HOW MONETARY INCENTIVES AND LOYALTY AFFECT GOAL REALIZATION A Dissertation Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the University of South Alabama in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Business Administration by Lawrence E. Goehrig MBA, Nova Southeastern University, 2013 BS, University of Florida, 1986 May 2023 To My Loving Wife, Carmen, For Whom I Am So Grateful. Thank you for your support and patience in my journey. ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS To my committee, thank you so much for your insights and encouragement. Joe, your scholarship and forward thinking which created the program that enabled me to continue my academic pursuits. Bill, your tireless work ethic, precise writing, and high expectations have helped push me to the finish line. Regina, you have always had guidance for me over the past ten years and constantly mentioned the benefits of a PhD and Joe Hair. Ben, thank you so much for your time, efforts and feedback which greatly helped accomplish my dissertation. To my wife, Carmen, thank you for being by my side and encouraging me to keep pushing during the tough times of these studies. Without your support, this paper would not have been possible. The hours, days, weeks, and months spent reading and writing could not have happened without you being the rock that you have always been. To the members of Cohort VI and all the PhD faculty, thank you for your friendship and support. Full weekends and late study sessions were bearable with the great friendships we developed through our time together. I look forward to the future with such a remarkable group in my corner. I cherish the times we had together while looking forward to future endeavors and successes. A special thank you to Britton for your great friendship and help over the years on this journey. You definitely are a great asset, and I am truly grateful for your help and guidance. Thank you to Dr John Riggs for your referral to Joe Hair and our meetings to let me know what this journey would be like and to stick it out. iii Thank you to Dr Matt Howard for your patience and time spent helping me understand research methods. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES ........................................................................................................... viii LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................................ x LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS ............................................................................................ xi ABSTRACT...................................................................................................................... xii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION ......................................................................................... 1 1.1 Statement of the Problem....................................................................................... 6 1.2 Purpose .................................................................................................................. 9 1.3 Contribution ........................................................................................................... 9 1.4 Summary of Remaining Chapters ........................................................................ 11 CHAPTER II LITERATURE REVIEW .......................................................................... 13 2.1 Goal Attainment Theories.................................................................................... 14 2.2 Monetary Incentives ............................................................................................ 19 2.3 Locus of Control .................................................................................................. 23 2.4 Dishonest Behavior .............................................................................................. 27 2.5 Employee Loyalty ................................................................................................ 34 Chapter III MODEL DEVELOPMENT AND HYPOTHESES ....................................... 40 3.1 Locus of Control .................................................................................................. 42 3.2 Monetary Incentives ............................................................................................ 42 3.3 Organization Employee Loyalty .......................................................................... 43 3.4 Dishonest Behavior .............................................................................................. 44 CHAPTER IV METHODS ............................................................................................... 49 4.1 Sample Criteria and Data Sources ....................................................................... 49 4.1.1 Analytical Process ..................................................................................... 49 4.1.2 Data Sources .............................................................................................. 50 4.1.3 Criteria ....................................................................................................... 50 4.2 Survey Instrument ................................................................................................ 51 4.3 Sample ................................................................................................................. 52 4.4 Scales ................................................................................................................... 54 4.4.1 Locus of Control ........................................................................................ 54 4.4.2 Employee Loyalty ..................................................................................... 54 4.4.3 Monetary Incentives .................................................................................. 55 v 4.4.4 Dishonest Behavior ................................................................................... 55 4.4.5 Goal Attainment ........................................................................................ 55 4.5 Bias, Validity, and Analysis ................................................................................ 56 4.5.1 Common Method Bias ............................................................................... 56 4.5.2 Analysis ..................................................................................................... 57 4.5.3 Pilot Testing............................................................................................... 57 CHAPTER V RESULTS .................................................................................................. 58 5.1 Analytical Process ............................................................................................... 58 5.2 Data Analysis and Findings ................................................................................. 58 5.2.1 Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling................................. 58 5.3 Assessment of Measurement Model .................................................................... 59 5.3.1 Data Distribution ....................................................................................... 59 5.3.2 Common Method Variance ....................................................................... 60 5.3.3 Estimate of Loadings, Significance, and Item Reliability ......................... 60 5.3.4 Composite Reliability ................................................................................ 63 5.3.5 Convergent Validity .................................................................................. 63 5.3.6 Discriminant Validity ................................................................................ 64 5.3.7 Nomological Validity ................................................................................ 66 5.4 Evaluation of the Structural Model...................................................................... 67 5.4.1 Assessment of Collinearity ........................................................................ 67 5.4.2 Path Coefficients and Significance ............................................................ 68 5.4.3 Coefficients of Determination (R2) ............................................................ 69 5.4.4 Effect Sizes (ƒ2) ......................................................................................... 70 5.4.5 PLSpredict ................................................................................................. 71 5.5 Overview of Hypotheses Results ......................................................................... 73 5.5.1 Assessment – Moderation and Mediation ................................................. 74 CHAPTER VI DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS .................................................... 78 6.1 Discussion ............................................................................................................ 78 6.1.1 Theoretical Implications ............................................................................ 80 6.1.2 Managerial Implications ............................................................................ 80 6.2 Conclusions.......................................................................................................... 81 6.3 Limitations ........................................................................................................... 81 6.4 Future Research ................................................................................................... 82 6.5 Research Conclusion and Summary .................................................................... 83 REFERENCES ................................................................................................................. 85 vi APPENDICES .................................................................................................................. 98 Appendix A Original Item Factor Loadings .............................................................. 98 Appendix B Invitation to Participate ....................................................................... 100 Appendix C Survey Statements ............................................................................... 102 Appendix D IRB Approval ...................................................................................... 105 BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH .......................................................................................... 106 vii LIST OF TABLES Table Page 3.1. Hypothesized Path Relationships in the Study for the Constructs, Mediation and Moderation .........................................................................................................................48 4.1. Demographics of Survey Respondents– Financial Sales Employees ........................53 5.1. Factor Loadings of the Constructs in the Study ..........................................................61 5.2. Descriptive Statistics of Variables ..............................................................................62 5.3. Correlation Values of the Variables Showing Significance Level .............................62 5.4. Internal Consistency Assessments for each Construct to Show Items Within the Construct are Similar .........................................................................................................63 5.5. Average Variance Extracted for Convergent Validity for Each Construct to Show Shared Variance Between the Construct and Each Item for That Construct .....................64 5.6. Fornell-Larcker Criterion for Discriminant Validity for Each Construct ...................65 5.7. Heterotrait-Monotrait Discriminant Validity for the Measurement Model ................66 5.8. Variance Inflation Factor for each Latent Variable to Show There are no Collinearity Issues ..................................................................................................................................67 5.9. Size and Significance of Path Coefficients .................................................................68 5.10. Coefficients of Determination for the Dependent Variables to Show the Predictive Power of the DV ................................................................................................................70 5.11. Effect Sizes of Constructs for the Structural Equation Model ..................................70 5.12. PLSpredict Summary of Items to Show Comparison of the Error Measurement in the Predictive Power ..........................................................................................................72 viii 5.13. PLSpredict Summary of Constructs..........................................................................72 5.14. Path Relationship, Betas, and Significance for the Proposed Hypotheses in This Study ..................................................................................................................................74 5.15. Outcomes of Mediation and Moderation on Path Relationship Hypotheses ............75 Appendix Table A1. Original Items – Factor Loadings for all Constructs...................................................98 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Research Model of Hypothesized Relationships Showing the Influences of the Variables on Goal Attainment...................................................................................... 41 2. Structural Model of One-Tail Test Results Showing the Strength of the Relationships and Their Significance ................................................................................................. 69 3. Graph A Showing the Moderating Effects of Employee Loyalty on the Relationship of Monetary Incentive to Goal Attainment .................................................................. 76 4. Graph B Showing the Moderating Effects of Employee Loyalty on the Relationship of Monetary Incentive to Dishonest Behavior ............................................................. 