Journal of Positive School Psychology 2022, Vol. 6, No. 2, 28 – 39 http://journalppw.com A REVIEW ON PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL BEING OF COUPLES DURING FIRST SEVEN YEARS OF MARITAL LIFE S. Arul Prabhahar1, Dr. G. Ramanathan2, H. Kalaivani3, Dr. G. Raja Jerald Selvakumar4 1 Ph.D. Research Scholar, Department of Psychology, Annamalai University, Annamalainagar, Tamilnadu. India. ORCHID ID: 0000-0001-9766-3633 2 Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, Government Arts and Science College (Autonomous), Coimbatore. 3 M.Sc Nursing Tutor, Bhubaneswar College of Nursing, All India Institute of Medical Science (AIIMS). 4 Assistant Professor, Dep. of Political Science, Hindustan Institute of Technology and Science, Padur, Chennai. Abstract Health is influenced by marital status, and this relationship varies by time and/or place. Married people live longer and are healthier than those who have never married, divorced, or been widowed. However, not every wedding are created equally: unhappy weddings have fewer advantages than happy marriages. Divorce rates have risen sharply over the globe. Despite the widespread belief that weddings improve mental health, there is no proof that the advantages of wedding are due to the social function of marriage rather than the qualities of those who marry. This review uses longitudinal analysis to compare those who were "very happy" in marriage to those who were "pretty happy" in marriage, "not too happy" in marriage, never married, divorced or separated, or widowed, using a nationally representative sample of married couples and joining measures of marital status and marital happiness. Not just lifespan but also well-being is key outcome factors when analyzing the connection among the marital status and well-being. The goal of this research is to look at the links between marital status, health behaviors, and illness in various couples, as well as see whether there are any gender disparities in those interactions. We evaluate whether marriage improves mental health after adjusting for premarital rates of disorder using a sample of 18, 21, and 24 year aged male and female who either stayed unmarried or got married or remained married in 7 year duration. In addition, we investigated whether women gain more from weddings than men in terms of mental health. Keywords: Psychological well-being, marital happiness, marital status, mental health, longitudinal analysis. I. INTRODUCTION People's marital status has a major influence on their wellbeing and lifespan. Unmarried people's psychological health leads to be poorer than that of their married individuals, according to a series of researches. Many factors assist in the greater well-being of married couples. Individuals who are stronger more likely to be married, whereas those who are less nutritious are much more likely to get divorced or parted from their spouses. Marriage also has a protective influence on one's health and wellbeing, as married individuals benefit from greater financial security, social support, and psychological security. Stressful events, such as marriage ending, are known to influence wellness. People who have been separated or have been bereaved may have changes in their immune, hormonal, and neurological systems as a result of marital dissolution [1]. Mental issues are closely linked to one's family situation, including one's marital status and whether or not one has children. Getting married or living with a partner has been shown to improve satisfaction with life and to be connected with improved psychological health, fewer depression symptoms, and a greater level of well-being. [2]. 29 Journal of Positive School Psychology It has become more common for people to pay attention to and be careful of what is going on in the here and now [3]. Trait mindfulness is linked to numerous signs of mental and interpersonal health [4],[5] although quantifying feature mindfulness without taking into consideration the environment in which one's degree of attentiveness is expressed may be difficult. No matter how attentive a person is, he or she can't be thoughtful all the moment or be aware in every situation. Even more importantly, being aware in some settings may have a greater effect on one's psychological and interpersonal wellbeing than one's average propensity to be attentive; hence, one's common inclination to be mindful and may not always be clinically meaningful. though the link between the two has not been confirmed. When it comes to marriage, those who are stronger are more likely to get married and continue to be married [9]. Married people enjoy superior SRH (self-rated health) [10] and lower death rates [11] as a consequence of their marital status. Healthy habits are promoted; dangerous activities are regulated and more people have access to health insurance, and social assistance and relationships are strengthened [12]. The "buffering hypothesis," which says that people with good social assistance are better able to deal with stress and hence mitigate its well-being repercussions, is a fundamental part of studies on the health and lifespan advantages of marriage [13]. Mindfulness may have an essential influence on a love relationship's mental and relational health. For instance, being attentive while waiting in line at a grocery shop is quite different than being thoughtful when talking to coworkers or standing in a queue at the grocery store. It is rare for a relationship to be both a source of pleasure and misery since it involves a higher sense