2.21 The left and right brain have different information processing •styles. The right brain gets the big pattern; the left focuses on small details. Ronald C. James • Brain and Behavior 63 One Brain, Two Styles The Frontal Lobes In general, the left hemisphere is mainly involved with analysis (breaking information into parts). It also processes information sequentially (in order, one item after the next). The right hemisphere appears to process information simultaneously and holistically (all at once) (Springer & Deutsch, 1998). To summarize further, you could say that the right hemisphere is better at assembling pieces of the world into a coherent picture; it sees overall patterns and general connections. The left brain focuses on small details. (See Figure 2.21.) The right brain sees the wide-angle view; the left zooms in on specifics. The focus of the left brain is local, the right is global (Hübner & Volberg, 2005). Do people normally do puzzles or draw pictures with just the right hemisphere? Do they do other things with only the left? Numerous books have been written about how to use the right brain to manage, teach, draw, ride horses, learn, and even make love (Clark, Boutros, & Mendez, 2005). But such books drastically oversimplify right-brain and left-brain differences. People normally use both sides of the brain at all times. It’s true that some tasks may make more use of one hemisphere or the other. But in most “real world” activities, the hemispheres share the work. Each does the parts it does best and shares information with the other side. A smart brain is one that grasps both the details and the overall picture at the same time. For instance, during a concert a flamenco guitarist will use his left brain to judge time and rhythm and coordinate the order of his hand movements. At the same time, he will use his right brain to recognize and organize melodies. The frontal lobes are associated with higher mental abilities and play a role in your sense of self. This area is also responsible for the control of movement. Specifically, an arch of tissue at the rear of the frontal lobes, called the primary motor area (or primary motor cortex), directs the body’s muscles. If this area is stimulated with an electrical current, various parts of the body will twitch or move. The drawing wrapped around the motor cortex in Figure 2.23 is out of proportion because it reflects the dexterity of body areas, not their size. The hands, for example, get more area than the feet. (See Figure 2.23.) If you’ve ever wondered why your hands are more skilled or agile than your feet, it’s partly because more motor cortex is devoted to the hands. Incidentally, due to neuroplasticity, learning and experience can alter these “motor maps.” For instance, violin, viola, and cello players have larger “hand maps” in the cortex (Hashimoto et al., 2004). Motor cortex is one brain area that contains mirror neurons. These are neurons that become active when we perform an action and when we merely observe someone else carrying out the same action. (For more information about mirror neurons, see “Mirror, Mirror in the Brain.”) The rest of the frontal lobes are often referred to as frontal association areas. Only a small portion of the cerebral cortex (the primary areas) directly controls the body or receives information from the senses. All the surrounding areas, which are called association areas (or association cortex), combine and process information. For example, if you see a rose, association areas will help you connect your primary sensory impressions with memories, so that you can recognize the rose and name it. Some association areas also contribute to higher mental abilities, such as language. For example, a person with damage to association areas in the left hemisphere may suffer aphasia (ah-FAZE-yah: an impaired ability to use language). One type of aphasia is related to Broca’s (BRO-cahs) area, a “speech center” that is part of the left frontal association area (for 5 percent of all people, the area is part of the right frontal association area). Damage to Broca’s area causes motor (or expressive) • Lobes of the Cerebral Cortex Each of the two hemispheres of the cerebral cortex can be divided into several smaller lobes. Some of the lobes of the cerebral cortex are defined by larger fissures on the surface of the cortex. Others are regarded as separate areas because their functions are quite different ( Figure 2.22). • Frontal lobe (sense of self, motor control, and higher mental abilities such as reasoning and planning) Parietal lobe (sensation such as touch, temperature, and pressure) • • Lobes of the cerebral cortex Areas on the cortex bordered by major fissures or defined by their functions. Frontal lobes A brain area associated with movement, the sense of self, and higher mental functions. Occipital lobe (vision) Temporal lobe (hearing and language) • Figure 2.22 Primary motor area (primary motor cortex) A brain area associated with control of movement. Mirror neuron A neuron that becomes active when a motor action is carried out and when another organism is observed carrying out the same action. Cerebellum (posture, coordination, muscle tone, and memory of skills and habits) Association area (association cortex) All areas of the cerebral cortex that are not primarily sensory or motor in function. Aphasia A speech disturbance resulting from brain damage. Broca’s area A language area related to grammar and pronunciation. 64 CHAPTER 2 Primary Motor s r ge Trun k Trunk Neck Head Shoulder Arm Elbow arm Forreist W nd Hattle Li ng le Ri idd x M de In Jaw Shoulder Elbow at Saliv Voc alization Lips Wrist Hand le Litting le R idd x M de b In humck T e N Br Eye ow Fac lid an e d Primary Somatosensory ip H Knee Ankle Toes T Ey hum rs No e b ge Fin Fac se e Up per lip Lips Lowe r lip Teeth, gums, an d jaw Tongue al x min ryn Pha -abdo a Intr ans org Fin eye ba ll Left hemisphere e Tongu M Swa ll a ion sticatioowing n Left hemisphere Temporal lobe Temporal lobe Primary somatosensory area Primary motor area • Figure 2.23 The lobes of the Prefrontal area cerebral cortex and the primary sensory, motor, visual, and auditory areas on each. Broca’s area The top diagrams show (in cross section) the relative amounts of cortex “assigned” to the sensory and motor control of various parts of the body. (Each cross section, or “slice,” of Primary auditory area the cortex has been turned 90 degrees so that you see it as it would appear from the back of the brain.) p Hi eg L Foot Toes Genitalia Primary visual area Wernicke’s area Cerebellum Pons Medulla aphasia, a great difficulty in speaking or writing (Ward, 2006). Generally, the person knows what she or he wants to say but can’t seem to fluently utter the words (Geschwind, 1979). Typically, a patient’s grammar and pronunciation are poor and speech is slow and labored. For example, the person may say “bife” for bike, “seep” for sleep, or “zokaid” for zodiac. The very front of the frontal association region is known as the prefrontal area (or prefrontal cortex). This part of the brain is related to more complex behaviors. If the frontal lobes are damaged, a patient’s personality and emotional life may change dramatically. Remember Phineas Gage, the railroad foreman described in Chapter 1? It’s likely that Gage’s personality changed after he suffered brain damage because the prefrontal cortex generates our sense of self, including an awareness of our current emotional state (Moran et al., 2006; Kawasaki et al., 2005). Reasoning or planning may also be affected (Goel & Dolan, 2004). Patients with damage to the frontal lobes often get “stuck” on mental tasks and repeat the same wrong answers over and over Spinal cord (Stuss & Knight, 2002). PET scans suggest that much of what we call intelligence is related to increased activity in the frontal areas of the cortex (Duncan, 2005). Sadly, drug abuse can damage this important area of the brain (Liu et al., 1998). The Parietal Lobes Bodily sensations register in the parietal (puh-RYE-ih-tal) lobes, located just above the occipital lobes. Touch, temperature, pressure, and other somatic sensations flow into the primary somatosensory (SO-mat-oh-SEN-so-ree) area (or primary somatosensory cortex) of the parietal lobes. Again we find that the map of bodily sensations is distorted. In the case of somatosensory cortex, the drawing in Figure 2.23 reflects the sensitivity of body areas, not their size. For example, the lips are large in the drawing because of their great sensitivity, whereas the back and trunk, which are less sensitive, are much smaller. Notice that the hands are also large in the map of body sensitivity — which is obviously an aid to musicians, typists, watchmakers, massage therapists, lovers, and brain surgeons. • Brain and Behavior 65 CRIT ICA L T H I N KI N G Italian researchers had just recorded an increase in the activity of a single neuron in the motor cortex of a monkey as it reached for food. A few seconds later, one of the researchers happened to reach for a snack of his own. The same neuron obligingly responded as if the monkey had reached for the food itself. Unexpectedly, a neuron involved in controlling a particular motor movement was also activated when the monkey merely observed that same motor movement in someone else. Just like that, the Italians discovered mirror neurons (Rizzolatti, Fogassi, & Gallese, 2006). Because they mirror actions performed by others, such neurons may explain how we can intuitively understand other people’s behavior. They may also underlie our ability to learn new skills by imitation (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004). The discovery of mirror neurons has triggered a flood of interest. Recently, researchers have confirmed that mirror neurons are found in various areas of the brain and appear to exist in the human brain as well (Bertenthal & Longo, 2007). In addition, neuroscientists speculate that newborn humans (and monkeys) are able to imitate others because networks of mirror neurons are activated when an infant watches someone perform an action. Then the same mirror network can be used to perform that action (Lepage & Théret, 2007). Similarly, human empathy (the ability to identify with another person’s experiences and feelings) may arise from activation of mirror neurons (de C. Hamilton, 2008). Mirror neurons may even partially explain autism spectrum disorders. In early childhood, children with autism begin to suffer from an impaired ability to interact and communicate with other people. Restricted and repetitive behavior such as head banging is also common. According to the “broken mirrors” hypothesis, autism may arise in infants whose mirror neuron system has been damaged by genetic defects or environmental risk factors (Ramachandran & Oberman, 2006). This explanation is attractive because autism’s primary features of impaired communication and social interaction appear to be related to the role that mirror neurons play in reflecting the actions and words of others. To date, these are just hypotheses that await empirical confirmation. More importantly, such possibilities have not yet led to any new therapies for autism. Nevertheless, the possibilities are exciting. China Photos/Getty Images Mirror, Mirror in the Brain The intense social isolation of autism spectrum disorder may arise because of damage to mirror neurons distributed throughout the brain. The Temporal Lobes The Occipital Lobes The temporal lobes are located on each side of the brain. Auditory information projects directly to the primary auditory area, making it the main site where hearing first registers. If we did a PET scan of your brain while you listened to your favorite MP3, your primary auditory area would be the first to light up, followed by association areas in your temporal lobes. Likewise, if we could electrically stimulate the primary auditory area of your temporal lobe, you would “hear” a series of sound sensations. An association area, called Wernicke’s (VER-nick-ees) area (see Figure 2.23), lies on the left temporal lobe (again, for 5 percent of all people, the area is on the right temporal lobe). Wernicke’s area also functions as a language site. If it is damaged, the result is a receptive (or fluent) aphasia. Although the person can hear speech, he or she has difficulty understanding the meaning of words. Thus, when shown a picture of a chair, someone with Broca’s aphasia might say “tssair.” In contrast, a Wernicke’s patient might fluently, but incorrectly, identify the photo as “truck” (Ward, 2006). At the back of the brain, we find the occipital (awk-SIP-ih-tal) lobes, the area of the cortex concerned with vision. Patients with tumors (cell growths that interfere with brain activity) in the • Prefrontal area (prefrontal cortex) The very front of the frontal lobes; involved in sense of self, reasoning, and planning. Parietal lobes Area of the brain where body sensations register. Primary somatosensory area (primary somatosensory cortex) A receiving area for body sensations. Temporal lobes Areas on each side of the brain where hearing registers in the brain. Primary auditory area Part of the temporal lobe where auditory information is first registered. Wernicke’s area A temporal lobe brain area related to language comprehension. Occipital lobes Portion of the cerebral cortex where vision registers in the brain. 66 CHAPTER 2 HUM AN D IVERS I T Y His and Hers Brains? Are men’s and women’s brains specialized in different ways? Yes, to some extent. Many physical differences between male and female brains have been found, although their significance remains debatable. In one series of studies, researchers observed brain activity while people did language tasks. Both men and women showed increased activity in Broca’s area, on the left side of the brain, exactly as expected. Surprisingly, however, the left and the right brain were activated in more than half the women tested (Shaywitz & Gore, 1995) ( Figure 2.24). Using both sides of the brain for language may be a big advantage. When Broca’s area is damaged, some women can use the right side of their brains to compensate for the loss (Hochstenbach et al., 1998). Thus, when a man says, “I have half a mind to tell you what I think,” he may be stating a curious truth. Despite this difference, the two sexes performed equally well on a task that involved sounding out words (Shaywitz et al., 1995). The researchers concluded that nature has given the brain different routes to the same ability. In a study of men and women with similar IQ scores, brain images revealed major differ- ences in brain areas involved in intelligence (Haier et al., 2004). In general, the men had more gray matter (neuron cell bodies), while the women had more white matter (axons coated in myelin). Further, the women had more gray and white matter concentrated in their frontal lobes than the men did. The men’s gray matter was split between their frontal and parietal lobes, while their white matter was mostly in the temporal lobes. Whatever else these differences mean, they show that the human brain can be specialized in different ways to arrive at the same capabilities. Shaywitz et al., 1995 NMR Research/Yale Medical School • 2.24 •sideFigure in men. Language tasks activate both sides of the brain in many women but only the left primary visual area, the part of cortex to first receive input from the eyes, experience blind spots in their vision. Do the primary visual areas of the cortex correspond directly to what is seen? Images are mapped onto the cortex, but the map is greatly stretched and distorted (Carlson, 2007). That’s why it’s important to avoid thinking of the visual area as being like a little TV screen in the brain. Visual information creates complex patterns of activity in nerve cells; it does not make a TV-like image. One of the most fascinating results of brain injury is visual agnosia (ag-KNOW-zyah), an inability to identify seen objects. Visual agnosia is often caused by damage to the association areas on the occipital lobes (Farah, 2004). This condition is sometimes referred to as “mindblindness.” For example, if we show Alice, an agnosia patient, a candle, she can see it and can describe it as “a long narrow object that tapers at the top.” Alice can even draw the candle accurately, but she cannot name it. However, if she is allowed to feel the candle, she will name it immediately. In short, Alice can still see color, size, and shape. She just can’t form the associations necessary to perceive the meanings of objects. Are agnosias limited to objects? No. A fascinating form of “mindblindness” is facial agnosia, an inability to perceive familiar faces (Farah, 2006). One patient with facial agnosia couldn’t recognize her husband or mother when they visited her in the hospital, and she was unable to identify pictures of her children. However, as soon as a visitor spoke she knew them immediately by their voices. Areas devoted to recognizing faces lie in association areas on the underside of the occipital lobes. These areas appear to have no other function. Why would part of the brain be set aside solely for identifying faces? From an evolutionary standpoint it is not really so surprising. After all, we are social animals, for whom facial recognition is very important. This specialization is just one example of what a marvelous organ of consciousness we possess. How much do individual brains differ? Could we find different specializations from brain to brain? Perhaps. “His and Hers Brains?” explains why. In summary, the bulk of our daily experience and all of our understanding of the world can be traced to the different areas Brain and Behavior of the cortex. The human brain is among the most advanced and sophisticated of the brain-bearing species on earth. This, of course, is no guarantee that this marvelous “biocomputer” will be put to full use. Still, we must stand in awe of the potential it represents. K N O W L E D GE B U I L D E R Hemispheres and Lobes of the Cerebral Cortex RECITE See if you can match the following: 1. _____ Corpus callosum 2. _____ Occipital lobes 3. _____ Parietal lobes 4. _____ Temporal lobes 5. _____ Frontal lobes 6. _____ Association cortex 7. _____ Aphasias 8. _____ Corticalization 9. _____ Left hemisphere 10. _____ Right hemisphere 11. _____ “Split brain” 12. _____ Agnosia 67 brain) can be fatal. Hunger, thirst, sleep, attention, sex, breathing, and many other vital functions are controlled by parts of the subcortex. Let’s take a quick tour of these brain areas, which can be divided into the brainstem (or hindbrain), the midbrain, and the forebrain. (The forebrain also includes the cerebral cortex, which we have already discussed because of its size and importance.) For our purposes the midbrain can be viewed as a link between the forebrain and the brainstem. Therefore, let us focus on the rest of the subcortex ( Figure 2.25). • The Hindbrain A. B. C. D. Visual area Language, speech, writing Motor cortex and abstract thinking Spatial skills, visualization, pattern recognition E. Speech disturbances F. Causes sleep G. Increased ratio of cortex in brain H. Bodily sensations I. Treatment for severe epilepsy J. Inability to identify seen objects K. Fibers connecting the cerebral hemispheres L. Cortex that is not sensory or motor in function M. Hearing REFLECT Critical Thinking 13. If you wanted to increase the surface area of the cerebral cortex so that more cerebral cortex would fit within the skull, how would you do it? 14. If your brain were removed, replaced by another, and moved to a new body, which would you consider to be yourself, your old body with the new brain, or your new body with the old brain? Relate Learning the functions of the brain lobes is like learning areas on a map. Try drawing a map of the cortex. Can you label all the different “countries” (lobes)? Can you name their functions? Where is the primary motor area? The somatosensory area? Broca’s area? Keep redrawing the map until it becomes more detailed and you can do it easily. Why are the lower brain areas so important? As the spinal cord joins the brain, it widens into the brainstem. The brainstem consists mainly of the medulla (meh-DUL-ah) and the cerebellum (ser-ahBEL-uhm). The medulla contains centers important for the reflex control of vital life functions, including heart rate, breathing, swallowing, and the like. Various drugs, diseases, and injuries can disrupt the medulla and end or endanger life. That’s why a karate chop to the back of the neck can be extremely dangerous. The pons, which looks like a small bump on the brainstem, acts as a bridge between the medulla and other brain areas. In addition to connecting with many other locations, including the cerebellum, the pons influences sleep and arousal. The cerebellum, which looks like a miniature cerebral cortex, lies at the base of the brain. The cerebellum primarily regulates posture, muscle tone, and muscular coordination. The cerebellum also stores memories related to skills and habits (Christian & Thompson, 2005). Again we see that experience shapes the brain: Musicians, who practice special motor skills throughout their lives, have larger than average cerebellums (Hutchinson et al., 2003). bridges In general, the cerebellum stores “know how” or “skill” memories. “Know what” memories, such as remembering a person’s name or knowing what the cerebellum does, are stored elsewhere in the brain. See Chapter 8, page 260. Primary visual area The part of occipital lobe that first receives input from the eyes. Answers: 1. K 2. A 3. H 4. M 5. C 6. L 7. E 8. G 9. B 10. D 11. I 12. J 13. One solution would be to gather the surface of the cortex into folds, just as you might if you were trying to fit a large piece of cloth into a small box. This, in fact, is probably why the cortex is more convoluted (folded or wrinkled) in higher animals. 14. Although there is no “correct” answer to this question, your personality, knowledge, personal memories, and self-concept all derive from brain activity — which makes a strong case for your old brain in a new body being more nearly the “real you.” Visual agnosia Inability to identify seen objects. Facial agnosia Inability to perceive familiar faces. The Subcortex — At the Core of the (Brain) Matter Gateway Question: What are the major parts of the subcortex? You could lose large portions of your cerebral cortex and still survive. Not so with the subcortex, the brain structures immediately below the cerebral cortex. Serious damage to the subcortex (lower Subcortex All brain structures below the cerebral cortex. Brainstem The lowest portions of the brain, including the cerebellum, medulla, pons, and reticular formation. Medulla The structure that connects the brain with the spinal cord and controls vital life functions. Pons An area on the brainstem that acts as a bridge between the medulla and other structures. Cerebellum A brain structure that controls posture and coordination. 68 CHAPTER 2 Cerebrum (Surface: cerebral cortex) Voluntary movements; sensations, learning, remembering, thinking, emotion, consciousness Corpus Callosum Band of fibers connecting the two hemispheres Thalamus Relay station to cortex for sensory information Hypothalamus Control of hunger, thirst, temperature, and other visceral and bodily functions Midbrain Conduction and switching center Cerebellum Muscle tone; body balance; coordination of skilled movement Pituitary Gland The ”master gland” of the endocrine system Medulla Centers for control over breathing, swallowing, digestion, heart rate Reticular Formation Arousal; attention; movement; reflexes Spinal Cord Conduction paths for motor and sensory impulses; local reflexes (reflex arc) Forebrain Midbrain Hindbrain • Figure 2.25 This simplified drawing shows the main structures of the human brain and describes some of their most important features. (You can use the color code in the foreground to identify which areas are part of the forebrain, midbrain, and hindbrain.) What happens if the cerebellum is injured? Without the cerebellum, tasks like walking, running, or playing catch become impossible. The first symptoms of a crippling disease called spinocerebellar degeneration are tremor, dizziness, and muscular weakness. Eventually, victims have difficulty merely standing, walking, or feeding themselves. Reticular Formation A network of fibers and cell bodies called the reticular (rehTICK-you-ler) formation (RF) lies inside the medulla and brainstem. As messages flow into the brain, the RF gives priority to some while turning others aside (Kalat, 2007). By doing so, the RF influences attention. The RF doesn’t fully mature until adolescence, which may be why children have such short attention spans. The RF also modifies outgoing commands to the body. In this way the RF affects muscle tone, posture, and movements of the eyes, face, head, body, and limbs. At the same time, the RF controls reflexes involved in breathing, sneezing, coughing, and vomiting. The RF also keeps us vigilant, alert, and awake. Incoming messages from the sense organs branch into a part of the RF called the reticular activating system (RAS). The RAS bombards the cortex with stimulation, keeping it active and alert. For instance, let’s say a sleepy driver rounds a bend and sees a deer standing in the road. The driver snaps to attention and applies the brakes. She can thank her RAS for arousing the rest of her brain and averting an accident. If you’re getting sleepy while reading this chapter, try pinching your ear — a little pain will cause the RAS to momentarily arouse your cortex. The Forebrain Like buried treasure, two of the most important parts of your body lie deep within your brain. The thalamus (THAL-uh-mus) and an area just below it called the hypothalamus (HI-po-THAL-uhmus) are key parts of the forebrain. (See Figure 2.25.) How could these be any more important than other areas already described? The thalamus acts as a final “switching station” for sensory messages on their way to the cortex. Vision, hearing, taste, and touch all pass through this small, footballshaped structure. Thus, injury to even small areas of the thalamus can cause deafness, blindness, or loss of any other sense, except smell. The human hypothalamus is about the size of a small grape. Small as it may be, the hypothalamus is a kind of master control center for emotion and many basic motives (Carlson, 2007). The hypothalamus affects behaviors as diverse as sex, rage, body temperature, hormone release, eating and drinking, sleep, waking, and emotion. (See Chapter 10, pages 324–325.) The hypothalamus is basically a “crossroads” that connects many areas of the brain. It is also the final pathway for many kinds of behavior. That is, the hypothalamus is the last place where many behaviors are organized or “decided on” before messages leave the brain, causing the body to react. • Brain and Behavior The Limbic System As a group, the hypothalamus, parts of the thalamus, the amygdala, the hippocampus, and other structures make up the limbic system ( Figure 2.26). The limbic system has a major role in producing emotion and motivated behavior. Rage, fear, sexual response, and intense arousal can be localized to various points in the limbic system. Laughter, a delightful part of human social life, also has its origins in the limbic system (Cardoso, 2000). During evolution, the limbic system was the earliest layer of the forebrain to develop. In lower animals, the limbic system helps organize basic survival responses: feeding, fleeing, fighting, or reproduction. In humans, a clear link to emotion remains. The amygdala (ah-MIG-dah-luh), in particular, is strongly related to fear. For example, during medical testing one woman reacted with a sudden outburst of fear and anger when the amygdala was stimulated, saying, “I feel like I want to get up from this chair! Please don’t let me do it! I don’t want to be mean! I want to get something and just tear it up!” (King, 1961). The amygdala provides a primitive, “quick pathway” to the cortex. Like lower animals, we can be startled and, as such, are able to react to dangerous stimuli before we fully know what is going on. In situations where true danger exists, such as in military combat, the amygdala’s rapid response may aid survival. However, disorders of the brain’s fear system can be very disruptive. An example is the war veteran who involuntarily dives into the bushes when he hears a car backfire (Fellous & LeDoux, 2005; LaBar & LeDoux, 2002). The role of the amygdala in emotion may also explain why people who suffer from phobias and disabling anxiety often feel afraid without knowing why (LeDoux, 1999). • Cingulate gyrus Mammillary body Thalamus Fornix 69 bridges Unconscious fear produced by the amygdala seems to explain why people who survive horrible experiences, such as a plane crash, can have debilitating fears years later. See the discussion of stress disorders in Chapter 14, page 483. Some parts of the limbic system have taken on additional, higher-level functions. A part called the hippocampus (HIP-ohCAMP-us) is important for forming lasting memories (Kumaran & Maguire, 2005). The hippocampus lies inside the temporal lobes, which is why stimulating the temporal lobes can produce memory-like or dream-like experiences. The hippocampus also helps us navigate through space. The right side of your hippocampus will become more active, for instance, if you mentally plan a drive across town (Maguire, Frackowiak, & Frith, 1997). Psychologists have discovered that animals will learn to press a lever to deliver a dose of electrical stimulation to the limbic system. The animals act like the stimulation is satisfying or pleasurable. Indeed, several areas of the limbic system act as reward, or “pleasure,” pathways. Many are found in the hypothalamus, where they overlap with areas that control thirst, sex, and hunger. Commonly abused drugs, such as cocaine, amphetamine, heroin, nicotine, marijuana, and alcohol, activate many of the same pleasure pathways. This appears to be part of the reason why these drugs are so powerfully rewarding (Kandel, Schwartz, & Jessell, 2003). You might also be interested to know that music you would describe as “thrilling” activates pleasure systems in your brain. This may explain some of the appeal of music that can send shivers down your spine (Blood & Zatorre, 2001). (It may also explain why people will pay so much for concert tickets!) Punishment, or “aversive,” areas have also been found in the limbic system. When these locations are activated, animals show discomfort and will work hard to turn off the stimulation. Because much of our behavior is based on seeking pleasure and avoiding pain, these discoveries continue to fascinate psychologists. Reticular formation (RF) A network within the medulla and brainstem; associated with attention, alertness, and some reflexes. Reticular activating system (RAS) A part of the reticular formation that activates the cerebral cortex. Hippocampus Hypothalamus Thalamus A brain structure that relays sensory information to the cerebral cortex. Hypothalamus A small area of the brain that regulates emotional behaviors and motives. Amygdala 2.26 Parts of the limbic system. Although only one side is shown •here,Figure the hippocampus and the amygdala extend out into the temporal lobes at each side of the brain. The limbic system is a sort of “primitive core” of the brain strongly associated with emotion. Limbic system A system in the forebrain that is closely linked with emotional response. Amygdala A part of the limbic system associated with fear responses. Hippocampus A part of the limbic system associated with storing memories. 70 CHAPTER 2 The Magnificent Brain Given the amount of information covered in our journey through the brain, a short review is in order. We have seen that the human brain is an impressive assembly of billions of sensitive cells and nerve fibers. The brain controls vital bodily functions, keeps track of the external world, issues commands to the muscles and glands, responds to current needs, regulates its own behavior, and even creates the “mind” and the magic of consciousness — all at the same time. A final note of caution is now in order. For the sake of simplicity we have assigned functions to each “part” of the brain as if it were a computer. This is only a half-truth. In reality, the brain is a vast information-processing system. Incoming information scatters all over the brain and converges again as it goes out through the spinal cord, to muscles and glands. The overall system is much, much more complicated than our discussion of separate “parts” implies. In addition, the brain constantly revises its circuits in response to changing life experiences (Kolb & Whishaw, 2006). We began our exploration of the brain with a virtuoso performance. Imagine the other extreme of being completely unable to move or speak. Even though you would remain alert and intelligent, you would be unable to communicate your simplest thoughts and feelings to others. Each year, this is the fate of thousands of people, like Kate Adamson, who are paralyzed by stroke, disease, or injury. In a very real sense, these people are locked in, prisoners in their own bodies (Smith & Delargy, 2005). What if they could “will” a computer to speak for them? Right on! Researchers have developed brain–computer interfaces that translate a patient’s EEG recordings into commands that can be used to control a computer (Hinterberger et al., 2003) and even access the Internet (Karim et al., 2006). in many ways (Carlson, 2007). Here is a brief sample: Pregnancy and motherhood cause the release of hormones that lead to the dramatic changes involved in maternal behavior (Kingsley & Lambert, 2006). Hormone output from the adrenal glands rises during stressful situations; androgens (“male” hormones) are related to the sex drive in both males and females; hormones secreted during times of high emotion intensify memory formation; at least some of the emotional turmoil of adolescence is due to elevated hormone levels; different hormones prevail when you are angry, rather than fearful. Even disturbing personality patterns may be linked to hormonal irregularities (Evardone, Alexander, & Morey, 2007). In fact, something as routine as watching a movie can alter hormone levels. After watching violent scenes from The Godfather, men had higher levels of the male hormone testosterone. For both men and women, watching a romantic film boosted a hormone that’s linked to relaxation and reproduction (Schultheiss, Wirth, & Stanton, 2004). Because these are just samples, let’s consider some additional effects hormones have on the body and behavior. The pituitary is a pea-sized globe hanging from the base of the brain. (Return to Figure 2.27.) One of the pituitary’s more important roles is to regulate growth. During childhood, the pituitary secretes a hormone that speeds body development. If too little growth hormone is released, a person may remain far smaller than average. If this condition is not treated, a child may be 6 to 12 inches shorter than age-mates. As adults, some will have hypoPineal gland (helps regulate body rhythms and sleep cycles) Pituitary gland (influences growth and lactation; also regulates the activity of other glands) The Endocrine System — My Hormones Made Me Do It Gateway Question: Does the glandular system affect behavior? Our behavior is not solely a product of the nervous system. The endocrine (EN-duh-krin) glands form an equally important parallel communication system in the body. The endocrine system is made up of glands that secrete chemicals directly into the bloodstream or lymph system ( Figure 2.27). These chemicals, called hormones, are carried throughout the body, where they affect both internal activities and visible behavior. Hormones are related to neurotransmitters. Like other transmitter chemicals, hormones activate cells in the body. To respond, the cells must have receptor sites for the hormone. Hormones affect puberty, personality, dwarfism, jet lag, and much more. How do hormones affect behavior? Although we are seldom directly aware of them, hormones affect us • Thyroid gland (regulates the rate of metabolism in the body) Adrenal glands (secretes hormones that arouse the body, help with adjustment to stress, regulate salt balance, and affect sexual functioning) • Pancreas (releases insulin to regulate blood sugar and hunger) Testes (secrete testosterone, which influences male sexual function) • Figure 2.27 Ovaries (secrete estrogen, which influences female sexual function) Brain and Behavior pituitary (HI-po-pih-TU-ih-ter-ee) dwarfism. Such individuals are perfectly proportioned, but tiny. Regular injections of growth hormone can raise a hypopituitary child’s height by several inches, usually to the short side of average. Too much growth hormone produces gigantism (excessive bodily growth). Secretion of too much growth hormone late in the growth period causes acromegaly (AK-row-MEG-uh-lee), a condition in which the arms, hands, feet, and facial bones become enlarged. Acromegaly produces prominent facial features, which some people have used as a basis for careers as character actors, wrestlers, and the like. The pituitary also governs the functioning of other glands (especially the thyroid, adrenal glands, and ovaries or testes). These glands in turn regulate such bodily processes as metabolism, responses to stress, and reproduction. In women, the pituitary controls milk output during breast-feeding. The pituitary is often called the “master gland” because it influences other endocrine glands. But the master has a master: The pituitary is directed by the hypothalamus, which lies directly above it. In this way, the hypothalamus can affect glands throughout the body. This, then, is the major link between the brain and hormones (Carlson, 2007). The pineal (pin-EE-ul) gland was once considered a useless remnant of evolution. In certain fishes, frogs, and lizards, the gland is associated with a well-developed light-sensitive organ, or so-called third eye. In humans, the function of the pineal gland is just now coming to light (so to speak). The pineal gland releases a hormone called melatonin (mel-ah-TONE-in) in response to 71 daily variations in light. Melatonin levels in the bloodstream rise at dusk and peak around midnight and fall again as morning approaches. As far as the brain is concerned, it’s bedtime when melatonin levels rise (Kennaway & Wright, 2002). bridges Melatonin can be used to reset the body’s “clock” and minimize jet lag for long-distance pilots, aircrews, and travelers. See Chapter 10, pages 322–323. The thyroid gland, located in the neck, regulates metabolism. As you may remember from a biology course, metabolism is the rate at which energy is produced and expended in the body. By altering metabolism, the thyroid can have a sizable effect on personality. A person suffering from hyperthyroidism (an overactive thyroid) tends to be thin, tense, excitable, and nervous. An underactive thyroid (hypothyroidism) in an adult can cause inactivity, sleepiness, slowness, obesity, and depression ( Joffe, 2006). In infancy, hypothyroidism limits development of the nervous system, leading to severe mental retardation. (See Chapter 9, pages 309–310.) When you are frightened or angry, some important reactions prepare your body for action: Your heart rate and blood pressure rise, stored sugar is released into the bloodstream for quick energy, your muscles tense and receive more blood, and your blood is prepared to clot more quickly in case of injury. As we Endocrine system Glands whose secretions pass directly into the bloodstream or lymph system. Getty Images Amanda Edwards/Getty Images Hormone A glandular secretion that affects bodily functions or behavior. Underactivity of the pituitary gland may produce a dwarf. Verne Troyer, best known for playing Mini-Me in the Austin Powers movies, has enjoyed an impressive career as an actor. Overactivity of the pituitary gland may produce a giant. Until his premature death in 2005, actor Matthew McGrory was best known for his role of Karl the Giant in the 2003 movie Big Fish. Growth hormone A hormone, secreted by the pituitary gland, that promotes body growth. Pituitary gland The “master gland” whose hormones influence other endocrine glands. Pineal gland Gland in the brain that helps regulate body rhythms and sleep cycles. Melatonin Hormone released by the pineal gland in response to daily cycles of light and dark. Thyroid gland Endocrine gland that helps regulate the rate of metabolism. CHAPTER 2 discussed earlier, these changes are controlled by the autonomic nervous system. Specifically, the sympathetic branch of the ANS causes the hormones epinephrine (ep-eh-NEF-rin) and norepinephrine to be released by the adrenal glands. (Epinephrine is also known as adrenaline, which may be more familiar to you.) Epinephrine, which is associated with fear, tends to arouse the body. Norepinephrine also tends to arouse the body, but it is linked with anger. The adrenal glands are located just under the back of the rib cage, atop the kidneys. The adrenal medulla, or inner core of the adrenal glands, is the source of epinephrine and norepinephrine. The adrenal cortex, or outer “bark” of the adrenal glands, produces a set of hormones called corticoids (KOR-tih-coids). One of their jobs is to regulate salt balance in the body. A deficiency of certain corticoids can evoke a powerful craving for the taste of salt in humans. The corticoids also help the body adjust to stress, and they are a secondary source of sex hormones. An oversecretion of the adrenal sex hormones can cause virilism (exaggerated male characteristics). For instance, a woman may grow a beard or a man’s voice may become so low it is difficult to understand. Oversecretion early in life can cause premature puberty (full sexual development during childhood). One of the most remarkable cases on record is that of a 5-year-old Peruvian girl who gave birth to a son (Strange, 1965). While we are on the topic of sex hormones, there is a related issue worth mentioning. One of the principal androgens, or “male” hormones, is testosterone, which is supplied in small amounts by the adrenal glands. (The testes are the main source of testosterone in males.) Perhaps you have heard about the use of anabolic steroids by athletes who want to “bulk up” or promote muscle growth. Most of these drugs are synthetic versions of testosterone. Although there is some disagreement about whether steroids actually improve athletic performance, it is widely accepted that they may cause serious side effects. Problems include voice deepening or baldness in women and shrinkage of the testicles, sexual impotence, or breast enlargement in men (Millman & Ross, 2003). Dangerous increases in hostility and aggression (“roid rage”) have also been linked with steroid use (Hartgens & Kuipers, 2004). Also common when steroids are used by younger adolescents are an increased risk of heart attack and stroke, liver damage, or stunted growth. Understandably, almost all major sports organizations ban the use of anabolic steroids. In this brief discussion of the endocrine system we have considered only a few of the more important glands. Nevertheless, this should give you an appreciation of how completely behavior and personality are tied to the ebb and flow of hormones in the body. A Look Ahead In the upcoming “Psychology in Action” section, we will return to the brain to see how hand preference relates to brain organization. You’ll also find out if being right- or left-handed affects your chances of living to a ripe old age. KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R Subcortex and Endocrine System RECITE 1. Three major divisions of the brain are the brainstem or _____________________, the _____________________, and the ___________________. 2. Reflex centers for heartbeat and respiration are found in the a. cerebellum b. thalamus c. medulla d. RF 3. A portion of the reticular formation, known as the RAS, serves as an ______________ system in the brain. a. activating b. adrenal c. adjustment d. aversive 4. The _____________ is a final relay, or “switching station,” for sensory information on its way to the cortex. 5. “Reward” and “punishment” areas are found throughout the _____________ system, which is also related to emotion. 6. Undersecretion from the thyroid can cause both a. dwarfism b. gigantism c. obesity d. mental retardation 7. The body’s ability to resist stress is related to the action of the adrenal _________________. REFLECT Critical Thinking 8. Subcortical structures in humans are quite similar to corresponding lower brain areas in animals. Why would knowing this allow you to predict, in general terms, what functions are controlled by the subcortex? 9. Where in all the brain’s “hardware” do you think the mind is found? What is the relationship between mind and brain? Relate If Mr. Medulla met Ms. Cerebellum at a party, what would they say their roles are in the brain? Would a marching band in a “reticular formation” look like a network? Would it get your attention? If you were standing in the final path for behavior leaving the brain, would you be in the thalamus? Or in the hy-path-alamus (please forgive the misspelling)? When you are emotional, do you wave your limbs around (and does your limbic system become more active)? Name as many of the endocrine glands as you can. Which did you leave out? Can you summarize the functions of each of the glands? Answers: 1. hindbrain, midbrain, forebrain 2. c 3. a 4. thalamus 5. limbic 6. c, d (in infancy) 7. cortex 8. Because the subcortex must be related to basic functions common to all higher animals: motives, emotions, sleep, attention, and vegetative functions, such as heartbeat, breathing, and temperature regulation. The subcortex also routes and processes incoming information from the senses and outgoing commands to the muscles. 9. This question, known as the mind–body problem, has challenged thinkers for centuries. One recent view is that mental states are “emergent properties” of brain activity. That is, brain activity forms complex patterns that are, in a sense, more than the sum of their parts. Or, to use a rough analogy, if the brain were a musical instrument, then mental life would be like music played on that instrument. 72 Brain and Behavior 73 P S YC HOL O GY IN A CT ION Handedness — Are You Dexterous or Sinister? Gateway Question: In what ways do rightand left-handed individuals differ? Around the world, left-handedness has been frowned upon. “Lefties” have often been characterized as clumsy, awkward, unlucky, or insincere. The Latin word for left is actually sinister! In contrast, right-handedness is the paragon of virtue. The Latin word for right is dexter, and “righties” are more likely to be referred to as dexterous, coordinated, skillful, and just. But is there any basis in fact for these attitudes? What causes handedness (a preference for the right or left hand)? Why are there more right-handed than left-handed people? How do left-handed and right-handed people differ? Does being left-handed create any problems—or benefits? The answers to these questions lead us back to the brain, where handedness begins. Let’s see what research has revealed about handedness, the brain, and you. Assessing Handedness Write your name on a sheet of paper, first using your right hand and then your left. You were probably much more comfortable writing with your dominant hand. This is interesting because there’s no real difference in the strength or dexterity of the hands themselves. The agility of your dominant hand is an outward expression of superior motor control on one side of the brain. If you are right-handed, there is literally more area on the left side of your brain devoted to controlling your right hand. If you are lefthanded, the reverse applies. The preceding exercise implies that you are either entirely right- or left-handed. But handedness is a matter of degree. To better assess your handedness, complete a few questions adapted from the Waterloo Handedness Questionnaire (Brown et al., 2006) by checking an answer for each question. The more “Rights” you check, the more right-handed you are. Are You Right- or Left-Handed? 1. Which hand would you use to spin a top? ____Right ____Left ____Either 2. With which hand would you hold a paintbrush to paint a wall? ____Right ____Left ____Either 3. Which hand would you use to pick up a book? ____Right ____Left ____Either 4. With which hand would you use a spoon to eat soup? ____Right ____Left ____Either 5. Which hand would you use to flip pancakes? ____Right ____Left ____Either 6. Which hand would you use to pick up a piece of paper? ____Right ____Left ____Either 7. Which hand would you use to draw a picture? ____Right ____Left ____Either 8. Which hand would you use to insert and turn a key in a lock? ____Right ____Left ____Either 9. Which hand would you use to insert a plug into an electrical outlet? ____Right ____Left ____Either 10. Which hand would you use to throw a ball? ____Right ____Left ____Either About 90 percent of all humans are righthanded; 10 percent are left-handed. Most people (about 75 percent) are strongly rightor left-handed. The rest show some inconsistency in hand preference. Which were you? Is there such a thing as being left-footed? Excellent question. Do you have “two left feet”? Sidedness is often measured by assessing hand, foot, eye, and ear preference (Greenwood et al., 2006). We also generally prefer breathing through one nostril over the other and even have a preference for which direction we lean our head when kissing (Barrett, Greenwood, & McCullagh, 2006). (Do you kiss “right”?) Nevertheless, handedness remains the single most important behavioral indicator of sidedness. If a person is strongly left-handed, does that mean the right hemisphere is dominant? Not necessarily. It’s true that the right hemisphere controls the left hand, but a left-handed person’s language-producing, dominant hemisphere may be on the opposite side of the brain. Brain Dominance About 97 percent of right-handers process speech in the left hemisphere and are leftbrain dominant ( Figure 2.28). A good 68 percent of left-handers produce speech from the left hemisphere, just as right-handed people do. About 19 percent of all lefties and 3 percent of righties use their right brain for language. Some left-handers (approximately 12 percent) use both sides of the brain for language processing. All told, 94 percent of the population uses the left brain for language (Coren, 1992). • Is there any way for a person to tell which of his or her hemispheres is dominant? One classic clue is the way you write. Righthanded individuals who write with a straight hand, and lefties who write with a hooked hand, are usually left-brain dominant for language. Left-handed people who write with their hand below the line, and righties who use a hooked position, are usually right-brain dominant (Levy & Reid, 1976). Another hint is provided by hand gestures. If you gesture mostly with your right hand as you talk, you probably process language in your left hemisphere. Gesturing with your left hand is associated with right-brain language processing (Hellige, 1993). Epinephrine An adrenal hormone that tends to arouse the body; epinephrine is associated with fear. (Also known as adrenaline.) Norepinephrine An adrenal hormone that tends to arouse the body; norepinephrine is associated with anger. (Also known as noradrenaline.) Adrenal glands Endocrine glands that arouse the body, regulate salt balance, adjust the body to stress, and affect sexual functioning. Handedness A preference for the right or left hand in most activities. Sidedness A combination of preference for hand, foot, eye, and ear. Dominant hemisphere A term usually applied to the side of a person’s brain that produces language. 74 CHAPTER 2 • Figure 2.28 Language is controlled by the left side of the brain in the majority of rightand left-handers. Entire Population Left-handers Right-handers Left 97% Left 94% Left 68% Right 19% Right 5% Bilateral 1% Bilateral 12% Right 3% Are your friends right-brained or leftbrained ( Figure 2.29)? Before you leap to any conclusions, be aware that writing position and gestures are not foolproof. The only sure way to check brain dominance is to do medical tests that involve assessing one cerebral hemisphere at a time (Kirveskari, Salmelin, & Hari, 2006). • Causes of Handedness • Hooked Left Straight Left Straight Right Hooked Right • Figure 2.29 Research suggests that the hand position used in writing may indicate which brain hemisphere is used for language. (Redrawn from an illustration by M. E. Challinor.) Advantage Right Are there any drawbacks to being left-handed? A small minority of lefties owe their hand preference to birth traumas (such as prematurity, low birth weight, and breech birth). These individuals have higher rates of allergies, learning disorders, and other problems (Betancur et al., 1990). Similarly, people with inconsistent handedness (as opposed to consistent left-handers) may be at risk for more immune-related diseases (Bryden, Bruyn, & Fletcher, 2005). Is it true that right-handed people live longer than left-handed people? It is true that there is a shortage of very old lefties. One possible explanation lies in the widespread finding that left-handers are more accident-prone (Dutta & Mandal, 2005). However, the supposed clumsiness of lefties may well be a result of living in • Figure 2.30 In this ultrasound image, a 4-month-old fetus sucks her right thumb. A study by psychologist Peter Hepper suggests that she will continue to prefer her right hand after she is born and that she will be right-handed as an adult. Custom Medical Stock Photo Is handedness inherited from parents? Yes, at least partly. Clear hand preferences are apparent even before birth, as can be seen in a fetal ultrasound image ( Figure 2.30). According to British psychologist Peter Hepper, prenatal handedness preferences persist for at least 10 years after birth (Hepper, McCartney, & Shannon, 1998; Hepper, Wells, & Lynch, 2005). This suggests that handedness cannot be dictated. Parents should never try to force a left-handed child to use the right hand. To do so may create speech or reading problems. Studies of twins show that hand preferences are not directly inherited like eye color or skin color (Ooki, 2005; Reiss et al., 1999). Yet, two left-handed parents are more likely to have a left-handed child than two righthanded parents are (McKeever, 2000). The best evidence to date shows that handedness is influenced by a single gene on the X (female) chromosome ( Jones & Martin, 2001). On the other hand, environmental factors such as learning, birth traumas, and social pressure to use the right hand can also affect which hand you end up favoring (Bailey & McKeever, 2004; McKeever et al., 2000). In the past, many left-handed children were forced to use their right hand for writing, eating, and other skills. This is especially true in collectivist cultures like India and Japan, where left-handedness is viewed as especially negative. Not surprisingly, the proportion of left-handers in these societies is only about half that found in individualist cultures such as the United States and Canada (Ida & Mandal, 2003). Advantage Left Actually, there are some clear advantages to being left-handed. Throughout history a notable number of artists have been lefties, from Leonardo da Vinci and Michelangelo to Pablo Picasso and M. C. Escher. Conceivably, because the right hemisphere is superior at imagery and visual abilities, there is some advantage to using the left hand for drawing or painting (Springer & Deutsch, 1998). At the least, lefties are definitely better at visualizing three-dimensional objects. This may be why there are more lefthanded architects, artists, and chess players than would be expected (Coren, 1992). Similarly, being right-handed does not guarantee sports superiority, as you can see in Table 2.1. By the way, chalk up a few more for the lefthanders, who have also done well in handball (Dane & Erzurumluoglu, 2003) and professional tennis (Holtzen, 2000). Lateralization refers to specialization in the abilities of the brain hemispheres. One striking feature of lefties is that they are gen- • Table 2.1 • Sports and Handedness Sport Handedness Advantage Baseball No overall left or right advantage Boxing Left Fencing Left Basketball Mixed and ambidextrous Ice hockey Mixed and ambidextrous Field hockey Mixed and ambidextrous Tennis Strong left or strong right Squash Strong left or strong right Badminton Strong left or strong right Left-handers have an advantage in sports such as fencing and boxing. Most likely, their movements are less familiar to opponents, who usually face right-handers (Coren, 1992). Adapted from Coren, 1992. erally less lateralized than the right-handed. In fact, even the physical size and shape of their cerebral hemispheres are more alike. If you are a lefty, you can take pride in the fact that your brain is less lopsided than most! In general, left-handers are more symmetrical on almost everything, including eye dominance, fingerprints — even foot size (Polemikos & Papaeliou, 2000). In some situations, less lateralization may be a real advantage. For instance, individuals who are moderately left-handed or ambidextrous seem to have better than average pitch memory, which is a basic musical skill. Correspondingly, more musicians are ambidextrous than would normally be expected (Springer & Deutsch, 1998). K N O WL E D GE B U I L D E R Handedness and Brain Lateralization RECITE 1. About 97 percent of left-handed people process language on the left side of the brain, the same as right-handed people do. T or F? 2. Left-handed individuals who write with their hand below the writing line are likely to be right-brain dominant. T or F? 3. People basically learn to be right- or left-handed. T or F? 4. In general, left-handed individuals show less lateralization in the brain and throughout the body. T or F? REFLECT Critical Thinking 5. News reports that left-handed people tend to die younger have been flawed in an important way: The average age of people in the lefthanded group was younger than that of subjects in the right-handed group. Why would this make a difference in the conclusions drawn? Math abilities may also benefit from fuller use of the right hemisphere. Students who are extremely gifted in math are much more likely to be left-handed or ambidextrous (Benbow, 1986). Even where ordinary arithmetic skills are concerned, lefties seem to excel (Annett, 2002; Annett & Manning, 1990). The clearest advantage of being lefthanded shows up when there is a brain injury. Because of their milder lateralization, lefthanded individuals typically experience less language loss after damage to either brain hemisphere, and they recover more easily (Geschwind, 1979). Maybe having “two left feet” isn’t so bad after all. Relate Think for a moment about what you “knew” about handedness and lefthanded people before you read this section. Which of your beliefs were correct? How has your knowledge about handedness changed? Answers: 1. F 2. T 3. F 4. T 5. Because we can’t tell if handedness or average age accounts for the difference in death rates. For example, if we start with a group of 20- to 30-year-old people, in which some die, the average age of death has to be between 20 and 30. If we start with a group of 30- to 40-year-old people, in which some die, the average age of death has to be between 30 and 40. Thus, the left-handed group might have an earlier average age at death simply because members of the group were younger to start with. a right-handed world. One study showed that left-handed locomotive engineers have higher accident rates and suggested that the cause was due to the design of locomotive controls (Bhushan & Khan, 2006). If it can be gripped, turned, or pulled, it’s probably designed for the right hand. Even toilet handles are on the right side. On the other hand, the shortage of very old lefties may just reflect the fact that, in the past, more left-handed children were forced to become right-handed. That makes it look like many lefties don’t survive to old age. In reality, they do, but many of them are masquerading as righties (Martin & Freitas, 2002)! 75 Bob Daemmrich/The Image Works Brain and Behavior Lateralization Differences between the two sides of the body, especially differences in the abilities of the brain hemispheres. 76 CHAPTER 2 chapter in review The dendrite and soma of a neuron combine neural input and send it down the axon to the axon terminals for output across the synapse to other neurons. • The firing of an action potential (nerve impulse) is basically an electrical event. • Communication between neurons is chemical: Neurotransmitters cross the synapse, attach to receptor sites, and excite or inhibit the receiving cell. • Chemicals called neuropeptides regulate activity in the brain. • All behavior can be traced to networks of neurons. • The brain’s circuitry is not static. The brain can “rewire” itself and even grow new nerve cells in response to changing environmental conditions. The nervous system can be divided into the central nervous system (CNS) and the peripheral nervous system (PNS), which includes the somatic (bodily) and autonomic (involuntary) nervous systems. • The brain carries out most of the “computing” in the nervous system. • The spinal cord connects the brain to the peripheral nervous system and can process simple reflex arcs. • The peripheral nervous system carries sensory information to the brain and motor commands to the body. • “Vegetative” and automatic bodily processes are controlled by the autonomic nervous system, which has a sympathetic branch and a parasympathetic branch. A major brain research strategy involves the localization of function to link specific structures in the brain with specific psychological or behavioral functions. • Biopsychologists study how processes in the body, brain, and nervous system relate to behavior. • To map the brain, researchers activate or disable specific areas and observe changes in behavior. • Brain structure is investigated though dissection and less intrusive CT scans and MRI scans. • Brain function is investigated through clinical case studies, electrical stimulation, ablation, deep lesioning, electrical recording, and microelectrode recording, as well as less intrusive EEG recordings, PET scans, and fMRI scans. The human cerebral cortex is largely responsible for our ability to use language, make tools, acquire complex skills, and live in complex social groups. The cerebral cortex is divided into left and right hemispheres connected by the corpus callosum. Each hemisphere is divided into four lobes: the frontal lobes, the parietal lobes, the temporal lobes, and the occipital lobes. • The human brain is marked by advanced corticalization, or enlargement of the cerebral cortex. Gateways to Brain and Behavior • The left cerebral hemisphere contains speech or language “centers” in most people. It also specializes in writing, calculating, judging time and rhythm, and ordering complex movements. • The right hemisphere is largely nonverbal. It excels at spatial and perceptual skills, visualization, and recognition of patterns, faces, and melodies. • The left hemisphere is good at analysis and it processes small details sequentially. The right hemisphere detects overall patterns; it processes information simultaneously and holistically. • “Split brains” can be created by cutting the corpus callosum. The split-brain individual shows a remarkable degree of independence between the right and left hemispheres. • The most basic functions of the lobes of the cerebral cortex are as follows: frontal lobes — motor control, speech, abstract thought, and sense of self; parietal lobes — bodily sensation; temporal lobes — hearing and language; occipital lobes — vision. Damage to any of these areas will impair the named functions. • Primary sensory and motor areas are found on the lobes of the cerebral cortex. • Association areas on the cortex are neither sensory nor motor in function. They are related to more complex skills such as language, memory, recognition, and problem solving. • Damage to either Broca’s area or Wernicke’s area causes speech and language problems known as aphasias. The brain can be subdivided into the forebrain, midbrain, and hindbrain. The subcortex includes hindbrain and midbrain brain structures as well as the lower parts of the forebrain, below the cortex. • The medulla contains centers essential for reflex control of heart rate, breathing, and other “vegetative” functions. • The cerebellum maintains coordination, posture, and muscle tone. • The reticular formation directs sensory and motor messages, and part of it, known as the RAS, acts as an activating system for the cerebral cortex. • The thalamus carries sensory information to the cortex. • The hypothalamus exerts powerful control over eating, drinking, sleep cycles, body temperature, and other basic motives and behaviors. • The limbic system is strongly related to emotion. It also contains distinct reward and punishment areas and an area known as the hippocampus that is important for forming memories. Brain and Behavior Endocrine glands serve as a chemical communication system within the body. The ebb and flow of hormones from the endocrine glands entering the bloodstream affect behavior, moods, and personality. • Many of the endocrine glands are influenced by the pituitary (the “master gland”), which is in turn influenced by the hypothalamus. Thus, the brain controls the body through both the fast nervous system and the slower endocrine system. The vast majority of people are right-handed and therefore left-brain dominant for motor skills. More than 90 percent of right-handed persons and about 70 percent of the left-handed also produce speech from the left hemisphere. • Brain dominance and brain activity determine if you are righthanded, left-handed, or ambidextrous. • Most people are strongly right-handed. A minority are strongly left-handed. A few have moderate or mixed hand preferences or they are ambidextrous. Thus, handedness is not a simple either/or trait. • Left-handed people tend to be less strongly lateralized than right-handed people (their brain hemispheres are not as specialized). 77 Probe the Brain Explore the motor homunculus of the brain interactively. Brain Maps Information on the functions of a healthy cortex and also some effects of brain injury. The Patient’s Journey: Living With Locked-In Syndrome Meet Nick, who lives with locked-in syndrome after suffering brainstem damage. Endo 101: The Endocrine System A description of the endocrine system and hormones. Anabolic Steroid Abuse An article on steroids and steroid abuse from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. What Is “Handedness”? Information about handedness and brain laterality, including a list of famous left handers. The Sinister Hand Interview on handedness with Dr. Michael Corballis. Left Brain, Right Brain Article on popular conceptions of the differences between brain hemispheres. Interactive Learning Web Resources For an up-to-date list of direct links to interesting sites, including those listed here, visit the student companion site for this book at www.cengage.com/psychology/coon Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior Book Companion Website www.cengage.com/psychology/coon Visit your book companion website, where you will find flash cards, practice quizzes, “Web Links,” and more to help you study. Neural Transmission View a series of animations about neural transmission illustrating the action potential and the resting potential. Synaptic Transmission Read more detail about synaptic transmission or view an animation. The Nervous System Explore an overview of the major divisions of the nervous system. The Whole Brain Atlas View images of various parts of the brain. The PET Scan Read more about PET scans. fMRI for Newbies Learn about fMRI; includes fMRI images. Split Brain Consciousness Explore the cerebral hemispheres and what happens when they are split. Just what you need to know NOW! Spend time on what you need to master rather than on information you already have learned. Take a pre-test for this chapter, and CengageNOW will generate a personalized study plan based on your results. The study plan will identify the topics you need to review and direct you to online resources to help you master those topics. You can then take a post-test to help you determine the concepts you have mastered and what you will need to work on. Try it out! Go to www.cengage.com/login to sign in with an access code or to purchase access to this product. CHAPTER 3 Human Development Gateway Theme The principles of development help us better understand not only children, but our own behavior as well. Paul Kuroda/Superstock Gateway Questions • How do heredity and environment affect development? • What can newborn babies do? • Of what significance is a child’s emotional bond with adults? • How important are parenting styles? • How do children acquire language? • How do children learn to think? • Why is the transition from adolescence to adulthood especially challenging? 78 • • How do we develop morals and values? • • • What is involved in well-being during later adulthood? What are the typical tasks and dilemmas through the life span? How do people typically react to death? How do effective parents discipline and communicate with their children? preview It’s a Girl! With those words, Olivia catches her first glimpse of Samantha, her tiny newborn baby. Frankly, at the moment Samantha looks something like a prune, with pudgy arms, stubby legs, and lots of wrinkles. Yet she looks so perfect—at least in her parents’ eyes. As Olivia and her husband Tom look at Samantha they wonder, “How will her life unfold? What kind of a person will she be?”“Will Samantha be a happy teenager?” “Will she marry, become a mother, find an interesting career?” Tom and Olivia can only hope that by the time Samantha is 83, she will have lived a full and satisfying life. What if we could skip ahead through Samantha’s life and observe her at various ages? What could we learn? Seeing the world through her eyes would be fascinating and instructive. For example, a child’s viewpoint can make us more aware of things we take for granted. Younger children, in particular, are very literal in their use of language. When Samantha was 3, she thought her bath was too hot and said to Tom, “Make it warmer, daddy.” At first, Tom was confused. The bath was already fairly hot. But then he realized that what she really meant was, “Bring the water closer to the temperature we call warm.” It makes perfect sense if you look at it that way. Research done by developmental psychologists tells a fascinating story about human growth and development. Let’s let Olivia, Tom, and Samantha represent parents and children everywhere, as we see what psychology can tell us about the challenges of growing up, maturing, aging, and facing death. Tracing Samantha’s development might even help you answer two very important questions: How did I become the person I am today? and Who will I become tomorrow? Gateway Question: How do heredity and environment affect development? When we think of development we naturally think of children “growing up” into adults. But even as adults we never really stop changing. Developmental psychology, the study of progressive changes in behavior and abilities, involves every stage of life from conception to death (or “the womb to the tomb”). Heredity and environment also affect us throughout life. Some events, such as when Samantha achieves sexual maturity, are mostly governed by heredity. Others, such as when Samantha learns to swim, read, or drive a car, are primarily a matter of environment. But which is more important, heredity or environment? Actually, neither. Biopsychologist Donald Hebb once offered a useful analogy: To define the area of a rectangle, what is more important, height or width? Of course, both dimensions are absolutely essential. If either is reduced to zero, there is no rectangle. Similarly, if Samantha grows up to become a prominent civil rights lawyer, her success will be due to both heredity and environment. While heredity gives each of us a variety of potentials and limitations, these are, in turn, affected by environmental influences, such as learning, nutrition, disease, and culture. Ultimately, the person you are today reflects a continuous interaction, or interplay, between the forces of nature and nurture (Kalat, 2007). Let’s look in more detail at this dance. Heredity Heredity (“nature”) refers to the genetic transmission of physical and psychological characteristics from parents to their children. An incredible number of personal features are set at conception, when a sperm and an ovum (egg) unite. Myrleen Ferguson Cate/PhotoEdit Nature and Nurture — It Takes Two to Tango Identical twins. Twins who share identical genes (identical twins) demonstrate the powerful influence of heredity. Even when they are reared apart, identical twins are strikingly alike in motor skills, physical development, and appearance. At the same time, twins are less alike as adults than they were as children, which shows environmental influences are at work (Larsson, Larsson, & Lichtenstein, 2004). How does heredity operate? The nucleus of every human cell contains DNA, deoxyribonucleic acid (dee-OX-see-RYE-bonew-KLEE-ik). DNA is a long, ladder-like chain of pairs of chemical molecules ( Figure 3.1). The order of these molecules, or organic bases, acts as a code for genetic information. The DNA in • Developmental psychology The study of progressive changes in behavior and abilities from conception to death. Heredity (“nature”) The transmission of physical and psychological characteristics from parents to offspring through genes. DNA Deoxyribonucleic acid, a molecular structure that contains coded genetic information. 