77 x LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS AVE Average Variance Extracted CCA Confirmatory Composite Analysis DB Dishonest Behavior DM Decision Maker DWB Deviant Workplace Behavior EL Employee Loyalty EUT Expected Utility Theory GA Goal Attainment HTMT Heterotrait-Monotrait Ratio LM Linear Model LOC Locus of Control MI Monetary Incentives PLS-SEM Partial Least Squares-Structural Equation Modeling SET Social Exchange Theory VIF Variance Inflation Factor xi ABSTRACT Lawrence E. Goehrig, Ph.D., University of South Alabama, May 2023. How Monetary Incentives and Loyalty Affect Goal Realization. Chair of Committee: William Gillis, Ph.D. Monetary incentives are widely known to help motivate employees. Mostly these monetary incentives are used to increase the effort of the employees to attain the goals of the firm. Attaining firm goals will increase revenues, profits, market share, and similarly valued outcomes. These monetary incentives are often a gamble for the firm as there is no direct method to show the impact monetary incentives will have on goal attainment. Other variables come into play when monetary incentives are used. Employee loyalty, locus of control, and dishonest behavior are three that were researched. Employee loyalty can help the firm attain goals when employees feel a connection to the firm and value the firm for their own wants and achievements. Employee loyalty was investigated to show if employee loyalty influences goal attainment when monetary incentives are involved. The research showed that monetary incentives are not as important to loyal employees. Locus of control also influences goal attainment. This effect is solely based on the level of locus of control of the employee and was related directly to goal attainment. Employee dishonesty was investigated to see the influence monetary incentives have on dishonest behavior and how that dishonest behavior affects goal attainment. All three of these variables were examined to see how much effect they have on goal attainment and, more importantly, how valuable employee loyalty is to the firm. xii Keywords: monetary incentives, locus of control, dishonest behavior, employee loyalty, goal attainment, expected utility theory, social exchange theory xiii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Monetary incentives are frequently suggested as a method for motivating and improving the performance of people (Bonner & Sprinkle, 2002). Monetary incentives may also motivate people to invest effort to acquire the skills needed to perform a task so that future performance and rewards will be higher than they otherwise would be (Bonner & Sprinkle, 2002). This increased effort can be an asset for the firm as employees may seek additional training, work longer hours, increase contacts, etc., which will both help the employee increase performance and help to attain the firm’s goals. Overall, then, individual incentives are thought to promote effort directed toward strategic development when more standard and predetermined conditions are not sufficient to attain desired performance and reward levels (Locke & Latham, 1990). However, employers have no way of knowing if they overpaid with the incentive, resulting in wasteful spending while meeting company objectives, or if the level of bonus is so high that employees are incentivized to commit dishonest behaviors. From the employees’ standpoint, thus, a dilemma arises because dishonesty often serves their self-interest at the expense of their morality and may even cause material loss to others (Graham et al., 2016). From the employers’ standpoint, goal setting is important in the work environment (Locke, 2004). Compared to vague easy goals, specific challenging goals boost performance, so long as the person is committed to the goal, has the requisite ability to attain it, and does not have conflicting goals. When these conditions are met, 1 there is a positive linear relationship between goal difficulty and task performance (Locke & Latham, 2006). Locke (2004) researched goals using bonuses to incentivize effort. Although this method provided a strong incentive to reach the goal, it was also found that there was considerable temptation for the participants to consider the short term only, and therefore, create short cuts, cheat, and fake results in order to fraudulently attain the incentive. Locke (2004) concluded that it is better to have no bonus system at all, other than simple merit pay, than to have a bad bonus system. Employees are considered the core of an organization and the success or failure of the organization is attributed to the performance of the employees (Murali et al., 2017). It is of prime importance that the employees are loyal to the organization and don’t actively search for other alternative opportunities. Loyalty, as a general term, can be defined as a person’s devotion or sentiment of attachment to a particular object, which may be another person or a group of persons, an idea, a duty, or a cause (Murali et al., 2017). Employee loyalty “is a deliberate commitment to further the best interests of one’s employer, even when doing so, might demand sacrificing some aspect of one’s self-interest beyond what would be required by one’s legal and other moral duties” (Elegido, 2013, p. 496). A loyal employee might work harder and increase effort to attain the goal. Bonner and Sprinkle (2002) found a positive relationship between employee loyalty and job performance. The relationship between employee loyalty and job performance shows that loyal and committed employees perform their duties better than disloyal ones (Khan et al., 2020). Loyalty acts as a key success factor in performance evaluations. Loyalty to one’s employer makes the employee more trustworthy and therefore more valuable as an 2 employee (Elegido, 2013). Older employees generally consider themselves more loyal to an organization than younger employees. It appears that as employees age and remain with an organization, they become more loyal and more satisfied with their job (Murali et al., 2017). The underpinning theory for studying the relationship between employee loyalty and dishonesty in the workplace is social exchange theory. According to Nawaz et al. (2014), the theories of social exchange (SET) provide the theoretical basis for employees’ creative and destructive behaviors. SET also provides a conceptual paradigm for understanding behavior at the workplace. So, it is supported by research, based on SET, that employee loyalty is negatively associated with workplace deviance (Bilal et al., 2020). This research explored the connection between employee loyalty and deviant workplace behavior (DWB) in the banking sector. The findings of the regression analysis revealed that employee’s loyalty had a significant negative impact on DWB, which confirmed Rishipal’s (2019) findings, which found a significant negative association in the hospitality sector between loyalty and counterproductive work behavior. Locus of control is another factor that might enhance goal attainment without the motivation of a monetary incentive. Locus of control has been conceptualized as “the extent to which people believe that the rewards they receive in life can be controlled by their own personal actions” (Wang et al., 2010, p. 761) and captures whether individuals “attribute the cause or control of events either to themselves or to the external environment” (Spector, 1982, p. 483). Locus of control relates to one’s generalized expectations about the ability to take responsibility for what happens. 3 This study explores the effect monetary incentives have on goal attainment when accounting for other directly related factors such as locus of control and other indirectly related factors such as employee loyalty. This study also shows the impact employee loyalty has in moderating the monetary incentive to dishonest behavior relationship. Also, this study shows the impact that employee loyalty has on moderating the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship. I posit that the moderating relationships with employee loyalty will show that monetary incentive will have less of an impact on goal attainment when employee loyalty is high. I also posit that monetary incentive will have less of an impact on the dishonest behavior relationship when moderated by employee loyalty. This study shows the importance of high employee loyalty to an organization. Employee loyalty was researched to help show the effect that the monetary incentive can contribute to the employee behaving in a dishonest manner to accomplish the goal and attain the monetary incentive. Employee deviance, or unethical behavior, or dishonesty, is defined here as voluntary behavior that violates significant organizational norms and in so doing threatens the well-being of an organization, its members, or both (Robinson & Bennett, 1995). Finally, I will investigate whether employee loyalty mitigates this dishonest behavior. The three theories that detail the mechanisms through which monetary incentives increase goal attainment is expected utility theory, agency theory and expectancy theory. Social exchange theory is also used to show how locus of control and employee loyalty are involved in goal attainment. 4 In expected utility theory, Mongin (1997) states that the decision maker (DM) chooses between risky or uncertain prospects by comparing expected utility values. In expected utility theory (EUT), it is assumed that individuals attempt to maximize the amount of a reward that they desire. When the reward is a monetary incentive, this theory helps explain the level of effort the employee may use to attain that monetary incentive. Expected utility theory (EUT) posits that the employee chooses between risky or uncertain prospects by comparing expected utility or outcomes (Mongin, 1997). A fundamental assumption is that individuals are completely rationale and have welldefined preferences (Bonner & Sprinkle, 2002). Thus, according to Lengwiler (2009), EUT posits that people use or should use the most rational choice with the highest expected value of the utility of different possible outcomes for their choices as a guide for making decisions. From an economic perspective, agency theory assumes that agents are selfinterested and risk averse. Agency theory recommends monetary incentives as a means of motivating increased effort and improvements in performance (Eisenhardt, 1989). Agency theory views the firm as a complex nexus of contracts where authority to perform specialized tasks is delegated by the principal (typically managers) to agents (the employees) with the expectation that the agent will act in the best interest of the firm (Eisenhardt, 1989). Expectancy theory posits that financial incentives will illicit increased effort only if the individual believes that there is a high probability of receiving increased results from expending more effort (Vroom, 1964). Lawler and Suttle (1973) further expanded on Vroom’s expectancy theory by explaining in their research that the expectancy model 5 of effort leading to successful performance and the expectancy that this action of increased effort will produce outcomes. Overall, all three theories examine how monetary incentives work by increasing effort to attain a goal, which, in turn, leads to increases in performance and a monetary reward (Bonner & Sprinkle, 2002). Locus of control emerged from Rotter’s (1954, 1966) seminal work on social learning theory. Social learning theory posits that individuals learn by observing the events that occur around them in a manner that ultimately influences behavior (Rotter, 1954). In this learning process, individuals develop expectancies that specific behaviors will result in reinforced results (Rotter, 1966). According to Rotter (1966), a relatively stable individual difference (i.e., locus of control) emerges over time that pertains to the extent to which individuals perceive a causal relationship between behavior and rewards. Although some individuals perceive that their behavior and personal attributes drive outcomes (i.e., an internal locus of control), others develop a general sense that external forces govern outcomes (Galvin et al., 2018). While locus of control has been studied in several contexts, this research is unique in examining how this variable may affect or may not affect goal attainment. 1.1 Statement of the Problem According to Nnubia (2020), there have been several issues associated with financial incentives on workers’ performance on the part of workers and managers in various business organizations. Three of these are (a) a poor incentive package which is a major factor affecting employees’ commitment and productivity, (b) the employees’ 6 unwillingness to increase their performance because they feel that their contributions are not well recognized by their organizations, and (c) the shortage of necessary management skills that could help in the formulation of a good monetary incentive policy (Nnubia, 2020). The issue of employee performance cannot be over emphasized. Employees want to achieve their own personal goals and employers want them to attain the organizational goals. These objectives should be reasonably aligned. Managers need to recognize that employee loyalty is a core component of an employee’s conduct and needs to be assessed from time to time (Bilal et al., 2020). Rewards are highly used to overcome dissatisfaction and to increase performance of employees (Bonner & Sprinkle, 2002). Another issue is that monetary incentives can create the opportunity for employees to act in a dishonest or unethical way to benefit themselves to attain the monetary incentive. Successful service firms, such as banking, will invest resources or maintain the long-term relationships in the programs in order to increase job satisfaction and employees’ performance (Nnubia, 2020). In turn, the increased effort increases performance, and that increased performance will make the goal attainment more possible or more likely and subsequently, the monetary incentive will be attained. Thus, my research question concerns how much employee loyalty influences the relationship between monetary incentive and goal attainment and how monetary incentives and employee loyalty affect goal realization. Also, how do monetary incentives really help goal attainment when measured against loyal employees and locus of control? This will help determine whether to emphasize employee loyalty within the organization, to hire employees with a high locus of control, or to primarily offer monetary incentives. 