See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/311438919 The Resurgence of Awe in Psychology: Promise, Hope, and Perils Article in The Humanistic Psychologist · February 2017 DOI: 10.1037/hum0000060 CITATIONS READS 20 1,778 1 author: Kirk J Schneider Saybrook University; Existential-Humanistic Institute; Teachers College, Columbia University 156 PUBLICATIONS 983 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Wiley World Handbook for Existential Therapy View project The Spirituality of Awe: Challenges to the Robotic Revolution View project All content following this page was uploaded by Kirk J Schneider on 21 November 2017. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. The Resurgence of Awe in Psychology: Promise, Hope, and Perils1 Kirk Schneider [Note: This article is in press with The Humanistic Psychologist, copyright, 2016 by the American Psychological Association (APA). This article may not exactly replicate the authoritative document to be published in the APA journal. It is not the copy of record] Bio Kirk Schneider, PhD is the recent past president of the Society for Humanistic Psychology (Division 32 of the American Psychological Association, 2015-2016); adjunct faculty at Saybrook University and Teachers College, Columbia University; and president-elect of the Existential-Humanistic Institute (ehinstitute.org). Dr. Schneider’s recent publications include The Polarized Mind, ExistentialHumanistic Therapy, and Awakening to Awe. Correspondence: kschneider56@gmail.com This article is adapted from my President’s Address entitled “Rediscovering Awe in Psychology and Life” delivered August 5th, 2016 to the Society for Humanistic Psychology (Division 32) at the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association. 1 1 Abstract The sense of awe, also defined as a perception of vastness that cannot be assimilated but can be accommodated; or as the experience of humility and wonder--adventure--toward living, has become a “hot” topic in psychology. This article considers the basis for this trend, some promising avenues of research, and several challenges to the mainstream--quantitative--perspective on awe. It is concluded that while mainstream perspectives on awe appear to have beneficial effects on an impressive array of human behaviors, the relative neglect of historically rooted, in depth, qualitative approaches to awe pose notable perils. Among these perils are the comparative reductionism of the findings on awe to overt and measurable reactions; the neglect of longer term, life-changing experiences of awe, and the neglect of the broader social implications of awe-based transformation. Keywords: awe, awe-based, positive psychology, happiness, humanistic, existential, social-political, methodology 2 William James (1902/1936) extolled it in his Varieties of Religious Experience; Rudolf Otto (1923/1958) in his Idea of the Holy; and Abraham Maslow (1966), Rollo May (1991), and Ernest Becker (1967) celebrated it in their chief psychospiritual treatises. But how has the concept they celebrated--the sense of awe--fared in psychology since? Not so well I would contend--and in fact dismally. In the last three decades, the sense of awe as a focus of study has virtually disappeared (Haidt, 2006). While mainstream psychology had included it as a kind of “add on,” particularly in its investigation of religion (e.g., Wulff, 1997), the sensibility had, for all intents and purposes, zero role in the major subdisciplines of psychology--from methodology to personality and from psychotherapy to healthcare-until recently, that is. At present, the sense of awe is one of the hottest topics in contemporary psychology (Bonner & Friedman, 2011; Gregoire, 2014). Beginning with the germinal research of Keltner & Haidt’s (2003) article called “Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion,” Haidt’s (2006) book on Happiness, Piff & Keltner’s (2015, May 22) New York Times piece on awe, and to a 3 lesser extent my own Rediscovery of Awe (2004) and Awakening to Awe (2009), the sensibility of awe is the subject of a surfeit of studies and articles. These studies range from the mainstream professional literature (e.g., Bonner & Friedman, 2011; Rudd, Vohs, & Aaker, 2012) to social media (e.g., The Huffington Post [Gregoire, 2014, September], to the mainstream press (e.g., Parade Magazine [Scott, 2016, October 9] and The New York Times [Piff & Keltner, 2015, May 22]). The sense of awe has also recently (June, 2016) become the focus of a major symposium sponsored by the University of California, Berkeley entitled the “Art and Science of Awe.” This conference, which was headlined by Keltner and other mainstream spokespersons for the study of awe, was attended by literally hundreds of enthusiastic supporters--I know because I was there. But what is this phenomenon that seems to have taken our profession--and much of the world--by storm? First it is a concept that is strongly attuned to our times. These are times in which the fastest growing “religious” constituency--the so-called “Nones,” or noncommitted religious seekers--are loath to commit to any one religious dogma but seek out a multiplicity of religious and spiritual “truths” (Weiner, 2011, December 11). To the extent that the sense of 4 awe cross-cuts all of the major world religions without identifying with any single one, it resonates powerfully with that pursuit. Moreover, the sense of