of interaction and connection than that of other social circumstances. Relational awareness refers to the inclination to be attentive in the setting of love relationships [6]. Partnership mindfulness seems to have a larger positive link with good relationship quality than trait mindfulness does, as well as a higher negative connection with bad relationship qualities, anxious connection, and avoidant bonding, according to the studies. III. MARITAL HEALTH Mental health is measured and protected in a collective community. Up to the majority of research was done in primarily the individual civilizations and to our understanding; this study is notable in that it is the first of its sort to have explored well-analyzed occurrences in the newly untouched terrain. The spillover impact of work-family disputes is shown to be varied in countries with a good history of marital adjustment [7], such as the cultures that should be recognized because of family commitments, where employees are more likely to feel weary and far less able to deal with pressure [8]. II. MARITAL STATUS AND HEALTH Various researches have shown that married persons had greater health than single ones, even HAPPINESS AND Based on marital functionality, marriage may have a positive or negative impact on wellness [14]. Improved physical wellness [15], improved SRH (self-rated health) [16], and fewer physical illnesses are all linked to good marital quality [17]. They discovered that patients who had coronary artery bypass transplantation had reduced death rates when their marriages were in better condition. Marriages of high quality, on the other hand, may help to alleviate the effects of stress on the body, while those of low quality may cause more stress on an ongoing basis. Social stress, a phrase used by [17], is a word used to describe the physiological and mental effects of difficult social communication. Marital stress is linked to a worse prognosis for women with coronary heart disease [18] and poor spouse conduct, such as criticism or hostility, is connected with worse wellness [16]. Wound healing was hindered in an experimental context because of negative marital relations [19]. Mental state may be impacted by a couple's marital quality, as per research. For example, [20] found that those who had a bad relationship were more likely to be depressed. People who leave troubled relationships do better than those who stay in them [21] or never join them in the initial place, as per research: people who have never wedded do better psychologically than those who are in miserable relationships [22]. Successful relationships may help alleviate stress, but poor marriages may exacerbate it, according to these new results. Table 1 shows how marital 30 S. Arul Prabhahar et al. status/happiness and total joyfulness, both separately and combined, are linked with the wellbeing and death rates. Every one of those models came with the entire control group. Table 1 Differences in fair/poor well-being and death risk by marital status/happiness and general happiness. 𝐅𝐚𝐢𝐫/𝐏𝐨𝐨𝐫 𝐬𝐞𝐥𝐟 − 𝐫𝐚𝐭𝐞𝐝 𝐡𝐞𝐚𝐥𝐭𝐡𝐛 Model 1 Model 2 𝐌𝐨𝐫𝐭𝐚𝐥𝐢𝐭𝐲 𝐫𝐢𝐬𝐤 𝐛 Model 3 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Marital status/happiness(Very happy marriage) Never married 1.56 *** 1.14 1.14 1.09 Divorced/separated 1.84*** 1.26** 1.16* 1.11 Widowed 1.24* 0.89+ 1.17* 1.10 Pretty happy marriage 1.58*** 1.26+ 0.96 0.99* Not too happy Marriage 2.20*** 1.14 1.40* 1.28+ General happiness(very happy) Pretty happy 1.99*** 1.95*** 1.14** 1.16** Not happy 4.62*** 4.35*** 1.24*** 1.22** IV. THE ROLE HAPPINESS OF GENERAL Relationships among pleasure and relationship status, wellness and lifespan may be influenced by general well-being. People who are joyful tend to be stronger and live much longer [23]. It's also important to note that both marital joy and total happiness and life fulfillment [24, 25] are significantly linked to one another. Researchers do not, however, know the degree to which health and lifespan are influenced by relationships among marriage and general satisfaction. The impacts of marital status and marital satisfaction on wellness and lifespan may be explained by a person's overall degree of satisfaction. If satisfaction in a wedding leads to fulfillment in life or the other way around, then the health advantages of joy in a wedding may be attributed to general happiness. When it comes to health, if marital and common joy has unique processes, we may discover that marital and overall joy have discrete, independent consequences. In addition to marital status and marital satisfaction, wellness and lifespan are other key factors that researchers take into account when studying the overall population in the United States. Elements that influence a person's sociodemographic status comprise age, gender, race, and parental status. SES (educational, earnings, and job) and religious affiliation are also taken into consideration. Possible confounding factors include marital contentment, physical health, and life span. Researchers have found significant differences in wellness, death, and marriage rates among geographic areas [26]. In addition to being a proven wellness and death risk factor, SES is also strongly linked to the relationship [27]. Lastly, a higher level of religiosity is linked to better marital satisfaction and a greater chance of getting engaged [28, 29]. V. INTENTION OF LIFE AND WELLBEING OUTCOMES A eudaimonic model of mental well-being was presented that included numerous variables. "The sensations the following conduct in the direction of, and