7 According to Locke and Latham (2002), specific and difficult goals (but those perceived as attainable), lead to greater performance than vague and easy goals. They also found workers are more motivated or more committed to attain the organizational goals when they perceive the goals as being relevant and difficult or challenging to attain. Finally, they found that goals can increase workers’ persistence to exert effort. These results suggest that goal setting may be an effective tool to boost a worker’s motivation and effort (Corgnet et al., 2015). The ability to set reasonable and attainable goals by the firm enhances employee effort and that increased employee effort will then transfer into increased production for the firm and in turn create an increase in output which will increase revenues and ultimately increase net profit to the firm (Locke & Latham, 2004). According to expected utility theory (EUT), workers expect compensation or a benefit for their work. One possible issue pointed out by EUT is that if the monetary incentive is not enough to motivate the workers in the firm, the workers will not put forth effort to attain the goal. Thus, firms will not be getting effort from their workers to attain the goals and increase profits. Therefore, one goal for the firm is to determine what level of monetary incentive is needed to motivate employees to perform better and increase effort to accomplish firm goals. The setting of these goals and the attachment of monetary incentives to attain the goals must be at a level that the employees trust that the firm is operating in a manner that the employees are cared for, and the goals are attainable (Bonner & Sprinkle, 2002). Otherwise, the employees may put forth minimal effort for their pay without considering the goals attached to the monetary incentives. Therefore, the employer must know how 8 loyal their employees are to determine how much a monetary incentive will really increase performance to attain the goal. For example, if a monetary incentive is given but the employees are highly loyal, then it is possible that monetary incentive was not needed to attain the goal and would have increased the net profit. 1.2 Purpose This study focuses on the motivational process of monetary incentives to help increase goal attainment. Therefore, this study will help obtain knowledge that can be used when setting goals and attaching monetary incentives for attaining those goals. Employee loyalty is researched in this study to help determine the impact employee loyalty has on goal attainment and the effect employee loyalty has on the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship to help determine if employee loyalty can be increased and monetary incentives can be decreased. 1.3 Contribution The main contribution is attempting to find how much monetary incentives help goal attainment when controlling for other variables including locus of control and employee loyalty. This study is researched in the financial services sector. In economics literature, monetary incentives are considered to be the most effective way to induce workers to exert effort (Corgnet et al., 2015). Money is a symbol of power, status, and respect; it plays a crucial role in meeting the social, security and physiological needs of a person (Daramola & Daramola, 2019). Employee motivation is linked closely to employee performance, the performance of employees will make or 9 break a company; therefore, it is important to find a method to motivate employees (Daramola & Daramola, 2019). The most obvious form of motivation for an employee is money (Daramola & Daramola, 2019) but other factors such as work life balance, and external circumstances may influence motivation. A second contribution will be for the financial services sector to help it determine whether to put resources into keeping, maintaining, and enhancing employee loyalty or whether to primarily focus on monetary incentives. Employees are the most important factor of organizations because the success of the organization depends upon the performance of employees (Khan et al., 2020). Employee loyalty increases profit, improves quality, reduces turnover, and increases the reputation of the organization (Khan et al., 2020). Therefore, it is imperative that the organization finds a way to maintain and attract loyal employees. A third contribution will be to show if high locus of control employees performs better for goal attainment without relying much on a monetary incentive as motivation. Managers may then be able to set better goals with the possibility of less monetary incentives if they have loyal employees with high locus of control. This study can see if and how much loyalty to the firm contributes to an increase in effort to attain the goal. A more precise area is the financial services industry. In that sector this study will show if monetary incentives really have a strong effect on goal attainment with loyal employees as well as the affect locus of control has on goal attainment. From the social learning theory, a contribution will be to show if the more loyal employees attain the goal with less motivation from the monetary incentive. This can show that newer employees becoming more loyal by observing that the more loyal employees attain the goals with 10 less motivation from the monetary incentive can help an organization by implementing methods to enhance the loyalty of their employees. 1.4 Summary of Remaining Chapters This chapter addresses the importance of determining the need to find if monetary incentives really affect goal attainment with loyal employees. The remainder of this dissertation outlines how this question will be researched. Chapter II, the literature review, concentrates on how the relevant literature has addressed monetary incentives. The three major research streams expected utility theory, agency theory and expectancy theory will be shown to influence worker performance to accomplish goals and attain the incentives. The fourth theory of social exchange theory will show how employees with locus of control and are loyal to the firm will work hard for the goal and not rely on a monetary incentive to motivate them. Included in this literature review will also be significant literature defending the need for this research contribution. Chapter III will develop the model and hypotheses which depict how monetary incentives contributes to increased effort and performance to accomplish the goal and attain the monetary incentive. The model will also measure how locus of control can increase goal attainment and that employee loyalty to the firm can act as motivation to attain the goal without any monetary incentive. Another measurement will be to show how dishonest behavior influences the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship while also showing the effects employee loyalty has on the dishonest behavior variable on goal attainment. 11 Chapter IV will present the method used and the analysis of the data. Chapter V will be the results and show the results of the hypothesis tests. Chapter VI will be the discussion and conclusion to restate the problem and summarize the findings. Chapter VI will also state what these findings contribute to theory, method, and practice. 12 CHAPTER II LITERATURE REVIEW A focal contribution of this study, noted in Chapter I, is to apply three theories to understand the effects of monetary incentives on goal attainment. Those three theories are expected utility theory, agency theory and expectancy theory. An additional related theory, social exchange theory, is examined to understand the effects of locus of control on goal attainment. A contribution of this study is a better understanding of the effect employee loyalty has on the monetary incentive – goal attainment relationship. Another contribution will be to study the effect employee loyalty has on the monetary incentive dishonest behavior relationship. This chapter reviews the previous research literature that has investigated monetary incentives, goal attainment, employee loyalty, locus of control, and dishonest behavior. The first section reviews theories used in monetary incentive research, such as agency theory, and expected utility theory as well as expectancy theory. The purpose of this section is to bring to light the accumulated knowledge found in monetary incentive research and show why there is a need to extend the research on monetary incentives. How valuable are monetary incentives to enhance goal realization? This question’s answer is expanded on by measuring the locus of control of employees to the goal attainment relationship. In addition, measuring employee loyalty to see the effect that employee loyalty has on moderating the goal attainment and dishonest behavior relationships with monetary incentives. These measurements will help analyze the impact that monetary incentives really have on goal attainment. The analysis will help determine 13 how valuable employee loyalty can be and the effect employee loyalty has on goal attainment when measured with the monetary incentives and dishonest behavior relationships. The second section reviews monetary incentive literature as it relates to goal attainment. The purpose of this section is to bring to light the limited research and studies we have on understanding the impact of locus of control and loyal employees on goal attainment. Overall, this review shows a need to further understand and study the effects of monetary incentives on goal attainment and the effects of employee loyalty. 2.1 Goal Attainment Theories For monetary incentives to be accepted as a positive force in goal attainment, performance improvement must be measured by results or outcomes instead of a process or an input. The industry must be one in which there are outputs or deliverables that increase firm profit, such as the financial industry. For example, industries that have resistance to change or has constraints to change like government entities, monetary incentives or performance related pay can fail (Spano & Monfardini, 2018). Employees perform better if they possess the necessary knowledge and skills, desire to do the work, and are adequately motivated (Ponta et al., 2020). The aspect that makes it hard to interpret the relationship between incentive and performance is linked to the identification of the contribution of individual employees (Ponta et al., 2020). According to Ponta et al., (2020), it is known that economic motivation is one of the most used stimuli to improve the firm’s performance. Focusing on the relationship between monetary incentives and employees’ performance, one of the most important 14 aspects is performance related pay. Employees work harder if they value the monetary rewards and trust that those awards are related to their increased efforts. Expected Utility Theory (EUT). Expected utility theory (EUT) states that decision makers (DM) choose between risky or uncertain prospects by comparing their expected utility values (Mongin, 1997). Early theoretical developments in economics advocated that individuals make decisions by ranking the choices based on perceived utility and select the one that provides the most usefulness (Stigler, 1950). Researchers went back to the 1700s and found from that era that individuals place subjective values (utilities) on monetary outcomes and the value of a gamble is the expectations of these utilities (Yaqub et al., 2009). Expected utility theory (EUT) generalized this concept of utility maximization under the microeconomic paradigm of rationality and served as a foundation to model decision-making with risky choices or uncertain outcomes (Mongin, 1997). Apart from the assumption of a rational economic agent who makes consistent decisions by objectively valuing the expected utility of probable outcomes, EUT has been academically evaluated as both a “positive” and a “normative” theory (Mongin, 1997). Therefore, even if it is primarily known as a descriptive theory based on observations – “How do individuals actually make decisions,” and consequently applied to make predictions; economists have appraised the EUT as prescriptive, “How should individuals ideally make decisions” (Sapre, 2021). There is a longstanding debate regarding the effectiveness of financial incentives in improving work performance (Robinson & Farkas, 2021). Robinson and Farkas (2021) continued to find in their study that task attributes and personal preferences moderate the 15 effects of monetary incentives on performance. Thus, monetary incentives may have a positive effect on goal attainment, but the incentive-performance relationship may have a bigger impact on job tasks, compensation and hiring ability. Yaqub et al. (2009) explains the origin and history of the EUT very well. In the 17th century, the efforts to describe rational behavior in mathematical terms ran into problems with the discovery of the St. Petersburg paradox. Daniel Bernoulli and Gabriel Cramer were the first to solve and explain the St. Petersburg paradox in 18th century. The solution to this paradox revealed that the value of a gamble is not, in general, equal to its expected value. Individuals place subjective values (utilities) on monetary outcomes and the value of a gamble is the expectations of these utilities (Bernoulli, 1738/1954). Since Bernoulli’s statement (or theory) was based on a cardinal (interval) utility scale, it failed to attract much attention from economists until the 1950s when economists were more interested in developing ordinal theories. John von Neumann, a German-Hungarian mathematician, and Oskar Morgenstern, an Austrian economist, were the first scientists who formally proved that EUT was a viable theory for rational decision making and that it can be derived from certain axioms. They propounded that any “normal” preference relation over a finite set of states can be written as an expected utility. That is why it is also called the von Neumann-Morgenstern utility. The theory became important as it was developed shortly after the Hicks-Allen “ordinal revolution” of the 1930s; and it revived the idea of cardinal utility in economic theory. Although the economic and simple tractability of EUT cannot be criticized, a crucial question is whether it provides a sufficiently accurate representation of actual choice behavior. 