79 80 CHAPTER 3 Figure 3.1 (Top left) Linked mol•ecules (organic bases) make up the “rungs” Sugar-phosphate backbone Organic bases Cell Nucleus Chromosome Biophoto Associates/Science-Source/ Photo Researchers, Inc. • Figure 3.2 This image, made with a scanning electron microscope, shows •several pairs of human chromosomes. (Colors are artificial.) is 1 chance in 4 that their children will get two blue-eye genes and have blue eyes ( Figure 3.3). In actuality, few of our characteristics are controlled by single genes. Instead, most are polygenic (pol-ih-JEN-ik), or controlled by many genes working in combination. Through the expression of genes, heredity determines eye color, skin color, and susceptibility to some diseases. Also, genes can switch on (or off ) at certain ages or developmental stages. In this way, heredity continues to exert a powerful influence throughout maturation, the physical growth and development of the body, brain, and nervous system. As the human growth sequence unfolds, genetic instructions influence body size and shape, height, intelligence, athletic potential, personality traits, sexual orientation, and a host of other details (Cummings, 2006) ( Table 3.1). • • Brown-eyed mother Mother's genes Father's genes each cell contains a record of all the instructions needed to make a human — with room left over to spare. In 2003, a major scientific milestone was reached when the Human Genome Project completed sequencing all 3 billion chemical base pairs in human DNA (U.S. Department of Energy Office of Science, 2005). Human DNA is organized into 46 chromosomes. (The word chromosome means “colored body.”) These thread-like structures hold the coded instructions of heredity ( Figure 3.2). A notable exception is sperm cells and ova, which contain only 23 chromosomes. Thus, Samantha received 23 chromosomes from Olivia and 23 from Tom. This is her genetic heritage. Genes are small areas of DNA that affect a particular process or personal characteristic. Sometimes, a single gene is responsible for an inherited feature, such as Samantha’s eye color. Genes may be dominant or recessive. When a gene is dominant, the feature it controls will appear every time the gene is present. When a gene is recessive, it must be paired with a second recessive gene before its effect will be expressed. For example, if Samantha got a blue-eye gene from Tom and a brown-eye gene from Olivia, Samantha will be brown-eyed, because brown-eye genes are dominant. If brown-eye genes are dominant, why do two brown-eyed parents sometimes have a blue-eyed child? If one or both parents have two brown-eye genes, the couple’s children can only be brown-eyed. But what if each parent has one brown-eye gene and one blue-eye gene? In that case, both parents would have brown eyes. Yet there Brown-eyed father on DNA’s twisted “molecular ladder.” The order of these molecules serves as a code for genetic information. The code provides a genetic blueprint that is unique for each individual (except identical twins). The drawing shows only a small section of a DNA strand. An entire strand of DNA is composed of billions of smaller molecules. (Bottom left) The nucleus of each cell in the body contains chromosomes made up of tightly wound coils of DNA. (Don’t be misled by the drawing: Chromosomes are microscopic, and the chemical molecules that make up DNA are even smaller.) DNA Brown-eyed child Brown-eyed child Blue-eyed child Brown-eyed child Figure 3.3 Gene patterns for children of brown-eyed parents, where each •parent has one brown-eye gene and one blue-eye gene. Because the brown-eye gene is dominant, 1 child in 4 will be blue-eyed. Thus, there is a significant chance that two brown-eyed parents will have a blue-eyed child. Human Development Table 3.1 81 • Human Growth Sequence Period Duration Descriptive Name Prenatal period Germinal period Embryonic period Fetal period From conception to birth First 2 weeks after conception 2–8 weeks after conception From 8 weeks after conception to birth Zygote Embryo Fetus Neonate Neonatal period Infancy From birth to a few weeks after birth From a few weeks after birth until child is walking securely; some children walk securely at less than a year, while others may not be able to until age 17–18 months Infant Early childhood From about 15–18 months until about 2–2½ years From age 2–3 to about age 6 Toddler Preschool child Middle childhood From about age 6 to age 12 School-age child Pubescence Period of about 2 years before puberty Point of development at which biological changes of pubescence reach a climax marked by sexual maturity From the beginning of pubescence until full social maturity is reached (difficult to fix duration of this period) Adolescent Adulthood Young adulthood (19–25) Adulthood (26–40) Maturity (41 plus) From adolescence to death; sometimes subdivided into other periods as shown at left Adult Senescence No defined limit that would apply to all people; extremely variable; characterized by marked physiological and psychological deterioration Adult (senile), “old age” Puberty Adolescence *Note: There is no exact beginning or ending point for various growth periods. The ages are approximate, and each period may be thought of as blending into the next. Table courtesy of Tom Bond. Readiness At what ages will Samantha be ready to feed herself, to walk alone, or to say goodbye to diapers? Such milestones tend to be governed by a child’s readiness for rapid learning. That is, minimum levels of maturation must occur before some skills can be learned. Parents are asking for failure when they try to force a child to learn skills too early (Schum et al., 2002). It is impossible, for instance, to teach children to walk or use a toilet before they have matured enough to control their bodies. Consider the eager parents who toilet trained an 18-month-old child in 10 trying weeks of false alarms and “accidents.” If they had waited until the child was 24 months old, they might have succeeded in just 3 weeks. Parents may control when toilet training starts, but maturation tends to dictate when it will be completed (Schum et al., 2002). The average age for completed toilet training is about 3 years (girls a little earlier, boys a little later) (Schum et al., 2001). So why fight nature? (The wet look is in.) connections disappear. As a result, early learning environments literally shape the developing brain, through “blooming and pruning” of synapses (Nelson, 1999). Although human culture is accelerating the rate at which human DNA is evolving, modern humans are still genetically quite similar to cave dwellers who lived 30,000 years ago (Hawks et al., 2007). Nevertheless, a bright baby born today could learn to become almost anything — a ballet dancer, an engineer, a gangsta rapper, or a biochemist who likes to paint in watercolors. But an Chromosomes Thread-like “colored bodies” in the nucleus of each cell that are made up of DNA. Genes Specific areas on a strand of DNA that carry hereditary information. Dominant gene A gene whose influence will be expressed each time the gene is present. Recessive gene A gene whose influence will be expressed only when it is paired with a second recessive gene. Environment Our environment also exerts a profound influence on our development. Environment (“nurture”) refers to the sum of all external conditions that affect a person. For example, the brain of a newborn baby has fewer dendrites (nerve cell branches) and synapses (connections between nerve cells) than an adult brain ( Figure 3.4). However, the newborn brain is highly plastic (capable of being altered by experience). During the first 3 years of life, millions of new connections form in the brain every day. At the same time, unused • Polygenic characteristics Personal traits or physical properties that are influenced by many genes working in combination. Maturation The physical growth and development of the body and nervous system. Readiness A condition that exists when maturation has advanced enough to allow the rapid acquisition of a particular skill. Environment (“nurture”) The sum of all external conditions affecting development, including especially the effects of learning. • CHAPTER 3 Neonate Six months Two years Figure 3.4 A rapid increase in brain synapses continues until about age 4. At that point, children actually have more brain synapses than adults do. Then, after age 10, the number slowly declines, reaching adult levels at about age 16. (Reprinted by permission of the publisher from The Postnatal Development of the Human Cerebral Cortex, Vols. I–III by Jesse LeRoy Conel, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, Copyright © 1939, 1975 by the President and Fellows of Harvard College.) Upper Paleolithic baby could have only become a hunter or food gatherer. Prenatal Influences Petit Format/Photo Researchers, Inc. Environmental factors actually start influencing development before birth. Although the intrauterine environment (interior of the womb) is highly protected, environmental conditions can nevertheless affect the developing child. For example, when Olivia was pregnant, Samantha’s fetal heart rate and movements increased when loud sounds or vibrations penetrated the womb (Kisilevsky et al., 2004). If Olivia’s health or nutrition had been poor, or if she had German measles, syphilis, or HIV; or used drugs; or was exposed to X-rays or radiation, Samantha’s growth sequence might have been harmed. In such cases babies can suffer from congenital problems, or “birth defects.” These environmental problems affect the Because of the rapid growth of basic structures, the developing fetus is sensitive to a variety of diseases, drugs, and sources of radiation. This is especially true during the first trimester (3 months) of gestation (pregnancy). developing fetus and become apparent at birth. In contrast, genetic disorders are inherited from parents. Examples are sickle-cell anemia, hemophilia, cystic fibrosis, muscular dystrophy, albinism, and some types of mental retardation. How is it possible for the embryo or the fetus to be harmed? No direct intermixing of blood takes place between a mother and her unborn child. Yet some substances — especially drugs — do reach the fetus. Anything capable of directly causing birth defects is called a teratogen (teh-RAT-uh-jen). Sometimes women are exposed to powerful teratogens, such as radiation, lead, pesticides, or polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs), without knowing it. But pregnant women do have direct control over many teratogens. For example, a woman who takes cocaine runs a serious risk of injuring her fetus (Schuetze & Eiden, 2006). In short, when a pregnant woman takes drugs, her unborn child does too. Unfortunately, in the United States this is one of the greatest risk factors facing unborn children (Coles & Black, 2006). If a mother is addicted to morphine, heroin, or methadone, her baby may be born with an addiction. Repeated heavy drinking during pregnancy causes fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS). Affected infants have low birth weight, a small head, bodily defects, and facial malformations. Many also suffer from emotional, behavioral, and mental handicaps (Golden, 2005). Tobacco is also harmful. Smoking during pregnancy greatly reduces oxygen to the fetus. Heavy smokers risk miscarrying or having premature, underweight babies who are more likely to die soon after birth. Children of smoking mothers score lower on tests of language and mental ability (Huijbregts et al., 2006). In other words, an unborn child’s future can go “up in smoke.” That goes for marijuana as well (Viveros et al., 2005). Ted Wood 82 Some of the typical features of children suffering from fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS) include a small nonsymmetrical head, a short nose, a flattened area between the eyes, oddly shaped eyes, and a thin upper lip. Many of these features become less noticeable by adolescence. However, mental retardation and other problems commonly follow the FAS child into adulthood. The child shown here represents a moderate example of FAS. Human Development 83 Early experiences can have particularly lasting effects. For example, children who are abused may suffer lifelong emotional problems (Goodwin, Fergusson, & Horwood, 2005). At the same time, extra care can sometimes reverse the effects of a poor start in life (Bornstein & Tamis-LeMonda, 2001). In short, environmental forces guide human development, for better or worse, throughout life. Why do some experiences have more lasting effects than others? Part of the answer lies in sensitive periods. These are times when children are more susceptible to particular types of environmental influences. Events that occur during a sensitive period can permanently alter the course of development (Bruer, 2001). For example, forming a loving bond with a caregiver early in life seems to be crucial for optimal development. Likewise, babies who don’t hear normal speech during their first year may have impaired language abilities (Thompson & Nelson, 2001). Allen Russell/Photolibrary Sensitive Periods Children who grow up in poverty run a high risk of experiencing many forms of deprivation. There is evidence that lasting damage to social, emotional, and cognitive development occurs when children must cope with severe early deprivation. Deprivation and Enrichment Some environments can be described as enriched or deprived. Deprivation refers to a lack of normal nutrition, stimulation, comfort, or love. Enrichment exists when an environment is deliberately made more stimulating, loving, and so forth. What happens when children suffer severe deprivation? Tragically, a few mistreated children have spent their first years in closets, attics, and other restricted environments. When first discovered, these children are usually mute, retarded, and emotionally damaged (Wilson, 2003). Fortunately, such extreme deprivation is unusual. Nevertheless, milder perceptual, intellectual, or emotional deprivation occurs in many families, especially those that must cope with poverty. Poverty can effect the development of children in at least two ways (Sobolewski & Amato, 2005). First, poor parents may not be able to give their children needed resources such as nutritious meals, health care, or learning materials (Bradley & Corwyn, 2002). As a result, impoverished children tend to be sick more often, their cognitive development lags, and they do poorly at school. Second, the stresses of poverty can also be hard on parents, leading to marriage problems, less positive parenting, and poorer parent-child relationships. The resulting emotional turmoil can damage a child’s socioemotional development. In the extreme, it may increase the risk of mental illness and delinquent behavior (Bradley & Corwyn, 2002). bridges Adults experience a number of disruptive effects when they are deprived of perceptual stimulation. See Chapter 6, page 198, for details. Adults who grew up in poverty often remain trapped in a vicious cycle of continued poverty. Because one in seven American families fall below the poverty line, this grim reality plays itself out in millions of American homes every day (Sobolewski & Amato, 2005). Can an improved environment enhance development? To answer this question, psychologists have created enriched environments that are especially novel, complex, and stimulating. Enriched environments may be the “soil” from which brighter children grow. To illustrate, let’s consider the effects of raising rats in a sort of “rat wonderland.” The walls of their cages were decorated with colorful patterns, and each cage was filled with platforms, ladders, and cubbyholes. As adults, these rats were superior at learning mazes. In addition, they had larger, heavier brains, with a thicker cortex (Benloucif, Bennett, & Rosenzweig, 1995). Of course, it’s a long leap from rats to people, but an actual increase in brain size is impressive. If extra stimulation can enhance the “intelligence” of a lowly rat, it’s likely that human infants also benefit from enrichment. Many studies have shown that enriched environments improve abilities or enhance development. It would be wise for Tom and Olivia to make a point of nourishing Samantha’s mind, as well as her body (Beeber et al., 2007). What can parents do to enrich a child’s environment? They can encourage exploration and stimulating play by paying attention to what holds the baby’s interest. It is better to “child-proof ” a house Congenital problems Problems or defects that originate during prenatal development in the womb. Genetic disorders Problems caused by defects in the genes or by inherited characteristics. Teratogen Radiation, a drug, or other substance capable of altering fetal development in nonheritable ways that cause birth defects. Sensitive period During development, a period of increased sensitivity to environmental influences. Also, a time during which certain events must take place for normal development to occur. Deprivation In development, the loss or withholding of normal stimulation, nutrition, comfort, love, and so forth; a condition of lacking. Enrichment In development, deliberately making an environment more stimulating, nutritional, comforting, loving, and so forth. 84 CHAPTER 3 than to strictly limit what a child can touch. There is also value in actively enriching sensory experiences. Infants are not vegetables. Babies should be surrounded by colors, music, people, and things to see, taste, smell, and touch. It makes perfect sense to take them outside, to hang mobiles over their cribs, to place mirrors nearby, to play music for them, or to rearrange their rooms now and then. Children progress most rapidly when they have responsive parents and stimulating play materials at home (Beeber et al., 2007). In light of this, it is wise to view all of childhood as a relatively sensitive period (Nelson, 1999). Reaction Range One way to visualize the interplay of heredity and environment is through the concept of reaction range, the limits that one’s environment places on the effects of heredity ( Figure 3.5). Let’s suppose that Samantha was born with genes for a normal level of musical ability. If Samantha grows up in a musically deprived environment, she might well end up with lower than average adult musical ability. If Olivia and Tom provide her with an enriched environment, she will probably have normal, or even above normal, musical ability. While Samantha might not be genetically capable of becoming a child prodigy, the environment her parents provide for her will also determine her developmental path. • Reciprocal Influences Nurture often affects the expression of hereditary tendencies through ongoing reciprocal influences. A good example of such influences is the fact that growing infants influence their parents’ behavior at the same time they are changed by it. Newborn babies differ noticeably in temperament. This is the inherited, physical core of personality. It includes sensitivity, irritability, distractibility, and typical mood (Kagan, 2004). About 40 percent of all newborns are easy children who are relaxed and agreeable. Ten percent are difficult children who are moody, intense, and easily angered. Slow-to-warm-up children (about 15 percent) are restrained, unexpressive, or shy. The remaining children do not fit neatly into a single category (Chess & Thomas, 1986). Because of differences in temperament, some babies are more likely than others to smile, cry, vocalize, reach out, or pay attention. As a result, babies rapidly become active participants in their own development. For example, Samantha is an easy baby who smiles frequently and is easily fed. This encourages Olivia to touch, feed, and sing to Samantha. Olivia’s affection rewards Samantha, causing her to smile more. Soon, a dynamic relationship blossoms between mother and child. Similarly, good parenting can reciprocally influence a very shy child who, in turn, becomes progressively less shy. The reverse also occurs: Difficult children make parents unhappy and elicit more negative parenting (Parke, 2004). Alternately, negative parenting can turn a moderately shy child into a very shy one. This suggests that inherited temperaments are dynamically modified by learning (Kagan, 2005). A person’s developmental level is his or her current state of physical, emotional, and intellectual development. To summarize, three factors combine to determine your developmental level at any stage of life. These are heredity, environment, and your own behavior, each tightly interwoven with the others. KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R High The Interplay of Heredity and Environment Reaction Range Average Low IQ Score RECITE Deprived Average Enriched Environment Figure 3.5 The effect of heredity on the development of human traits, such •as intelligence, can often be influenced by environmental circumstances. A child with average genes for intelligence growing up in an average environment might have an average IQ score as an adult. However, growing up in a deprived environment might result in a somewhat lower IQ score and growing up in an enriched environment might result in a somewhat higher IQ score. In this way, the environment sets a range within which our hereditary potential is actually expressed. This range is called the reaction range. 1. Areas of the DNA molecule called genes are made up of dominant and recessive chromosomes. T or F? 2. Most inherited characteristics can be described as polygenic. T or F? 3. If one parent has a one dominant brown-eye gene and one recessive blue-eye gene and the other parent has two dominant brown-eye genes, what is the chance that their child will have blue eyes? a. 25 percent b. 50 percent c. 0 percent d. 75 percent 4. The orderly sequence observed in the unfolding of many basic responses can be attributed to ___________________________. 5. “Slow-to-warm-up” children can be described as restrained, unexpressive, or shy. T or F? 6. A ________________________ __________________ is a time of increased sensitivity to environmental influences. 7. As a child develops there is a continuous ________________________ between the forces of heredity and environment. REFLECT Critical Thinking 8. Environmental influences can interact with hereditary programming in an exceedingly direct way. Can you guess what it is? Continued Human Development avoid falling. You can observe the rooting reflex (reflexive head turning and nursing) by touching Samantha’s cheek. Immediately, she will turn toward your finger, as if searching for something. How is such turning adaptive? The rooting reflex helps infants find a bottle or a breast. Then, when a nipple touches the infant’s mouth, the sucking reflex (rhythmic nursing) helps her obtain needed food. Like other reflexes, this is a genetically programmed action. At the same time, food rewards nursing. Because of this, babies quickly learn to nurse more actively. Again, we see how the interplay of nature and nurture alters a baby’s behavior. The Moro reflex is also interesting. If Samantha’s position is changed abruptly or if she is startled by a loud noise, she will make a hugging motion. This reaction has been compared to the movements baby monkeys use to cling to their mothers. (We leave it to the reader’s imagination to decide if there is any connection.) Relate Can you think of clear examples of some ways in which heredity and environmental forces have combined to affect your development? How would maturation affect the chances of teaching an infant to eat with a spoon? What kind of temperament did you have as an infant? How did it affect your relationship with your parents or caregivers? Answers: 1. F 2. T 3. c 4. maturation 5. T 6. sensitive period 7. interaction 8. Environmental conditions sometimes turn specific genes on or off, thus directly affecting the expression of genetic tendencies (Gottlieb, 1998). The Newborn — More Than Meets the Eye Gateway Question: What can newborn babies do? At birth the human neonate (NEE-oh-NATE: newborn infant) will die if not cared for by adults. Newborn babies cannot lift their heads, turn over, or feed themselves. Does this mean they are inert and unfeeling? Definitely not! Contrary to common belief, newborn babies are not oblivious to their surroundings. Infants have mental capacities that continue to surprise researchers and delight parents. The emergence of many of these capacities, as well as a baby’s physical abilities, is closely related to maturation of the brain, nervous system, and body. Likewise, a baby’s early emotional life unfolds on a timetable that is largely controlled by maturation. Neonates like Samantha can see, hear, smell, taste, and respond to pain and touch. Although their senses are less acute, babies are very responsive. Samantha will follow a moving object with her eyes and will turn in the direction of sounds. Samantha also has a number of adaptive infant reflexes (Siegler, DeLoache, & Eisenberg, 2006). To elicit the grasping reflex, press an object in the neonate’s palm and she will grasp it with surprising strength. Many infants, in fact, can hang from a raised bar, like little trapeze artists. The grasping reflex aids survival by helping infants Perceptual and Cognitive Development Thirty years ago, many people thought of newborn babies as mere bundles of reflexes, like the ones just described. But infants are capable of much more. For example, psychologist Andrew Meltzoff has found that babies are born mimics (Meltzoff, 2005). Figure 3.6 shows Meltzoff as he sticks out his tongue, opens his mouth, and purses his lips at a 20-day-old girl. Will she imitate him? Videotapes of babies confirm that they imitate adult facial gestures. As early as 9 months of age, infants can imitate actions a day after seeing them (Heimann & Meltzoff, 1996). Such mimicry obviously aids rapid learning in infancy. How intelligent are neonates? Babies are smarter than many people think. From the earliest days of life, babies are learning how the world works. They immediately begin to look, touch, taste, and otherwise explore their surroundings. From an evolutionary perspective, a baby’s mind is designed to soak up information, which it does at an amazing pace (Meltzoff & Prinz, 2002). In the first months of life, babies are increasingly able to think, to learn from what they see, to make predictions, and to search for explanations. For example, Jerome Bruner (1983) observed that 3- to 8-week-old babies seem to understand that a person’s voice and body should be connected. If a baby hears his mother’s voice coming from where she is standing, the baby will remain calm. If her voice comes from a loudspeaker several feet away, the baby will become agitated and begin to cry. Another look into the private world of infants can be drawn from testing their vision. However, such testing is a challenge because infants cannot talk. Robert Fantz invented a device called a looking chamber to find out what infants can see and what holds their attention ( Figure 3.7a). Imagine that Samantha is placed • Blend Images/SuperStock • Newborn babies display a special interest in the human face. A preference for seeing their mother’s face develops rapidly and encourages social interactions between mother and baby. 85 Reaction range The limits environment places on the effects of heredity. Temperament The physical core of personality, including emotional and perceptual sensitivity, energy levels, typical mood, and so forth. Developmental level An individual’s current state of physical, emotional, and intellectual development. 86 CHAPTER 3 From A. N. Meltzoff & M. K. Moore, “Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates,” Science, 1977, 198, 75–78. Figure 3.6 Infant imitation. In the top row of photos, •Andrew Meltzoff makes facial gestures at an infant. The bottom © Rubberball Productions/Getty Images row records the infant’s responses. Videotapes of Meltzoff and of tested infants helped ensure objectivity. (c) • (b) (a) (d) Figure 3.7 (a) Eye movements and fixation points of infants are observed in Fantz’s “looking chamber.” (b) Thirteen-week-old infants prefer concentric and curved patterns like those on the left to nonconcentric and straight-line patterns like those on the right. (c) When they are just days old, infants pay more attention to the faces of people who are gazing directly at them. (d) Infants look at normal faces longer than at scrambled faces and at both faces longer than designs, like the one on the right. (Photo a courtesy of David Linton. Drawing from “The Origin of Form Perception” by Robert L. Fantz, Copyright © 1961 by Scientific American, Inc. All rights reserved.) on her back inside the chamber, facing a lighted area above. Next, two objects are placed in the chamber. By observing the movements of Samantha’s eyes and the images they reflect, we can tell what she is looking at. Such tests show that adult vision is about 30 times sharper, but babies can see large patterns, shapes, and edges. Fantz found that 3-day-old babies prefer complex patterns, such as checkerboards and bull’s-eyes, to simpler colored rectangles. Other researchers have learned that infants are excited by circles, curves, and bright lights ( Figure 3.7b) (Brown, 1990). Immediately after birth, Samantha will be aware of changes in the position • of objects (Slater et al., 1991). When she is 6 months old, she will be able to recognize categories of objects that differ in shape or color. By 9 months of age she will be able to tell the difference between dogs and birds or other groups of animals (Mandler & McDonough, 1998). By 1 year of age, she will see as well as her parents (Sigelman & Rider, 2006). So, there really is a person inside that little body! Neonates can most clearly see objects about a foot away from them. It is as if they are best prepared to see the people who love and care for them (Gopnik, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 2000). Perhaps that’s why babies have a special fascination with human faces. Just Human Development 1. Fetal posture (newborn) 2. Holds chin up (1 month) 6. Stands holding furniture (9 months) 3. Holds chest up (2 months) 7. Crawls (10 months) • 8. Walks if led (11 months) 4. Sits when supported (4 months) 9. Stands alone (11 months) 87 5. Sits alone (7 months) 10. Walks alone (12 months) Figure 3.8 Motor development. Most infants follow an orderly pattern of motor development. Although the order in which children progress is similar, there are large individual differences in the ages at which each ability appears. The ages listed are averages for American children. It is not unusual for many of the skills to appear 1 or 2 months earlier than average or several months later (Harris & Liebert, 1991). Parents should not be alarmed if a child’s behavior differs some from the average. hours after they are born, babies begin to prefer seeing their mother’s face, rather than a stranger’s (Walton, Bower, & Bower, 1992). When babies are only 2 to 5 days old, they will pay more attention to a person who is gazing directly at them, rather than one who is looking away (Farroni et al., 2004) ( Figure 3.7c). In a looking chamber, most infants will spend more time looking at a human face pattern than a scrambled face or a colored oval ( Figure 3.7d). When real human faces are used, infants prefer familiar faces to unfamiliar faces. However, this reverses at about age 2. At that time, unusual objects begin to interest the child. For instance, in a classic study Jerome Kagan (1971) showed face masks to 2-year-olds. Kagan found that the toddlers were fascinated by a face with eyes on the chin and a nose in the middle of the forehead. He believes the babies’ interest came from a need to understand why the scrambled face differed from what they had come to expect. Such behavior is further evidence that babies actively try to make sense of their surroundings (Gopnik, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 2000). • • Motor Development As we noted previously, the emergence of many basic abilities is closely tied to maturation, which will be evident, for example, as Samantha learns motor skills, such as crawling and walking. Of course, the rate of maturation varies from child to child. Nevertheless, the order of maturation is almost universal. For instance, Samantha will be able to sit without support from Tom before she has matured enough to crawl. Indeed, infants around the world typically sit before they crawl, crawl before they stand, and stand before they walk ( Figure 3.8). What about my weird cousin Emo who never crawled? Like cousin Emo, a few children substitute rolling, creeping, or shuffling for • crawling. A very few move directly from sitting to standing and walking. Even so, their motor development is orderly. In general, muscular control spreads in a pattern that is cephalocaudal (SEF-eh-lo-KODul: from head to toe) and proximodistal (PROK-seh-moe-DIS-tul: from the center of the body to the extremities). Even if cousin Emo flunked Elementary Crawling, his motor development followed the standard top-down, center-outward pattern (Piek, 2006). Although maturation has a big impact, motor skills don’t simply “emerge.” Samantha must learn to control her actions. When babies are beginning to crawl or walk, they actively try new movements and select those that work. Samantha’s first efforts may be flawed — a wobbly crawl or some shaky first steps. However, with practice, babies “tune” their movements to be smoother and more effective. Such learning is evident from the very first months of life (Piek, 2006; Thelen, 2000; Figure 3.9). • Emotional Development Early emotional development also follows a pattern closely tied to maturation (Panksepp & Pasqualini, 2005). Even the basic emotions of anger, fear, and joy—which appear to be unlearned—take time to develop. General excitement is the only emotion newborn infants clearly express. However, as Tom and Olivia can tell you, a baby’s emotional life blossoms rapidly. One researcher (Bridges, 1932) observed that all the basic human emotions appear before age 2. Bridges found that emotions appear in a consistent order and that the first basic split is between pleasant and unpleasant emotions ( Figure 3.10). Experts do not yet agree on how quickly emotions unfold (Oster, 2005). For example, psychologist Carroll Izard thinks that infants can express several basic emotions as early as 10 weeks of • CHAPTER 3 Michael Newman/PhotoEdit 88 • Figure 3.9 Psychologist Carolyn Rovee-Collier has shown that babies as young as 3 months old can learn to control their movements. In her experiments, babies lie on their backs under a colorful crib mobile. A ribbon is tied around the baby’s ankle and connected to the mobile. Whenever babies spontaneously kick their legs, the mobile jiggles and rattles. Within a few minutes, infants learn to kick faster. Their reward for kicking is a chance to see the mobile move (Hayne & Rovee-Collier, 1995). Affection for children Affection for adults Elation Joy Delight Excitement Distress Anger Jealousy Disgust Fear Birth 3 6 9 12 18 24 Months • Figure 3.10 The traditional view of infancy holds that emotions are rapidly differentiated from an initial capacity for excitement. (After K. M. B. Bridges, 1932. Reprinted by permission of the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.) age. When Izard looks carefully at the faces of babies, he sees abundant signs of emotion ( Figure 3.11). The most common infant expression, he found, is not excitement, but interest — followed by joy, anger, and sadness (Izard et al., 1995). If Izard is right, then emotions are “hardwired” by heredity and related to evolution. Perhaps that’s why smiling is one of a baby’s most common reactions. Smiling probably helps babies survive by inviting parents to care for them (Izard et al., 1995). At first, a baby’s smiling is haphazard. By the age of 8 to 12 months, however, infants smile more frequently when another person is nearby (Jones & Hong, 2001). This social smile is especially rewarding to parents. Infants can even use their social smile to communicate inter- • • Figure 3.11 Infants display many of the same emotional expressions as adults do. Carroll Izard believes such expressions show that distinct emotions appear within the first months of life. Other theorists argue that specific emotions come into focus more gradually, as an infant’s nervous system matures. Either way, parents can expect to see a full range of basic emotions by the end of a baby’s first year. est in an object, like the time Samantha smiled when her mother held up her favorite teddy bear (Venezia et al., 2004). On the other hand, when new parents see and hear a crying baby, they feel annoyed, irritated, disturbed, or unhappy. Babies the world over, it seems, rapidly become capable of letting others know what they like and dislike. (Prove this to yourself sometime by driving a baby buggy.) With dazzling speed, human infants are transformed from helpless babies to independent persons. By her third year, Samantha will have a unique personality and she will be able to stand, walk, talk, and explore. At no other time after birth does development proceed more rapidly. During the same period, Samantha’s relationships with other people will expand as well. Before we explore that topic, here’s a chance to review what you’ve learned. KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R The Neonate and Early Maturation RECITE 1. If an infant is startled, he or she will make movements similar to an embrace. This is known as the a. grasping reflex b. rooting reflex c. Moro reflex d. adaptive reflex 2. During infancy, a capacity for imitating others first becomes evident at about 9 months of age. T or F? Continued Human Development involves becoming aware of oneself as a person. When you look in a mirror, you recognize the image staring back as your own — except, perhaps, early on Monday mornings. Like many such events, initial self-awareness depends on maturation of the nervous system. In a typical test of self-recognition, infants are shown images of themselves on a TV. Most infants have to be 18 months old before they recognize themselves (Nielsen & Dissanayake, 2004). 3. After age 2, infants tested in a looking chamber show a marked preference for familiar faces and simpler designs. T or F? 4. General excitement or interest is the clearest emotional response present in newborn infants, but meaningful expressions of delight and distress appear soon after. T or F? 5. Neonates display a social smile as early as 10 days after birth. T or F? REFLECT Critical Thinking 6. If you were going to test newborn infants to see if they prefer their own mother’s face to that of a stranger, what precautions would you take? Attachment The real core of social development is found in the emotional attachment, or close emotional bond, that babies form with their primary caregivers. To investigate mother–infant relationships, Harry Harlow separated baby rhesus monkeys from their mothers at birth. The real mothers were replaced with surrogate (substitute) mothers. Some were made of cold, unyielding wire. Others were covered with soft terry cloth ( Figure 3.12). When the infants were given a choice between the two mothers, they spent most of their time clinging to the cuddly terry-cloth mother. This was true even when the wire mother held a bottle, making her the source of food. The “love” and attachment displayed toward the cloth replicas was identical to that shown toward natural mothers. For example, when frightened by rubber snakes, wind-up toys, and other “fear stimuli,” the infant monkeys ran to their cloth mothers and clung to them for security. These classic studies suggest that attachment begins with contact comfort, the pleasant, reassuring feeling infants get from touching something soft and warm, especially their mother. Relate What infant reflexes have you observed? Can you give an example of how heredity and environment interact during motor development? To know what a baby is feeling would it be more helpful to be able to detect delight and distress (Bridges) or joy, anger, and sadness (Izard)? • Answers: 1. c 2. F 3. F 4. T 5. F 6. In one study of the preferences of newborns, the hair color and complexion of strangers was matched to that of the mothers. Also, only the mother’s or stranger’s face was visible during testing. And finally, a scent was used to mask olfactory (smell) cues so that an infant’s preference could not be based on the mother’s familiar odor (Bushnell, Sai, & Mullin, 1989). Social Development — Baby, I’m Stuck on You Gateway Question: Of what significance is a child’s emotional bond with adults? Infants begin to develop self-awareness and to become aware of others at about the same time they first form an emotional bond with an adult. Each of these developments is an important step toward entering the social world. Parents are the most important influences in early social development. Later, playing with other children begins to extend a child’s social life beyond the family. Like all humans, babies are social creatures. Their early social development lays a foundation for relationships with parents, siblings, friends, and relatives. A first basic step into the social world monkeys become attached to the cloth “contact-comfort” mother but not to a similar wire mother. This is true even when the wire mother provides food. Contact comfort may also underlie the tendency of children to become attached to inanimate objects, such as blankets or stuffed toys. However, a study of 2- to 3-year-old blanket-attached children found that they were no more insecure than others (Passum, 1987). (So maybe Linus is okay after all.) Social smile Smiling elicited by social stimuli, such as seeing a parent’s face. Social development The development of self-awareness, attachment to parents or caregivers, and relationships with other children and adults. Emotional attachment An especially close emotional bond that infants form with their parents, caregivers, or others. Surrogate mothers A substitute mother (often an inanimate dummy in animal research). Contact comfort A pleasant and reassuring feeling human and animal infants get from touching or clinging to something soft and warm, usually their mother. Harry Harlow, U. of WI Primate Laboratory 3.12 An infant monkey clings •to aFigure cloth-covered surrogate mother. Baby Ursula Markus/Photo Researchers, Inc. A sense of self, or self-awareness, develops at about age 18 months. Before children develop self-awareness, they do not recognize their own image in a mirror. Typically, they think they are looking at another child. Some children hug the child in the mirror or go behind it looking for the child they see there (Lewis, 1995). 89 90 CHAPTER 3 Figure 3.13 In the United •States, about two thirds of all chil- Most parents are familiar with the storm of crying that sometimes occurs when babies are left alone at bedtime. Bedtime distress can be a mild form of separation anxiety. As many parents know, it is often eased by the presence of “security objects,” such as a stuffed animal or favorite blanket (Donate-Bartfield & Passman, 2004). dren from middle-class families are securely attached. About 1 child in 3 is insecurely attached. (Percentages are approximate.) Attachment Category 5% Unclassified 10% Ambivalent (From Kaplan, 1998.) Michael Newman/PhotoEdit 22% Avoidant There is a sensitive period (roughly the first year of life) during which attachment must occur for optimal development. Mothers usually begin to feel attached to their baby before birth. For their part, as babies mature, they become more and more capable of bonding with their mothers. For the first few months, babies respond more or less equally to everyone. By 2 or 3 months, most babies prefer their mothers to strangers. By around 7 months, babies generally become truly attached to their mothers, crawling after her if they can. Shortly thereafter they also begin to form attachments to other people as well, such as father, grandparents, or siblings (Sigelman & Rider, 2006). Returning to Samantha’s story, we find that attachment keeps her close to Olivia, who provides safety, stimulation, and a secure “home base” from which Samantha can go exploring. A direct sign that an emotional bond has formed appears around 8 to 12 months of age. At that time Samantha will display separation anxiety (crying and signs of fear) when she is left alone or with a stranger. Mild separation anxiety is normal. When it is more intense, it may reveal a problem. At some point in their lives, about 1 in 20 children suffer from separation anxiety disorder (Dick-Niederhauser & Silverman, 2006). These children are miserable when they are separated from their parents, whom they cling to or constantly follow. Some fear that they will get lost and never see their parents again. Many refuse to go to school, which can be a serious handicap. Children tend to grow out of the disorder (Kearney et al., 2003), but if separation anxiety is intense or lasts for more than a month, parents should seek professional help for their child (Masi, Mucci, & Millepiedi, 2001). Attachment Quality According to psychologist Mary Ainsworth (1913–1999), the quality of attachment is revealed by how babies act when their mothers return after a brief separation. Infants who are securely attached have a stable and positive emotional bond. They are upset by the mother’s absence and seek to be near her when she returns. Insecureavoidant infants have an anxious emotional bond. They tend to turn away from the mother when she returns. Insecure-ambivalent attachment is also an anxious emotional bond. In this case, babies have mixed feelings: They both seek to be near the returning mother and angrily resist contact with her ( Figure 3.13). • 63% Secure Attachment can have lasting effects. Infants who are securely attached at the age of 1 year show more resiliency, curiosity, problem-solving ability, and social skill in preschool (Collins & Gunnar, 1990). In contrast, attachment failures can be quite damaging. Consider, for example, the plight of children raised in severely overcrowded Romanian orphanages (Wilson, 2003). These children got almost no attention from adults for the first year or two of their lives. Some have now been adopted by American and Canadian families, but many are poorly attached to their new parents. Some, for instance, will wander off with strangers; they are anxious and remote, and they don’t like to be touched or make eye contact with others (O’Conner et al., 2003). In short, for some children, a lack of affectionate care early in life leaves a lasting emotional impact well into adulthood. (See “What’s Your Attachment Style?”) Promoting Secure Attachment The key to secure attachment is a mother who is accepting and sensitive to her baby’s signals and rhythms. Poor attachment occurs when a mother’s actions are inappropriate, inadequate, intrusive, overstimulating, or rejecting. An example is the mother who tries to play with a drowsy infant or who ignores a baby who is looking at her and vocalizing. The link between sensitive caregiving and secure attachment appears to apply to all cultures (Posada et al., 2002). What about attachment to fathers? Fathers of securely attached infants tend to be outgoing, agreeable, and happy in their marriage. In general, a warm family atmosphere — one that includes sensitive mothering and fathering — produces secure children (Belsky, 1996; Gomez & McLaren, 2007). Day Care Does commercial day care interfere with the quality of attachment? It depends on the quality of day care. Overall, high-quality day care does not adversely affect attachment to parents (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 1999). In fact, highquality day care can actually improve children’s social and mental skills (Mercer, 2006). Children in high-quality day care tend to have better relationships with their mothers and fewer behavior problems. They also have better cognitive skills and language abilities (Burchinal et al., 2000; Vandell, 2004). However, all the positive effects just noted are reversed for lowquality day care. Low-quality day care is risky and it may weaken Human Development 91 D ISCOVERI N G P S Y CH OL O G Y What’s Your Attachment Style? Do our first attachments continue to affect us as adults? Some psychologists believe they do, by influencing how we relate to friends and lovers (Bridges, 2003; Sroufe et al., 2005). Read the following statements and see which best describes your adult relationships. SECURE ATTACHMENT STYLE In general, I think most other people are well intentioned and trustworthy. I find it relatively easy to get close to others. I am comfortable relying on others and having others depend on me. I don’t worry much about being abandoned by others. I am comfortable when other people want to get close to me emotionally. AVOIDANT ATTACHMENT STYLE I tend to pull back when things don’t go well in a relationship. I am somewhat skeptical about the idea of true love. I have difficulty trusting my partner in a romantic relationship. Other people tend to be too eager to seek commitment from me. I get a little nervous if anyone gets too close emotionally. AMBIVALENT ATTACHMENT STYLE I have often felt misunderstood and unappreciated in my romantic relationships. My friends and lovers have been somewhat unreliable. I love my romantic partner but I worry that she or he doesn’t really love me. attachment. Poor-quality day care can even create behavior problems that didn’t exist beforehand (Pierrehumbert et al., 2002). Parents are wise to carefully evaluate and monitor the quality of day care their children receive. What should parents look for when they evaluate the quality of child care? Parents seeking quality should insist on at least the following: a small number of children per caregiver; small overall group size (12 to 15); trained caregivers; minimal staff turnover; and stable, consistent care (Howes, 1997). (Also, avoid any childcare center with the words zoo, menagerie, or stockade in its name.) Attachment and Affectional Needs A baby’s affectional needs (needs for love and affection) are every bit as important as more obvious needs for food, water, and physical care. All things considered, creating a bond of trust and affection between the infant and at least one other person is a key event during the first year of life. Parents are sometimes afraid of “spoiling” babies with too much attention, but for the first year or two this is nearly impossible. In fact, a later capacity to experience warm and loving relationships may depend on it. Parental Influences — Life with Mom and Dad Gateway Question: How important are parenting styles? From the first few years of life, when caregivers are the center of a child’s world, through to adulthood, the style and quality of mothering and fathering are very important. I would like to be closer to my romantic partner, but I’m not sure I trust her or him. Do any of the preceding statements sound familiar? If so, they may describe your adult attachment style. Most adults have a secure attachment style that is marked by caring, supportiveness, and understanding. However, it’s not unusual to have an avoidant attachment style that reflects a tendency to resist intimacy and commitment to others (Collins et al., 2002). An ambivalent attachment style is marked by mixed feelings about love and friendship (Tidwell, Reis, & Shaver, 1996). Do you see any similarities between your present relationships and your attachment experiences as a child? Parenting Styles Psychologist Diana Baumrind (1991, 2005) has studied the effects of three major parental styles, which are identifiable patterns of parental caretaking and interaction with children. See if you recognize the styles she describes. Authoritarian parents enforce rigid rules and demand strict obedience to authority. Typically they view children as having few rights but adult-like responsibilities. The child is expected to stay out of trouble and to accept, without question, what parents regard as right or wrong. (“Do it because I say so.”) The children of authoritarian parents are usually obedient and self-controlled. But they also tend to be emotionally stiff, withdrawn, apprehensive, and lacking in curiosity. Separation anxiety Distress displayed by infants when they are separated from their parents or principal caregivers. Secure attachment A stable and positive emotional bond. Insecure-avoidant attachment An anxious emotional bond marked by a tendency to avoid reunion with a parent or caregiver. Insecure-ambivalent attachment An anxious emotional bond marked by both a desire to be with a parent or caregiver and some resistance to being reunited. Affectional needs Emotional needs for love and affection. Parental styles Identifiable patterns of parental caretaking and interaction with children. Authoritarian parents Parents who enforce rigid rules and demand strict obedience to authority. 92 CHAPTER 3 Overly permissive parents give little guidance, allow too much freedom, or don’t hold children accountable for their actions. Typically, the child has rights similar to an adult’s but few responsibilities. Rules are not enforced, and the child usually gets his or her way. (“Do whatever you want.”) Permissive parents tend to produce dependent, immature children who misbehave frequently. Such children are aimless and likely to “run amok.” Baumrind describes authoritative parents as those who supply firm and consistent guidance, combined with love and affection. Such parents balance their own rights with those of their children. They control their children’s behavior in a caring, responsive, nonauthoritarian way. (“Do it for this reason.”) Effective parents are firm and consistent, not harsh or rigid. In general, they encourage the child to act responsibly, to think, and to make good decisions. This style produces children who are resilient (good at bouncing back after bad experiences) and develop the strengths they need to thrive even in difficult circumstances (Kim-Cohen et al., 2004; Masten, 2001). The children of authoritative parents are competent, self-controlled, independent, assertive, and inquiring. They know how to manage their emotions and use positive coping skills (Eisenberg et al., 2003; Lynch et al., 2004). To read more about effective parenting, see this chapter’s Psychology in Action section. It might seem that the father’s role as a playmate makes him less important. Not so. Samantha’s playtime with Tom is actually very valuable. From birth onward, fathers pay more visual attention to children than mothers do. Fathers are much more tactile (lifting, tickling, and handling the baby), more physically arousing (engaging in rough-and-tumble play), and more likely to engage in unusual play (imitating the baby, for example) (Crawley & Sherrod, 1984; Paquette et al., 2003). In comparison, mothers speak to infants more, play more conventional games (such as peekaboo), and, as noted, spend much more time in caregiving. Young children who spend a lot of time playing with their fathers tend to be more competent in many ways (TamisLeMonda et al., 2004). Overall, fathers can be as affectionate, sensitive, and responsive as mothers are. Nevertheless, infants and children tend to get very different views of males and females. Females, who offer comfort, nurturance, and verbal stimulation, tend to be close at hand. Males come and go, and when they are present, action, exploration, and risk-taking prevail. It’s no wonder, then, that the parental styles of mothers and fathers have a major impact on children’s gender role development (Lindsay, Mize, & Pettit, 1997; Videon, 2005). Maternal and Paternal Influences Don’t mothers and fathers parent differently? Yes. Although maternal influences (all the effects a mother has on her child) generally have a greater impact, fathers do make a unique contribution to parenting (Santrock, 2007). Although fathers are spending more time with their children, mothers still do most of the nurturing and caretaking, especially of young children (Craig, 2006). Studies of paternal influences (the sum of all effects a father has on his child) reveal that fathers are more likely to play with their children and tell them stories. In contrast, mothers are typically responsible for the physical and emotional care of their children ( Figure 3.14). • 3.5 3 2.5 2 1.5 1 0.5 Ca re Pa ss iv e In te ra ct iv e Ph Ca ys re ic al /e m ot io na lC Tr ar av e el /C om m un ic at io n 0 Type of child care BananaStock/SuperStock Fathers Mothers Fathering typically makes a contribution to early development that differs in emphasis from mothering. 3.14 Mother-child and father-child interactions. This graph shows •whatFigure occurred on routine days in a sample of more than 1,400 Australian homes. Mothers spend about twice as long each day on child care, compared with fathers. Further, mothers spend more time on physical and emotional care (such as feeding, bathing, soothing) than on interactive care (such as playing, reading, activities); fathers show the reverse pattern. Finally, mothers spend more time on travel (for instance, driving children to sports or music lessons), communication (such as talking to teachers about their children), and passive care (supervising children while they play). (Adapted from Craig, 2006.) 93 Human Development Ethnic Differences: Four Flavors of Parenting Do ethnic differences in parenting affect children in distinctive ways? Diana Baumrind’s work provides a good overall summary of the effects of parenting. However, her conclusions are probably most valid for families whose roots lie in Europe. Child rearing in other ethnic groups often reflects different customs and beliefs. Cultural differences are especially apparent with respect to the meaning attached to a child’s behavior. Is a particular behavior “good” or “bad”? Should it be encouraged or discouraged? The answer will depend greatly on parents’ cultural values (Leyendecker et al., 2005). Making generalizations about groups of people is always risky. Nevertheless, some typical differences in child-rearing patterns have been observed in North American ethnic communities (Kaplan, 1998; Parke, 2004). expect respect, obedience, self-control, and self-discipline from their children. Arab-American Families In Middle Eastern cultures, children are expected to be polite, obedient, disciplined, and conforming. Punishment generally consists of spankings, teasing, or shaming in front of others. ArabAmerican fathers tend to be strong authority figures who demand obedience so that the family will not be shamed by a child’s bad behavior. Success, generosity, and hospitality are highly valued in Arab-American culture. The pursuit of family honor encourages hard work, thrift, conservatism, and educational achievement. The welfare of the family is emphasized over individual identity. Thus, Arab-American children are raised to respect their parents, members of their extended family, and other adults as well (Erickson & Al-Timimi, 2001; Medhus, 2001). African-American Families Traditional African-American values emphasize loyalty and interdependence among family members, security, developing a positive identity, and not giving up in the face of adversity. AfricanAmerican parents typically stress obedience and respect for elders (Dixon, Graber, & Brooks-Gunn, 2008). Child discipline tends to be fairly strict (Parke, 2004), but many African-American parents see this as a necessity, especially if they live in urban areas where safety is a concern. Self-reliance, resourcefulness, and an ability to take care of oneself in difficult situations are also qualities that African-American parents seek to promote in their children. Hispanic Families Asian-American Families Asian cultures tend to be group oriented, and they emphasize interdependence among individuals. In contrast, Western cultures value individual effort and independence. This difference is often reflected in Asian-American child-rearing practices (Chao & Tseng, 2002). Asian-American children are taught that their behavior can bring either pride or shame to the family. Therefore, they are obliged to set aside their own desires when the greater good of the family is at stake (Parke, 2004). Parents tend to act as teachers who encourage hard work, moral behavior, and achievement. For the first few years, parenting is lenient and permissive. However, after about age 5, Asian-American parents begin to Steven J. Gold Like African-American parents, Hispanic parents tend to have relatively strict standards of discipline (Dixon, Graber, & BrooksGunn, 2008). They also place a high value on family values, family pride, and loyalty. Hispanic families are typically affectionate and indulgent toward younger children. However, as children grow older, they are expected to learn social skills and to be calm, obedient, courteous, and respectful. In fact, such social skills may be valued more than cognitive skills (Delgado & Ford, 1998). In addition, Hispanic parents tend to stress cooperation more than competition. Such values can put Hispanic children at a disadvantage in highly competitive, European-American culture. In other ethnic communities, norms for effective parenting often differ in subtle ways from parenting styles in Euro-American culture. Overly permissive parents Parents who give little guidance, allow too much freedom, or do not require the child to take responsibility. Authoritative parents Parents who supply firm and consistent guidance combined with love and affection. Maternal influences The aggregate of all psychological effects mothers have on their children. Paternal influences The aggregate of all psychological effects fathers have on their children. CHAPTER 3 Implications Child rearing is done in a remarkable variety of ways around the world. In fact, many of the things we do in North America, such as forcing young children to sleep alone, would be considered odd or wrong in other cultures. In the final analysis, parenting can only be judged if we know what culture or ethnic community a child is being prepared to enter (Leyendecker et al., 2005). K NOW LED G E B U I L D E R Social Development in Childhood RECITE 1. Clear signs of self-awareness or self-recognition are evident in most infants by the time they reach 8 months of age. T or F? 2. The development of separation anxiety in an infant corresponds to the formation of an attachment to parents. T or F? 3. High-quality day care can actually improve children’s social and mental skills. T or F? 4. Fathers are more likely to act as playmates for their children, rather than caregivers. T or F? 5. According to Diana Baumrind’s research, effective parents are authoritarian in their approach to their children’s behavior. T or F? 6. Asian-American parents tend to be more individually oriented than parents whose ethnic roots are European. T or F? REFLECT Critical Thinking 7. Can you think of another way to tell if infants have self-awareness? 8. Can emotional bonding begin before birth? Relate Do you think that your experiences as a child, such as your early attachment pattern, really affect your life as an adult? Can you think of any examples from your own life? Do you know any parents who have young children and who are authoritarian, permissive, or authoritative? What are their children like? Do you think parenting depends on ethnicity? If so, why? If not, why not? Language Development — Fast-Talking Babies Gateway Question: How do children acquire language? There’s something almost miraculous about a baby’s first words. As infants, how did we manage to leap into the world of language? Even a quick survey like this one reveals that both maturation (nature) and social development (nurture) provide a foundation for language learning. Let’s begin with a quick survey of language development. Language development is closely tied to maturation (Carroll, 2008; Gleason, 2005). As every parent knows, babies can cry from birth on. By 1 month of age they use crying to gain attention. Typically, parents can tell if an infant is hungry, angry, or in pain from the tone of the crying (Kaplan, 1998). Around 6 to 8 weeks of age, babies begin cooing (the repetition of vowel sounds such as “oo” and “ah”). By 7 months of age, Samantha’s nervous system will mature enough to allow her to grasp objects, smile, laugh, sit up, and babble. In the babbling stage, the consonants b, d, m, and g are combined with the vowel sounds to produce meaningless language sounds: dadadadada or bababa. At first, babbling is the same around the world. But soon, the language spoken by parents begins to have an influence. That is, Japanese babies start to babble in a way that sounds like Japanese, Mexican babies babble in Spanish-like sounds, and so forth (Gopnik, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 2000; Kuhl, 2004). At about 1 year of age, children respond to real words such as no or hi. Soon afterward, the first connection between words and objects forms, and children may address their parents as “Mama” or “Dada.” By age 18 months to 2 years, Samantha’s vocabulary may include a hundred words or more. At first there is a single-word stage, during which children use one word at a time, such as “go,” “juice,” or “up.” Soon after, words are arranged in simple two-word sentences called telegraphic speech: “Want-Teddy,” “Mama-gone.” Language and the Terrible Twos At about the same time that children begin to put two or three words together they become much more independent. Two-yearolds understand some of the commands parents make, but they are not always willing to carry them out. A child like Samantha may assert her independence by saying “No drink,” “Me do it,” “My cup, my cup,” and the like. It can be worse, of course. A 2-year-old may look at you intently, make eye contact, listen as you shout “No, no,” and still pour her juice on the cat. © Baby Blues Partnership. Reprinted with special permission of King Features Syndicate. 94 Answers: 1. F 2. T 3. T 4. T 5. F 6. F 7. Another successful method is to secretly rub a spot of rouge on an infant’s nose. The child is then placed in front of a mirror. The question is, Will the child touch the red spot, showing recognition of the mirror image as his or her own? The probability that a child will do so jumps dramatically during the second year. 8. It certainly can for parents. When a pregnant woman begins to feel fetal movements, she becomes aware that a baby is coming to life inside of her. Likewise, prospective parents who hear a fetal heartbeat at the doctor’s office or see an ultrasound image of the fetus begin to become emotionally attached to the unborn child (Santrock, 2007). Human Development 95 During their second year, children become increasingly capable of mischief and temper tantrums. Thus, calling this time “the terrible twos” is not entirely inappropriate. One-yearolds can do plenty of things parents don’t want them to do. However, it’s usually 2-year-olds who do things because you don’t want them to (Gopnik, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 2000). Perhaps parents can take some comfort in knowing that a stubborn, negative 2-year-old is simply becoming 85 50 20 Medium high positive Neutral attention Avert more independent. When Samantha Figure 3.15 Infant engagement scale. These samples from a 90-point scale show various levels of infant engageis 2, Olivia and Tom would be wise to ment, or attention. Babies participate in prelanguage “conversations” with parents by giving and withholding attention remember that “this, too, shall pass.” and by smiling, gazing, or vocalizing. (From Beebe et al., 1982.) After age 2, the child’s comprehension and use of words takes a dramatic leap forward. From this to communicate long before the child can speak. A readiness to point on, vocabulary and language skills grow at a phenomenal interact socially with parents may be as important as innate lanrate (Fernald, Perfors, & Marchman, 2006). By first grade, Samanguage recognition. The next section explains why. tha will be able to understand around 8,000 words and use about Early Communication 4,000. She will have truly entered the world of language. How do parents communicate with infants before they can talk? Parents go to a great deal of trouble to get babies to smile and vocalize The Roots of Language ( Figure 3.15). In doing so, they quickly learn to change their What accounts for this explosion of language development? Linguist actions to keep the infant’s attention, arousal, and activity at optimal Noam Chomsky (1975, 1986) has long claimed that humans have levels. A familiar example is the “I’m-Going-to-Get-You Game.” In a biological predisposition or hereditary readiness to develop it, the adult says, “I’m gonna getcha . . . . I’m gonna getcha . . . . language. According to Chomsky, language patterns are inborn, I’m gonna getcha . . . . Gotcha!” Through such games, adults and much like a child’s ability to coordinate walking. If such inborn babies come to share similar rhythms and expectations (Carroll, language recognition does exist, it may explain why children 2008). Soon a system of shared signals is created, including toucharound the world use a limited number of patterns in their first ing, vocalizing, gazing, and smiling. These help lay a foundation sentences. Typical patterns include (Mussen et al., 1979): for later language use (Tamis-LeMonda, Bornstein, & Baumwell, 2001). Specifically, signals establish a pattern of “conversational” Identification: “See kitty.” turn-taking (alternate sending and receiving of messages). Nonexistence: “Allgone milk.” • • Possession: Agent-Action: Negation: Question: “My doll.” “Mama give.” “Not ball.” “Where doggie?” Does Chomsky’s theory explain why language develops so rapidly? It is certainly part of the story. But many psychologists feel that Chomsky underestimates the importance of learning (Tomasello, 2003) and the social contexts that shape language development (Hoff, 2006). Psycholinguists (specialists in the psychology of language) have shown that imitation of adults and rewards for correctly using words (as when a child asks for a cookie) are an important part of language learning. Also, babies actively participate in language learning by asking questions, such as “What dis?” (Domingo & Goldstein-Alpern, 1999). When a child makes a language error, parents typically repeat the child’s sentence with needed corrections (Bohannon & Stanowicz, 1988; Hoff, 2006) or ask a clarifying question to draw the child’s attention to the error (Saxton, Houston-Price, & Dawson, 2005). More important is the fact that parents and children begin Olivia “Oh what a nice little smile!” “Yes, isn’t that nice?” “There.” “There’s a nice little smile.” “Well, pardon you!” “Yes, that’s better, isn’t it?” “Yes.” “Yes.” “What’s so funny?” Samantha (smiles) (burps) (vocalizes) (smiles) Biological predisposition The presumed hereditary readiness of humans to learn certain skills, such as how to use language, or a readiness to behave in particular ways. Signal In early language development, any behavior, such as touching, vocalizing, gazing, or smiling, that allows nonverbal interaction and turn-taking between parent and child. 96 CHAPTER 3 From the outside, such exchanges may look meaningless. In reality, they represent real communication. Samantha’s vocalizations and attention provide a way of interacting emotionally with Olivia and Tom. Infants as young as 4 months engage in vocal turn-taking with adults ( Jaffe et al., 2001). The more children interact with parents, the faster they learn to talk and the faster they learn thinking abilities (Dickinson & Tabors, 2001; Hoff & Tian, 2005). One study found that 6-week-old babies gaze at an adult’s face in rhythm with the adult’s speech (Crown et al., 2002). Unmistakably, social relationships contribute to early language learning (Hoff, 2006). Parentese When they talk to infants, parents use an exaggerated pattern of speaking called motherese or parentese. Typically, they raise their tone of voice; use short, simple sentences; repeat themselves more; and use frequent gestures (Gogate, Bahrick, & Watson, 2000). They also slow their rate of speaking and use exaggerated voice inflections: “Did Samantha eat it A-L-L UP?” What is the purpose of such changes? Parents are apparently trying to help their children learn language (Soderstrom, 2007). When a baby is still babbling, parents tend to use long, adult-style sentences. But as soon as the baby says its first word they switch to parentese. By the time babies are 4 months old they prefer parentese over normal speech (Cooper et al., 1997). In addition to being simpler, parentese has a distinct “musical” quality (Trainor & Desjardins, 2002). No matter what language mothers speak, the melodies, pauses, and inflections they use to comfort, praise, or give warning are universal. Psychologist Anne Fernald has found that mothers of all nations talk to their babies with similar changes in pitch. For instance, we praise babies with a rising, then falling pitch (“BRA-vo!” “GOOD girl!”). Warnings are delivered in a short, sharp rhythm (“Nein! Nein!” “Basta! Basta!” “Not! Dude!”). To give comfort, parents use low, smooth, drawn-out tones (“Oooh poor baaa-by,” “Oooh pobrecito”). A high-pitched, rising melody is used to call attention to objects (“See the pretty BIRDIE?”) (Fernald, 1989). Parentese helps parents get babies’ attention, communicate with them, and teach them language (Thiessen, Hill, & Saffran, 2005). Later, as a child’s speaking improves, parents tend to adjust their speech to the child’s language ability. Especially from 18 months to 4 years of age, parents seek to clarify what a child says and prompt the child to say more. In summary, some elements of language are innate. Nevertheless, our inherited tendency to learn language does not determine if we will speak English or Vietnamese, Spanish or Russian. Environmental forces also influence whether a person develops simple or sophisticated language skills. The first 7 years of life are a sensitive period in language learning (Eliot, 1999). Clearly, a full flowering of speech requires careful cultivation. KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R Language Development in Childhood RECITE 1. The development of speech and language usually occurs in which order? a. crying, cooing, babbling, telegraphic speech b. cooing, crying, babbling, telegraphic speech c. babbling, crying, cooing, telegraphic speech d. crying, babbling, cooing, identification 2. Simple two-word sentences are characteristic of ________________ _______________ speech. 3. Noam ________________________ has advanced the idea that language acquisition is built on innate patterns. 4. Prelanguage turn-taking and social interactions would be of special interest to a psycholinguist. T or F? 5. The style of speaking known as ______________________________ is higher in pitch and has a musical quality. REFLECT As with motherese, parents use a distinctive style when singing to an infant. Even people who speak another language can tell if a tape-recorded song was sung to an infant or an adult. Likewise, lullabies remain recognizable when electronic filtering removes words (Trehub et al., 1993a, 1993b). Relate In order, see if you can name and imitate the language abilities you had as you progressed from birth to age 2 years. Now see if you can label and imitate some basic elements of parentese. In your own words, state at least one argument for and against Chomsky’s view of language acquisition. You are going to spend a day with a person who speaks a different language than you do. Do you think you would be able to communicate with the other person? How does this relate to language acquisition? Answers: 1. a 2. telegraphic 3. Chomsky 4. T 5. Parentese or motherese 6. Children in professional homes receive many educational benefits that are less common in welfare homes. Yet, even when such differences are taken into account, brighter children tend to come from richer language environments (Hart & Risley, 1999). Gary Conner/Index Stock Imagery/Photolibrary Critical Thinking 6. The children of professional parents hear more words per hour than the children of welfare parents, and they also tend to score higher on tests of mental abilities. How else could their higher scores be explained? Human Development 97 eyes. Is it still there? How do you know? As an adult, you can keep an image of the object in your “mind’s eye.” According to Piaget, newborn babies cannot create internal representations such as mental images. As a result, they lack object permanence, an understanding that objects continue to exist when they are out of sight. Cognitive Development — Think Like a Child Gateway Question: How do children learn to think? Now that we have Samantha talking, let’s move on to a broader view of intellectual development. Jean Piaget ( Jahn pea-ah-ZHAY) provided some of the first great insights into how children develop thinking abilities when he proposed that children’s cognitive skills progress through a series of maturational stages. Also, many psychologists have become interested in how children learn the intellectual skills valued by their culture. Typically, children do this with guidance from skilled “tutors” (parents and others). How different is a child’s understanding of the world from that of an adult? According to the Swiss psychologist and philosopher Jean Piaget (1951, 1952), their thinking is, generally speaking, less abstract. They tend to base their understanding on particular examples and objects they can see or touch. Also, children use fewer generalizations, categories, and principles. bridges Concepts and language are other types of internal representation. See Chapter 9, page 284, for more information. Jean Piaget believed that all children mature through a series of distinct stages in intellectual development. Many of his ideas came from observing his own children as they solved various thought problems. (It is tempting to imagine that Piaget’s illustrious career was launched one day when his wife said to him, “Watch the children for a while, will you, Jean?”) For this reason, in the first 2 years of life, Samantha’s intellectual development will be largely nonintellectual and nonverbal. She will be mainly concerned with learning to coordinate information from her senses with her motor movements. But sometime during their first year, babies begin to actively pursue disappearing objects. By age 2, they can anticipate the movement of an object behind a screen. For example, when watching an electric train, Samantha will look ahead to the end of a tunnel, rather than staring at the spot where the train disappeared. In general, developments in this stage indicate that the child’s conceptions are becoming more stable. Objects cease to appear and disappear magically, and a more orderly and predictable world replaces the confusing and disconnected sensations of infancy. Mental Processes The Preoperational Stage (2–7 Years) Piaget was convinced that intellect grows through processes he called assimilation and accommodation. Assimilation refers to using existing mental patterns in new situations. Let’s say that Benjamin pounds on blocks with his favorite toy, a plastic hammer. For his birthday Benjamin gets an oversized toy wrench. If he uses the wrench for pounding, it has been assimilated to an existing knowledge structure. In accommodation, existing ideas are modified to fit new requirements. For instance, a younger child might think that a dime is worth less than a (larger) nickel. However, as children begin to spend money, they must alter their ideas about what “more” and “less” mean. Thus, new situations are assimilated to existing ideas, and new ideas are created to accommodate new experiences. Piaget’s ideas have deeply affected our view of children (Feldman, 2004). The following is a brief summary of what he found. Close your eyes again. Imagine the room you sleep in. What would it look like if you were perched on the ceiling? You have now mentally transformed your image of the room. According to Piaget, even though preoperational children can form mental images or ideas, they cannot easily transform those images or ideas in their minds. Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development Look up from this book until your attention is attracted to something else in the room. Now close your Jean Piaget — philosopher, psychologist, and keen observer of children. Assimilation In Piaget’s theory, the application of existing mental patterns to new situations (that is, the new situation is assimilated to existing mental schemes). Accommodation In Piaget’s theory, the modification of existing mental patterns to fit new demands (that is, mental schemes are changed to accommodate new information or experiences). Sensorimotor stage Stage of intellectual development during which sensory input and motor responses become coordinated. Farrell Grehan/Corbis The Sensorimotor Stage (0–2 Years) Motherese (or parentese) A pattern of speech used when talking to infants, marked by a higher-pitched voice; short, simple sentences; repetition, slower speech; and exaggerated voice inflections. Object permanence Concept, gained in infancy, that objects continue to exist even when they are hidden from view. Preoperational stage Period of intellectual development during which children begin to use language and think symbolically, yet remain intuitive and egocentric in their thought. Transformation The ability to mentally change the shape or form of a mental image or idea. 98 CHAPTER 3 FogStock LLC/Index Stock Imagery/Photolibrary Before the age of 6 or 7, children begin to think symbolically and use language. But the child’s thinking is still very concrete and intuitive (it makes little use of reasoning and logic). (Do you remember thinking as a child that the sun and the moon followed you when you took a walk?). Let’s visit Samantha at age 5: If you show her a short, wide glass full of milk and a taller, narrow glass (also full), she will most likely tell you that the taller glass contains more milk (even if it doesn’t). Samantha will tell you this even if she watches you pour milk from the short glass into an empty, tall glass. Older children can easily mentally reverse the pouring of the milk to see that the shape of container is irrelevant to the volume of milk it contains. But Samantha is preoperational; she cannot engage in the mental operation of transforming the tall glass of milk back into a short, wide glass. Thus, she is not bothered by the fact that the milk appears to be transformed from a smaller to a larger amount. Instead, she responds only to the fact that taller seems to mean more ( Figure 3.16). After about age 7, children are no longer fooled by this situation. Perhaps that’s why 7 has been called the “age of reason.” From age 7 on, we see a definite trend toward more logical, adult-like thought (Flavell, 1992). During the preoperational stage, the child is also quite egocentric (unable to take the viewpoint of other people). The child’s ego seems to stand at the center of his or her world. To illustrate, show Samantha a two-sided mirror. Then hold it between you and her, so she can see herself in it. If you ask her what she thinks you can see, she imagines that you see her face reflected in the mirror, instead of your own. She cannot mentally transform the view she sees into the view you must be seeing. Crossing a busy street can be dangerous for the preoperational child. Because their thinking is still egocentric, younger children cannot understand why the driver of a car can’t see them if they can see the car. Children under the age of 7 also cannot consistently judge speeds and distances of oncoming cars. Adults can easily overestimate the “street smarts” of younger children. It is advisable to teach children to cross with a light, in crosswalks, or with assistance. • Such egocentrism explains why children can seem exasperatingly selfish or uncooperative at times. If Benjamin blocks your view by standing in front of the TV, he assumes that you can see it if he can. If you ask him to move so you can see better, he may move so that he can see better! Benjamin is not being selfish in the ordinary sense. He just doesn’t realize that your view differs from his. In addition, the child’s use of language is not as sophisticated as it might seem. Children have a tendency to confuse words with the objects they represent. If Benjamin calls a toy block a “car” and you use it to make a “house,” he may be upset. To children, the name of an object is as much a part of the object as its size, shape, and color. This seems to underlie a preoccupation with name-calling. To the preoperational child, insulting words may really hurt. Samantha was once angered by her older brother. Searching for a way to retaliate against her larger and stronger foe, she settled on, “You panty-girdle!” It was the worst thing she could think of saying. The Concrete Operational Stage (7–11 Years) Tony Freeman/PhotoEdit The hallmark of this stage is the ability to carry out mental operations such as reversing thoughts. A 4-year-old boy in the preoperational stage might have a conversation like this (showing what happens when a child’s thinking lacks reversibility): • Figure 3.16 Children under age 7 intuitively assume that a volume of liquid increases when it is poured from a short, wide container into a taller, thinner one. This boy thinks the tall container holds more than the short one. Actually each holds the same amount of liquid. Children make such judgments based on the height of the liquid, not its volume. “Do you have a brother?” “Yes.” “What’s his name?” “Sam.” “Does Sam have a brother?” “No.” Reversibility of thought allows children in the concrete operational stage to recognize that if 4 2 8, then 2 4 does, too. Younger children must memorize each relationship separately. Human Development • The Formal Operations Stage (11 Years and Up) After about the age of 11, children begin to break away from concrete objects and specific examples. Thinking is based more on abstract principles, such as “democracy,” “honor,” or “correlation.” Children who reach this stage can think about their own thoughts, and they become less egocentric. Older children and young adolescents also gradually become able to consider hypothetical possibilities (suppositions, guesses, or projections). For example, if you ask a younger child, “What do you think would happen if it suddenly became possible for people to fly?” the child might respond, “People can’t fly.” Older children are better able to consider such possibilities. Full adult intellectual ability is attained during the stage of formal operations. Older adolescents are capable of inductive and deductive reasoning, and they can comprehend math, physics, philosophy, psychology, and other abstract systems. They can learn to test hypotheses in a scientific manner. Of course, not everyone reaches this level of thinking. Also, many adults can think formally about some topics, but their thinking becomes concrete when the topic is unfamiliar. This implies that formal thinking may be more a result of culture and learning than maturation. In any case, after late adolescence, improvements in intellect are based on gaining specific knowledge, experience, and wisdom, rather than on any leaps in basic thinking capacity. How can parents apply Piaget’s ideas? Piaget’s theory suggests that the ideal way to guide intellectual development is to provide experiences that are only slightly novel, unusual, or challenging. Remember, a child’s intellect develops mainly through accommodation. It is usually best to follow a one-step-ahead strategy, in which your teaching efforts are aimed just beyond a child’s current level of comprehension (Brainerd, 2003). Parents should avoid forced teaching, or “hothousing,” which is like trying to force plants to bloom prematurely. Forcing children to learn reading, math, gymnastics, swimming, or music at an accelerated pace can bore or oppress them. True intellectual enrichment respects the child’s interests. It does not make the child feel pressured to perform (Alvino et al., 1996). For your convenience, Table 3.2 briefly summarizes each Piagetian stage. To help you remember Piaget’s theory, the table describes what would happen at each stage if we played a game of Monopoly with the child. You’ll also find brief suggestions about how to relate to children in each stage. • Piaget Today Piaget’s theory is a valuable “road map” for understanding how children think. However, many psychologists are convinced that Piaget gave too little credit to the effects of the learning environment. For example, children who grow up in villages where pottery is made can correctly answer questions about the conservation of clay at an earlier age than Piaget would have predicted. According to learning theorists, children continuously gain specific knowledge; they do not undergo stage-like leaps in general mental ability (Siegler, 2004). On the other hand, the growth in © The New Yorker Collection, May 20, 2006 Barbara Smaller from cartoonbank.com. All Rights Reserved. Thus, a preoperational child may know that 4 9 36, without being able to tell you what 9 4 equals. The development of mental operations allows mastery of conservation (the concept that mass, weight, and volume remain unchanged when the shape of objects changes). Children have learned conservation when they understand that rolling a ball of clay into a “snake” does not increase the amount of clay. Likewise, pouring liquid from a tall, narrow glass into a shallow dish does not reduce the amount of liquid. In each case the volume remains the same despite changes in shape or appearance. The original amount is conserved. (See Figure 3.16.) During the concrete operational stage, children begin to use concepts of time, space, and number. The child can think logically about very concrete objects or situations, categories, and principles. Such abilities help explain why children stop believing in Santa Claus when they reach this stage. Because they can conserve volume, they realize that Santa’s sack couldn’t possibly hold enough toys for millions of girls and boys. 99 Intuitive thought Thinking that makes little or no use of reasoning and logic. Egocentric thought Thought that is self-centered and fails to consider the viewpoints of others. Concrete operational stage Period of intellectual development during which children become able to use the concepts of time, space, volume, and number, but in ways that remain simplified and concrete, rather than abstract. Conservation In Piaget’s theory, mastery of the concept that the weight, mass, and volume of matter remains unchanged (is conserved) even when the shape or appearance of objects changes. Formal operations stage Period of intellectual development characterized by thinking that includes abstract, theoretical, and hypothetical ideas. 