congruent with, one's real potential" [78] is a more exact definition of eudaimonia. A person's eudaimonic viewpoint may be characterized by the degree to which they believe their life is meaningful, intended, 31 and direction. The goal in life is a facet of mental well-being. Mental well-being has a long history of health advantages that have been thoroughly established in the scientific literature. [79] Symptoms of depression are more likely to emerge in those who don't have a good sense of direction in life, according to one study. The greater goal in life also forecasts better emotional rehabilitation from unpleasant stimuli, explaining the impact of stressful life experiences on bad consequences [80]. Sources of stress may be seen as less challenging for people who have a better sense of purpose, and they may use appropriate coping mechanisms to preserve their complete health [81]. Carers who had a better meaningful life were less likely to have physical and emotional challenges while caring for a companion with a functional disability in a recent study of caregivers. Emotional caring problems were also reduced when care receivers had a greater sense of purpose in life [82]. Psychosocial resources provided by partner carers’ and care receivers' self-reported reasons to live seem to be relevant to the well-being and well-being assessment in a dyadic situation. Journal of Positive School Psychology favorably associated with more effective communication actions during couple disputes and adversely associated with perceived and reported emotional negativity during and after partner argument [33]. Trait awareness is likely to affect how individuals in relationships behave and assess the association since it is linked to the inclination to be aware of and pay attention to what is happening in the current moment while engaging with a loving relationship. To be expected, the large majority of studies on the subject of mindful awareness in intimate relationships have relied on assessments of trait mindfulness. However, as previously mentioned, trait mindfulness may vary according to the type, making it a less than an ideal sign of mindfulness in intimate relationships. Research of awareness and loving relationships might gain from incorporating assessments of relationship awareness to gain new ideas and lines of investigation. VI. TRAIT AND RELATIONSHIP MINDFULNESS IN RELATIONAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL OUTCOMES There's indeed reason to believe that relationship awareness may explain changes in the level of good and negative relations in addition to those related to trait mindfulness [34]. A sample of 20year-old college students in a loving relationship was included in this study, and roughly 41% of these individuals stated being in their present loving relationship for less than a year. The quality of a person's relationships may be improved by practicing mindfulness, especially in long-term partnerships [35]. Therefore, it is significant to highlight that the average age of individuals in this research was over 36, that over 3 quarters of the partners in this research indicate being wedded, and that the average partnership duration of these partners was approximately 12 years. Thus, the current research is unusual in that it focuses on a group of couples where awareness may have an even greater influence on their relationship quality. Mindfulness is connected to a wide range of mental traits that anticipate better love relationships. Trait mindfulness, for instance, has a positive correlation with both empathetic concern and the ability to take on new perspectives [30]. Enhanced marital happiness and a decreased likelihood of relationship breakdown have been related to the trait of mindfulness [31, 32]. People's approach to dispute resolution with love partners may be influenced by their innate awareness. Trait mindfulness, for instance, is substantially and The improvement of one's mental health is linked to the practice of mindfulness as a habit. People who have a high level of trait mindfulness are more proficient at identifying and modifying their psychological states to meet their fundamental psychological demands [36]. For example, worrying, obsession, and the hiding of thoughts and feelings are all connected with depression and stress and are inconsistent with awareness [37]. An extensive body of research shows a substantial negative As we become older, we tend to lose our sense of meaning in life [83]. Numerous studies have shown, however, that decreasing wellness is not always linked to advancing years [84]. To better understand how mental well-being in couples may ease the pressure of a problematic marriage relationship, it is necessary to study goals in life in the context of marital partnerships. 