Chapter 6 Vision General Principles of Perception • Each of our senses has specialized receptors that are sensitive to a particular kind of energy • Receptors for vision are sensitive to light • Receptors “transduce” (convert) energy into electrochemical patterns so that the brain can perceive sights, sounds, smells, etc. General Principles of Perception (cont’d.) • Law of specific nerve energies states that activity by a particular nerve always conveys the same type of information to the brain – Example: impulses in one neuron indicate light; impulses in another neuron indicate sound • Which neurons respond, the amount of response, and the timing of response influence what we perceive The Eye and Its Connections to the Brain • Light enters the eye through an opening in the center of the iris called the pupil • Light is focused by the lens and the cornea onto the rear surface of the eye known as the retina – The retina is lined with visual receptors • Light from the left side of the world strikes the right side of the retina and vice versa The Eye and Its Connections to the Brain (cont’d.) • Visual receptors send messages to neurons called bipolar cells, located closer to the center of the eye • Bipolar cells send messages to ganglion cells that are even closer to the center of the eye – The axons of ganglion cells join one another to form the optic nerve that travels to the brain The Eye and Its Connections to the Brain (cont’d.) • Amacrine cells are additional cells that receive information from bipolar cells and send it to other bipolar, ganglion, or amacrine cells • Amacrine cells control the ability of the ganglion cells to respond to shapes, movements, or other specific aspects of visual stimuli The Eye and Its Connections to the Brain (cont’d.) • The optic nerve consists of the axons of ganglion cells that band together and exit through the back of the eye and travel to the brain • The point at which the optic nerve leaves the back of the eye is called the blind spot because it contains no receptors The Eye and Its Connections to the Brain (cont’d.) • The central portion of the retina is the fovea and allows for acute and detailed vision – Packed tight with receptors – Nearly free of ganglion axons and blood vessels The Eye and Its Connections to the Brain (cont’d.) • Each receptor in the fovea attaches to a single bipolar cell and a single ganglion cell known as a midget ganglion cell • Each cone in the fovea has a direct line to the brain which allows the registering of the exact location of input • Our vision is dominated by what we see in the fovea The Eye and Its Connections to the Brain (cont’d.) • In the periphery of the retina, a greater number of receptors converge into ganglion and bipolar cells – Detailed vision is less in peripheral vision – Allows for the greater perception of much fainter light in peripheral vision The Eye and Its Connections to the Brain • The arrangement of visual receptors in the eye is highly adaptive – Example: predatory birds have a greater density of receptors on the top of the eye; rats have a greater density on the bottom of the eye Visual Receptors: Rods and Cones • The vertebrate retina consists of two kinds of receptors: – Rods: most abundant in the periphery of the eye and respond to faint light (120 million per retina) – Cones: most abundant in and around the fovea (6 million per retina) • Essential for color vision & more useful in bright light Visual Receptors: Rods and Cones (cont’d.) • Though cones are outnumbered, they provide about 90% of the brain’s input • The average number of axons in the optic nerve is one million, but some people may have two or three times as many • Heightened visual responses are important in many activities – Example: top tennis, squash, and fencing athletes show faster brain responses to visual stimuli Visual Receptors: Rods and Cones (cont’d.) • Photopigments: chemicals contained by both rods and cones that release energy when struck by light – Consist of 11-cis-retinal bound to proteins called opsins • Light energy converts 11-cis-retinal quickly into all-trans-retinal • Light is thus absorbed and energy is released that activates second messengers within the cell Color Vision • The perception of color is dependent upon the wavelength of the light • “Visible” wavelengths are dependent upon the species’ receptors • The shortest wavelength humans can perceive is 400 nanometers (violet) • The longest wavelength that humans can perceive is 700 nanometers (red) Color Vision (cont’d.) • Discrimination among colors depend upon the combination of responses by different neurons • Two major interpretations of color vision include the following: – Trichromatic theory/Young-Helmholtz theory – Opponent-process theory Color Vision (cont’d.) • Trichromatic theory: color perception occurs through the relative rates of response by three kinds of cones – Short wavelength, medium-wavelength, long-wavelength Color Vision (cont’d.) • Trichromatic theory explained: – Each cone responds to a broad range of wavelengths, but some more than others – The ratio of activity across the three types of cones determines the color – More intense light increases the brightness of the color but does not change the ratio • Incomplete theory of color vision – Example: negative color afterimage Color Vision (cont’d.) • The opponent-process theory suggests that we perceive color in terms of paired opposites – The brain has a mechanism that perceives color on a continuum from red to green and another from yellow to blue – A possible mechanism for the theory is that bipolar cells are excited by one set of wavelengths and inhibited by another Color Vision (cont’d.) • Both the opponent-process and trichromatic theory have limitations • Color constancy, the ability to recognize color