100 CHAPTER 3 Table 3.2 • Piaget—A Guide for Parents Piaget Monopoly Game Guidelines for Parents Sensorimotor Stage (0–2 Years) The stage during which sensory input and motor responses become coordinated. The child puts houses, hotels, and dice in her mouth and plays with “Chance” cards. Active play with a child is most effective at this stage. Encourage explorations in touching, smelling, and manipulating objects. Peekaboo is a good way to establish the permanence of objects. The child plays Monopoly, but makes up her own rules and cannot understand instructions. Specific examples and touching or seeing things continues to be more useful than verbal explanations. Learning the concept of conservation may be aided by demonstrations with liquids, beads, clay, and other substances. Concrete Operational Stage (7–11 Years) The period of cognitive development during which children begin to use concepts of time, space, volume, and number, but in ways that remain simplified and concrete. The child understands basic instructions and will play by the rules but is not capable of hypothetical transactions dealing with mortgages, loans, and special pacts with other players. Children are beginning to use generalizations, but they still require specific examples to grasp many ideas. Expect a degree of inconsistency in the child’s ability to apply concepts of time, space, quantity, and volume to new situations. Formal Operations Stage (11 Years and Up) The period of intellectual development marked by a capacity for abstract, theoretical, and hypothetical thinking. The child no longer plays the game mechanically; complex and hypothetical transactions unique to each game are now possible. It is now more effective to explain things verbally or symbolically and to help children master general rules and principles. Encourage the child to create hypotheses and to imagine how things could be. Preoperational Stage (2–7 Years) The period of cognitive development when children begin to use language and think symbolically, yet remain intuitive and egocentric. continue to be debated (Feldman, 2004). Where early infancy is concerned, even Piaget’s observations may need updating. It looks like Piaget greatly underestimated the mental abilities of infants. Infant Cognition 7 to 15 years 3 to 6 years Growth 16 to 20 years Pruning • Figure 3.17 Between the ages of 3 and 6 a tremendous wave of growth occurs in connections among neurons in the frontal areas of the brain. This corresponds to the time when children make rapid progress in their ability to think symbolically. Between ages 7 and 15, peak synaptic growth shifts to the temporal and parietal lobes. During this period children become increasingly adept at using language, a specialty of the temporal lobes. In the late teens, the brain actively destroys unneeded connections, especially in the frontal lobes. This pruning of synapses sharpens the brain’s capacity for abstract thinking (Restak, 2001). connections between brain cells occurs in waves that parallel some of Piaget’s stages ( Figure 3.17). Thus, the truth may lie somewhere between Piaget’s stage theory and modern learning theory. On a broad scale, many of Piaget’s observations have held up well. However, his explanations of childhood thinking abilities • What evidence is there that Piaget underestimated infant abilities? Piaget believed that infants under the age of 1 year cannot think (use internal representations). Babies, he said, can have no memory of people and objects that are out of sight. Yet, we now know that infants begin forming representations of the world very early in life. For example, babies as young as 3 months of age appear to know that objects are solid and do not disappear when out of view (Baillargeon, 2004). Why did Piaget fail to detect the thinking skills of infants? Most likely, he mistook babies’ limited physical skills for mental incompetence. Piaget’s tests required babies to search for objects or reach out and touch them. Newer, more sensitive methods are uncovering abilities Piaget missed. One such method takes advantage of the fact that babies, like adults, act surprised when they see something “impossible” or unexpected occur. To use this effect, psychologist Renee Baillargeon (1991, 2004) puts on little “magic shows” for infants. In her “theater,” babies watch as possible and impossible events occur with toys or other objects. Some 3-month-old infants act surprised and gaze longer at impossible events. An example is seeing two solid objects appear to pass through each other. By the time they are 8 months old, babies can remember where objects are (or should be) for at least 1 minute ( Figure 3.18). Piaget believed that abilities like those described in Figure 3.18 emerge only after a long period of sensorimotor development. How- • • Human Development 101 Toy placed behind screen Toy retrieved 3.18 The panels on the left show a possible event, in which an infant watches as a toy is placed behind the right of two screens. After a •delayFigure of 70 seconds, the toy is brought into view from behind the right screen. In the two panels on the right, an impossible event occurs. The toy is placed behind the left screen and retrieved from behind the right. (A duplicate toy was hidden there before testing.) Eight-month-old infants react with surprise when they see the impossible event staged for them. Their reaction implies that they remember where the toy was hidden. Infants appear to have a capacity for memory and thinking that greatly exceeds what Piaget claimed is possible during the sensorimotor period. (Adapted from Baillargeon et al., 1989.) ever, it’s clear that babies quickly acquire the capacity to form concepts about the world (Eimas, Quinn, & Cowan, 1994). It looks as if further study is likely to refine and amend the ideas that grew from Piaget’s fateful decision to “watch the children for a while.” Another criticism of Piaget is that he underestimated the impact of culture on mental development. The next section tells how Samantha will master the intellectual tools valued by her culture. Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory While Piaget stressed the role of maturation in cognitive development, Russian scholar Lev Vygotsky (1896–1934) focused on the impact of sociocultural factors. Vygotsky’s key insight is that children’s thinking develops through dialogues with more capable persons (Vygotsky, 1962, 1978). How does that relate to intellectual growth? So far, no one has ever published A Child’s Guide to Life on Earth. Instead, children must learn about life from various “tutors,” such as parents, teachers, and older siblings. Even if A Child’s Guide to Life on Earth did exist, we would need a separate version for every culture. It is not enough for children to learn how to think. They must also learn specific intellectual skills valued by their culture. Like Piaget, Vygotsky believed that children actively seek to discover new principles. However, Vygotsky emphasized that many of a child’s most important “discoveries” are guided by skillful tutors. Psychologist David Shaffer (2002) offers the following example: Annie, a 4-year-old, has just received her first jigsaw puzzle as a birthday present. She attempts to work the puzzle but gets nowhere until her father comes along, sits down beside her, and gives her some tips. He suggests that it would be a good idea to put together the corners first, points to the pink area at the edge of one corner piece and says, “Let’s look for another pink piece.” When Annie seems frustrated, he places two inter- locking pieces near each other so that she will notice them, and when Annie succeeds, he offers words of encouragement. As Annie gradually gets the hang of it, he steps back and lets her work more and more independently. (p. 260) Interactions like this are most helpful when they take place within a child’s zone of proximal development. What did Vygotsky mean by that? The word proximal means close or nearby. Vygotsky realized that, at any given time, some tasks are just beyond a child’s reach. The child is close to having the mental skills needed to do the task, but it is a little too complex to be mastered alone. However, children working within this zone can make rapid progress if they receive sensitive guidance from a skilled partner (LeBlanc & Bearison, 2004). (Notice that this is similar to the one-step-ahead strategy described earlier.) Vygotsky also emphasized a process he called scaffolding. A scaffold is a framework or temporary support. Vygotsky believed that adults help children learn how to think by “scaffolding,” or supporting, their attempts to solve problems or discover principles (Daniels, 2005). To be most effective, scaffolding must be responsive to a child’s needs. For example, as Annie’s father helped her with the puzzle, he tailored his hints and guidance to match her evolving abilities. The two of them worked together, step by step, so that Annie could better understand how to assemble a puzzle. In a sense, Annie’s father set up a series of temporary bridges that helped her move into new mental territory. As predicted by Vygotsky’s theory, the reading skills of 8- to 10-year-old children Zone of proximal development Refers to the range of tasks a child cannot yet master alone, but that she or he can accomplish with the guidance of a more capable partner. Scaffolding The process of adjusting instruction so that it is responsive to a beginner’s behavior and supports the beginner’s efforts to understand a problem or gain a mental skill. 102 CHAPTER 3 are closely related to the amount of verbal scaffolding their mothers provided at ages 3 and 4 (Dieterich et al., 2006). During their collaborations with others, children learn important cultural beliefs and values. For example, imagine that a boy wants to know how many baseball cards he has. His mother helps him stack and count the cards, moving each card to a new stack as they count it. She then shows him how to write the number on a slip of paper so he can remember it. This teaches the child not only about counting, but also that writing is valued in our culture. In other parts of the world, a child learning to count might be shown how to make notches on a stick or tie knots in a cord. Implications Vygotsky saw that grown-ups play a crucial role in what children know. As they try to decipher the world, children rely on adults to help them understand how things work. Vygotsky further noticed that adults unconsciously adjust their behavior to give children the information they need to solve problems that interest the child. In this way, children use adults to learn about their culture and society (Gopnik, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 2000; LeBlanc & Bearison, 2004). K NOW LED GE B U I L D E R Cognitive Development in Childhood RECITE Match each item with one of the following stages. A. Sensorimotor B. Preoperational C. Concrete operational D. Formal operations 1. _____ egocentric thought 5. _____ conservation 2. _____ abstract or hypothetical 6. _____ reversibility thought 3. _____ purposeful movement 7. _____ object permanence 4. _____ intuitive thought 8. _____ nonverbal development 9. Assimilation refers to applying existing thought patterns or knowledge to new situations. T or F? 10. Newer methods for testing infant thinking abilities frequently make note of whether an infant is _____________________________ by seemingly _____________________________ events. 11. Vygotsky called the process of providing a temporary framework of supports for learning new mental abilities _____________________ ____________. REFLECT Critical Thinking 12. Using Piaget’s theory as a guide, at what age would you expect a child to recognize that a Styrofoam cup has weight? Relate You are going to make cookies with children of various ages. See if you can name each of Piaget’s stages and give an example of what a child in that stage might be expected to do. You have been asked to help a child learn to use a pocket calculator to do simple addition. How would you go about identifying the child’s zone of proximal development for this task? How would you scaffold the child’s learning? Adolescence and Young Adulthood — The Best of Times, the Worst of Times Gateway Question: Why is the transition from adolescence to adulthood especially challenging? Adolescence and young adulthood is a time of change, youthful exploration, and exuberance. It can also be a time of worry and problems, especially in today’s world. It might even be fair to describe this period as “the best of times, the worst of times.” During adolescence, a person’s identity and moral values come into sharper focus even as the transition to adulthood is occurring at ever-later ages. Adolescence is the culturally defined period between childhood and adulthood. Socially, the adolescent is no longer a child, yet not quite an adult. Almost all cultures recognize this transitional status. However, the length of adolescence varies greatly from culture to culture. For example, most 14-year-old girls in North America live at home and go to school. In contrast, many 14-year-old females in rural villages of many poorer countries are married and have children. In our culture, 14-year-olds are adolescents. In others, they may be adults. Is marriage the primary criterion for adult status in North America? No, it’s not even one of the top three criteria. Today, the most widely accepted standards are (1) taking responsibility for oneself, (2) making independent decisions, and (3) becoming financially independent. In practice, this typically means breaking away from parents by taking a job and setting up a separate residence (Arnett, 2001). Puberty Many people confuse adolescence with puberty. However, puberty is a biological event, not a social status. During puberty, hormonal changes promote rapid physical growth and sexual maturity. Biologically, most people reach reproductive maturity in the early teens. Social and intellectual maturity, however, may lie years ahead. Young adolescents often make decisions that affect their entire lives, even though they are immature mentally and socially. The tragically high rates of teenage pregnancy and drug abuse are prime examples. Despite such risks, most people do manage to weather adolescence without developing any serious psychological problems (Steinberg, 2001). How much difference does the timing of puberty make? For boys, maturing early is generally beneficial. Typically, it enhances their self-image and gives them an advantage socially and athletically. Early-maturing boys tend to be more relaxed, dominant, selfassured, and popular. However, early puberty does carry some risks because early-maturing boys are also more likely to get into trouble with drugs, alcohol, and antisocial behavior (Steinberg, 2001). For girls, the advantages of early maturation are less clear-cut. In elementary school, fast-maturing girls are less popular and have poorer self-images, perhaps because they are larger and heavier than their classmates (Deardorff et al., 2007). By junior high, how- Answers: 1. B 2. D 3. A 4. B 5. C 6. C 7. A 8. A 9. T 10. surprised, impossible 11. scaffolding 12. Seventy-five percent of 4- to 6-year-olds say that a Styrofoam cup has no weight after lifting it! Most children judge weight intuitively (by the way an object feels) until they begin to move into the concrete operational stage (Smith, Carey, & Wiser, 1985). Human Development 103 HU M AN D I VERS I T Y Ethnic Diversity and Identity Ethnic heritage can have a powerful influence on personal identity (Weisskirch, 2005). For adolescents of ethnic descent, the question is often not just “Who am I?” Rather, it is “Who am I at home? Who am I at school? Who am I with friends from my neighborhood?” As ethnic minorities in America continue to grow in status and prominence, adolescents are less and less likely to feel rejected or excluded because of their ethnic heritage as they try to find their place in society. This is fortunate since ethnic adolescents have often faced degrading stereotypes concern- ing their intelligence, sexuality, social status, manners, and so forth. The result can be lowered self-esteem and confusion about roles, values, and personal identity (de las Fuentes & Vasquez, 1999). At the same time, the increasingly multicultural nature of contemporary American society raises new questions for adolescents about what it means to be American (Schwartz, 2008). In forming an identity, adolescents of ethnic descent face the question of how they should think of themselves. Is Lori an American or a Chinese American or both? ever, early development includes sexual features. This leads to a more positive body image, greater peer prestige, and adult approval (Brooks-Gunn & Warren, 1988). Early-maturing girls tend to date sooner and are more independent and more active in school. However, like their male counterparts, they are also more often in trouble at school and more likely to engage in early sex (Flannery et al., 1993). As you can see, there are costs and benefits associated with early puberty. One added cost of early maturation is that it may force premature identity formation. When Samantha is a teenager and she begins to look like an adult, she may be treated like an adult. Is Jaime a Latino, a Chicano, or a Mexican American? The answer typically depends on how strongly adolescents identify with their family and ethnic community. Teens who take pride in their ethnic heritage have higher selfesteem, a better self-image, and a stronger sense of personal identity (Roberts et al., 1999; Tse, 1999). They are also less likely to engage in drug use (Marsiglia et al., 2004) or violent behavior (French, Kim, & Pillado, 2006). Group pride, positive models, and a more tolerant society could do much to keep a broad range of options open to all adolescents. Ideally, this change can encourage greater maturity and independence. However, if the search for identity ends too soon, it may leave Samantha with a distorted, poorly formed sense of self ( Figure 3.19). • The Search for Identity Identity formation is a key task of adolescence (Schwartz, 2008). Of course, problems of identity occur at other times too. But in a very real sense, puberty signals that it’s time to begin forming a new, more mature self-image (Douvan, 1997). Many problems stem from unclear standards about the role adolescents should play within society. Are they adults or children? Should they be autonomous or dependent? Should they work or play? Such ambiguities make it difficult for young people to form clear images of themselves and of how they should act (Alsaker, 1995). Answering the question “Who am I?” is also spurred by cognitive development. After adolescents have attained the stage of formal operations, they are better able to ask questions about their place in the world and about morals, values, politics, and social relationships. Then too, being able to think about hypothetical possibilities allows the adolescent to contemplate the future and ask more realistically, “Who will I be?” (Côté, 2006b). (See “Ethnic Diversity and Identity.”) Figure 3.19 Dramatic differences in physical size and maturity are found in •adolescents of the same age. The girls pictured are all 13, the boys 16. Maturation Adolescence The culturally defined period between childhood and adulthood. that occurs earlier or later than average can affect the “search for identity.” (Reprinted Puberty The biologically defined period during which a person matures sexually and becomes capable of reproduction. with permission of Nelson Prentiss.) 104 CHAPTER 3 C R IT ICAL T H I N KI N G The Twixters As you read this text, we encourage you to reflect on new ideas and concepts by thinking critically about them. Consider, for example, the term adulthood. Is becoming an adult strictly a biological event? Meet 22-year-old Kirsten: Kirsten is a “twixter”: twentysomething, still living at home, not yet married, with no children, and no settled career. In England, twixsters are called “kippers” (Kids In Parents’ Pockets Eroding Retirement Savings). In Australia they are “boomerang kids” (they always come back home). And in Germany they are “Nesthocker” (nest squatters). Are twixters adolescents who are taking longer to find their identity? Or are they young adults avoiding their need to enter the adult world? Are they self-indulgent individuals trapped in a “maturity gap” (Galambos, Barker, & TiltonWeaver, 2003)? Or are they part of a new social status that could be called “emerging adulthood” (Arnett & Tanner, 2006)? According to psychologist Jeffrey Arnett, emerging adulthood is increasingly common in affluent Westernized cultures that allow young people to take longer to settle into their adult roles (Arnett, 2000, 2004). However, in less affluent countries, as in poorer parts of America, most adolescents continue to “become adults” at much younger ages (Arnett & Galambos, 2003). Thus, words like adolescent or adulthood cannot be defined solely in terms of physical maturation. Sociocultural factors also play a role in defining when we stop being children or become adults (Arnett, 2004). © Zits Partnership. Reprinted with special permission of King Features Syndicate. “When our mothers were our age, they were engaged. . . . They at least had some idea what they were going to do with their lives. . . . I, on the other hand, will have a dual degree in majors that are ambiguous at best and impractical at worst (English and political science), no ring on my finger and no idea who I am, much less what I want to do. . . . I realize that having nothing ahead to count on means I now have to count on myself; that having no direction means forging one of my own.” (Page, 1999). The Transition to Adulthood Today the challenge of identity formation is further complicated by the fact that more and more young people are deferring young adulthood, preferring to prolong identity explorations into their 20s before they commit to long-term choices in love and work (Arnett, 2000, 2004; Arnett & Tanner, 2006). According to sociologist James Côté, Western industrialized societies, like the United States and Canada, are becoming increasingly tolerant of this period of extended adolescence (Côté, 2006a). (See “The Twixters.”) Samantha may live with Olivia and Tom until her mid-20s, delaying her transition to adulthood. Alternately, she may make the transition to young adulthood during the traditional 18-to-21 period. Regardless, she will eventually face the primary adult issues of marriage, children, and career. How she manages, especially in her core relationships, will determine whether she feels a sense of intimacy or feels isolated from others. In many ways adolescence and young adulthood are more emotionally turbulent than midlife or old age. One important aspect of the emotional turbulence of adolescence and young adulthood is the struggle with right and wrong — in other words, the need to develop moral values. Moral Development — Growing a Conscience Gateway Question: How do we develop morals and values? A person with a terminal illness is in great pain. She is pleading for death. Should extraordinary medical efforts be made to keep her alive? A friend of yours desperately needs to pass a test and asks you to help him cheat. Will you do it? These are moral questions, or questions of conscience. Human Development Moral development starts in childhood and continues into adulthood (Turiel, 2006). Through this process, we acquire values, beliefs, and thinking patterns that guide responsible behavior. Moral values are especially likely to come into sharper focus during adolescence and the transition to adulthood, as capacities for self-control and abstract thinking increase (Hart & Carlo, 2005). Let’s take a brief look at this interesting aspect of personal development. Levels of Moral Development How are moral values acquired? In an influential account, psychologist Lawrence Kohlberg (1981) held that we learn moral values through thinking and reasoning. To study moral development, Kohlberg posed dilemmas to children of different ages. The following is one of the moral dilemmas he used (Kohlberg, 1969, adapted). A woman was near death from cancer, and there was only one drug that might save her. It was discovered by a druggist who was charging 10 times what it cost to make the drug. The sick woman’s husband could only pay $1,000, but the druggist wanted $2,000. He asked the druggist to sell it cheaper or to let him pay later. The druggist said no. So the husband became desperate and broke into the store to steal the drug for his wife. Should he have done that? Was it wrong or right? Why? Each child was asked what action the husband should take. Kohlberg classified the reasons given for each choice and identified three levels of moral development. Each is based not so much on the choices made, but on the reasoning used to arrive at a choice. At the lowest, preconventional level, moral thinking is guided by the consequences of actions (punishment, reward, or an exchange of favors). For example, a person at this level might reason that “The man shouldn’t steal the drug because he could get caught and sent to jail” (avoiding punishment) or “It won’t do him any good to steal the drug because his wife will probably die before he gets out of jail” (self-interest). At the second, or conventional level, reasoning is based on a desire to please others or to follow accepted authority, rules, and values. For example, a person at this intermediate level might say “He shouldn’t steal the drug because others will think he is a thief. His wife would not want to be saved by thievery” (avoiding disapproval) or “Although his wife needs the drug, he should not break the law to get it. Everyone has to obey the law. His wife’s condition does not justify stealing” (traditional morality of authority). At the highest, or postconventional level, moral behavior is directed by self-chosen ethical principles that tend to be general, comprehensive, or universal. People at this level place high value on justice, dignity, and equality. For example, a highly principled person might say “He should steal the drug and then inform the authorities that he has done so. He will have to face a penalty, but he will have saved a human life” (self-chosen ethical principles). Does everyone eventually reach the highest level? People advance at different rates, and many fail to reach the postconventional level 105 of moral reasoning. In fact, many do not even reach the conventional level. For instance, one English survey revealed that 11 percent of men and 3 percent of women would commit murder for $1 million if they could be sure of getting away with the crime (“They’d kill,” 1991). The preconventional level is most characteristic of young children and delinquents (Forney, Forney, & Crutsinger, 2005). Conventional, group-oriented morals are typical of older children and most adults. Kohlberg estimated that only about 20 percent of the adult population achieves postconventional morality, representing self-direction and higher principles. (It would appear that few of these people enter politics!) Developing a “moral compass” is an important part of growing up. Many of the choices we make every day involve fundamental questions of right and wrong. Being able to think clearly about such questions is essential to becoming a responsible adult. Justice or Caring? Carol Gilligan (1982) pointed out that Kohlberg’s system is concerned mainly with justice. Based on studies of women who faced real-life dilemmas, Gilligan argued that there is also an ethic of caring about others. As one illustration, Gilligan presented the following story to 11- to 15-year-old American children. The Porcupine and the Moles Seeking refuge from the cold, a porcupine asked to share a cave for the winter with a family of moles. The moles agreed. But because the cave was small, they soon found they were being scratched each time the porcupine moved about. Finally, they asked the porcupine to leave. But the porcupine refused, saying, “If you moles are not satisfied, I suggest that you leave.” Boys who read this story tended to opt for justice in resolving the dilemma: “It’s the moles’ house. It’s a deal. The porcupine leaves.” In contrast, girls tended to look for solutions that would keep all parties happy and comfortable, such as “Cover the porcupine with a blanket.” Gilligan’s point is that male psychologists have, for the most part, defined moral maturity in terms of justice and autonomy. From this perspective, a woman’s concern with relationships can look like a weakness rather than a strength. (A woman who is concerned about what pleases or helps others would be placed at the conventional level in Kohlberg’s system.) But Gilligan believes that caring is also a major element of moral development, and she Moral development The development of values, beliefs, and thinking abilities that act as a guide regarding what is acceptable behavior. Preconventional moral reasoning Moral thinking based on the consequences of one’s choices or actions (punishment, reward, or an exchange of favors). Conventional moral reasoning Moral thinking based on a desire to please others or to follow accepted rules and values. Postconventional moral reasoning Moral thinking based on carefully examined and self-chosen moral principles. 106 CHAPTER 3 suggests that males may lag in achieving it (Botes, 2000; Gilligan & Attanucci, 1988). Does the evidence support Gilligan’s position? Several studies have found little or no difference in men’s and women’s overall moral reasoning abilities (Glover, 2001; Wilson, 1995). Indeed, both men and women may use caring and justice to make moral decisions. The moral yardstick they use appears to depend on the situation they face (Wark & Krebs, 1996). Just the same, Gilligan deserves credit for identifying a second major way in which moral choices are made. It can be argued that our best moral choices combine justice and caring, reason and emotion — which may be what we mean by wisdom (Pasupathi & Staudinger, 2001). The Story of a Lifetime — Rocky Road or Garden Path? K NOW LED G E B U I L D E R Gateway Question: What are the typical tasks and dilemmas through the life span? Erik Erikson’s psychosocial theory provides a good overview of the major psychological conflicts that occur during a “typical” life. In later adulthood we all face many additional challenges, including physical aging. Regardless, it is possible to age successfully. We also must face our own mortality and inevitable death. Every life is marked by a number of developmental milestones. These are notable events, markers, or turning points in personal development. Some examples include graduating from school, voting for the first time, getting married, watching a child leave home (or move back!), the death of a parent, becoming a grandparent, retirement, and one’s own death. Thus far, we have traced Samantha’s progress through childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood. What challenges lie ahead for her? Adolescence, Young Adulthood, and Moral Development Erikson’s Psychosocial Theory RECITE 1. In North America the primary criterion for the transition from adolescence to adulthood is marriage. T or F? 2. Identify formation is spurred by ________________ and ________________ ________________. 3. According to Jeffrey Arnett, the trend in affluent Westernized cultures toward allowing young people to take longer to settle into their adult roles is best referred to as a. emerging adulthood b. hurried childhood c. a maturity gap d. extended adolescence 4. According to Kohlberg, the conventional level of moral development is marked by a reliance on outside authority. T or F? 5. Self-interest and avoiding punishment are elements of postconventional morality. T or F? 6. About 80 percent of all adults function at the postconventional level of moral reasoning. T or F? 7. Gilligan regards gaining a sense of justice as the principal basis of moral development. T or F? REFLECT Critical Thinking 8. Are labels like “adolescent” or “young adult” reflective of heredity or environment? Relate To what extent does the concept of identity formation apply to your own experience during adolescence? Do you know any twixters? (Are you one?) Do you think twixters are adolescents taking longer to find their identity or young adults avoiding their need to establish themselves in the world of adults? At what stage of moral development do you think most terrorists function? Perhaps the best way to get a preview of Samantha’s life is to consider some of the major psychological challenges she is likely to encounter. Broad similarities can be found in the life stages of infancy, childhood, adolescence, young adulthood, middle adulthood, and old age. Each stage confronts a person with new developmental tasks that must be mastered for optimal development. Examples are learning to read in childhood, adjusting to sexual maturity in adolescence, and establishing a vocation as an adult. In an influential book titled Childhood and Society (1963), personality theorist Erik Erikson (1903–1994) suggests that we face a specific psychosocial dilemma, or “crisis,” at each stage of life. A psychosocial dilemma is a conflict between personal impulses and the social world. Resolving each dilemma creates a new balance between a person and society. A string of “successes” produces healthy development and a satisfying life. Unfavorable outcomes throw us off balance, making it harder to deal with later crises. Life becomes a “rocky road,” and personal growth is stunted. Table 3.3 lists Erikson’s dilemmas. • Table 3.3 • Erikson’s Psychosocial Dilemmas Age Characteristic Dilemma Birth to 1 year Trust versus mistrust 1 to 3 years Autonomy versus shame and doubt 3 to 5 years Initiative versus guilt 6 to 12 years Industry versus inferiority Adolescence Identity versus role confusion Young adulthood Intimacy versus isolation Middle adulthood Generativity versus stagnation Late adulthood Integrity versus despair Answers: 1. F 2. puberty, cognitive development 3. a 4. T 5. F 6. F 7. F 8. Environment, rather than heredity, is the better answer. Even better, the meanings of terms like “adolescence” or “adult” vary considerably from culture to culture, indicating that it is really a matter of definition (Côté, 2006a). Human Development 107 dom to play, ask questions, use imagination, and choose activities. Feelings of guilt about initiating activities are formed if parents criticize severely, prevent play, or discourage a child’s questions. Sarah Putnum/Index Stock Imagery/Photolibrary Stage Four, 6–12 Years: Industry versus Inferiority Personality theorist Erik Erikson (1903–1994) is best known for his life-stage theory of human development. What are the major developmental tasks and life crises? A brief description of each psychosocial dilemma follows. Stage One, First Year of Life: Trust versus Mistrust During the first year of life, children are completely dependent on others. Erikson believes that a basic attitude of trust or mistrust is formed at this time. Trust is established when babies are given warmth, touching, love, and physical care. Mistrust is caused by inadequate or unpredictable care and by parents who are cold, indifferent, or rejecting. Basic mistrust may later cause insecurity, suspiciousness, or an inability to relate to others. Notice that trust comes from the same conditions that help babies become securely attached to their parents. Stage Two, 1–3 Years: Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt In stage two, children express their growing self-control by climbing, touching, exploring, and trying to do things for themselves. Tom and Olivia fostered Samantha’s sense of autonomy by encouraging her to try new skills. However, her first efforts were sometimes crude, involving spilling, falling, wetting, and other “accidents.” If Tom and Olivia had ridiculed or overprotected Samantha, they might have caused her to feel shameful about her actions and doubt her abilities. Stage Three, 3–5 Years: Initiative versus Guilt In stage three, children move beyond simple self-control and begin to take initiative. Through play, children learn to make plans and carry out tasks. Parents reinforce initiative by giving children free- Many events of middle childhood are symbolized by that fateful day when you first entered school. With dizzying speed your world expanded beyond your family, and you faced a whole series of new challenges. The elementary school years are a child’s “entrance into life.” In school, children begin to learn skills valued by society, and success or failure can affect a child’s feelings of adequacy. Children learn a sense of industry if they win praise for productive activities, such as building, painting, cooking, reading, and studying. If a child’s efforts are regarded as messy, childish, or inadequate, feelings of inferiority result. For the first time, teachers, classmates, and adults outside the home become as important as parents in shaping attitudes toward oneself. Stage Five, Adolescence: Identity versus Role Confusion As we have noted, adolescence is often a turbulent time. Erikson considers a need to answer the question “Who am I?” the primary task during this stage of life. As Samantha matures mentally and physically, she will have new feelings, a new body, and new attitudes. Like other adolescents, she will need to build a consistent identity out of her talents, values, life history, relationships, and the demands of her culture (Côté & Levine, 2002). Her conflicting experiences as a student, friend, athlete, worker, daughter, lover, and so forth must be integrated into a unified sense of self. Persons who fail to develop a sense of identity suffer from role confusion, an uncertainty about who they are and where they are going. Stage Six, Young Adulthood: Intimacy versus Isolation What does Erikson believe is the major conflict in early adulthood? In stage six, the individual feels a need for intimacy in his or her life. After establishing a stable identity, a person is prepared to Developmental task Any skill that must be mastered, or personal change that must take place, for optimal development. Psychosocial dilemma A conflict between personal impulses and the social world. Trust versus mistrust A conflict early in life about learning to trust others and the world. Autonomy versus shame and doubt A conflict created when growing self-control (autonomy) is pitted against feelings of shame and doubt. Initiative versus guilt A conflict between learning to take initiative and overcoming feelings of guilt about doing so. Industry versus inferiority A conflict in middle childhood centered around lack of support for industrious behavior, which can result in feelings of inferiority. Identity versus role confusion A conflict of adolescence, involving the need to establish a personal identity. 108 CHAPTER 3 share meaningful love or deep friendship with others. By intimacy, Erikson means an ability to care about others and to share experiences with them. In line with Erikson’s view, 75 percent of college-age men and women rank a good marriage and family life as important adult goals (Bachman & Johnson, 1979). And yet, marriage or sexual involvement is no guarantee of intimacy: Many adult relationships remain shallow and unfulfilling. Failure to establish intimacy with others leads to a deep sense of isolation (feeling alone and uncared for in life). This often sets the stage for later difficulties. a sense of integrity (self-respect). This allows them to face aging and death with dignity. If previous life events are viewed with regret, the elderly person experiences despair (heartache and remorse). In this case, life seems like a series of missed opportunities. The person feels like a failure and knows it’s too late to reverse what has been done. Aging and the threat of death then become sources of fear and depression. The Life Span in Perspective To squeeze a lifetime into a few pages, we had to ignore countless details. Although much is lost, the result is a clearer picture of an entire life cycle. Is Erikson’s description, then, an exact map of Samantha’s past and her future — or your own? Probably not. Still, psychosocial dilemmas are major events in many lives. Knowing about them may allow you to anticipate typical trouble spots in your own life. You may also be better prepared to understand the problems and feelings of friends and relatives at various points in the life cycle. Stage Seven, Middle Adulthood: Generativity versus Stagnation According to Erikson, an interest in guiding the next generation provides emotional balance in mature adulthood. Erikson called this quality generativity. It is expressed by caring about oneself, one’s children, and future generations. Generativity may be achieved by guiding one’s own children or by helping other children (as a teacher or coach, for example). Productive or creative work can also express generativity. In any case, a person’s concerns and energies must broaden to include the welfare of others and society as a whole. Failure to do this is marked by a stagnant concern with one’s own needs and comforts. Life loses meaning, and the person feels bitter, dreary, and trapped (Friedman, 2004). Later Adulthood: Will You Still Need Me When I’m 64? Gateway Question: What is involved in well-being during later adulthood? Although Erikson’s dilemmas extend into adulthood, they are not the only challenges adults face. Others are all too familiar: marital strife, divorce, career difficulties, unemployment, health problems, financial pressures, legal conflicts, and personal tragedies — to name but a few. How do people maintain a state of well-being as they run the gauntlet of modern life? Psychologist Carol Ryff (1995; Ryff, Singer, & Palmersheim, 2004) believes that wellbeing during adulthood has six elements: Stage Eight, Late Adulthood: Integrity versus Despair What does Erikson see as the conflicts of old age? Old age is a time of reflection. According to Erikson, when Samantha grows old she must be able to look back over her life with acceptance and satisfaction. People who have lived richly and responsibly develop Jeff Greenberg/PhotoEdit 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. According to Erikson, an interest in future generations characterizes optimal adult development. Self-acceptance Positive relations with others Autonomy (personal freedom) Environmental mastery A purpose in life Continued personal growth Ryff found that for many older adults, age-related declines are offset by positive relationships and greater mastery of life’s demands (Ryff & Keyes, 1995). Thus, sharing life’s joys and sorrows with others, coupled with a better understanding of how the world works, can help carry people through midlife and into their later years (Ryff & Singer, 2000; Ryff, Singer, & Palmersheim, 2004). Despite the emphasis on youth in our culture, middle age and beyond can be a rich period of life in which people feel secure, happy, and self-confident (Rubenstein, 2002). A Midlife Crisis? But don’t people face a “midlife crisis” at this point in their lives? Yes and no. Serious difficulties at the midpoint of life are certainly not universal. Most people thrive during middle adulthood and have Human Development no special problems. Only about a quarter of men and women believe they have experienced a midlife crisis (Wethington, Kessler, & Pixley, 2004). Nevertheless, midlife does pose special challenges. According to Roger Gould (1975), a psychiatrist interested in adult personality, North American adults actually experience two “crisis” points in their development. Crisis of Questions According to Gould, around the age of 30 many people experience a minor life crisis. The heart of this crisis is a serious questioning of what life is all about. People tend to ask themselves, “Is this it?” and confidence in previous choices and values can waver. Unsettled by these developments, the person actively searches for a style of living that will bring more meaning to life. Marriages are particularly vulnerable during this time of dissatisfaction. Extramarital affairs and divorces are common symptoms of the “crisis of questions.” Crisis of Urgency People in their late 30s and 40s are typically beginning to become more aware of the reality of death. Having a limited number of years to live begins to exert pressure on the individual. Intensified attempts are made to succeed at a career or to achieve one’s life goals. Generativity, in the form of nurturing, teaching, or serving others, helps alleviate many of the anxieties of this stage. If a midlife crisis does occur, what does it look like? Psychologist Daniel Levinson carried out a classic in-depth study of adulthood and identified several periods when people typically make major transitions. A transition period ends one life pattern and opens the door to new possibilities (Levinson, 1986). At such times, people address concerns about their identity, their work, and relationships to others. Levinson’s first study focused on men. As they approached the midlife transition (between the ages of 37 and 41), most men went through a period of instability, anxiety, and change. (Notice that this corresponds closely to Gould’s crisis-of-urgency period.) In a later study, Levinson found that most of what he learned about men also applies to women (Levinson & Levinson, 1996). Of the men Levinson studied, roughly half defined the midlife period as a “last chance” to achieve their goals. Such goals were often stated as a key event, such as reaching a certain income or becoming a supervisor, a full professor, a shop steward, and so forth. For these men the midlife period was stressful but manageable. A smaller percentage of men experienced a serious midlife decline. Many of these men had to face the fact that they had chosen a dead-end job or lifestyle. Others had achieved financial success but felt that what they were doing was pointless. In a third pattern, a few hardy individuals appeared to “break out” of a seriously flawed life structure. For them, a decision to “start over” was typically followed by 8 to 10 years of rebuilding. In what ways does the midlife transition differ for women? Compared with men, women were less likely to enter adulthood with specific “goals.” As a result, they were less likely to define “success” 109 in terms of some key event. Rather than focusing on external goals, women tended to seek changes in personal identity at midlife. For example, a woman might become more self-reliant and independent — qualities she might have regarded as “masculine” earlier in life (Levinson & Levinson, 1996). But make no mistake, midlife can be challenging for women, too (Wethington, 2000). In another survey of middle-aged women, two thirds said they made major changes in their lives between ages 37 and 43 (Stewart & Vandewater, 1999). Midcourse Corrections In summary, people tend to move through cycles of stability and transition in their adult lives (Ornstein & Isabella, 1990). However, it is more common to make a “midcourse correction,” at midlife than it is to survive a “crisis” (Lachman, 2004). Ideally, the midlife transition involves reworking old identities, achieving valued goals, finding one’s own truths, and preparing for old age. Taking stock may be especially valuable at midlife, but reviewing past choices to prepare for the future is helpful at any age (Lewchanin & Zubrod, 2001). For some people, difficult turning points in life can serve as “wake-up calls” that create opportunities for personal growth (Wethington, 2003). Old Age After the late 50s, personal development is complicated by physical aging. However, it is wrong to believe that most elderly people are sickly, infirm, or senile. (Nowadays, 60 is the new 40, an idea both of your authors whole-heartedly agree with!) Only about 5 percent of those older than 65 are in nursing homes. Mentally, many elderly persons are at least as capable as the average young adult. On intellectual tests, top scorers over the age of 65 match the average for men younger than 35. What sets these silver-haired stars apart? Typically they are people who have continued to work and remain intellectually active (Salthouse, 2004). Gerontologist Warner Schaie (1994, 2005) found that you are most likely to stay mentally sharp in old age if: 1. You remain healthy. 2. You live in a favorable environment. (You are educated and have a stimulating occupation, an above-average income, and an intact family.) 3. You are involved in intellectually stimulating activities (reading, travel, cultural events, continuing education, clubs, professional associations). Intimacy versus isolation The challenge of overcoming a sense of isolation by establishing intimacy with others. Generativity versus stagnation A conflict of middle adulthood in which self-interest is countered by an interest in guiding the next generation. Integrity versus despair A conflict in old age between feelings of integrity and the despair of viewing previous life events with regret. Transition period Time span during which a person leaves an existing life pattern behind and moves into a new pattern. 110 CHAPTER 3 Tony Ranze/AFP/Getty Images At age 77, John Glenn became the oldest person to fly into space, in October 1998. Glenn was also the first American astronaut to orbit Earth, in 1962. As Glenn’s space adventure shows, aging does not inevitably bring an end to engaging in challenging activities. 4. 5. 6. 7. You have a flexible personality. You are married to a smart spouse. You maintain your perceptual processing speed. You were satisfied with your accomplishments in midlife. A shorter summary of this list is “Those who live by their wit die with their wits.” Successful Aging What are the keys to successful aging? They are not unlike the elements of well-being at midlife. Four psychological characteristics shared by the healthiest, happiest older people are (de Leon, 2005; Vaillant, 2002): Optimism, hope, and an interest in the future Gratitude and forgiveness; an ability to focus on what is good in life Empathy; an ability to share the feelings of others and see the world through their eyes Connection with others; an ability to reach out, to give and receive social support Ageism is often expressed through patronizing language. Older people are frequently spoken to in an overly polite, slow, loud, and simple way implying that they are infirm, even when they are not (Nelson, 2005). Popular stereotypes of the “dirty old man,” “meddling old woman,” “senile old fool,” and the like also help perpetuate myths about aging. But such stereotypes are clearly wrong: A tremendous diversity exists among the elderly — ranging from the infirm to aerobic-dancing grandmothers. In many occupations, older workers perform well in jobs that require both speed and skill. Of course, people do experience a gradual loss of fluid abilities (those requiring speed or rapid learning) as they age but often this can be offset by many crystallized abilities (learned knowledge and skills), such as vocabulary and stored-up facts, which may actually improve — at least into the 60s (Schaie, 2005). Overall, very little loss of job performance occurs as workers grow older. In the professions, wisdom and expertise can usually more than compensate for any loss of mental quickness (Ericsson, 2000). Basing retirement solely on a person’s age makes little sense. Death and Dying — The Final Challenge Gateway Question: How do people typically react to death? DEAR ABBY: Do you think about dying much? (signed) CURIOUS DEAR CURIOUS: No, it’s the last thing I want to do. “I’m not afraid of dying. I just don’t want to be there when it happens.” Woody Allen “Why not? Why not?” LSD guru Timothy Leary (his last words before dying) The statistics on death are very convincing: Everyone dies. In spite of this, most of us are poorly informed about a process that is as basic as birth. We have seen throughout this chapter that it is valuable to understand major trends in the course of development. With this in mind, let us now explore emotional responses to death, the inevitable conclusion of every life. Aging and Ageism You have almost certainly encountered ageism in one way or another. Ageism, which refers to discrimination or prejudice based on age, can oppress the young as well as the old. For instance, a person applying for a job may just as well be told “You’re too young” as “You’re too old.” In some societies ageism is expressed as respect for the elderly. In Japan, for instance, aging is seen as positive, and greater age brings more status and respect. In most Western nations, however, ageism tends to have a negative impact on older individuals (Ng, 2002). © Michael Newman/PhotoEdit Actually, these are excellent guidelines for well-being at any stage of adulthood. In summary, enlightened views of aging call for an end to the forced obsolescence of the elderly. As a group, older people represent a valuable source of skill, knowledge, and energy that we can’t afford to cast aside. As we face the challenges of this planet’s uncertain future, we need all the help we can get! Death may be inevitable, but it can be faced with dignity and sometimes even humor. Mel Blanc’s famous sign-off, “That’s all folks,” is engraved on a marble headstone over his grave. Blanc was the voice of Bugs Bunny, Porky Pig, and many other cartoon characters. Human Development 111 It might seem that as people grow older they would fear death more. However, older persons actually have fewer death fears than younger people. Older people more often fear the circumstances of dying, such as pain or helplessness, rather than death itself (Thorson & Powell, 1990). These findings seem to show a general lack of death fears, but they may actually reflect a widespread denial of death. Notice how denial is apparent in the language used to talk about death: Often we speak of a dead person as having “passed away,” “expired,” “gone to God,” or “breathed one’s last.” Many people have little direct experience with death until they, themselves, are fairly old. The average person’s exposure to death consists of the artificial and unrealistic portrayals of death on TV. By the time the average person is 17 years old, she or he will have witnessed thousands of TV deaths. With few exceptions these are homicides, not deaths due to illness or aging. Not all terminally ill persons display all these reactions, nor do they always occur in this order. Individual styles of dying vary greatly. Generally, there does tend to be a movement from initial shock, denial, and anger toward eventual acceptance. However, some people who seem to have accepted death may die angry and raging against the inevitable. Conversely, the angry fighter may let go of the struggle and die peacefully. In general, one’s approach to dying will mirror his or her style of living (Yedidia & MacGregor, 2001). It is a mistake, then, to think that Kübler-Ross’s list is a series of stages to go through in order or that there is something wrong if a person does not show all these emotions. Rather, the list describes typical reactions to impending death. Note, as well, that many of the same reactions accompany any major loss, be it divorce, loss of a home due to fire, death of a pet, or loss of a job. Reactions to Impending Death Implications A direct and highly influential account of emotional responses to death comes from the work of Elisabeth Kübler-Ross (1926– 2004). Kübler-Ross was a thanatologist (THAN-ah-TOL-oh-jist: one who studies death). Over the years she spent hundreds of hours at the bedsides of the terminally ill, where she observed five basic emotional reactions to impending death (Kübler-Ross, 1975). 1. Denial and isolation. A typical first reaction is to deny death’s reality and isolate oneself from information confirming that death is really going to occur. Initially the person may be sure that “It’s all a mistake.” “Surely,” she or he thinks, “the lab reports have been mixed up or the doctor made an error.” This sort of denial may proceed to attempts to avoid any reminder of the situation. 2. Anger. Many dying individuals feel anger and ask, “Why me?” As they face the ultimate threat of having life torn away, their anger may spill over into rage toward the living. Even good friends may temporarily evoke anger because their health is envied. 3. Bargaining. In another common reaction, the terminally ill bargain with themselves or with God. The dying person thinks, “Just let me live a little longer and I’ll do anything to earn it.” Individuals may bargain for time by trying to be “good” (“I’ll never smoke again”), by righting past wrongs, or by praying that if they are granted more time they will dedicate themselves to their religion. 4. Depression. As death draws near and the person begins to recognize that it cannot be prevented, feelings of futility, exhaustion, and deep depression may set in. The person realizes she or he will be separated from friends, loved ones, and the familiar routines of life, and this causes a profound sadness. 5. Acceptance. If death is not sudden, many people manage to come to terms with dying and accept it calmly. The person who accepts death is neither happy nor sad, but at peace with the inevitable. Acceptance usually signals that the struggle with death has been resolved. The need to talk about death ends, and silent companionship from others is frequently all the person desires. How can I make use of this information? First, it can help both the dying and survivors to recognize and cope with periods of depression, anger, denial, and bargaining. Second, it helps to realize that close friends or relatives may feel many of the same emotions before or after a person’s death because they, too, are facing a loss. Perhaps the most important thing to recognize is that the dying person needs to share feelings with others and to discuss death openly. Too often, dying persons feel isolated and separated from others. Adults tend to “freeze up” with someone who is dying. For such people, thanatologist Kirsti Dyer (2001) has this advice: • • • • • • • Be yourself and relate person to person. Be ready to listen again and again. Be respectful. Be aware of feelings and nonverbal cues. Be comfortable with silence. Be genuine. Most of all, be there. Understanding what the dying person is going through may make it easier for you to offer support at this important time. A simple willingness to be with the person and to honestly share his or her feelings can help bring dignity, acceptance, and meaning to death (Holstein, 1997). Today, many terminally ill individuals also benefit from hospice care, which can improve the quality of life in a person’s final days. Hospices typically offer support, guidance, pain relief, and companionship. In short, the dying person is made comfortable and feels loved and respected (Lynn, 2001). As each of us faces the end of life, to die well may be no less an accomplishment than to live well. Ageism Discrimination or prejudice based on a person’s age. Thanatologist A specialist who studies emotional and behavioral reactions to death and dying. CHAPTER 3 K NOW LED G E B U I L D E R Challenges Across the Lifespan RECITE As a way to improve your memory, you might find it helpful to summarize Erikson’s eight life stages. Complete this do-it-yourself summary and compare your answers to those given below. ——————————————————————————————————— Stage Crisis Favorable Outcome ——————————————————————————————————— First year of life 1. ______________ vs. Faith in the environ2. ______________ ment and in others ——————————————————————————————————— Ages 1–3 Autonomy vs. Feelings of self3. ______________ control and adequacy ——————————————————————————————————— Ages 3–5 4. ______________ vs. Ability to begin one’s guilt own activities ——————————————————————————————————— Ages 6–12 Industry vs. Confidence in 5. ______________ productive skills, learning how to work ——————————————————————————————————— Adolescence 6. ______________ vs. An integrated image role confusion of oneself as a unique person ——————————————————————————————————— Young adulthood Intimacy vs. Ability to form bonds 7. ______________ of love and friendship with others ——————————————————————————————————— Middle adulthood Generativity vs. 8. ______________ Concern for family, society, and future generations ——————————————————————————————————— Late adulthood 9. ______________ vs. Sense of dignity and 10. __________________ fulfillment, willingness to face death 11. Nearly everyone experiences a midlife crisis sometime around age 40. T or F? 12. After age 65, a large proportion of older people show significant signs of mental disability and most require special care. T or F? 13. Job performance tends to decline rapidly in older workers. T or F? 14. In the reaction that Kübler-Ross describes as bargaining, the dying individual asks, “Why me?” T or F? REFLECT Critical Thinking 15. Trying to make generalizations about development throughout life is complicated by at least one major factor. What do you think it is? Relate See if you can think of a person you know who is facing one of Erikson’s psychosocial dilemmas. Now see if you can think of specific people who seem to be coping with each of the other dilemmas. See if you can describe three instances of ageism you have witnessed. Answers: 1. Trust 2. mistrust 3. shame or doubt 4. Initiative 5. inferiority 6. Identity 7. isolation 8. stagnation 9. Integrity 10. despair 11. F 12. F 13. F 14. F 15. Different cohorts (groups of people born in the same year) live in different historical times. People born in various decades may have very different life experiences. This makes it difficult to identify universal patterns (Stewart & Ostrove, 1998). 112 PSY C HO L O GY IN A CT ION Effective Parenting — Raising Healthy Children And yet, it is not easy to have a positive interaction while disciplining your child. This is one reason why overly permissive parents avoid disciplining their children. Effective discipline is created through communication that is fair but loving, authoritative yet sensitive. It socializes a child without destroying the bond of love and trust between parent and child. Children should feel free to express their deepest feelings. However, this does not mean they can do whatever they please. Rather, the child is allowed to move freely within consistent, well-defined boundaries for acceptable behavior. Effective Discipline Image 100/SuperStock Gateway Question: How do effective parents discipline and communicate with their children? Authoritative parents help their children grow up with a capacity for love, joy, fulfillment, responsibility, and self-control through positive parent-child interactions. Positive interactions occur when parents spend enjoyable time encouraging their children in a loving and mutually respectful fashion (Dinkmeyer, McKay, & Dinkmeyer, 1997). As any parent can tell you, it is all well and good to talk about positive interactions until little Johnny misbehaves (and he will, count on it!). As children mature and become more independent, parents must find ways to control their children’s behavior. (“No, you may not smear banana pudding on daddy’s face.”) When parents fail to provide discipline (guidance regarding acceptable behavior), children become antisocial, aggressive, and insecure. Parents typically discipline children in one of three ways. Power assertion refers to physical punishment or a show of force, such as taking away toys or privileges. As an alternative, some parents use withdrawal of love (withholding affection) by refusing to speak to a child, threatening to leave, rejecting the child, or otherwise acting as if the child is temporarily unlovable. Management techniques combine praise, recognition, approval, rules, reasoning, and the like to encourage desirable behavior. Each of these approaches can control a child’s behavior, but their side effects differ considerably. What are the side effects? Power-oriented techniques — particularly harsh or severe 113 Calvin and Hobbes © Watterson. Reprinted with permission of Universal Press Syndicate. All rights reserved. Human Development Calvin and Hobbes physical punishment — are associated with fear, hatred of parents, and a lack of spontaneity and warmth. Most children show no signs of long-term damage from spanking — if spanking is backed up by supportive parenting (Baumrind, Larzelere, & Cowan, 2002). However, emotional damage does occur if spankings are severe, frequent, or coupled with harsh parenting. In addition, frequent spanking tends to increase aggression, and it leads to more problem behaviors, not fewer (McLoyd & Smith, 2002). After reviewing many studies, psychologist Elizabeth Gershoff concludes that parents should minimize spanking or avoid it entirely (Gershoff, 2002). bridges Punishment also has important effects on learning. For more tips on how to use punishment wisely, see Chapter 7, pages 240–241. Withdrawal of love produces children who tend to be self-disciplined. You could say that such children have developed a good conscience. Often, they are described as “model” children or as unusually “good.” But as a side effect, they are also frequently anxious, insecure, and dependent on adults for approval. Management techniques also have limitations. Most important is the need to carefully adjust to a child’s level of understanding. Younger children don’t always see the connection between rules, explanations, and their own behavior. Nevertheless, management techniques receive a big plus in another area: There is a direct connection between discipline and a child’s self-esteem. How does discipline affect self-esteem? If you regard yourself as a worthwhile person, you have self-esteem. High self-esteem is essential for good emotional health. In elementary school, children with high self-esteem tend to be more popular, cooperative, and successful in class. Children with low self-esteem are more withdrawn and tend to perform below average (Amato & Fowler, 2002). Low self-esteem is related to physical punishment and the withholding of love. And why not? What message do children receive if a parent beats them or tells them they are not worthy of love? Thus, it is best to minimize physical punishment and avoid withdrawal of love. In contrast, high self-esteem is promoted by management techniques. Children who feel that their parents support them emotionally tend to have high self-esteem (Amato & Fowler, 2002; Nielsen & Metha, 1994). Can self-esteem ever get too high? Yes it can. According to clinical psychologist Maggie Mamen, many modern parents try to “empower” their children by imposing few limits on behavior, making them feel special, and giving them everything they want (Mamen, 2004). But such good intentions can backfire, leaving parents with children who have developed an artificially high level of self-esteem and a sense of entitlement. That is, overly permissive parenting produces spoiled, self-indulgent children who have little self-control (Baumrind, 1991). Their sense of entitlement can lead them to bully other children to get their way or even to engage in criminal activity. As adults, such children may become addicted to seeking ways to enhance their self-esteem. For example, they may place excessive importance on being physically attractive, leading to stress, drug and alcohol use, and eating disorders (Crocker & Park, 2004). Consistent Discipline. Individual parents choose limits on behavior that are more “strict” or less “strict.” But this choice is less important than consistency (maintaining stable rules of conduct). Consistent discipline gives a child a sense of security and stability. Inconsistency makes the child’s world seem insecure and unpredictable. What does consistent discipline mean in practice? To illustrate the errors parents often make, let’s consider a few examples of inconsistency (Fontenelle, 1989). The following are mistakes to avoid: Saying one thing and doing something else. You tell the child, “Bart, if you don’t eat your brussels sprouts you can’t have any dessert.” Then you feel guilty and offer him some dessert. Making statements you don’t mean. “If you don’t quiet down, I’m going to stop the car and make you walk home.” Changing no to yes, especially to quiet a nagging child. A good example is the parent who first refuses to buy the child a toy and later gives in and buys it. Responding differently to the same misbehavior. One day a child is sent to his room for fighting with his sister. The next day the fighting is overlooked. Inconsistency makes children feel angry and confused because they cannot control the consequences of their own behavior. Inconsistency also gives children the message: “Don’t believe what I say because I usually don’t mean it.” Power assertion The use of physical punishment or coercion to enforce child discipline. Withdrawal of love Withholding affection to enforce child discipline. Management techniques Combining praise, recognition, approval, rules, and reasoning to enforce child discipline. Self-esteem Regarding oneself as a worthwhile person; a positive evaluation of oneself. Consistency With respect to child discipline, the maintenance of stable rules of conduct. 114 CHAPTER 3 Using Discipline Constructively. At one time or another, most parents use power assertion, withdrawal of love, or management techniques to control their children. Each mode of discipline has its place. However, physical punishment and withdrawal of love should always be used with caution. Here are some guidelines: 1. Parents should separate disapproval of the act from disapproval of the child. Instead of saying, “I’m going to punish you because you are bad,” say, “I’m upset about what you did.” 2. State specifically what misbehavior you are punishing. Explain why you have set limits on this kind of conduct. 3. Punishment should never be harsh or injurious. Don’t physically punish a child while you are angry. Also remember that the message “I don’t love you right now” can be more painful and damaging than any spanking. 4. Punishment, such as a scolding or taking away privileges, is most effective when done immediately. This statement is especially true for younger children. 5. Spanking and other forms of physical punishment are not particularly effective for children younger than age 2. The child will only be confused and frightened. Spankings also become less effective after age 5 because they tend to humiliate the child and breed resentment. 6. As discussed earlier, many psychologists believe that children should never be spanked. If you do use physical punishment, reserve it for situations that pose an immediate danger to younger children; for example, when a child runs into the street. 7. Remember, too, that it is usually more effective to reward children when they are being good than it is to punish them for misbehavior. After age 5, management techniques are the most effective form of discipline, especially techniques that emphasize communication and the relationship between parent and child. Communicating Effectively with Children Creative communication is another important ingredient of successful child management (Bath, 1996). Child expert Haim Ginott (1965) believed that making a distinction between feelings and behavior is the key to clear communication. Because children (and parents, too) do not choose how they feel, it is important to allow free expression of feelings. Accepting Feelings. The child who learns to regard some feelings as “bad,” or unacceptable, is being asked to deny a very real part of his or her experience. Ginott encouraged parents to teach their children that all feelings are appropriate; it is only actions that are subject to disapproval. Consider this typical conversation excerpted from Ginott’s classic book (1965): Son: I am stupid, and I know it. Look at my grades in school. Father: You just have to work harder. Son: I already work harder and it doesn’t help. I have no brains. Father: You are smart, I know. Son: I am stupid, I know. Father: (loudly) You are not stupid! Son: Yes, I am! Father: You are not stupid. Stupid! By debating with the child, the father misses the point that his son feels stupid. It would be far more helpful for the father to encourage the boy to talk about his feelings. For instance, he might say, “You really feel that you are not as smart as others, don’t you? Do you feel this way often?” In this way, the child is given a chance to express his emotions and to feel understood. The father might conclude by saying, “Look, son, in my eyes you are a fine person. But I understand how you feel. Everyone feels stupid at times.” He might also offer some encouragement: “You’re improving. Look at the progress you’ve made.” I-Messages. Child psychologist Thomas Gordon (2000) believes that parents should send I-messages to their children, rather than you-messages. What’s the difference? You-messages take the form of threats, name-calling, accusing, bossing, lecturing, or criticizing. Generally, you-messages tell children what’s “wrong” with them. An I-message tells children what effect their behavior had on you. For example, after a hard day’s work, Maria wants to sit down and rest awhile. She begins to relax with a newspaper when her 5-year-old daughter starts banging loudly on a toy drum. Many parents would respond with a you-message such as “You go play outside this instant.” (bossing) or “Don’t you ever make such a racket when someone is reading.” (lecturing). Gordon suggests sending an I-message such as, “I am very tired, and I would like to read. I feel upset and can’t read with so much noise.” This forces the child to accept responsibility for the effects of her actions (Dinkmeyer, McKay, & Dinkmeyer, 1997). Using Natural and Logical Consequences. Sometimes events automatically discourage misbehavior. For example, a child who refuses to eat dinner will get uncomfortably hungry. A child who throws a temper tantrum may gain nothing but a sore throat and a headache if the tantrum is ignored (Fontenelle, 1989). In such instances, a child’s actions have natural consequences (intrinsic effects). In situations that don’t produce natural consequences, parents can set up logical consequences (rational and reasonable effects). For example, a parent might say, “We’ll go to the zoo when you’ve picked up all those toys,” or “You can play with your dolls as soon as you’ve taken your bath,” or “You two can stop arguing or leave the table until you’re ready to join us.” The concept of logical, parent-defined consequences can be combined with I-messages to handle many day-to-day instances of misbehavior. The key idea is to use an I-message to set up consequences and then give the child a choice to make: “Michelle, we’re trying to watch TV. You can settle down and watch with us or go play elsewhere. You decide which you’d rather do” (Dinkmeyer, McKay, & Dinkmeyer, 1997). How could Maria have dealt with her 5-year-old — the one who was banging on a drum? A response that combines an I-message with logical consequences would be, “I would like for you to stop banging on that drum; otherwise, please take it outside.” If the child continues to bang on the drum inside the house, then she has caused the toy to be put away. If she takes it outside, she has made a decision to play with the drum in a way that respects her mother’s wishes. In this way, both parent and child have been allowed to maintain a sense of self-respect and a needless clash has been averted. After you have stated consequences and let the child decide, be sure to respect the child’s choice. If the child repeats the misbehavior, you can let the consequences remain in effect longer. But later, give the child another chance to cooperate. With all child management techniques, remember to be firm, kind, consistent, respectful, and encouraging. And most of all, try every day to live the message you wish to communicate. Human Development Effective Parenting RECITE 1. Effective discipline gives children freedom within a structure of consistent and well-defined limits. T or F? 2. One good way to maintain consistency in child management is to overstate the consequences for misbehavior. T or F? 3. Spankings and other physical punishments are most effective for children under the age of 2. T or F? 4. Giving recognition for progress and attempts to improve is an example of parental _____________________________________. 5. I-messages are a gentle way of accusing a child of misbehavior. T or F? 6. In situations where natural consequences are unavailable or do not discourage misbehavior, parents should define logical consequences for a child. T or F? REFLECT Critical Thinking 7. Several Scandinavian countries have made it illegal for parents to spank their own children. Does this infringe on the rights of parents? chapter in review 8. If power assertion is a poor way to discipline children, why do so many parents use it? Relate What do you think are the best ways to discipline children? How would your approach be classified? What are its advantages and disadvantages? Parents can probably never be completely consistent. Think of a time when your parents were inconsistent in disciplining you. How did it affect you? Think of a you-message you have recently given a child, family member, roommate, or spouse. Can you change it into an I-message? Answers: 1. T 2. F 3. F 4. encouragement 5. F 6. T 7. Such laws are based on the view that it should be illegal to physically assault any person, regardless of their age. Although parents may believe they have a “right” to spank their children, it can be argued that children need special protection because they are small, powerless, and dependent (Durrant & Janson, 2005). 8. Most parents discipline their children in the same ways that they themselves were disciplined. Parenting is a responsibility of tremendous importance, for which most people receive almost no training. K N O WL E D GE B U I L D E R 115 Gateways to Human Development Heredity (nature) and environment (nurture) are interacting forces that are both necessary for human development. However, caregivers can only influence environment. Although neonates will die if not cared for at birth they are far from helpless. They possess adaptive reflexes, are responsive to their senses, begin to learn immediately, and are aware of their actions. • The chromosomes and genes in each cell of the body carry hereditary instructions. Most characteristics are polygenic and reflect the combined effects of dominant and recessive genes. • Maturation of the body and nervous system underlies the orderly development of motor skills, cognitive abilities, emotions, and language. • Many early skills are subject to the principle of readiness. • Prenatal development is influenced by environmental factors, such as various teratogens, including diseases, drugs, and radiation, as well as the mother’s diet, health, and emotions. • During sensitive periods in development, infants are more sensitive to specific environmental influences. • Early perceptual, intellectual, or emotional deprivation seriously retards development, whereas deliberate enrichment of the environment has a beneficial effect on infants. • In general, environment sets a reaction range within which maturation unfolds. • Temperament is hereditary. Most infants fall into one of three temperament categories: easy children, difficult children, and slow-to-warm-up children. • A child’s developmental level reflects heredity, environment, and the effects of the child’s own behavior. • Infant development is strongly influenced by heredity. However, environmental factors such as nutrition, parenting, and learning are also important. • The human neonate has a number of adaptive reflexes, including the grasping, rooting, sucking, and Moro reflexes. • Tests in a looking chamber reveal a number of visual preferences in the newborn. The neonate is drawn to bright lights and circular or curved designs. • Infants prefer human face patterns, especially familiar faces. In later infancy, interest in the unfamiliar emerges. • The rate of maturation varies from person to person. Also, learning contributes greatly to the development of basic motor skills. You-message Threatening, accusing, bossing, lecturing, or criticizing another person. I-message A message that states the effect someone else’s behavior has on you. Natural consequences The effects that naturally tend to follow a particular behavior. Logical consequences Reasonable consequences that are defined by parents. 