awe is correlated with many behavioral states that excite the new spiritual seekers. These states include increased altruism, patience, gratitude, creativity, and even, relative to other “positive” states such as “happiness” an increased preference for substantive (as distinct from frivolous) travel destinations (Piff & Keltner, 2015, May 22; Rudd, Vohs, & Aaker, 2012; Van Capellen & Saroglou, 2012)! The sense of awe, even more than happiness, has also correlated with the lowering of disease-promoting inflammation, an increased frequency of “piloerections,” that is goose bumps, and a general sense of life-satisfaction (Rudd, Vohs, & Aaker, 2012; Stellar et al., 2015). Mainstream Research on Awe As previously indicated, the resurgence of conventional research on awe is due largely to Keltner & Haidt’s (2003) article. This piece inspired dozens of spin-off studies examining the correlations between awe and prosocial behaviors. These studies have been especially appealing in the field of positive psychology 5 where quantitative-experimental demonstrations of the power of awe are noteworthy (Bonner & Friedman,2011; Sundararajan, 2002). The problem with these aforementioned studies from an existential-humanistic standpoint, however is three-fold: 1) they are based on a narrow and objectivizing methodology; 2) this methodology may yield commensurately narrow and transient results; and 3) some of the results may ironically contribute to rather than form a hedge against our “quick fix,” awe-depleted culture. Methodological Narrowness A large proportion of awe studies are based on overt and measurable (that is, quantitative-experimental) data (Bonner & Friedman, 2011; Sundararajan, 2002). While this approach is not necessarily problematic in itself, it does raise questions about the generalizability of its findings for the deeper, experiential aspects of awe that resist articulation by quantitative assessment (Bonner & Friedman, 2011). Consider, for example, the conventional definition of awe, originally formulated by Keltner and Haidt (2003). This definition in effect states that awe is a perception of vastness that cannot be assimilated but can be accommodated. The problem with 6 this definition however is not that it’s in error, but that it only goes so far. The classic definition of awe through history conveys that not only can awe not be assimilated, that is, integrated within one’s own cognitive schemas, but that, ultimately, it also cannot be accommodated, that is, integrated within external schemas, such as religions (Otto, 1923/1958). This is because, fundamentally, awe is based on the mystery of existence, and mystery, by definition, cannot be assimilated or accommodated; it is beyond schematization. This state of affairs is also why historically, there is consistently an element of anxiety in the awe-based state; it is not merely the thrill of the vast and unknown but also the dread (Webster’s New Collegiate Dictionary, 1988). Or as I have stated it in my qualitative studies of awe, it is perceived as a co-mingling of “thrill and anxiety,” “humility and wonder of living” (Schneider, 2004, 2009). To put it succinctly, the challenge of awe is precisely that it cannot be assimilated or accommodated; it must be lived with, often for long periods of time, without the hope of being solved or comfortably categorized. Another and related difficulty with the conventional view is that because the standardized definition of awe is confined to an overt and measurable calculus (that of “accommodation”), the methodology 7 used to investigate the definition is also confined to a calculus. Hence to test people’s accommodation to vastness, they are “primed” by comparatively short and accommodative scenes, such as videos of waterfalls, astronauts, landscapes and other brief exposures to nature (Mikulak, 2015, April). But to what extent are these glimpses reflective of the awe people experience in real life, and how enduring are their results? The conventional findings also show that, based on paper and pencil tests, physiological instruments, and other objectivized measures, many people perceive positive changes in their lives. But to what extent are these instruments measuring the kind of awe that has been articulated throughout the ages--great works of art, meaningful innovations, or lifetimes of struggle, inquiry, and engagement? These latter exemplars of awe may only be captured very partially by the conventional scales--or perhaps not at all! On the other hand if there was ever a time where mixed methods were called for this is it. Such quantitative and qualitative methods could considerably enrich our understanding of the long-term and multifaceted implications of awe-inspired lives.2 Because I feel so passionately about the need to augment quantitativeexperimental studies of awe with that of qualitative-depth studies, I asked the Science Director of the University of California, Berkeley Greater Good Science 2 8 For example, in my own, phenomenologically-based investigations, I found that awe is associated with an ongoing appreciation for life in all its vicissitudes(变化、变迁,兴衰), from the melancholic to the ecstatic, and from the deeply troubling and even mortifying to the poignant and profound. This “whole enchilada” approach to life differentiated into six basic themes or what I call “lenses” through which participants in my 2009 study cultivated a sense of awe: These entailed 1) an acute awareness of the passing nature of time, 2) the attunement to wonder and surprise, 3) the realization of a cosmic context to everyday experiences, 4) the perception of the intricacy and subtleties of life, 5) the experience of being deeply, emotionally moved, and 6) the appreciation for solitude. I also found that the sense of awe has rich potential to reform the very building blocks of modern industrialized society, from its implications for the transformation of our parenting to our educational system to our work setting to our religious and spiritual settings and even to our Center, which was chiefly responsible for the Art and Science of Awe conference, what she thought about such a perspective. Her answer was that she welcomed qualitative research as a complement to the prevailing quantitative approaches but that neither she nor many of her colleagues knew enough about such qualitative investigations to prominently implement them in their studies (Emiliana SimonThomas, personal communication, June 4th, 2016). This for me is one of the sad facts about the current state of our mainstream profession--where one of the most important dimensions of human experience--awe--can only basically be investigated using methods that would seem partially suitable to its subject matter. 9 legislative and deliberative settings in local communities (see Schneider, 2009, 2013, 2016). While granting that these are ambitious elaborations of comparatively limited qualitative data, they are nevertheless vital in my view if we are to address a phenomenon as rich and far-ranging as awe. Two Kinds of Awe What I’m beginning to sense then is that there appear to be two basic contemporary forms of awe: that which I call (following a Japanese adage) the “quick boil” form, and that which I term the “slow simmer form.” The quick boil form, represented notably by mainstream quantitative-experimental research, and the slow simmer form, reflected palpably by more literary, qualitative forms of inquiry, seem to echo a larger, virtually archetypal clash at the heart of our Western industrialized culture. On the one hand, we have the ostensibly efficient and glamorous findings of mainstream research. These findings are relatively unambiguous (as reflected for example, by the increase of prosocial preferences or physiological resilience in response to brief awe-eliciting stimuli). The findings are also comparatively marketable and fit comfortably with seductive 10 consumerist desires. For example, who would not want brisk ways to make themselves feel “awesome;” and the more that “awesomeness” (like “happiness”) can be commodified, or even “prescribed” as one presenter put it at the aforementioned Art and Science of Awe conference, so much the better. Now to be fair, I know that many of the mainstream researchers of awe do not pursue their investigations on the basis of financial rewards, but my larger point is that because of the calculating and standardizing methodological paradigm out of which they practice, some mainstream investigators contribute unwittingly to this mercantile, means/ends mentality. It will not be long in my view before a brain scientist identifies a neural correlate for awe--and this will be activated and deactivated at will. But the question persists: will such a discovery reflect the subtle and manifold experiences of awe, and especially those of the slow simmer variety? Personally, I doubt it. The slow simmer variety of awe entails hardwon, ambiguously tinged life experiences, experiences that are cultivated through persistent engagements, complicated relationships, and unsettling perceptions (see Schneider, 2009; Sundararajan, 2002)--not merely behaviors and brain functions. 11 Here for example is an excerpt from an interviewee in Awakening to Awe that would be hard-pressed to operationalize: Awe is, in one special sense, the excitement of participation. Translated into process, awe befriends depth psychotherapy—not by promising to remove all pain, rather by addressing (with reverence) the pained person; not by eradicating his conflict. Instead by paying attention to the role of friction and combat as the exile’s resolve to cross the desert; not by encouraging the positive, more by paying attention to who one is; finally, not by dismissing sin, but more important, by seeking fellowship within the tragedy of alienation and estrangement. (Cited in Schneider, 2009, p. 117). To sum, and as the excerpt above illustrates, there is great need for the mixed method study of awe. Prevailing psychological research is dominated by quantitative-experimental methods that partially but not optimally in my view, capture the richness of the compelling human experience of awe. In my and others’ studies awe is not merely a “means-ends” phenomenon, a “place to get to” that can be formulated and “prescribed,” but an organic experience that 12 has transformed the lives of many profound inquirers and sufferers of the human predicament (I think not only of scholars and artists here but also those in my study, Awakening to Awe, who were former gang leaders and drug addicts; abused children and infirmed adults). Qualitative research has also shown awe to be a byproduct of a life deeply lived, and not necessarily a specific scene or event. This issue is illuminated most acutely by the youthful videos of Jason Silva, another featured presenter at the Art and