despite changes in lighting, is not easily explained by these theories • Retinex theory suggests the cortex compares information from various parts of the retina to determine the brightness and color for each area – Better explains color and brightness constancy Color Vision (cont’d.) • Color vision deficiency is an impairment in perceiving color differences • Gene responsible is contained on the X chromosome (~8% of men & <1% of women) • Caused by either the lack of a type of cone or a cone has abnormal properties • Most common form is difficulty distinguishing between red and green – Results from the long- and mediumwavelength cones having the same photopigment An Overview of the Mammalian Visual System • Rods and cones of the retina make synaptic contact with horizontal cells and bipolar cells • Horizontal cells are cells in the eye that make inhibitory contact onto bipolar cells • Bipolar cells make synapses onto amacrine cells and ganglion cells • The different cells are specialized for different visual functions An Overview of the Mammalian Visual System (cont’d.) • Ganglion cell axons form the optic nerve • The optic chiasm is the place where the two optic nerves leaving the eye meet • In humans, half of the axons from each eye cross to the other side of the brain • Most ganglion cell axons go to the lateral geniculate nucleus, a smaller amount to the superior colliculus, and fewer to other areas The Neural Basis of Visual Perception • The lateral geniculate nucleus is part of the thalamus specialized for visual perception – Destination for most ganglion cell axons – Sends axons to other parts of the thalamus and to the visual areas of the occipital cortex – Cortex and thalamus feed information back and forth to each other Processing in the Retina • Lateral inhibition is the reduction of activity in one neuron by activity in neighboring neurons • The response of cells in the visual system depends upon the net result of excitatory and inhibitory messages it receives • Lateral inhibition is the retina’s way responsible of sharpening contrasts to emphasize the borders of objects Further Processing • The receptive field refers to the part of the visual field that either excites or inhibits a cell in the visual system of the brain • For a receptor, the receptive field is the point in space from which light strikes it • For other visual cells, receptive fields are derived from the visual field of cells that either excite or inhibit – Example: ganglion cells converge to form the receptive field of the next level of cells Further Processing (cont’d.) • Ganglion cells of primates generally fall into three categories: – Parvocellular neurons – Magnocellular neurons – Koniocellular neurons Further Processing (cont’d.) • Parvocellular neurons: – Mostly located in or near the fovea – Have smaller cell bodies and small receptive fields – Are highly sensitive to detect color and visual detail Further Processing (cont’d.) • Magnocellular neurons: – Are distributed evenly throughout the retina – Have larger cell bodies and visual fields – Are highly sensitive to large overall pattern and moving stimuli Further Processing (cont’d.) • Koniocellular neurons: – Have small cell bodies – Are found throughout the retina – Have several functions, and their axons terminate in many different places Further Processing (cont’d.) • Cells of the lateral geniculate have a receptive field similar to those of ganglion cells: – An excitatory or inhibitory central portion and a surrounding ring of the opposite effect The Primary Visual Cortex • Pattern recognition in the cerebral cortex occurs in a few places • The primary visual cortex (area V1) receives information from the lateral geniculate nucleus and is the area responsible for the first stage of visual processing • Some people with damage to V1 show blindsight: an ability to respond to visual stimuli that they report not seeing The Primary Visual Cortex (cont’d.) • Hubel and Weisel (1959, 1998) distinguished various types of cells in the visual cortex: – Simple cells – Complex cells – End-stopped/hypercomplex cells The Primary Visual Cortex (cont’d.) • Simple cells: – Fixed excitatory and inhibitory zones – The more light that shines in the excitatory zone, the more the cell responds – The more in the inhibitory zone, the less the cell responds – Bar-shaped or edge-shaped receptive fields with vertical and horizontal orientations outnumbering diagonal ones The Primary Visual Cortex (cont’d.) • Complex cells: – Located in either V1or V2 – Have large receptive field that can not be mapped into fixed excitatory or inhibitory zones – Responds to a pattern of light in a particular orientation and most strongly to a moving stimulus The Primary Visual Cortex (cont’d.) • End-stopped or hypercomplex cells: – Are similar to complex cells but with a strong inhibitory area at one end of its bar shaped receptive field – Respond to a bar-shaped pattern of light anywhere in its large receptive field, provided the bar does not extend beyond a certain point The Primary Visual Cortex (cont’d.) • In the visual cortex, cells are grouped together in columns perpendicular to the surface • Cells within a given column process similar information – Respond either mostly to the right or left eye, or respond to both eyes equally – Do not consistently fire at the same time The Primary Visual Cortex (cont’d.) • Cells in the visual cortex may be feature detectors, neurons whose response indicate the presence of a particular feature/ stimuli • Prolonged exposure to a given visual feature decreases sensitivity to that feature Development of the Visual Cortex • Animal studies have greatly contributed to the understanding of the development of vision • Early lack of stimulation of one eye: leads to synapses in the visual cortex