116 CHAPTER 3 • Emotions develop in a consistent order, starting with generalized excitement in newborn babies. Three of the basic emotions — fear, anger, and joy — may be unlearned. Early social development lays a foundation for relationships with parents, siblings, friends, and relatives and the emotional attachment of human infants is a critical early event. • Opportunities for social interaction increase as infants develop self-awareness and they begin to actively seek guidance from adults. • Infant attachment is reflected by separation anxiety. The quality of attachment can be classified as secure, insecure-avoidant, or insecure-ambivalent. • Secure attachment is fostered by consistent care from parents who are sensitive to a baby’s signals and rhythms. • High-quality day care is not harmful and can even be helpful to preschool children. Low-quality care can be risky. • Meeting a baby’s affectional needs is as important as meeting needs for physical care. Studies suggest that parental styles have a substantial impact on emotional and intellectual development. • Three major parental styles are authoritarian, permissive, and authoritative (effective). Authoritative parenting appears to benefit children the most. • Whereas mothers typically emphasize caregiving, fathers tend to function as playmates for infants. Both caregiving styles contribute to the competence of young children. • The ultimate success of various parenting styles depends on what culture or ethnic community a child will enter. • Parenting styles vary across cultures. Language development proceeds from crying to cooing, then babbling, the use of single words, and then to telegraphic speech. • Learning to use language is a cornerstone of early intellectual development. • The underlying patterns of telegraphic speech suggest a biological predisposition to acquire language. This innate tendency is augmented by learning. • Prelanguage communication between parent and child involves shared rhythms, nonverbal signals, and turn-taking. • Motherese or parentese is a simplified, musical style of speaking that parents use to help their children learn language. The intellect of a child is less abstract than that of an adult. Jean Piaget theorized that children mature through a fixed series of cognitive stages by applying a combination of assimilation and accommodation. Lev Vygotsky emphasized the role of interactions with more competent partners. • Piaget held that children mature through a fixed series of cognitive stages. The stages and their approximate age ranges are sensorimotor (0–2), preoperational (2–7), concrete operational (7–11), and formal operations (11–adult). • Caregivers should offer learning opportunities that are appropriate for a child’s level of cognitive development. • Learning principles provide an alternate explanation that assumes cognitive development is continuous; it does not occur in stages. • Recent studies of infants under the age of 1 year suggest that they are capable of thought well beyond that observed by Piaget. • Lev Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory emphasizes that a child’s mental growth takes place in a child’s zone of proximal development, where a more skillful person may scaffold the child’s progress. As children rely on adults to help them discover new skills and principles they learn cultural beliefs and values. Adolescents must form their identity and values at a time when they are also dealing with puberty and transition to adulthood is occurring at ever-later ages. • The timing of puberty can complicate the task of identity formation, a major task of adolescence. • Transitioning from childhood to adulthood requires the formation of a personal identity, the major life task of adolescence. • Identity formation is even more challenging for adolescents of ethnic descent. • In Western industrialized societies the transition into adulthood is further complicated as it is increasingly delayed well into the 20s. As in other types of development, we develop morals and values as we grow through several levels. • Lawrence Kohlberg identified preconventional, conventional, and postconventional levels of moral reasoning. • Developing mature moral standards is also an important task of adolescence. • Most people function at the conventional level of morality, but some never get beyond the selfish, preconventional level. Only a minority of people attain the highest, or postconventional level, of moral reasoning. • Carol Gilligan distinguished between Kohlberg’s justice perspective and a caring perspective. Mature adult morality likely involves both. Erik Erikson identified a series of challenges that occur across the lifespan. These range from a need to gain trust in infancy to the need to live with integrity in old age. • Personal development does not end after adolescence. Periods of stability and transition occur throughout adulthood. • We face a specific “crisis,” or psychosocial dilemma, at each life stage. • Successful resolution of the dilemmas produces healthy development, whereas unsuccessful outcomes make it harder to deal with later crises. Human Development Well-being during adulthood consists of six elements: self-acceptance, positive relations with others, autonomy, environmental mastery, having a purpose in life, and continued personal growth. • Physical aging starts early in adulthood. Every adult must find ways to successfully cope with aging. • Only a minority of people have a midlife crisis, but midlife course corrections are more common. • Even if no crisis occurs, people tend to move through repeated cycles of stability and transition throughout adulthood. • Intellectual declines associated with aging are limited, at least through one’s 70s. This is especially true of individuals who remain mentally active. • Successful lives are based on happiness, purpose, meaning, and integrity. • Ageism refers to prejudice, discrimination, and stereotyping on the basis of age. It affects people of all ages but is especially damaging to older people. Most ageism is based on stereotypes, myths, and misinformation. Typical emotional reactions to impending death include denial, anger, bargaining, depression, and acceptance, but not necessarily in that order or in every case. 117 Web Resources For an up-to-date list of direct links to interesting sites, including those listed here, visit the student companion site for this book at www.cengage.com/psychology/coon Human Genome Project Learn more about your human genetic heritage. Crack Babies A photostory by Ken Kobre. Scaffolding as a Teaching Strategy Download a paper on the application of Vygotsky’s idea of scaffolding to teaching. A Positive Approach to Identity Formation of Biracial Children Join the debate about multiethnicity and identity formation. Delayed Adulthood Read two articles about delayed adulthood. Kohlberg Dilemmas Try your hand at answering several moral dilemmas. Hospice A website of information about death, bereavement, and hospices. Discipline: Logical & Natural Consequences Read about ways to effectively structure discipline. • Death is a natural part of life. There is value in understanding it and accepting it. Positive parent-child interactions occur when parents spend enjoyable time encouraging their children in a loving and mutually respectful fashion. • Effective parental discipline tends to emphasize child management techniques (especially communication), rather than power assertion or withdrawal of love. • Consistency is also an important aspect of effective parenting. • Effective parents allow their children to express their feelings but place limits on their behavior. • Much misbehavior can be managed by use of I-messages and the application of natural and logical consequences. Interactive Learning Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior Book Companion Website www.cengage.com/psyc hology/coon Visit your book companion website, where you will find flash cards, practice quizzes, web links, and more to help you study. Just what you need to know NOW! Spend time on what you need to master rather than on information you already have learned. Take a pre-test for this chapter, and CengageNOW will generate a personalized study plan based on your results. The study plan will identify the topics you need to review and direct you to online resources to help you master those topics. You can then take a post-test to help you determine the concepts you have mastered and what you will need to work on. Try it out! Go to www.cengage.com.com/login to sign in with an access code or to purchase access to this product. CHAPTER 4 Sensation and Reality Gateway Theme Sensory systems link us to the external world and shape the flow of information to the brain. Tim Flach/Getty Images Gateway Questions • In what ways are our senses limited? • How does the visual system function? • How do we perceive colors? • How do we adjust to the dark? • What are the mechanisms of hearing? 118 • • • • How do the chemical senses operate? What are the somesthetic senses? Why are we more aware of some sensations than others? How can pain be reduced in everyday situations? preview Can’t You Hear the Bats? One of your authors once stood at dusk at the mouth of a huge limestone cave on the island of Borneo. For some 20 minutes, fruit bats streamed out of the cave until a dense cloud of half a million bats swirled overhead in the gathering darkness. Besides the sheer spectacle, what most struck him was the silence. Aside from the occasional whisper of bat wings swishing by his ear, he heard nothing. And yet every single bat was shouting — at a pitch so high a human being couldn’t hear it. Echoes from those shouts allowed the bats to avoid colliding with each other. At that moment, he was keenly aware of the limitations of his senses. Do you imagine that the ear is like a digital recorder or the eye is like a camera, giving us a completely accurate “picture” of reality? Perhaps you already understand that this view greatly oversimplifies how we sense and perceive the world. Certainly, as our bat story shows, the senses do not capture complete information about the environment. While this may seem to be some sort of evolutionary oversight, just the opposite is the case. We collect enough sensory information to survive while reducing the amount of sensory information bombarding the brain. This selectivity helps prevent the brain from overloading. Regardless, at this very moment you are bathed in a swirling kaleidoscope of electromagnetic radiation, heat, pressure, vibrations, molecules, and mechanical forces. Without the senses, all of this would seem like nothing more than a void of silence and darkness. The next time you drink in the beauty of a sunset, a flower, or a friend, remember this: Sensation makes it all possible. Psychophysics — The Limits of Sensibility tions into sound waves. Pluck a string and the guitar will produce a sound. However, stimuli that don’t cause the string to move will have no effect. For instance, if you shine a light on the string, or pour cold water on it, the guitar will remain silent. (The owner of the guitar, however, might get quite loud at this point!) Thus, the eye transduces electromagnetic radiation, the ear transduces sound waves, and so on. Many other types of stimuli cannot be sensed directly because we have no sense receptors to transduce their energy. For example, humans cannot sense the bioelectric fields of other living beings, but sharks have special organs that can (Fields, 2007). (Do they hear the fields or feel them or what?) Similarly, humans can transduce only visible light, which is a tiny slice of the electromagnetic spectrum (entire spread of electromagnetic wavelengths). The spectrum also includes infrared and ultraviolet light, radio waves, television broadcasts, gamma rays, and other energies (look ahead at Figure 4.3). In contrast, the eyes of honeybees transduce parts of the electromagnetic spectrum invisible to humans. As you can see, our rich sensory experience is only a small part of what could be sensed and what some animals can sense. Gateway Question: In what ways are our senses limited? Physical energy, in the form of light waves or heat or sound waves, stimulates your senses. An instant later you see a snowball whiz past your nose or feel the warmth of the sun on your face or hear a catchy tune on the radio. In that instant, a remarkable series of events will have transpired as you detect, analyze, and interpret sensory information. In an approach called psychophysics, physical energy (such as sound waves or electromagnetic radiation) is measured and related to dimensions of the resulting sensations we experience (such as loudness or brightness). Psychophysical investigations have revealed much about our sensory capacities and their limits. Our eyes, for example, provide us with stunningly wide access to the world. In one instant you can view a star light-years away, and in the next, you can peer into the microscopic universe of a dewdrop. Yet, vision also dramatically narrows what we can observe. Like the other senses, vision acts as a data reduction system. Your senses send only the most important data to your brain (Sekuler & Blake, 2006). Before we examine specific senses in more detail, let’s explore how the senses reduce the amount of information the brain must process. • Absolute Thresholds Before you can experience a sensation, a stimulus (physical energy) must be above a certain minimum intensity. The necessary minimum defines the absolute threshold for a sensory system. For Transduction How does data reduction take place? To begin, our senses limit what we can experience because they do not transduce all the physical energies surrounding us. Sensory receptors, such as the eyes and ears, are biological transducers, devices that convert one kind of energy into another (Fain, 2003). For example, a guitar transduces string vibra- Psychophysics Study of the relationship between physical stimuli and the sensations they evoke in a human observer. Transducers Devices that convert one kind of energy into another. Absolute threshold The minimum amount of physical energy necessary to produce a sensation. 119 CHAPTER 4 Table 4.1 • Absolute Thresholds Sensory Modality Absolute Threshold Vision Candle flame seen at 30 miles on a clear dark night Hearing Tick of a watch under quiet conditions at 20 feet Taste 1 teaspoon of sugar in 2 gallons of water Smell 1 drop of perfume diffused into a three-room apartment Touch A bee’s wing falling on your cheek from 1 centimeter above © Robert Rattner 120 From Galanter, 1962. example, very soft sounds (which could be heard if they were just a little louder) fall below the absolute threshold for human hearing. Of course, owls have much lower absolute thresholds for hearing, which allows them to hunt at night. What is the quietest sound that humans can hear? The weakest light that we can see? The lightest touch that we can feel? Testing for absolute thresholds shows just how sensitive we are. For example, it only takes three photons of light striking visual receptors at the back of the eye to produce a sensation. A photon (FOE-tahn: one quantum of energy) is the smallest possible “package” of light. Responding to three photons is like seeing a candle flame 30 miles away! Table 4.1 gives approximate absolute thresholds for the five major senses. Some sensory systems have upper limits as well as lower ones. For example, if we test for pitch sensitivity (higher and lower tones), we find that humans can hear sounds down to 20 hertz (vibrations per second) and up to about 20,000 hertz. This is an impressive range — from the lowest rumble of a pipe organ to the highest squeak of a stereo “tweeter.” On the lower end, the threshold is as low as practical. If your ears could sense tones below 20 hertz, you would hear the movements of your own muscles. Imagine how disturbing it would be to hear your body creak and groan like an old ship as you move. The 20,000 hertz upper threshold for human hearing, on the other hand, could easily be higher. Bats, dogs, cats, and other animals can hear sounds well above this limit. That’s why a “silent” dog whistle (which may make sounds as high as 40,000 to 50,000 hertz) can be heard by dogs but not by humans. For dogs the sound exists. For humans it is beyond awareness. It’s easy to see how thresholds define the limits of the sensory world in which we live. (If you want to buy a stereo system for your dog, you will have a hard time finding one that reproduces sounds above 20,000 hertz!) Wouldn’t the absolute threshold be different for different people? Not only do absolute thresholds vary for different people, they also change from time to time for a single person. The type of stimulus, the state of your nervous system, and the costs of false “detections” all make a difference (Goldstein, 2007). Emotional factors are also important. Unpleasant stimuli, for example, may raise the threshold for recognition. Is “subliminal” perception possible? Yes, under limited circumstances. Anytime information is processed below the normal limen (LIE-men: threshold or limit) for awareness, it is subliminal. Subliminal perception was demonstrated by a study in which college • Absolute thresholds define the sensory worlds of humans and animals, sometimes with serious consequences. The endangered Florida manatee (“sea cow”) is a peaceful, plant-eating creature that can live for more than 60 years. For the last decade, the number of manatees killed by boats has climbed alarmingly. The problem? Manatees have poor sensitivity to the low-frequency sounds made by slow-moving boats. Current laws require boats to slow down in manatee habitats, which may actually increase the risk to these gentle giants (Gerstein, 2002). students saw photos of a person flashed on a screen. Each time before the face appeared, it was preceded by a subliminal image that was shown for just a fraction of a second. Some were images that made viewers feel good (such as cute kittens). Others made them feel bad (for example, a face on fire). All the emotional images were flashed too briefly to be recognized. Nevertheless, they altered the impressions students formed of the target person (Krosnick et al., 1992). Apparently, some emotional impact gets through, even when a stimulus is below the level of conscious awareness (Arndt, Allen, & Greenberg, 2001). To find out if such effects could be applied to advertising, read “Subliminal Seduction or Subliminal Myths?” Difference Thresholds Psychophysics also involves the study of difference thresholds. Here we are asking, “How much must a stimulus change (increase or decrease) before it becomes just noticeably different?” The study of just noticeable differences (JNDs) led to one of psychology’s first natural “laws.” Weber’s (VAY-bears) law can be roughly stated as follows: The amount of change needed to produce a JND is a constant proportion of the original stimulus intensity. Here are some Weber’s proportions for common judgments: Pitch Weight Loudness Taste 1/333 (1/3 of 1 percent) 1/50 (2 percent) 1/10 (10 percent) 1/5 (20 percent) Notice how much more sensitive hearing is than taste. Very small changes in pitch and loudness are easy to detect. If a voice or a musical instrument is off pitch by 1/3 of 1 percent, you’ll probably notice it. For taste, we find that a 20-percent change is necessary to produce a JND. If a cup of coffee has 5 teaspoons of sugar in it, you will have to add 1 more (1/5 of 5) before it will be noticeably sweeter. If you’re salting soup, it takes a lot of cooks to spoil the broth! Sensation and Reality 121 CRIT ICA L T H I N KI N G Subliminal Seduction or Subliminal Myth? performed no better than students who just listened to relaxing ocean sounds without subliminal messages and students who listened to nothing at all (Russell, Rowe, & Smouse, 1991). In this instance, subliminal messages had no effect, even when people wanted to be influenced. In most cases, people who think they have been helped by subliminal messages have likely experienced nothing more than a placebo effect (Froufe & Schwartz, 2001). However, before writing off subliminal advertising altogether, let’s consider the work of psychologist Johan Karremans and his colleagues (Karremans, Stroebe, & Claus, 2006). They suggest that only simple messages, such as single words, can be processed subliminally, which is why subliminal self-help materials are usually ineffective. They also provide evidence that subliminally flashing the brand name of a drink can increase the likelihood that people will buy it, but only if they are already thirsty. Perhaps subliminal advertising can work under limited circumstances. Nevertheless, advertisers are still better off using the loudest, clearest, most attention-demanding stimuli available — as most do (Smith & Rogers, 1994). © Michael Maslin from cartoon bank.com. All Rights Reserved. Could subliminal perception ever be used against us? The sensationalistic book Subliminal Seduction (Key, 1973) voiced popular fears of attempts to influence us through subliminal messages embedded in advertising. But could it work? In a famous early attempt, a New Jersey theater flashed the words Eat popcorn and Drink Coca-Cola on the screen for 1/3,000 second every 5 seconds during movies. Dramatic claims that popcorn and Coke sales increased as a result later turned out to be falsehoods. The advertising “expert” responsible admitted he faked the whole thing. By lying about his ability to control audiences, he had hoped to gain customers for his marketing business (Pratkanis, 1992; Shrum, 2004). Regardless, the possibility of subliminal seduction was so horrifying that such advertising was banned in the United States, Britain, and Australia (Karremans, Stroebe, & Claus, 2006). Despite these fears, some businesses actually sell subliminal messages to people who want them to work. Each year, consumers spend many millions of dollars on so-called subliminal self-help tapes and CDs (Pratkanis & Aronson, 2001). “Subliminal messages” embedded in relaxing music or the soothing sounds of ocean waves purportedly influence “subconscious motivation” to help listeners lose weight, relieve pain, find romance, succeed financially, improve grades, and so forth. In one study, students who listened to subliminal messages meant to improve their study habits, and, hence, their grades, Sensory Analysis and Coding What we experience is greatly influenced by sensory analysis. As they process information, the senses divide the world into important perceptual features (basic stimulus patterns). For vision, such features include lines, shapes, edges, spots, colors, and other patterns (Hubel & Wiesel, 2005). Look at Figure 4.1 and notice how eye-catching the single vertical line is among a group of slanted lines. This effect, which is called pop-out, occurs because your visual system is highly sensitive to perceptual features such as line orientation (Adler & Orprecio, 2006). In some instances, the senses act as feature detectors because they are attuned to very specific stimuli. Frog eyes, for example, are highly sensitive to small, dark, moving spots. In other words, • Subliminal perception Perception of a stimulus below the threshold for conscious recognition. Difference threshold A change in stimulus intensity that is detectable to an observer. Just noticeable difference (JND) Any noticeable difference in a stimulus. Weber’s law The just noticeable difference is a constant proportion of the original stimulus intensity. Sensory analysis Separation of sensory information into important elements. Perceptual features Basic elements of a stimulus, such as lines, shapes, edges, or colors. 122 CHAPTER 4 including pressure — into visual features. As a result, you experience light sensations, not pressure. Also important in producing this effect is sensory localization in the brain. Sensory localization means that the type of sensation you experience depends on which brain area is activated. Some brain areas receive visual information, others receive auditory information, and still others receive taste or touch. (See Chapter 2, pages 63–67.) Knowing which brain areas are active tells us, in general, what kinds of sensations you are feeling. Sensory localization makes it possible to artificially restore sight, hearing, or other senses. For example, in July 2006, a woman named Cheri Robinson became the sixteenth person in the world with an implant that allows a miniature television camera to send electrical signals to her brain’s visual cortex ( Figure 4.2). She can now “see” 100 dots of light. Like a sports scoreboard, these lights can be used to form crude letters (Warren & Normann, 2005). Eventually, increasing the number of dots could make reading and “seeing” large objects, such as furniture and doorways, possible. It is fascinating to realize that “seeing” and “hearing” take place in the brain, not in the eye or ear. Information arriving from the sense organs creates sensations. When the brain organizes sensations into meaningful patterns, we speak of perception, which will be covered in Chapter 5. It’s now time to examine each of the senses in more detail. In the next section, we will begin with vision, which is perhaps the most magnificent sensory system of all. Before you read more, it might be a good idea to stop and review some of the ideas we have covered. • Figure 4.1 Visual pop-out. Pop-out is so basic that babies as young as 3 •months respond to it. (Adapted from Adler & Orprecio, 2006.) they are basically “tuned” to detect bugs flying nearby (Lettvin, 1961). But the insect (spot) must be moving, or the frog’s “bug detectors” won’t work. A frog could starve to death surrounded by dead flies. After they have selected and analyzed information, sensory systems must code it. Sensory coding refers to changing important features of the world into messages understood by the brain (Hubel & Wiesel, 2005). To see coding at work, try closing your eyes for a moment. Then take your fingertips and press firmly on your eyelids. Apply enough pressure to “squash” your eyes slightly. Do this for about 30 seconds and observe what happens. (Readers with eye problems or contact lenses should not try this.) Did you “see” stars, checkerboards, and flashes of color? These are called phosphenes (FOSS-feens: visual sensations caused by mechanical excitation of the retina). They occur because the eye’s receptor cells, which normally respond to light, are also somewhat sensitive to pressure. Notice though, that the eye is only prepared to code stimulation — KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R Sensation and Psychophysics RECITE 1. Sensory receptors are biological ___________________, or devices for converting one type of energy to another. 2. The minimum amount of stimulation necessary for a sensation to occur defines the ___________________ ______________________. Continued Visual cortex Electrodes Actual image Cameras Perceived image • Figure 4.2 An artificial visual system. Sensation and Reality INVISIBLE LONG WAVES VISIBLE LIGHT SPECTRUM INVISIBLE SHORT WAVES Infrared rays (beyond red) 1500 1000 123 Ultraviolet rays (beyond violet) 700 Radio 600 TV Microwaves 500 Infrared 400 U-V X-rays 300 Gamma Cosmic rays rays • Figure 4.3 The visible spectrum. 3. Subliminal stimuli have been shown to have an effect on the behavior of viewers. T or F? 4. Lettvin found that a frog’s eyes are especially sensitive to phosphenes. T or F? 5. Important features of the environment are transmitted to the brain through a process known as a. phosphenation b. coding c. detection d. programming REFLECT Critical Thinking 6. Is a doorbell a transducer? 7. If the human ear were more sensitive than it is now, our hearing would be impaired. How could this be true? 8. When promoters of self-help “subliminal tapes” are challenged to provide evidence that their products work, what study do you think they most often cite? Relate How does sensation affect what you are experiencing right now? What if data reduction didn’t occur? What if you could transduce other energies? What if your senses were tuned to detect different perceptual features? What if your absolute thresholds were higher or lower for each sense? How would the sensory world you live in change? Vision — Catching Some Rays Gateway Question: How does the visual system function? In the morning when you awaken and open your eyes you effortlessly become aware of the visual richness of the world around you. But the ease with which normally sighted people can see conceals incredible complexity. Vision is an impressive sensory system, worthy of a detailed discussion. What are the basic dimensions of light and vision? As we have noted, various wavelengths of light make up the visible spectrum (the narrow spread of electromagnetic energies to which the eyes respond). Visible light starts at “short” wavelengths of 400 nanometers (nan-OM-et-er: one billionth of a meter), which we sense as purple or violet. Longer light waves produce blue, green, yellow, orange, and red, which has a wavelength of 700 nanometers ( Figure 4.3). The term hue refers to the basic color categories of red, orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo, and violet. As just noted, various hues (or color sensations) correspond to the wavelength of the light that reaches our eyes (Sekuler & Blake, 2006). White light, in contrast, is a mixture of many wavelengths. Hues (colors) from a narrow band of wavelengths are very saturated, or “pure.” (An • Sensory coding Codes used by the sense organs to transmit information to the brain. Sensation A sensory impression; also, the process of detecting physical energies with the sensory organs. Perception The mental process of organizing sensations into meaningful patterns. Visible spectrum The narrow spread of the electromagnetic spectrum to which the eyes are sensitive. Answers: 1. transducers 2. absolute threshold 3. T 4. F 5. b 6. In a broad sense, it is. The button converts mechanical energy from your finger into an electrical signal that is converted again into mechanical energy in order to strike a bell; the physical vibrations of the bell then produce sound waves that are transduced into nerve impulses by the ears of the person in the house. 7. Under ideal conditions, vibrations of the eardrum as small as one billionth of a centimeter (one tenth the diameter of a hydrogen atom) can be heard. Therefore, if your ears were more sensitive, they would convert the random movement of air molecules into a constant roaring or hissing noise. 8. Good guess! That’s right, it’s still the faked “Eat popcorn/drink Coca-Cola” study (Pratkanis, 1992). 124 CHAPTER 4 intense “fire-engine” red is more saturated than a muddy “brick” red.) A third dimension of vision, brightness, corresponds roughly to the amplitude, or height of light waves. Waves of greater amplitude are “taller,” carry more energy, and cause the colors we see to appear brighter or more intense. For example, the same “brick” red would look bright under intense, high-energy illumination and drab under dim light. Structure of the Eye Although the visual system is much more complex than any digital camera, both cameras and eyes have a lens to focus images on a light-sensitive layer at the back of a closed space. In a camera, this layer is the digital image sensor. In the eye, it is a layer of photoreceptors (light-sensitive cells) in the retina, an area about the size and thickness of a postage stamp ( Figure 4.4). How does the eye focus? Most focusing is done at the front of the eye by the cornea, a clear membrane that bends light inward. The lens makes additional, smaller adjustments. Your eye’s focal point changes when muscles attached to the lens alter its shape. This • Ciliary muscle Aqueous humor Fovea Blind spot Iris Pupillary opening Optic nerve Cornea process is called accommodation. In cameras, focusing is done more simply — by changing the distance between the lens and the image sensor. Visual Problems Focusing is also affected by the shape of the eye. If your eye is too short, nearby objects will be blurred, but distant objects will be sharp. This is called hyperopia (HI-per-OPE-ee-ah: farsightedness). If your eyeball is too long, images fall short of the retina and you won’t be able to focus distant objects. This results in myopia (my-OPE-ee-ah: nearsightedness). When the cornea or the lens is misshapen, part of vision will be focused and part will be fuzzy. In this case, the eye has more than one focal point, a problem called astigmatism (ah-STIG-mah-tiz-em). All three visual defects can be corrected by reshaping the cornea through laser eye surgery or by placing glasses (or contact lenses) in front of the eye to change the path of light ( Figure 4.5). As people age, the lens becomes less flexible and less able to accommodate. The result is presbyopia (prez-bee-OPE-ee-ah: old vision, or farsightedness due to aging). Perhaps you have seen a grandparent or older friend reading a newspaper at arm’s length because of presbyopia. If you now wear glasses for nearsightedness, you may need bifocals as you age. ( Just like your authors. Sigh.) Bifocal lenses correct near vision and distance vision. • Light Control There is one more major similarity between the eye and a camera. In front of the lens in both is a mechanism that controls the amount of light entering. In the eye, this mechanism is the iris; in a camera, it is the diaphragm ( Figure 4.6). The iris is a colored circular muscle that gives your eyes their blue or brown or green color. By expanding and contracting, the iris changes the size of the pupil (the opening at the center of the eye). With some cameras, you can adjust the sensitivity of the digital image sensor. The sensitivity of the retina also changes in brighter • Retinal arteries and veins Lens Retina • Figure 4.4 The human eye, a simplified view. Point of focus Distant point Near point Misshapen cornea Misshapen lens Part of image is focused; part is not Concave lens (a) Nearsighted Eye • Figure 4.5 Convex lens (b) Farsighted Eye Nonsymmetrical lens (c) Astigmatic Eye Visual defects and corrective lenses: (a) A myopic (longer than usual) eye. The concave lens spreads light rays just enough to increase the eye’s focal length. (b) A hyperopic (shorter than usual) eye. The convex lens increases refraction (bending), returning the point of focus to the retina. (c) An astigmatic (lens or cornea not symmetrical) eye. In astigmatism, parts of vision are sharp and parts are unfocused. Lenses to correct astigmatism are unsymmetrical. Sensation and Reality 125 • 120 million, can’t detect colors ( Figure 4.7). Pure rod vision is black and white. However, the rods are much more sensitive to light than the cones are. The rods therefore allow us to see in very dim light. Notice in Figure 4.7 that light does not fall directly on the rods and cones. It must pass through the outer layers of the retina. Note, too, that the rods and cones face the back of the eye! Only Figure 4.6 The iris and diaphragm. about one half of the light falling on the front of the eye ever reaches the rods and cones — testimony to the eye’s amazing light sensitivity. Surprisingly, the retina has a “hole” in it: Each eye has a blind Direction of light spot because there are no receptors where the optic nerve passes out of the eye and blood vessels enter ( Figure 4.7). The blind spot shows that vision depends greatly on the brain. If you close one eye, part of what you see will fall on the blind spot of your open eye ( Figure 4.8a). Why isn’t there a gap in your vision? The answer is that the visual cortex of the brain actively fills in the gap with patterns from Fibers of the optic nerve surrounding areas ( Figure 4.8b). By closing one eye, you can visually “behead” other peoGanglion cell ple by placing their images on your blind Amacrine cell Retina spot. (Just a hint for some classroom fun.) The brain can also “erase” distracting information. Roll your eyes all the way to the Bipolar right and then close your right eye. You neuron Horizontal should clearly see your nose in your cell left eye’s field of vision. Now, open Retina your right eye again and your nose Photoreceptor will nearly disappear as your brain cells: Cone disregards its presence. Optic Rod nerve It’s tempting to think of vision Pigment layer as a movie-like projection of “picof retina tures” to the brain. However, this mistaken notion Choroid layer immediately raises the question, “Who’s watching Sclera the movie?” From the retina on, vision becomes quite Figure 4.7 Anatomy of the retina. The retina lies behind the vitreous humor, unlike a digital camera, which merely records and displays the which is the jelly-like substance that fills the eyeball. The rods and cones are much digital images it has captured. Instead, vision is a complex system • • • • • Omikron/Photo Researchers, Inc. n of ses ctio l Dire e impu nerv • smaller than implied here. The smallest are 1 micron (one millionth of a meter) wide. The lower-left photograph shows rods and cones as seen through an electron microscope. In the photograph the cones are colored green and the rods blue. Retina The light-sensitive layer of cells at the back of the eye. or dimmer light, but the retina can only adapt slowly. By making rapid adjustments, the iris allows us to move quickly from darkness to bright sunlight, or the reverse. In dim light the pupils dilate (enlarge), and in bright light they constrict (narrow). When the iris is wide open, the pupil is 17 times larger than at its smallest. Were it not for this, you would be blinded for some time after walking into a darkened room. Accommodation Changes in the shape of the lens of the eye. Hyperopia Difficulty focusing nearby objects (farsightedness). Myopia Difficulty focusing distant objects (nearsightedness). Astigmatism Defects in the cornea, lens, or eye that cause some areas of vision to be out of focus. Presbyopia Farsightedness caused by aging. Iris Circular muscle that controls the amount of light entering the eye. Rods and Cones Pupil The opening at the front of the eye through which light passes. The eye has two types of “image sensors,” consisting of receptor cells called rods and cones (Goldstein, 2007). The 5 million cones in each eye work best in bright light. They also produce color sensations and fine details. In contrast, the rods, numbering about Cones Visual receptors for colors and daylight visual acuity. Rods Visual receptors for dim light that produce only black and white sensations. Blind spot An area of the retina lacking visual receptors. 126 CHAPTER 4 4.8 Experiencing the blind spot. •(a)Figure With your right eye closed, stare at the upper-right cross. Hold the book about 1 foot from your eye and slowly move it back and forth. You should be able to locate a position that causes the black spot to disappear. When it does, it has fallen on the blind spot. With a little practice you can learn to make people or objects you dislike disappear too! (b) Repeat the procedure described, but stare at the lower cross. When the white space falls on the blind spot, the black lines will appear to be continuous. This may help you understand why you do not usually experience a blind spot in your visual field. (a) (b) for analyzing patterns of light. Thanks to the Nobel Prize– winning work of biopsychologists David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel, we now know that vision acts more like an incredibly advanced computer than a television or movie camera. Hubel and Wiesel directly recorded the activities of single cells in the brain’s visual cortex in cats and monkeys. As they did, they noted the area of the retina to which each cell responded. Then they aimed lights of various sizes and shapes at the retina and recorded how often the corresponding brain cell fired nerve impulses ( Figure 4.9a). The results were fascinating. Many brain cells responded only to lines of a certain width or orientation. These same cells didn’t get the least bit “excited” over a dot of light or overall illumination ( Figure 4.9b). Other cells responded only to lines at certain angles, or lines of certain lengths, or lines moving in a particular direction (Hubel & Wiesel, 1979). The upshot of such findings is that cells in the brain, like the frog’s retina described earlier, act as feature detectors. The brain seems to first analyze information into lines, angles, shading, movement, and other basic features. Then, other brain areas combine these features into meaningful visual experiences. (This concept is discussed further in Chapter 5.) Reading this page is a direct result of such feature analysis. Given the size of the task, it’s little wonder that as much as 30 percent of the human brain may be involved in vision. (To further follow the pathways visual infor- • • mation takes through the brain, see “Blindsight: The ‘What’ and the ‘Where’ of Vision.”) Visual Acuity The rods and cones also affect visual acuity, or sharpness. The cones lie mainly at the center of the eye. In fact, the fovea (FOEvee-ah: a small cup-shaped area in the middle of the retina) contains only cones — about 50,000 of them. Like a newspaper photograph made of many small dots, the tightly packed cones in the fovea produce the sharpest images. Normal acuity is designated as 20/20 vision: At 20 feet in distance, you can distinguish what the average person can see at 20 feet ( Figure 4.11). If your vision is 20/40, you can only see at 20 feet what the average person can see at 40 feet. If your vision is 20/200, everything is a blur and you need glasses! Vision that is 20/12 would mean that you can see at 20 feet what the average person must be 8 feet nearer to see, indicating better than average acuity. American astronaut Gordon Cooper, who claimed to see railroad lines in northern India from 100 miles above, had 20/12 vision. • Peripheral Vision What is the purpose of the rest of the retina? Areas outside the fovea also get light, creating a large region of peripheral (side) vision. The rods are most numerous about 20 degrees from the center of the retina, so much of our peripheral vision is rod vision. Although Receptive field for single cell in cortex Total visual field 4.9 (a) A “typical” brain •cellFigure responds to only a small area of the total field of vision. In this example, the cell responds to stimuli that fall above and left of the center of vision. The bar graph (b) illustrates how a brain cell may act as a feature detector. Notice how the cell primarily responds to just one type of stimulus. (Adapted from Hubel, 1979.) Center of vision (a) Frequency of nerve impulses Stimulus High Low Test stimuli (b) Sensation and Reality 127 B RAIN W AVES Blindsight: The “What” and the “Where” of Vision was, but she had enough sight to know where it was in her visual field (James et al., 2003). What patients like D. F. teach us is that the brain has assigned the job of seeing to different brain regions (Deco, Rolls, & Horowitz, 2004). One series of regions, the ventral pathway, is responsible for the “what” of vision, whereas another series of regions, the dorsal pathway, is responsible for the “where” of vision ( Figure 4.10). D. F. suffered damage in her ventral pathway so she could not process the “what” of vision, but her intact dorsal pathway could still process the “where” of vision. • pathway Ventral t) (Wha B (a) K • Figure 4.10 The ventral and dorsal The rods are also highly responsive to dim light. Because most rods are 20 degrees to each side of the fovea, the best night vision comes from looking next to an object you wish to see. Test this yourself some night by looking at, and next to, a very dim star. C E (b) S Primary visual cortex visual pathways. F L What happens if someone suffers brain damage to the dorsal pathway? In a rare case, a woman with just such damage had great difficulty crossing the street. Although she had no trouble recognizing cars (the what), she could not tell where they were. She could not even distinguish approaching cars from parked cars (Zeki, 1991). way ath ) lp sa here or D (W As you read this book, you may find yourself wondering why psychologists are so interested in the brain. Brainwaves boxes like this one are designed to help you think about how the biopsychological perspective contributes to a better understanding of human behavior. Meet a woman we will call D. F., who suffered brain damage that caused severe visual agnosia (Goodale et al., 1991). If D. F. was shown an object, she could not recognize it. Remarkably, even though she couldn’t recognize objects, D. F. could successfully manipulate them. For example, in one test she was given a card and asked to insert it into a slot at a certain angle. Although she could not describe the slot’s orientation, she had no difficulty inserting the card into it. You could say that D. F. displayed blindsight: When shown an object, she was blind to what the object T (c) 4.11 Tests of visual acuity. Here are some common tests of visual acu•ity.Figure In (a) sharpness is indicated by the smallest grating still seen as individual lines. The Snellen chart (b) requires that you read rows of letters of diminishing size until you can no longer distinguish them. The Landolt rings (c) require no familiarity with letters. All that is required is a report of which side has a break in it. rod vision is not very sharp, the rods are quite sensitive to movement in peripheral vision. To experience this characteristic of the rods, look straight ahead and hold your hand beside your head, at about 90 degrees. Wiggle your finger and slowly move your hand forward until you can detect motion. You will become aware of the movement before you can actually “see” your finger. Seeing “out of the corner of the eye” is important for sports, driving, and walking down dark alleys. People who suffer from tunnel vision (a loss of peripheral vision) feel as if they are wearing blinders (Godnig, 2003). Tunnel vision can also occur temporarily when we are overloaded by a task. For example, if you were playing a demanding video game you might be excused for not noticing that a friend had walked up beside you. Color Vision — There’s More to It Than Meets the Eye Gateway Question: How do we perceive colors? What would you say is the brightest color? Red? Yellow? Blue? Actually, there are two answers to this question, one for the rods and one for the cones. The cones are most sensitive to the yellowish green part of the spectrum. In other words, if all colors are tested in daylight (with each reflecting the same amount of light), then yellowish green appears brightest. Yellow-green fire trucks and the bright yellow vests worn by roadside work crews are a reflection of this fact. To what color are the rods most sensitive? Remember that the rods do not produce color sensations. If you were looking at a very dim colored light, you wouldn’t see any color. Even so, one light would appear brighter than the others. When tested this way, the rods are most sensitive to blue-green lights. Thus, at night or in dim light, when rod vision prevails, the brightest-colored light will be one that Visual acuity The sharpness of visual perception. Fovea An area at the center of the retina containing only cones. Peripheral vision Vision at the edges of the visual field. 128 CHAPTER 4 is blue or blue-green. For this reason, police and highway patrol cars in many states now have blue emergency lights for night work. Also, you may have wondered why the taxiway lights at airports are blue. It seems like a poor choice, but blue is actually highly visible to pilots. Color Theories How do the cones produce color sensations? The trichromatic (TRYkro-MAT-ik) theory of color vision holds that there are three types of cones, each most sensitive to either red, green, or blue. Other colors result from combinations of these three. A basic problem with the trichromatic theory is that four colors of light — red, green, blue, and yellow — seem to be primary (you can’t get them by mixing other colors). Also, why is it impossible to have a reddish green or a yellowish blue? These problems led to the development of a second view, known as the opponent-process theory, which states that vision analyzes colors into “either-or” messages (Goldstein, 2007). That is, the visual system can produce messages for either red or green, yellow or blue, black or white. Coding one color in a pair (red, for instance) seems to block the opposite message (green) from coming through. As a result, a reddish green is impossible, but a yellowish red (orange) can occur. According to opponent-process theory, fatigue caused by making one response produces an afterimage of the opposite color as the system recovers. Afterimages are visual sensations that persist after a stimulus is removed—like seeing a spot after a flashbulb goes off. To see an afterimage of the type predicted by opponent-process theory, look at Figure 4.12 and follow the instructions there. Which color theory is correct? Both! The three-color theory applies to the retina, where three different types of cone have been found. Each contains a different type of iodopsin (i-oh-DOP-sin), a light-sensitive pigment that breaks down when struck by light. This triggers action potentials and sends neural messages to the brain. The three types of cones are most sensitive to red, green, or blue. Other colors result from combinations of these three. (Black and white sensations are produced by the rods, which contain a • chemical called rhodopsin (row-DOP-sin), another light-sensitive visual pigment.) As predicted, each form of iodopsin is most sensitive to light in roughly the red, green, or blue region. The three types of cones fire nerve impulses at different rates to produce various color sensations ( Figure 4.13). In contrast, the opponent-process theory better explains what happens in optic pathways and the brain after information leaves the eye. For example, some nerve cells in the brain are excited by the color red and inhibited by the color green. So both theories are “correct.” One explains what happens in the eye itself. The other explains how colors are analyzed after messages leave the eye (Gegenfurtner & Kiper, 2003). • Constructing Colors The preceding explanations present a fairly mechanical view of how colors are sensed. In reality, color experiences are more complex. For example, the apparent color of an object is influenced by the colors of other nearby objects. This effect is called simultaneous color contrast. It occurs because brain cell activity in one area of the cerebral cortex can be altered by activity in nearby areas. Simultaneous contrast can make it difficult to paint a picture or decorate a room. If you add a new color to a canvas or a room, all the existing colors will suddenly look different. Typically, each time a new color is added, all the other colors must be adjusted ( Figure 4.14). More striking than simultaneous contrast is the fact that color experiences are actively constructed in the brain. The brain does not simply receive prepackaged color messages. It must generate color from the data it receives. As a result, it is possible to experience color where none exists. (See Figure 4.15 for an example.) Indeed, all our experiences are at least partially constructed from the information surrounding us. (We’ll explore this idea further later in this chapter.) • • Firing rates of cones Color experienced Blue Green Red Yellow Orange Purple White • Figure 4.12 Negative afterimages. Stare at the dot near the middle of the flag for at least 30 seconds. Then look immediately at a plain sheet of white paper or a white wall. You will see the American flag in its normal colors. Reduced sensitivity to yellow, green, and black in the visual system, caused by prolonged staring, results in the appearance of complementary colors. Project the afterimage of the flag on other colored surfaces to get additional effects. B G R 4.13 Firing rates of blue, green, and red cones in response to differ•entFigure colors. The taller the colored bar, the higher the firing rates for that type of cone. As you can see, colors are coded by differences in the activity of all three types of cones in the normal eye. (Adapted from Goldstein, 2007.) Sensation and Reality 129 (a) (b) 4.14 Notice how different the gray-blue color looks when it is placed •onFigure different backgrounds. Unless you are looking at a large, solid block of color, simultaneous contrast is constantly affecting your color experiences. Michael Newman/PhotoEdit (c) 4.15 On the left is a “star” made of red lines. On the right, the red lines •areFigure placed on top of longer black lines. Now, in addition to the red lines, you will see a glowing red disk, with a clear border. Of course, no red disk is printed on this page. No ink can be found between the red lines. The glowing red disk exists only in your mind. (After Hoffman, 1999, p. 111.) Color Blindness and Color Weakness Do you know anyone who regularly draws hoots of laughter by wearing clothes of wildly clashing colors? Or someone who sheepishly tries to avoid saying what color an object is? If so, you probably know someone who is color blind. What is it like to be color blind? What causes color blindness? A person who is color blind cannot perceive colors. It is as if the world is a black-and-white movie. The color-blind person either lacks cones or has cones that do not function normally (Deeb, 2004). Such total color blindness is rare. In color weakness, or partial color blindness, a person can’t see certain colors. Approximately 8 percent of Caucasian males are red-green color blind (but fewer Asian American, African American, and Native American males, and less than 1 percent of women, are) (Delpero et al., 2005). These people see both reds and greens as the same color, usually a yellowish brown ( Figure 4.16). Another type of color • • Figure 4.16 Color blindness and color weakness. (a) Photograph illustrates normal color vision. (b) Photograph is printed in blue and yellow and gives an impression of what a red-green color-blind person sees. (c) Photograph simulates total color blindness. If you are totally color blind, all three photos will look nearly identical. weakness, involving yellow and blue, is extremely rare (Hsia & Graham, 1997). (See “Are You Color Blind?”) Color blindness is caused by changes in the genes that control red, green, and blue pigments in the cones. Red-green color weak- Trichromatic theory Theory of color vision based on three cone types: red, green, and blue. Opponent-process theory Theory of color vision based on three coding systems (red or green, yellow or blue, black or white). Simultaneous color contrast Changes in perceived hue that occur when a colored stimulus is displayed on backgrounds of various colors. Color blindness A total inability to perceive colors. Color weakness An inability to distinguish some colors. 130 CHAPTER 4 D ISC O VERIN G P S YCH OLOG Y Are You Color Blind? How can I tell if I am color blind? Surprisingly, it is not as obvious as you might think; some of us reach adulthood without knowing. The Ishihara test is commonly used to measure color blindness and weakness. In the test, numbers and other designs made of dots are placed on a background also made of Figure 4.17 A replica of the •Ishihara test for color blindness. • dots ( Figure 4.17). The background and the numbers are of different colors (red and green, for example). A person who is color blind sees only a jumble of dots. If you have normal color vision you can detect the numbers or designs (Birch & McKeever, 1993; Coren, Ward, & Enns, 2004). The chart below • Figure 4.17 lists what people with normal color vision and color blindness see. Because this chart is just a replica, it is not a definitive test of color blindness. Nevertheless, if you can’t see all the embedded designs, you may be color blind or color weak. Sensation and Reality ness is a recessive, sex-linked trait. That means it is carried on the X, or female, chromosome. Women have two X chromosomes, so if they receive only one defective color gene, they still have normal vision. Color-weak men, however, have only one X chromosome, so they can inherit the defect from their mothers (who usually don’t display any color weakness). How can color-blind individuals drive? Don’t they have trouble with traffic lights? Red-green color-blind individuals have normal vision for yellow and blue, so the main problem is telling red lights from green. In practice, that’s not difficult. The red light is always on top, and the green light is brighter than the red. Also, “red” traffic signals have yellow light mixed in with the red and a “green” light that is really blue-green. Dark Adaptation — Let There Be Light! Gateway Question: How do we adjust to the dark? What happens when the eyes adjust to a dark room? Dark adaptation is the dramatic increase in retinal sensitivity to light that occurs after a person enters the dark (Goldstein, 2007). Consider walking into a theater. If you enter from a brightly lighted lobby, you practically need to be led to your seat. After a short time, however, you can see the entire room in detail (including the couple kissing over in the corner). It takes about 30 to 35 minutes of complete darkness to reach maximum visual sensitivity ( Figure 4.18). At that point, your eye will be 100,000 times more sensitive to light. What causes dark adaptation? Remember that both rods and cones contain light-sensitive visual pigments. When struck by light, visual pigments bleach, or break down chemically. The afterimages you have seen after looking at a flashbulb are a result of this bleaching. To restore light sensitivity, the visual pigments must recombine, which takes time. Night vision is due mainly to an increase in rhodopsin, the rod pigment. When completely dark adapted, the human eye is almost as sensitive to light as the eye of an owl. Low • Sensitivity to light Rods only High Cones only 0 5 10 15 20 25 Time in the dark (minutes) 30 As you might have noticed, a few seconds of exposure to bright white light can completely wipe out dark adaptation. That’s why you should be sure to avoid looking at oncoming headlights when you are driving at night — especially the new bluish-white xenon lights. Under normal conditions, glare recovery takes about 20 seconds, plenty of time for an accident. After a few drinks, it may take 30 to 50 percent longer because alcohol dilates the pupils, allowing more light to enter. Note, too, that dark adaptation occurs more slowly as we grow older. This is one reason why injuries caused by falling in the dark become more common among the elderly ( Jackson, Owsley, & McGwin, 1999). Is there any way to speed up dark adaptation? The rods are insensitive to extremely red light. That’s why submarines, airplane cockpits, and ready rooms for fighter pilots are illuminated with red light. In each case, people can move quickly into the dark without having to adapt. Because the red light doesn’t stimulate the rods, it is as if they had already spent time in the dark. Can eating carrots really improve vision? One chemical “ingredient” of rhodopsin is retinal, which the body makes from vitamin A. (Retinal is also called retinene.) When too little vitamin A is available, less rhodopsin is produced. Thus, a person lacking vitamin A may develop night blindness. In night blindness, the person can see normally in bright light while using the cones, but becomes blind at night when the rods must function. Carrots are an excellent source of vitamin A, so they could improve night vision for someone suffering a deficiency, but not the vision of anyone with an adequate diet (Carlson, 2005). KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R Vision RECITE 1. The __________________ ___________________ is made up of electromagnetic radiation with wavelengths between 400 and 700 nanometers. 2. Hyperopia is related to a. farsightedness b. having an elongated eye c. corneal astigmatism d. lack of cones in the fovea 3. Hubel and Wiesel found that cells in the visual cortex of the brain function as ________________________ ______________________. 4. In dim light, vision depends mainly on the ____________________. In brighter light, color and fine detail are produced by the ____________________. 5. The fovea has the greatest visual acuity due to the large concentration of rods found there. T or F? 6. The term “20/20 vision” means that a person can see at 20 feet what can normally be seen from 20 feet. T or F? Continued Figure 4.18 Typical course of dark adaptation. The black line shows how the •threshold for vision lowers as a person spends time in the dark. (A lower threshold means that less light is needed for vision.) The green line shows that the cones adapt first, but they soon cease adding to light sensitivity. Rods, shown by the red line, adapt more slowly. However, they continue to add to improved night vision long after the cones are fully adapted. 131 Dark adaptation Increased retinal sensitivity to light. Night blindness Blindness under conditions of low illumination. 132 CHAPTER 4 D ISC O VERIN G P S YCH OLOG Y Felicia Rule Going Biosonar Although Ben Underwood has been blind since the age of 3, he has learned to use echolocation to do many things that normally sighted children take for granted. This chapter opened with a story about echolocation (also known as biosonar), the remarkable ability of bats to use the echoes of their own voices to judge distance. Not human at all, right? Don’t tell that to teenager Ben Underwood, who has been sightless since the age of 3, when retinal cancer claimed his eyes (Engber, 2006). In 2006, Ben proudly proclaimed during a television interview, “I’m not blind, I just can’t see.” Sure enough, Ben can ride a bike, climb trees, skate, and even play video games. He does it by using echolocation. It turns out that bat echolocation is especially powerful because bats use very highpitched sounds. But any sounds will do, and Ben makes clicking sounds with his tongue. With practice he has learned to use his own echoes to navigate through the world. Although Ben discovered echolocation all by 7. For the cones, the most visible color is a. reddish orange b. blue-green c. yellow-orange d. yellowish green 8. The eyes become more sensitive to light at night because of a process known as ________________________ _________________________. REFLECT Critical Thinking 9. William James once said, “If a master surgeon were to cross the auditory and optic nerves, we would hear lightning and see thunder.” Can you explain what James meant? 10. Sensory transduction in the eye takes place first in the cornea, then in the lens, then in the retina. T or F? Relate Pretend you are a beam of light. What will happen to you at each step as you pass into the eye and land on the retina? What will happen if the eye is not perfectly shaped? How will the retina know you’ve arrived? How will it tell what color of light you are? What will it tell the brain about you? himself (!), it turns out that researchers have known about human echolocation for 50 years and have even proposed training blind people to echolocate (Kellogg, 1962). Although blind people may be better at echolocating, there is no reason why the rest of us can’t do it as well (Rosenblum, Gordon, & Jarquin, 2000). Try this: Blindfold yourself and have a friend move a large plate or pan closer or farther away from you. All the while, make some noise. Click like Ben does or sing a song or whistle. Don’t expect to hear your own echo as a separate sound. Noticeable echoes only occur when sounds bounce off objects far away. Instead, expect to hear slight differences when an object is moved. With some practice, you can tell when the plate or pan is closer and when it is farther away. Congratulations, you’ve gone biosonar. Hearing — Good Vibrations Gateway Question: What are the mechanisms of hearing? Rock, classical, jazz, rap, country, electronic, hip-hop — whatever your musical taste, you have probably been moved by the riches of sound. Hearing also collects information from all around the body, such as detecting the approach of an unseen car (Yost, 2007). Vision, in all its glory, is limited to stimuli in front of the eyes (unless, of course, your “shades” have rearview mirrors attached). Compression Rarefaction Amplitude Compression Wavelength Rarefaction Time 4.19 Waves of compression in the air, or vibrations, are the stimulus •forFigure hearing. The frequency of sound waves determines their pitch. The amplitude determines loudness. Answers: 1. visible spectrum 2. a 3. feature detectors 4. rods, cones 5. F 6. T 7. d 8. dark adaptation 9. The explanation is based on localization of function: If a lightning flash caused rerouted messages from the eyes to activate auditory areas of the brain, we would experience a sound sensation. Likewise, if the ears transduced a thunderclap and sent impulses to the visual area, a sensation of light would occur. 10. False. The cornea and lens bend and focus light rays, but they do not change light to another form of energy. No change in the type of energy takes place until the retina converts light to nerve impulses. Sensation and Reality External Ear (air conduction) Auditory canal 133 • Figure 4.20 Anatomy of the ear. The entire ear is a mechanism for changing waves of air pressure into nerve impulses. The inset in the foreground (Cochlea “Unrolled”) shows that as the stapes moves the oval window, the round window bulges outward, allowing waves to ripple through fluid in the cochlea. The waves move membranes near the hair cells, causing cilia or “bristles” on the tips of the cells to bend. The hair cells then generate nerve impulses carried to the brain. (See an enlarged cross section of cochlea in Figure 4.21.) Inner Ear (fluid conduction) (bone conduction by ossicles) Vestibular apparatus Incus Malleus Stapes Auditory nerve • Cochlea Scala vestibuli (with perilymph) Pinna Cochlear canal (with endolymph) Round window Oval window Tympanic membrane (eardrum) Stapes Scala tympani (with perilymph) Cochlea in Cross Section Oval window waves collide with the tympanic membrane (eardrum), setting it in motion. This, in turn, Cochlear causes three small bones (the auditory ossicles) canal (OSS-ih-kuls) to vibrate ( Figure 4.20). The ossicles are the malleus (MAL-ee-us), Perilymph Round Cochlea “Unrolled” (fluid inside cochlea) window incus, and stapes (STAY-peas). Their common names are the hammer, anvil, and stirrup. The ossicles link the eardrum with the What is the stimulus for hearing? If you throw cochlea (KOCK-lee-ah: a snail-shaped organ a stone into a quiet pond, a circle of waves will that makes up the inner ear). The stapes is spread in all directions. In much the same way, attached to a membrane on the cochlea Basilar Auditory nerve Hair cells membrane sound travels as a series of invisible waves of com- fibers called the oval window. As the oval window Organ of Corti pression (peaks) and rarefaction (RARE-eh-fakmoves back and forth, it makes waves in a shun: valleys) in the air. Any vibrating object — a fluid inside the cochlea. tuning fork, the string of a musical instrument, or the vocal Inside the cochlea tiny hair cells detect waves in the fluid. The cords — will produce sound waves (rhythmic movement of air hair cells are part of the organ of Corti (KOR-tee), which makes molecules). (To learn how to use sound waves to act like a bat, read up the center part of the cochlea ( Figure 4.21). A set of stereocilia (STER-ee-oh-SIL-ih-ah), or “bristles,” atop each hair cell “Going Biosonar.”) Other materials, such as fluids or solids, can brush against the tectorial membrane when waves ripple through also carry sound. But sound does not travel in a vacuum or the airfluid surrounding the organ of Corti. As the stereocilia are bent, less realm of outer space. Movies that show characters reacting to transduction takes place and nerve impulses are triggered, which the “roar” of alien starships or titanic battles in deep space are in then flow to the brain. (Are your ears “bristling” with sound?) error. The frequency of sound waves (the number of waves per secHow are higher and lower sounds detected? The frequency theond) corresponds to the perceived pitch (higher or lower tone) of ory of hearing states that as pitch rises, nerve impulses of a correa sound. The amplitude, or physical “height,” of a sound wave tells sponding frequency are fed into the auditory nerve. That is, an how much energy it contains. Psychologically, amplitude corre800-hertz tone produces 800 nerve impulses per second. (Hertz sponds to sensed loudness (sound intensity) ( Figure 4.19). • • • How We Hear Sounds How are sounds converted to nerve impulses? Hearing involves an elaborate chain of events that begins with the pinna (PIN-ah: the visible, external part of the ear). In addition to being a good place to hang earrings or balance pencils, the pinna acts like a funnel to concentrate sounds. After they are guided into the ear canal, sound Hair cells Receptor cells within the cochlea that transduce vibrations into nerve impulses. Organ of Corti Center part of the cochlea, containing hair cells, canals, and membranes. Frequency theory Holds that tones up to 4,000 hertz are converted to nerve impulses that match the frequency of each tone. 134 CHAPTER 4 Stereocilia • Dr. G. Oran Bredberg/SPL/Photo Researchers Hair cells Basilar membrane Figure 4.21 A closer view of the hair cells shows how movement of fluid in the cochlea causes the bristling “hairs” or cilia to bend, generating a nerve impulse. Oval window Stapes Round window High frequency Basilar membrane Medium frequency Figure 4.23 A highly magnified electron microscope photo of the cilia •(orange bristles) on the top of human hair cells. (Colors are artificial.) For example, the eardrums or ossicles may be damaged or immobilized by disease or injury. In many cases, conductive hearing loss can be overcome with a hearing aid, which makes sounds louder and clearer. Sensorineural hearing loss results from damage to the inner ear hair cells or auditory nerve. The hair cells, which are about as thin as a cobweb, are very fragile ( Figure 4.23). By the time you are 65, more than 40 percent of them will be gone, mainly those that transduce high pitches (Chisolm, Willott, & Lister, 2003). This explains why younger students are beginning to download very high-pitched ring tones for their cell phones: If their teacher has aging ears, the students can hear the ring tone while their teacher cannot. (Your authors may have experienced this effect without knowing it!) Many jobs, hobbies, and pastimes can cause noise-induced hearing loss, a common form of sensorineural hearing impairment that occurs when very loud sounds damage hair cells (as in hunter’s notch). If you work in a noisy environment or enjoy loud music, motorcycling, snowmobiling, hunting, or similar pursuits, you are risking noise-induced hearing loss. Consider taking precautions because dead hair cells are never replaced: When you abuse them, you lose them. So sensorineural hearing loss cannot be treated? Not easily, because hearing aids are of little or no help. However, in many cases of sensorineural hearing loss, the auditory nerve is actually intact. This finding has spurred the development of cochlear implants that bypass hair cells and stimulate the auditory nerves directly. As you can see in Figure 4.24, wires from a microphone carry electrical signals to an external coil. A matching coil under the skin picks up the signals and carries them to one or more areas of the cochlea. The latest implants make use of place theory to separate higher and lower tones. This has allowed some formerly deaf persons to hear human voices, music, and other higher-frequency sounds. About 60 percent of all multichannel implant patients can understand some spoken words and appreciate music (Leal et al., 2003; Tye-Murray, Spencer, & Woodworth, 1995). Some deaf • Low frequency • Figure 4.22 Here we see a simplified side view of the cochlea “unrolled.” Remember that the basilar membrane is the elastic “roof” of the lower chamber of the cochlea. The organ of Corti, with its sensitive hair cells, rests atop the basilar membrane. The colored line shows where waves in the cochlear fluid cause the greatest deflection of the basilar membrane. (The amount of movement is exaggerated in the drawing.) Hair cells respond most in the area of greatest movement, which helps identify sound frequency. refers to the number of vibrations per second.) This explains how sounds up to about 4,000 hertz reach the brain. But what about higher tones? Place theory states that higher and lower tones excite specific areas of the cochlea. High tones register most strongly at the base of the cochlea (near the oval window). Lower tones, on the other hand, mostly move hair cells near the narrow outer tip of the cochlea ( Figure 4.22). Pitch is signaled by the area of the cochlea most strongly activated. Place theory also explains why hunters sometimes lose hearing in a narrow pitch range. “Hunter’s notch,” as it is called, occurs when hair cells are damaged in the area affected by the pitch of gunfire. • Hearing Loss What causes other types of hearing loss? The two most common types of hearing loss afflict some 278 million people worldwide (Tennesen, 2007). Conductive hearing loss occurs when the transfer of vibrations from the outer ear to the inner ear is weak. • Sensation and Reality Typical Decibel Level el Skin Internal coil External coil Dangerous Time Exposure 180 Receiver circuitry 135 Examples Space Shuttle launch 170 160 Cochlea Hearing loss certain 150 140 Eardrum Electrode to cochlea Extremely loud 130 Any exposure dangerous Immediate danger 80 70 Average automobile 110 Very loud • Figure 4.24 A cochlear implant, or “artificial ear.” Siren at 50 feet Stereo headset (full volume) Threshold of pain Thunder, rock concert Basketball or hockey crowd Riveter Factory noise, chain saw Subway, tractor, power mower Screaming child Bus, motorcycle, snowmobile Loud home stereo, food blender Heavy traffic 120 To microphone and sound processor Shotgun blast Jet airplane 100 90 Less than 8 hours More than 8 hours children learn to speak. Those who receive a cochlear Normal conversation 60 implant before age 2 learn spoken language at a near norQuiet auto 50 Quiet mal rate (Dorman & Wilson, 2004). Quiet office 40 At present, artificial hearing remains crude. All but the 30 most successful cochlear implant patients describe the Whisper at 5 feet Very quiet 20 Broadcast studio when quiet sound as “like a radio that isn’t quite tuned in.” In fact, 30 Studio for making sound pictures 10 percent of all adults who have tried implants have given Just audible up on them. But cochlear implants are improving. And 0 Figure 4.25 Loudness ratings and potential hearing damage. even now it is hard to argue with enthusiasts like Kristen Cloud. Shortly after Kristen received an implant, she was able to hear a siren and avoid being struck by a speeding car. She says simply, “The implant saved my life.” How loud must a sound be to be hazardous? The danger of hearSmell and Taste — The Nose Knows ing loss depends on both the loudness of sound and how long you When the Tongue Can’t Tell are exposed to it. Daily exposure to 85 decibels or more may cause Gateway Question: How do the chemical senses operate? permanent hearing loss (Sekuler & Blake, 2006). Even short periods at 120 decibels (a rock concert) may cause a temporary threshold Unless you are a wine taster, a perfume blender, a chef, or a gourshift (a partial, transitory loss of hearing). Brief exposure to 150 met, you may think of olfaction (smell) and gustation (taste) as decibels (a jet airplane nearby) can cause permanent hearing loss. minor senses. Certainly you could survive without these two You might find it interesting to check the decibel ratings of chemical senses (receptors that respond to chemical molecules). some of your activities in Figure 4.25. Don’t be fooled by the But don’t be deceived. Life without these senses can be difficult numbers, though. Decibels are plotted on a logarithmic scale (like (Drummond, Douglas, & Olver, 2007). One person, for instance, earthquake intensity!). Every 20 decibels increases the sound presalmost died because he couldn’t smell the smoke when his apartsure by a factor of 10. In other words, a rock concert at 120 deciment building caught fire. Without the chemical senses, people bels is not just twice as powerful as a normal voice at 60 decibels. may be unable to cook, may have difficulty maintaining a healthy It is actually 1,000 times stronger. Be aware that amplified musical body weight due to diminished appetites, and may even be poiconcerts, iPod-style headphones, and “boom-box” car stereos can also damage your hearing. If tinnitus (tin-NYE-tus: a ringing or buzzing sensation) folPlace theory Theory that higher and lower tones excite specific areas of lows exposure to loud sounds, chances are that hair cells have been the cochlea. damaged. Almost everyone has tinnitus at times, especially with Conductive hearing loss Poor transfer of sounds from the eardrum to increasing age. But after repeated sounds that produce this warnthe inner ear. ing, you can expect to become permanently hard-of-hearing. A Sensorineural hearing loss Loss of hearing caused by damage to the study of people who regularly go to amplified concerts found that inner ear hair cells or auditory nerve. 44 percent had tinnitus and most had some hearing loss (MeyerNoise-induced hearing loss Damage caused by exposing the hair cells Bisch, 1996). to excessively loud sounds. The next time you are exposed to a very loud sound, remember Olfaction The sense of smell. Figure 4.24 and take precautions against damage. (Remember, too, that for temporary ear protection, fingers are always handy.) Gustation The sense of taste. • • • CHAPTER 4 soned by spoiled food (Brisbois et al., 2006; Gilbert & Wysocki, 1987). Besides, olfaction and gustation add pleasure to our lives. Skilled novelists always include descriptions of odors and tastes in their writings because they are among the most memorable sensations. Perhaps authors intuitively realize that a scene is incomplete without smells and tastes. Likewise, this chapter would be incomplete without a description of the chemical senses. The Sense of Smell Smell receptors respond to airborne molecules. As air enters the nose, it flows over roughly 5 million nerve fibers embedded in the lining of the upper nasal passages. Molecules passing over the fibers trigger nerve signals that are sent to the brain. The extreme close-up of an olfactory receptor cell in Figure 4.26c shows the thread-like fibers that project into the air flow inside the nose. Receptor proteins on the surface of the fibers are sensitive to various airborne molecules. When a fiber is stimulated, it sends signals to the brain. How are different odors produced? This is still an unfolding mystery. One hint comes from a type of dysosmia (dis-OZE-me-ah: defective smell), a sort of “smell blindness” for a single odor. Loss of sensitivity to specific types of odors suggests there are receptors for specific odors. Indeed, the molecules that produce a particular odor are quite similar in shape. Specific shapes produce the following types of odors: floral (flower-like), camphoric (camphor-like), musky (have you ever smelled a sweaty musk ox?), minty (mintlike), and etherish (like ether or cleaning fluid). Does this mean that there are five different types of olfactory receptors? Actually, in humans, about 1,000 types of smell receptors are believed to exist (Bensafi et al., 2004). It appears that different- • Afferent fibers of olfactory nerve shaped “holes,” or “pockets,” exist on the surface of olfactory receptors. Like a piece fits in a puzzle, chemicals produce odors when part of a molecule matches a hole of the same shape. This is the lock and key theory of olfaction. Further, molecules trigger activity in different combinations of odor receptors. Thus, humans can detect at least 10,000 different odors. Just as you can make many thousands of words from the 26 letters of the alphabet, many combinations of receptors are possible, resulting in many different odors. Scents are also identified, in part, by the location of the receptors in the nose that are activated by a particular odor. And finally, the number of activated receptors tells the brain how strong an odor is (Bensafi et al., 2004). The brain uses these distinctive patterns of messages it gets from the olfactory receptors to recognize particular scents (Laurent et al., 2001). What causes dysosmia? Five people out of 100 experience some degree of dysosmia (Bramerson et al., 2004). Risks include infections, allergies, and blows to the head (which may tear the olfactory nerves). Exposure to chemicals such as ammonia, photodeveloping solutions, and hairdressing potions can also cause dysosmia. If you value your sense of smell, be careful what you breathe (Drummond, Douglas, & Olver, 2007; Herz, 2001). It might seem that various odors are inherently good or bad smelling. But newborn infants show no signs to reacting more strongly to “good” versus “bad” odors. The U.S. military once tried to create a stink bomb that could be used for clearing people out of an area. No matter how foul the smell, nothing could be found that was universally repelling. It appears that likes and dislikes for various scents are learned (Herz, 2001). For example, a person who smelled roses for the first time at her mother’s funeral might dislike the scent of roses. Or one who smelled a skunk for the first time during a backyard birthday party might like the scent of Olfactory bulb Richard Costana, Discover Magazine, 1993 136 To cerebral cortex Cribriform plate of ethmoid bone Olfactory nerve fibers Basal cell (c) Supporting cell Receptor cell (bipolar) Cilia Nasal cavity (a) Nasal mucous membrane (b) • Figure 4.26 Receptors for the sense of smell (olfaction). (a) Olfactory nerve fibers respond to gaseous molecules. Receptor cells are shown in cross section to the left. (b) Olfactory receptors are located in the upper nasal cavity. (c) On the right, an extreme close-up of an olfactory receptor shows fibers that sense gaseous molecules of various shapes. Sensation and Reality skunk. If you don’t like the fetid, rotten odor of “ripe” cheeses, you just grew up in the wrong culture! Pheromones: A Special Scent? Among mammals, pheromones (FAIR-oh-moans: airborne chemical signals) greatly affect mating, sexual behavior, recognizing family members, and territorial marking (Kalat, 2007). For example, when a female pig is exposed to the pheromones in a male pig’s breath, she immediately becomes sexually receptive. The vomeronasal (voh-MARE-oh-NAZE-ul) organ (VNO) is the sense organ for pheromones. Until recently, humans were assumed to have only a vestigial VNO or none at all. Now, however, scientists believe they have located the VNO in humans (Hays, 2003). The suspected human vomeronasal organ looks like a small pit inside the nose (one on each side of the septum). These pits are lined with nerve cells and respond to chemicals that are suspected pheromones (Benson, 2002). Does this mean that someone could use pheromones to “turn on” their girlfriend or boyfriend? It is doubtful that human pheromones directly release sexual behavior. At best the effect would be indirect because pheromones are not smelled, felt, seen, tasted, or heard. In humans, they appear to unconsciously produce general moods, such as well-being, attraction, unease, or anxiety (Olsson et al., 2006). When people say that their relationships are influenced by good or bad “chemistry,” there may be some truth to it. Pheromones could add to the intoxicating feelings of romantic attraction or the sourness of instant dislike. In fact, one group of researchers believe that adding a pheromone to aftershave lotion can make men more sexually attractive (McCoy & Pitino, 2002). (However, the most likely human pheromones are found in underarm sweat, which is not exactly an enticing thought.) Even then, social context appears to influence the effect. For instance, the ability of pheromones to induce a positive mood depends on whether a person of the opposite sex is nearby ( Jacob, Hayreh, & McClintock, 2001). Evidence for the existence of human pheromones remains preliminary and controversial. (Men shouldn’t expect “Boar’s Breath” cologne to be offered anytime soon!) Nevertheless, the possibilities are intriguing (Ebster & Kirk-Smith, 2005). For instance, human pheromones appear to explain why the menstrual cycles of women who live together tend to become synchronized. It’s also possible that pheromones may one day be used to decrease anxiety, curb hunger, relieve premenstrual discomforts, or aid sex therapy. Only further study will tell whether searching for these special scents makes sense. 