16 Agency Theory. In the economic perspective, agency theory assumes that agents are self-interested and risk averse and recommends monetary incentives as a means of motivating increased effort and improvements in performance (Eisenhardt, 1989). Agency theory views the firm as a complex nexus of contracts where authority to perform specialized tasks is delegated by the principal (typically managers) to agents (the employees) with the expectation that the agent will act in the best interest of the firm (Eisenhardt, 1989). However, there are numerous studies where the principals (managers) may be acting in their own personal best interest (i.e., to get a monetary incentive for themselves) and thus can affect the agent’s (employee) performance and their actions may not be in the best interest of the firm. For agency theory to be shown as a factor in monetary incentives, both parties must be acting in the best interest of the firm. Expectancy Theory. Expectancy theory posits that financial incentives will illicit increased effort only if the individual believes that there is a high probability of receiving increased results from expending more effort (Vroom, 1964). Vroom’s (1964) instrumentality theory represents the first attempt to use cognitively oriented assumptions as the basis for a general theory of work motivation. Vroom defines motivation as the “force” impelling a person to perform a particular action, as determined by the interaction of (a) the person’s expectancy that the act will be followed by a particular outcome, and (b) the valence of that outcome whereas an outcome is positively valent when the person prefers attaining it to not attaining it. Khan et al. (2020) expanded on Vroom’s expectancy theory to closely relate to modern era and suggests that the behavior will develop certain attitudes among employees and that will lead to actions. Lawler and Suttle (1973) further expanded on Vroom by explaining in their research that the 17 expectancy model of effort leading to successful performance and the expectancy that this action will produce outcomes or results. Their expectancy model defines motivation as a function of the combination of the following variables: the perceived likelihood that effort toward a behavioral or task goal will lead to the successful accomplishment of that goal, the likelihood that the successful accomplishment of the goal will result in securing outcomes or rewards, and the valence of these outcomes. Lawler and Suttle (1973) further explain in their research that job behavior is a joint function of ability, role perceptions and motivation. Also, that expectancy theory proposes a causal relationship between expectancy attitudes and motivation. Further explained by Lawler and Suttle (1973) is that there needs to be better measures for motivation. Theoretically, monetary incentives work by increasing effort, which, in turn, leads to increases in performance. It is critical to determine how to maximize the effectiveness of monetary incentives. Expectancy theory describes in detail the mechanism through which monetary incentives are presumed to lead to increases in effort (Bonner & Sprinkle, 2002). In expected utility theory, the employee will receive a reward for accomplishing the task. In expectancy theory the employee will see an increased result for their increased performance (Bonner & Sprinkle, 2002). Based on expectancy theory, Robinson and Farkas (2021) concluded that individual perceptions of job characteristics and performance feedback jointly affect effort and performance and they provided evidence in their study that decline in performance is associated with dissatisfaction with prior performance. An easier explanation for expectancy theory is that the employee will be more motivated to perform 18 and accomplish the goal and therefore attain the monetary incentive if there is a positive relationship between their effort and their performance to attain that monetary incentive. Decline in performance can be attributed to their personal perception. If one attains the monetary incentive but the performance was tedious or viewed by the employee as dissatisfactory, then the continuance of that performance to attain further monetary incentives may be viewed as not worth the effort based on their personal perception. The use of financial incentives and, in particular, monetary incentives, is based primarily on the theoretical propositions of expectancy theory, whereas expectancy theory is an analytical tool to study how a reward system motivates the employees (Ponta et al., 2020). Employees modify their behavior based on their estimation of anticipated outcomes and the motivation for work is related to a series of causes and effects: first, persons should trust that their increased efforts will translate into a better performance; second, a satisfactory performance should lead to a wanted reward; third, the reward should fulfill a significant need for the individual. Finally, the desire to fulfill this need should be strong enough to make the initial efforts worthwhile (Ponta et al., 2020). 2.2 Monetary Incentives Monetary incentives are rewards given to employees or individuals for excellent performance or attaining their goal. They are a management tool to motivate employees and increase performance levels (Khan et al., 2020). Examples include a bonus, salary increase, commission or paid time off. These monetary incentives are used to motivate workers to increase performance. Motivating workers is a crucial dimension of labor 19 relationships that has been studied at length (Corgnet et al., 2015). In the economic literature, the principal-agent paradigm has emphasized the importance of monetary incentives as the most effective way to induce workers to exert effort (Corgnet et al., 2015). Corgnet et al. (2015) explain in their literature review that specific and difficult goals, perceived as attainable, lead to greater performance than vague and easy goals. Second, workers are more motivated or more committed to attain goals when they perceive their goals as being relevant and difficult to attain. Finally, goals are shown to increase workers’ persistence to exert effort. Corgnet et al. (2015) references Locke and Latham’s (2006) research on goal setting and expand on the incentives used to attain those goals. If the goals are relevant to the employees, the employees will increase effort to attain the goals and incentives (Corgnet et al., 2015). Gneezy and Rustichini (2000) found in their research that although performance increases with significant monetary compensation, small monetary incentives may undermine performance compared to a situation with no compensation at all. Pokorny (2008) reported experimental evidence that very high monetary incentives can also decrease performance as that research showed an increase in lying to attain the goal. Thus, it is not only the case that low rewards can do worse than no rewards at all; very high incentives may also have a detrimental effect on workers’ motivation (Corgnet et al., 2015). Robinson and Farkas (2021) found that motivation and performance are also grounded in economic agency theory, which assumes that agents are self-interested and risk adverse; they recommend monetary incentives as a means of motivating increased 20 effort and improvements in performance. They continued their research to show that the expectancy theory posits that financial incentives will elicit increased effort only if the individual believes that there is a high probability of receiving rewards from expending more effort. Every experienced executive knows the importance of rewarding good performance and how difficult it is to design an incentive system that works. (Locke, 2004). Locke further explains that the original idea of monetary incentives, or pay for performance, is to foster performance by making rewards contingent on employee performance (Locke, 2004). In order to provide a significant monetary incentive to accomplish the goals, there must be a difference between the reward for attaining the goal and not attaining the goal. This leaves no ambiguity about what is required of the person to receive the significant bonus (Locke, 2004). In Nnubia’s (2020) work, research data demonstrated that it is obvious that monetary incentives play a vital role in increasing employee performance. This is in line with the view of Jack Welch who stated, “If you pick the right people and give them the opportunity to spread their wings – and put compensation and incentive as a carrier behind it – you almost don’t have to manage them” (Nnubia, 2020, p. 15). However a 2003 Wall Street Journal article stated that in 83 percent of companies with a monetary incentive system, the monetary incentive system did not work at all or was only somewhat successful (Locke, 2004). Locke (2004) researched stretch goals with a strong incentive to reach a goal. However, he also found that there was considerable temptation to think short term, and therefore create short cuts. It has been said that it is better to have no incentive system at all, than to have a bad one (Locke, 2004). Bad 21 incentive plans encourage people to do wrong things in the wrong way and goals should be set for the desired outcomes (Locke, 2004). Another issue with the level of monetary incentive attributed to the goal is that the level of incentive can create and increase competition. This competition can significantly increase dishonest behaviors as employees strive to attain the monetary incentive and recognition. Recent studies and reviews on the topic of monetary incentives for goal attainment have concluded that financial incentives are positively related to performance (Glaser et al., 2017). These studies have also noted that the underlying belief of these monetary incentive programs is that they only produce positive effects. In Glaser et al. (2017), researchers studied the size of the incentive and whether the size or amount of the incentive spurred more intense aggression to attain the incentive. Their study found that the incentive is related to more negative interpersonal behavior and aggression. They concluded that pay for performance, or monetary incentives, increases competitiveness and therefore increases the likelihood of unhealthy competitive behavior. Research also found that in the accounting field, monetary incentives positively affected motivation and performance for those that did not lack the skill needed (Bonner & Sprinkle, 2002). In their findings, Daramola and Daramola (2019), found that monetary incentives were a motivational factor for performance. Their study showed the employees should be included in the discussion of what the motivating incentives should be. Nnubia (2020) conducted their research on a manufacturing firm and the study revealed a positive association between salary, wage, commission, and job performance. 22 Ponta et al., (2020) conducted their research on a public administration company and determined that monetary incentives have a positive impact on employee performance. Khan et al. (2020) found in their study of hospital staff that monetary incentives and job performance are positively related to each other. Furthermore, their findings suggest that there is a positive association between monetary incentives and employee loyalty which indicates that an increase in the monetary incentives, increases the motivation level of the employees, and makes them loyal to their organization (Khan et al., 2020). Al-Belushi and Khan (2017) suggest that organizations need to provide the right kind of monetary incentive to their employees because it will increase their satisfaction level and enhance their loyalty towards the organization. Idowu et al. (2019) concluded that a sufficient incentive package is required to maintain the loyalty of employees and increase job performance. 2.3 Locus of Control Locus of control emerged from Rotter’s (1954, 1966) seminal work on social learning theory. Social learning theory posits that individuals learn by observing the events that occur around them in a manner that ultimately influences behavior (Rotter, 1954). In this learning process, individuals develop expectancies that specific behaviors will create particular results (Rotter, 1966). Reinforcement acts to strengthen an expectancy that a behavior or outcome will be followed by more reinforcement in the future (Rotter, 1966). According to Rotter (1966), a relatively stable individual difference (i.e., locus of control) emerges over time that pertains to the extent to which individuals 23 perceive a causal relationship between behavior and rewards. Although some individuals perceive that their behavior and personal attributes drive outcomes (i.e., an internal locus of control), others develop a general sense that external forces govern outcomes (Galvin et al., 2018). Specifically, these unique characteristics shape the extent to which individuals attribute their ability to achieve future success or failure to their own actions (Galvin et al., 2018). Locus of control involves an assessment of the environment and external rewards (Rotter, 1966; Spector, 1982). In fact, Johnson et al. (2015) concluded that locus of control may be a fundamental evaluation of the environment rather than the self. As social learning theory would suggest, a locus of control orientation is acquired and strongly influenced by childhood experiences (Galvin et al., 2018). Locus of control is a personality trait that represents the extent to which people believe that the rewards they receive in life can be controlled by their own personal actions (Wang et al., 2010). Antecedents of locus of control are thought to revolve around early life influences, including parenting style, socioeconomic circumstances, and childhood experiences. Social learning theory, which provides the theoretical foundation for locus of control, asserts that reinforcement alters (i.e., strengthens or weakens) expectancy of certain behaviors or events (Rotter, 1966; Spector, 1982). Although the exact genesis of locus of control is not precisely understood, research suggests children begin to interpret control over their experiences as they consider the source of reinforcement (Galvin et al., 2018). As social learning theory would suggest, a locus of control orientation is acquired and strongly influenced by childhood experiences. However, little is known about how physiological or social factors experienced in one’s youth contribute to the loci 24 experiences as an adult (Galvin et al., 2018). These perceptions result in a generalized belief regarding “the causal relationship between one’s own behavior and its consequences,” which ultimately influences the actions of the individual (Rotter, 1966, p. 2). In essence, social learning theory suggests that over time individuals conclude what type of behavior will lead to desired or undesired results, and develop expectations of future exchanges (Galvin et al., 2018). Locus of control, a concept that has long played a role in psychology, is among the fundamental personality characteristics economists examine (Heywood et al., 2017). Heywood et al. (2017) further expands on locus of control by stating those individuals with more focus believe that investing in human capital has a higher return. Thus, such individuals perform better in school and are more likely to complete high school and attend college. They are also more likely to make long-term investments in personal health (Heywood et al., 2017). Motivation largely depends on this perception of the extent of control. If individuals believe that they cannot produce the desired effects, they have virtually no motivation to put forth effort (Bandura, 1991). Thus, as important as incentives can be, they need not be synonymous