32 S. Arul Prabhahar et al. correlation among bad mind functioning, such as depression and stress [38]. Many research on trait awareness concentrate on its advantages outside of the setting of life of the person, such as intimate relationships, despite substantial evidence supporting its positive effects on psychological wellbeing. Earlier research has shown that loving relationships have a distinct impact on psychological functioning [39]. Mindfulness in loving relationships may have a essential effect on psychological health, however, studies that take into account both characteristic and relational awareness may shed light on this question. VII. INTERDEPENDENCY IN MINDFULNESS AND RELATIONAL AND THE PSYCHOLOGICAL OUTCOMES For both the individual and their companion, a person's personality qualities have a role [40]. Because of this, the link between characteristic awareness, mental and relational health probably stretches from one person to other. Interpersonal mechanisms of characteristic awareness have been previously shown in research investigations. When a couple has greater characteristic awareness, they are less likely to withdraw from each other and more likely to assist each other [41]. Couples' feelings of support, reactivity, and overall marital happiness are all positively and substantially correlated with an individual's level of trait mindfulness [42]. In another research, military partners' marital quality and trait awareness were linked [43]. Even though awareness is an internal practice, there is proof that it may be correctly assessed by others [44]. To summarize, there is scientific proof to assist the suggestion that a person's and their partner's mental and relational well-being are linked to their characteristic mindfulness. The very few research employing an interpersonal method is constrained by the lack of context-sensitive assessments of awareness. There are still questions about how being mindful in a loving relationship affects one's own and one's companion's mental and emotional health. VIII. WELL-BEING AND HEALTH INDICATORS MENTAL For instance, an individual feel of well-being might be characterized as the mental balancing point among his or her available resources and problems [45]. The Office for National Statistics in the United Kingdom (ONS) held a public discussion on the issue via several forums to produce national well-being measurements [46]. When asked, "What are the most important aspects of life?" and what is well-being? The most common replies are: Well-being was defined as "Fitness", "Having excellent ties with friends and family" and "Work satisfaction (and financial stability)" [47]. Empirical research has shown that well-being is correlated with socioeconomic level [48]. In our research, researchers were using a wide range of measures to match the health of participants: a simplified direct question about life happiness; the CASP12 multi-item quality of life rate; a direct question about fulfillment with social support networks; and the EURO-D levels of depression rate. We'll go into further depth about each of these three measurements in the sections that follow. Controls in our studies include wellness, education, and financial well-being variables [49, 50]. During the first measure, the expressed Life, researchers are concerned with how individuals feel about the quality of their lives. To get this information, you answer a direct question that asks respondents to scale their level of contentment with life on a range of zero to 10. In terms of reliability and validity, this scale is adequate [51, 52]. The CASP-12, or life quality rating, is the second assessment [53], which is intended to collect life quality in elder years. On a range of 1 (rarely) to 4 (often), participants indicate whether they agree with twelve assertions (never). The 12 questions cover 4 aspects of life: control, autonomy, enjoyment, and selfrealization, yielding an overall index varying from twelve to 48 (poor to the great quality of life). Researchers scale it from Zero to Ten (poor to the great quality of life). The 3rd metric is expressed as Network fulfillment. Respondents rate their contentment with their social network on a range of zero to ten (low to high fulfillment). When respondents stated that they have no one with whom they discuss issues or who is essential to them, they were queried how pleased they were with this reality. 33 Journal of Positive School Psychology The EURO-D depressive rating [54] is the fourth metric. It serves as a predictor of symptoms of depression and incorporates features of delayed psychological health. It is a reliable method of comparing depressed symptoms throughout European nations [55, 56]. Sadness, pessimism, suicidality, guilty, sleepiness, enthusiasm, irritation, hunger, exhaustion, attention, pleasure, and tearfulness all contribute to the EURO-D depression rating. Participants are requested if every one of those parameters has an indication. It yields an aggregate index that ranges from zero (not sad) to twelve (very unhappy) (very depressed). Researchers normalize it to a range of zero (very depressed) to ten (not depressed) and term it devoid of depression symptoms. Figure 1 presented the mean of the health metrics over a year in a marriage partnership is presented. While networking and life fulfillment retain largely consistent as people get older, quality of life and the absence of depressive symptoms indexes diminish at an older age. Except for the absence of a depressed symptoms index, wherein male respondents had on average a 0.73 point higher index than female respondents, the graphs for men and women respondents are very comparable. Figure 1 Average health score and mental well-being measure for seven years IX. ATTACHMENT AVOIDANCE AND THE CONNECTION AMONG TOUCH AND PSYCHOLOGICAL HEALTH Attachment rejection is linked to greater levels of sadness, tension, and somatic symptoms [57], as well as a lesser level of positive everyday impact [58]. These negative correlations also apply to their companions that indicate higher levels of depressive symptoms [59]. A touching feel could help to explain these connections. Attachment rejection could, for instance, moderate the relationship between touching feel and happiness. A person with poor attachment rejection may have a