137 describes a pleasant savory or “brothy” taste associated with certain amino acids in chicken soup, some meat extracts, kelp, tuna, human milk, cheese, and soybeans. The receptors for umami are sensitive to glutamate, a substance found in monosodium glutamate (MSG) (Sugimoto & Ninomiya, 2005). Perhaps MSG’s reputation as a “flavor enhancer” is based on the pleasant umami taste (McCabe & Rolls, 2007). At the very least, we may finally know why chicken soup is such a “comfort food.” If there are only four or five tastes, how can there be so many different flavors? Flavors seem more varied because we tend to include sensations of texture, temperature, smell, and even pain (“hot” chili peppers) along with taste. Smell is particularly important in determining flavor (Shepherd, 2006). If you plug your nose and eat small bits of apple, potato, and onion, they will “taste” almost exactly alike. So do gourmet jelly beans! It is probably fair to say that subjective flavor is half smell. That’s why food loses its “taste” when you have a cold. Taste buds (taste-receptor cells) are mainly located on the top side of the tongue, especially around the edges. However, a few are found elsewhere inside the mouth ( Figure 4.27). As food is chewed, it dissolves and enters the taste buds, where it sets off nerve impulses to the brain (Northcutt, 2004). Much like smell, sweet and bitter tastes appear to be based on a lock-and-key match between molecules and intricately shaped receptors. Saltiness and sourness, however, are triggered by a direct flow of charged atoms into the tips of taste cells (Lindemann, 2001). People seem to have very different tastes. Why is that? Some differences are genetic. The chemical phenylthiocarbamine (FEENil-thigh-oh-CAR-bah-meen), or PTC, tastes bitter to about 70 percent of those tested and has no taste for the other 30 percent. Sensitivity to tastes has a genetic basis (Bartoshuk, 2000). In general, taste sensitivity is related to how many taste buds you have on your tongue. Some people have as few as 500 taste buds, whereas others have as many as 10,000. Those with many taste buds are “supertasters” who need only half as much sugar in their coffee to make it sweet (Bartoshuk et al., 2004). Supertasters tend to have stronger tastes for sweet, bitter, and irritants such as alcohol and capsaicin (the chemical that makes chilies hot). Women are more often supertasters. Nontasters tend to prefer sweets and fatty foods, which may be why supertasters tend to be slimmer than nontasters. The sense of taste also varies with age. Taste cells only live for several days. With aging, cell replacement slows, so the sense of taste diminishes. That’s why many foods you disliked in childhood may seem appetizing now. Children who will not eat broccoli, • Taste and Flavors There are at least four basic taste sensations: sweet, salt, sour, and bitter. We are most sensitive to bitter, less sensitive to sour, even less sensitive to salt, and least sensitive to sweet. This order may have helped prevent poisonings when most humans foraged for food, because bitter and sour foods are more likely to be inedible. Most experts now believe that a fifth taste quality exists (Chandrashekar et al., 2006). The Japanese word umami (oo-MAH-me) Dysosmia Loss or impairment of the sense of smell. Lock and key theory of olfaction Holds that odors are related to the shapes of chemical molecules. Pheromone An airborne chemical signal. Taste bud The receptor organ for taste. 138 CHAPTER 4 Circular papilla Filamentous papilla Omikron/Photo Researchers, Inc. Taste bud Hairlike ending of taste receptor (a) • Figure 4.27 Receptors for taste: (a) The tongue is covered with small protrusions called spinach, liver, and so on may be having a very different taste experience than an adult. Aside from this fact, however, most taste preferences are acquired. Would you eat the coagulated secretion of the modified skin glands of a cow after it had undergone bacterial decomposition? If you would, you are a cheese fancier! K NOW LED G E B U I L D E R Hearing, Smell, and Taste RECITE 1. The frequency of a sound wave corresponds to how loud it is. T or F? 2. Which of the following is not a part of the cochlea? a. ossicles b. pinna c. tympanic membrane d. all of the above 3. Which of the following is not important for the transduction of sound? a. pinna b. ossicles c. phosphenes d. oval window e. hair cells 4. According to the place theory of hearing, higher tones register most strongly near the base of the cochlea. T or F? 5. Sensorineural hearing loss occurs when the auditory ossicles are damaged. T or F? 6. Daily exposure to sounds with a loudness of _______ decibels may cause permanent hearing loss. 7. Cochlear implants have been used primarily to overcome a. conductive hearing loss b. hunter’s notch c. sensorineural hearing loss d. tinnitus 8. Olfaction appears to be at least partially explained by the _____________ _____________ _____________ theory of molecule shapes and receptor sites. 9. From the standpoint of survival, we are fortunate that we are least sensitive to bitter tastes. T or F? Sensory nerve (c) (d) REFLECT Critical Thinking 10. Why do you think your voice sounds so different when you hear a tape recording of your speech? 11. Smell and hearing differ from vision in a way that may aid survival. What is it? Relate Close your eyes and listen to the sounds around you. As you do, try to mentally trace the events necessary to convert vibrations in the air into the sounds you are hearing. Review the discussion of hearing if you leave out any steps. What is your favorite food odor? What is your favorite taste? Can you explain how you are able to sense the aroma and taste of foods? Answers: 1. F 2. d 3. c 4. T 5. F 6. 85 7. c 8. lock and key 9. F 10. The answer lies in another question: How else might vibrations from the voice reach the cochlea? Other people hear your voice only as it is carried through the air. You hear not only that sound, but also vibrations conducted by the bones of your skull. 11. Both smell and hearing can detect stimuli (including signals of approaching danger) around corners, behind objects, and behind the head. papillae. (b) Most taste buds are found around the top edges of the tongue (shaded area). However, some are located elsewhere, including under the tongue. Stimulation of the central part of the tongue (b) causes no taste sensations. All four primary taste sensations occur anywhere that taste buds exist. (c) An enlarged drawing shows that taste buds are located near the base of papillae. (d) Detail of a taste bud. These receptors also occur in other parts of the digestive system, such as the lining of the mouth. The Somesthetic Senses — Flying by the Seat of Your Pants Gateway Question: What are the somesthetic senses? A gymnast “flying” through a routine on the uneven bars may rely as much on the somesthetic senses as on vision (soma means “body”; esthetic means “feel”). Even the most routine activities, such as walking, running, or passing a sobriety test, would be impossible without somesthetic information from the body. You would find it very difficult to move, stay upright, or even stay alive without touch, pain, balance, and other bodily senses. They are an essential part of our sensory world. What are the somesthetic senses? Somesthetic sensitivity includes the skin senses (touch), the kinesthetic senses (receptors in Sensation and Reality muscles and joints that detect body position and movement), and the vestibular senses (receptors in the inner ear for balance, gravity, and acceleration). Let’s begin with the skin senses. The Skin Senses It’s difficult to imagine what life would be like without the sense of touch, but the plight of Ian Waterman gives a hint. After an illness, Waterman permanently lost all feeling below his neck. Now, in order to know what position his body is in he has to be able to see it. If he moves with his eyes closed, he has no idea where he is moving. If the lights go out in a room, he’s in big trouble (Cole, 1995). Skin receptors produce at least five different sensations: light touch, pressure, pain, cold, and warmth. Receptors with particular shapes appear to specialize somewhat in various sensations ( Figure 4.28). However, free nerve endings alone can produce all five sensations (Carlson, 2005). Altogether, the skin has about 200,000 nerve endings for temperature, 500,000 for touch and pressure, and 3 million for pain. Does the number of receptors in an area of skin relate to its sensitivity? Yes. Your skin could be “mapped” by applying heat, cold, touch, pressure, or pain to points all over your body. Such testing would show that the number of skin receptors varies and that sensitivity generally matches the number of receptors in a given area. As a rough-and-ready illustration, try this two-point touch test: • The density of touch receptors on various body areas can be checked by having a friend apply two pencil points to the skin with varying distances between them. Without looking, you should respond “one” or “two” each time. Record the distance between the pencils each time you feel two points. 139 You should find that two points are recognizable when they are 1/10 inch apart on the fingertips, 1/4 inch on the nose, and 3 inches at the middle of the back. Generally speaking, important areas such as the lips, tongue, face, hands, and genitals have a higher density of receptors. Of course, the sensation you ultimately feel will depend on brain activity. Pain The number of pain receptors also varies, right? Yes, like the other skin senses, pain receptors vary in their distribution. About 230 pain points per square centimeter (about a half inch) are found behind the knee, 180 per centimeter on the buttocks, 60 on the pad of the thumb, and 40 on the tip of the nose. (Is it better, then, to be pinched on the nose or behind the knee? It depends on what you like!) Pain fibers are also found in the internal organs. Stimulation of these fibers causes visceral pain. Curiously, visceral pain is often felt on the surface of the body, at a site some distance from the point of origin. Experiences of this type are called referred pain ( Figure 4.29). For example, a person having a heart attack may feel pain in the left shoulder, arm, or even the little finger. Pain from the skin, muscles, joints, and tendons is known as somatic (bodily) pain. Somatic pain carried by large nerve fibers is sharp, bright, fast, and seems to come from specific body areas (McMahon & Koltzenburg, 2005). This is the body’s warning system. Give yourself a small jab with a pin and you will feel this type of pain. As you do this, notice that warning pain quickly disappears. Much as we may dislike warning pain, it is usually a signal that the body has been, or is about to be, damaged. Without warning pain, we would be unable to detect or prevent injury. Children who are born with a rare inherited insensitivity to pain repeatedly burn themselves, break bones, bite off parts of their tongues, and become ill without knowing it (Cox et al., 2006). As you might imagine, it’s also hard for people with congenital pain insensitivity to have empathy for the pain of others (Danziger, Prkachin, & Willer, 2006). • Outer layer of skin Merkel’s disks Free nerve endings Meissner corpuscle’s Krause’s end-bulb Nerve endings around hair follicle Pacinian corpuscle 4.28 The skin senses include touch, pressure, pain, cold, and warmth. •ThisFigure drawing shows different forms the skin receptors can take. The only clearly specialized receptor is the Pacinian corpuscle, which is highly sensitive to pressure. Free nerve endings are receptors for pain and any of the other sensations. For reasons that are not clear, cold is sensed near the surface of the skin and warmth is sensed deeper (Carlson, 2005). Somesthetic sense Sensations produced by the skin, muscles, joints, viscera, and organs of balance. Skin senses The senses of touch, pressure, pain, heat, and cold. Kinesthetic senses The senses of body movement and positioning. Vestibular senses The senses of balance, position in space, and acceleration. Skin receptors Sensory organs for touch, pressure, pain, cold, and warmth. Visceral pain Pain originating in the internal organs. Referred pain Pain that is felt in one part of the body but comes from another. Somatic pain Pain from the skin, muscles, joints, and tendons. Warning system Pain based on large nerve fibers; warns that bodily damage may be occurring. 140 CHAPTER 4 Liver and gallbladder Lung and diaphragm Heart Pancreas Small intestine Ovary Appendix Kidney Stomach Ovary Colon Urinary bladder Figure 4.30 Hold a variety of elongated objects upright between your •fingertips. Close your eyes and move each object about. Your ability to estimate the size, length, shape, and orientation of each object will be quite accurate. (After Turvey, 1996.) • Figure 4.29 Visceral pain often seems to come from the surface of the body, even though its true origin is internal. Referred pain is believed to result from the fact that pain fibers from internal organs enter the spinal cord at the same location as sensory fibers from the skin. Apparently, the brain misinterprets the visceral pain messages as impulses from the body’s surface. Studies have shown that dynamic touch provides surprisingly detailed information about objects, such as their size and shape ( Figure 4.30). How are we able to make such judgments? Psychologist M. T. Turvey has found that most bodily motions form an arc or a combination of arcs (Turvey, 1996). Dynamic touch is largely a matter of sensing the inertia of objects as they move through these arcs. The fact that dynamic touch is a reliable source of information is what makes it possible for us to use a wide range of tools, utensils, and objects as if they were extensions of our bodies (Hove, Riley, & Shockley, 2006). • The Vestibular System A second type of somatic pain is carried by small nerve fibers. This type is slower, nagging, aching, widespread, and very unpleasant (McMahon & Koltzenburg, 2005). It gets worse if the pain stimulus is repeated. This is the body’s reminding system. It reminds the brain that the body has been injured. For instance, lower back pain often has this quality. Sadly, the reminding system can cause agony long after an injury has healed, or in terminal illnesses, when the reminder is useless. This chapter’s Psychology in Action section describes some ways to control pain. If you got carried away with the earlier pin demonstration, maybe you should read ahead now! Dynamic Touch A carpenter swings a hammer with practiced precision. A juggler fluidly tosses and catches five balls. An ice hockey player embeds his stick in an opposing player’s helmet. In sports and everyday life, touch is rarely static. Most skilled performances rely on dynamic touch, which combines sensations from skin receptors with kinesthetic information from the muscles and tendons. Space flight might look like fun. But if you ever get a ride into space, it is about 70 percent likely that your first experience in orbit will be throwing up. Weightlessness and space flight affect the vestibular system, often causing severe motion sickness ( Figure 4.31). Within the vestibular system, fluid-filled sacs called otolith (OH-toe-lith) organs are sensitive to movement, acceleration, and gravity. The otolith organs contain tiny crystals in a soft, gelatin-like mass. The tug of gravity or rapid head movements can cause the mass to shift. This, in turn, stimulates hair-like receptor cells, allowing us to sense gravity, acceleration, and movement through space (Lackner & DiZio, 2005). Three fluid-filled tubes called the semicircular canals are the sensory organs for balance. If you could climb inside these tubes, you would find that head movements cause the fluid to swirl about. As the fluid moves, it bends a small “flap,” or “float,” called the crista, that detects movement in the semicircular canals. A crista can be found within each ampulla (am-PULL-ah), a wider part of the canal. The bending of each crista again stimulates hair cells and signals head rotation. • 141 Sensation and Reality Inner Ear Ampulla Enlarged and Opened Crista Fluid Semicircular canals Nerve fibers Ampullae JSC/NASA Figure 4.31 The vestibular •system. (See text for explanation.) Otolith organs Cochlea Weightlessness presents astronauts with a real challenge in sensory adaptation. Why would sensory conflict cause nausea? You can probably blame (or thank) evolution. Many poisons disturb the vestibular system, vision, and the body. Therefore, we may have evolved so that we react to sensory conflict by vomiting to expel poison. The value of this reaction, however, may be of little comfort to anyone who has ever been “green” and miserable with motion sickness. In space, sensory conflict can be especially intense. During weightlessness, merely pulling on one’s shoes can result in a backward somersault. Under such conditions, the otolith organs send unexpected signals to the brain, and head movements are no longer confirmed by the semicircular canals. Few of the messages the brain receives from the vestibular system and kinesthetic receptors agree with a lifetime of past experience. What can be done to minimize motion sickness? Whether you are in outer space, playing a particularly intense video game, or just sitting in the backseat of a moving car, motion sickness is a very unpleasant experience. If you face motion sickness, there are a few things you can try that should help (Harm, 2002). If you know beforehand that you might be at risk, consider taking nonprescription motion sickness pills. If you are trapped in the moment, try Jiang Jin/SuperStock What causes motion sickness? According to sensory conflict theory, dizziness and nausea occur when sensations from the vestibular system don’t match sensations from the eyes and body (Flanagan, May, & Dobie, 2004). On solid ground, information from the vestibular system, vision, and kinesthesis usually matches. However, in a heaving, pitching boat, car, or airplane, a serious mismatch can occur — causing disorientation and heaving of another kind. Many people become nauseated the first time they experience virtual reality. Why? Because virtual reality also creates a sensory conflict: Computer-generated visual images change as if the viewer’s body is in motion, but the vestibular system tells viewers that they are standing still (Merhi et al., 2007). The result? The scenery may not be real but the nausea is. Reminding system Pain based on small nerve fibers; reminds the brain that the body has been injured. Dynamic touch Touch experienced when the body is in motion; a combination of sensations from skin receptors, muscles, and joints. Sensory conflict theory Explains motion sickness as the result of a mismatch between information from vision, the vestibular system, and kinesthesis. 142 CHAPTER 4 minimizing sensory conflict by moving your head as little as possible. Either close your eyes, fixate on an unmoving point (such as the horizon), or look above the horizon at the unmoving sky. Also, lie down if you can. The otoliths are less sensitive to vertical movements when you are horizontal, and your head will move less. Finally, anxiety intensifies motion sickness. Try slow, deep breathing and other relaxation techniques that you can use when needed. Image projected on screen Slide containing “F” Projector lens Slide projector Retinal image Contact lens (a) bridges Relaxation methods are described in Chapter 15 of this book. Adaptation, Attention, and Gating — Tuning In and Tuning Out Gateway Question: Why are we more aware of some sensations than others? You are surrounded by sights, sounds, odors, tastes, and touch sensations. Which are you aware of ? Each of the senses we have described is continuously active. Even so, many sensory events never reach awareness because of sensory adaptation, selective attention, and sensory gating. Let’s see how information is filtered by these processes. Sensory Adaptation Think about walking into a house where fried fish, sauerkraut, and head cheese were prepared for dinner. (Some dinner!) You would probably pass out at the door, yet people who had been in the house for some time wouldn’t be aware of the food odors. Similarly, smokers often don’t get how much nonsmokers are bothered by the smell of tobacco smoke. Why? Because sensory receptors respond less to unchanging stimuli, a process called sensory adaptation. Fortunately, the olfactory (smell) receptors adapt quickly. When exposed to a constant odor, they send fewer and fewer nerve impulses to the brain until the odor is no longer noticed. Adaptation to pressure from a wristwatch, waistband, ring, or glasses is based on the same principle. Sensory receptors generally respond best to changes in stimulation. No one wants or needs to be reminded 16 hours a day that his shoes are on. If change is necessary to prevent sensory adaptation, why doesn’t vision undergo adaptation like the sense of smell does? If you stare at something, it certainly doesn’t go away. The rods and cones, like other receptor cells, would respond less to a constant stimulus. However, the eye normally makes thousands of tiny movements every minute. These movements are caused by physiological nystagmus (nis-TAG-mus: involuntary tremors of the eye muscles). Although they are too small to be seen, the movements shift visual images from one receptor cell to another. Constant eye movement ensures that images always fall on fresh, unfatigued rods and cones. Evidence for this comes from (b) Figure 4.32 Stabilized images. (a) A miniature slide projector attached to a •contact lens moves each time the eye moves. As a result, the projected image disappears in a few seconds because it does not move on the retina. (b) A similar effect occurs when changes in brightness do not define a distinct edge. In this case, eye movements cannot prevent adaptation. Therefore, if you stare at the dot, the lighter area will disappear. (After Cornsweet, 1970.) fitting people with a contact lens that has a miniature slide projector attached to it ( Figure 4.32a). Because the projector follows the exact movements of the eye, an image can be stabilized on the retina. When this is done, projected geometric designs fade from view within a few seconds (Pritchard, 1961). You can get a similar effect by staring at Figure 4.32b. Because the lighter circle does not form a distinct edge, the retina adapts to the brightness difference. As it does, the circle gradually disappears. • • Selective Attention As you sit reading this page, receptors for touch and pressure in the seat of your pants are sending nerve impulses to your brain. Although these sensations have been present all along, you were probably not aware of them until just now. This “seat-of-the-pants phenomenon” is an example of selective attention (voluntarily focusing on a specific sensory input). We are able to “tune in on” a single sensory message while excluding others. Another familiar example of this is the “cocktail party effect.” When you are in a Sensation and Reality group of people, surrounded by voices, you can still select and attend to the voice of the person you are facing. Or if that person gets dull, you can eavesdrop on conversations all over the room. (Be sure to smile and nod your head occasionally!) Actually, no matter how interesting your companion may be, your attention will probably shift away if you hear your own name spoken somewhere in the room (Conway, Cowan, & Bunting, 2001). We do find what others say about us to be very interesting, don’t we? Sensory Gating What makes selective attention possible? Selective attention appears to be based on the ability of brain structures to select and divert incoming sensory messages (Sekuler & Blake, 2006). But what about messages that haven’t reached the brain? Is it possible that some are blocked while others are allowed to pass? Evidence suggests there are sensory gates that control the flow of incoming nerve impulses in just this way. In particular, sensory gating refers to facilitating or blocking sensory messages in the spinal cord (Melzack & Katz, 2004; Melzack & Wall, 1996). Pain Gates A fascinating example of sensory gating is provided by Ronald Melzack and Patrick Wall (1996), who study “pain gates” in the spinal cord. Melzack and Wall noticed, as you may have, that one type of pain will sometimes cancel another. Their gate control theory suggests that pain messages from different nerve fibers pass through the same neural “gate” in the spinal cord. If the gate is “closed” by one pain message, other messages may not be able to pass through (Melzack & Katz, 2004; Melzack & Wall, 1996). How is the gate closed? Messages carried by large, fast nerve fibers seem to close the spinal pain gate directly. Doing so can prevent slower, “reminding system” pain from reaching the brain. Pain clinics use this effect by applying a mild electrical current to the skin. Such stimulation, felt only as a mild tingling, can greatly reduce more agonizing pain. Messages from small, slow fibers seem to take a different route. After going through the pain gate, they pass on to a “central biasing system” in the brain. Under some circumstances, the brain then sends a message back down the spinal cord, closing the pain gates ( Figure 4.33). Melzack and Wall believe that gate control theory explains the painkilling effects of acupuncture. Acupuncture is the Chinese medical art of relieving pain and illness by inserting thin needles into the body. As the acupuncturist’s needles are twirled, heated, or electrified, they activate small pain fibers. These relay through the biasing system to close the gates to intense or chronic pain (Melzack & Wall, 1996). Studies have shown that acupuncture produces short-term pain relief for 40 to 80 percent of patients tested (Ernst, 1994; Weidenhammer et al., 2007). Acupuncture has an interesting side effect not predicted by sensory gating. People given acupuncture often report feelings of light-headedness, relaxation, or euphoria. How are these feelings explained? The answer seems to lie in the body’s ability to pro- • 143 4.33 Diagram of a sensory •gateFigure for pain. A series of pain impulses going through the gate may prevent other pain messages from passing through. Or pain messages may relay through a “central biasing mechanism” that exerts control over the gate, closing it to other impulses. Pain sensation Central biasing mechanism Spinal cord Brainstem Sensory gate Small nerve fibers Large nerve fibers duce opiate-like chemicals. To combat pain, the brain causes the pituitary gland to release a painkilling chemical called betaendorphin (BAY-tah-en-DOR-fin: from endo, “within,” and orphin, “opiate”). Chemically, beta-endorphin is quite similar to morphine. Such discoveries help explain the painkilling effect of placebos (fake pills or injections), which raise endorphin levels. A release of endorphins also seems to underlie “runner’s high,” masochism, acupuncture, and the euphoria sometimes associated with childbirth, painful initiation rites, or eating hot chili peppers (Kalat, 2007; Sternberg et al., 1998). In each case, pain and stress cause the release of endorphins. These in turn induce feelings of pleasure or euphoria similar to being “high” on morphine. The “high” often felt by long-distance runners serves as a good example of the endorphin effect. In one experiment, subjects were tested for pain tolerance. After running 1 mile, each was tested again. In the second test, all could withstand pain about 70 percent longer than before. The runners were then given naloxone, a drug that blocks the effects of endorphins. Following another 1-mile run the subjects were tested again. This time they had lost their earlier protection from pain (Haier et al., 1988). A similar effect occurs with first-time parachute jumpers. After the jump, their endorphin levels increase dramatically and they are much less sensitive to pain ( Janssen & Arntz, 2001). Actually, these observa- Sensory adaptation A decrease in sensory response to an unchanging stimulus. Selective attention Voluntarily focusing on a specific sensory input. Sensory gating Alteration of sensory messages in the spinal cord. Gate control theory Proposes that pain messages pass through neural “gates” in the spinal cord. Beta-endorphin A natural, painkilling brain chemical similar to morphine. 144 CHAPTER 4 Herve Donnezan/Photo Researchers, Inc. (left) An acupuncturist’s chart. (right) Thin stainless steel needles are inserted into areas defined by the chart. Modern research has begun to explain the painkilling effects of acupuncture (see text). Acupuncture’s claimed ability to cure diseases is more debatable. tions tie in nicely with the idea of pain gates. The central biasing system, which closes pain gates in the spinal cord, is highly sensitive to morphine and other opiate painkillers (Kalat, 2007; Melzack & Wall, 1996). People who say they are “addicted” to running, or bungee jumping, or eating spicy foods may be closer to the truth than they realize. (Can pain gates and endorphins explain every type of pain? See “The Matrix: Do Phantoms Live Here?”) Conclusion and a Look Ahead The senses supply raw data to the brain, but the information remains mostly meaningless until it is interpreted. It’s as if the senses provide only the jumbled pieces of a complex puzzle. In the next chapter we will explore some perceptual processes that help us put the puzzle together. A variety of psychological factors affect the severity of pain. Because you may not want to try acupuncture or electrical stimulation to control everyday pain, the following discussion describes several practical ways to reduce pain. Before we turn to that useful topic, here’s a chance to rehearse what you’ve learned. K NOW LED GE B U I L D E R Somesthetic Senses, Adaptation, Attention, and Gating 6. Sensory conflict theory appears to explain space sickness, but it does not seem to apply to other types of motion sickness. T or F? 7. Sensory adaptation refers to an increase in sensory response that accompanies a constant or unchanging stimulus. T or F? 8. The brain-centered ability to influence what sensations we will receive is called a. sensory gating b. central adaptation c. selective attention d. sensory biasing 9. The painkilling effects of acupuncture appear to result from __________________ _______________ and the release of betaendorphin. REFLECT Critical Thinking 10. What special precautions would you have to take to test the ability of acupuncture to reduce pain? 11. In a very real sense, we all live slightly in the past. How could that be true? Relate Stand on one foot with your eyes closed. Now touch the tip of your nose with your index finger. Which of the somesthetic senses did you use to perform this feat? Imagine you are on a boat ride with a friend who starts to feel queasy. Can you explain to your friend what causes motion sickness and what she or he can do to prevent it? As you sit reading this book, which sensory inputs have undergone adaptation? What new inputs can you become aware of by shifting your focus of attention? RECITE Answers: 1. d 2. T 3. large 4. dynamic touch 5. T 6. F 7. F 8. c 9. sensory gating 10. At the very least, you would have to control for the placebo effect by giving fake acupuncture to control group members. However, a true double-blind study would be difficult to do. Acupuncturists would always know if they were giving a placebo treatment or the real thing, which means they might unconsciously influence subjects. 11. For all the senses, it takes a split second for sensory receptors to sense a change in external stimuli and for a neural message to arrive at the brain. Therefore, by the time we are aware of an event, such as a very brief flash of light, it is already over. 1. Which of the following is a somesthetic sense? a. gestation b. olfaction c. rarefaction d. kinesthesis 2. Pain that originates in the internal organs is sometimes felt on the surface of the body as “referred pain.” T or F? 3. Warning pain is carried by ______________ nerve fibers. 4. An ability to sense the inertia of objects as we move them through space is the basis for __________________________ _______________________. 5. Head movements are detected primarily in the semicircular canals, gravity by the otolith organs. T or F? Sensation and Reality 145 B RAIN W AVES The Matrix: Do Phantoms Live Here? In the popular Matrix films, Neo, as played by Keanu Reeves, discovers that machines have imprisoned humans in a phantom world called the Matrix, in order to steal human energy for their own use. Actually, the idea of a “matrix” is not totally farfetched. In fact, your own brain may be creating a neuromatrix that allows you to perceive your own body. A person who suffers an amputation doesn’t need to believe in the Matrix to encounter phantoms. Most amputees have phantom limb sensations, including pain, for months or years after losing a limb (Fraser, 2002; Halbert, Crotty, & Cameron, 2002). Because the phantom limb feels so “real,” a patient with a recently amputated leg may inadvertently try to walk on it, risking further injury. Sometimes, phantom limbs feel like they are stuck in awkward positions. For instance, one man can’t sleep on his back because his missing arm feels like it is twisted behind him. What causes phantom limbs? Gate control theory cannot explain phantom limb pain (Hunter, Katz, & Davis, 2003). Because pain can’t be coming from the missing limb, it cannot pass through pain gates to the brain (after all, it’s missing!). Instead, according to Ronald Melzack (1999; Melzack & Katz, 2006), the brain creates a body image called the neuromatrix. This internal model of the body generates our sense of bodily self. Although amputation may remove a limb, as far as the neuromatrix in the brain is concerned, the limb still exists. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) confirms that sensory and motor areas of the brain are more active when a person feels a phantom limb (Rosen et al., 2001). Even though pain signals no longer come from the amputated limb, the neuromatrix evidently interprets other sensory experiences as pain from the missing limb. Sometimes the brain gradually reorganizes to adjust for the sensory loss (Wu & Kaas, 2002). For example, a person who loses an arm may at first have a phantom arm and hand. After many years, the phantom may shrink, until only a hand is felt at the shoulder. Perhaps more vividly than others, people with phantom limbs are reminded that the sensory world we experience is constructed, moment-by-moment, not by some futuristic machines but by our very own neuromatrix in our brains. PSYC HOL O GY IN A CT ION Controlling Pain — This Won’t Hurt a Bit In some cultures, people endure tattooing, stretching, cutting, and burning with little apparent pain. How do they do it? Very likely the answer lies in a reliance on psychological factors that anyone can use to reduce pain, such as anxiety reduction, control, attention, and interpretation (Mailis-Gagnon & Israelson, 2005). Gateway Question: How can pain be reduced in everyday situations? Steve Raymer/National Geographic/Getty Images Anxiety The basic sensory message of pain can be separated from emotional reactions to it. Fear or high levels of anxiety almost always increase pain. (Anxiety is a feeling of apprehension or uneasiness similar to fear, but based on an unclear threat.) A dramatic reversal of this effect is the surprising lack of pain displayed by soldiers wounded in battle. Being excused from further combat apparently produces a flood of relief. This emotional state leaves many soldiers insensitive to wounds that would agonize a civilian (Melzack & Wall, 1996). In general, unpleasant emotions increase pain and pleasant emotions decrease it (Rainville, 2004). Control A moment’s reflection should convince you that the most upsetting pain is that over which you have no control. Loss of control seems to increase pain by increasing anxiety and emotional distress. Conversely, if you can regulate a painful stimulus, you have control over it. People who are allowed to regulate, avoid, or control a painful stimulus suffer less. In general, the more control you feel you have over a painful stimulus, the less pain you will experience. (Vallerand, Saunders, & Anthony, 2007). To apply this principle, you should arrange a signal so your doctor, dentist, or Phantom limb The illusory sensation that a limb still exists after it is lost through accident or amputation. Anxiety Apprehension, dread, or uneasiness similar to fear but based on an unclear threat. Control Where pain is concerned, control refers to an ability to regulate the pain stimulus. CHAPTER 4 body piercer will know when to start and stop a painful procedure. Attention Distraction can also radically reduce pain. As you’ll recall, attention refers to voluntarily focusing on a specific sensory input. Pain, even though it is highly persistent, can be selectively “tuned out” (at least partially), just like any other sensation. Children in one experiment tolerated pain better when they were distracted by interacting with a headmounted audiovisual display (Dahlquist et al., 2007). Similarly, burn patients, who must undergo excruciating pain while their bandages are changed, can be distracted from their pain with video games and virtual reality (Hoffman et al., 2001). Less dramatically, have you ever temporarily forgotten about a toothache or similar pain while absorbed in a movie or book? As this suggests, concentrating on pleasant, soothing images can be especially helpful. Instead of listening to the whir of a dentist’s drill, for example, you might imagine that you are lying in the sun on a beach, listening to the roar of the surf. Or take an iPod along and crank up your favorite MP3s (Bushnell, Villemure, & Duncan, 2004). At home, music can also be a good distractor from chronic pain (Mitchell et al., 2007). Interpretation The meaning, or interpretation, you give a stimulus also affects pain (Turk & Melzack, 2001). For example, if you give a child a swat on the behind while playing, you’ll probably get a burst of laughter. Yet the same swat given as punishment may bring tears. The effects of interpretation are illustrated by a classic experiment in which thinking of pain as pleasurable (denying the pain) greatly increased pain tolerance (Neufeld, 1970). Another study found that people who believe a painful procedure will improve their health feel less pain during the procedure (Staats et al., 1998). Coping with Pain How can these facts be applied? In a sense, they already are applied to childbirth. Prepared childbirth training, which promotes birth with a minimum of drugs or painkillers, uses all four factors. To prepare for natural childbirth, the expectant mother learns in great detail what to anticipate at each stage of labor. This greatly relieves her fears and anxieties. During labor, she attends to sensations that mark her progress and she adjusts her breathing accordingly. Her attention is shifted to sensations other than pain, resulting in less discomfort (Leventhal et al., 1989). Also, her positive attitude is maintained by use of the term “contractions” rather than “labor pains.” Finally, because of her months of preparation and her active participation, she feels in control of the situation. Natural childbirth techniques reduce pain by an average of about 30 percent. Many women find this reduction quite helpful. However, it is important to remember that labor can produce very severe pain. A woman should not feel guilty if she needs painkillers during labor. Many women who have had prepared childbirth training still end up asking for an epidural block. For moderate pain, it can make quite a difference to reduce anxiety, redirect attention, and increase control. When you anticipate pain (during a trip to the doctor, dentist, or tattoo parlor), you can lower anxiety by making sure that you are fully informed. Be sure that someone explains everything that will happen or could happen to you. Also, be sure to fully discuss any fears you have. If you are physically tense, you can use relaxation exercises to lower your level of arousal. Relaxation methods involve tensing and then releasing muscles in various parts of the body. bridges A typical technique is described in detail in the Psychology in Action section of Chapter 15. The desensitization procedure described there may also help reduce anxiety. Distraction and Reinterpretation Some dentists are now equipped to help you shift attention away from pain. Patients are actively distracted with video games and headphones playing music. In other situations, focusing on some external object may help you shift attention away from pain. Pick a tree outside a window, a design on the wall, or some other stimulus and examine it in great detail. Prior practice in meditation can be a tremendous aid to such attention shifts. Research suggests that dis- Dr. Anna Kalmanovich, Boxborough MA/todaysdentalob.com 146 New technologies like this audiovisual headset offer new ways of distracting attention from painful experiences such as visits to the dentist. traction of this type works best for mild or brief pain. For chronic or strong pain, reinterpretation is more effective (McCaul & Malott, 1984). Counterirritation Is there any way to increase control over a painful stimulus? Practically speaking, the choices may be limited. One possibility is related to Ronald Melzack’s gate control theory of pain, which suggests that sending mild pain messages to the spinal cord and brain may effectively close the neurological gates to more severe or unpredictable pain. Such procedures, known as counterirritation, are evident in some of the oldest techniques used to control pain: applying ice packs, hot-water bottles, mustard packs, vibration, or massage to other parts of the body. Pain clinics use counterirritation by applying a mild electrical current to the skin. This causes only a mild tingling, but it can greatly reduce more agonizing pain (Köke et al., 2004). For more extreme pain, the electrical current can be applied directly to the spinal cord (Linderoth & Foreman, 2006). These facts suggest a way to minimize pain that is based on increased control, counterirritation, and the release of endorphins. If Sensation and Reality This fact can be used to mask one pain with a second painful stimulus that is under your control. For instance, if you are having a tooth filled, try pinching yourself or digging a fingernail into a knuckle while the dentist is working. Focus your attention on the pain K N O WL E D GE B U I L D E R Pain Control RECITE 1. Like heightened anxiety, increased control tends to increase subjective pain. T or F? 2. In one experiment, participants given the task of interacting with a head-mounted audiovisual display experienced less pain than participants who paid attention to the pain stimulus. T or F? 3. Imagining a pleasant experience can be an effective way of reducing pain in some situations. T or F? 4. The concept of counterirritation holds that relaxation and desensitization are key elements of pain control. T or F? chapter in review The senses act as data reduction systems in order to prevent the brain from being overwhelmed by sensory input. • Private sensations do not correspond perfectly to external stimuli. Studies in psychophysics relate physical energies to the sensations we experience. • The sensory organs transduce a limited range of physical energies into nerve impulses. • The minimum amount of physical energy necessary to produce a sensation defines the absolute threshold. The amount of change necessary to produce a just noticeable difference in a stimulus defines a difference threshold. • There is evidence that subliminal perception occurs, but subliminal advertising is largely ineffective. • Sensory analysis and coding influence what we experience. • Sensory processing is localized in specific parts of the brain. The visible spectrum is transduced by rods and cones in the retina leading to the construction of visual experience by the brain. • The visible spectrum consists of electromagnetic radiation in a narrow range. • The eyes and the brain form a complex system for sensing light. Vision is based on an active, computer-like analysis of light patterns. • Four common visual defects are myopia (nearsightedness), hyperopia (farsightedness), presbyopia (loss of accommodation), and astigmatism. you are creating, and increase its intensity anytime the dentist’s work becomes more painful. This strategy may seem strange, but it works. Generations of children have used it to take the edge off a spanking. REFLECT Critical Thinking 5. What measures would you take to ensure that an experiment involving pain is ethical? Relate Think about a strategy you have used for reducing pain at the doctor, dentist, or some other painful situation. Did you alter anxiety, control, attention, or interpretation? Can you think of any ways in which you have used counterirritation to lessen pain? Answers: 1. F 2. T 3. T 4. F 5. Experiments that cause pain must be handled with care and sensitivity. Participation must be voluntary, the source of pain must be noninjurious, and subjects must be allowed to quit at any time. you pinch yourself, you can easily create and endure pain equal to that produced by many medical procedures (receiving an injection, having a tooth drilled, and so on). The pain doesn’t seem too bad because you have control over it, and it is predictable. 147 Gateways to Sensation • The rods and cones are the eye’s photoreceptors in the retina. • The rods specialize in peripheral vision, night vision, seeing black and white, and detecting movement. • The cones, found exclusively in the fovea and otherwise toward the middle of the eye, specialize in color vision, acuity, and daylight vision. Our ability to see colors is explained by the trichromatic theory (in the retina) and by the opponent-process theory (in the visual system beyond the eyes). • The rods and cones differ in color sensitivity. Yellowish green is brightest for cones, blue-green for the rods (although they will see it as colorless). • Total color blindness is rare, but 8 percent of males and 1 percent of females are red-green color-weak. Color weakness is a sex-linked trait carried on the X chromosome. The Ishihara test is used to detect color blindness and color weakness. Attention Voluntarily focusing on a specific sensory input. Interpretation Where pain is concerned, the meaning given to a stimulus. Counterirritation Using mild pain to block more intense or longlasting pain. 148 CHAPTER 4 • Dark adaptation, an increase in sensitivity to light, is caused mainly by increased concentration of visual pigments in both the rods and the cones but mainly by rhodopsin recombining in the rods. Vitamin A deficiencies may cause night blindness. Sound waves are transduced by the eardrum, auditory ossicles, oval window, cochlea, and ultimately, hair cells. • Frequency theory explains how we hear tones up to 4,000 hertz; place theory explains tones above 4,000 hertz. • Two basic types of hearing loss are conductive hearing loss and sensorineural hearing loss. • Noise-induced hearing loss is a common form of sensorineural hearing loss caused by exposure to loud noise. Olfaction (smell) and gustation (taste) are chemical senses responsive to airborne or liquefied molecules. • It is also suspected that humans are sensitive to pheromones, although the evidence for this sense remains preliminary. • The lock and key theory of olfaction partially explains smell. In addition, the location of the olfactory receptors in the nose helps identify various scents. • Sweet and bitter tastes are based on a lock-and-key coding of molecule shapes. Salty and sour tastes are triggered by a direct flow of ions into taste receptors. The somesthetic senses include the skin senses, kinesthesis, and the vestibular senses. • The skin senses are touch, pressure, pain, cold, and warmth. Sensitivity to each is related to the number of receptors found in an area of skin. • Distinctions can be made among various types of pain, including visceral pain, somatic pain, referred pain, warning system pain, and reminding system pain. • Various forms of motion sickness are related to messages received from the vestibular system, which detects gravity and movement. • According to sensory conflict theory, motion sickness is caused by a mismatch of visual, kinesthetic, and vestibular sensations. Motion sickness can be avoided by minimizing sensory conflict. Incoming sensations are affected by sensory adaptation (a reduction in the number of nerve impulses sent), by selective attention (selection and diversion of messages in the brain), and by sensory gating (blocking or alteration of messages flowing toward the brain). • Selective gating of pain messages apparently takes place in the spinal cord. Gate control theory proposes an explanation for many pain phenomena, except phantom limb pain. Pain can be reduced or controlled by altering factors that affect pain intensity. • Pain is greatly affected by anxiety, control over the stimulus, attention, and the interpretation placed on an experience • Pain can be reduced by controlling these factors through distraction, reinterpretation, counterirritation. Web Resources For an up-to-date list of direct links to interesting sites, including those listed here, visit the student companion site for this book at www.cengage.com/psychology/coon Psychophysics Explore absolute and discrimination thresholds as well as other psychophysics phenomena. The Joy of Visual Perception An online book about visual perception, including information on the eye. Virtual Tour of the Ear This site provides educational information about the ear and hearing and provides quick access to ear and hearing Web resources. Taste and Smell Disorders Read about various disorders of taste and smell and what can be done about them. American Pain Foundation Find out more about pain and its treatment. Interactive Learning Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior Book Companion Website www.cengage.com/psychology/coon Visit your book companion website, where you will find flash cards, practice quizzes, web links, and more to help you study. Just what you need to know NOW! Spend time on what you need to master rather than on information you already have learned. Take a pre-test for this chapter, and CengageNOW will generate a personalized study plan based on your results. The study plan will identify the topics you need to review and direct you to online resources to help you master those topics. You can then take a post-test to help you determine the concepts you have mastered and what you will need to work on. Try it out! Go to www.cengage.com/login to sign in with an access code or to purchase access to this product. CHAPTER 5 Perceiving the World Gateway Theme We actively construct our perceptions out of the information provided by our senses and our past experience; the resulting perceptions are not always accurate representations of events. © 2006 “Pintos” by Bev Doolittle®, courtesy of The Greenwich Workshop, Inc. www.greenwichworkshop.com Gateway Questions • In general, how do we construct our perceptions? • Is perception altered by attention, motives, and emotions? • What basic principles do we use to group sensations into meaningful patterns? • What are perceptual constancies, and what is their role in perception? • • • • • How is it possible to see depth and judge distance? What effect does learning have on perception? To what extent do we see what we expect to see? Is extrasensory perception possible? How can I learn to perceive events more accurately? 149 preview Murder! One of your authors was in a supermarket when an 8-year-old girl suddenly came running around a corner. She looked back and screamed, “Stop! Stop! You’re killing him! You’re killing my father!” Here is your author’s account of what followed: I retraced the girl’s path and saw two men on the floor, struggling violently. The guy on top had his victim by the throat. There was blood everywhere. It was a murder in progress! As a witness, I would have told a jury what I just told you, were it not for an important fact: No murder ever took place that day at the supermarket. When the police arrived, they quickly discovered that the “guy on the bottom” had passed out and hit his head. That caused the cut (actually quite minor) that explained the “blood everywhere.” The “guy on top” saw the first man fall and was trying to loosen his collar. Perception: That Extra Step “Marilyn Numerisée #420” © Yvaral 1990, Courtesy Circle Gallery Gateway Question: In general, how do we construct our perceptions? You wake up. Without a hint of effort, you see the rising sun. I see the sunrise because my retinas send information to my brain, right? It may seem as if incoming sensations automatically give rise Visual perception involves finding meaningful patterns in complex stimuli. If you look closely at this painting by the artist Yvaral, you will see that it is entirely made up of small, featureless squares. A computer would have no clue what these squares portray. Likewise, an infant or newly sighted person would see only a jumble of meaningless colors. But because the squares form a familiar pattern, you should easily see Marilyn Monroe’s face. (Or is that Madonna?). 150 Obviously, the girl had misunderstood what was happening to her father. As a psychologist, I still find it fascinating that her words so dramatically influenced what I saw. In the last chapter we discussed sensation, the process of bringing information into the nervous system. This chapter is about perception, or how we assemble sensations into meaningful patterns. As we perceive events, the brain actively selects, organizes, and integrates sensory information to construct a “picture” or model of the world. This process is so automatic that it can take a drastic misperception like that of your author to call attention to it. Perception creates faces, melodies, works of art, illusions, and on occasion, “murders” out of the raw material of sensation. Let’s see how this takes place. to your experiences. But they don’t. It takes the extra step known as perception. Like most experiences, seeing a “sunrise” is a perceptual construction, or mental model of an event, that is actively created by your brain. To better understand perception, think again of the eye as being like a digital camera. Both eye and camera focus light on image-capturing surfaces. Both send information onward — your eyes to the brain and the camera first to a memory chip, and from there to your computer. But what happens when your computer receives a digital image? Does the computer recognize that photo of you and your best friend sitting on a beach last summer? Actually, not at all, because computers store photos as lists of numbers. Each number tells the computer what color and brightness to turn on a particular dot (pixel) on the screen. This allows your computer to display the photo, but the computer has no clue about what that photo means. In sharp contrast, when you look at a scene, you don’t just mindlessly record the action potentials from your rods and cones. Instead, you recognize and understand patterns in the messages. This is what creates a perceptual experience, such as seeing a sunrise or a photograph. It is easy to take perception for granted, but computer scientists have had great difficulty creating even rudimentary artificial vision. For instance, it is not yet possible for video surveillance systems in stores to tell if a robbery is in progress and call the police. And yet, you have had such a system since you were an infant: Your senses send data to your brain, which creates meaningful perceptions of the world around you. If you were in line at a grocery store, you would know in an instant if a robbery were taking place. In other words, current computer systems are great at capturing, storing, and displaying huge volumes of data, but they Perceiving the World are not yet very good at recognizing or understanding any of it. In contrast, your brain is very good at it. In this chapter we will explore perception, to learn how that extra step takes place. Bottom-Up and Top-Down Processing Moment by moment, our perceptions are typically constructed in both bottom-up and top-down fashion. Think about the process of building a house: Raw materials, such as lumber, doors, tiles, carpets, screws, and nails, must be painstakingly fit together. At the same time, a building plan guides how the raw materials are assembled. Our brain builds perceptions in similar ways. In bottom-up processing, we start constructing at the “bottom,” with raw materials. That is, we begin with small sensory units (features), and build upward to a complete perception. The reverse also occurs. In top-down processing, preexisting knowledge is used to rapidly organize features into a meaningful whole (Goldstein, 2007). If you put together a picture puzzle you’ve never seen before, you are relying mainly on bottom-up processing: You must assemble small pieces until a recognizable pattern begins to emerge. Top-down processing is like putting together a puzzle you have solved many times: After only a few pieces are in place, your past experience gives you the plan to rapidly fill in the final picture. Both types of processing are illustrated by Figure 5.1. Also, look ahead to Figure 5.6. The first time you see the photo you probably will process it bottom-up, by picking out features. The next time, because of top-down processing, you’ll likely recognize the photo instantly. Now that you have the general idea, let’s look at perception in more detail. • • 151 Perception and Attention — May I Have Your . . . Attention! Gateway Question: Is perception altered by attention, motives, and emotions? You are surrounded by sights, sounds, odors, tastes, and touch sensations. Which are you aware of ? The first stage of perception is usually attention. As you may recall from Chapter 4, selective attention refers to the fact that we give some messages priority and put others on hold (Klein, 2004). (What if you can’t easily focus your attention? See “Pay Attention!” to find out.) You might find it helpful to think of selective attention as a bottleneck, or narrowing in the information channel linking the senses to perception. When one message enters the bottleneck, it seems to prevent others from passing through ( Figure 5.2). Imagine, for instance, that you are driving a car and approaching an intersection. You need to be sure the traffic light is still green. Just as you are about to check it, your passenger points to a friend at the side of the road. If you then fail to notice the light just changed to red, an accident may be just seconds away. Have you ever felt overloaded when trying to do several things at once? Divided attention arises when you must divide your mental effort among tasks, each of which requires more or less attention. Divided attention is related to our limited capacity for storing and thinking about information. For example, when people first learn to drive, almost all their attention is needed to steer, • 5.2 The attentional “spotlight” •canFigure be widened or narrowed. If you focus on local details in this drawing you will see the letter A repeated 13 times. If you broaden your field of attention to encompass the overall pattern, you will see the letter H. (After Lamb & Art © Estate of Al Held/Licensed by VAGA, NY, NY Yund, 1996.) Figure 5.1 This painting by abstract artist Al Held is 9 feet by 9 feet. If you •process the painting “bottom-up,” all you will see is two small dark geometric shapes. Would you like to try some top-down processing? Knowing the painting’s title will allow you to apply your knowledge and see the painting in an entirely different way. The title? It’s The Big N. Can you see it now? Perception The mental process of organizing sensations into meaningful patterns. Perceptual construction A meaningful mental model of external events. Bottom-up processing Organizing perceptions by beginning with low-level features. Top-down processing Applying higher-level knowledge to rapidly organize sensory information into a meaningful perception. Selective attention Giving priority to a particular incoming sensory message. Divided attention Allotting mental space or effort to various tasks or parts of a task. 152 CHAPTER 5 T HE C LIN ICA L FI LE Pay Attention! What if you can’t? What if your everyday experience is a giant kaleidoscope of rapidly shifting sensory impressions and thoughts? Easily distracted, you may not even notice your teacher exhorting you yet again to “Focus on your work!” Couple that with an inability to sit quietly at your desk and a tendency to say or do the first thing that pops into your mind and you will likely not be teacher’s pet or at the top of your class. People with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have difficulty controlling their attention and are prone to display hyperactive and impulsive behavior (DSM-IV-TR, 2000). According to the National Institute of Mental Health, over two million American children have ADHD and about half will continue to show some symptoms as adults (NIMH, 2008). The diagnosis of ADHD is very controversial. On the one side are those who insist that ADHD is a biological disorder that is best treated with medications like Ritalin, a stimulant that exerts a curiously calming effect. On the other side are those who insist that ADHD is far too often overdiagnosed or misdiagnosed and that giving millions of young children drugs is inappropriate and wrong (Bratter, 2007). Complicating matters is the fact that boys are over twice as likely to be Charles Platiau/Reuters/Corbis brake, shift, and so forth. However, as a skill becomes more automatic, it requires less attention. In driving, greater skill frees mental capacity for other things, such as tuning the car’s radio or carrying on a conversation. Yet even as some driving skills become automated, divided attention can be hazardous (Wester et al., 2008). Many automobile accidents occur when drivers are looking at friends at the side of the road, using cell phones, tending to children, reading maps, touching up their makeup, and the like. Are some stimuli more attention getting than others? Yes. Very intense stimuli usually command attention. Stimuli that are brighter, louder, or larger tend to capture attention: A gunshot in a library would be hard to ignore. If a brightly colored hot-air bal- In many sports, experts are much better than beginners at paying attention to key information. Compared with novices, experts scan actions and events more quickly and focus on only the most meaningful information. This allows experts to make decisions and react more quickly (Bard, Fleury, & Goulet, 1994). diagnosed with ADHD than girls (Froehlich et al., 2007), leading some critics to claim that parents and teachers should give their exuberant boys more time to actively play outdoors rather than forcing them to sit still indoors for hours at a time. Is ADHD a real biological disorder or an unfair label applied to active children? Hopefully further research will answer this important question. Until then, your authors are content to note that this controversy highlights a major theme of this chapter, namely that perception is partly in the eye of the beholder. (You were paying attention, weren’t you?) loon ever lands at your college campus, it will almost certainly draw a crowd. Repetitious stimuli, repetitious stimuli, repetitious stimuli, repetitious stimuli, repetitious stimuli, repetitious stimuli are also attention getting. A dripping faucet at night makes little noise by normal standards, but because of repetition, it may become as attention getting as a single sound many times louder. This effect is used repeatedly, so to speak, in television and radio commercials. ATTENTION IS ALSO FREQUENTLY RELATED TO contrast OR change IN STIMULATION. The contrasting type styles in the preceding sentence draw attention because they are unexpected. Geoffrey Loftus and Norman Mackworth (1978) found that people who look at drawings like Figure 5.3 focus first and longest on unexpected objects (the octopus, in this case). • Figure 5.3 One of the drawings used by Loftus and Mackworth (1978) to •investigate attention. Observers attend to unexpected objects longer than they do to expected objects. In this drawing, observers looked longer at the octopus than they did at a tractor placed in the same spot. What do you think would happen if a tractor were shown upside down or on the roof of the barn? Perceiving the World Inattentional Blindness As we take in information, attention is the key that unlocks the door to perception. In fact, psychologists Arien Mack and Irvin Rock believe that perception cannot occur without attention. Let’s say, for example, that you are staring intently at a computer screen, waiting to see if a black cross will appear. The cross flashes on the screen for a split second and you say, “Yes I saw the cross.” At the same time, a small blue square is flashed near the cross, also for a split second. Do you see it, too? Amazingly, when people are tested in this way, many never see the blue square. While their attention is intensely focused on one object, they are blind to another—even though it is right in front of their eyes. The image of the blue square falls on the retina, but it might as well be invisible. Mack and Rock call this effect inattentional blindness (blindness caused by not attending to a stimulus) (Mack & Rock, 1998). Not seeing something that is plainly before your eyes is most likely to occur when your attention is narrowly focused (Mack, 2002; Most et al., 2005). Inattentional blindness is vividly illustrated by a study in which participants were shown a film of two basketball teams, one wearing black shirts and the other wearing white. Observers were asked to watch the film closely and count how many times a basketball passed between members of one of the teams, while ignoring the other team. As observers watched and counted, a person wearing a gorilla suit walked into the middle of the basketball game, faced the camera, thumped its chest, and walked out of view. Half the observers failed to notice this rather striking event (Simons & Chabris, 1999). This effect probably explains why fans of opposing sports teams often act as if they had seen two completely different games. In a similar way, using a cell phone while driving can cause inattentional blindness. Instead of ignoring a gorilla, you might miss seeing another car, a motorcyclist, or a pedestrian while your attention is focused on the phone (Bressan & Pizzighello, 2008). 153 may play without your really attending to it. When a stimulus is repeated without change, the OR habituates, or decreases. (Also, see “The ‘Boiled Frog Syndrome.’”) Interestingly, creative people habituate more slowly than average. We might expect that they would rapidly become bored with a repeated stimulus. Instead, it seems that creative people actively attend to stimuli, even those that are repeated (Colin, Moore, & West, 1996). Motives, Emotions, and Perception Our motives and emotions also play a role in shaping our perceptions. For example, if you are hungry, food-related words are more likely to gain your attention than nonfood words (Mogg et al., 1998). Advertisers, of course, know that their pitch will be more effective if it gets your attention. That’s why ads are loud, repetitious, and often intentionally irritating. They also take advantage of two motives that are widespread in our society: anxiety and sex. Everything from mouthwash to automobile tires is merchandised by using sex to gain attention (LaTour & Henthorne, 2003). Other ads combine sex with anxiety. Deodorant, soaps, toothpaste, and countless other articles are pushed in ads that play on desires to be attractive, to have “sex appeal,” or to avoid embarrassment. Our emotions can also influence what we perceive. According to psychologist Barbara Frederickson, negative emotions generally narrow our perceptual focus or “spotlight.” This increases the likelihood of inattentional blindness. In contrast, positive emotions can actually broaden the scope of attention (Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005). For example, we are much better at recognizing faces of people from our own race than from others. (This other-race effect is discussed in more detail later). But when people are in positive moods, their ability to recognize people from other races improves ( Johnson & Fredrickson, 2005). Habituation KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R Change, contrast, and incongruity are perhaps the most basic sources of attention. We quickly habituate (respond less) to predictable and unchanging stimuli. Notice that repetition without variation leads to habituation. Repetition is attention getting when it is irritating or annoying. A dripping faucet varies in timing just enough to gain attention. In contrast, we quickly habituate to the steady tick of a clock. How does habituation differ from sensory adaptation? As described in Chapter 4, adaptation decreases the actual number of sensory messages sent to the brain. When messages do reach the brain, the body has a sort of “What is it?” reaction, known as an orientation response. An orientation response (OR) prepares us to receive information from a stimulus: The pupils enlarge, brain-wave patterns shift, breathing stops briefly, blood flow to the head increases, and we turn toward the stimulus. Have you ever seen someone stop to take a second look? If so, you have observed an orientation response. Now, think about what happens when you download some new music for your MP3 player. At first the music holds your attention all the way through. But when the music becomes “old,” the song Perception and Attention RECITE 1. In top-down processing of information, individual features are analyzed and assembled into a meaningful whole. T or F? 2. Selective attention is promoted by all but one of the following. Which does not fit? a. habituation b. contrast c. change d. intensity Continued Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Disorder usually arising in early childhood characterized by inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsiveness. Inattentional blindness Failure to perceive a stimulus that is in plain view, but not the focus of attention. Habituation A decrease in perceptual response to a repeated stimulus. Orientation response Bodily changes that prepare an organism to receive information from a particular stimulus. 154 CHAPTER 5 C R IT ICAL T H I N KI N G The “Boiled Frog Syndrome” nuclear warheads, degradation of the environment, global deforestation, global warming, erosion of the ozone layer, and runaway human population growth. Robert Ornstein, a psychologist, and Paul Ehrlich, a biologist, believe that many of the large-scale threats we face are similar to the “boiled frog syndrome.” Frogs placed in a pan of water that is slowly heated cannot detect the gradual rise in temperature. They will sit still until they die. Like the doomed frogs, many people seem unable to detect Randy Ury/Corbis As we have noted, the perceptual system is impressed most by dramatic changes. Humans evolved to detect sharp changes and distinctive events, such as the sudden appearance of a lion, a potential mate, or sources of food. We are far less able to detect gradual changes. Perceptual capacities that aided survival when humans were hunters and gatherers may now be a handicap. Many of the threats facing civilization develop v e r y s l o w l y. E x a m p l e s include the stockpiling of 3. The occurrence of an orientation response shows that habituation is complete. T or F? 4. Changes in brain waves and increased blood flow to the head are part of an OR. T or F? 5. Research shows that positive emotions can broaden the “attentional spotlight.” T or F? REFLECT Critical Thinking 6. Cigarette advertisements in the United States are required to carry a warning label about the health risks of smoking. How have tobacco companies made these labels less visible? Relate Have you ever tried to listen to two people who were talking to you at the same time? What happens to your ability to process information when there’s a conflict in selective attention? You have almost certainly misperceived a situation at some time because of the influence of motives or perceptions. How were your perceptions influenced? gradual but deadly trends in modern civilization (Ornstein & Ehrlich, 1989). To avoid disasters, it may take a conscious effort by large numbers of people to see the “big picture” and reverse lethal but easily overlooked patterns (O’Neill, 2005). The relatively new field of community psychology is dedicated to helping overcome our own narrow perspectives to perceive important larger patterns (Nelson & Prilleltensky, 2005). Understanding how perception shapes “reality” may ultimately prove to be a matter of life or death. Are you paying attention? richness of your newfound visual experience but, like a computer, be unable to recognize any of it. You’re kidding, right? Nope. Newly sighted persons must learn to recognize objects, to read clocks, numbers, and letters, and to judge sizes and distances (Gregory, 2003). For instance, a cataract patient named Mr. S. B. had been blind since birth. After an operation restored his sight at age 52, Mr. S. B. struggled to use his vision. Mr. S. B. soon learned to tell time from a large clock and to read block letters he had known only from touch. At a zoo, he recognized an elephant from descriptions he had heard. However, handwriting meant nothing to him for more than a year after he regained sight, and many objects were meaningless until he touched them. Thus, Mr. S. B. slowly learned to organize his sensations into meaningful perceptions. Gestalt Principles How are sensations organized into perceptions? The Gestalt psychologists proposed that the simplest organization is constructed by grouping some sensations into an object, or figure, that stands out on a plainer background (Goldstein, 2007). Figure-ground organization is probably inborn, because it is the first perceptual ability to appear after cataract patients regain sight. In normal figure-ground perception, only one figure is seen. In reversible figures, however, figure and ground can be switched. In Figure 5.4 it is equally possible to see either a wineglass figure on a dark background or two face profiles on a light background. As you shift from one pattern to the other, you should get a clear sense of what figure-ground organization means. Answers: 1. F 2. a 3. F 4. T 5. T 6. Advertisers place health warnings in the corners of ads, where they attract the least possible attention. Also, the labels are often placed on “busy” backgrounds so that they are partially camouflaged. Finally, the main images in ads are designed to strongly attract attention. This further distracts readers from seeing the warnings. • Perceptual Organization — Getting It All Together Gateway Question: What basic principles do we use to group sensations into meaningful patterns? What would it be like to have your vision restored after a lifetime of blindness? Actually, a first look at the world can be disappointing because your newfound ability to sense the world does not guarantee that you can perceive it. You may be thrilled by the sheer bridges See Chapter 1, page 24, for a brief history of Gestalt psychology. Perceiving the World 155 • Figure 5.4 A reversible figure-ground design. Do you see two faces in profile or a wineglass? Are there other Gestalt organizing principles? The Gestalt psychologists identified several other principles that bring some order to your perceptions ( Figure 5.5). • 1. Nearness. All other things being equal, stimuli that are near each other tend to be constructed, or grouped, together (Quinn, Bhatt, & Hayden, 2008). Thus, if three people stand near each other and a fourth person stands 10 feet away, the adjacent three will be seen as a group and the distant person as an outsider (see Figure 5.5a). 2. Similarity. “Birds of a feather flock together,” and stimuli that are similar in size, shape, color, or form tend to be grouped together (see Figure 5.5b). Picture two bands marching side by side. If their uniforms are the same colors, the bands will be seen as one large group, not as two separate groups. 3. Continuation, or continuity. Perceptions tend toward simplicity and continuity. In Figure 5.5c it is easier to visualize a wavy line on a squared-off line than it is to see a complex row of shapes. 4. Closure. Closure refers to the tendency to complete a figure so that it has a consistent overall form. Each of the drawings in Figure 5.5d has one or more gaps, yet each is perceived as a recognizable figure. The “shapes” that appear in the two right drawings in Figure 5.5d are illusory figures (implied shapes that are not actually bounded by an edge or an outline). Even young children see these shapes, despite knowing that they are “not really there.” Illusory figures reveal that our tendency to form shapes — even with minimal cues — is powerful. • • (a) Principle of nearness Notice how differently a group of six objects can be perceptually organized, depending on their spacing. • (b) Principle of similarity In these examples, organization depends on similarity of color. • Similarity and nearness can be combined to produce a new organization. (c) Principle of continuity • This? plus or This? (d) Principle of closure (e) Principle of common region • Figure 5.5 How we organize perceptions. Figure-ground organization Part of a stimulus appears to stand out as an object (figure) against a less prominent background (ground). 156 CHAPTER 5 • interpretation). If you look at a cloud, you may discover dozens of ways to organize its contours into fanciful shapes and scenes. Even clearly defined stimuli may permit more than one interpretation. Look at Necker’s cube in Figure 5.7 if you doubt that perception is an active process. Visualize the top cube as a wire box. If you stare at the cube, its organization will change. Sometimes it will seem to project upward, like the lower left cube; other times it will project downward. The difference lies in how your brain interprets the same information. In short, we actively construct meaningful perceptions; we do not passively record the events and stimuli around us (Coren, Ward, & Enns, 2004). In some instances, a stimulus may offer such conflicting information that perceptual organization becomes impossible. For example, the tendency to make a three-dimensional object out of a drawing is frustrated by the “three-pronged widget” ( Figure 5.8), an impossible figure. Such patterns cannot be organized into stable, consistent, or meaningful perceptions. If you cover either end of the drawing in Figure 5.8, it makes sense perceptually. However, a problem arises when you try to organize the entire drawing. Then, conflicting information prevents you from forming a stable perception. Learning to organize his visual sensations was only one of the hurdles Mr. S. B. faced in learning to see. In the next section, we will consider some others. 5. Contiguity. A principle that can’t be shown in Figure 5.5 is contiguity, or nearness in time and space. Contiguity is often responsible for the perception that one thing has caused another (Buehner & May, 2003). A psychologist friend of ours demonstrates this principle in class by knocking on his head with one hand while knocking on a wooden table (out of sight) with the other. The knocking sound is perfectly timed with the movements of his visible hand. This leads to the irresistible perception that his head is made of wood. 6. Common region. As you can see in Figure 5.5e, stimuli that are found within a common area tend to be seen as a group (Palmer, & Beck, 2007). On the basis of similarity and nearness, the stars in Figure 5.5e should be one group and the dots another. However, the colored backgrounds define regions that create three groups of objects (four stars, two stars plus two dots, and four dots). Perhaps the principle of common region explains why we tend to mentally group together people from a particular country, state, province, or geographic region. • • • • • Clearly, the Gestalt principles offer us some basic “plans” for organizing parts of our day-to-day perceptions in top-down fashion. So too does learning and past experience. Take a moment and look for the camouflaged animal pictured in Figure 5.6. (Camouflage patterns break up figure-ground organization). If you had never seen similar animals before, could you have located this one? Mr. S. B. would have been at a total loss to find meaning in such a picture. In a way, we are all detectives, seeking patterns in what we see. In this sense, a meaningful pattern represents a perceptual hypothesis, or initial plan or guess about how to organize sensations. Have you ever seen a “friend” in the distance, only to have the person turn into a stranger as you drew closer? Preexisting ideas and expectations actively guide our interpretation of sensations (Most et al., 2005). The active, constructive nature of perception is perhaps most apparent for ambiguous stimuli (patterns allowing more than one • E.R. Degginger/Animals Animals •cube.Figure 5.7 Necker’s Figure 5.8 (Above) An impossible •figure — the “three-pronged widget.” Figure 5.6 A challenging example of perceptual organization. Once the cam•ouflaged insect (known as a giant walking stick) becomes visible, it is almost impos- (Below) It might seem that including more information in a drawing would make perceptual conflicts impossible. However, Japanese artist Shigeo Fukuda has shown otherwise. (“Disappearing Column” © Shigeo sible to view the picture again without seeing the insect. Fukuda, 1985.) Perceiving the World 157 Perceptual Constancies — Taming an Unruly World Gateway Question: What are perceptual constancies, and what is their role in perception? When Mr. S. B. first regained his vision, he could only judge distance in familiar situations (Gregory, 1990). One day he was found crawling out of a hospital window to get a closer look at traffic on the street. It’s easy to understand his curiosity, but he had to be restrained. His room was on the fourth floor! Why would Mr. S. B. try to crawl out of a fourth-story window? Couldn’t he at least tell distance from the size of the cars? No, you must be visually familiar with objects to use their size to judge distance. Try holding your left hand a (a) (b) few inches in front of your nose and your right Figure 5.9 Shape constancy. (a) When a door is open, its image actually forms a trapezoid. Shape conhand at arm’s length. Your right hand should stancy is indicated by the fact that it is still perceived as a rectangle. (b) With great effort you may be able to appear to be about half the size of your left see this design as a collection of flat shapes. However, if you maintain shape constancy, the distorted squares hand. Still, you know your right hand did not strongly suggest the surface of a sphere. (From Spherescapes-1 by Scott Walter and Kevin McMahon, 1983.) suddenly shrink, because you have seen it many times at various distances. We call this size constancy: The per(inborn). An example is the ability to see a line on a piece of paper. ceived size of an object remains the same, even though the size of Likewise, even newborn babies show some evidence of size conits image on the retina changes. stancy (Granrud, 2006; Slater, Mattock, & Brown, 1990). HowTo perceive your hand accurately, you had to draw on past expeever, many of our perceptions are empirical, or based on prior rience to provide a top-down plan for constructing your percepexperience. For instance, cars, houses, and people look like toys tion. Some of these plans are so basic they seem to be native when seen from a great distance or from an unfamiliar perspective, such as from the top of a skyscraper. This suggests that although some size constancy is innate, it is also affected by learning (Granrud, 2004). In shape constancy the shape of an object remains stable, even though the shape of its retinal image changes. You can demonstrate shape constancy by looking at this page from directly overhead and then from an angle. Obviously, the page is rectangular, but most of the images that reach your eyes are distorted. Yet, though the book’s image changes, your perception of its shape remains constant. (For additional examples, see Figure 5.9.) On the highway, alcohol intoxication impairs size and shape constancy, adding to the accident rate among drunk drivers (Farrimond, 1990). Let’s say that you are outside in bright sunlight. Beside you, a friend is wearing a gray skirt and a white blouse. Suddenly a cloud shades the sun. It might seem that the blouse would grow dimmer, but it still appears to be bright white. This happens because the blouse continues to reflect a larger proportion of light than nearby • Mark Richards/PhotoEdit • Almost everyone’s family album has at least one photo like this. Extreme viewing angles can make maintaining size constancy difficult, even for familiar objects. Perceptual hypothesis An initial guess regarding how to organize (perceive) a stimulus pattern. Size constancy The perceived size of an object remains constant, despite changes in its retinal image. Shape constancy The perceived shape of an object is unaffected by changes in its retinal image. 