becoming gradually unresponsive to input from that eye • Early lack of stimulation of both eyes: cortical responses become sluggish but do not cause blindness Development of the Visual Cortex (cont’d.) • Sensitive/critical periods are periods of time during the lifespan when experiences have a particularly strong and enduring effect • Critical period ends with the onset of chemicals that inhibit axonal sprouting • Changes that occur during critical period require both excitation and inhibition of some neurons • Cortical plasticity is greatest in early life, but never ends Development of the Visual Cortex (cont’d.) • Stereoscopic depth perception is a method of perceiving distance in which the brain compares slightly different inputs from the two eyes • Relies on retinal disparity or the discrepancy between what the left and the right eye sees • The ability of cortical neurons to adjust their connections to detect retinal disparity is shaped through experience Development of the Visual Cortex (cont’d.) • Strabismus is a condition in which the eyes do not point in the same direction – Usually develops in childhood – Also known as “lazy eye” • If two eyes carry unrelated messages, cortical cell strengthens connections with only one eye • Development of stereoscopic depth perception is impaired Development of the Visual Cortex (cont’d.) • Early exposure to a limited array of patterns leads to nearly all of the visual cortex cells becoming responsive to only that pattern • Astigmatism refers to a blurring of vision for lines in one direction caused by an asymmetric curvature of the eyes – 70% of infants have astigmatism Development of the Visual Cortex (cont’d.) • Study of people born with cataracts but had them removed at age 7 or 12 indicate that vision can be restored gradually, but problems persist: – Difficulty in recognizing objects – Unable to tell that components are part of a whole – Best prognosis is for children whose vision problems are corrected early in life The “What” and “Where” Paths • The secondary visual cortex (area V2) receives information from area V1, processes information further, and sends it to other areas • Information is transferred between area V1 and V2 in a reciprocal nature The “What” and “Where” Paths (cont’d.) • The ventral stream refers to the path that goes through temporal cortex; the “what” path – Specialized for identifying and recognizing objects • The dorsal stream refers to the visual path in the parietal cortex; the “where” path – Helps the motor system to find objects and move towards them The “What” and “Where” Paths (cont’d.) • The two streams communicate – Each participates in identifying what and where an object is • Damaging either stream will produce different deficits – Ventral stream damage: can see where objects are but cannot identify them – Dorsal stream damage: can identify objects but not know where they are Detailed Analysis of Shape • Receptive fields become larger and more specialized as visual information goes from simple cells to later areas of visual processing • The inferior temporal cortex contains cells that respond selectively to complex shapes but are insensitive to distinctions that are critical to other cells • Cells in this cortex respond to identifiable objects Detailed Analysis of Shape (cont’d.) • Shape constancy is the ability to recognize an object’s shape despite changes in direction or size • The inferior temporal neuron’s ability to ignore changes in size and direction contributes to our capacity for shape constancy • Damage to the pattern pathways of the cortex can lead to deficits in object recognition Detailed Analysis of Shape (cont’d.) • Visual agnosia is the inability to recognize objects despite satisfactory vision – Caused by damage to the pattern pathway usually in the temporal cortex Detailed Analysis of Shape (cont’d.) • Face recognition occurs relatively soon after birth • People with cataracts removed at 2-6 months develop nearly normal vision but have slight difficulties in distinguishing faces • Newborns show strong preference for a rightside-up face and support idea of a built-in face recognition system • Facial recognition continues to develop gradually into adolescence Detailed Analysis of Shape (cont’d.) • Prosopagnosia is the inability to recognize faces – Occurs after damage to the fusiform gyrus of the inferior temporal cortex – The fusiform gyrus responds much more strongly to faces than anything else Color Perception • Color perception depends on both the light reflected on an object and how it compares with objects around it – Area V4 may be responsible for color constancy and visual attention – Color constancy: the ability to recognize something as being the same color despite changes in lighting Motion Perception • Motion perception involves a variety of brain areas in all four lobes of the cerebral cortex • The middle-temporal cortex (MT/V5) responds to a stimulus moving in a particular direction • Cells in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal cortex (MST) respond to expansion, contraction or rotation of a visual stimulus • Both receive input from the magnocellular path; color-insensitive Motion Perception (cont’d.) • Motion blindness refers to the inability to determine the direction, speed and whether objects are moving – Likely caused by damage in area MT • Some people are blind except for the ability to detect which direction something is moving – Area MT probably gets some visual input despite significant damage to area V1 Motion Perception (cont’d.) • Several mechanisms prevent confusion or blurring of images during eye movements – Saccades are a decrease in the activity of the visual cortex during quick eye movements – Neural activity and blood flow decrease 75 ms before and during eye movements