with motivation (Cobb-Clark, 2015). From a psychological viewpoint, workers’ decisions reflect more than the extrinsic motivation to earn more money (Heywood et al., 2017). Workers are also intrinsically motivated by the need to feel competent. Workers derive feelings of pride, self-worth, and self-esteem from successfully accomplishing tasks and achieving goals even if wage goals are irrelevant (Heywood et al., 2017). Heywood et al.’s (2017) study that recognized basic personality traits as drivers of economic choices has been hailed as 25 an important addition to the perspective of labor economists and others interested in worker behavior. Among these traits, seen as largely fixed in grown adults, locus of control seems central for understanding the sorting of workers across jobs. Early scholarship demonstrated that locus of control was a key predictor of various work‐related outcomes, ranging from job attitudes and affect to motivation and behavior (Galvin et al., 2018). In organizational settings, rewards or outcomes include promotions, favorable circumstances, salary increases and general career advancement (Spector, 1988). In addition to its role as an antecedent, research literature also posits locus of control as a moderator. Extant work reveals that one’s locus orientation can alter the effects of both personal and environ-mental factors (Galvin et al., 2018). For instance, the influence of workload on stress and organizational constraints on counterproductive behavior is higher among individuals with high locus of control (Galvin et al., 2018). Strong intrinsic motivation characterizes those with internal locus of control, and reinforcement is central to the effort–outcome link in expectancy theory (Galvin et al., 2018). Expectancy value theory (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) posits that motivation to engage in behavior is a function of expectations and valences. Individuals with high locus of control expect success and that, in turn, motivates their behavior (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002). Locus of control also has a strong, positive relationship with other work‐related attitudes, such as organizational satisfaction and affective commitment (Wang et al., 2010). This construct captures an individuals’ sense of control over work‐related 26 outcomes. Work locus of control generally predicts organizational‐related outcomes more effectively than general locus of control (Wang et al., 2010). Locus of control positively relates to motivation-related variables, including psychological empowerment, self-efficacy, self-esteem, and intrinsic task motivation (Johnson et al., 2008). This relates to expectancy theory in goal attainment. Whereas the motivation to work harder and attain the goals and engage in behaviors to attain the goals is also a function of the expectancy theory. Johnson et al. (2015), also found that locus of control has shown relationships with behavioral outcomes such as job performance, in addition to positively relating to volitional behaviors (i.e., the act of making your own decisions without others’ influence). This suggests that locus of control tends to be associated with positive attitudes, behavior, and well-being in the workplace (Galvin et al., 2018). Padmanabhan (2021) conducted a study on locus of control and job satisfaction. That research was designed to determine if there was a difference between males and females in addition to other measures, such as absenteeism and the negative effects of people that do not like their job. There was no significant difference between males and females concerning locus of control (Padmanabhan, 2021). There was also no significant difference between males and females for job satisfaction (Padmanabhan, 2021). 2.4 Dishonest Behavior Dishonest behavior in this study refers to the acts of the employee for their own benefit. Deviant behaviors are more severe and intentional to harm the organization. That 27 area is not studied in this research. I am only researching the employee that acts dishonestly for their own benefit to attain the monetary incentive. Employees are the strength of any organization and organizations with employees that have dishonest behaviors never last long (Bilal et al., 2020). It is necessary for management to identify and reduce dishonest behaviors to achieve the goals of the organization. According to Bennet and Robinson (2000), workplace dishonest behavior accounts for corporation losses in the billions. Almost daily there are media reports of dishonest behavior in the workplace. This dishonest behavior may harm the organization or its members or both. Individuals tend to overlook morally questionable aspects of goal-directed unethical behaviors or see them in a positive light when a goal is being pursued (Melnikoff & Bailey, 2018). Therefore, individuals might be somewhat “immune” to the negative impacts of these behaviors during the pursuit of a goal (Zhang et al., 2020). The conventional economic model assumes that people are willing to misreport private information if the material incentives of acting dishonestly outweigh those of acting honestly (Gerlach et al., 2019). The seminal work of Gneezy (2005) reports that people exhibit an aversion to lie, and that lying comes at a moral cost. The decision to lie is sensitive to a variety of factors, such as monetary consequences and strength of incentives and intrinsic cost (Mitra & Shahriar, 2020). Mitra and Shahriar’s (2020) study demonstrate how an unethical act or dishonest behavior is governed by the interplay between norm and benefit. Their findings showed that changes in the perception of a descriptive norm of 28 lying counteracts the opposing impact of changes in the benefit from lying (Mitra & Shahriar, 2020). Opportunism corresponds to the frailty of motive ‘which requires a certain degree of circumspection and distrust’ in the transaction cost economics scheme of things (Williamson, 1993, p. 97). Williamson’s (1993) insistence that opportunism be accorded co-equal status with bounded rationality does not imply that most economic agents are engaged in dishonest behavior practices most of the time. Rather, most economic agents are engaged in business-as-usual, with little or no thought to opportunism for dishonest behavior, most of the time (Williamson, 1993). Bradach and Eccles (1989), contend that mutual dependence between exchange partners promotes trust, which contrasts sharply with the argument central to transaction cost economics that dependence fosters opportunistic behavior. What transaction cost economics says, however, is that because opportunistic agents will not self-enforce openended promises to behave responsibly, efficient exchange will be realized only if dependencies are supported by credible commitments (Bradach & Eccles, 1989). A traditional incentive scheme works well if the only agency problem comes from the fact that management dislikes working hard (Williamson, 1993). The reason incentive schemes may be less effective under these conditions is that a very large incentive payment may be required to induce management to give up control (Williamson, 1993). The conventional economic model assumes that people are willing to misreport private information if the material incentives of acting dishonestly outweigh those of acting honestly (Gerlach et al., 2019). Also, the majority of those who behave dishonestly do so only to the extent that they can appear honest (Gerlach et al., 2019). The degree to 29 which individuals engage in dishonest behavior largely depends on personal and situational factors (Gerlach et al., 2019). Gerlach et al. (2019) showed that the degree and direction of dishonest behavior depends on situational factors and on personal factors. Examples of those factors can be personal medical expenses, holidays, or wanting a luxury item. In Pascual-Ezama et al. (2013), found that to reduce the cost of supervision and encourage workers to actively contribute, organizations often distribute incentives. Financial incentives are commonly used in the labor market, and the effect of those incentives varies depending on the type of task (Pascual-Ezama et al., 2013). However, incentives not only have effects on subjects’ motivation and performance but also on cheating (Pascual-Ezama et al., 2013). According to Nagin et al. (2002) employees are “rational cheaters.” They anticipate the consequences and firms respond with monitoring and incentive systems. Employees have many incentives for being dishonest. For example, bank sales employees should advise customers how to best invest their money. However, bank employees have sales targets that will affect the quality of their advice to increase sales and, in turn, their salary (Pascual-Ezama et al., 2013). According to Pascual-Ezama et al. (2013), with respect to the level of cheating and considering that there are different types of cheaters, the Theory of Self-Concept Maintenance suggests that workers typically solve this motivational dilemma adaptively by finding a balance or equilibrium between two motivating forces: financial incentive and positive self-concept. In this equilibrium, workers can derive some financial benefit from behaving dishonestly (but not too dishonestly) and still maintain their positive selfconcept in terms of being honest individuals. Pascual-Ezama et al. (2013) also found in 30 their study that the lack of supervision contributed to an increase in dishonesty as did the lack of motivation in workers. They continued to explain that when there were highly supervised workers, the dishonesty was very low. They also discovered that workers who had a lack of motivation in their job were highly dishonest. In Pascual-Ezama et al.’s (2013) conclusion, their results show the importance of a firm’s hiring procedure. Companies should place a high value on hiring employees who display a preference for the tasks they would need to complete during their job duties. They also concluded that because the performance of motivated employees ultimately benefits the organization, human resource professionals should understand and support antecedents that empower their most capable employees. In ending their conclusion, they surmised that monetary incentives could modulate the amount of cheating and in some cases can increase dishonesty. Performance or ability is rewarded by companies, the means of achieving these targets are often imperfectly monitored for the employees and provide a scope for dishonest acts on the part of the employee (Kaushik et al., 2022). A rational individual would commit a crime as long as the marginal benefit exceeds the marginal cost of committing that crime and will cheat to the maximum extent possible (Kaushik et al., 2022). Kaushik et al. (2022) found in their study that participants who were paid monetary incentives on average cheated by an amount significantly different from zero. Participants in their study were overall more dishonest when there was an independent payment involved. Also in their study, they found that individuals were not motivated to cheat when there was no financial reward for the goal. Mazar et al. (2008) found that an 31 individual’s desire to not appear unintelligent compared to their co-workers also drove dishonest behavior. Increasing economic satisfaction increased the magnitude of dishonesty in a study by Kaushik et al. (2022). Different levels of economic satisfaction showed a change in the magnitude of dishonesty; the level of dishonesty increased when increasing the level of economic satisfaction (Kaushik et al., 2022). Based on their study, that result will relate to an increase in goal attainment based on an increase in opportunity for dishonest behavior as the monetary incentive increases. Kaushik et al. (2022) concluded that when the threat of detection was zero (or very marginal) and there was an incentive, the level of cheating increased higher than the level of no incentive. They also concluded that decreasing the probability of detection of cheating to zero significantly increases the magnitude of cheating for the individuals that are paid the financial incentive. In Le Maux et al. (2021), their study analyzed both theoretically and empirically how monetary incentives affect dishonesty. Dishonesty is a major concern in modern societies. Dishonesty is at the heart of the principal-agent problem, and can cause substantial damage by eroding trust, creating uncertainty, reducing efficiency, and being harmful for collaboration (Le Maux et al., 2021). A study by Kajackaite and Gneezy (2017) showed that dishonesty increases with payoffs (incentives) increasing further increases when the concern about being exposed as a liar or cheat are removed. Le Maux et al. (2021) varied the amount of incentive in their study. They also observed and measured the dishonesty on an individual level. They concluded from their study that dishonesty rises over time in all payoff amounts (Le Maux et al., 2021). 32 Honesty is a moral virtue that is consistently endorsed as desirable across societies, yet we choose not to be honest from time to time (Mitra & Shahriar, 2020). The dilemma arises because dishonesty often serves our self-interest at the expense of our morality and may even cause material loss to others and our decisions that involve moral judgement are greatly influenced by social norms (Mitra & Shahriar, 2020). In Mitra and Shahriar (2020), they studied the decision-maker’s propensity to lie and the attributes that influenced that decision. They studied how monetary incentives influenced lying and the perception of the descriptive norm of lying (i.e., the belief about how likely others are to lie in that same situation). Specifically, they studied how lying behavior is influenced by two simultaneous and potentially counteracting changes: the benefit from lying is lowered (raised), and the decision-maker is intervened to believe in a higher (lower) propensity to lie among peers (Mitra & Shahriar, 2020). Mitra and Shahriar (2020) indicated from their study that a law-and-order policy aimed at curbing dishonest behavior via reducing incentives may not be sufficient when a society or organization operates under the norm of a high level of dishonesty. Kajackaite and Gneezy (2017) did a study on how cheating behavior is affected by incentives. The economic consequences of cheating are large, and hence, understanding the factors that influence the decision to lie is important in order to understand many economic behaviors (Kajackaite & Gneezy, 2017). Their study was to better understand how the size of the incentive affects behavior. As incentives were increased, the lying increased as it became more beneficial (Kajackaite & Gneezy, 2017). In Kajackaite and Gneezy (2017), they showed that when they modified the standard to eliminate the concern about being exposed as a liar, the participants react to 33 the incentives to lie. Their results showed that incentives increase cheating and more so where the concerns about being exposed are eliminated. When benefit from lying increases, more people choose to lie (Kajackaite & Gneezy, 2017). Their findings have important implications for constructing policies to reduce cheating, such as when people are less concerned about the chance of being exposed, it might make sense to reduce the incentives to cheat (Kajackaite & Gneezy, 2017). 