158 CHAPTER 5 C R IT ICAL T H I N KI N G A Bird’s-Eye View Although it is tempting to assume that other animals perceive depth much as we do, this is not the case. Different species actually see the world in rather different ways. Most variations in vision have a purpose. Evolutionary psychologists theorize that human depth perception is an evolutionary holdover — from life in the treetops. The superb depth perception that helped our distant ancestors swing from branch to branch now helps us swing at a softball or avoid erratic drivers in traffic. It might seem that birds would have acute stereoscopic vision, and some do. But most • birds are prey for other animals. When you spend life as a potential meal, it’s important to detect approaching predators (Waldvogel, 1990). That’s why many birds have an unusually wide field of view ( Figure 5.10a and b). An extreme case is the American woodcock, a bird that can survey a 360-degree panorama without moving its eyes or head. The tradeoff for this wide-angle view is a very limited area of binocular vision, where depth perception is strongest ( Figure 5.10c). But to the woodcock, an ability to spot hungry preda- • • tors is probably more valuable than depth perception. What does the world look like to a woodcock? Computer scientist Ping-Kang Hsiung (1990) used optical ray-tracing to simulate the woodcock’s view ( Figure 5.10e and f ). As you can see in Figure 5.10, even a pretty foxy predator would have trouble sneaking up on a woodcock. When it comes to human vision, perhaps it’s too bad that a little of the woodcock’s wide-angle vision didn’t get thrown in as well. • (a) (b) (c) (d) (e) (f) • Figure 5.10 (a) When viewed from above the head, a human’s field of view for the right and left eyes contains a large area of overlapping, stereoscopic vision (darker shading). (b) The barn swallow’s vision, like that of many birds, covers a much wider field of view than ours. Although the swallow’s area of binocular vision is smaller than a human’s, the swallow has sharper peripheral vision. (c) A bird called the American woodcock can see all the way around its head. Binocular vision is limited to a narrow band, but an extremely wide field of view helps the woodcock detect predators. (Adapted from Waldvogel, 1990.) (d) This image, created by Ping-Kang Hsiung (1990), shows how an imaginary scene would look to a person standing across the road from a rather strange hitchhiker. (e, f) This is what a woodcock’s left and right eyes would see if the bird were at the same point as the human in view d. (Computer graphics courtesy of Dr. Hsiung.) Perceiving the World objects. Brightness constancy refers to the fact that the brightness of objects appears to stay the same as lighting conditions change. However, this holds true only if the blouse and other objects are all illuminated by the same amount of light. You could make an area on your friend’s gray skirt look whiter than the shaded blouse by shining a bright spotlight on the skirt. To summarize, the energy patterns reaching our senses are constantly changing, even when they come from the same object. Size, shape, and brightness constancy rescue us from a confusing world in which objects would seem to shrink and grow, change shape as if made of rubber, and light up or fade like neon lamps. One of the most amazing perceptual feats is our capacity to construct the experience of three-dimensional space from flat retinal images. We’ll explore that topic in a moment, but first here’s a chance to rehearse what you’ve learned. K N O WL E D GE B U I L D E R Gestalt Principles and Perceptual Constancies RECITE 1. The first and most basic perceptual organization to emerge when sight is restored to a blind person is: a. continuity b. nearness constancy c. recognition of numbers and letters d. figure-ground 2. Gestalt principles offer us some basic “plans” for organizing parts of our day-to-day perceptions in top-down fashion. T or F? 3. At times, meaningful perceptual organization represents a __________________________ , or “guess,” held until the evidence contradicts it. 4. The design known as Necker’s cube is a good example of an impossible figure. T or F? 5. Which among the following are subject to basic perceptual constancy? a. figure-ground organization b. size c. ambiguity d. brightness e. continuity f. closure g. shape h. nearness REFLECT Critical Thinking 6. People who have taken psychedelic drugs, such as LSD or mescaline, often report that the objects and people they see appear to be changing in size, shape, and brightness. This suggests that such drugs disrupt what perceptual process? Relate As you look around the area in which you are now, how are the Gestalt principles helping to organize your perceptions? Try to find a specific example for each principle. If you needed to explain the perceptual constancies to a friend, what would you say? Why are the constancies important for maintaining a stable perceptual world? 159 Depth Perception — What if the World Were Flat? Gateway Question: How is it possible to see depth and judge distance? Close one of your eyes, hold your head very still, and stare at a single point across the room. If you don’t move your head or eyes, your surroundings will appear to be almost flat, like a painting or photograph. But even under these conditions you will still have some sense of depth. Now, open both eyes and move your head and eyes as usual. Suddenly, the “3-D” perceptual world returns. How are we able to perceive depth and space? Depth perception is the ability to see three-dimensional space and to accurately judge distances. Without depth perception, another form of top-down processing, you would be unable to drive a car or ride a bicycle, play catch, shoot baskets, thread a needle, or simply navigate around a room (Howard & Rogers, 2001,a). The world would look like a flat surface. (See “A Bird’s-Eye View” on page 158.) Mr. S. B. had trouble with depth perception after his sight was restored. Is depth perception learned? Some psychologists (nativists) hold that depth perception is inborn. Others (the empiricists) view it as learned. Most likely, depth perception is partly learned and partly innate (Witherington et al., 2005). Some evidence on the issue comes from work with the visual cliff. Basically, a visual cliff is a glass-topped table ( Figure 5.11). On one side a checkered surface lies directly beneath the glass. On the other side, the checkered surface is 4 feet below. This makes the glass look like a tabletop on one side and a cliff, or drop-off, on the other. To test for depth perception, 6- to 14-month-old infants were placed in the middle of the visual cliff. This gave them a choice of crawling to the shallow side or the deep side. (The glass prevented them from doing any “skydiving” if they chose the deep side.) Most infants chose the shallow side. In fact, most refused the deep side even when their mothers tried to call them toward it (Gibson & Walk, 1960). If the infants were at least 6 months old when they were tested, isn’t it possible that they learned to perceive depth? Yes. More recent research has shown that depth perception begins to develop as early as 2 weeks of age (Yonas, Elieff, & Arterberry, 2002). It is very likely that at least a basic level of depth perception is innate. Yet, the development of depth perception is not complete until about 6 months, suggesting that it depends on both brain maturation and individual experience. Then why do some older babies crawl off tables or beds? As soon as infants become active crawlers, they refuse to cross the deep side of the visual cliff. However, older infants who have just learned to walk must again learn to avoid the “deep” side of the visual cliff • Brightness constancy The apparent (or relative) brightness of objects remains the same as long as they are illuminated by the same amount of light. Depth perception The ability to see three-dimensional space and to accurately judge distances. Answers: 1. d 2. T 3. hypothesis 4. F 5. b, d, g 6. Perceptual constancies (size, shape, and brightness). 160 CHAPTER 5 • Figure 5.11 Human infants and newborn animals refuse to go over the edge of the visual cliff. Glass over patterned surface Glass only Shallow side Mark Richards/PhotoEdit Deep side (Witherington et al., 2005). Besides, even babies who perceive depth may not be able to catch themselves if they slip. A lack of coordination — not an inability to see depth — probably explains most “crash landings” after about 4 months of age. We learn to construct our perception of three-dimensional space by using a variety of depth cues. Depth cues are features of the environment and messages from the body that supply information about distance and space. Some cues require two eyes (bin- Stereoscopic: optic nerve transmissions from each eye are relayed to both sides of brain Floor pattern seen through glass ocular depth cues), whereas others will work with just one eye (monocular depth cues). Binocular Depth Cues The most basic source of depth perception is retinal disparity (a discrepancy in the images that reach the right and left eyes). Retinal disparity, which is a binocular cue, is based on the fact that the eyes are about 2.5 inches apart. Because of this, each eye receives a Binocular: both eyes have overlapping fields of vision Allows depth perception with accurate distance estimation Bob Western (a) • (b) Figure 5.12 (c) (a) Stereoscopic vision. (b) The photographs show what the right and left eyes would see when viewing a plant. Hold the page about 6 to 8 inches from your eyes. Allow your eyes to cross and focus on the overlapping image between the two photos. Then try to fuse the leaves into one image. If you are successful, the third dimension will appear like magic. (c) Now do the same with the random dot stereogram, but with your eyes 10 inches from the page. With luck, you will see a diamond shape hovering over the background. (See text for explanation.) (Julesz, 1971) 161 © 2009 Magic Eye Inc. www.magiceye.com Perceiving the World • Figure 5.13 This popular style of computer-generated art creates a 3-D illusion by superimposing two patterns. There are mismatches between some areas of the two patterns. This simulates retinal disparity and creates a sensation of depth. To get the 3-D effect, hold the stereogram about 8 inches from the end of your nose. Relax your eyes and look through the art, as if you were focusing on something in the distance. If you’re patient, you may see a 3-D globe. slightly different view of the world. Try this: Put a finger in front of your eyes and as close to your nose as you can. First close one eye and then the other, over and over again. You should notice that your finger seems to jump back and forth as you view the different images reaching each eye. However, when the two different images are fused into one overall image, stereoscopic vision (threedimensional sight) occurs (Howard & Rogers, 2001b). The result is a powerful sensation of depth ( Figure 5.12). Retinal disparity can be used to produce 3-D movies by filming with two cameras separated by several inches. Later, both images are simultaneously projected on a screen. Audience members wear glasses that filter out one of the images to each eye. Because each eye gets a separate image, normal stereoscopic vision is duplicated. Try the following demonstration of retinal disparity and fusion. • Totally Tubular Roll a piece of paper into a tube. Close your left eye. Hold the tube to your right eye like a telescope. Look through the tube at some object in the distance. Place your left hand against the tube halfway down its length and in front of your left eye. Now open your left eye. You should see a “hole” in your hand. You couldn’t expect a professional photographer to do a better job of blending the two images than your visual system does automatically. How does retinal disparity produce depth? Perceiving depth is more than a simple blending of two “pictures” of the world. In Figure 5.12c you will find two random dot stereograms (patterns of dots that produce an illusion of depth). Notice that they contain no objects, lines, or edges. Just the same, when the stereograms are properly viewed (one to each eye), a center area seems to float above the background. Such designs show that the brain is very sensitive to any mismatch of information from the eyes. In Figure 5.12c, depth comes from shifting dots in the center of one square so that they do not match dots in the other square (Howard & Rogers, 2001a, 2000b; Julesz, 1971). (Also see Figure 5.13.) To a large extent our experience of three-dimensional space is constructed from countless tiny differences between what the right and left eyes see. Direct studies of the brain have shown that visual areas do, in fact, contain cells that detect disparities (Cumming & DeAngelis, 2001). Convergence is a second binocular depth cue. When you look at a distant object, the lines of vision from your eyes are parallel. You are normally not aware of it, but whenever you estimate a distance under 50 feet (as when you play catch or shoot trash can hoops with the first draft of your essay), you are using convergence. How? Muscles attached to the eyeball feed information on eye position to the brain to help it judge distance ( Figure 5.14). • 5.14 •TheFigure eyes must converge, or turn in toward the nose, to focus close objects. The eyes shown are viewed from above the head. • • • Depth cues Perceptual features that impart information about distance and three-dimensional space. Binocular depth cues Perceptual features that impart information about distance and three-dimensional space that require two eyes. Monocular depth cues Perceptual features that impart information about distance and three-dimensional space that require just one eye. Stereoscopic vision Perception of space and depth caused chiefly by the fact that the eyes receive different images. 162 CHAPTER 5 (a) (b) (c) (d) (e) Figure 5.15 (a) Linear perspective. (b) Relative size. (c) Light and shadow. (d) Overlap. (e) Texture gradients. Drawings in the top row show fairly “pure” •examples of each of the pictorial depth cues. In the bottom row, the pictorial depth cues are used to assemble a more realistic scene. You can feel convergence by exaggerating it: Focus on your fingertip and bring it toward your eyes until they almost cross. You can actually feel the muscles that control eye movement working harder and harder as your fingertip gets closer. If you repeat this little exercise with one eye shut, you will feel no increase in muscle tension as your finger gets closer. It takes both eyes to converge. This is why we say convergence is also a binocular depth cue. If disparity and convergence are so important, can a person with one eye perceive depth? Yes, but not as well as a person with two eyes. Overall, stereoscopic vision is 10 times better for judging depth than perception based on just one eye (Rosenberg, 1994). Try driving a car or riding a bicycle with one eye closed. You will find yourself braking too soon or too late, and you will have difficulty estimating your speed. (“But officer, my psychology text said to . . .”) Despite this, you will be able to drive, although it will be more difficult than usual. This is possible because your single eye is making use of monocular depth cues. Monocular Depth Cues As their name implies, monocular depth cues can be used to construct a three-dimensional perception with information received from just one eye (Sekuler & Blake, 2006). One such cue is accommodation, the bending of the lens to focus on nearby objects. Sensations from muscles attached to each lens flow back to the brain. Changes in these sensations help us judge distances within about 4 feet of the eyes. This information is available even if you are just using one eye, so accommodation is a monocular cue. Beyond 4 feet though, accommodation has limited value. Obviously, it is more important to a watchmaker or a person trying to thread a needle than it is to a basketball player or someone driving an automobile. Other monocular depth cues are also referred to as pictorial depth cues, because a good movie, painting, or photograph can create a convincing sense of depth where none exists. How is the illusion of depth created on a two-dimensional surface? Pictorial depth cues are monocular cues found in paintings, drawings, and photographs that impart information about space, depth, and distance. To understand how these cues work, imagine that you are looking outdoors through a window. If you trace everything you see onto the glass, you will have an excellent drawing, with convincing depth. If you then analyze what is on the glass you will find the following features ( Figure 5.15): • 1. Linear perspective. This cue is based on the apparent convergence of parallel lines in the environment. If you stand between two railroad tracks, they appear to meet near the horizon, even though they actually remain parallel. Because you know they are parallel, their convergence implies great distance ( Figure 5.15a). 2. Relative size. If an artist wishes to depict two objects of the same size at different distances, the artist makes the more distant object smaller ( Figure 5.15b). Special effects in films create sensational illusions of depth by rapidly changing the image size of planets, airplanes, monsters, or what have you. (Also see Figure 5.16.) • • • Linear perspective is a very powerful cue for depth. Because of the depth cues implied in this drawing, the upper cross on the vertical line appears to be diagonal. It is actually a right angle. The lower cross, which appears to be a right angle, is actually diagonal to the vertical line. (After Enns & Coren, 1995.) Dennis Coon Figure 5.16 On a dry lake bed, relative size is just about the only depth cue •available for judging the camera’s distance from this vintage aircraft. What do you estimate the distance to be? For the answer, look ahead to • Figure 5.21. 3. Height in the picture plane. Objects that are placed higher (closer to the horizon line) in a drawing tend to be perceived as more distant. In the upper frame of Figure 5.15b the black columns look like they are receding into the distance partly because they become smaller but also because they move higher in the drawing. 4. Light and shadow. Most objects are lighted in ways that create clear patterns of light and shadow. Copying such patterns of light and shadow can give a two-dimensional design a three-dimensional appearance ( Figure 5.15c). (Also, look ahead to Figure 5.17 for more information on light and shadow.) 5. Overlap. Overlap (or interposition) occurs when one object partially blocks another object. Hold your hands up and ask a friend across the room which is nearer. Relative size will give the answer if one hand is much nearer to your friend than the other. But if one hand is only slightly closer than the other, your friend may not be able to tell — until you slide one hand in front of the other. Overlap then removes any doubt ( Figure 5.15d ). 6. Texture gradients. Changes in texture also contribute to depth perception. If you stand in the middle of a cobblestone street, the street will look coarse near your feet. However, its texture will get smaller and finer if you look into the distance ( Figure 5.15e). 7. Aerial perspective. Smog, fog, dust, and haze add to the apparent distance of an object. Because of aerial perspective, distant objects tend to be hazy, washed out in color, and lacking in detail. Aerial haze is often most noticeable when it is missing. If you have ever seen a distant mountain range on a crystal-clear day, it might have looked like it was only a few miles away. In reality, you could have been viewing them through 50 miles of crystal-clear air. 8. Relative motion. Relative motion, also known as motion parallax (PAIR-ah-lax), can be seen by looking out a window and moving your head from side to side. Notice that nearby objects appear to move a sizable distance as your head moves. Trees, houses, and telephone poles that are farther • • • • 163 M.C. Escher’s “Convex and Concave” © 2009 The M.C. Escher Company-Holland. All rights reserved. www.mcescher.com Perceiving the World • Figure 5.17 (Above) When judging depth we usually assume that light •comes mainly from one direction, usually from above. Squint a little to blur the image you see here. You should perceive a collection of globes projecting outward. If you turn this page upside down, the globes should become cavities. (After Ramachandran, 1995.) (Below) The famed Dutch artist M. C. Escher violated our assumptions about light to create the dramatic illusions of depth found in his 1955 lithograph Convex and Concave. In this print, light appears to come from all sides of the scene. (Courtesy of the Collection Haags Gemeente Museum, The Hague. © 1994 M. C. Escher/Cordon Art, Baarn, The Netherlands. All rights reserved.) away appear to move slightly in relation to the background. Distant objects like hills, mountains, or clouds don’t seem to move at all. When combined, pictorial cues can create a powerful illusion of depth. (See Table 5.1 for a summary of all the depth cues we have discussed.) Is motion parallax really a pictorial cue? Strictly speaking it is not, except in the two-dimensional world of movies, television, or animated cartoons. However, when parallax is present, we almost always perceive depth. Much of the apparent depth of a good movie comes from relative motion captured by the camera. • Pictorial depth cues Features found in paintings, drawings, and photographs that impart information about space, depth, and distance. 164 CHAPTER 5 Table 5.1 • Summary of Visual Depth Cues Binocular Cues • Retinal disparity • Convergence Monocular Cues • Accommodation • Pictorial depth cues (listed below) Linear perspective Relative size Height in the picture plane Light and shadow Overlap Texture gradients Aerial perspective Relative motion (motion parallax) • Figure 5.18 illustrates the defining feature of motion parallax. Imagine that you are in a bus and watching the passing scenery (with your gaze at a right angle to the road). Under these conditions, nearby objects will appear to rush backward. Those farther away, such as distant mountains, will seem to move very little or not at all. Objects that are more remote, such as the sun or moon, will appear to move in the same direction you are traveling. (That’s why the moon appears to “follow” you when you take a stroll at night.) Are pictorial depth cues universal? Not entirely. Some cultures use only selected pictorial cues to represent depth. People in these cultures may not easily recognize other cues (Deregowski, 1972). For example, researcher William Hudson tested members of remote tribes who do not use relative size to show depth in drawings. These people perceive simplified drawings as flat designs. As you can see in Figure 5.19, they do not assume, as we do, that a larger image means that an object is closer. Of course, members of • 5.19 A Hudson test picture. Two-dimensional perceivers assume •theFigure hunter is trying to spear the distant elephant rather than the nearby antelope. Some acquaintance with conventions for representing depth in pictures and photographs seems necessary. (From “Pictorial Perception and Culture” by J. B. Deregowski. © 1972 Scientific American, Inc. All rights reserved.) non-Western cultures can learn to interpret drawings of depth if they are given a chance to practice (Mshelia & Lapidus, 1990). The Moon Illusion How do the depth perception cues relate to daily experience? We constantly use both pictorial cues and bodily cues to sense depth and judge distances. Consider an intriguing effect called the moon illusion (perceiving the moon as larger when it is low in the sky). When the moon is on the horizon, it tends to look like a silver dollar. When it is directly overhead, it looks more like a dime. Contrary to what some people believe, the moon is not magnified by the atmosphere. But the moon looks nearly twice as large when it’s low in the sky (Ross & Plug, 2002). This occurs, in part, because the moon’s apparent distance is greater when it is near the horizon than when it is overhead (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2000). But if it seems farther away, shouldn’t it look smaller? No. When the moon is overhead, few depth cues surround it. In contrast, when you see the moon on the horizon, it is behind houses, trees, telephone poles, and mountains. These objects add numerous depth cues, which cause the horizon to seem more distant than the sky overhead. Picture two balloons, one 10 feet away and the second 20 feet away. Suppose the more distant balloon is inflated until its image matches the image of the nearer balloon. How do we know the more distant balloon is larger? Because its image is the same size as a balloon that is closer. Similarly, the moon makes the same-size image on the horizon as it does overhead. However, the horizon seems more distant because more depth cues are present. As a result, the horizon moon must be perceived as larger (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2000) ( Figure 5.20). This explanation is known as the apparent-distance hypothesis (the horizon seems more distant than the night sky). You can test it by removing depth cues while looking at a horizon moon. Try looking at the moon through a rolled-up paper tube or make your hands into a “telescope” and look at the next large moon you see. It will immediately appear to shrink when viewed without depth cues (Ross & Plug, 2002). • • Direction of travel Figure 5.18 The apparent motion of objects viewed during travel depends on their distance from the observer. Apparent motion can also be influenced by an observer’s point of fixation. At middle distances, objects closer than the point of fixation appear to move backward; those beyond the point of fixation appear to move forward. Objects at great distances, such as the sun or moon, always appear to move forward. Perceiving the World 165 KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R Depth Perception RECITE 1. The visual cliff is used to test for infant sensitivity to linear perspective. T or F? 2. Write an M or a B after each of the following to indicate if it is a monocular or binocular depth cue. accommodation _____ convergence _____ retinal disparity _____ linear perspective _____ motion parallax _____ overlap _____ relative size _____ 3. Which of the depth cues listed in question 2 are based on muscular feedback? ________________________________________. 4. Interpretation of pictorial depth cues requires no prior experience. T or F? 5. The apparent distance hypothesis provides a good explanation of the a. moon illusion b. horizontal-vertical illusion c. Zulu illusion d. effects of inattentional blindness REFLECT 5.20 The Ponzo illusion may help you understand the moon illu•sion.Figure Picture the two white bars as resting on the railroad tracks. In the drawing, the upper bar is the same length as the lower bar. However, because the upper bar appears to be farther away than the lower bar, we perceive it as longer. The same logic applies to the moon illusion. Critical Thinking 6. Scientists believe that the famous Dutch artist Rembrandt had a visual defect that prevented him from perceiving depth. (He had a wandering eye.) How might this have aided him as an artist? Relate Part of the rush of excitement produced by action movies and video games is based on the sense of depth they create. Return to the list of pictorial depth cues. What cues have you seen used to portray depth? Try to think of specific examples in a movie or game you have seen recently. Dennis Coon Answers: 1. F 2. accommodation (M), convergence (B), retinal disparity (B), linear perspective (M), motion parallax (M), overlap (M), relative size (M) 3. accommodation or convergence 4. F 5. a 6. An artist must transfer a three-dimensional scene onto a flat canvas. Because Rembrandt could not see depth, it might have been easier for him to put what he saw onto a two-dimensional surface. Figure 5.21 Before you can use familiar size to judge distance, objects must •actually be the size you assume they are. Either these men are giants, or the model airplane was closer than you may have thought when you looked at • Figure 5.16. To what extent has the apparent-distance hypothesis been confirmed? The father and son team of Lloyd and James Kaufman projected images of the moon on a mirror. This allowed them to superimpose an artificial moon on the sky. In addition, the mirrors were moveable. Volunteer observers reported that as the moon moved closer, it appeared to get smaller. This effect was most dramatic when the moon was near the horizon, where more depth cues are found. This is the strongest confirmation yet of the apparent-distance theory (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2000). Perceptual Learning — What if the World Were Upside Down? Gateway Question: What effect does learning have on perception? England is one of the few countries in the world where people drive on the left side of the road. Because of this reversal, it is not unusual for visitors to step off curbs in front of cars — after carefully looking for traffic in the wrong direction. As this example suggests, learning has a powerful impact on top-down processing in perception. Moon illusion The apparent change in size that occurs as the moon moves from the horizon (large moon) to overhead (small moon). Apparent-distance hypothesis An explanation of the moon illusion stating that the horizon seems more distant than the night sky. 166 CHAPTER 5 Perceptual Habits In general, learning creates perceptual habits (ingrained patterns of organization and attention) that affect our daily experience. Stop for a moment and look at Figure 5.23. The left face looks somewhat unusual, to be sure. But the distortion seems mild—until you turn the page upside down. Viewed normally, the face looks quite grotesque. Why is there a difference? Apparently, most people have little experience with upside-down faces. Perceptual learning, therefore, has less impact on our perceptions of an upside-down face. With a face in the normal position, you know what to expect and where to look. Also, you tend to see the entire face as a recognizable pattern. When a face is inverted, we are forced to perceive its individual features separately (Caharel et al., 2006). Top-down perceptual processing habits can easily lead our perceptions astray. Because we have seen thousands of rooms shaped roughly like a box, we habitually construct perceptions based on this assumption. This need not be true, however. An Ames room (named for the man who designed it) is a lopsided space that appears square when viewed from a certain point ( Figure 5.24). • (b) (c) How does learning affect perception? The term perceptual learning refers to changes in the brain that alter how we construct sensory information into percepts (Fahle & Poggio, 2002). For example, to use a computer, you must learn to pay attention to specific stimuli, such as icons and cursors. We also learn to tell the difference between stimuli that seemed identical at first. An example is the novice chef who discovers how to tell the difference between dried basil, oregano, and tarragon. In other situations, we learn to focus on just one part of a group of stimuli. This saves us from having to process all the stimuli in the group. For instance, a linebacker in football may be able to tell if the next play will be a run or a pass by watching one or two key players, rather than the entire opposing team (Seitz & Watanabe, 2005). Even something as simple as figure-ground perception is affected by learning. For example, if you cut a shape out of dark paper and place it on a white background, other people are more likely to see it as a figure if it resembles a familiar object. As another example, in Figure 5.22a you can probably shift between seeing the white shape or the black shape as an object. Now look at Figure 5.22b. Does the lower black shape seem more like it’s the figure than the upper white shape? Next consider Figure 5.22c. Notice how the white area in Figure 5.22c seems like it has become the figure. In our daily experience, objects below the horizon are usually closer to us. Also, we typically see more objects below the horizon than above it. Because of such experiences, we are more likely to perceive areas below the “horizon” line in a drawing as objects or figures (Vecera, Vogel, & Woodman, 2002). Does this mean you figure that Figure 5.23 The effects of prior experience on perception. The doctored face looks far worse when viewed right side up because it can be related to past experience. figures are close to the ground? • • • • • • • Figure 5.24 The Ames room. From the front, the room looks normal; actually, the right-hand corner is very short, and the left-hand corner is very tall. In addition, the left side of the room slants away from viewers. The diagram shows the shape of the room and reveals why people appear to get bigger as they cross the room toward the nearer, shorter right corner. Mark McKenna Peephole Bettmann/Corbis • (a) Figure 5.22 Perceiving the World 167 HU M AN D I VERS I T Y Do They See What We See? According to psychologist Richard Nisbett and his colleagues, people from different cultures do, in fact, perceive the world differently. European Americans are individualistic people who tend to focus on themselves and their sense of personal control. In contrast, East Asians are collectivist people who tend to focus on their personal relationships and social responsibilities. As a consequence, European Americans tend to explain actions in terms of internal factors (“she did it because she chose to do it”). In comparison, East Asians tend to explain actions in terms of their social context (“he did it because it was his responsibility to his family”) (Norenzayan & Nisbett, 2000). Do such cultural differences affect our everyday perception of objects and events? Apparently they do. In one study, American and Japanese participants were shown drawings of everyday scenes, such as a farm. Later, they saw a slightly changed version of the scene. Some of the changes were made to the focal point, or figure of the scene. Other changes altered the surrounding context, or ground of the scene. Americans, it turns out, were better at detecting changes in the figure of a scene. Japanese participants were better at finding alterations in the background (Nisbett & Miyamoto, 2005). This illusion is achieved by carefully distorting the proportions of the walls, floor, ceiling, and windows. Because the left corner of the Ames room is farther from a viewer than the right, a person standing in that corner looks very small; one standing in the nearer, shorter right corner looks very large. A person who walks from the left to the right corner, will seem to “magically” grow larger. Before we continue, read aloud the short phrase in Figure 5.25. Did you read, “Paris in the spring”? If so, look again. The word the appears twice in the phrase. Because of past experience with the English language, good readers often overlook the repeated word. Again, the effects of perceptual learning are apparent. (Now you know why it is easy to miss spelling mistakes while proofreading your essays.) As mentioned in Chapter 4, the brain is especially sensitive to perceptual features such as lines, shapes, edges, spots, and colors. At least some of this sensitivity appears to be learned. Colin Blakemore and Graham Cooper raised kittens in a room with only vertical stripes on the walls. Another set of kittens saw only horizontal stripes. When returned to normal environments, the “horizontal” cats could easily jump onto a chair, but when walking on the floor, they bumped into chair legs. “Vertical” cats, on the other hand, easily avoided chair legs, but they missed when trying to jump to horizontal surfaces. The cats raised with vertical stripes were “blind” to horizontal lines, and the “horizontal” cats acted Paris as if vertical lines were invisible. In in the such cases, there is an actual the spring decrease in brain cells tuned to the missing features (Blakemore Figure 5.25 & Cooper, 1970). • • To explain this difference, Chua, Boland, and Nisbett (2005) presented American and Chinese participants with pictures of a figure (such as a tiger) placed on a ground (such as a jungle) and monitored their eye-movement patterns. The Americans focused their eye movements on the figure; Chinese participants made more eye movements around the ground. In other words, Westerners have a relatively narrow focus of attention, whereas Easterners have a broader focus of attention. Apparently, the society we live in can, indeed, influence even our most basic perceptual habits. Perceptual features might seem removed from daily experience. Nevertheless, they can have a profound effect on human behavior. In recognizing faces, for example, a consistent other-race effect occurs. This is a sort of “they all look alike to me” bias in perceiving persons from other racial and ethnic groups. In tests of facial recognition, people are much better at recognizing faces of their own race than others. One reason for this difference is that we typically have more experience with people from our own race. As a result, we become very familiar with the features that help us recognize different persons. For other groups, we lack the perceptual expertise needed to accurately separate one face from another (Sporer, 2001). Okay, so maybe members of different races or ethnic groups have developed perceptual habits that lead them to see in-group faces differently, but we all see everything else the same, right? For an answer, see “Do They See What We See?” Inverted Vision Would it be possible for an adult to adapt to a completely new perceptual world? An answer comes from an experiment in which a person wore goggles that turned the world upside down and reversed objects from right to left. At first, even the simplest tasks — walking, eating, and so forth — were incredibly difficult. Imagine trying to reach for a door handle and watching your hand shoot off in the wrong direction. Perceptual learning Changes in how we construct sensory information into percepts that can be attributed to prior experience. Perceptual habits Well-established patterns of perceptual organization and attention. Perceptual features Important elements of a stimulus pattern, such as lines, shapes, edges, spots, and colors. CHAPTER 5 David Young-Wolff/PhotoEdit 168 • Figure 5.26 Are the center dots in both figures the same size? Inverted vision. Adaptation to complete inversion of the visual world is possible, but challenging. Participants in the experiment also reported that head movements made the world swing violently through space, causing severe headaches and nausea. Yet after several days they began to adapt to inverted vision. Their success, although not complete, was impressive. Did everything turn upright again for the humans? No. While they wore the goggles, their visual images remained upside down. But in time they learned to perform most routine activities, and their inverted world began to seem relatively normal. In later experiments, some people wearing inverting lenses were able to successfully drive cars. One person even flew an airplane (Kohler, 1962). These feats are like driving or flying upside down, with right and left reversed. Some ride! Interacting with a new visual world through active movement (self-generated action) seems to be a key to rapid adaptation. In one experiment, people wore glasses that grossly distorted vision. Those who walked on their own adapted more quickly than persons pushed around in a wheeled cart (Held, 1971). Why does movement help? Probably because commands sent to the muscles can be related to sensory feedback. Remaining immobile would be like watching a weird movie over which you have no control. There would be less reason for any perceptual learning to occur. The Context of Perception The external context in which a stimulus is judged is an important factor affecting perception. Context refers to information surrounding a stimulus. For example, a man 6 feet in height will look “tall” when surrounded by others of average height, and “short” among a group of professional basketball players. In Figure 5.26, the center circle is the same size in both designs. But like the man in different company, context alters the circle’s apparent size. The importance of context is also shown by Figure 5.27. What do you see in the middle? If you read across, context causes it to be organized as a 13. Reading down makes it a B. • • • Figure 5.27 Context alters the meaning of the middle figure. Illusions Perceptual learning is responsible for a number of illusions. In an illusion, length, position, motion, curvature, or direction is consistently misjudged. Note that illusions are distorted perceptual constructions of stimuli that actually exist. In a hallucination, people perceive objects or events that have no external reality (Lepore, 2002). For example, they hear voices that are not there. (See “Staying in Touch with Reality.”) Illusions and hallucinations can be distinguished by reality testing. What do you mean by reality testing? In any situation having an element of doubt or uncertainty, reality testing involves obtaining additional information to check your perceptions. If you think you see a 3-foot-tall butterfly, you can confirm you are hallucinating by trying to touch its wings. To detect an illusion, you may have to measure a drawing or apply a straightedge to it. Figure 5.28 shows a powerful illusion called Fraser’s spiral. What appears to be a spiral is actually made up of a series of closed circles. Most people cannot spontaneously see this reality. Instead, they must carefully trace one of the circles to confirm what is “real” in the design. • 169 Perceiving the World T HE CLI N I CA L FI LE Staying in Touch with Reality Even though Nash suffered from schizophrenia, he eventually learned to use his ability to engage in reality testing to sort out which of his experiences were perceptions and which were hallucinations. Unlike John Nash, most people who experience full-blown hallucinations also have a limited ability to reality test (Hohwy & Rosenberg, 2005). Curiously, “sane hallucinations” also occur. Charles Bonnet syndrome is a rare condition that afflicts mainly older people who are partially blind but not mentally disturbed. They may “see” people, animals, buildings, plants, and other objects appear and disappear in front of their eyes. One older man suffering from partial blindness and leukemia complained of seeing animals in his house, including cattle and bears (Jacob et al., 2004). However, people experiencing “sane hallucinations” can more easily tell that their hallucinations aren’t real because their capacity to reality test is not impaired. Such unusual experiences show how powerfully the brain seeks meaningful patterns in sensory input and the role reality testing plays in our normal perceptual experience. Dennis Coon Just imagine that often, and without warning, you hear a voice shouting, “Buckets of blood!” or see blood spattering across the walls of your bedroom. Chances are people would think you are mentally disturbed. Hallucinations are a major symptom of psychosis, dementia, epilepsy, migraine headaches, alcohol withdrawal, and drug intoxication (Spence & David, 2004) and are one of the clearest signs that a person has “lost touch with reality.” Yet consider the case of mathematician John Nash (the subject of A Beautiful Mind, the winner of the 2002 Oscar for best film). Illusions are a fascinating challenge to our understanding of perception. On occasion, they also have practical uses. An illusion called stroboscopic (strobe-oh-SKOP-ik) movement puts the “motion” in motion pictures. Stroboscopic movement refers to the illusory motion perceived when objects are shown in rapidly changing positions. The strobe lights used on dance floors reverse this illusion. Each time the strobe flashes, it “freezes” dancers in particular positions. However, if the light flashes fast enough, normal motion is seen. In a similar way, movies project a rapid series of “snapshots,” so the gaps in motion are imperceptible. Context Information surrounding a stimulus. Illusion A misleading or distorted perception. Hallucination An imaginary sensation, such as seeing, hearing, or smelling something that does not exist in the external world. Figure 5.28 The limits of pure perception. Even simple designs are easily •misperceived. Fraser’s spiral is actually a series of concentric circles. The illusion is so Reality testing Obtaining additional information to check on the accuracy of perceptions. powerful that people who try to trace one of the circles sometimes follow the illusory spiral and jump from one circle to the next. (After Seckel, 2000.) Stroboscopic movement Illusion of movement in which an object is shown in a rapidly changing series of positions. 170 CHAPTER 5 (a) Which of the horizontal lines is longer? (b) Is the diagonal a single straight line? Check it with a ruler. (c) Is this a drawing of a staircase descending from upper left to lower right . . . or is it the view of the underneath of a staircase from lower right to upper left? (d) Are these lines parallel? Cover some of the slash marks to see. (e) Which line is longer, horizontal or vertical? (f) Notice how the background distorts the square. (g) Which quadrilateral is larger? (h) Which column is shortest? Which is longest? • Figure 5.29 Some interesting perceptual illusions. Such illusions are a normal part of visual perception. Can other illusions be explained? Not in all cases, or to everyone’s satisfaction. Generally speaking, size and shape constancy, habitual eye movements, continuity, and perceptual habits combine in various ways to produce the illusions in Figure 5.29. Rather than attempt to explain all the pictured illusions, let’s focus on one deceptively simple example. Consider the drawing in Figure 5.29a. This is the familiar Müller-Lyer (MEOO-ler-LIE-er) illusion, in which the horizontal line with arrowheads appears shorter than the line with Vs. A • • • Figure 5.30 Why does line (b) in the Müller-Lyer illusion look longer than line (a)? Probably because it looks more like a distant corner than a nearer one. Because the vertical lines form images of the same length, the more “distant” line must be perceived as larger. As you can see in the drawing on the right, additional depth cues accentuate the Müller-Lyer illusion. (After Enns & Coren, 1995.) (a) (b) quick measurement will show that they are the same length. How can we explain this illusion? Evidence suggests it is based on a lifetime of experience with the edges and corners of rooms and buildings. Richard Gregory (2000) believes you see the horizontal line with the Vs as if it were the corner of a room viewed from inside ( Figure 5.30). The line with arrowheads, on the other hand, suggests the corner of a room or building seen from outside. In other words, cues that suggest a 3-D space alter our perception of a 2-D design (Enns & Coren, 1995). • Perceiving the World View I View II 171 View III Figure 5.31 “Young woman/old woman” illustrations. As an interesting demonstration of perceptual expectancy, show some of your •friends view I and some view II (cover all other views). Next show your friends view III and ask them what they see. Those who saw view I should see the old woman in view III; those who saw view II should see the young woman in view III. Can you see both? (After Leeper, 1935.) Earlier, to explain the moon illusion, we said that if two objects make images of the same size, the more distant object must be larger. This is known formally as size-distance invariance (the size of an object’s image is precisely related to its distance from the eyes). Gregory believes the same concept explains the Müller-Lyer illusion. If the V-tipped line looks farther away than the arrowheadtipped line, you must compensate by seeing the V-tipped line as longer. This explanation presumes that you have had years of experience with straight lines, sharp edges, and corners — a pretty safe assumption in our culture. Is there any way to show that past experience causes the illusion? If we could test someone who saw only curves and wavy lines as a child, we would know if experience with a “square” culture is important. Fortunately, the Zulus, a group of people in South Africa, live in a “round” culture. In their daily lives, Zulus rarely encounter a straight line: Their houses are shaped like rounded mounds and arranged in a circle, tools and toys are curved, and there are few straight roads or square buildings. What happens if a Zulu looks at the Müller-Lyer design? The typical Zulu villager does not experience the illusion. At most, she or he sees the V-shaped line as slightly longer than the other (Gregory, 1990). This seems to confirm the importance of past experience and perceptual habits in determining our view of the world. But, like many topics in psychology, room for debate remains. The Müller-Lyer illusion also seems to be partly based on directly misperceiving the location of the ends of the lines (Morgan, Hole, & Glennerster, 1990). Thus, it could be that both apparent size and misperception cause the illusion. Perceptual Expectancies — On Your Mark, Get Set Gateway Question: To what extent do we see what we expect to see? An excellent example of how top-down processing influences our experiences is found in perceptual expectancies. What is a perceptual expectancy? A runner in the starting blocks at a track meet is set to respond in a certain way. Likewise, past experience, motives, context, or suggestions may create a perceptual expectancy (or set) that prepares you to perceive in a certain way. If a car backfires, runners at a track meet may jump the gun. As a matter of fact, we all frequently jump the gun when perceiving. Beginning artists are notoriously bad at drawing faces. Why? When drawing a face, a novice tends to think “nose, mouth, eyes, ears” and tries to draw what he or she thinks each of these features looks like, instead of what they actually look like (Cohen & Bennett, 1997). In essence, an expectancy is a perceptual hypothesis we are very likely to apply to a stimulus—even if applying it is inappropriate. Perceptual sets often lead us to see what we expect to see. For example, let’s say you are driving across the desert. You are very low on gas. Finally, you see a sign approaching. On it are the words FUEL AHEAD. You relax, knowing you will not be stranded. But as you draw nearer, the words on the sign become FOOD AHEAD. Most people have had similar experiences in which expectations altered their perceptions. To observe perceptual expectancies firsthand, perform the demonstration described in Figure 5.31. Perceptual expectancies are frequently created by suggestion. In one study (wine snobs take note), participants given a taste of a $90 wine reported that it tasted better than a $10 wine. Functional MRI images confirmed that brain areas related to pleasure were indeed more active when participants tasted the more expensive wine (Plassmann et al., 2008). The twist is that exactly the same wine was served in both cases. Merely suggesting that the • Müller-Lyer illusion Two equal-length lines tipped with inward or outward pointing Vs appear to be of different lengths. Size-distance invariance The strict relationship between the distance an object lies from the eyes and the size of its image. Perceptual expectancy (or set) A readiness to perceive in a particular manner, induced by strong expectations. CHAPTER 5 wine was expensive created a perceptual expectancy that it would taste better. And so it did (advertisers also take note). Does it really make that much difference what you call someone or something? Perceptual categories, especially those defined by labels, do make a difference. This is especially true in perceiving people, where even trained observers may be influenced. For example, in one study, psychotherapists were shown a videotaped interview. Half the therapists were told that the man being interviewed was applying for a job. The rest were told that the man was a mental patient. Therapists who thought the man was a job applicant perceived him as “realistic,” “sincere,” and “pleasant.” Those who thought he was a patient perceived him as “defensive,” “dependent,” and “impulsive” (Langer & Abelson, 1974). In the next section, we will go beyond normal perception to ask, “Is extrasensory perception possible?” Before we do that, here’s a chance to answer the question, “Is remembering the preceding discussion possible?” K NOW LED GE B U I L D E R Perceptual Learning and Expectancies RECITE 1. Perceptual habits may become so ingrained that they lead us to misperceive a stimulus. T or F? 2. The Ames room is used to test for adaptation to inverted vision. T or F? 3. Perceptual learning seems to program the brain for sensitivity to important __________________________ of the environment. 4. An important factor in adaptation to inverted vision is a. learning new categories b. active movement c. overcoming illusions d. the horizontal-vertical invariance 5. Reality testing can be used to distinguish hallucinations and illusions. T or F? 6. Size-distance relationships appear to underlie which two illusions? _______________________ and ______________________________ 7. When a person is prepared to perceive events in a particular way, it is said that a perceptual expectancy or ____________________ exists. REFLECT Critical Thinking 8. What size object do you think you would have to hold at arm’s length to cover up a full moon? Relate How has perceptual learning affected your ability to safely drive a car? For example, what do you pay attention to at intersections? Where do you habitually look as you are driving? If you spent a year hiking the Amazon River Basin, what effect might it have on your perception of the Müller-Lyer illusion? You have almost certainly misperceived a situation at some time because of a perceptual expectancy. How were your perceptions influenced? Extrasensory Perception — Do You Believe in Magic? Gateway Question: Is extrasensory perception possible? About half the general public believes in the existence of extrasensory perception (ESP) — the purported ability to perceive events in ways that cannot be explained by known sensory capacities (Wiseman & Watt, 2006). Actually, it’s surprising that even more people aren’t believers. ESP and other paranormal events are treated as accepted facts in many movies and television programs. Regardless, very few psychologists share this belief. What is the evidence for and against extrasensory perception? Parapsychology is the study of ESP and other psi phenomena (events that seem to defy accepted scientific laws). (Psi is pronounced like “sigh.”) Parapsychologists seek answers to the questions raised by three basic forms that ESP could take. These are: 1. Telepathy. The purported ability to communicate directly with another person’s mind. When the other person is dead, the communications are called mediumship. 2. Clairvoyance. The purported ability to perceive events or gain information in ways that appear unaffected by distance or normal physical barriers. 3. Precognition. The purported ability to perceive or accurately predict future events. Precognition may take the form of prophetic dreams that foretell the future. While we are at it, we might as well toss in another purported psi ability: 4. Psychokinesis. The purported ability to exert influence over inanimate objects by willpower (“mind over matter”). (Psychokinesis cannot be classed as a type of ESP, but it is frequently studied by parapsychologists.) The formal investigation of psi events owes much to the late J. B. Rhine, who tried to study ESP objectively. Many of Rhine’s experiments made use of Zener cards (a deck of 25 cards, each bearing one of five symbols) ( Figure 5.32). In a typical clairvoyance test, people try to guess the symbols on the cards as they are turned up from a shuffled deck. In a typical telepathy test, a receiver tries to guess the correct symbol by reading the mind of a sender looking at a card. Pure guessing in these tests will produce an average score of 5 “hits” out of 25 cards. • + 172 Figure 5.32 ESP cards used by J. B. Rhine, an early experi•menter in parapsychology. Answers: 1. T 2. F 3. features 4. b 5. T 6. moon illusion, Müller-Lyer illusion 7. set 8. The most popular answers range from a quarter to a softball. Actually, a pea held in the outstretched hand will cover a full moon (Kunkel, 1993). If you listed an object larger than a pea, be aware that perceptions, no matter how accurate they seem, may distort reality. Perceiving the World An Appraisal of ESP Psychologists as a group are highly skeptical about psi abilities. If you’ve ever had an apparent clairvoyant or telepathic experience you might be convinced that ESP exists. However, the difficulty of excluding coincidence makes such experiences less conclusive than they might seem. Consider a typical “psychic” experience: During the middle of the night, a woman away for a weekend visit suddenly had a strong impulse to return home. When she arrived she found the house on fire with her husband asleep inside (Rhine, 1953). An experience like this is certainly striking, but it does not confirm the reality of ESP. You may not realize it, but such coincidences occur quite often. On any given night, many, many people might act on a “premonition.” If, by coincidence, one person’s hunch turns out to be correct, it may be reinterpreted as precognition or clairvoyance (Marks, 2000; Wiseman & Watt, 2006). In contrast, the vast majority of false premonitions will simply be forgotten. Fraud and Skepticism Unfortunately, some of Rhine’s most dramatic early experiments used badly printed Zener cards that allowed the symbols to show faintly on the back. It is also very easy to cheat, by marking cards with a fingernail or by noting marks on the cards caused by normal use. Even if this were not the case, there is evidence that early experimenters sometimes unconsciously gave people cues about cards with their eyes, facial gestures, or lip movements. In short, none of 173 the early studies in parapsychology were done in a way that eliminated the possibility of deliberate fraud or the accidental “leakage” of helpful information (Alcock, Burns, & Freeman, 2003). Modern parapsychologists are now well aware of the need for double-blind experiments, security and accuracy in record keeping, meticulous control, and repeatability of experiments (Milton & Wiseman, 1997; O’Keeffe & Wiseman, 2005). In the last 10 years, hundreds of experiments have been reported in parapsychological journals. Many of them seem to support the existence of psi abilities. Then why do most psychologists remain skeptical about psi abilities? For one thing, fraud continues to plague the field. The need for skepticism is especially great anytime there’s money to be made from purported psychic abilities. For example, the owners of the “Miss Cleo” TV-psychic operation were convicted of felony fraud in 2002. “Miss Cleo,” supposedly a Jamaican psychic, was really just an actress from Los Angeles. People who paid $4.99 a minute for a “reading” from “Miss Cleo” actually reached one of several hundred operators. These people were hired through ads that read, “No experience necessary.” Despite being entirely faked, the “Miss Cleo” scam brought in more than $1 billion before it was shut down. Anyone can learn to do “cold readings” (Wood et al., 2003) well enough to produce satisfied customers. Cold reading is a set of techniques that are used to lead people to believe in the truth of what a psychic or medium is saying about them. These include a reliance on many of the same techniques used by practitioners of the horoscope, such as uncritical acceptance, positive instances, and the Barnum effect. (Remember Chapter 1, pages 18–19?) The “psychic” begins a “reading” by making general statements about a person. The “psychic” then plays “hot and cold” by attending to the person’s facial expressions, body language, or tone of voice. When the “psychic” is “hot” (on the right track), the “psychic” continues to make similar statements about the person. If the person’s reactions signal that the “psychic” is “cold,” the psychic drops that topic or line of thought and tries another (Hyman, 2007). © Kes/www.CartoonStock.com Extrasensory perception The purported ability to perceive events in ways that cannot be explained by known capacities of the sensory organs. Parapsychology The study of extranormal psychological events, such as extrasensory perception. Psi phenomena Events that seem to lie outside the realm of accepted scientific laws. Telepathy The purported ability to directly know another person’s thoughts. Clairvoyance The purported ability to perceive events at a distance or through physical barriers. Precognition The purported ability to accurately predict future events. Psychokinesis The purported ability to mentally alter or influence objects or events. Zener cards A deck of 25 cards bearing various symbols and used in early parapsychological research. 174 CHAPTER 5 results of flipping the coin. Does this mean that the person, for a time, had precognition — then lost it? Parapsychologists tend to believe the answer is yes. Skeptics assume that nothing more than random matching occurred, as in the two-coin example. Reinterpretation is also a problem in psi experiments. For example, ex-astronaut Edgar Mitchell claimed he did a successful telepathy experiment from space. Yet news accounts never mentioned that on some trials Mitchell’s “receivers” scored above chance, whereas on others they scored below chance. Although you might assume that below-chance trials were failures to find telepathy, Mitchell reinterpreted them as “successes,” claiming that they represented intentional “psi missing.” But, as skeptics have noted, if both high scores and low scores count as successes, how can you lose? Of course, in many ESP tests the outcome is beyond debate. A good example is provided by ESP experiments done through newspapers, radio, and television. In these mass media studies, people attempted to identify ESP targets from a distance. The results of more than 1.5 million ESP trials done through the mass media are easy to summarize: There was no significant ESP effect (Milton & Wiseman, 1999). Zero. Zip. Nada. Clearly, state lottery organizers have nothing to fear! Inconclusive Results, Statistics and Chance Inconsistency is another major problem in psi research. For every study with positive results, there are many others that fail and are never published (Alcock, 2003). Unfortunately, many of the most spectacular findings in parapsychology simply cannot be replicated (reproduced or repeated) (Hyman, 1996a). More important, improved research methods usually result in fewer positive results (Hyman, 1996b; O’Keeffe & Wiseman, 2005). Even the same researchers using the same experimental subjects typically can’t get similar results every time (Schick & Vaughn, 2001). It is rare — in fact, almost unheard of — for a person to maintain psi ability over any sustained period (Alcock, Burns, & Freeman, 2003). ESP researchers believe this “decline effect” shows that parapsychological skills are very fragile. But critics argue that a person who only temporarily scores above chance has just received credit for a run of luck (a statistically unusual outcome that could occur by chance alone). When the run is over, it is not fair to assume that ESP is temporarily gone. We must count all attempts. To understand the run-of-luck criticism, imagine that you flip a coin 100 times and record the results. You then flip another coin 100 times, again recording the results. The two lists are compared. For any 10 pairs of flips, we would expect heads or tails to match 5 times. Let’s say that you go through the list and find a set of 10 pairs where 9 out of 10 matched. This is far above chance expectation. But does it mean that the first coin “knew” what was going to come up on the second coin? The idea is obviously silly. Now, what if a person guesses 100 times what will come up on a coin. Again, we might find a set of 10 guesses that matches the • (a) Figure 5.33 Stage ESP If psychic phenomena do occur, they certainly can’t be controlled well enough to be used by entertainers. Stage ESP simulates ESP for the purpose of entertainment. Like stage magic, it is based on sleight of hand, deception, and patented gadgets ( Figure 5.33). A case in point is self-proclaimed “psychic,” Uri Geller, a former nightclub magician who “astounded” audiences — and some scientists — with apparent telepathy, psychokinesis, and precognition. • (b) (c) Fake psychokinesis. (a) The performer shows an observer several straight keys. While doing so, he bends one of the keys by placing its tip in the slot of another key. Normally, this is done out of sight, behind the “psychic’s” hand. It is clearly shown here so that you can see how the deception occurs. (b) Next, the “psychic” places the two keys in the observer’s hand and closes it. By skillful manipulation, the observer has been kept from seeing the bent key. The performer then “concentrates” on the keys to “bend them with psychic energy.” (c) The bent key is revealed to the observer. “Miracle” accomplished! (Adapted from Randi, 1997.) Perceiving the World Implications After close to 130 years of investigation, it is still impossible to say conclusively whether psi events occur. As we have seen, a close look at psi experiments often reveals serious problems of evidence, procedure, and scientific rigor (Alcock, Burns, & Freeman, 2003; Hyman, 2007; Stokes, 2001). The more closely psi experiments are examined, the more likely it is that claimed successes will evaporate (Alcock, 2003; Stokes, 2001). As one critic put it, positive ESP results usually mean “Error Some Place” (Marks, 2000). Yet, being a skeptic does not mean a person is against something. It means that you are unconvinced. The purpose of this discussion, then, has been to counter the uncritical acceptance of psi events that is rampant in the media. What would it take to scientifically demonstrate the existence of ESP? Quite simply, a set of instructions that would allow any competent, unbiased observer to produce a psi event under standardized conditions that rule out any possibility of fraud (Schick & Vaughn, 2001). As we mentioned in Chapter 1, professional magician and skeptic James Randi even offers a $1,000,000 prize to anyone who can demonstrate evidence of psi events under standardized conditions. No one has yet claimed the prize. (You can read all about it if you google the James Randi Educational Foundation.) Undoubtedly, some intrepid researchers will continue their attempts to supply just that. Others remain skeptics and consider 130 years of inconclusive efforts reason enough to abandon the concept of ESP (Marks, 2000). At the least, it seems essential to be carefully skeptical of evidence reported in the popular press or by researchers who are uncritical “true believers.” (But then, you already knew we were going to say that, didn’t you!) A Look Ahead In this chapter we have moved from basic sensations to the complexities of perceiving people and events. We have also probed some of the controversies concerning ESP. In the Psychology in Action section, we will return to “everyday” perception for a look at perceptual awareness. KNOWL E DG E B U I L DE R Extrasensory Perception RECITE 1. Four purported psi events investigated by parapsychologists are clairvoyance, telepathy, precognition, and _____________________. 2. Zener cards were used in early studies of a. psi phenomena b. inattentional blindness c. the Müller-Lyer illusion d. top-down processing 3. Natural, or “real-life,” occurrences are regarded as the best evidence for the existence of ESP. T or F? 4. Replication rates are very high for ESP experiments. T or F? 5. Skeptics attribute positive results in psi experiments to statistical runs of luck. T or F? REFLECT Critical Thinking 6. What would you estimate is the chance that two people will have the same birthday (day and month, but not year) in a group of 30 people? 7. A “psychic” on television offers to fix broken watches for viewers. Moments later, dozens of viewers call the station to say that their watches miraculously started running again. What have they overlooked? Relate Let’s say that a friend of yours is an avid fan of TV shows that feature paranormal themes. See if you can summarize for her or him what is known about ESP. Be sure to include evidence for and against the existence of ESP and some of the thinking errors associated with nonskeptical belief in the paranormal. Answers: 1. psychokinesis 2. a 3. F 4. F 5. T 6. Most people assume that this would be a relatively rare event. Actually there is a 71 percent chance that two people will share a birthday in a group of 30. Most people probably underestimate the natural rate of occurrence of many seemingly mysterious coincidences (Alcock, Burns, & Freeman, 2003). 7. When psychologists handled watches awaiting repair at a store, 57 percent began running again, with no help from a “psychic.” Believing the “psychic’s” claim also overlooks the impact of big numbers: If the show reached a large audience, at least a few “broken” watches would start working merely by chance. During testing, it seemed that Geller could sense which of 10 film canisters contained a hidden object, he correctly guessed the number that would come up on a die shaken in a closed box, and he reproduced drawings sealed in envelopes. Was Geller cheating, or was he using some ability beyond normal perception? There is little doubt that Geller was cheating (Randi, 1997). It’s now clear that tests of Geller’s performance were incredibly sloppy. For instance, Geller reproduced sealed drawings in a room next to the one where the drawings were made. Original reports failed to mention that there was a hole in the wall between the rooms, through which Geller could have heard descriptions of the pictures as they were being drawn. Likewise, in the “die in the box” tests Geller was allowed to hold the box, shake it, and have the honor of opening it. Why weren’t such details reported? Sensational and uncritical reporting of apparent paranormal events is widespread. Hundreds of books, articles, and television programs are produced each year by people who are getting rich by promoting unsupported claims. If a person did have psychic powers, he or she would not have to make a living by entertaining others. A quick trip to a casino would allow the person to retire for life. 175 Replicate To reproduce or repeat. Run of luck A statistically unusual outcome (as in getting five heads in a row when flipping a coin) that could still occur by chance alone. Stage ESP The simulation of ESP for the purpose of entertainment. 176 CHAPTER 5 PSY C HO L O GY IN A CT ION Perception and Objectivity — Believing Is Seeing Gateway Question: How can I learn to perceive events more accurately? Have you ever seen the sun set? You may think you have. Yet, in reality, we know the sun does not “set.” Instead, our viewing angle changes as the earth turns, until the sun is obscured by the horizon. Want to try the alternative? This evening, stand facing the west. With practice, you can learn to feel yourself being swept backward on the rotating surface of the earth as you watch an unmoving sun recede in the distance. This radical shift in perspective illustrates the limitations of “objective” observation. As we have learned in this chapter, seeing a “sunset” is a perceptual construction (mental model) of an external event. As we have seen, perception reflects the needs, expectations, attitudes, values, and beliefs of the perceiver. In this light, the phrase “seeing is believing” must be modified. Clearly, we see what we believe, as well as believe what we see ( Figure 5.34). In some cases, subjective perception nurtures the personal vision valued in art, music, poetry, and scientific innovation. Often, however, it is a real liability. In the courtroom, eyewitness testimony can be a key to proving guilt or innocence. The claim “I saw it with my own eyes” still carries a lot of weight with a jury. Too many jurors (unless they have taken a psychology course) tend to assume that eyewitness testimony is nearly infallible (Durham & Dane, 1999). Even U.S. judges are vulnerable to overoptimism about eyewitness testimony (Wise & Safer, 2004). But, to put it bluntly, eyewitness testimony is frequently wrong (Wells & Olson, 2003). Recall, for instance, that one of your authors would have sworn in court that he had seen a murder taking place at the supermarket — if he hadn’t received more information to correct his misperceptions. bridges Distortions in memory also affect the accuracy of eyewitness testimony. See Chapter 8, pages 256–259. What about witnesses who are certain that their perceptions were accurate? Should juries believe them? Actually, having confidence in your testimony has almost no bearing on its accuracy (Brewer & Wells, 2006)! Psychologists are gradually convincing lawyers, judges, and police that eyewitness errors are common (Yarmey, 2003). Even so, thousands of people have been wrongfully convicted (Scheck, Neufeld, & Dwyer, 2000). AP/Wide World Photo • Unfortunately, perception rarely provides an “instant replay” of events. As stated earlier, impressions formed when a person is surprised, threatened, or under stress are especially prone to distortion (Yuille & Daylen, 1998). In addition, being questioned tends to make witnesses more confident about what they saw, even if they are wrong. Thus, police questioning can actually degrade the value of the testimony witnesses give later, in court (Shaw, 1996). “I Saw It with My Own Eyes” • Figure 5.34 It is difficult to look at this simple drawing without perceiving depth. Yet the drawing is nothing more than a collection of flat shapes. Turn this page counterclockwise 90 degrees and you will see 3 Cs, one within another. When the drawing is turned sideways, it seems nearly flat. However, if you turn the page upright again, a sense of depth will reappear. Clearly, you have used your knowledge and expectations to construct an illusion of depth. The drawing itself would only be a flat design if you didn’t invest it with meaning. Even in broad daylight, eyewitness testimony is untrustworthy. In 2001 an American Airlines plane crashed near Kennedy International Airport in New York. Hundreds of people saw the plane go down. Half of them said the plane was on fire. Flight recorders showed there was no fire. One witness in five saw the plane make a right turn. An equal number saw it make a left turn! As one investigator commented, the best witness may be a “kid under 12 years old who doesn’t have his parents around.” Adults, it seems, are easily swayed by their expectations. Because impressions formed when a person is surprised, threatened, or under stress are especially prone to distortion, witnesses to crimes often disagree. As a dramatic demonstration of this problem, a college professor was attacked by an actor in a staged assault. Immediately after the event, 141 witnesses were questioned in detail. Their descriptions were then compared with a videotape made of the staged “crime.” The total accuracy score for the group (on features such as appearance, age, weight, and height of the assailant) was only 25 percent of the maximum possible (Buckhout, 1974). Similarly, a study of real eyewitness cases found that the wrong person was chosen from police lineups 25 percent of the time (Levi, 1998). Wouldn’t the victim of a crime remember more than a mere witness? Not necessarily. A revealing study found that eyewitness accuracy is virtually the same for witnessing a crime (seeing a pocket calculator stolen) as it is for being a victim (seeing one’s own watch stolen) (Hosch & Cooper, 1982). Placing more weight on the testimony of victims may be a serious mistake. In many crimes, victims fall prey to weapon focus. Understandably, they fix their entire attention on the knife, gun, or other weapon used by an attacker. In doing so, they fail to notice details of appear- Perceiving the World Table 5.2 • Factors Affecting the Accuracy of Eyewitness Perceptions Sources of Error Summary of Findings 1. Wording of questions An eyewitness’s testimony about an event can be affected by how the questions put to that witness are worded. 2. Postevent information Eyewitness testimony about an event often reflects not only what was actually seen but also information obtained later on. 3. Attitudes, expectations An eyewitness’s perception and memory of an event may be affected by his or her attitudes and expectations. 4. Alcohol intoxication Alcohol intoxication impairs later ability to recall events. 5. Cross-racial perceptions Eyewitnesses are better at identifying members of their own race than they are at identifying people of other races. 6. Weapon focus The presence of a weapon impairs an eyewitness’s ability to accurately identify the culprit’s face. 7. Accuracyconfidence An eyewitness’s confidence is not a good predictor of his or her accuracy. 8. Exposure time The less time an eyewitness has to observe an event, the less well she or he will perceive and remember it. 9. Unconscious transference Eyewitnesses sometimes identify as a culprit someone they have seen in another situation or context. 10. Color perception Judgments of color made under monochromatic light (such as an orange street light) are highly unreliable. 