positive relationship with touch, whereas those with strong attachment rejection may have a less favorable or even negative relationship with the contact. The theoretical justification for this forecast comes from the assumption that far more avoidantly connected people attempt to retain self-reliance and individuality since they view others as unreliable and trusted [60]. As a consequence, they seek lesser connectedness in their interactions [61] and are more contact averse [62]. In contrast to more protected and anxiously linked persons, those with greater attachment rejection indicated no elevation in state secure 34 S. Arul Prabhahar et al. attachment in an experimental situation where their love companion was taught to contact them [63]. Increasing evidence suggests, however, that even if they have negative opinions toward proximity, those who are more attachment avoidant can gain from their companions' good activities. They gain greater levels of support from their companion, despite their tendency to reject social help [64]. Furthermore, among avoidantly linked people, pleasant connection interactions with a romantic companion—such as thinking positively about one's spouse or sharing everyday productive thoughts with them—forecast decreased bad effects [65]. Seeing a companion as thankful protects avoidantly connected people from experiencing lower relationship value [66]. Since contact feel may be another kind of positive that might enhance the well-being of avoidantly connected individuals, a second theory is that attachment rejection leads to less prevalent physical contact, which explains why a connective rejection is linked to worse mental well-being (a mediation hypothesis). X. NEGATIVE INTERACTION SYMPTOMS AND MARITAL DEPRESSIVE Adverse marital connection refers to how critical, disappointed, unpleasant, or demanding an individual perceives their companion [67] and is more predictive of health than good parts of the connection [68]. Negative marital connections have been proven to have a negative impact on well-being outcomes, such as decreasing physical and psychological health and raising death risk [68]. Stress and dispute have deleterious consequences on psychological health, as per the marital discord model of depression [69], resulting in increased depressive symptoms among married couples. The majority of the early research was on individual-level evaluations that linked bad marital connections to depressed symptoms [68]. Long-term studies have also discovered that unfavorable marital connections have a cumulative impact on a partner's health over a period [70]. Dyadic techniques to explore the actor and companion impact of poor marital connection on healthcare outcomes, especially depressive symptoms, are becoming more popular [71]. Partnership impacts are connections among the membership of the dyad that is independent of one's own effect [72]. Actor impacts are connections within an individual, whereas companion effects are interactions among members of the dyad that are independent of one's impact. [73] Investigated comparable connections among wedding couples and identified no indication of ones companion marital fulfillment anticipating the other's depression symptoms in one of the early dyadic research. Eventual, [74] found that marital pleasure has cross-spousal impacts on later depressive symptoms in partners in long-term relationships. The concurrent and longitudinal relationships between unfavorable marital connection and depression symptoms have recently been established in research of middleaged and elder married persons. [75] Demonstrated a link between unfavorable marital connections and depressed indicators in individuals and couples. Furthermore, [76] discovered that individual and spousal views of a bad marital relationship were linked to an increase in depressive symptoms. The established cross-spousal impacts may be due to couples' sensitivity to everyone's sentiments and activities. As a consequence, dyadic evaluations of a marital connection play a significant role in impacting psychological health, especially with lengthy connections. Figure 2 shows the use of dyadic actor partner interdependence methods to model the actor and partner impacts of negative marital interaction on depressive symptoms. 35 Journal of Positive School Psychology Figure 2 Hypothesized model representing the actor and companions impacts of negative marital connection on depressive symptoms and the mediation effect of intention in life at the actor and companion levels. There is a definite link among unpleasant marital connection and depressed symptoms, and this connection is harmful to couples' physical wellbeing and other parts of functioning, especially as they get elder [77]. To reduce the dyadic impact of bad marital connection on depressive symptoms and associated health consequences, it's critical to look at prospective possible areas. XI. CONCLUSION Researchers analyzed how relational mindfulness was associated with self and companion mental health functioning and relational health while controlling for characteristic awareness in this research. The interpersonal relationships between partnership mindfulness and relational outcomes discovered in years support the idea that one's interior state of mindfulness emerges in actions regarding one's romantic partner. People who remain single had lower levels of depression and drinking issues over the seven years analyzed, whereas those who marry have lower rates of depression and alcohol issues. Even though some of the