Science of Awe conference. His virtually manic dashes through milestones of life-experience--the formation of an embryo, the immensities of space, the creations of Mozart--are mind-blowing; but they are also mind-numbing because we know intuitively that more, much more, is embodied by such scenarios than can be captured in a three-minute short (see, for example, his video titled “Radical Openness” at https://www.diygenius.com/get-your-mind-blown-jason-silvas-shotsof-awe-and-wonder/). Finally, prevailing research also does not capture the larger social implications of an awe-based reformation. I have touched upon some of the sectors of that reformation, but provide fuller details in my books (Schneider, 2004, 2009, 2013, 2016), along with those by 13 many kindred scholars, such as James (1902/1936), Maslow (1966), May (1991), Becker (1967), Marcel (1967), Mendelowitz (2008), and Tillich (1952). It is high time for psychology to spread its wings and go farther in its reply to perceptual psychologist J.J. Gibson (1988) who wrote: “Psychology or at least American psychology is a second rate discipline. The main reason is that it does not stand in awe of its subject matter” (p. 1). To be sure, psychology is once again standing in awe of its subject matter; but how and with what degree of depth? I argue that the manner and substance with which psychology is now standing in awe is wanting. It is hopeful, but also potentially perilous in its seduction by reductionist-consumerist forces. The answer to this dilemma will not be simple but is imperative if we are to foster a more gratifying path. The embrace of mixed methods--the slow simmer of depth--will help light the way. 14 References Becker, E. (1967). Beyond alienation: A philosophy of education for the crisis of democracy. New York: Braziller. Bonner, E.T. & Friedman, H.L. (2011). A conceptual clarification of the experience of awe: An interpretative phenomenological analysis, The Humanistic Psychologist, 39, 222-235.To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08873267.2011.593372 Haidt, J. (2006). The happiness hypothesis: Finding modern truth in ancient wisdom. New York: Basic. Gregoire, C. (2014, September 22). How Awe-Inspiring Experiences Can Make You Happier, Less Stressed And More Creative. Huffington Post. Retrieved 10/14/2016 http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2014/09/22/the-psychology-ofawe_n_5799850.html James, W. (1902/1936). The varieties of religious experience. New York: The Modern Library. Keltner, D. & Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 297-314. Marcel, G. (1967). The philosophy of existentialism. New York: 15 Citadel. Maslow, A. (1966). The Psychology of science: A reconnaissance. Chicago: Regnery. May, R. (1991). The cry for myth. New York: Norton. Mendelowitz, E. (2008). Ethics and Lao Tzu: Intimations of character. Colorado Springs: University of the Rockies Press. Mikulak, A. (2015, April). All about awe: Science explores how life’s small marvels elevate cognition and emotion. American Psychological Society Observer, 28. [Retrieved 10/15/16 at http://www.psychologicalscience.org/publications/observer/201 5/april-15/all-about-awe.html] Otto, R. The idea of the holy. (New York: Oxford University Press, 1923/1958). Piff, P. & Keltner, D. (2015, May, 22). Why do we experience awe? New York Times Sunday Review. (Retrieved 10/14/16 http://www.nytimes.com/2015/05/24/opinion/sunday/why-dowe-experience-awe.html?_r=0 Reed, E. S. (1988), James J. Gibson and the Psychology of Perception, New Haven and London: Yale University Press. Rudd, M., Vohs, K. D., & Aaker, J. (2012). Awe expands people’s 16 perception of time, alters decision making, and enhances wellbeing. Psychological Science, 23, 1130–1136. doi: 10.1177/0956797612438731 Schneider, K. (2004). Rediscovery of awe: Splendor, mystery, and the fluid center of life. St. Paul, MN: Paragon House. Schneider, K. (2008). Existential-integrative psychotherapy: Guideposts to the core of practice. New York: Routledge. Schneider, K. (2009). Awakening to awe: Personal stories of profound transformation. Lanham, MD: Jason Aronson. Schneider, K. (2013). The polarized mind: Why it’s killing us and what we can do about it. Colorado Springs, CO: University Professors Press. Schneider, K. (2016). The spirituality of awe: Challenges to the robotic revolution. Manuscript in preparation. Stellar, J., John-Henderson, N., Anderson, C., Gordon, A. M., McNeil, G., & Keltner, D. (2015). Positive affect and markers of inflammation: Discrete positive emotions predict lower levels of inflammatory cytokines. Emotion. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/emo0000033 Sundararajan, L. (2002). Religious awe: Potential contributions of 17 negative theology to psychology, "positive" or otherwise. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 22,174197. Tillich, P. (1952). The courage to be. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Van Capellen, P. & Saroglou, V. (2012). Awe activates religious and spiritual feelings and behavioral intentions. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 4, 223-236. Webster’s New Collegiate Dictionary (1988). Springfield, MA: Miriam-Webster. Weiner, E. (2011, December 11). Americans: Undecided about God? New York Times Sunday Review. Wulff, D. M. (1997). Psychology of religion: Classic and contemporary. New York: John Wiley & Sons. 18 View publication stats