2.5 Employee Loyalty Employees are considered the core of an organization and the success or failure of the organization is attributed to the performance of the employees (Murali et al., 2017). Loyalty can be defined as “a strong tie that binds an employee to his/her company even when it may not be economically sound for him/her to stay there” (Logan, 1984). Therefore, it is important for the organization to attract and retain their talented employees as talented employees influence the performance of the organization in a positive manner (Khan et al., 2020). In this study, employee loyalty is being researched as an individual construct for the sake of the employee being loyal to the company. Organizational commitment is a separate construct and not being researched in this study. This study is researching the individual employee’s loyalty to the firm and other employees for the sake of their motivation to attain the goal. This study is not using organizational loyalty as a construct either. This study is only researching the employee’s individual loyalty to the firm as their motivation to attain the goal. 34 The economic performance of organizations is becoming ever more dependent on the participation, commitment, and, more generally, loyalty of their employees (Murali et al., 2017). Loyalty has become one of the vital concerns for organizations, especially in the context of the economic tensions related to the “psychological contract” between employers and employees (Murali et al., 2017). Employee loyalty is a deliberate commitment to further the best interest of one’s employer, even when doing so may demand sacrificing some aspect of one’s self-interest beyond what would be required by one’s legal and other moral duties (Elegido, 2013). An employee’s loyalty to the occupation, his/her emotional investment, and the regularity of his/her commitment to the organization, are key factors that determine the longevity and the performance of organizations (Bakker & Schaufeli, 2008). The quality of employees, their competencies, loyalty, and commitment are extremely important for business performance achievement (Antoncic & Antoncic, 2011). According to Antoncic and Antoncic (2011), long-term business objectives of the company can be achieved when employee loyalty can be established. In practice, it often happens that the employer expects or requires its employees to be loyal but fails to provide a positive atmosphere at work; such attempts to obtain loyalty can be almost always far from successful. The main objective of creating the environment of employee loyalty is to achieve a situation in which employees will knowingly and without coercion become committed, accept responsibilities, and pursue them at their own best efforts. To achieve employee loyalty in the company, a company must take good care of employees (Antoncic & Antoncic, 2011). A sense of belonging is associated with confidence and accepting the objectives and values of the firm and it is accompanied by the employees’ 35 willingness and commitment to the efforts (Antoncic & Antoncic, 2011). Employee loyalty can contribute to greater efficiency, better business results, firm growth, reduced employee turnover, etc. Loyal employees also contribute to the creation of the image that the company has toward its environment and outside stakeholders, such as customers. The trust of employees in the company very importantly defines the employee welfare at work and satisfaction with work. Internal service quality can influence employee satisfaction, employee loyalty and productivity. Employees are key to achieving the internal quality of service in the company and hence for the business results of enterprises (Antoncic & Antoncic, 2011). Hart and Thompson (2007) elaborate in their study that in the workplace, individuals must adopt a fluid approach to assessing the demands to be loyal as contexts and relationships change. Without addressing this fluidity, it is difficult if not impossible to predict behavioral or attitudinal outcomes of people’s perceptions that others have been loyal or disloyal (Hart & Thompson, 2007). Kyle LaMalfa (2007) pointed out that employers need to understand why employees are emotionally connected to the business. That connection is usually much more than salaries, training, or benefits. Research has shown that emotionally connected employees are the best employees as they are engaged and productive, and they feel authorized and appreciated (Murali et al., 2017). Employee loyalty is critical for organizations as continuous turnover can be very expensive (Reichheld, 2006). If employees feel like the organization is listening to them, recognizing them for their contributions, they will more likely be loyal to the company (Murali et al., 2017). 36 In Murali et al.’s (2017) study, they were able to expand on how loyalty acts as a key factor in performance evaluation of an employee and sees loyalty from an employee’s perspective. Employees in general with increasing age consider themselves loyal to their organization. Compared to the employees in the lower age band those in the higher age band are more satisfied working for their current company. Murali et al.’s (2017) concluded that organizations that have long time employees understand the need for employee loyalty to the organization. Examples they found in organizations showed that individuals who are single or are not settled with their family in a particular city would be more likely to change their current job when they get better job prospects elsewhere. This was rarely seen in the employees who are married and are settled in a city (Murali et al., 2017). They further elaborated that from the employee’s perspective, loyalty is seen as a factor which may not necessarily bring them monetary benefits but would make the drawbacks in them (Murali et al., 2017). Pandey and Khare (2012) found in their study that employees are a vital resource for nearly all organizations, especially since they represent a significant investment in terms of locating, recruiting, and training let alone salaries, healthcare plans, bonuses, etc. The longer an employee works for a company the more valuable they become. They continued in their findings that employee loyalty to the organization has sometimes been viewed as an attitude. However, it is not so much an attitude (or thought component) that is important in organizations, but rather it is the bottom-line action component (Pandey & Khare, 2012). Thus, organizations need to invest in employees and enhance their performance through higher level of motivation and commitment (Khan et al., 2020). Organizations 37 need to include financial incentives and rewards such as profit sharing, bonus, promotion, and stock ownership in their strategy to maintain the relationship and increase their employee’s loyalty (Saleem, 2011). Employee loyalty is the relative strength of an individual’s identification with and involvement in a particular organization (Mowday et al., 1982). According to Mowday et al. (1979), this behavior can be characterized by three related factors — strong belief and acceptance of the organization’s goals and values, a willingness to exert considerable effort on behalf of the organization, and a strong desire to maintain membership in the organization. Satisfied employees will become loyal when they perceive their organization as offering the opportunities to learn, grow, and at the same time providing a clear established career path they can pursue in the organization (Pandey & Khare, 2012). Training and development are the biggest factors that lead to employee loyalty and to be committed employees look forward to the opportunities of continuous learning to improve their skills and knowledge (Pandey & Khare, 2012). Benefits of employee loyalty include trust, identification, commitment, participation, and attachment. Other benefits recently have included increasing profit, improving quality, reducing turnover, and increasing reputation of the organization (Khan et al., 2020). Today employers are more inclined towards employee loyalty because of its linkages with attendance and organizational citizenship and are frequently searching for new methods of promoting loyalty and as a result, employee loyalty decreases the turnover and old approaches are no longer efficient (Khan et al., 2020). Loyalty is 38 dependent upon the satisfaction of the employee which means that satisfied employees are more loyal than dissatisfied employees and loyalty is about showing pride in an organization and not complaining about the organization (Khan et al., 2020). 39 CHAPTER III MODEL DEVELOPMENT AND HYPOTHESES In the previous chapters, I discussed the theories that affect goal attainment. The four antecedents discussed were monetary incentives, locus of control, dishonest behavior, and employee loyalty. Goal attainment is affected by these four antecedents. I posit that once a goal is in place, monetary incentives increase goal attainment. My study researches the effect of employee loyalty and locus of control on goal attainment. My study addresses how monetary incentive increases goal attainment and by showing how employee loyalty affects that monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship. Finally, I investigate how employee dishonest behavior impacts goal attainment. My research posits that dishonest behavior positively mediates the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship. Monetary incentives increase goal attainment based on the research in the literature review. I argue that a monetary incentive increases goal attainment but that this effect will be reduced when employees are highly loyal. Thus, I hope to show how employee loyalty negatively affects the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship. Monetary incentives may also increase the likelihood that employees will be dishonest. This dishonest behavior could result in fraudulently attaining the goal. However, I posit that employee loyalty attenuates the monetary incentive to dishonest behavior relationship to. This study hopes to show how employee loyalty positively affects goal attainment and negatively affects dishonest behavior as well as how important locus of control is for goal attainment. 40 My model, as shown in Figure 1, posits that adding a monetary incentive increases the likelihood of goal attainment. My model also shows that locus of control has a positive effect on goal attainment directly without the influence of a monetary incentive. My model also shows that a monetary incentive has less of an effect on goal attainment with loyal employees. Additionally, the monetary incentive has a positive influence on dishonest behavior to attain the goal and therefore that dishonest behavior has a positive effect on goal attainment. Lastly monetary incentives have a minimal effect on dishonest behavior when employee loyalty is high. Hypotheses are developed to show these variables influences on the goal attainment relationship. My variables and the contribution of these variables are explained below. Figure 1. Research model of hypothesized relationships showing the influences of the variables on goal attainment. 41 3.1 Locus of Control Locus of Control stems from the social learning theory (Rotter, 1954). Social learning theory posits that individuals learn by observing the events that occur around them in a manner that ultimately influences behavior (Rotter, 1954). Locus of control is a fundamental individual difference variable that reflects individuals’ beliefs about the degree of control they have over events in their lives (Galvin et al., 2018). Locus of control has been conceptualized as “the extent to which people believe that the rewards they receive in life can be controlled by their own personal actions” (Wang et al., 2010, p. 761) and captures whether individuals “attribute the cause or control of events either to themselves or to the external environment” (Spector, 1982, p. 482). Using this concept of locus of control, people with high locus of control work hard enough on their own to attain the goals set by management. According to Galvin et al. (2018), locus of control positively relates to motivation. These employees with high locus of control want to be successful and are self-motivated and work harder to attain the goals. Thus: Hypothesis 1: Locus of control is positively associated with goal attainment. 3.2 Monetary Incentives As previously mentioned, monetary incentives are rewards in the form of bonus, raise, commission or financial gain given to individual employees for excellent performance or for attaining their goal through money (Ponta et al., 2020). Thus, monetary incentives have value to increase performance and effort to attain the goal. Monetary incentives for employees are studied under expected utility theory (Bonner & 42 Sprinkle, 2002). Expected utility theory (EUT) posits that the employee chooses between risky or uncertain prospects by comparing expected utility or outcomes (Mongin, 1997). These monetary incentives are used to motivate workers to increase effort. Motivating workers is a crucial dimension of labor relationships that have been studied at length (Corgnet et al., 2015). The prime purpose of a monetary incentive is to motivate the employees and encourage them to excel in their job performances (Al-Belushi & Khan, 2017). According to Nnubia (2020), modern corporate organizations have deemed it imperative to incorporate an effective monetary incentive plan for workers as part of their corporate goals and objectives. This is believed to shape a work force focused on strategic performance goals with the capability of achieving them. This positively and significantly influences the overall corporate performance. The prime purpose of a monetary incentive towards successful accomplishment is to motivate employees and encourage them to excel in their job performances (Al-Belushi & Khan, 2017). Thus: Hypothesis 2: Monetary incentive is positively associated with goal attainment. 