advantages of marriage arise from the fact that few depressed girls are more likely to marry, being married and remaining married improves mental wellbeing after controlling for previous phases of mental health. Although some of the advantages of marriage arise from the fact that few depressed girls are more likely to marry, being married and remaining married improves psychological wellbeing after controlling for previous phases of mental health. However, they had a strong inclination to have less physical contact with their companions. This was one of the main reasons they reported lower levels of happiness than individuals with reduced attachment rejection. Because individuals gained from touching despite their unfavorable thoughts toward it, discovering ways to promote openness to affectionate contact among those who are more prone to attachment rejection is an interesting area for future research. S. Arul Prabhahar et al. REFERENCES [1] Kim, A., Lee, J.A. and Park, H.S., 2018. Health behaviors and illness according to marital status in middle-aged Koreans. Journal of Public Health, 40(2), pp.e99e106. [2] Becker, C., Kirchmaier, I. and Trautmann, S.T., 2019. Marriage, parenthood and social network: Subjective well-being and mental health in old age. PloS one, 14(7), p.e0218704. [3] Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. [4] Bowlin, S. L., & Baer, R. A. (2012). Relationships between mindfulness, selfcontrol, and psychological functioning.Personality and Individual Differences, 52(3), 411–415. [5] Lenger, K. A., Gordon, C. L., & Nguyen, S. P. (2017). Intra-individual and crosspartner associations between the five facets of mindfulness and relationship satisfaction. Mindfulness, 8(1), 171–180. [6] Kimmes, J. G., Jaurequi, M. E., May, R. W., Srivastava, S., & Fincham, F. D. (2017). Mindfulness in the context of romantic relationships: Initial development and validation of the Relationship Mindfulness Measure. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy Advance online publication [7] Liu, H., Ngo, H. Y., and Cheung, F. M. (2016). Work-family enrichment and marital satisfaction among Chinese couples: a crossover-spillover perspective. Int. J. Stress Manag. 23, 209–231 [8] Brough, P., and O’Driscoll, M. P. (2005). “Work-family conflict and stress,” in Research Companion to Organizational Health Psychology, eds A. Antoniou, and C. Cooper, (Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publisher), 346–365. [9] Fu, H., & Goldman, N. (1996). Incorporating health into models of marriage choice: Demographic and sociological perspectives. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58(3), 740–758. [10] Kane, A. A. (2013). Marital status and selfrated health. Master’s thesis, San Diego State University, Health, and Human Services. 36 [11] Liu, H., & Umberson, D. J. (2008). The times they are a changin’: Marital status and health differentials from 1972 to 2003. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 49(3), 239–253. [12] Carr, D., & Springer, K. W. (2010). Advances in families and health research in the 21st century. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 72(3), 743–761. [13] Rook, K. S. (1984). The negative side of social interaction: Impact on psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(5), 1097–1108. [14] Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127(4), 472–503. [15] Miller, R. B., Hollist, C. S., Olsen, J., & Law, D. (2013). Marital quality and health over 20 years: A growth curve analysis. Journal of Marriage and Family, 75(3), 667–680. [16] Bookwala, J. (2005). The role of marital quality in physical health during the mature years. Journal of Aging and Health, 17(1), 85–104. [17] Rook, K. S. (1990). Parallels in the study of social support and social strain. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9(1), 118–132. [18] Orth-Gomer, K., Wamala, S. P., Horsten, M., Schenck-Gustafsson, K., Schneiderman, N., & Mittleman, M.A. (2000). Marital stress worsens prognosis in women with coronary heart disease: The Stockholm Female Coronary Risk Study. JAMA, 284(23), 3008–3014. [19] Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin,127(4), 472–503. [20] Ross, C. E. (1995). Reconceptualizing marital status as a continuum of social attachment. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57(1), 129–140. [21] Hawkins, D. N., & Booth, A. (2005). Unhappily ever after: Effects of long-term, low-quality marriages on well-being. Social Forces, 84(1), 451–471. [22] Williams, K. (2003). Has the future of marriage arrived? A contemporary examination of gender, marriage, and psychological well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 44(4), 470. [23] Diener, E., & Chan, M. Y. (2011). Happy people live longer: Subjective well-being contributes to health and longevity. 37 [24] [25] [26] [27] [28] [29] [30] [31] [32] [33] [34] Journal of Positive School Psychology Applied Psychology: Health and WellBeing, 3(1), 1–43 Proulx, C. M., Helms, H. M., & Buehler, C. (2007). Marital quality and personal wellbeing: A meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 69(3), 576–593. Carr, D., Freedman, V. A., Cornman, J. C., & Schwarz, N. (2014). Happy marriage, happy life? Marital quality and subjective well-being in later life. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 76(5), 930–948. Fenelon, A. (2013). Geographic divergence in mortality in the United States. Population and Development Review, 39(4), 611–634. Elo, I. T. (2009). Social class differentials in health and mortality: Patterns and explanations in comparative perspective. Annual Review of Sociology, 35, 553–572. Mahoney, A. (2010). Religion in families, 1999–2009: A relational spirituality framework. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 72(4), 805–827. Hummer, R. A., Rogers, R. G., Nam, C. B., & Ellison, C. G. (1999). Religious involvement and US adult mortality. Demography, 36(2), 273–285. Wachs, K., & Cordova, J. V. (2007). Mindful relating: Exploring mindfulness and emotion repertoires in intimate relationships. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 33(4), 464–481. Jones, K. C., Welton, S. R., Oliver, T. C., & Thoburn, J. W. (2011). Mindfulness, spousal attachment, and marital satisfaction: A mediated model. The Family Journal, 19(4), 357–361. Saavedra, M. C., Chapman, K. E., & Rogge, R. D. (2010). Clarifying links between attachment and relationship quality: Hostile conflict and mindfulness as moderators. Journal of Family Psychology, 24(4), 380–390 Barnes, S., Brown, K. W., Krusemark, E., Campbell, W. K., & Rogge, R. D. (2007). The role of mindfulness in romantic relationship satisfaction and responses to relationship stress. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 33(4), 482–500. Kimmes, J. G., Jaurequi, M. E., May, R. W., Srivastava, S., & Fincham, F. D. (2017). Mindfulness in the context of romantic relationships: Initial development and validation of the Relationship [35] [36] [37] [38] [39] [40] [41] [42] [43] [44] [45] Mindfulness Measure. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy. Karremans, J. C., Schellekens, M. P., & Kappen, G. (2017). Bridging the sciences of mindfulness and romantic relationships: A theoretical model and research agenda. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(1), 29–49. Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. Tomlinson, E. R., Yousaf, O., Vittersø, A. D., & Jones, L. (2018). Dispositional mindfulness and psychological health: A systematic review. Mindfulness, 9(1), 22– 43. Whisman, M. A., & Baucom, D. H. (2012). Intimate relationships and psychopathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 15(1), 4–13. Kashy, D. A., & Kenny, D. A. (1999). The analysis of data from dyads and groups. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 451–477). New York: Cambridge University Press. Barnes, S., Brown, K. W., Krusemark, E., Campbell, W. K., & Rogge, R. D. (2007). The role of mindfulness in romantic relationship satisfaction and responses to relationship stress. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 33(4), 482–500. Williams, A. M., & Cano, A. (2014). Spousal mindfulness and social support in couples with chronic pain. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 30(6), 528–535. Zamir, O., Gewirtz, A. H., & Zhang, N. (2017). Actor–partner associations of mindfulness and marital quality after military deployment. Family Relations, 66(3), 412–424 May, L. M., & Reinhardt, K. M. (2018). Self-other agreement in the assessment of mindfulness using the Five-Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire. Mindfulness, 9(1), 105–116. Dodge R, Daly A, Huyton J, Sanders L. The challenge of defining wellbeing. 38 S. Arul Prabhahar et al. [46] [47] [48] [49] [50] [51] [52] [53] [54] [55] [56] International Journal of Wellbeing. 2012; 2: 222–235. Matheson J. Measuring What Matters: National Statistician‘s Reflections on the National Debate on Measuring National Well-being. London: Office for National Statistics. 2011. Evans J. Findings from the National Wellbeing Debate. London: Office for National Statistics. 2011. Pinquart M, So¨rensen S. Influences of Socioeconomic Status, Social Network, and Competence on Subjective Well-Being in Later Life: A Meta-Analysis. Psychology and Aging. 2000; 15: 187–224. Diener E, Suh E. Measuring Quality of Life: Economic, Social, and Subjective Indicators. Social Indicators Research. 1997; 40: 189–21. Knesebeck OVD, Wahrendorf M, Hyde M, Siegrist J. Socio-economic position and quality of life among older people in 10 European countries: results of the SHARE study. Ageing and Society. 2007; 27: 269– 284. Pavot W, Diener E. The Affective and Cognitive Context of Self-Reported Measures of Subjective Well- Being. Social Indicators Research. 1993; 28: 1–20. Beckie TM, Hayduk LA. Measuring Quality of Life. Social Indicators Research. 1997; 42: 21–39. Hyde M, Wiggins RD, Higgs P, Blane DB. A measure of quality of life in early old age: The theory, development and properties of a needs satisfaction model (CASP-19). Aging & Mental Health. 2003; 7: 186– 194. Prince MJ, Reischies F, Beekman ATF, Fuhrer R, Jonker C, Kivela S-L, et al. Development of the EURO–D scale–a European Union initiative to compare symptoms of depression in 14 European centres. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1999; 174: 330–338. Castro-Costa E, Dewey M, Stewart R, Banerjee S, Huppert F, Mendonca-Lima C, et al. Ascertaining late-life depressive symptoms in Europe: an evaluation of the survey version of the EURO-D scale in 10 nations. The SHARE project. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2008; 17: 12– 29. Prince MJ, Beekman ATF, Deeg DJH, Fuhrer R, Kivela S-L, Lawlor BA, et al. [57] [58] [59] [60] [61] [62] [63] [64] [65] [66] Depression symptoms in late life assessed using the EURO–D scale. Effect of age, gender and marital status in 14 European centres. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1999; 174: 339–345 Kafetsios, K., & Sideridis, G. D. (2006). Attachment, social support and well-being in young and older adults. Journal of Health Psychology, 11(6), 863–876. Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support seeking and support giving within couples in an anxietyprovoking situation: The role of attachment styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(3), 434–446. Whiffen, V. E. (2005). The role of partner characteristics in attachment insecurity and depressive symptoms. Personal Relationships, 12(3), 407–423. Collins, N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2004). An attachment theory perspective on closeness and intimacy. In D. J. Mashek & A. Aron(Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 163–187) Lawrence Erlbaum. Mashek, D. J., & Sherman, M. D. (2004). Desiring less closeness with intimate others. In D. J. Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 343–356). Lawrence Erlbaum. Ozolins, A., & Sandberg, C. (2009). Development of a multifactor scale measuring the psychological dimensions of touch avoidance. International Journal of Psychology, 3, 33–56. Jakubiak, B. K., & Feeney, B. C. (2017). Affectionate touch to promote relational, psychological, and physical well-being in adulthood: A theoretical model and review of the research.Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(3), 228–252. Girme, Y. U., Overall, N. C., Simpson, J. A., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (2015). “All or nothing”: Attachment avoidance and the curvilinear effects of partner support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(3), 450–475. Stanton, S. C. E., Campbell, L., & Pink, J. C. (2017). Benefits of positive relationship experiences for avoidantly attached individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(4), 568–588. Park, Y., Impett, E. A., MacDonald, G., & Lemay, E. P. (2019). Saying “thank you”: Partners’ expressions of gratitude protect 39 [67] [68] [69] [70] [71] [72] [73] [74] [75] [76] [77] Journal of Positive School Psychology relationship satisfaction and commitment from the harmful effects of attachment insecurity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,117(4), 773–806. Schuster, T. L., Kessler, R. C., & Aseltine, R. H. (1990). Supportive interactions, negative interactions, and depressed mood. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18(3), 423–438. Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127(4), 472–503. Beach, S. R. H., Katz, J., Kim, S., & Brody, G. H. (2003). Prospective effects of marital satisfaction on depressive symptoms in established marriages: A dyadic model. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 20(3), 355–371. Umberson, D., Williams, K., Powers, D. A., Liu, H., & Needham, B. (2006). You make me sick: Marital quality and health over the life course. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 47(1), 1–16. Carr, D., & Utz, R. L. (2020). Families in later life: A decade in review. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 82(1), 346–363. Cook, W. L., & Kenny, D. A. (2005). The actor–partner interdependence model: A model of bidirectional effects in developmental studies.International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(2), 101–109. Fincham, F. D., Beach, S. R. H., Harold, G. T., & Osborne, L. N. (1997). Marital satisfaction and depression: Different causal relationships for men and women? Psychological Science, 8(5), 351–356. Beach, S. R. H., Sandeen, E., & O’Leary, K. D. (1990). Depression in marriage: A model for etiology and treatment. New York: Guilford Press. Kwak, M., & Ingersoll-Dayton, B. (2020). Relationships between negative exchanges and depressive symptoms in older couples: The moderating roles of care and gender. Journal of Social and PersonalRelationships, 37(3), 843–864. Bulanda, J. R., Yamashita, T., & Brown, J. S. (2020). Marital quality, gender, and later-life depressive symptom trajectories. Journal of 8 E. IRANI ET AL Women & Aging. Advance online publication. Wickrama, K. A. S., Lorenz, F. O., Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1997). Marital quality and physical illness: A latent [78] [79] [80] [81] [82] [83] [84] growth curve analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 59(1), 143–155. Waterman, A. S. (1984). The psychology of individualis. New York, NY: Praeger. Wood, A. M., & Joseph, S. (2010). The absence of positive psychological (eudemonic) well-being as a risk factor for depression: A ten yearcohort study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 122(3), 213–217. Schaefer, S. M., Morozink Boylan, J., van Reekum, C. M., Lapate, R. C.,Norris, C. J., Ryff, C. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2013). Purpose in life predicts better emotional recovery from negative stimuli. PLoS One,8(11), e80329. Kim, E. S., Delaney, S. W., & Kubzansky, L. D. (2019). Sense of purpose in life and cardiovascular disease: Underlying mechanisms and future directions. Current Cardiology Reports, 21(11), 135. Polenick, C. A., Renn, B. N., & Birditt, K. S. (2018). Dyadic effects of depressive symptoms on medical morbidity in middleaged and older couples. Health Psychology, 37(1), 28–36. Hill, P. L., Schultz, L. H., Jackson, J. J., & Andrews, J. A. (2019). Parentchild conflict during elementary school as a longitudinal predictor of sense of purpose in emerging adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 48(1), 145–153. McKnight, P. E., & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Purpose in life as a system that creates and sustains health and well-being: An integrative, testable theory. Review of General Psychology, 13(3), 242–251.