11. Stress Very high levels of stress impair the accuracy of eyewitness perceptions. Adapted from Kassin et al., 2001. ance, dress, or other clues to identity (Pickel, French, & Betts, 2003). Additional factors that consistently lower eyewitness accuracy are summarized in Table 5.2 (Kassin et al., 2001; Wells & Olson, 2003). • Implications Now that DNA testing is available, more than 200 people who were convicted of murder, rape, and other crimes in the United States have been exonerated. Most of these innocent people were convicted mainly on the basis of eyewitness testimony. Each also spent years in prison before being cleared (Foxhall, 2000). How often are everyday perceptions as inaccurate or distorted as those of an emotional eyewitness? The answer we have been moving toward is very frequently. Bearing this in mind may help you be more tolerant of the views of others and more cautious about your own objectivity. It may also encourage more frequent reality testing on your part. Psychologist Sidney Jourard once offered a good example of reality testing. One of Jourard’s students believed her roommate was stealing from her. The student gradually became convinced of her roommate’s guilt but said nothing. As her distrust and anger grew, their relationship turned cold and distant. Finally, at Jourard’s urging, she confronted her roommate. The roommate cleared herself immediately and expressed relief when the puzzling change in their relationship was explained ( Jourard, 1974). With their friendship reestablished, the true culprit was soon caught. (The cleaning woman did it!) If you have ever concluded that someone was angry, upset, or unfriendly without checking the accuracy of your perceptions, you have fallen into a subtle trap. Personal objectivity is an elusive quality, requiring frequent reality testing to maintain. At the very least, it pays to ask a person what she or he is feeling when you are in doubt. Clearly, most of us could learn to be better “eyewitnesses” to daily events. Positive Psychology: Perceptual Awareness Do some people perceive things more accurately than others? Humanistic psychologist Abraham Maslow (1969) believed that some people perceive themselves and others with 177 unusual accuracy. Maslow characterized these people as especially alive, open, aware, and mentally healthy. He found that their perceptual styles were marked by immersion in the present; a lack of self-consciousness; freedom from selecting, criticizing, or evaluating; and a general “surrender” to experience. The kind of perception Maslow described is like that of a mother with her newborn infant, a child at Christmas, or two people in love. The Value of Paying Attention Whereas the average person has not reached perceptual restriction of the “if you’ve seen one tree, you’ve seen them all” variety, the fact remains that most of us tend to look at a tree and classify it into the perceptual category of “trees in general” without really appreciating the miracle standing before us. How, then, can we bring about dishabituation (a reversal of habituation) on a day-today basis? Does perceptual clarity require years of effort? Fortunately, a more immediate avenue is available. The deceptively simple key to dishabituation is this: Pay attention. The following story summarizes the importance of attention: One day a man of the people said to Zen Master Ikkyu: “Master, will you please write for me some maxims of the highest wisdom?” Ikkyu immediately took his brush and wrote the word “Attention.” “Is that all?” asked the man. “Will you not add something more?” Ikkyu then wrote twice running: “Attention. Attention.” “Well,” remarked the man rather irritably, “I really don’t see much depth or subtlety in what you have just written.” Then Ikkyu wrote the same word three times running: “Attention. Attention. Attention.” Half angered, the man demanded, “What does that word ‘Attention’ mean anyway?” And Ikkyu answered gently: “Attention means attention.” (Kapleau, 1966) To this we can add only one thought, provided by the words of poet William Blake: “If the doors of perception were cleansed, man would see everything as it is, infinite.” Dishabituation A reversal of habituation. CHAPTER 5 How to Become a Better “Eyewitness” to Life Here’s a summary of ideas from this chapter to help you maintain and enhance perceptual awareness and accuracy. 1. Remember that perceptions are constructions of reality. Learn to regularly question your own perceptions. Are they accurate? Could another interpretation fit the facts? What assumptions are you making? Could they be false? How might your assumptions be distorting your perceptions? 2. Break perceptual habits and interrupt habituation. Each day, try to get away from habitual, top-down processing and do some activities in new ways. For example, take different routes when you travel to work or school. Do routines, such as brushing your teeth or combing your hair, with your nonpreferred hand. Try to look at friends and family mem- bers as if they are persons you just met for the first time. 3. Seeking out-of-the-ordinary experiences. The possibilities here range from trying foods you don’t normally eat to reading opinions very different from your own. Experiences ranging from a quiet walk in the woods to a trip to an amusement park may be perceptually refreshing. 4. Beware of perceptual sets. Anytime you pigeonhole people, objects, or events, there is a danger that your perceptions will be distorted by expectations or preexisting categories. Be especially wary of labels and stereotypes. Try to see people as individuals and events as unique, onetime occurrences. 5. Be aware of the ways in which motives and emotions influence perceptions. It is difficult to avoid being swayed by your own interests, needs, desires, and emotions. But be aware of this trap and K NOW LED G E B U I L D E R Perceptual Awareness and Accuracy RECITE 1. Most perceptions can be described as active constructions of external reality. T or F? 2. Inaccuracies in eyewitness perceptions obviously occur in “real life,” but they cannot be reproduced in psychology experiments. T or F? 3. Accuracy scores for facts provided by witnesses to staged crimes may be as low as 25 percent correct. T or F? 4. Victims of crimes are more accurate eyewitnesses than are impartial observers. T or F? 5. A good antidote to perceptual habituation can be found in conscious efforts to a. reverse sensory gating b. pay attention c. achieve visual accommodation d. counteract shape constancy actively try to see the world through the eyes of others. Taking the other person’s perspective is especially valuable in disputes or arguments. Ask yourself, “How does this look to her or him?” 6. Make a habit of engaging in reality testing. Actively look for additional evidence to check the accuracy of your perceptions. Ask questions, seek clarifications, and find alternate channels of information. Remember that perception is not automatically accurate. You could be wrong — we all are frequently. 7. Pay attention. Make a conscious effort to pay attention to other people and your surroundings. Don’t drift through life in a haze. Listen to others with full concentration. Watch their facial expressions. Make eye contact. Try to get in the habit of approaching perception as if you are going to have to testify later about what you saw and heard. REFLECT Critical Thinking 6. Return for a moment to the incident described at the beginning of this chapter. What perceptual factors were involved in the first version of the “murder”? How did the girl affect what was seen? Relate Because perceptions are constructions or models of external events, we should all engage in more frequent reality testing. Can you think of a recent event when a little reality testing would have saved you from misjudging a situation? To improve your own perceptual awareness and accuracy, which strategies would you emphasize first? chapter in review Answers: 1. T 2. F 3. T 4. F 5. b 6. The girl’s misperception, communicated so forcefully to one of your authors, created a powerful expectancy that influenced what he perceived. Also, the event happened quickly (the exposure time was brief ), and the stressful or emotional nature of the incident encouraged his own misperception. 178 Gateways to Perception Perception is an active process of constructing sensations into a meaningful mental representation of the world. Attended sensations become part of the percept; motive and emotions influence attention. • Perceptions are based on simultaneous bottom-up and topdown processing. Complete percepts are assembled out of small sensory features in “bottom-up” fashion guided by preexisting knowledge applied “top-down” to help organize features into a meaningful whole. • Attention is selective, and it may be divided among various activities. Attention is closely related to stimulus intensity, repetition, contrast, change, and incongruity. • The phenomenon called inattentional blindness suggests that there can be no perception without attention. Perceiving the World 179 • Attention is accompanied by an orientation response. When a stimulus is repeated without change, the orientation response undergoes habituation. • Personal motives and values often alter perceptions by changing the evaluation of what is seen or by altering attention to specific details. Perceptual sets often lead us to perceive or misperceive what we expect to see. The Gestalt principles of organization bring order to our perceptions. • Parapsychology is the study of purported psi phenomena, including telepathy (as well as mediumship), clairvoyance, precognition, and psychokinesis. • Research in parapsychology remains controversial owing to a variety of problems and shortcomings. • The more carefully controlled an ESP experiment is, the less likely it is to produce evidence that ESP occurs. • Stage ESP is based on deception and tricks. • Separating figure and ground (object and background) is the most basic perceptual organization. • The following Gestalt principles also help organize sensations: nearness, similarity, continuity, closure, contiguity, common region, and combinations of the preceding. • Basic elements of line drawings appear to be universally recognized. • Perceptual organization shifts for ambiguous stimuli. Impossible figures resist stable organization altogether. • A perceptual organization may be thought of as a hypothesis held until evidence contradicts it. Perceptual constancies allow us to find regularity in the constantly changing energy patterns reaching our senses. • In vision, the image projected on the retina is constantly changing, but the external world appears stable and undistorted because of size, shape, and brightness constancy. Various visual cues are used to construct the experience of depth. • A basic, innate capacity for depth perception is present soon after birth. • Depth perception depends on binocular depth cues of retinal disparity and convergence. • Stereoscopic vision is created mainly by retinal disparity and the resulting overlap and mismatch of visual sensations. • Depth perception also depends on the monocular depth cue of accommodation. • Monocular “pictorial” depth cues also underlie depth perception. They are linear perspective, relative size, height in the picture plane, light and shadow, overlap, texture gradients, aerial haze, and motion parallax. • The moon illusion can be explained by the apparent-distance hypothesis, which emphasizes that many depth cues are present when the moon is near the horizon, and few are present when it is overhead. Perceptual learning influences the top-down organization and interpretation of sensations. • Studies of inverted vision show that even the most basic organization is subject to a degree of change. Active movement speeds adaptation to a new perceptual environment. • Perceptual judgments are not made in a vacuum. They are almost always related to context. • One of the most familiar of all illusions, the Müller-Lyer illusion, seems to be related to perceptual learning, linear perspective, size-distance invariance relationships, and mislocating the end points of the figure. • Suggestion, motives, emotions, attention, and prior experience combine in various ways to create perceptual sets, or expectancies. The bulk of the evidence to date is against the existence of ESP. Awareness of the factors that can distort perception is the key to improving perceptual accuracy. • Perception is an active construction of events. This is one reason why eyewitness testimony is surprisingly unreliable. Eyewitness accuracy is further damaged by weapon focus and a number of similar factors. • Perceptual accuracy is enhanced by reality testing, dishabituation, and conscious efforts to pay attention. • It is also valuable to break perceptual habits, to beware of perceptual sets, and to be aware of the ways in which motives and emotions influence perceptions. Web Resources For an up-to-date list of direct links to interesting sites, including those listed here, visit the student companion site for this book at www.cengage.com/psychology/coon Figure/Ground in Graphic Design Explore the use of the figureground principle in graphic design. Gallery of Illusions An interesting and fun website that presents illusory stimuli that are fun to view and that provides a basis to learn about sensation and perception. 3-D Stereograms Find the 3-D images hidden in these pictures. Perceptual Learning Explore an example of perceptual learning through wine tasting. James Randi Education Foundation Win $1,000,000 for proving, under scientifically controlled conditions, that you have ESP. The Innocence Project Read about real cases of people convicted through eyewitness testimony but later exonerated by DNA evidence. Avoiding Habituation Read about how advertisers apply the idea of habituation to the design of advertisements. 180 CHAPTER 5 Interactive Learning Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior Book Companion Website www.cengage.com/psychology/coon Visit your book companion website, where you will find flash cards, practice quizzes, web links, and more to help you study. Just what you need to know NOW! Spend time on what you need to master rather than on information you already have learned. Take a pre-test for this chapter, and CengageNOW will generate a personalized study plan based on your results. The study plan will identify the topics you need to review and direct you to online resources to help you master those topics. You can then take a post-test to help you determine the concepts you have mastered and what you will need to work on. Try it out! Go to www.cengage.com/login to sign in with an access code or to purchase access to this product. CHAPTER 6 States of Consciousness Gateway Theme Understanding states of consciousness can promote self-awareness and enhance personal effectiveness. Corbis/SuperStock Gateway Questions • What is an altered state of consciousness? • What are the effects of sleep loss or changes in sleep patterns? • • • • • • Why do we sleep? What are some sleep disorders and unusual sleep events? Do dreams have meaning? • What is hypnosis? Do meditation and sensory deprivation have any benefits? What are the effects of the more commonly used psychoactive drugs? How can dreams be used to promote personal understanding? 181 preview A Visit to Several States (of Consciousness) In Los Angeles, California, an aspiring actor consults a hypnotist for help in reducing her stage fright. In the American Southwest, a Navajo elder gives his congregation peyote tea, a sacrament in the Native American Church, as a drumbeat resounds in the darkness. In Toronto, Canada, three businesswomen head for a popular tavern after a particularly stressful day. In Big Bend National Park in southern Texas, a college student spends a day walking through the wilderness, in a quiet state of mindfulness meditation. In New Zealand, a Maori tohunga (priest) performs a nightlong ritual to talk to the spirits who created the world in the mythical period the Aborigines call Dreamtime. In Northern Ireland, a nun living in a convent spends an entire week in silent prayer and contemplation. In Berkeley, California, an artist spends 2 hours in a flotation chamber to clear her head before resuming work on a large painting. At a park in Amsterdam, a group of street musicians smoke a joint and sing for spare change. In Tucson, Arizona, one of your authors pours himself another cup of coffee. States of Consciousness — The Many Faces of Awareness instances, altered states have important cultural meanings. (See “Consciousness and Culture” for more information.) An unconscious person will die without constant care. Yet as crucial as consciousness is, we can’t really explain how it occurs (Robinson, 2008). Nevertheless, it is possible to identify various states of consciousness and to explore the role they play in our lives. Let’s begin with a look at the most common altered state, sleep and dreaming. Gateway Question: What is an altered state of consciousness? To be conscious means to be aware. Consciousness consists of all the sensations, perceptions, memories, and feelings you are aware of at any instant (Koch, 2004). (See “What Is It Like to Be a Bat?”) We spend most of our lives in waking consciousness, a state of clear, organized alertness. In waking consciousness we perceive times, places, and events as real, meaningful, and familiar. But states of consciousness related to fatigue, delirium, hypnosis, drugs, and euphoria may differ markedly from “normal” awareness. Everyone experiences at least some altered states, such as sleep, dreaming, and daydreaming (Blackmore, 2004). In everyday life, changes in consciousness may also accompany long-distance running, listening to music, making love, or other circumstances. Altered States of Consciousness How are altered states distinguished from normal awareness? During an altered state of consciousness (ASC), changes occur in the quality and pattern of mental activity. Typically there are distinct shifts in our perceptions, emotions, memories, time sense, thoughts, feelings of self-control, and suggestibility (Siegel, 2005). Definitions aside, most people know when they have experienced an ASC. Are there other causes of ASCs? In addition to the ones mentioned, we could add: sensory overload (a rave, Mardi Gras crowd, or mosh pit), monotonous stimulation (such as “highway hypnotism” on long drives), unusual physical conditions (high fever, hyperventilation, dehydration, sleep loss, near-death experiences), restricted sensory input, and many other possibilities. In some 182 Each of these people seeks to alter consciousness — in different ways, to different degrees, and for different reasons. As these examples suggest, consciousness can take many forms. In the discussion that follows, we will begin with the familiar realms of sleep and dreaming and then move to more exotic states of consciousness. Sleep — A Nice Place to Visit Gateway Question: What are the effects of sleep loss or changes in sleep patterns? Each of us will spend some 25 years of life asleep. Because sleep is familiar, many people think they know all about it. But many common-sense beliefs about sleep are false. For example, you are not totally unresponsive during sleep. A sleeping mother may ignore a jet thundering overhead but wake at the slightest whimper of her child. Likewise, you are more likely to awaken if you hear your own name spoken, instead of another. It’s even possible to do simple tasks while asleep. In one experiment, people learned to avoid an electric shock by touching a switch each time a tone sounded. Eventually, they could do it without waking. (This is much like the basic survival skill of turning off your alarm clock without waking.) Of course, sleep does impose limitations. Don’t expect to learn math, a foreign language, or other complex skills while asleep — especially if the snooze takes place in class (Froufe & Schwartz, 2001). But do expect that a good sleep will help you remember what you learned the day before (Fenn, Nusbaum, & Margoliash, 2003; Saxvig et al., 2008). States of Consciousness 183 CRIT ICA L T H I N KI N G What Is It Like to Be a Bat? Imagine hurtling through the air on leather wings while shrieking noisily. Suddenly, the faint echo of your own voice draws your attention to a moth that is frantically trying to evade you. You careen after it, twisting through the pitch-black jungle. Dodging trees and other bats, you catch the moth and savor your first meal of the still-young night. In his famous essay, “What Is It Like to Be a Bat?” Thomas Nagel (1974) points out that we can learn a lot about bats from an objec- tive, third-person point of view. Scientifically, we know that bats use echolocation (they emit sounds and interpret the echoes) to hunt insects at night. But what does that feel like from a subjective, first-person point of view? Have you ever been curious about what it is like to be a bat, or a dog, or a cat? What runs through Rover’s mind when he sniffs other dogs? Does Fluffy have dreams? Are they as strange as ours? Do cats ever worry about the future? Do they like music? Do animals feel joy? The Need for Sleep sleep. However, The Guinness Book of Records no longer recognizes sleep deprivation competitions because of possible health risks, so Randy still holds the “official” record.) What are the costs of sleep loss? At various times, Randy’s speech was slurred, and he couldn’t concentrate, remember clearly, or name common objects (Coren, 1996). Sleep loss also typically causes trembling hands, drooping eyelids, inattention, irritability, staring, increased pain sensitivity, and general discomfort (Doran, Van Dongen, & Dinges, 2001). Most people experience hypersomnia (hi-per-SOM-nee-ah: excessive daytime sleepiness) after even a few hours of sleep loss. Hypersomnia is a common problem during adolescence (Carskadon, Acebo, & Jenni, 2004). Rapid physical changes during puberty increase the need for sleep. However, the quality and quantity of sleep time tends to decrease during the teen years (Fukuda & Ishihara, 2001). Most people who have not slept for a day or two can still do interesting or complex mental tasks. But they have trouble paying attention, staying alert, and doing simple or boring routines (Belenky et al., 2003). They are also susceptible to microsleeps, which are brief shifts in brain activity to the pattern normally recorded during How strong is the need for sleep? Sleep is an innate biological rhythm that can never be entirely ignored (Lavie, 2001; Mistlberger, 2005). Of course, sleep will give way temporarily, especially at times of great danger. As comedian and filmmaker Woody Allen once put it, “The lion and the lamb shall lie down together, but the lamb will not be very sleepy.” However, there are limits to how long humans can go without sleep. A rare disease that prevents sleep always ends with stupor, coma, and death (Dauvilliers et al., 2004) ( Figure 6.1). How long could a person go without sleep? With few exceptions, 4 days or more without sleep becomes hell for everyone. The world record is held by Randy Gardner, who at age 17 went 264 hours (11 days) without sleep. Surprisingly, Randy needed only 14 hours of sleep to recover. As Randy found, most symptoms of sleep deprivation (sleep loss) are reversed by a single night’s rest. (In 2007, a man named Tony Wright went 266 hours without Timothy Ross/The Image Works • • Figure 6. 1 Not all animals sleep, but like humans, those that do have powerful sleep needs. For example, dolphins must voluntarily breathe air, which means they face the choice of staying awake or drowning. The dolphin solves this problem by sleeping on just one side of its brain at a time! The other half of the brain, which remains awake, controls breathing (Jouvet, 1999). According to Nagel, we cannot directly know the first-person experience of animals (or even other people, for that matter). The difficulty of knowing other minds is why the early behaviorists distrusted introspection. (Remember Chapter 1?) A key challenge for psychology is to use objective studies of the brain and behavior to help us understand the mind and consciousness, which are basically private phenomena (Koch, 2004; Robinson, 2008). Consciousness Mental awareness of sensations, perceptions, memories, and feelings. Waking consciousness A state of clear, organized alertness. Altered state of consciousness (ASC) A condition of awareness distinctly different in quality or pattern from waking consciousness. Biological rhythm Any repeating cycle of biological activity, such as sleep and waking cycles or changes in body temperature. Sleep deprivation Being prevented from getting desired or needed amounts of sleep. Hypersomnia Excessive daytime sleepiness. Microsleep A brief shift in brain-wave patterns to those of sleep. 184 CHAPTER 6 HUM AN D IVERS I T Y Throughout history, people have found ways to alter consciousness (Siegel, 2005). A dramatic example is the sweat lodge ceremony of the Sioux Indians. During the ritual, several men sit in total darkness inside a small chamber heated by coals. Cedar smoke, bursts of steam, and sage fill the air. The men chant rhythmically. The heat builds. At last they can stand it no more. The door is thrown open. Cooling night breezes rush in. And then? The cycle begins again — often to be repeated four or five times more. Like the yoga practices of Hindu mystics or the dances of the Whirling Dervishes of Turkey, the ritual “sweats” of the Sioux are meant to cleanse the mind and body. When they are especially intense, they bring altered awareness and personal revelation. People seek some altered states for pleasure, as is often true of drug intoxication. Yet as the Sioux illustrate, many cultures regard altered consciousness as a pathway to personal enlightenment. Indeed, all cultures and most religions recognize and accept some alterations of consciousness. However, the meaning given to these states varies greatly — from signs of “madness” and “possession” by spirits, to life-enhancing breakthroughs. Thus, cultural conditioning greatly affects what altered states we recognize, seek, consider normal, and attain (de Rios & Grob, 2005). sleep. Imagine placing an animal on a moving treadmill, over a pool of water. Even under these conditions, animals soon drift into repeated microsleeps. For a pilot or machine operator, this can spell disaster (Hardaway & Gregory, 2005). If a task is monotonous (such as factory work or air traffic control), no amount of sleep loss is safe. In fact, if you lose just 1 hour of sleep a night, it can affect your mood, memory, ability to pay attention, and even your health (Maas, 1999). Sleep helps keep the brain healthy by regulating its temperature, conserving energy, and aiding brain development and repair. Biologically, sleep is a necessity, not a luxury. When you drive, remember that microsleeps can lead to macroaccidents. Even if your eyes are open, you can fall asleep for a few seconds. A hundred thousand crashes every year are caused by sleepiness (Rau, 2005). Although coffee helps (Kamimori et al., 2005), if you are struggling to stay awake while driving, you should stop, quit fighting it, and take a short nap. Severe sleep loss can cause a temporary sleep-deprivation psychosis (loss of contact with reality). Confusion, disorientation, delusions, and hallucinations are typical of this reaction. Fortunately, such “crazy” behavior is uncommon. Hallucinations and delusions rarely appear before 60 hours of wakefulness (Naitoh, Kelly, & Englund, 1989). How can I tell how much sleep I really need? Pick a day when you feel well rested. Then sleep that night until you wake without an alarm clock. If you feel rested when you wake up, that’s your natural sleep need. If you’re sleeping fewer hours than you need, you’re building up a sleep debt (Maas, 1999). Sleep Patterns Sleep was described as an innate biological rhythm. What does that mean? Daily sleep and waking periods create a variety of sleep patterns. Rhythms of sleep and waking are so steady that they con- Expuesto-Nicolas Randall/Alamy Consciousness and Culture In many cultures, rituals of healing, prayer, purification, or personal transformation are accompanied by altered states of consciousness. tinue for many days, even when clocks and light-dark cycles are removed. However, under such conditions, humans eventually shift to a sleep-waking cycle that averages slightly more than 24 hours (Czeisler et al., 1999) ( Figure 6.2). This suggests that external time markers, especially light and dark, help tie our sleep rhythms to days that are exactly 24 hours long. Otherwise, many of us would drift into our own unusual sleep cycles (Duffy & Wright, Jr., 2005). • bridges Daily sleep cycles can be disrupted by rapid travel across time zones (jet lag) and by shift work. See Chapter 10, pages 322–323, for more information. What is the normal range of sleep? A few rare individuals can get by on an hour or two of sleep a night — and feel perfectly fine. Only a small percentage of the population are short sleepers, averaging 5 hours of sleep or less per night. On the other end of the scale we find long sleepers, who doze 9 hours or more (Grandner & Kripke, 2004). The majority of us sleep on a familiar 7- to 8-hourper-night schedule. Urging everyone to sleep 8 hours would be like advising everyone to wear medium-size shoes. We need less sleep as we get older, right? Yes, total sleep time declines throughout life. Those older than 50 average only 6 hours of sleep a night. In contrast, infants spend up to 20 hours a day sleeping, usually in 2- to 4-hour cycles. As they mature, most children go through a “nap” stage and eventually settle into a steady cycle of sleeping once a day ( Figure 6.3). Perhaps we should all continue to take an afternoon “siesta.” Midafternoon sleepiness is a natural part of the sleep cycle. Brief, well-timed naps can help • States of Consciousness (16 hours awake and 8 hours asleep). For instance, one study showed that 28-hour “days” work for some people. Overall, sleep patterns may be bent and stretched, but they rarely yield entirely to human whims (Åkerstedt, 2007). Time of Day Noon Midnight Noon Week number 4 Unscheduled, free-running rhythm 5 Scheduled light-dark cycle 6 6.2 Sleep rhythms. Bars show periods of sleep during the fourth, •fifth,Figure and sixth weeks of an experiment with a human subject. During unscheduled periods, the subject was allowed to select times of sleep and lighting. The result was a sleep rhythm of about 25 hours. Notice how this free-running rhythm began to advance around the clock as they fell asleep later each day. When periods of darkness (shaded area) were imposed during the fifth week, the rhythm quickly resynchronized with 24-hour days. (Adapted from Czeisler et al., 1981.) Awake Asleep 185 Newborn Stages of Sleep — The Nightly Roller-Coaster Gateway Question: Why do we sleep? What causes sleep? Early sleep experts thought that something in the bloodstream must cause sleep. But conjoined twins, whose bodies are joined at birth, show that this is false ( Figure 6.4). During waking hours, a sleep hormone (sleep-promoting chemical) collects in the brain and spinal cord, not in the blood. If this substance is extracted from one animal and injected into another, the second animal will sleep deeply for many hours (Cravatt et al., 1995). Notice, however, that this explanation is incomplete. For example, why would a well-rested person have to fight to stay awake during a boring midday meeting? Whether you are awake or asleep right now depends on the balance between separate sleep and waking systems. Brain circuits and chemicals in one of the systems promote sleep (Steiger, 2007). A network of brain cells in the other system responds to chemicals that inhibit sleep. The two systems seesaw back and forth, switching the brain between sleep and wakefulness (Lavie, 2001). Note that the brain does not “shut down” during sleep. Rather, the pattern of activity changes. • 1 year 4 years 10 years • 6 PM 12 6 AM 12 Yale Joel/TimePix/Getty Images Adult 6 PM Figure 6.3 Development of sleep patterns. Short cycles of sleep and waking gradually become the night-day cycle of an adult. Although most adults don’t take naps, midafternoon sleepiness is a natural part of the sleep cycle. (Reprinted from Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology, 17, Williams, Agnew, & Webb et al., Sleep Patterns in young adults: An EEG study, 376-381, 1964, with permission from Elsevier.) maintain alertness in people like truck drivers and hospital interns, who often must fight to stay alert (Garbarino et al., 2004). Busy people may be tempted to sleep less. However, people on shortened cycles — for example, 3 hours of sleep to 6 hours awake — often can’t get to sleep when the cycle calls for it. That’s why astronauts continue to sleep on their normal earth schedule while in space. Adapting to longer-than-normal days is more promising. Such days can be tailored to match natural sleep patterns, which have a ratio of 2 to 1 between time awake and time asleep Figure 6.4 These conjoined twins share the same blood supply, yet one •sleeps while the other is awake. (Photo by Yale Joel, Life Magazine. © 1954 Time, Inc.) Sleep-deprivation psychosis A major disruption of mental and emotional functioning brought about by sleep loss. Sleep patterns The order and timing of daily sleep and waking periods. Sleep hormone A sleep-promoting substance found in the brain and spinal cord. 186 CHAPTER 6 (HIP-nik: sleep) jerk. (This is quite normal, so have no fear about admitting to your friends that you fell asleep with a hypnic jerk.) In stage 1 sleep the EEG is made up mainly of small, irregular waves with some alpha. Persons awakened at this time may or may not say they were asleep. Sleep Stages How does brain activity change when you fall asleep? Changes in tiny electrical signals (brainwaves) generated by the brain can be amplified and recorded with an electroencephalograph (eh-LEK-tro-en-SEF-uh-lo-graf ), or EEG. When you are awake and alert, the EEG reveals a pattern of small fast waves called beta waves ( Figure 6.5). Immediately before sleep, the pattern shifts to larger and slower waves called alpha waves. (Alpha waves also occur when you are relaxed and allow your thoughts to drift.) As the eyes close, breathing becomes slow and regular, the pulse rate slows, and body temperature drops. Soon after, we descend into slow-wave sleep through four distinct sleep stages. Stage 2 • As sleep deepens, body temperature drops further. Also, the EEG begins to include sleep spindles, which are short bursts of distinctive brain-wave activity (Fogel et al., 2007). Spindles seem to mark the true boundary of sleep. Within 4 minutes after spindles appear, most people will say they were asleep. Stage 3 Stage 1 In stage 3, a new brainwave called delta begins to appear. Delta waves are very large and slow. They signal a move to deeper slowwave sleep and a further loss of consciousness. As you enter light sleep (stage 1 sleep), your heart rate slows even more. Breathing becomes more irregular. The muscles of your body relax. This may trigger a reflex muscle twitch called a hypnic Stage 4 Most people reach deep sleep (the deepest level of normal sleep) in about 1 hour. Stage 4 brainwaves are almost pure slow-wave delta, and the sleeper is in a state of oblivion. If you make a loud noise during stage 4, the sleeper will wake up in a state of confusion and may not remember the noise. BSIP/Photo Researchers, Inc. The Dual Process Hypothesis of Sleep (a) • Awake Beta waves Eyes closed, relaxed Alpha waves Stage 1 Small, irregular waves Stage 2 Sleep spindles Stage 3 Delta waves appear Stage 4 Mostly delta (b) Figure 6.5 (a) Photograph of an EEG recording session. The boy in the background is asleep. (b) Changes in brain-wave patterns associated with various stages of sleep. Actually, most wave types are present at all times, but they occur more or less frequently in various sleep stages. There is much more to a night’s sleep than a simple descent into stage 4. Fluctuations in other sleep hormones cause recurring cycles of deeper and lighter sleep throughout the night (Steiger, 2007). During these repeated periods of lighter sleep, a curious thing happens: the sleeper’s eyes occasionally move under the eyelids. (If you ever get a chance to watch a sleeping child, roommate, or spouse, you may see these eye movements.) Rapid eye movements, or REMs, are associated with dreaming ( Figure 6.6). In addition to rapid eye movements, REM sleep is marked by a return of fast, irregular EEG patterns similar to stage 1 sleep. In fact, the brain is so active during REM sleep that it looks as if the person is awake (Rock, 2004). The two most basic states of sleep, then, are non-REM (NREM) sleep (which occurs during stages 1, 2, 3, and 4) and REM sleep, with its associated dreaming (Rock, 2004). Earlier, we noted some of the biological benefits of sleep. According to the dual process hypothesis of sleep, REM and NREM sleep have two added purposes: They help “refresh” the brain and store memories (Ficca & Salzarulob, 2004). • The Function of NREM Sleep What is the function of NREM sleep? NREM sleep is dream-free about 90 percent of the time and is deepest early in the night during the first few stage 4 periods. Your first period of stage 1 sleep usually lacks REMs and dreams. Later stage 1 periods typically include a shift into REM sleep. Dreamless slow-wave NREM sleep increases after physical exertion and may help us recover from bodily fatigue. It also appears to “calm” the brain during the earlier part of a night’s sleep (Tononi & Cirelli, 2003). According to the dual process hypothesis, we are bombarded by information throughout the day, which causes our neural net- States of Consciousness 187 Hours of Sleep 24 Hours 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Awake NREM Sleep stages REM Awake REM 1 NREM 2 NREM 3 NREM 4 (a) • Dreams (b) Figure 6.6 (a) Average proportion of time adults spend daily in REM sleep and NREM sleep. REM periods add up to about 20 percent of total sleep time. (b) Typical changes in stages of sleep during the night. Notice that dreams mostly coincide with REM periods. works to become more and more active. As a result, your brain requires more and more energy to continue functioning. Slow wave sleep early in the night brings overall brain activation levels back down, allowing a “fresh” approach to the next day. Consider for a moment the amazing jumble of events that make up a day. Some experiences are worth remembering (like what you are reading right now, of course) and others are not so important (like what you were thinking when you put on your socks). As slow wave sleep reduces overall activation in the brain, less important experiences may fade away and be forgotten. If you wake up feeling clearer about what you studied the previous night, it might be because your brain doesn’t “sweat the small stuff ”! The Function of REM Sleep What then, is the purpose of REM sleep? According to the dual process hypothesis, while NREM sleep “calms” the brain, REM sleep appears to “sharpen” our memories of the previous day’s more important experiences (Saxvig et al., 2008). Daytime stress tends to increase REM sleep, which may rise dramatically when there is a death in the family, trouble at work, a marital conflict, or other emotionally charged events. The value of more REM sleep is that it helps us sort and retain memories, especially memories about strategies for solving problems (Walker & Stickgold, 2006). This is why, after studying for a long period, you may remember more if you go to sleep, rather than pulling an all-nighter. (REMember to get some REM!) Early in life, REM sleep may stimulate the developing brain. Newborn babies have lots of new experiences to process so they spend a hearty 8 or 9 hours a day in REM sleep. That’s about 50 percent of their total sleep time. REM Sleep and Dreaming Roughly 85 percent of the time, people awakened during REMs report vivid dreams. Some eye movements correspond to dream activities. Dream that you are watching a tennis match, and you will probably move your eyes from side to side. However, people who were born blind still have REMs, so eye movements are not just a result of “watching” dream images (Shafton, 1995). REM sleep is easy to observe in pets, such as dogs and cats. Watch for eye and face movements and irregular breathing. (You can forget about your pet iguana, though. Reptiles show no signs of REM sleep.) Dreams during REM sleep tend to be longer, clearer, more detailed, more bizarre, and more “dream-like” than thoughts and images that occur in NREM sleep (Hobson, Pace-Schott, & Stickgold, 2000). Also, brain areas associated with imagery and emotion become more active during REM sleep. This may explain why REM dreams tend to be more vivid than NREM dreams (Braun, Balkin, & Herscovitch, 1998; Rock, 2004). Speaking very loosely, it’s as if the dreaming brain were reviewing messages left on a telephone answering machine, to decide which are worth keeping. During the day, when information is streaming in, the brain may be too busy to efficiently select useful memories. When the conscious brain is “off-line,” we are better able to build new memories. What happens to the body when a person dreams? REM sleep is a time of high emotion. The heart beats irregularly. Blood pressure and breathing waver. Both males and females appear to be sexually aroused: Men usually have an erection, and genital blood flow increases in women. This occurs for all REM sleep, so it is not strictly related to erotic dreams ( Jouvet, 1999). Electroencephalograph (EEG) A device designed to detect, amplify, and record electrical activity in the brain. Beta waves Small, fast brain waves associated with being awake and alert. Alpha waves Large, slow brain waves associated with relaxation and falling asleep. Sleep stages Levels of sleep identified by brain-wave patterns and behavioral changes. Light sleep Stage 1 sleep, marked by small irregular brainwaves and some alpha waves. Sleep spindles Distinctive bursts of brainwave activity that indicate a person is asleep. Delta waves Large, slow brainwaves that occur in deeper sleep (stages 3 and 4). Deep sleep Stage 4 slow-wave sleep; the deepest form of normal sleep. Rapid eye movements (REMs) Swift eye movements during sleep. REM sleep Sleep marked by rapid eye movements and a return to stage 1 EEG patterns. Non-REM (NREM) sleep Non–rapid eye movement sleep characteristic of stages 2, 3, and 4. 188 CHAPTER 6 C R IT ICAL T H I N KI N G They Came from Outer Space? During REM sleep, your body becomes quite still, as if you were paralyzed. Imagine for a moment the results of acting out some of your recent dreams. Very likely, REM-sleep paralysis prevents some hilarious — and dangerous — nighttime escapades. When it fails, some people thrash violently, leap out of bed, and may attack their bed partners. A lack of muscle paralysis during REM sleep is called REM behavior disorder (Ochoa & Pulido, 2005). One patient suffering from the disorder tied himself to his bed every night. That way, he couldn’t jump up and crash into furniture or walls (Shafton, 1995). And yet sometimes sleep paralysis can go a little too far. (See “They Came from Outer Space?”) In a moment we will survey some additional sleep problems — if you are still awake. First, here are a few questions to check your memory of our discussion so far. K NOW LEDGE B U I L D E R Altered States and Sleep RECITE 1. Changes in the quality and pattern of mental activity define a. an EEG b. an REM c. SIDS d. an ASC 2. Delusions and hallucinations typically continue for several days after a sleep-deprived individual returns to normal sleep. T or F? 3. Alyssa experiences a microsleep while driving. Most likely, this indicates that she a. was producing mostly beta waves b. had high levels of sleep hormones in her bloodstream c. switched from delta waves to alpha waves d. was sleep deprived some people who have sleep-related hallucinations believe they have been abducted by space aliens or sexually abused (McNally & Clancy, 2005). Detroit Institute of the Arts/SuperStock hypnopompic (hip-neh-POM-pik: “upon awakening”) hallucinations. According to psychologist Al Cheyne, these hallucinations may include bizarre experiences, such as sensing that an alien being is in your bedroom; feeling something pressing on your chest, suffocating you; or feeling like you are floating out of your body (Cheyne, 2005; Cheyne, Rueffer, & Newby-Clark, 1999). Although most of us shrug off these weird experiences, some people try to make sense of them. Earlier in history, people interpreted these hallucinated intruders as angels, demons, or witches and believed that their out-of-body experiences were real (Cheyne, Rueffer, & Newby-Clark, 1999). However, as our culture changes so do our interpretations of sleep experiences. Today, for example, Swiss artist Henry Fuseli drew on hypnopompic imagery as an inspiration for his famous painting, The Nightmare. 4. Older adults, and particularly the elderly, sleep more than children do because the elderly are more easily fatigued. T or F? 5. Alpha waves are to presleep drowsiness as _____________________ ___________ are to stage 4 sleep. 6. Rapid eye movements indicate that a person is in deep sleep. T or F? 7. Which of the following would normally be most incompatible with moving your arms and legs while asleep? a. REM sleep b. sleep spindles c. delta waves d. NREM sleep REFLECT Critical Thinking 8. Why might it be better for the unscheduled human sleep–waking cycle to average more than 24 hours, instead of less? 9. In addition to helping store memories, what biological advantages might sleeping provide? Relate Make a quick list of some altered states of consciousness you have experienced. What do they have in common? How are they different? What conditions caused them? Imagine that you are a counselor at a sleep clinic. You must explain the basics of sleep and dreaming to a new client who knows little about these topics. Can you do it? Answers: 1. d 2. F 3. d 4. F 5. delta waves 6. F 7. a 8. Sleep experts theorize that the 25-hour average leaves a little “slack” in the cycle. External time markers can then retard the body cycle slightly to synchronize it with light-dark cycles. If the body cycle were shorter than 24 hours, we all might have to “stretch” every day to adjust. 9. Lowering body activity and metabolism during sleep may help conserve energy and lengthen life. Also, natural selection may have favored sleep because animals that remained active at night probably had a higher chance of being killed. (We’ll bet they had more fun, though.) Imagine opening your eyes shortly before dawn, attempting to roll over in your bed, and suddenly realizing that you are entirely paralyzed. While lying helplessly on your back and unable to cry out for help, you become aware of sinister figures lurking in your bedroom. As they move closer to your bed, your heart begins to pound violently and you feel as if you are suffocating. You hear buzzing sounds and feel electrical sensations shooting throughout your body. Within moments, the visions vanish and you can move once again. Terrified, you wonder what has just happened (McNally & Clancy, 2005, p.114). Sleep paralysis, which normally prevents us from moving during REM sleep, can also occur just as you begin to wake up. During such episodes, people sometimes have States of Consciousness Gateway Question: What are some sleep disorders and unusual sleep events? Sleep quality has taken a beating in North America. Artificial lighting, frenetic schedules, exciting pastimes, smoking, drinking, overstimulation, and many other factors have contributed to a near epidemic of sleep problems. Sleep disturbances are a serious risk to health and happiness. Sleep clinics treat thousands of people each year who suffer from sleep disorders or complaints. These disturbances range from daytime sleep attacks to sleepwalking and terrifying nightmares ( Table 6.1). Let’s explore some of the more interesting problems these people face. • Insomnia Staring at the ceiling at 2 am is pretty low on most people’s list of favorite pastimes. Yet about 60 million Americans have frequent or chronic insomnia (NINDS, 2007). Insomnia includes diffi- • Table 6.1 Sleep Disturbances — Things That Go Wrong in the Night Hypersomnia Excessive daytime sleepiness. This can result from depression, insomnia, narcolepsy, sleep apnea, sleep drunkenness, periodic limb movements, drug abuse, and other problems. Insomnia Difficulty in getting to sleep or staying asleep; also, not feeling rested after sleeping. Narcolepsy Sudden, irresistible, daytime sleep attacks that may last anywhere from a few minutes to a half hour. Victims may fall asleep while standing, talking, or even driving. Nightmare disorder Vivid, recurrent nightmares that significantly disturb sleep. Periodic limb movement syndrome Muscle twitches (primarily affecting the legs) that occur every 20 to 40 seconds and severely disturb sleep. REM behavior disorder A failure of normal muscle paralysis, leading to violent actions during REM sleep. Restless legs syndrome An irresistible urge to move the legs to relieve sensations of creeping, tingling, prickling, aching, or tension. Sleep apnea During sleep, breathing stops for 20 seconds or more until the person wakes a little, gulps in air, and settles back to sleep; this cycle may be repeated hundreds of times per night. Sleep drunkenness A slow transition to clear consciousness after awakening; sometimes associated with irritable or aggressive behavior. Sleep terror disorder The repeated occurrence of night terrors that significantly disturb sleep. Sleep-wake schedule disorder A mismatch between the sleep-wake schedule demanded by a person’s bodily rhythm and that demanded by the environment. Sleepwalking disorder Repeated incidents of leaving bed and walking about while asleep. culty in going to sleep, frequent nighttime awakenings, waking too early, or a combination of these problems. Insomnia can harm people’s work, health, and relationships (Sateia & Nowell, 2004). Types and Causes of Insomnia Worry, stress, and excitement can cause temporary insomnia and a self-defeating cycle. First, excess mental activity (“I can’t stop turning things over in my mind”) and heightened arousal block sleep. Then, frustration and anger over not being able to sleep cause more worry and arousal. This further delays sleep, which causes more frustration, and so on (Espie, 2002). A good way to beat this cycle is to avoid fighting it. Get up and do something useful or satisfying when you can’t sleep. (Reading a textbook might be a good choice of useful activities.) Return to bed only when you begin to feel that you are struggling to stay awake. If sleeping problems last for more than 3 weeks, then a diagnosis of chronic insomnia can be made. Drug-dependency insomnia (sleep loss caused by withdrawal from sleeping pills) can also occur. There is real irony in the billion dollars a year North Americans spend on sleeping pills. Nonprescription sleeping pills such as Sominex, Nytol, and Sleep-Eze have little sleep-inducing effect. Barbiturates are even worse. These prescription sedatives decrease both stage 4 sleep and REM sleep, drastically lowering sleep quality. In addition, many users become “sleeping-pill junkies” who need an ever-greater number of pills to get to sleep. Victims must be painstakingly weaned from their sleep medicines. Otherwise, terrible nightmares and “rebound insomnia” may drive them back to drug use. It’s worth remembering that although alcohol and other depressant drugs may help a person get to sleep, they greatly reduce sleep quality (Nau & Lichstein, 2005). Even newer drugs, such as Ambien and Lunesta, which induce The New Yorker Collection 2006 Barbara Smaller from The Cartoon Bank. All Rights Reserved. Sleep Disturbances — Showing Nightly: Sleep Wars! Insomnia Difficulty in getting to sleep or staying asleep. Bond & Wooten, 1996; DSM-IV-TR, 2000; Hauri & Linde, 1990 189 190 CHAPTER 6 sleep, have drawbacks. Possible side effects include amnesia, impaired judgment, increased appetite, decreased sex drive, depression, and sleepwalking. Rebound insomnia is also a risk, making these drugs at best a temporary remedy for insomnia. Behavioral Remedies for Insomnia If sleeping pills are a poor way to treat insomnia, what can be done? Sleep specialists prefer to treat insomnia with lifestyle changes and behavioral techniques (Montgomery & Dennis, 2004). Treatment for chronic insomnia usually begins with a careful analysis of a patient’s sleep habits, lifestyle, stress levels, and medical problems. All the approaches discussed in the following list are helpful for treating insomnia (Nau & Lichstein, 2005): 1. Stimulus control. Insisting on a regular schedule helps establish a firm body rhythm, greatly improving sleep. This is best achieved by exercising stimulus control, which refers to linking a response with specific stimuli. It is important to get up and go to sleep at the same time each day, including weekends (Bootzin & Epstein, 2000). In addition, insomniacs are told to avoid doing anything but sleeping when they are in bed. They are not to study, eat, watch TV, read, pay the bills, worry, or even think in bed. (Lovemaking is okay, however.) In this way, only sleeping and relaxation become associated with going to bed at specific times (Bootzin & Epstein, 2000). 2. Sleep restriction. Even if an entire night’s sleep is missed, it is important not to sleep late in the morning, nap more than an hour, sleep during the evening, or go to bed early the following night. Instead, restricting sleep to normal bedtime hours avoids fragmenting sleep rhythms (Shneerson, 2005). 3. Paradoxical intention. Another helpful approach is to remove the pressures of trying to go to sleep. Instead, the goal becomes trying to keep the eyes open (in the dark) and stay awake as long as possible (Nau & Lichstein, 2005). This allows sleep to come unexpectedly and lowers performance anxiety (Espie, 2002). 4. Relaxation. Some insomniacs lower their arousal before sleep by using a physical or mental strategy for relaxing, such as progressive muscle relaxation, meditation, or blotting out worries with calming images. It is also helpful to schedule time in the early evening to write down worries or concerns and plan what to do about them the next day, in order to set them aside before going to bed. 6. Food intake. What you eat can affect how easily you get to sleep. Eating starchy foods increases the amount of tryptophan (TRIP-tuh-fan: an amino acid) reaching the brain. More tryptophan, in turn, increases the amount of serotonin in the brain, which is associated with relaxation, a positive mood, and sleepiness. Thus, to promote sleep, try eating a starchy snack, such as cookies, bread, pasta, oatmeal, pretzels, or dry cereal. If you really want to drop the bomb on insomnia, try eating a baked potato (which may be the world’s largest sleeping pill!) (Sahelian, 1998). 7. Stimulants. Stimulants, such as coffee and cigarettes, should be avoided. It is also worth remembering that alcohol, although not a stimulant, impairs sleep quality. Sleepwalking, Sleeptalking, and Sleepsex Sleepsex? As strange as it may seem, many waking behaviors can be engaged in while asleep, such as driving a car, cooking, playing a musical instrument, and eating (Plazzi et al., 2005). The most famous, sleepwalking, is eerie and fascinating in its own right. Somnambulists (som-NAM-bue-lists: those who sleepwalk) avoid obstacles, descend stairways, and on rare occasions may step out of windows or in front of automobiles. Sleepwalkers have been observed jumping into lakes, urinating in garbage pails or closets (phew!), shuffling furniture around, and even brandishing weapons (Schenck & Mahowald, 2005). The sleepwalker’s eyes are usually open, but a blank face and shuffling feet reveal that the person is still asleep. If you find someone sleepwalking, you should gently guide the person back to bed. Awakening a sleepwalker does no harm, but it is not necessary. Does sleepwalking occur during dreaming? No. Remember that people are normally immobilized during REM sleep. EEG studies have shown that somnambulism occurs during NREM stages 3 and 4 (Stein & Ferber, 2001). Sleeptalking also occurs mostly during NREM sleep. The link with deep sleep explains why sleeptalking makes little sense and why sleepwalkers are confused and remember little when awakened (DSM-IV-TR, 2000). Oh, yes, you’re curious about sleepsex. There is, of course, an official name for it: sexsomnia (Shapiro, Trajanovic, & Fedoroff, 2003). Sexsomnia is not as exciting as it might sound: Just imagine being startled wide awake by your bed partner, who is asleep but attempting to have sex with you (Mangan, 2004). Nightmares and Night Terrors bridges Learning how to achieve deep relaxation is a highly useful skill. See Chapter 15, pages 505–507, for more information. 5. Exercise. Strenuous exercise during the day promotes sleep. It is best if done about 6 hours before bedtime (Maas, 1999). However, exercise in the evening is helpful only if it is very light. Stage 4 sleep is also the realm of night terrors. These frightening episodes are quite different from ordinary nightmares ( Table 6.2). A nightmare is simply a bad dream that takes place during REM sleep. Frequently occurring nightmares (one a week or more) are associated with higher levels of psychological distress (Levin & Fireman, 2002). During stage 4 night terrors, a person suffers total panic and may hallucinate frightening dream images into the bedroom. An attack may last 15 or 20 minutes. When it is over, the person awakens drenched in sweat but only vaguely remembers the terror. Because night terrors occur during NREM sleep (when • 191 States of Consciousness Table 6.2 • Was It a Nightmare or a Night Terror? Nightmare Night Terror Stage of sleep REM NREM Activity Slight or no movement Violent body movement, sits up, cries out, may run Emotion Fear or anxiety Terror and disorganizing panic Mental state when awakened Coherent, can be calmed Incoherent, disoriented, cannot be calmed, may be hallucinating Physiological changes No perspiration Perspires heavily Recall Dream activity usually remembered Amnesia for episode the body is not immobilized), victims may sit up, scream, get out of bed, or run around the room. Victims remember little afterward. (Other family members, however, may have a story to tell.) Night terrors are most common in childhood, but they continue to plague about 2 out of every 100 adults (Kataria, 2004; Ohayon, Guilleminault, & Priest, 1999). most effective treatments is the use of a continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) mask to aid breathing during sleep. Other treatments include weight loss and surgery for breathing obstructions (Collop, 2005). SIDS Sleep apnea is suspected as one cause of sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS), or “crib death.” Each year 2500 babies are victims of SIDS (NICHD, 2006). In the “typical” crib death, a slightly premature or small baby with some signs of a cold or cough is bundled up and put to bed. A short time later, parents find the child has died. A baby deprived of air will normally struggle to begin breathing again. However, SIDS babies seem to have a weak arousal reflex. This prevents them from changing positions and resuming breathing after an episode of apnea (Horne et al., 2001). Babies at risk for SIDS must be carefully watched for the first 6 months of life. To aid parents in this task, a special monitor may be used that sounds an alarm when breathing or pulse becomes weak ( Figure 6.7). Babies at risk for SIDS are often premature; have a shrill, high-pitched cry; engage in “snoring,” breath-holding, or frequent awakening at night; breathe mainly through an open mouth; or remain passive when their face rolls into a pillow or blanket. • How to Eliminate a Nightmare Sleep Apnea Some sage once said, “Laugh and the whole world laughs with you; snore and you sleep alone.” Nightly “wood sawing” is often harmless, but it can signal a serious problem. A person who snores loudly, with short silences and loud gasps or snorts, may suffer from apnea (AP-nee-ah: interrupted breathing). In sleep apnea, breathing stops for periods of 20 seconds to 2 minutes. As the need for oxygen becomes intense, the person wakes a little and gulps in air. She or he then settles back to sleep. But soon, breathing stops again. This cycle is repeated hundreds of times a night. As you might guess, apnea victims are extremely sleepy during the day (Collop, 2005). What causes sleep apnea? Some cases occur because the brain stops sending signals to the diaphragm to maintain breathing. Another cause is blockage of the upper air passages. One of the Michael Newman/PhotoEdit Is there any way to stop a recurring nightmare? A bad nightmare can be worse than any horror movie. It’s easy to leave a theater, but we often remain trapped in terrifying dreams. Nevertheless, most nightmares can be banished by following three simple steps. First, write down your nightmare, describing it in detail. Next, change the dream any way you wish, being sure to spell out the details of the new dream. The third step is imagery rehearsal, in which you mentally rehearse the changed dream before you fall asleep again (Krakow & Zadra, 2006). Imagery rehearsal may work because it makes upsetting dreams familiar while a person is awake and feeling safe. Or perhaps it mentally “reprograms” future dream content. In any case, the technique has helped many people. Figure 6.7 Infants at risk for SIDS are often attached to devices that monitor •breathing and heart rate during sleep. An alarm sounds to alert parents if either pulse or respiration falters. SIDS rarely occurs after an infant is 1 year old. Babies at risk for SIDS should be placed on their sides or on their backs. Stimulus control Linking a particular response with specific stimuli. Somnambulists People who sleepwalk; occurs during NREM sleep. Nightmare A bad dream that occurs during REM sleep. Night terror A state of panic during NREM sleep. Sleep apnea Repeated interruption of breathing during sleep. Sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) The sudden, unexplained death of an apparently healthy infant. 192 CHAPTER 6 “Back to Sleep” Sleeping position is another major risk factor for SIDS. Healthy infants are better off sleeping on their backs or sides. (Premature babies, those with respiratory problems, and those who often vomit may need to sleep facedown. Ask a pediatrician for guidance.) Remember, “back to sleep” is the safest position for most infants (Hauck et al., 2002). It is also worth eliminating soft objects such as pillows, quilts, comforters, or sheepskins from the baby’s bed. Narcolepsy One of the most dramatic sleep problems is narcolepsy (NARkoe-lep-see), or sudden, irresistible sleep attacks. These last anywhere from a few minutes to a half hour. Victims may fall asleep while standing, talking, or even driving. Emotional excitement, especially laughter, commonly triggers narcolepsy. (Tell an especially good joke and a narcoleptic may fall asleep.) More than half of all victims also suffer from cataplexy (CAT-uh-plex-see), a sudden temporary paralysis of the muscles, leading to complete body collapse. It’s easy to understand why narcolepsy can devastate careers and relationships (Thorpy, 2006). Because sudden paralysis happens during dreaming, is there a connection between narcolepsy and REM sleep? Yes, narcoleptics tend to fall directly into REM sleep. Thus, the narcoleptic’s sleep attacks and paralysis appear to occur when REM sleep intrudes into the waking state (Mignot, 2001). Fortunately, narcolepsy is rare. It runs in families, which suggests that it is hereditary (Chabas et al., 2003). In fact, this has been confirmed by breeding several generations of narcoleptic dogs. (These dogs, by the way, are simply outstanding at learning the trick “Roll over and play dead.”) There is no known cure for narcolepsy, but a drug named sodium oxybate reduces the frequency and intensity of attacks (Thorpy, 2006). Dreams — A Separate Reality? Gateway Question: Do dreams have meaning? When REM sleep was discovered in 1952, it ushered in a “Golden Era” of dream inquiry. To conclude our discussion of sleep, let’s consider some age-old questions about dreaming. Does everyone dream? Do dreams occur in an instant? Most people dream four or five times a night, but not all people remember their dreams. “Nondreamers” are often surprised by their dreams when first awakened during REM sleep. Dreams are usually spaced about 90 minutes apart. The first dream lasts only about 10 minutes; the last averages 30 minutes and may run as long as 50. Dreams, therefore, occur in real time, not as a “flash” (Shafton, 1995). REM Sleep Revisited How important is REM sleep? To answer this question, sleep expert William Dement awakened volunteers each time they entered REM sleep. Soon, their need for “dream time” grew more urgent. By the fifth night, many had to be awakened 20 or 30 times to prevent REM sleep. When the volunteers were finally allowed to sleep undisturbed, they dreamed extra amounts. This effect, called a REM rebound, explains why alcoholics have horrible nightmares after they quit drinking. Alcohol reduces sleep quality by suppressing REM sleep, thus setting up a powerful rebound when it is withdrawn (Stein & Friedmann, 2005). Dement’s volunteers complained of memory lapses, poor concentration, and anxiety. For a while, it was thought that people deprived of REM sleep might go crazy. But later experiments showed that missing any sleep stage can cause a rebound for that stage. In general, daytime disturbances are related to the total amount of sleep lost, not to the type of sleep lost (Devoto et al., 1999). Dream Theories How meaningful are dreams? Some theorists believe that dreams have deeply hidden meanings. Others regard dreams as nearly meaningless. Yet others hold that dreams reflect our waking thoughts, fantasies, and emotions (Beck, 2004). Let’s examine all three views. Psychodynamic Dream Theory Psychodynamic theories of dreaming emphasize internal conflicts and unconscious forces. Sigmund Freud’s landmark book, The Interpretation of Dreams (1900), first advanced the idea that many dreams are based on wish fulfillment (an expression of unconscious desires). One of Freud’s key proposals was that dreams express unconscious desires and conflicts as disguised dream symbols (images that have deeper symbolic meaning). Understanding a dream, then, requires analyzing the dream’s manifest content (obvious, visible meaning) to uncover its latent content (hidden, symbolic meaning). For instance, a woman who dreams of stealing her best friend’s wedding ring and placing it on her own hand may be unwilling to consciously admit that she is sexually attracted to her best friend’s husband. Similarly, a journey might symbolize death, and horseback riding or dancing, sexual intercourse. bridges Interpreting dreams is an important part of Freudian psychoanalysis. See Chapter 15, pages 498–500. Do all dreams have hidden meanings? Probably not. Freud realized that some dreams are trivial “day residues” or carryovers from ordinary waking events. On the other hand, dreams do tend to reflect a person’s current concerns, so Freud wasn’t entirely wrong. The Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis Psychiatrists Allan Hobson and Robert McCarley have a radically different view of dreaming, called the activation-synthesis hypothesis. They believe that during REM sleep several lower brain centers are “turned on” (activated) in more or less random States of Consciousness 193 SuperStock, Inc./SuperStock Dream Worlds According to psychodynamic theory, dream imagery often has symbolic meaning. How would you interpret French artist Henri Rousseau’s dreamlike image, titled The Dream? The fact that dreams don’t have a single unambiguous meaning is one of the shortcomings of Freudian dream theory. fashion. However, messages from the cells are blocked from reaching the body, so no movement occurs. Nevertheless, the cells continue to tell higher brain areas of their activities. Struggling to interpret this random information, the brain searches through stored memories and manufactures (synthesizes) a dream (Hobson, 2000; 2005). However, frontal areas of the cortex, which control higher mental abilities, are mostly shut down during REM sleep. This explains why dreams are more primitive and more bizarre than daytime thoughts (Hobson, 2000). How does that help explain dream content? According to the activation-synthesis hypothesis, dreams are basically meaningless. Let’s use the classic chase dream as an example. In such dreams we feel we are running but not going anywhere. This occurs because the brain is told the body is running, but it gets no feedback from the motionless legs. To try to make sense of this information, the brain creates a chase drama. A similar process probably explains dreams of floating or flying. So dreams have no meaning? The activation-synthesis hypothesis doesn’t rule out the idea that dreams have some meaning. Because dreams are created from memories and past experiences, parts of dreams can sometimes reflect each person’s mental life, emotions, and concerns (Hobson, 2000). Neurocognitive Dream Theory William Domhoff offers a third view of dreaming. According to his neurocognitive dream theory, dreams actually have much in common with waking thoughts and emotions. Most dreams reflect ordinary waking concerns. Domhoff believes this is true because many brain areas that are active when we are awake remain active during dreaming (Domhoff, 2001; 2003; 2005). From this perspective, our dreams are a conscious expression of REM sleep processes that are sorting and storing daily experiences. Thus, we shouldn’t be surprised if a student who is angry at a teacher dreams of embarrassing the teacher in class, a lonely person dreams of romance, or a hungry child dreams of food. It is not necessary to seek deep symbolic meanings to understand these dreams. Which dream theory is the most widely accepted? Each theory has strengths and weaknesses. However, studies of dream content do support neurocognitive theory’s focus on the continuity between dreams and waking thought. Rather than being exotic or bizarre, most dreams reflect everyday events (Hall, 1966; Pesant & Zadra, 2006). For example, athletes tend to dream about the previous day’s athletic activities (Erlacher & Schredl, 2004). In general, the favorite dream setting is a familiar room in a house. Action usually takes place between the dreamer and two or three other emotionally important people — friends, enemies, loved ones, or employers. Dream actions are also mostly familiar: running, jumping, riding, sitting, talking, and watching. About half of all dreams have sexual elements. Dreams of flying, floating, and falling occur less frequently. However, note that such dreams lend some support to the activationsynthesis hypothesis, because they are not everyday events. (Unless you are a trapeze artist.) Are most dreams happy or sad? If you ask people in the morning what they dreamed about, they mention more unpleasant emotions than pleasant emotions (Merritt et al., 1994). However, it may be that dreams of fear, anger, or sadness are easier to remember. When people are awakened during REM sleep, they report equal numbers of positive and negative emotions (Fosse, Stickgold, & Hobson, 2001). Even if many dreams can be viewed as just a different form of thought, many psychologists continue to believe that some dreams have deeper meaning (White & Taytroe, 2003; Wilkinson, 2006). There seems to be little doubt that dreams can make a difference in our lives: Veteran sleep researcher William Dement once dreamed that he had lung cancer. In the dream a doctor told Dement he would die soon. At the time, Dement was smoking two packs of cigarettes a day. He says, “I will never Narcolepsy A sudden, irresistible sleep attack. Cataplexy A sudden temporary paralysis of the muscles. REM rebound The occurrence of extra rapid eye movement sleep following REM sleep deprivation. Psychodynamic theory Any theory of behavior that emphasizes internal conflicts, motives, and unconscious forces. Wish fulfillment Freudian belief that many dreams express unconscious desires. Dream symbols Images in dreams that serve as visible signs of hidden ideas, desires, impulses, emotions, relationships, and so forth. Manifest dream content The surface, “visible” content of a dream; dream images as they are remembered by the dreamer. Latent dream content The hidden or symbolic meaning of a dream, as revealed by dream interpretation and analysis. Activation-synthesis hypothesis An attempt to explain how random activity in lower brain centers results in the manufacture of relatively bizarre dreams by higher brain centers. Neurocognitive dream theory Proposal that dreams reflect everyday waking thoughts and emotions. 194 CHAPTER 6 forget the surprise, joy, and exquisite relief of waking up. I felt reborn.” Dement quit smoking the following day. (For more information about dreaming, see the Psychology in Action section later in this chapter.) K NOW LEDGE B U I L D E R Sleep Disturbances and Dreaming RECITE 1. Which of the following is not a behavioral remedy for insomnia? a. daily hypersomnia b. stimulus control c. progressive relaxation d. paradoxical intention 2. Eating a snack that is nearly all starch can promote sleep because it increases __________ in the brain. a. beta waves b. tryptophan c. EEG activity d. hypnic cycling 3. Night terrors, sleepwalking, and sleeptalking all occur during stage 1, NREM sleep. T or F? 4. Sleep _________________ is suspected as one cause of SIDS. 5. People who suffer from sudden daytime sleep attacks have which sleep disorder? a. narcolepsy b. REM behavior disorder c. somnambulism d. sleep spindling 6. According to the activation-synthesis hypothesis of dreaming, dreams are constructed from __________________________ to explain messages received from nerve cells controlling eye movement, balance, and bodily activity. 7. The favored setting for dreams is a. work b. school c. outdoors or unfamiliar places d. familiar rooms 8. Sharpening memories and facilitating their storage is one function of a. activation-synthesis cycles b. REM sleep c. deep sleep d. NREM sleep REFLECT Critical Thinking 9. Even without being told that somnambulism is an NREM event, you could have predicted that sleepwalking doesn’t occur during dreaming. Why? Relate Almost everyone suffers from insomnia at least occasionally. Are any of the techniques for combating insomnia similar to strategies you have discovered on your own? How many sleep disturbances can you name (including those listed in Table 6.1)? Are there any that you have experienced? Which do you think would be most disruptive? Do you think the activation-synthesis hypothesis provides an adequate explanation of your own dreams? Have you had dreams that seem to reflect Freudian wish fulfillment? Do you think your dreams have symbolic meaning or reflect everyday concerns? • Hypnosis — Look into My Eyes Gateway Question: What is hypnosis? “Your body is becoming heavy. You can barely keep your eyes open. You are so tired you can’t move. Relax. Let go. Relax. Close your eyes and relax.” These are the last words a textbook should ever say to you, and the first a hypnotist might say. Interest in hypnosis began in the 1700s with Austrian doctor Franz Mesmer, whose name gave us the term mesmerize (to hypnotize). Mesmer believed he could cure disease with magnets. Mesmer’s strange “treatments” are related to hypnosis because they actually relied on the power of suggestion, not magnetism (Waterfield, 2002). For a time, Mesmer enjoyed quite a following. In the end, however, his theories of “animal magnetism” were rejected and he was branded a fraud. The term hypnosis was later coined by English surgeon James Braid. The Greek word hypnos means “sleep,” and Braid used it to describe the hypnotic state. Today we know that hypnosis is not sleep. Confusion about this point remains because some hypnotists give the suggestion, “Sleep, sleep.” However, EEG patterns recorded during hypnosis are different from those observed when a person is asleep or pretending to be hypnotized (Barabasz, 2000). Theories of Hypnosis If hypnosis isn’t sleep, then what is it? That’s a good question. Hypnosis is often defined as an altered state of consciousness, characterized by narrowed attention and an increased openness to suggestion (Kallio & Revonsuo, 2003; Kosslyn et al., 2000). Notice that this definition assumes hypnosis is a distinct state of consciousness. The best-known state theory of hypnosis was proposed by Ernest Hilgard (1904–2001), who argued that hypnosis causes a dissociative state, or “split” in awareness. To illustrate, he asked hypnotized subjects to plunge one hand into a painful bath of ice water. Subjects told to feel no pain said they felt none. The same subjects were then asked if there was any part of their mind that did feel pain. With their free hand, many wrote, “It hurts,” or “Stop it, you’re hurting me,” while they continued to act painfree (Hilgard, 1977, 1994). Thus, one part of the hypnotized person says there is no pain and acts as if there is none. Another part, which Hilgard calls the hidden observer, is aware of the pain but remains in the background. The hidden observer is a detached part of the hypnotized person’s awareness that silently observes events. In contrast, nonstate theorists argue that hypnosis is not a distinct state at all. Instead it is merely a blend of conformity, relaxation, imagination, obedience, and role-playing (Kirsch, 2005). For example, many theorists believe that all hypnosis is really selfhypnosis (autosuggestion). From this perspective, a hypnotist merely helps another person to follow a series of suggestions. These suggestions, in turn, alter sensations, perceptions, thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (Lynn & Kirsch, 2006). (See “Swinging Suggestions.”) Answers: 1. a 2. b 3. F 4. apnea 5. a 6. memories 7. d 8. b 9. Because people are immobilized during REM sleep and REM sleep is strongly associated with dreaming. This makes it unlikely that sleepwalkers are acting out dreams. 195 States of Consciousness D ISCOVERI N G P S Y CH OL O G Y Here’s a demonstration you can use to gain insight into hypnosis. Tie a short length of string (about 6 inches) to a small, heavy object, such as a ring or a small metal nut. Hold the ring at eye level, about a foot from your face. Concentrate on the ring and notice that it will begin to move, ever so slightly. As it does, focus all your attention on the ring. Narrow your attention to a beam of energy and mentally push the ring away from you. Each time the ring swings away, push on it, using only mental force. Then release it and let it swing back toward you. Continue to mentally push and release the ring until it is swinging freely. For the best results, try this now, before reading more. Did the ring move? If it did, you used autosuggestion to influence your own behavior in a subtle way. Suggestions that the ring would swing caused your hand to make tiny micromuscular movements. These, in turn, caused the ring to move — no special mental powers or supernatural forces are involved. As is true of hypnotic suggestion, the ring’s movement probably seemed to be automatic. Obviously, you could just intentionally swing the ring. However, if you responded to suggestion, the movement seemed to happen without any effort on your part. In the same way, when people are hypnotized, their actions seem to occur without any voluntary intent. Incidentally, autosuggestion likely underlies other phenomena, such as how Ouija boards answer questions without any conscious movements by the person using the pointer. Dennis Coon Swinging Suggestions Regardless of which theoretical approach finally prevails, both views suggest that hypnosis can be explained by normal principles. It is not mysterious or “magical,” despite what stage hypnotists might have you think. remain in control of their behavior and aware of what is going on. For instance, most people will not act out hypnotic suggestions that they consider immoral or repulsive (such as disrobing in public or harming someone) (Kirsch & Lynn, 1995). The Reality of Hypnosis Hypnotic Susceptibility How is hypnosis done? Hypnotists use many different methods. Still, all techniques encourage a person (1) to focus attention on what is being said, (2) to relax and feel tired, (3) to “let go” and accept suggestions easily, and (4) to use vivid imagination (Druckman & Bjork, 1994). Basically, you must cooperate to become hypnotized. What does it feel like to be hypnotized? You might be surprised at some of your actions during hypnosis. You also might have mild feelings of floating, sinking, anesthesia, or separation from your body. Personal experiences vary widely. A key element in hypnosis is the basic suggestion effect (a tendency of hypnotized persons to carry out suggested actions as if they were involuntary). Hypnotized persons feel like their actions and experiences are automatic — they seem to happen without effort. Here is how one person described his hypnotic session: Can everyone be hypnotized? About 8 people out of 10 can be hypnotized, but only 4 out of 10 will be good hypnotic subjects. People who are imaginative and prone to fantasy are often highly responsive to hypnosis (Kallio & Revonsuo, 2003). But people who lack these traits may also be hypnotized. If you are willing to be hypnotized, chances are good that you could be. Hypnosis depends more on the efforts and abilities of the hypnotized person than the skills of the hypnotist. But make no mistake; people who are hypnotized are not merely faking their responses. Hypnotic susceptibility refers to how easily a person can become hypnotized. It is measured by giving a series of suggestions and counting the number of times a person responds. A typical hypnotic test is the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, shown I felt lethargic, my eyes going out of focus and wanting to close. My hands felt real light. . . . I felt I was sinking deeper into the chair. . . . I felt like I wanted to relax more and more. . . . My responses were more automatic. I didn’t have to wish to do things so much or want to do them. . . . I just did them. . . . I felt floating . . . very close to sleep. (Hilgard, 1968) Could I be hypnotized against my will? Contrary to the way hypnosis is portrayed in movies, hypnotized people generally Hypnosis An altered state of consciousness characterized by narrowed attention and increased suggestibility. Hidden observer A detached part of the hypnotized person’s awareness that silently observes events. Basic suggestion effect The tendency of hypnotized persons to carry out suggested actions as if they were involuntary. Hypnotic susceptibility One’s capacity for becoming hypnotized. 196 CHAPTER 6 Table 6.3 • Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale Suggested Behavior Effects of Hypnosis What can (and cannot) be achieved with hypnosis? Many abilities have been tested during hypnosis, leading to the following conclusions (Burgess & Kirsch, 1999; Chaves, 2000): Criterion of Passing 1. Postural sway Falls without forcing 2. Eye closure Closes eyes without forcing 3. Hand lowering (left) Lowers at least 6 inches by end of 10 seconds 4. Immobilization (right arm) Arm rises less than 1 inch in 10 seconds 5. Finger lock Incomplete separation of fingers at end of 10 seconds 6. Arm rigidity (left arm) Less than 2 inches of arm bending in 10 seconds 7. Hands moving together Hands at least as close as 6 inches after 10 seconds 8. Verbal inhibition (name) Name unspoken in 10 seconds 9. Hallucination (fly) Any movement, grimacing, acknowledgment of effect 1. Superhuman acts of strength. Hypnosis has no more effect on physical strength than instructions that encourage a person to make his or her best effort. 