3.3 Organization Employee Loyalty Employees are considered the core of an organization and the success or failure of the organization is attributed to the performance of the employees (Murali et al., 2017). It is of prime importance that the employees are loyal to the organization and don’t actively search for other alternative opportunities (Pandey & Khare, 2012). Loyalty, as a general term can be defined as a person’s devotion or sentiment of attachment to a particular 43 object, which may be another person or a group of persons, an ideal, a duty, or a cause (Murali et al., 2017). A loyal employee works hard for the company to attain the company goals. Employee loyalty “is a deliberate commitment to further the best interests of one’s employer, even when doing so may demand sacrificing some aspect of one’s self-interest beyond what would be required by one’s legal and other moral duties” (Elegido, 2013, p. 497). The more loyal an employee is, the more that employee believes in the vision, culture and values of the company and continues to work hard and attain the goals. Loyalty towards one’s employer can increase an employee’s motivation to work (Elegido, 2013). I argue that employee loyalty moderates the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship, such that monetary incentive will not mean as much when employees are loyal simply because the loyalty towards the organization will replace the need for the monetary incentive. This would be because the monetary incentive does not have a significantly increased effect on goal attainment for loyal employees. Using motivational techniques is common practice and this study posits that the more loyal the employee the less the monetary incentives matter to attain the goal. Thus: Hypothesis 3: Employee loyalty negatively moderates the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship such that when employee loyalty is high, monetary incentives have less of an impact on goal attainment. 3.4 Dishonest Behavior Dishonest behavior is a major concern in society (Le Maux et al., 2021). Dishonest behaviors include cheating, fare-dodging, questionable research practices, tax 44 evasion, misrepresentation of output at work, and misuse of public funds amongst other fraudulent activities (Le Maux et al., 2021). The economic consequences of cheating are large, and hence, understanding the factors that influence the decision to lie is important to understand many economic behaviors (Kajackaite & Gneezy, 2017). People increase their level of acceptable dishonest behavior if presented with the right incentive (PascualEzama et al., 2013). Regarding incentives, both economic and social incentives motivate employees who enjoy their job and are more efficient, while at the same time, it seems that employees increase their band of acceptable dishonest behavior if presented with the right incentive (Pascual-Ezama et al., 2013). In short, this shows that people are more likely to do something dishonest if money is involved and the higher the amount of money, the more likely this type of behavior becomes. Thus: Hypothesis 4: Monetary incentive is positively associated with dishonest behavior. However, this dishonest behavior is not without purpose. Its purpose is to attain the goal in order to receive the monetary incentive. When an individual is determined to achieve a certain goal, the motivation to act morally may be temporarily inhibited as mental resources are diverted from moral standards to goal attainment (Barsky, 2008). When the benefit from dishonest behavior increases, more people choose to act dishonestly (Kajackaite & Gneezy, 2017). A person commits an offense if the expected utility to him exceeds the utility he could get by using his time and other resources at other activities. Some persons become 45 “criminals,” therefore, not because their basic motivation differs from that of other persons, but because their benefits and costs differ (Becker, 1968). A rational individual would commit a crime as long as the benefit exceeds the cost of committing the crime (Kaushik et al., 2022). People are willing to act dishonest if the incentives to act dishonest outweigh those of acting honestly (Gerlach et al., 2019). Therefore, when a monetary incentive is added to help motivate employees to attain the goal, there will be dishonest behaviors. Thus: Hypothesis 5: Dishonest behavior increases goal attainment. Individuals tend to overlook morally questionable aspects of goal-directed unethical behaviors or see them in a positive light when a goal is being pursued (Melnikoff & Bailey, 2018). Therefore, they may be somewhat “immune” to the negative impacts of these behaviors during the pursuit of a goal (Zhang et al., 2020). Thus, it seems clear that while performance is rewarded, the means of achieving those productivity targets are imperfectly monitored and provides an opportunity for dishonest acts (Kaushik et al., 2022). Performance pressure creates an expectation to perform at a certain level, and when this level cannot be met, it produces a conflict between what one is able to do (performance) and what one is expected to do. This conflict can be resolved by lying (Grover, 1993) or other dishonest behaviors. It seems reasonable that performance pressure and reward should support one another. Reward or performance pressure alone might be enough to lead some people to lie, and when combined, they provide even greater impetus to lie (Grover, 1993). Thus, the higher the monetary incentive as a 46 reward, the greater the pressure to achieve it. If dishonest behavior can attain that reward, then: Hypothesis 6: Dishonest behavior mediates the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship. However, when employees are very loyal, dishonest behavior may not be as prevalent. People view deceit as unethical and morally wrong, especially when it benefits the liar at the expense of others (Hildreth & Anderson, 2018). Employee loyalty is a significant attribute to gain a competitive advantage in any organization (Dutta & Dhir, 2021). Employee loyalty should also moderate the monetary incentive to opportunity for dishonesty relationship negatively. High employee loyalty acts like an ethical brake and as a monetary incentive is used to attain the goal, employee loyalty decreases the monetary incentive to opportunity for dishonest behavior relationship. A loyal employee acts and behaves for the greater good of the organization and they do not want the organization to look bad or fail (Hildreth & Anderson, 2018). The greater the loyalty of the employee the less likely that employee is to act dishonestly to attain the goal. Thus: Hypothesis 7: Employee loyalty negatively moderates the monetary incentive to dishonest behavior relationship, such that, when employee loyalty is high, there is less dishonest behavior. Below is Table 3.1 which lists the seven hypotheses and the path relationships. 47 Table 3.1. Hypothesized Path Relationships in the Study for the Constructs, Mediation and Moderation Hypothesis Path Relationship Hypothesis 1 Hypothesis 2 Hypothesis 3 Hypothesis 4 Hypothesis 5 Hypothesis 6 Hypothesis 7 LOC → GA MI → GA EL → moderates MI → GA MI → DB DB → GA DB → mediates MI → GA EL → moderates MI → DB Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. This table outlines the seven hypotheses used for this study. LOC and MI are measured to GA to show the impact that each construct has on GA. EL is used to see the impact employee loyalty has on moderating the GA and DB relationships with MI. DB is used to see the impact dishonest behavior has on GA and the moderating effect of DB on the MI to GA relationship. 48 CHAPTER IV METHODS This chapter sets forth the procedure used for evaluating the hypotheses. In the first section, the criteria used to select the participants is outlined. The data source is then explained followed by the sample size criteria and minimums. The next section describes the strategy used for the data collection followed by the survey development, and an overview of the sample. The fourth section explains the scales and how the validity of the scales is determined. The last section elaborates on the procedures used to determine the measurement of variables, establish reliability, validity, and determine the effect of biases. Finally, the hypotheses are evaluated. The study uses a survey randomly distributed using a Qualtrics survey and distributed by Prolific as a means make sure that the participants fit the criteria for the study. This is the best way to express a statement facing the participants in the financial industry to help determine the value they place on monetary incentives and researching their loyalty and locus of control. 4.1 Sample Criteria and Data Sources 4.1.1 Analytical Process Partial Least Squares-Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) is the analytical modeling technique used in this study. First, unlike other frequently used analytical techniques, PLS-SEM focuses on predicting the variance of the dependent variables when 49 assessing the entire model (Hair et al., 2011; Hair et al., 2021). Second, PLS-SEM enables researchers to assess multiple dependent variables simultaneously. Third, PLSSEM is the technique of choice when conducting exploratory research, which is the focus of this research. Finally, PLS-SEM is a non-parametric statistical method not requiring normally distributed data and therefore provide both flexibility of analysis and the ability to assess overly complex models (Hair, Black, et al., 2019; Hair et al., 2017). The survey was sent to 350 respondents. Using the pool of subjects from Prolific, data cleaning was very minimal. Only eight subjects were removed from the data pool. The final qualified sample consisted of 342 participants. 4.1.2 Data Sources The respondents received a Qualtrics survey from Prolific with statements asking about their salary, monetary incentive structure, loyalty, locus of control and dishonest behavior. The object is to see if their loyalty, locus of control and monetary incentive affects goal attainment. There are also questions pertaining to their years in the financial industry and with their current employer. There will be statements about their loyalty to their current company. There will also be statements to determine their locus of control and dishonest behavior. 4.1.3 Criteria The survey was developed using Qualtrics. The participant eligibility required they be over the age of twenty-five and understand the English language. The participants must be in the finance or financial sales sector where they have a base salary and attain monetary incentives for accomplishing their goal. Participation was voluntary and the participants were compensated for their time. All information provided is anonymous, 50 including the decisions to participate or not participate. There was no identifying information collected. To make sure the participants are engaged in the survey, there was several attention checks. The attention checks were in a statement that directs the respondent to “Please select Very Likely for this question” or some similar type of statement. 4.2 Survey Instrument Several scales developed in prior research were updated and adapted for clarification and a more concise outcome. The survey was pre-tested, and feedback was obtained. The pretest consisted of ten PhD students with a minimum “all but dissertation (ABD)” status, and five faculty members familiar with research design and the theoretical foundation of the research. I then tested with an online pre-test to obtain feedback. This enhanced the clarity of the survey and made sure it was no longer than 11 minutes. The survey included general demographic questions such as tenure at the company, tenure in the industry, gender, and age for all participants. There were several salary and length of employment categories. This method captures if their effort to attain the goal will increase when a monetary incentive is attached to that goal. Goal attainment scales were used from previous research. Locus of control scales and employee loyalty scales were used from previous research to help determine the level of employee loyalty and locus of control when measuring the effect of a monetary incentive attached to the goal. Dishonest behavior scales were used from previous research to evaluate the potential influence a monetary 51 incentive may have on dishonest behavior and if employee loyalty influences dishonest behavior. 4.3 Sample The sample for this research is United States employees in the financial sales or insurance sales industry. The website, Prolific, was utilized to pre-screen the sample and recruit respondents. The Prolific online platform maintains a panel with a total population of 123,645 eligible participants for this type of research. After inputting the control variables for age and industry, the total population of eligible respondents became 946. To ensure an appropriately sized sample, Hair et al.’s (2017) rule of ten observations for one variable was initially considered. The method recommends that a theoretical model with five variables requires a minimum of fifty observations. Similar recommended sample size guidelines based on the concept of power (Cohen, 1992) require a minimum of eighty-five observations to achieve a statistical power of 80% for the number of paths for 1% significance and a minimum R2 of 0.10. Both sample size guidelines were met by the sample of 342 used in this study. The final sample of financial sales employees who completed an online survey was delivered through Qualtrics. Table 4.1 provides the demographics of the sample. 