2. Memory. There is some evidence that hypnosis can enhance memory (Wagstaff et al., 2004). However, it frequently increases the number of false memories as well. For this reason, many states now bar persons from testifying in court if they were hypnotized to improve their memory of a crime they witnessed. bridges 10. Eye catalepsy Eyes remain closed at end of 10 seconds 11. Posthypnotic (changes chairs) Any partial movement response 12. Amnesia test Three or fewer items recalled Adapted from Weitzenhoffer & Hilgard, 1959. • in Table 6.3. In the test, various suggestions are made, and the person’s response is noted. For instance, you might be told that your left arm is becoming more and more rigid and that it will not bend. If you can’t bend your arm during the next 10 seconds, you have shown susceptibility to hypnotic suggestions. (Also see Figure 6.8.) • Should the police use hypnosis to enhance the memories of witnesses? The evidence generally says no. See Chapter 8, page 258. 3. Amnesia. A person told not to remember something heard during hypnosis may claim not to remember. In some instances this may be nothing more than a deliberate attempt to avoid thinking about specific ideas. However, brief memory loss of this type actually does seem to occur (Barnier, McConkey, & Wright, 2004). 4. Pain relief. Hypnosis can relieve pain (Keefe, Abernethy, & Campbell, 2005). It can be especially useful when chemical painkillers are ineffective. For instance, hypnosis can reduce phantom limb pain (Oakley, Whitman, & Halligan, 2002). (As discussed in Chapter 4, amputees sometimes feel phantom pain that seems to come from a missing limb.) 5. Age regression. Given the proper suggestions, some hypnotized people appear to “regress” to childhood. However, most theorists now believe that “age-regressed” subjects are only acting out a suggested role. 6. Sensory changes. Hypnotic suggestions concerning sensations are among the most effective. Given the proper instructions, a person can be made to smell a small bottle of ammonia and respond as if it were a wonderful perfume. It is also possible to alter color vision, hearing sensitivity, time sense, perception of illusions, and many other sensory responses. Dennis Coon Hypnosis is a valuable tool. It can help people relax, feel less pain, and make better progress in therapy (Chapman, 2006). Generally, hypnosis is more successful at changing subjective experience than it is at modifying behaviors such as smoking or overeating. 6.8 In one test of hypnotizability, subjects attempt to pull their hands •apartFigure after hearing suggestions that their fingers are “locked” together. Stage Hypnosis On stage the hypnotist intones, “When I count to three, you will imagine that you are on a train to Disneyland and growing younger and younger as the train approaches.” Responding to these sugges- States of Consciousness tions, grown men and women begin to giggle and squirm like children on their way to a circus. How do stage entertainers use hypnosis to get people to do strange things? They don’t. Little or no hypnosis is needed to do a good hypnosis act. Stage hypnosis is often merely a simulation of hypnotic effects. Stage hypnotists make use of several features of the stage setting to perform their act (Barber, 2000). 1. Waking suggestibility. We are all more or less open to suggestion, but on stage people are unusually cooperative because they don’t want to “spoil the act.” As a result, they will readily follow almost any instruction given by the entertainer. 2. Selection of responsive subjects. Participants in stage hypnotism (all volunteers) are first “hypnotized” as a group. Then, anyone who doesn’t yield to instructions is eliminated. 3. The hypnosis label disinhibits. Once a person has been labeled “hypnotized,” she or he can sing, dance, act silly, or whatever, without fear or embarrassment. On stage, being “hypnotized” takes away personal responsibility for one’s actions. 4. The hypnotist as a “director.” After volunteers loosen up and respond to a few suggestions, they find that they are suddenly the stars of the show. Audience response to the antics on stage brings out the “ham” in many people. All the “hypnotist” needs to do is direct the action. 5. The stage hypnotist uses tricks. Stage hypnosis is about 50 percent taking advantage of the situation and 50 percent deception. One of the more impressive stage tricks is to rigidly suspend a person between two chairs. This is astounding only because the audience does not question it. Anyone can do it, as is shown in the photographs and instructions in Figure 6.9. Try it! • 197 To summarize, hypnosis is real, and it can significantly alter private experience. Hypnosis is a useful tool in a variety of settings. Nightclubs, however, are not one of these settings. Stage “hypnotists” entertain; they rarely hypnotize. Meditation and Sensory Deprivation — Chilling, the Healthy Way Gateway Question: Do meditation and sensory deprivation have any benefits? Throughout history, meditation and sensory deprivation have been widely used as a means of altering consciousness through deep relaxation. Let’s see what they have in common and how they differ. Meditation Meditation is a mental exercise used to alter consciousness. In general, meditation focuses attention and interrupts the typical flow of thoughts, worries, and analysis. People who use meditation to reduce stress often report less daily physical tension and anxiety (Andresen, 2000). Brain scans (positron emission tomography [PET] and functional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI]) reveal changes in the activity of the frontal lobes during meditation, which suggests that it may be a distinct state of consciousness (Cahn & Polich, 2006; Farb et al., 2007). Meditation takes two major forms. In concentrative meditation, you attend to a single focal point, such as an object, a thought, or your own breathing. In contrast, mindfulness meditation is “open,” or expansive. In this case, you widen your attention to embrace a total, nonjudgmental awareness of the world (Lazar, 2005). An example is losing all self-consciousness while walking in the wilderness with a quiet and receptive mind. Although it may not seem so, mindfulness meditation is more difficult to attain than concentrative meditation. For this reason, we will discuss concentrative meditation as a practical self-control method. Performing Concentrative Meditation How is concentrative meditation done? The basic idea is to sit still and quietly focus on some external object or on a repetitive internal stimulus, such as your own breathing or humming (Blackmore, 2004). As an alternative, you can silently repeat a mantra (a word used as the focus of attention in concentrative meditation). Typi- Dennis Coon Stage hypnosis Use of hypnosis to entertain; often, merely a simulation of hypnosis for that purpose. Meditation A mental exercise for producing relaxation or heightened awareness. 6.9 Arrange three chairs as shown. Have someone recline as shown. •AskFigure him to lift slightly while you remove the middle chair. Accept the applause Concentrative meditation Mental exercise based on attending to a single object or thought. gracefully! (Concerning hypnosis and similar phenomena, the moral, of course, is, “Suspend judgment until you have something solid to stand on.”) Mindfulness meditation Mental exercise based on widening attention to become aware of everything experienced at any given moment. 198 CHAPTER 6 cal mantras are smooth, flowing sounds that are easily repeated. A widely used mantra is the word “om.” A mantra could also be any pleasant word or a phrase from a familiar song, poem, or prayer. If other thoughts arise as you repeat a mantra, just return attention to it as often as necessary to maintain meditation. The Relaxation Response The benefits of meditation include lowered heart rate, blood pressure, muscle tension, and other signs of stress (Lazar et al., 2000). Medical researcher Herbert Benson believes that the core of meditation is the relaxation response — an innate physiological pattern that opposes your body’s fight-or-flight mechanisms. Benson feels, quite simply, that most of us have forgotten how to relax deeply. People in his experiments learned to produce the relaxation response by following these instructions: Sit quietly and comfortably. Close your eyes. Relax your muscles, beginning at your feet and progressing up to your head. Relax them deeply. Become aware of breathing through your nose. As you breathe out, say a word like “peace” silently to yourself. Don’t worry about how successful you are in relaxing deeply. Just let relaxation happen at its own pace. Don’t be surprised by distracting thoughts. When they occur, ignore them and continue repeating “peace.” (Adapted from Benson, 1977; Lazar et al., 2000) As a stress-control technique, meditation may be a good choice for people who find it difficult to “turn off ” upsetting thoughts when they need to relax. In one study, a group of college students who received just 90 minutes of training in the relaxation response experienced greatly reduced stress levels (Deckro et al., 2002). The physical benefits of meditation include lowered heart rate, blood pressure, muscle tension, and other signs of stress (Lazar et al., 2000), as well as improved immune system activity (Davidson et al., 2003). According to Shauna Shapiro and Roger Walsh (2006), meditation has benefits beyond relaxation. Practiced regularly, meditation may foster mental well-being and positive mental skills such as clarity, concentration, and calm. In this sense, meditation may share much in common with psychotherapy. Indeed, research has shown that mindfulness meditation relieves a variety of psychological disorders, from insomnia to anxiety disorders. It can also reduce aggression and the illegal use of psychoactive drugs (Shapiro & Walsh, 2006). Regular meditation may even help people develop self-awareness and maturity (Travis, Arenander, & DuBois, 2004). Sensory Deprivation The relaxation response can also be produced by brief sensory deprivation. Sensory deprivation (SD) refers to any major reduction in the amount or variety of sensory stimulation. What happens when stimulation is greatly reduced? A hint comes from reports by prisoners in solitary confinement, arctic explorers, high-altitude pilots, long-distance truck drivers, and radar operators. When faced with limited or monotonous stimulation, people sometimes have bizarre sensations, dangerous lapses Figure 6.10 A sensory isolation chamber. Small flotation tanks like the one •pictured have been used by psychologists to study the effects of mild sensory deprivation. Subjects float in darkness and silence. The shallow body-temperature water contains hundreds of pounds of Epsom salts so that subjects float near the surface. Mild sensory deprivation produces deep relaxation. in attention, and wildly distorted perceptions. Intense or prolonged SD is stressful and disorienting. Yet, oddly enough, brief periods of sensory restriction can be very relaxing. Psychologists have explored the possible benefits of sensory restriction using small isolation tanks like the one pictured in Figure 6.10. An hour or two spent in a flotation tank, for instance, causes a large drop in blood pressure, muscle tension, and other signs of stress (van Dierendonck & Te Nijenhuis, 2005). Of course, it could be argued that a warm bath has the same effect. Nevertheless, brief sensory deprivation appears to be one of the surest ways to induce deep relaxation (Suedfeld & Borrie, 1999). Like meditation, sensory restriction may also help with more than relaxation. Mild sensory deprivation can help people quit smoking, lose weight, and reduce their use of alcohol and drugs (van Dierendonck & Te Nijenhuis, 2005). Psychologist Peter Suedfeld calls such benefits Restricted Environmental Stimulation Therapy (REST). Deep relaxation makes people more open to suggestion, and sensory restriction interrupts habitual behavior patterns. As a result, REST can “loosen” belief systems and make it easier to change bad habits (Suedfeld & Borrie, 1999). REST also shows promise as a way to stimulate creative thinking (Norlander, Bergman, & Archer, 1998). Other researchers have reported that REST sessions can enhance performance in skilled sports, such as gymnastics, tennis, basketball, darts, and marksmanship (Druckman & Bjork, 1994; Norlander, Bergman, & Archer, 1999). There is also evidence that REST can relieve chronic pain and reduce stress (Bood et al., 2006). Clearly, there is much yet to be learned from studying “nothingness.” • Summary To summarize, research suggests that meditation and mild sensory deprivation are ways to elicit the relaxation response. For many people, sitting quietly and “resting” can be as effective. Similar stress reduction occurs when people set aside time daily to engage in other restful activities, such as muscle relaxation, positive daydreaming, and even leisure reading. However, if you are the type of person who finds it difficult to ignore upsetting thoughts, then concentrative meditation might be a good way to promote relaxation. Practiced regularly, meditation and REST may even help improve overall mental health — something almost everyone could use in our fast-paced society. States of Consciousness Did you “space out” anytime today? Most of us have occasional moments of reduced awareness. Mindfulness is the opposite of such mindless moments: It involves an open, nonjudgmental awareness of current experience. In other words, mindfulness is similar to the state that people who practice receptive meditation are trying to achieve. A person who is mindful is fully present, moment-by-moment. She or he is acutely aware of every thought, emotion, or sensation, but does not judge it or react to it. The person is fully “awake” and attuned to immediate reality. Psychologists interested in positive mental states have begun to study the effects of mindfulness. For example, cancer patients who are taught mindfulness meditation have lower levels of distress and a greater sense of well-being. Such benefits apply to healthy people, too. In general, mindfulness is associated with self-knowledge and well-being (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Anyone who has a tendency to sleepwalk through life — and that’s most of us at times — would be wise to be mindful of the value of mindfulness. 10. Regular meditators report lower levels of stress and a greater sense of well-being. What other explanations must we eliminate before this effect can be regarded as genuine? Relate How have your beliefs about hypnosis changed after reading the preceding section? Can you think of specific examples in which hypnosis was misrepresented, such as in high school assemblies, stage acts, movies, or TV dramas? Various activities can produce the relaxation response. When do you experience states of deep relaxation, coupled with a sense of serene awareness? What similarities do these occurrences have to meditation? Have you experienced any form of sensory restriction or sensory deprivation? How did you react? Would you be willing to try REST in order to break a bad habit? Answers: 1. d 2. a 3. b 4. F 5. F 6. b 7. T 8. F 9. Most experiments on hypnosis include a control group in which people are asked to simulate being hypnotized. Without such controls, the tendency of subjects to cooperate with experimenters makes it difficult to identify true hypnotic effects. 10. Studies on the effects of meditation must control for the placebo effect and the fact that those who choose to learn meditation may not be a representative sample of the general population. Positive Psychology: Mindfulness and Well-Being 199 K N O W L E D GE B U I L D E R Hypnosis, Meditation, and Sensory Deprivation RECITE 1. In Ernest Hilgard’s dissociative state theory of hypnosis, awareness is split between normal consciousness and a. disinhibition b. autosuggestion c. memory d. the hidden observer 2. Tests of hypnotic susceptibility measure a person’s tendency to respond to a. suggestion b. imagery rehearsal c. stimulus control techniques d. the activation-synthesis effect 3. Which of the following can most definitely be achieved with hypnosis? a. unusual strength b. pain relief c. improved memory d. sleep-like brainwaves 4. The focus of attention in concentrative meditation is “open,” or expansive. T or F? 5. Mantras are words said silently to oneself to end a session of meditation. T or F? 6. Which terms do not belong together? a. concentrative meditation — relaxation response b. mindfulness meditation — mantra c. sensory deprivation — REST d. meditation — alter consciousness 7. The most immediate benefit of meditation appears to be its capacity for producing the relaxation response. T or F? 8. Prolonged periods of extreme sensory deprivation lower anxiety and induce deep relaxation. T or F? REFLECT Critical Thinking 9. What kind of control group would you need in order to identify the true effects of hypnosis? Drug-Altered Consciousness — The High and Low of It Gateway Question: What are the effects of the more commonly used psychoactive drugs? The most common way to alter human consciousness is to administer a psychoactive drug (a substance capable of altering attention, judgment, memory, time sense, self-control, emotion, or perception). In fact, most Americans regularly use consciousness-altering drugs (don’t forget that caffeine, alcohol, and nicotine are mildly psychoactive). Psychoactive drugs alter consciousness by directly influencing brain activity (Julien, 2005). (See “How Psychoactive Drugs Affect the Brain.”) Many psychoactive drugs can be placed on a scale ranging from stimulation to depression ( Figure 6.11). A stimulant (upper) is a substance that increases activity in the body and nervous system. A depressant (downer) does the reverse. • Relaxation response The pattern of internal bodily changes that occurs at times of relaxation. Sensory deprivation (SD) Any major reduction in the amount or variety of sensory stimulation. Restricted Environmental Stimulation Therapy (REST) A form of sensory deprivation that results in a variety of psychological benefits. Mindfulness A state of open, nonjudgmental awareness of current experience. Psychoactive drug A substance capable of altering attention, memory, judgment, time sense, self-control, mood, or perception. Stimulant (upper) A substance that increases activity in the body and nervous system. Depressant (downer) A substance that decreases activity in the body and nervous system. 200 CHAPTER 6 Drug Effects STIMULATION Drug Groups Death Strychnine Convulsions Extreme nervousness, tremors Anxiety, palpitations Amphetamines Cocaine (large dose) Feeling of well-being, euphoria Distortion of time and space Antidepressants Cocaine (small dose) Hallucinogens (LSD, mescaline, marijuana) Nicotine Caffeine Increased alertness NEUTRAL AREA Tranquilizers Anxiety relief Feeling of well-being, euphoria Loss of pain Narcotics, barbiturates, alcohol (small dose) Narcotics (medium dose) Barbiturates, alcohol (medium dose) Hypnotics Narcotics, barbiturates, alcohol (large dose) Anesthetics Drowsiness Sleep Loss of consciousness Convulsions Death • DEPRESSION Figure 6.11 Spectrum and continuum of drug action. Many drugs can be rated on a stimulation-depression scale according to their effects on the central nervous system. Although LSD, mescaline, and marijuana are listed here, the stimulation-depression scale is less relevant to these drugs. The principal characteristic of such hallucinogens is their mind-altering quality. Because drugs that can ease pain, induce sleep, or end depression have a high potential for abuse, the more powerful psychoactive drugs are controlled substances (Goldberg, 2006). Nevertheless, more than 20 million Americans are currently illicit drug users (SAMHSA, 2005). Drug abuse has been one of the most persistent of all social problems in Western nations. Why is drug abuse so common? People seek drug experiences for many reasons, ranging from curiosity and a desire to belong to a group, to a search for meaning or an escape from feelings of inadequacy. Many abusers turn to drugs in a self-defeating attempt to cope with life. All the frequently abused drugs produce immediate feelings of pleasure. The negative consequences follow much later. This combination of immediate pleasure and delayed punishment allows abusers to feel good on demand. In time, of course, most of the pleasure goes out of drug abuse and the abuser’s problems get worse. But if an abuser merely feels better (however briefly) after taking a drug, drug taking can become compulsive (Higgins, Heil, & Lussier, 2004). In contrast, people who stop using drugs often say that they quit because the drawbacks had come to exceed the benefits (Toneatto et al., 1999). The best predictors of adolescent drug use and abuse are drug use by peers, parental drug use, delinquency, parental maladjust- ment, poor self-esteem, social nonconformity, and stressful life changes. One study found that adolescents who abuse drugs tend to be maladjusted, alienated, impulsive, and emotionally distressed (Masse & Tremblay, 1997). Antisocial behavior, school failure, and risky sexual behavior are also commonly associated with drug abuse (Ary et al., 1999). Such patterns make it clear that taking drugs is a symptom, rather than a cause, of personal and social maladjustment (Ksir, Hart, & Ray, 2006). Drug Dependence Another reason why drug abuse is so common is that taking most psychoactive drugs tends to create dependencies. Once you get started, it can be hard to stop. Drug dependence falls into two broad categories. When a person compulsively uses a drug to maintain bodily comfort, a physical dependence (addiction) exists ( Julien, 2005). Addiction occurs most often with drugs that cause withdrawal symptoms (physical illness that follows removal of a drug). Withdrawal from drugs such as alcohol, barbiturates, and opiates can cause violent flu-like symptoms of nausea, vomiting, diarrhea, chills, sweating, and cramps. Addiction is often accompanied by a drug tolerance (reduced response to a drug). This leads users to take larger and larger doses to get the desired effect. Persons who develop a psychological dependence feel that a drug is necessary to maintain their comfort or well-being. Usually, they intensely crave the drug and its rewarding qualities (Winger et al., 2005). Psychological dependence can be just as powerful as physical addiction. That’s why some psychologists define addiction as any compulsive habit pattern. By this definition, a person who has lost control over drug use, for whatever reason, is addicted. In fact, most people who answer yes to both of the following questions have an alcohol or drug problem and should seek professional help (Brown et al., 1997): • • In the last year, did you ever drink or use drugs more than you meant to? Have you felt you wanted or needed to cut down on your drinking or drug use in the last year? Patterns of Abuse Some drugs, of course, have a higher potential for abuse than others. Heroin is certainly more dangerous than caffeine. However, this is only one side of the picture. It can be as useful to classify drug-taking behavior as it is to rate drugs. For example, some people remain social drinkers for life, whereas others become alcoholics within weeks of taking their first drink (Robinson & Berridge, 2003). In this sense, drug use can be classified as experimental (short-term use based on curiosity), socialrecreational (occasional social use for pleasure or relaxation), situational (use to cope with a specific problem, such as needing to stay awake), intensive (daily use with elements of dependence), or compulsive (intense use and extreme dependence). The last three categories of drug taking tend to be damaging no matter what drug is used. States of Consciousness 201 B RAIN W AVES How Psychoactive Drugs Affect the Brain Psychoactive drugs influence the activity of brain cells (Kalat, 2007). Typically, drugs imitate or alter the effects of neurotransmitters, the chemicals that carry messages between brain cells. Some drugs, such as Ecstasy, amphetamines, and some antidepressants, cause more neurotransmitters to be released, increasing the activity of brain cells. Other drugs, such as cocaine, slow the removal of neurotransmitters after they are released. This prolongs the action of transmitter chemicals and typically has a stimulating effect. Other drugs, such as nicotine and opiates, directly stimulate brain cells by mimicking neurotransmitters. Another possibility is illustrated by alcohol and tranquilizers. These drugs affect certain types of brain cells that cause relaxation and relieve anxiety. Some drugs fill receptor sites on brain cells and block incoming messages. Other possibilities also exist, which is why drugs can have such a wide variety of effects on the brain (Julien, 2005). Nearly all addictive drugs stimulate the brain’s reward circuitry, producing feelings of pleasure (Kalat, 2007). In particular, addictive drugs stimulate a brain region called the nucleus accumbens to release the neurotransmitter dopamine, which results in intensified feelings of pleasure ( Figure 6.12). As one expert put it, addictive drugs fool brain-reward pathways. “As a result, the reward pathway signals, ‘That felt good. Let’s do it again. Let’s remember exactly how we did it.’” This creates a compulsion to repeat the drug experience. It’s the hook that eventually snares the addict • Dopamine projections to prefrontal cortex (Restak, 2001). In the end, the addictive drug physically changes the brain’s reward circuitry, making it even harder for the addict to overcome his or her addiction (Robinson & Berridge, 2003). Adolescents are especially susceptible to addiction because brain systems that restrain their risk-taking are not as mature as those that reward pleasure seeking (Chambers, Taylor, & Potenza, 2003). Figure 6.12 Addictive •drugs increase dopamine activity in the medial forebrain bundle and the nucleus accumbens, stimulating the frontal cortex and giving rise to intensified feelings of pleasure. Nucleus accumbens Medial forebrain bundle (a path of axons that release dopamine) Drugs of Abuse • Note in Table 6.4 that the drugs most likely to lead to physical dependence are alcohol, amphetamines, barbiturates, cocaine, codeine, heroin, methadone, morphine, and tobacco (nicotine). Using any of the drugs listed in Table 6.4 can result in psychological dependence. Note too that people who take drugs intravenously are at high risk for developing hepatitis and AIDS. The discussion that follows focuses on the drugs most often abused by students. A more complete summary of frequently abused psychoactive drugs is given in Table 6.4. • • bridges Area in midbrain sion. Today, the main legitimate medical use of amphetamines is to treat childhood hyperactivity and overdoses of depressant drugs. Illicit use of amphetamines is widespread, however, especially by people seeking to stay awake and by those who think drugs can improve mental or physical performance (Iversen, 2006). Methamphetamine is a more potent variation of amphetamine. It can be snorted, injected, or eaten. Of the various types of amphetamine, methamphetamine has created the largest drug problem. “Bergs,” “glass,” “meth,” or “crystal,” as it is known on the street, can be made cheaply in backyard labs and sold for massive profits. In addition to ruining lives through addiction, it has fueled a violent criminal subculture. See Chapter 11, pages 379–380, for more information about AIDS. Uppers — Amphetamines, Cocaine, MDMA, Caffeine, Nicotine Amphetamines are synthetic stimulants. Common street names for amphetamine are “speed,” “bennies,” “dexies,” “go,” and “uppers.” These drugs were once widely prescribed for weight loss or depres- Physical dependence (addiction) Physical addiction, as indicated by the presence of drug tolerance and withdrawal symptoms. Withdrawal symptoms Physical illness and discomfort following the withdrawal of a drug. Drug tolerance A reduction in the body’s response to a drug. Psychological dependence Drug dependence that is based primarily on emotional or psychological needs. 202 CHAPTER 6 Table 6.4 • Comparison of Psychoactive Drugs Name Classification Medical Use Usual Dose Duration of Effect Alcohol Sedative-hypnotic Solvent, antiseptic Varies 1–4 hours Amphetamines Stimulant Relief of mild depression, control of narcolepsy and hyperactivity 2.5–5 milligrams 4 hours Barbiturates Sedative-hypnotic Sedation, relief of high blood pressure, anticonvulsant 50–100 milligrams 4 hours Benzodiazepines Anxiolytic (antianxiety drug) Tranquilizer 2–100 milligrams 10 minutes–8 hours Caffeine Stimulant Counteract depressant drugs, treatment of migraine headaches Varies Varies Cocaine Stimulant, local anesthetic Local anesthesia Varies Varied, brief periods Codeine Narcotic Ease pain and coughing 30 milligrams 4 hours GHB Sedative-hypnotic Experimental treatment of narcolepsy, alcoholism 1–3 grams (powder) 1–3 hours Heroin Narcotic X Pain relief Varies 4 hours LSD Narcotic Experimental study of mental function, alcoholism 100–500 milligrams 10 hours Marijuana (THC) Relaxant, euphoriant; in high doses, hallucinogen Treatment of glaucoma and side effects of chemotherapy 1–2 cigarettes 4 hours MDMA Stimulant/hallucinogen None 125 milligrams 4–6 hours Mescaline Hallucinogen None 350 micrograms 12 hours Methadone Narcotic Pain relief 10 milligrams 4–6 hours Morphine Narcotic Pain relief 15 milligrams 6 hours PCP Anesthetic None 2–10 milligrams 4–6 hours, plus 12-hour recovery Psilocybin Hallucinogen None 25 milligrams 6–8 hours Tobacco (nicotine) Stimulant Emetic (nicotine) Varies Varies Question marks indicate conflict of opinion. It should be noted that illicit drugs are frequently adulterated and thus pose unknown hazards to the user. Amphetamines rapidly produce a drug tolerance. Most abusers end up taking ever-larger doses to get the desired effect. Eventually, some users switch to injecting methamphetamine directly into the bloodstream. True speed freaks typically go on binges lasting several days, after which they “crash” from lack of sleep and food. Abuse How dangerous are amphetamines? Amphetamines pose many dangers. Large doses can cause nausea, vomiting, extremely high blood pressure, fatal heart attacks, and disabling strokes. It is important to realize that amphetamines speed up the use of the body’s resources; they do not magically supply energy. After an amphetamine binge, people suffer from crippling fatigue, depression, confusion, uncontrolled irritability, and aggression. Repeated amphetamine use damages the brain. Amphetamines can also cause amphetamine psychosis, a loss of contact with reality. Affected users have paranoid delusions that someone is out to get them. Acting on these delusions, they may become violent, resulting in suicide, self-injury, or injury to others (Iversen, 2006). A potent smokable form of crystal methamphetamine has added to the risks of stimulant abuse. This drug, known as “ice” on the street, is highly addictive. Like “crack,” the smokable form of cocaine, it produces an intense high. But also like crack (discussed in a moment), crystal methamphetamine leads very rapidly to compulsive abuse and severe drug dependence. States of Consciousness 203 Physical Dependence Potential Psychological Dependence Potential Organic Damage Potential Effects Sought Long-term Symptoms Sense alteration, anxiety reduction, sociability Cirrhosis, toxic psychosis, neurologic damage, addiction Yes Yes Yes Alertness, activeness Loss of appetite, delusions, hallucinations, toxic psychosis Yes Yes Yes Anxiety reduction, euphoria Addiction with severe withdrawal symptoms, possible convulsions, toxic psychosis Yes Yes Yes Anxiety relief Irritability, confusion, depression, sleep disorders Yes Yes No, but can affect fetus Wakefulness, alertness Insomnia, heart arrhythmias, high blood pressure No? Yes Yes Excitation, talkativeness Depression, convulsions Yes Yes Yes Euphoria, prevent withdrawal discomfort Addiction, constipation, loss of appetite Yes Yes No Intoxication, euphoria, relaxation Anxiety, confusion, insomnia, hallucinations, seizures Yes Yes No? Euphoria, prevent withdrawal discomfort Addiction, constipation, loss of appetite Yes Yes No* Insightful experiences, exhilaration, distortion of senses May intensify existing psychosis, panic reactions No No? No? Relaxation; increased euphoria, perceptions, sociability Possible lung cancer, other health risks Yes Yes Yes? Excitation, euphoria Personality change, hyperthermia, liver damage No Yes Yes Insightful experiences, exhilaration, distortion of senses May intensify existing psychosis, panic reactions No No? No? Prevent withdrawal discomfort Addiction, constipation, loss of appetite Yes Yes No Euphoria, prevent withdrawal discomfort Addiction, constipation, loss of appetite Yes Yes No* Euphoria Unpredictable behavior, suspicion, hostility, psychosis Debated Yes Yes Insightful experiences, exhilaration, distortion of senses May intensify existing psychosis, panic reactions No No? No? Alertness, calmness, sociability Emphysema, lung cancer, mouth and throat cancer, cardiovascular damage, loss of appetite Yes Yes Yes *Persons who inject drugs under nonsterile conditions run a high risk of contracting AIDS, hepatitis, abscesses, or circulatory disorders. Cocaine Cocaine (“coke,” “snow,” “blow,” “snuff ”) is a powerful central nervous system stimulant extracted from the leaves of the coca plant. Cocaine produces feelings of alertness, euphoria, well-being, power, boundless energy, and pleasure ( Julien, 2005). At the turn of the twentieth century, dozens of nonprescription potions and cure-alls contained cocaine. It was during this time that Coca-Cola was indeed the “real thing.” From 1886 until 1906, when the U.S. Pure Food and Drug Act was passed, Coca-Cola contained cocaine (which has since been replaced with caffeine). How does cocaine differ from amphetamines? The two are very much alike in their effects on the central nervous system. The main difference is that amphetamine lasts several hours; cocaine is snorted and quickly metabolized, so its effects last only about 15 to 30 minutes. Abuse How dangerous is cocaine? Cocaine’s capacity for abuse and social damage rivals that of heroin. When rats and monkeys are given free access to cocaine, they find it irresistible. Many, in fact, end up dying of convulsions from self-administered overdoses of the drug. Even casual or first-time users risk having convulsions, a heart attack, or a stroke (Lacayo, 1995). Cocaine increases the chemical messengers dopamine (DOPE-ah-meen) and noradrenaline (norah-DREN-ah-lin). Noradrenaline arouses the brain, and dopamine produces a “rush” of pleasure. This combination is so power- 204 CHAPTER 6 National Library of Medicine cells to release extra amounts of serotonin. The physical effects of MDMA include dilated pupils, elevated blood pressure, jaw clenching, loss of appetite, and elevated body temperature (Braun, 2001). Although some users believe that Ecstasy increases sexual pleasure, it diminishes sexual performance, impairing erection in 40 percent of men and retarding orgasm in both men and women. (Zemishlany, Aizenberg, & Weizman, 2001). Cocaine was the main ingredient in many nonprescription elixirs before the turn of the twentieth century. Today cocaine is recognized as a powerful and dangerous drug. Its high potential for abuse has damaged the lives of countless users. fully rewarding that cocaine users run a high risk of becoming compulsive abusers (Ridenour et al., 2005). A person who stops using cocaine does not experience heroinlike withdrawal symptoms. Instead, the brain adapts to cocaine abuse in ways that upset its chemical balance, causing depression when cocaine is withdrawn. First, there is a jarring “crash” of mood and energy. Within a few days, the person enters a long period of fatigue, anxiety, paranoia, boredom, and anhedonia (an-he-DAWN-ee-ah: an inability to feel pleasure). Before long, the urge to use cocaine becomes intense. So, although cocaine does not fit the classic pattern of addiction, it is ripe for compulsive abuse. Even a person who gets through withdrawal may crave cocaine months or years later (Withers et al., 1995). If cocaine were cheaper, 9 out of 10 users would progress to compulsive abuse. In fact, rock cocaine (“crack,” “rock,” or “roca”), which is cheaper, produces very high abuse rates. Here are some signs of cocaine abuse: • • • Compulsive use. If cocaine is available — say, at a party — you can’t say no to it. Loss of control. Once you have had some cocaine, you will keep using it until you are exhausted or the cocaine is gone. Disregarding consequences. You don’t care if the rent gets paid, your job is endangered, your lover disapproves, or your health is affected, you’ll use cocaine anyway. Anyone who thinks she or he has a cocaine problem should seek advice at a drug clinic or a Cocaine Anonymous meeting. Although quitting cocaine is extremely difficult, three out of four abusers who remain in treatment do succeed in breaking their coke dependence (Simpson et al., 1999; Sinha et al., 2006). MDMA (“Ecstasy”) The drug MDMA (methylenedioxymethamphetamine, or “Ecstasy”) is also chemically similar to amphetamine. In addition to producing a rush of energy, users say it makes them feel closer to others and heightens sensory experiences. Ecstasy causes brain Abuse Ecstasy use in North America has declined slightly from a peak around 2002, perhaps because of widespread negative publicity. Regardless, every year more than half a million Americans try Ecstasy for the first time (SAMHSA, 2005). Every year, emergency room doctors see many MDMA cases, including MDMArelated deaths. Some of these incidents are caused by elevated body temperature (hyperthermia) or heart arrhythmias, which can lead to collapse. Ecstasy users at “rave” parties try to prevent overheating by drinking water to cool themselves. This may help to a small degree, but the risk of fatal heat exhaustion is real. MDMA can also cause severe liver damage, which can be fatal (Braun, 2001). In addition, Ecstasy users are more likely to abuse alcohol and other drugs, to neglect studying, to party excessively, and to engage in risky sex (Strote, Lee, & Wechsler, 2002). Ironically, Ecstasy use at “rave” parties actually does intensify the impact of the music. We say ironically because the end result is often overstimulation of the brain, which can result in a “rebound” depression (Michelangelo et al., 2006). It may take another decade before Ecstasy’s full impact on health emerges. For now, we know that repeated use of MDMA damages serotonergic brain cells. This damage lasts for years. It leads to feelings of anxiety or depression that can persist for months after a person stops taking Ecstasy. In addition, heavy users typically do not perform well in tests of learning and memory and show signs of underlying brain damage (Quednow et al., 2006). Despite its street name, Ecstasy may be a ticket to agony for many users (Iversen, 2006; Kuhn & Wilson, 2001). Caffeine Caffeine is the most frequently used psychoactive drug in North America. (And that’s not counting Seattle!) Caffeine stimulates the brain by blocking chemicals that normally inhibit or slow nerve activity ( Julien, 2005). Its effects become apparent with doses as small as 50 milligrams, the amount found in about onehalf cup of brewed coffee. Physically, caffeine causes sweating, talkativeness, tinnitus (ringing in the ears), and hand tremors (Nehlig, 2004). Psychologically, caffeine suppresses fatigue or drowsiness and increases feelings of alertness (Wesensten et al., 2002). Some people have a hard time starting a day (or writing another paragraph) without it. How much caffeine did you consume today? It is common to think of coffee as the major source of caffeine, but there are many others. Caffeine is found in tea, many soft drinks (especially colas), chocolate, and cocoa. More than 2,000 nonprescription States of Consciousness 205 drugs also contain caffeine, including stay-awake pills, cold remedies, and many name-brand aspirin products. Are there any serious drawbacks to using caffeine? Overuse of caffeine may result in an unhealthy dependence known as caffeinism. Insomnia, irritability, loss of appetite, chills, racing heart, and elevated body temperature are all signs of caffeinism. Many people with these symptoms drink 15 or 20 cups of coffee a day. However, even at lower dosages, caffeine can intensify anxiety and other psychological problems (Hogan, Hornick, & Bouchoux, 2002). Caffeine poses a variety of health risks. Caffeine encourages the growth of breast cysts in women, and it may contribute to bladder cancer, heart problems, and high blood pressure. Pregnant women should consider giving up caffeine entirely because of a suspected link between caffeine and birth defects. Pregnant women who consume as little as 2 cups coffee a day increase the risk of having a miscarriage (Cnattingius et al., 2000). It is customary to think that caffeine is not a drug. But as few as 2.5 cups of coffee a day (or the equivalent) can be a problem. People who consume even such modest amounts may experience anxiety, depression, fatigue, headaches, and flu-like symptoms during withdrawal ( Juliano & Griffiths, 2004). About half of all caffeine users show some signs of dependence (Hughes et al., 1998). It is wise to remember that caffeine is a drug and use it in moderation. Nicotine Nicotine is a natural stimulant found mainly in tobacco. Next to caffeine and alcohol, it is the most widely used psychoactive drug ( Julien, 2005). How does nicotine compare with other stimulants? Nicotine is a potent drug. It is so toxic that it is sometimes used to kill insects! In large doses it causes stomach pain, vomiting and diarrhea, cold sweats, dizziness, confusion, and muscle tremors. In very large doses, nicotine may cause convulsions, respiratory failure, and death. For a nonsmoker, 50 to 75 milligrams of nicotine taken in a single dose could be lethal. (Chain-smoking about 17 to 25 cigarettes will produce this dosage.) Most first-time smokers get sick on one or two cigarettes. In contrast, regular smokers build a tolerance for nicotine. A heavy smoker may inhale 40 cigarettes a day without feeling ill. Abuse How addictive is nicotine? A vast array of evidence confirms that nicotine is very addictive (Spinella, 2005). The average age of first use is 15, and it takes about a year before dependence sets in (Baker, Brandon, & Chassin, 2004). Among regular smokers who are 15 to 24 years old, 60 percent are addicted (Breslau et al., 2001). For many smokers, withdrawal from nicotine causes headaches, sweating, cramps, insomnia, digestive upset, irritability, and a sharp craving for cigarettes (NIDA, 2006). These symptoms may last from 2 to 6 weeks and may be worse than heroin withdrawal. The Everett Collection Abuse Actress and comedienne Lily Tomlin, here shown portraying one of her comedic characters, Ernestine, once took up smoking for a role in a movie (Shadows and Fog) and developed a 4-pack-a-day habit. As Tomlin’s experience shows, the best way to avoid developing a nicotine addiction is to not begin smoking in the first place. Indeed, relapse patterns are nearly identical for alcoholics, heroin addicts, cocaine abusers, and smokers who try to quit (Stolerman & Jarvis, 1995). Up to 90 percent of people who quit smoking relapse within a year, and 20 percent relapse even after 2 years of abstinence (Krall, Garvey, & Garcia, 2002). Impact on Health How serious are the health risks of smoking? If you think smoking is harmless, or that there’s no connection between smoking and cancer, you’re kidding yourself. A burning cigarette releases a large variety of potent carcinogens (car-SIN-oh-jins: cancer-causing substances). According to the U.S. Surgeon General (USDHHS, 2004), “smoking harms nearly every organ of the body,” leading to an increased risk of many cancers (such as lung cancer), cardiovascular diseases (such as stroke), respiratory diseases (such as chronic bronchitis), and reproductive disorders (such as decreased fertility). Together, these health risks combine to reduce the life expectancy of the average smoker by 10 to 15 years. (Skeptics please note: Wayne McLaren, who portrayed the rugged “Marlboro Man” in cigarette ads, died of lung cancer at age 51.) More people die every year from tobacco use than from motor vehicle injuries, murders, suicides, alcohol use, illegal drug use, and AIDS combined (NCCDPHP, 2004). Anhedonia An inability to feel pleasure. Caffeinism Excessive consumption of caffeine, leading to dependence and a variety of physical and psychological complaints. CHAPTER 6 © Clive Goddard/www.CartoonStock.com 206 By the way, urban cowboys and Skol bandits, the same applies to chewing tobacco and snuff. A 30-minute exposure to one pinch of smokeless tobacco is equivalent to smoking three or four cigarettes. Along with all the health risks of smoking, users of smokeless tobacco also run a higher risk of developing oral cancer (American Lung Association, 2006). Smokers don’t just risk their own health; they also endanger those who live and work nearby. Secondary smoke causes about 3,000 lung cancer deaths and as many as 62,000 heart disease deaths each year in the United States alone. It is particularly irresponsible of smokers to expose young children, who are especially vulnerable, to secondhand smoke (American Lung Association, 2006). Quitting Smoking Is it better for a person to quit smoking abruptly or taper down gradually? Most people try to quit by themselves. Some try to quit cold turkey, whereas others try to taper down gradually. Although going cold turkey has its advocates, gradually quitting works better for more people. Going cold turkey makes quitting an all-or-nothing proposition. Smokers who smoke even one cigarette after “quitting forever” tend to feel they’ve failed. Many figure they might just as well resume smoking. Those who quit gradually accept that success may take many attempts, spread over several months. If you are going to quit by going cold turkey, you will have a better chance of success if you decide to quit now rather than at some time in the future (West & Sohal, 2006). The best way to taper off is scheduled gradual reduction (Riley et al., 2002). There are many ways in which smoking can be gradually reduced. For example, the smoker can (1) delay having a first cigarette in the morning and then try to delay a little longer each day, (2) gradually reduce the total number of cigarettes smoked each day, or (3) quit completely, but for just 1 week, then quit again, a week at a time, for as many times as necessary to make it stick. Deliberately scheduling the gradually stretching of time periods between cigarettes is a key part of this program. Scheduled smoking apparently helps people learn to cope with the urge to smoke. As a result, people using this method are more likely remain permanent nonsmokers than people using other approaches (Cinciripini, Wetter, & McClure, 1997). Whatever approach is taken, quitting smoking is not easy (Abrams et al., 2003). Many people find that using nicotine patches or gum helps them get through the withdrawal period (Shiffman et al., 2006). Also, as we have noted, anyone trying to quit should be prepared to make several attempts before succeeding. But the good news is tens of millions of people have quit. bridges Behavioral self-management techniques can be very useful for breaking habits such as smoking. See Chapter 7, pages 247–248, and Chapter 15, pages 522–526. Downers — Sedatives, Tranquilizers, and Alcohol The most widely used downers, or depressant drugs, are barbiturates, GHB, benzodiazepine (ben-zoe-die-AZ-eh-peen) tranquilizers, and alcohol. These drugs are much alike in their effects. In fact, barbiturates and tranquilizers are sometimes referred to as “solid alcohol.” Let’s examine the properties of each. Barbiturates Barbiturates are sedative drugs that depress brain activity. Common barbiturates include amobarbital, pentobarbital, secobarbital, and Tuinal. On the street they are known as “downers,” “blue heavens,” “yellow jackets,” “lows,” “goof balls,” “reds,” “pink ladies,” “rainbows,” or “tooies.” Medically, barbiturates are used to calm patients or to induce sleep. At mild dosages, barbiturates have an effect similar to alcohol intoxication. Higher dosages can cause severe mental confusion or even hallucinations. Barbiturates are often taken in excess amounts because a first dose may be followed by others, as the user becomes uninhibited or forgetful. Overdoses first cause a loss of consciousness. Then they severely depress brain centers that control heartbeat and breathing. The result is death (McKim, 2007). GHB Would you swallow a mixture of degreasing solvent and drain cleaner to get high? Apparently, a lot of people would. A miniepidemic of GHB (gamma-hydroxybutyrate) use has taken place in recent years, especially at nightclubs and raves. GHB (“goop,” “scoop,” “max,” “Georgia Home Boy”) is a central nervous system depressant that relaxes and sedates the body. Users describe its effects as being similar to alcohol. Mild GHB intoxication tends to produce euphoria, a desire to socialize, and a mild loss of inhibi- States of Consciousness tions. GHB’s intoxicating effects typically last 3 to 4 hours, depending on the dosage. Abuse At lower dosages, GHB can relieve anxiety and produce relaxation. However, as the dose increases, its sedative effects may result in nausea, a loss of muscle control, and either sleep or a loss of consciousness. Potentially fatal doses of GHB are only three times the amount typically taken by users. This narrow margin of safety has led to numerous overdoses, especially when GHB was combined with alcohol. An overdose causes coma, breathing failure, and death. Also, GHB inhibits the gag reflex, so some users choke to death on their own vomit. In 2000, the U.S. government classified GHB as a controlled substance, making its possession a felony. Clinical evidence increasingly suggests that GHB is addictive and a serious danger to users. Two out of three frequent users have lost consciousness after taking GHB. Heavy users who stop taking GHB have withdrawal symptoms that include anxiety, agitation, tremor, delirium, and hallucinations (Miotto et al., 2001). As if the preceding weren’t enough reason to be leery of GHB, here’s one more to consider: GHB is often manufactured in homes with recipes and ingredients purchased on the Internet. As mentioned earlier, it can be produced by combining degreasing solvent with drain cleaner (Falkowski, 2000). If you want to degrease your brain, GHB will do the trick. Tranquilizers A tranquilizer is a drug that lowers anxiety and reduces tension. Doctors prescribe benzodiazepine tranquilizers to alleviate nervousness and stress. Valium is the best-known drug in this family; others are Xanax, Halcion, and Librium. Even at normal dosages these drugs can cause drowsiness, shakiness, and confusion. When used at too high a dosage or for too long, benzodiazepines are addictive (McKim, 2007). A drug sold under the trade name Rohypnol (ro-HIP-nol) has added to the problem of tranquilizer abuse. This drug, which is related to Valium, is cheap and 10 times more potent. It lowers inhibitions and produces relaxation or intoxication. Large doses induce short-term amnesia and sleep. “Roofies,” as they are known on the street, are odorless and tasteless. They have been used to spike drinks, which are given to the unwary. Victims of this “date rape” drug are then sexually assaulted or raped while they are unconscious. (Be aware, however, that drinking too much alcohol is by far the most common prelude to rape.) Abuse Repeated use of barbiturates can cause physical dependence. Some abusers suffer severe emotional depression that may end in suicide. Similarly, when tranquilizers are used at too high a dosage or for too long, addiction can occur. Many people have learned the hard way that their legally prescribed tranquilizers are as dangerous as many illicit drugs (McKim, 2007). 207 Combining barbiturates or tranquilizers with alcohol is especially risky. When mixed, the effects of both drugs are multiplied by a drug interaction (one drug enhances the effect of another). Drug interactions are responsible for many hundreds of fatal drug overdoses every year (Goldberg, 2006). All too often, depressants are gulped down with alcohol or added to a spiked punch bowl. This is the lethal brew that left a young woman named Karen Ann Quinlan in a coma that lasted 10 years, ending with her death. It is no exaggeration to restate that mixing depressants with alcohol can be deadly. Alcohol Alcohol is the common name for ethyl alcohol, the intoxicating element in fermented and distilled liquors. Contrary to popular belief, alcohol is not a stimulant. The noisy animation at drinking parties is due to alcohol’s effect as a depressant. As Figure 6.13 shows, small amounts of alcohol reduce inhibitions and produce feelings of relaxation and euphoria. Larger amounts cause evergreater impairment of the brain until the drinker loses consciousness. Alcohol is also not an aphrodisiac. Rather than enhancing sexual arousal, it usually impairs performance, especially in males. As William Shakespeare observed long ago, drink “provokes the desire, but it takes away the performance.” Some people become aggressive and want to argue or fight when they are drunk. Others become relaxed and friendly. How can the same drug have such different effects? When a person is drunk, thinking and perception become dulled or shortsighted, a condition that has been called alcohol myopia (my-OH-pea-ah). Only the most obvious and immediate stimuli catch a drinker’s attention. Worries and “second thoughts” that would normally restrain behavior are banished from the drinker’s mind. That’s why many behaviors become more extreme when a person is drunk. On college campuses, drunken students tend to have accidents, get into fights, sexually assault others, or engage in risky sex. They also destroy property and disrupt the lives of students who are trying to sleep or study (Brower, 2002) • Abuse Alcohol, the world’s favorite depressant, breeds our biggest drug problem. More than 20 million people in the United States and Canada have serious drinking problems. One American dies every 20 minutes in an alcohol-related car crash. The level of alcohol abuse among adolescents and young adults is alarming. Of 18-year-olds, 15 percent are heavy drinkers and 40 percent have engaged in binge drinking (SAMHSA, 2005). Binge drinking is Tranquilizer A drug that lowers anxiety and reduces tension. Drug interaction A combined effect of two drugs that exceeds the addition of one drug’s effects to the other. Alcohol myopia Shortsighted thinking and perception that occurs during alcohol intoxication. Binge drinking Consuming five or more drinks in a short time (four drinks for women). 208 CHAPTER 6 • Figure 6.13 The behavioral effects of alcohol are related to blood alcohol content and the resulting suppression of higher mental function. Arrows indicate the typical threshold for legal intoxication in the United States. (From Jozef Cohen, Eyewitness Series in Psychology, SS 7, p. 44. Copyright © 1970 by Rand McNally and Company. Reprinted by permission of the author’s estate.) Alcohol Consumed Neural Representation 2 ounces 90 proof whiskey Affects higher nervous centers; drinker loses inhibitions, forgoes conventions and courtesies, relaxes .05% blood alcohol 6 ounces 90 proof whiskey Affects deeper motor areas; drinker staggers, has slurred speech, is overconfident, acts on impulse .15% blood alcohol defined as downing five or more drinks in a short time (four drinks for women). Apparently, many students think it’s entertaining to get completely wasted and throw up on their friends. However, binge drinking is a serious sign of alcohol abuse. It is responsible for 1,400 college student deaths each year and thousands of trips to the ER (Wechsler et al., 2002). Binge drinking is of special concern because the brain continues to develop into the early 20s. Research has shown that teenagers and young adults who drink too much may lose as much as 10 percent of their brain power — especially their memory capacity (Brown et al., 2000). Such losses can have a long-term impact on a person’s chances for success in life. In short, getting drunk is a slow but sure way to get stupid (Wechsler & Wuethrich, 2002). At Risk Behavioral Effect 10 ounces 90 proof whiskey Affects emotional centers of midbrain; drinker has impaired motor reactions and unsteady gait; sensations are distorted; drinker tends to see double, to fall asleep .25% blood alcohol 16 ounces 90 proof whiskey Affects sensory area of cerebellum; senses are dulled; drinker is in stupor .4% blood alcohol 24 ounces 90 proof whiskey .6% blood alcohol Affects perceptual areas; drinker loses consciousness; only functions of breathing and heartbeat remain 32 ounces 90 proof whiskey Affects entire brain; heartbeat and respiration stop; death .8% blood alcohol Children of alcoholics and those who have other relatives who abuse alcohol are at greater risk for becoming alcohol abusers themselves. The increased risk appears to be partly genetic. It is based on the fact that some people have stronger cravings for alcohol after they drink (Hutchison et al., 2002). Women also face some special risks. For one thing, alcohol is absorbed faster and metabolized more slowly by women’s bodies. As a result, women get intoxicated from less alcohol than men do. Women who drink are also more prone to liver disease, osteoporosis, and depression. As few as three drinks a week may increase a woman’s risk of breast cancer by 50 percent. Positive reinforcement — drinking for pleasure — motivates most people who consume alcohol. What sets alcohol abusers apart is that they also drink to cope with negative emotions, such as anxiety and depression. That’s why alcohol abuse increases with the level of stress in people’s lives. People who drink to relieve bad feelings are at great risk of becoming alcoholics (Kenneth, Carpenter, & Hasin, 1998). Recognizing Problem Drinking What are the signs of alcohol abuse? Because alcohol abuse is such a common problem, it is important to recognize the danger signals. If you can answer yes to even one of the following questions, you may have a problem with drinking (adapted from the College Alcohol Problems Scale, revised; Maddock et al., 2001): As a Result of Drinking Alcoholic Beverages I . . . 1. engaged in unplanned sexual activity. 2. drove under the influence. 3. did not use protection when engaging in sex. 4. engaged in illegal activities associated with drug use. 5. felt sad, blue, or depressed. 6. was nervous or irritable. 7. felt bad about myself. 8. had problems with appetite or sleeping. States of Consciousness Table 6.5 209 • Drinking in Moderation Dougie Wallace/Alamy Approximate Number of Drinks per Hour to Stay Below 0.05 Blood Alcohol* Your Weight (pounds) Male Female 100 0.75 0.60 120 1.00 0.75 140 1.25 0.90 160 1.30 1.00 180 1.50 1.10 200 1.60 1.20 220 1.80 1.35 One drink 12 ounces beer, 4 ounces wine, 2.5 ounces brandy, or 1.25 ounces 80 proof liquor. *Table entries are approximate, owing to individual differences in metabolism, recency of meals, and other factors. Estimates are from tables prepared by Vogler and Bartz (1982, 1992). Binge drinking and alcohol abuse have become serious problems among college students. Many alcohol abusers regard themselves as “moderate” drinkers, which suggests that they are in denial about how much they actually drink (Grant & Dawson, 1997). 3. Limit drinking primarily to the first hour of a social event or party. Pace your drinking using the information from Table 6.5. 4. Practice how you will politely but firmly refuse drinks. 5. Learn how to relax, meet people, and socialize without relying on alcohol. • Perhaps the simplest way to identify problem drinkers is to ask a single question: “When was the last time you had more than five drinks (four for women) in a day?” Eighty-six percent of the people who answer “less than 3 months ago” are alcohol abusers (Williams & Vinson, 2001). Moderated Drinking Almost everyone has been to a party spoiled by someone who drank too much too fast. Those who avoid overdrinking have a better time, and so do their friends. But how do you avoid drinking too much? After all, as one wit once observed, “The conscience dissolves in alcohol.” Psychologists Roger Vogler and Wayne Bartz (1982, 1992) provide a partial answer. Vogler and Bartz observe that drinking makes you feel good while blood alcohol is rising and remains below a level of about 0.05. In this range, people feel relaxed, euphoric, and sociable. At higher levels they go from moderately intoxicated to thoroughly drunk. Later, as blood alcohol begins to fall, those who overdrink become sick and miserable. Table 6.5 shows the approximate amount per hour that can be consumed without exceeding the 0.05 blood alcohol level. (Even at this level, driving may be impaired.) By pacing themselves, those who choose to drink can remain comfortable, pleasant, and coherent during a long social event. In short, if you drink, it might be wise to learn your “magic” number from Table 6.5. It takes skill to regulate drinking in social situations, where the temptation to drink can be strong. If you choose to drink, here are some guidelines that may be helpful: • • Paced Drinking 1. Think about your drinking beforehand and plan how you will manage it. 2. Drink slowly, eat while drinking or drink on a full stomach, and make every other drink (or more) a nonalcoholic beverage. Adapted from Miller & Munoz, 2005; Vogler & Bartz, 1992. Limiting your own drinking may help others as well. When people are tempted to drink too much, their main reason for stopping is that “other people were quitting and deciding they’d had enough” ( Johnson, 2002). Treatment Treatment for alcohol dependence begins with sobering up the person and cutting off the supply. This phase is referred to as detoxification (literally, “to remove poison”). It frequently produces all the symptoms of drug withdrawal and can be extremely unpleasant. The next step is to try to restore the person’s health. Heavy abuse of alcohol usually causes severe damage to body organs and the nervous system. After alcoholics have “dried out” and some degree of health has been restored, they may be treated with tranquilizers, antidepressants, or psychotherapy. Unfortunately, the success of these procedures has been limited. One mutual-help approach that has been fairly successful is Alcoholics Anonymous (AA). AA takes a spiritual approach while acting on the premise that it takes a former alcoholic to understand and help a current alcoholic. Participants at AA meetings admit that they have a problem, share feelings, and resolve to stay “dry” one day at a time. Other group members provide support for those struggling to end dependency. (Other “12-step” programs, such as Cocaine Anonymous and Narcotics Anonymous use the same approach.) Detoxification In the treatment of alcoholism, the withdrawal of the patient from alcohol. 210 CHAPTER 6 Eighty percent of those who remain in AA for more than 1 year get through the following year without a drink. However, AA’s success rate may simply reflect the fact that members join voluntarily, meaning they have admitted they have a serious problem (Morgenstern et al., 1997). Sadly, it seems that alcohol abusers will often not face their problems until they have “hit rock bottom.” If they are willing, though, AA presents a practical approach to the problem (Vaillant, 2005). Other groups offer a rational, nonspiritual approach to alcohol abuse that better fits the needs of some people. Examples include Rational Recovery and Secular Organizations for Sobriety (SOS). Other alternatives to AA include medical treatment, group therapy, and individual psychotherapy (Buddie, 2004). There is a strong tendency for abusive drinkers to deny they have a problem. The sooner they seek help, the better. Hallucinogens — Tripping the Light Fantastic Marijuana is the most popular illicit drug in America (SAMHSA, 2005). The main active chemical in marijuana is tetrahydrocannabinol (tet-rah-hydro-cah-NAB-ih-nol), or THC for short. THC is a mild hallucinogen (hal-LU-sin-oh-jin: a substance that alters sensory impressions). Other hallucinogenic drugs include LSD and PCP. LSD and PCP The drug LSD (lysergic acid diethylamide, or “acid”) is perhaps the best-known hallucinogen. Even when taken in tiny amounts, LSD can produce hallucinations and psychotic-like disturbances in thinking and perception. Two other common hallucinogens are mescaline (peyote) and psilocybin (“magic mushrooms,” or “shrooms”). Incidentally, the drug PCP (phencyclidine, or “angel dust”) can have hallucinogenic effects. However, PCP, which is an anesthetic, also has stimulant and depressant effects. This potent combination can cause extreme agitation, disorientation, violence — and too often, tragedy. All the hallucinogens, including marijuana, typically affect neurotransmitter systems that carry messages between brain cells ( Julien, 2005). Marijuana Marijuana and hashish are derived from the hemp plant Cannabis sativa. Marijuana (“pot,” “grass,” “Ganja,” “MJ”) consists of the dried leaves and flowers of the hemp plant. Hashish is a resinous material scraped from Cannabis leaves. Marijuana’s psychological effects include a sense of euphoria or well-being, relaxation, altered time sense, and perceptual distortions. At high dosages, however, paranoia, hallucinations, and delusions can occur (Ksir, Hart, & Ray, 2006). All considered, marijuana intoxication is relatively subtle by comparison to drugs such as LSD or alcohol. Despite this, driving a car while high on marijuana can be extremely hazardous. As a matter of fact, driving under the influence of any intoxicating drug is dangerous. No overdose deaths from marijuana have been reported. However, marijuana cannot be considered harmless. Particularly worrisome is the fact that THC accumulates in the body’s fatty tissues, especially in the brain and reproductive organs. Even if a person smokes marijuana just once a week, the body is never entirely free of THC. Scientists have located a specific receptor site on the surface of brain cells where THC binds to produce its effects ( Figure 6.14). • Figure 6.14 This thin slice of a rat’s brain has been washed with a radioactive •THC-like drug. Yellowish areas show where the brain is rich in THC receptors. In addi- Artists have tried at times to capture the effects of hallucinogens. Here, the artist depicts visual experiences he had while under the influence of LSD. tion to the cortex, or outer layer of the brain, THC receptors are found in abundance in areas involved in the control of coordinated movement. Naturally occurring chemicals similar to THC may help the brain cope with pain and stress. However, when THC is used as a drug, high doses can cause paranoia, hallucinations, and dizziness (Julien, 2005). (From “Marijuana and the Brain,” Science News, vol. 143.) States of Consciousness 211 Long-Term Health Risks These receptor sites are found in large numbers in the cerebral cortex, which is the seat of human consciousness (Julien, 2005). In addition, THC receptors are found in areas involved in the control of skilled movement. Naturally occurring chemicals similar to THC may help the brain cope with pain and stress. However, when THC is used as a drug, high dosages can cause paranoia, hallucinations, and dizziness. Marijuana’s long-term effects include the following health risks: 1. Marijuana smoke contains 50 percent more cancer-causing hydrocarbons and 16 times more tar than tobacco smoke does. Thus, smoking several joints a week may be the equivalent of smoking a dozen cigarettes a day. In regular users, marijuana increases the risk of a variety of cancers, including prostate and cervical cancer (Hashibe, 2005). 2. Marijuana temporarily lowers sperm production in males, and users produce more abnormal sperm. This could be a problem for a man who is marginally fertile and wants to have a family (Schuel et al., 1999). 3. In experiments with female monkeys, THC causes abnormal menstrual cycles and disrupts ovulation. Other animal studies show that THC causes a higher rate of miscarriages and that it can reach the developing fetus. As is true for so many other drugs, it appears that marijuana should be avoided during pregnancy. 4. THC can suppress the body’s immune system, increasing the risk of disease. 5. In animals, marijuana causes genetic damage within cells of the body. It is not known to what extent this happens in humans, but it does suggest that marijuana can be detrimental to health. 6. Activity levels in the cerebellum are lower than normal in marijuana abusers. This may explain why chronic marijuana users tend to show some loss of coordination (Volkow et al., 1996). 7. There is some evidence that THC damages parts of the brain important for memory (Chan et al., 1998). 8. Children whose mothers smoked marijuana during pregnancy show lowered ability to succeed in challenging, goal-oriented activities (Fried & Smith, 2001; Noland et al., 2005). Dangers of Marijuana Use Does marijuana produce physical dependence? Yes, according to recent studies (Lichtman & Martin, 2006). Frequent users of marijuana find it very difficult to quit, so dependence is a risk (Budney & Hughes, 2006). But marijuana’s potential for abuse lies primarily in the realm of psychological dependence, not physical addiction. For about a day after a person smokes marijuana, his or her attention, coordination, and short-term memory are impaired (Pope, Gruber, & Yurgelun-Todd, 1995). Frequent marijuana users show small declines in learning, memory, attention, and thinking abilities (Solowij et al., 2002). When surveyed at age 29, nonusers are healthier, earn more, and are more satisfied with their lives than people who smoke marijuana regularly (Ellickson, Martino, & Collins, 2004). People who smoke five or more joints a week score 4 points lower on IQ tests. This is enough to dull their learning capacity. In fact, many people who have stopped using marijuana say they quit because they were bothered by short-term memory loss and concentration problems. Fortunately, IQ scores and other cognitive measures rebound in about a month after a person quits using marijuana (Grant et al., 2001). In other words, people who smoke dope may act like dopes, but if they quit, there’s a good chance they will regain their mental abilities. Although much is still unknown, marijuana appears to pose a wide range of health risks. Only future research will tell for sure “what’s in the pot.” Courtesy of Dr. Lester Grinspoon/Harvard Medical School A Look Ahead An outdated antimarijuana poster demonstrates the kind of misinformation that has long been attached to this drug. Research has finally begun to sort out what risks are associated with use of marijuana. Of the many states of consciousness we have discussed, dreaming remains one of the most familiar — and the most surprising. Are there lessons to be learned from dreams? What personal insights lie hidden in the ebb and flow of your dream images? Let’s find out in the upcoming Psychology in Action section. Hallucinogen A substance that alters or distorts sensory impressions. CHAPTER 6 K NOW LED G E B U I L D E R Psychoactive Drugs RECITE 1. Addictive drugs stimulate the brain’s reward circuitry by affecting a. neurotransmitters b. alpha waves c. tryptophan levels d. delta spindles 2. Which of the drugs listed below are known to cause a physical dependence? a. heroin b. morphine c. codeine d. methadone e. barbiturates f. alcohol g. marijuana h. amphetamines i. nicotine j. cocaine k. GHB 3. Amphetamine psychosis is similar to extreme __________________, in which the individual feels threatened and suffers from delusions. 4. Cocaine is very similar to which of the following in its effects on the central nervous system? a. Seconal b. codeine c. Cannabis d. amphetamine 5. Drug interaction is a special danger when a person combines a. marijuana and amphetamine b. barbiturates and alcohol c. alcohol and cocaine d. marijuana and THC 6. College students may overdrink as they try to keep up with how much they falsely imagine that their peers drink. T or F? 7. Treatment for alcohol dependence begins with sobering up the person and cutting off the supply. This is referred to as a. “hitting bottom” b. the crucial phase c. detoxification d. clinical anhedonia REFLECT Critical Thinking 8. The U.S. government, which helps fund antismoking campaigns and smoking-related health research, also continues to subsidize tobacco growers. Can you explain this contradiction? 9. Why do you think there is such a contrast between the laws regulating marijuana and those regulating alcohol and tobacco? Relate What legal drugs did you use in the last year? Did any have psychoactive properties? How do psychoactive drugs differ from other substances in their potential for abuse? Answers: 1. a 2. All of them do 3. paranoia 4. d 5. b 6. T 7. c 8. Neither can we. 9. Drug laws in Western societies reflect cultural values and historical patterns of use. Inconsistencies in the law often cannot be justified on the basis of pharmacology, health risks, or abuse potential. 212 PSY C HO L O GY IN A CT ION Exploring and Using Dreams Gateway Question: How can dreams be used to promote personal understanding? No matter what theory of dreaming we favor, dreams can be thought of as a message from yourself to yourself. Thus, the way to understand dreams is to remember them, write them down, look for the messages they contain, and become deeply acquainted with your own symbol system. Here’s how: How to Catch a Dream 1. Before going to sleep, plan to remember your dreams. Keep a pen and paper or a recorder beside your bed. 2. If possible, arrange to awaken gradually without an alarm. Natural awakening almost always follows soon after a REM period. 3. If you rarely remember your dreams, you may want to set an alarm clock to go off an hour before you usually awaken. 4. 5. 6. 7. Although less desirable than awakening naturally, this may let you catch a dream. Upon awakening, lie still and review the dream images with your eyes closed. Try to recall as many details as possible. If you can, make your first dream record (whether by writing or by recording) with your eyes closed. Opening your eyes will disrupt dream recall. Review the dream again and record as many additional details as you can remember. Dream memories disappear quickly. Be sure to describe feelings as well as the plot, characters, and actions of the dream. Put your dreams into a permanent dream diary. Keep dreams in chronological order and review them periodically. This procedure will reveal recurrent themes, conflicts, and emo- tions. It almost always produces valuable insights. 8. Remember, a number of drugs suppress dreaming by interfering with REM sleep ( Table 6.6). • Dream Work At one time or another almost everyone has had a dream that seemed to have deep meaning (Rock, 2004). Exploring everyday dream life can be a source of personal enrichment and personal growth (Cartwright & Lamberg, 1992). What strategies do psychologists use to interpret dreams? Let’s start with Sigmund Freud’s pioneering approach. To unlock dreams, Freud identified four dream processes, or mental filters, that disguise the meanings of dreams. The first is condensation, in which several people, objects, or events are combined into a single dream image. A dream character that looks States of Consciousness • Table 6.6 Effects of Selected Drugs on Dreaming Drug Effect on REM Sleep Alcohol Decrease Amphetamines Decrease Barbiturates Decrease Caffeine None Cocaine Decrease Ecstasy Decrease (by interrupting sleep) LSD Slight increase Marijuana Slight decrease or no effect Opiates Decrease Valium Decrease Courtesy of Maryanne Mott like a teacher, acts like your father, talks like your mother, and is dressed like your employer might be a condensation of authority figures in your life. Displacement is a second way of disguising dream content. Displacement may cause important emotions or actions of a dream to be redirected toward safe or seemingly unimportant images. Thus, a student angry at his parents might dream of accidentally wrecking their car instead of directly attacking them. How would you try to find the meaning of this dream? A traditional approach is to look for symbolic messages, as well as literal meanings. If you find yourself wearing a mask in a dream, for instance, it could relate to important roles that you play at school, work, or home. It could also mean that you want to hide or that you are looking forward to a costume party. However, to accurately interpret a dream, it is important to learn your own “vocabulary” of dream images and meanings. Keeping a dream diary is the first step toward gaining valuable insights. A third dream process is symbolization. As mentioned earlier, Freud believed that dreams are often expressed in images that are symbolic rather than literal. That’s why it helps to ask what feelings or ideas a dream image might symbolize. Let’s say, for example, that a student dreams of coming to class naked. A literal interpretation would be that the student is an exhibitionist. A more likely symbolic meaning is that the student feels vulnerable or unprepared in the class. Secondary elaboration is the fourth method by which dream meanings are disguised. Secondary elaboration is the tendency to make a dream more logical and to add details when remembering it. The fresher a dream memory is, the more useful it is likely to be. Looking for condensation, displacement, symbolization, and secondary elaboration may help you unlock your dreams. But there are other techniques that may be more effective. Dream theorist Calvin Hall (1974) preferred to think of dreams as plays and the dreamer as a playwright. Although Hall points out that dream images and ideas tend to be more primitive than waking thoughts, much can be learned by simply considering the setting, cast of characters, plot, and emotions portrayed in a dream. Another dream theorist, Rosalind Cartwright, suggests that dreams are primarily “feeling statements.” According to her, the overall emotional tone (underlying mood) of a dream is a major clue to its meaning (Cartwright & Lamberg, 1992). Is the dream comical, threatening, joyous, or depressing? Were you lonely, jealous, frightened, in love, or angry? Because each dream has several possible meanings or levels of meaning, there is no fixed way to work with it. Telling the dream to others and discussing its meaning can be a good start. Describing it may help you relive some of the feelings in the dream. Also, family members or friends may be able to offer interpretations to which you would be blind. Watch for verbal or visual puns and other playful elements in dreams. For example, if you dream that you are in a wrestling match and your arm is pinned behind your back, it may mean that you feel someone is “twisting your arm” in real life. The meaning of most dreams will yield to a little detective work. Try asking a series of 213 questions about dreams you would like to understand: Probing Dreams 1. Who was in the dream? Were there humans, animals, or mythical characters? Do you recognize any of the characters? 2. What social interactions were taking place? Were those interactions friendly? Aggressive? Sexual? 3. What activities were taking place? Were they physical activities or not? 4. Was there striving? Was the striving successful or not? 5. Was the dream about good fortune or misfortune? 6. What emotions were present in the dream? Was there anger, apprehension, confusion, happiness, or sadness? 7. What were the physical surroundings like? What was the setting? Were there any physical objects present? (Adapted from the Hall-Van de Castle system of dream content analysis; Domhoff, 2003.) If you still have trouble seeing the meaning of a dream, you may find it helpful to use a technique developed by Fritz Perls. Perls, the originator of Gestalt therapy, considered most dreams a special message about what’s missing in our lives, what we avoid doing, or feelings that need to be “re-owned.” Perls believed that dreams are a way of filling in gaps in personal experience (Perls, 1969). An approach that Perls found helpful is to “take the part of ” or “speak for” each of the characters and objects in the dream. In other words, if you dream about a strange man standing behind a doorway, you would speak aloud to the man, then answer for him. To use Perls’ method, you would even speak for the door, perhaps saying something like, “I am a barrier. I keep you safe, but I also keep you locked Dream processes Mental filters that hide the true meanings of dreams. Condensation Combining several people, objects, or events into a single dream image. Displacement Directing emotions or actions toward safe or unimportant dream images. Symbolization The nonliteral expression of dream content. Secondary elaboration Making a dream more logical and complete while remembering it. 214 CHAPTER 6 inside. The stranger has something to tell you. You must risk opening me to learn it.” A particularly interesting dream exercise is to continue a dream as waking fantasy so that it may be concluded or carried on to a more meaningful ending. As the world of dreams and your personal dream language become more familiar, you will doubtless find many answers, paradoxes, intuitions, and insights into your own behavior. Using Your Dreams It is possible to learn to use dreams for our own purposes. For example, as mentioned previously, nightmare sufferers can use imagery rehearsal to modify their own nightmares (Germain et al., 2004; Krakow & Zadra, 2006). Similarly, it is possible to use your dreams to enhance creativity (Stickgold & Walker, 2004). Dreams and Creativity History is full of cases where dreams have been a pathway to creativity and discovery. A striking example is provided by Dr. Otto Loewi, a pharmacologist and winner of a Nobel Prize. Loewi had spent years studying the chemical transmission of nerve impulses. A tremendous breakthrough in his research came when he dreamed of an experiment 3 nights in a row. The first 2 nights he woke up and scribbled the experiment on a pad. But the next morning, he couldn’t tell what the notes meant. On the third night, he got up after having the dream. This time, instead of making notes he went straight to his laboratory and performed the crucial experiment. Loewi later said that if the experiment had occurred to him while awake he would have rejected it. Loewi’s experience gives some insight into using dreams to produce creative solutions. Inhibitions are reduced during dreaming, which may be especially useful in solving problems