52 Table 4.1. Demographics of Survey Respondents who are Financial Sales Employees Demographic Field Number Gender Female Male Other Age 25-29 30-39 40-49 50-59 60+ Ethnicity African American Asian Caucasian Hispanic Other Salary Less than 30K 31K-60K 61K-90K 91K-120K 121K-150K 151K-180K 181K-210K Greater than 210K Education Level High School Graduate Some College 2-year Degree 4-year Degree Professional Degree (MBA) Doctorate Demographic Field Education Level Mean 3.79 Percent 127 213 2 37 62 1 81 127 81 36 17 24 37 24 10 5 26 33 272 9 2 7.5 9.5 80 2.5 0.5 24 105 96 58 28 18 7 7 7 30.5 28 17 8 5.5 2 2 12 39 30 191 69 2 3.5 11 9 56 20 0.5 Std. Deviation 1.02 53 Variance 1.04 Count 342 4.4 Scales 4.4.1 Locus of Control The statements for this construct come from Spector (1988). The purpose of this construct is to get a more precise idea of the employee’s work ethic and behavior as an independent variable to goal attainment. Sample questions include “To make a lot of money you have to know the right people,” “A job is what you make of it,” and “Most people are capable of doing their jobs well if they make the effort.” These questions are measured on a six-point scale ranging from 1 = Disagree Very Much to 6 = Agree Very Much. 4.4.2 Employee Loyalty The statements for this construct are from Pandey and Khare (2012) and Dutta and Dhir (2021). The purpose of this construct is to determine the loyalty of the employees. This employee loyalty will be broken down by salary, length of time in industry, length of time at their current company and age of employee. This construct will help show as an independent variable how valuable loyalty to the company is when looking at monetary incentives as a motivator for goal attainment and to see how loyalty deters dishonest behavior to attain the goal. Sample questions include “I speak positively about my company to friends and relatives,” “I would like to stay with this company for the future,” and “I would not change companies if I got an offer.” These questions are measured on a five-point scale ranging from 1 = Strongly Agree to 5 =Strongly Disagree. 54 4.4.3 Monetary Incentives The statements for this construct are from Al-Belushi and Khan (2017). The purpose of this construct is to see how important monetary incentives are as a motivating factor to attain the goal. Sample questions are “Monetary incentives are important to me,” “Financial incentives encourage employees to produce more at my company,” and “Monetary incentives have a positive effect on my motivation at work.” These questions are measured on a five-point scale ranging from 1 = Strongly Agree to 5 =Strongly Disagree. 4.4.4 Dishonest Behavior The statements for this construct are from Kaptein (2008) and Bennett and Robinson (2000). The purpose of this construct is to help determine how dishonest behavior’s influence on goal attainment as an independent variable when monetary incentives are involved. This construct will show how employees become dishonest to obtain the monetary incentive and help show how employee loyalty helps deter dishonest behavior. Sample questions are “I would falsify or manipulate financial reporting information to attain the goal,” “I would engage in false or deceptive sales and marketing practices (e.g., creating unrealistic expectations) to gain the sale,” and “I accept inappropriate gifts, favors, entertainment, or kickbacks from customers.” These questions are measured on a five-point scale ranging from 1 = Never to 5 = Almost Always. 4.4.5 Goal Attainment The statements for this construct are from Khazanov et al. (2020). The purpose of this construct is the dependent variable. The questions in the survey will help show how the employees feel about attaining their workplace goal. Sample questions are “I expect 55 to master the tasks I undertake to reach my company goal,” “I acquire the ability to perform the tasks to reach my goal,” and “I achieve my personal goals.” These questions are measured on a five-point scale ranging from 1 = Not At All to 5 = Totally. 4.5 Bias, Validity, and Analysis 4.5.1 Common Method Bias When multiple constructs are measured by the same method, such as a survey, part of the covariance among the constructs may be attributed to the fact that the same method is used. Common method variance creates a divergence between the observed and true relationships (Doty & Glick, 1998) that can imply significant relationships that do not exist (Williams & Brown, 1994). One means of preventing common method bias is to use different response formats for the measurement of dependent and independent variables (Podsakoff et al., 2003). This study employs this technique. Some of the constructs are on a five-point scale and some on a six-point scale. There are different range answers on some of the fivepoint scale variables in order to prevent identical options with all the five-point variables. Other techniques for controlling common method bias include protecting respondent anonymity, counterbalancing question order, and improving scale items. All these design measures are incorporated into the current study. However, constructs measured by the survey may still be vulnerable to common method variance, which is a possible limitation of the current study. 56 4.5.2 Analysis To test the hypotheses, Partial Least Squared Structural Equation Modeling PLSSEM was utilized. SMART PLS software was used to test the measurement and structural model. 4.5.3 Pilot Testing To ensure the quality of survey responses, a pilot test was administered to a convenience sample (Hair et al., 2015). The pilot study targeted several professors at a university, sales associates at DHL Aviation, and sales associates at PNC Bank. While the sample size was small (n = 20), problematic items on the survey were identified and corrected. Once the pilot test was analyzed, the final survey instrument was more precise and yielded accurate results (Perneger et al., 2015). 57 CHAPTER V RESULTS 5.1 Analytical Process In this study, Partial Least Squares-Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) was the analytical modeling technique used. PLS-SEM focuses on predicting the variance of the dependent variables when assessing the entire structural model (Hair et al., 2011; Hair et al., 2021). PLS-SEM is the technique of choice when conducting exploratory research, which is the focus of this research, and when prediction is the primary statistical objective of the analytical process. Finally, PLS-SEM is a non-parametric statistical method not requiring normally distributed data and therefore provides both flexibility of analysis and the ability to assess highly complex models (Hair, Black, et al., 2019; Hair et al., 2017; Hair et al., 2021). The survey was sent to 360 respondents. Using the pool of subjects from Prolific, data cleaning was very minimal. Only four subjects failed the attention checks. Fourteen participants failed to answer enough questions or did not complete the survey and were eliminated. The final qualified sample consisted of 342 participants. 5.2 Data Analysis and Findings 5.2.1 Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling The theoretical research model examined in this research is complex and involves mediation and moderation. The application of Partial Least Squares Structural Equation 58 Modeling (PLS-SEM), therefore, facilitates a better understanding of the relationships proposed in the research (Hair & Sarstedt, 2020). The following sections outline the procedures followed. 5.3 Assessment of Measurement Model As described in Hair, Howard, and Nitzl (2020), PLS-SEM involves a two-step process. The first step is referred to as confirmatory composite analysis (CCA) and explores and confirms the measurement model. The second step examines the structural relationships and predictive ability of the theoretical model. The formative measurement model is evaluated per the CCA guidelines for the following criteria: Item loadings, composite reliability, average variance extracted, discriminant validity, nomological validity and predictive validity. 5.3.1 Data Distribution Each item in the study has a varying degree of departure from normality. There were 20 of the original 45 items that were considered highly skewed, falling outside the range -1 to +1 (Bulmer, 1979). All other items were moderately skewed or approximately symmetric. Whether positively or negatively skewed, every item fit within the acceptable range of -2 to +2 for structural equation modeling (Kline, 2011). The data is moderately leptokurtic or platykurtic for each item and well within the acceptable range of -7 to +7 (Hair, Black, et al., 2019). Slightly non-normal data distribution will not affect the study due to this research’s use of PLS-SEM (Hair, Howard, & Nitzl, 2020). 59 5.3.2 Common Method Variance This study uses contextual data from a single time period and is a cross-sectional design (Hair, Page, & Brunsveld, 2020). To reduce the likelihood of common method variance, the research design and questionnaire were executed through the application of a variety of scaling methods and sequencing based on guidelines by Podsakoff et al. (2003; 2012). 5.3.3 Estimate of Loadings, Significance, and Item Reliability Indicator validity was assessed by evaluating the size of the factor loadings. To capture sufficient variance from each item within the construct, Hair, Sarstedt, and Ringle (2019) suggest removing any items below the .708. There were five items that were removed but were very close as they were over .670 but below .708. Removing them increased the factor loadings of the remaining items and all constructs had at least three items that were greater than .708 (Hair, Risher, et al., 2019). The PLS algorithm was executed followed by initially screening to identify the items below .708. Indicator loadings met or exceeded .708 (Hair et al., 2021). The result was a total of 18 items being removed. After removal of these items, all outer loadings met recommended guidelines and were highly significant (p values < .05). By squaring the loadings, item reliability measures display the amount of variance shared by each item to the construct (Hair, Black, et al., 2019). The factor loadings are displayed in Table 5.1 60 Table 5.1. Factor Loadings of the Constructs in the Study Variable DB DB1 DB5 DB6 DB7 DB8 EL1 EL10 0.730 0.889 0.821 0.767 0.852 EL GA LOC MI 0.741 0.827 EL2 EL3 0.886 0.782 EL4 EL5 EL6 EL7 EL8 GA1 GA2 0.833 0.747 0.792 0.870 0.845 0.789 0.728 GA6 GA7 LOC2 LOC3 LOC4 LOC8 MI6 MI7 MI8 0.820 0.800 0.792 0.869 0.770 0.778 0.878 0.735 0.928 Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. 61 Table 5.2. Descriptive Statistics of the Variables Variable Monetary Incentive Dishonest Behavior Goal Attainment Employee Loyalty Locus of Control Minimum Maximum Mean Std Deviation 1 1 1 1 1 5 5 5 5 6 4.287 4.869 4.087 3.862 2.435 .764 .440 .769 .988 1.079 Note. N = 342. Table 5.3. Correlation Values of the Variables Showing Significance Level Variable DB EL GA LOC MI DB EL GA LOC MI EL x MI 0.235 0.292 -0.235 0.158 -0.082 0.524 -0.423 0.057 0.134 -0.467 0.189 -0.079 -0.072 -0.054 -0.119 Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. All correlations above .102 are significant at p < .05 and all correlations above .162 are correlated at p < .01. The highest correlations are those variables with employee loyalty. The highest correlation is between employee loyalty and goal attainment. There were some correlations that were the lowest, but still significant. The MI correlations were the ones on the lowest end. The moderating relationship correlation of EL moderating the MI to DB relationship was low and not significant. None of the effect VIFs were above 62 1.5, well within acceptable tolerances for ordinary least squares regression (Hair et al., 2021). 5.3.4 Composite Reliability The measure of composite reliability was evaluated next. The measures of both Cronbach’s alpha and composite reliability criteria minimums (> 0.70) were met for all constructs (Hair, Risher, et al., 2019). Cronbach’s alpha applies to reflective measurement models like this one (Hair et al., 2021). Averaging the correlations of the items within the construct, the most similar items were identified. There were no constructs that violated the composite reliability maximum of 0.95 that would indicate redundancy with the items of those constructs (Hair, Risher, et al., 2019). Table 5.4 outlines the internal consistency reliability measures for each construct. Table 5.4. Internal Consistency Assessments for each Construct to Show Items Within the Construct are Similar Variable DB EL GA LOC MI Cronbach’s alpha Composite reliability 0.871 0.936 0.796 0.816 0.829 0.885 0.940 0.809 0.823 0.947 Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. 5.3.5 Convergent Validity Convergent validity tests to make sure all of the items used for the construct are valid for that construct and correlate to the other items testing for that construct. Average 63 variance extracted (AVE) is the measure used to evaluate convergent validity (Hair et al., 2021). All constructs meet the AVE criterion of 0.5 or greater. These measures indicated the shared variance between the construct and their items. The AVE values for each construct are outlined in Table 5.5 below. Table 5.5. Average Variance Extracted for Convergent Validity for Each Construct to Show Shared Variance Between the Construct and Each Item for That Construct Variable AVE DB EL GA LOC MI 0.662 0.665 0.616 0.645 0.724 Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. 5.3.6 Discriminant Validity Discriminant validity measures the extent to which the constructs are representing and measuring distinctly different concepts (Hair, Risher, et al., 2019). For example, when measuring abstract constructs such as employee loyalty, it is important to determine that all constructs measure a different concept (Hair & Sarstedt, 2021; Henseler et al., 2015). Measuring individual items cross loadings is one way to assess the uniqueness of the construct. The Fornell-Larcker criterion is another recommended measurement of discriminant validity. The Fornell-Larcker criterion takes the square root of the average variance extracted (AVE) for each construct and should exceed the correlation to other 64 constructs when measuring distinct constructs (Fornell & Larcker, 1981). The variance from each item should be more highly contributed to the construct being measured (Chin, 1998). All items’ variances are highest on the intended constructs. The Fornell-Larcker criterion values are outlined in Table 5.6 below. Table 5.6. Fornell-Larcker Criterion for Discriminant Validity for Each Construct Variable DB EL GA LOC MI DB EL GA LOC MI 0.814 0.235 0.292 -0.235 0.158 0.815 0.524 -0.423 0.057 0.785 -0.467 0.189 0.803 -0.072 0.851 Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. The table above shows the off-diagonal values are all lower compared to the ondiagonal (bolded) values. The Fornell-Larcker criterion for discriminant validity is therefore met. Heterotrait-monotrait ratio (HTMT) is another measurement of discriminant validity. HTMT is a more rigorous metric than the Fornell-Larcker criterion to ensure that each construct uniquely captures the phenomenon. The HTMT criterion confidence levels were assessed after bootstrapping 5000 subsamples (Hair et al., 2021). All measurements met the rule of thumb below 0.900 (Hair, Risher, et al., 2019). The Heterotrait-Monotrait discriminant validity measurements are in Table 5.7. 65 Table 5.7. Heterotrait-Monotrait Discriminant Validity for the Measurement Model Variable DB EL GA LOC MI EL x DB DB EL GA LOC MI EL x DB EL x MI 0.256 0.335 0.273 0.156 0.763 0.087 0.583 0.484 0.071 0.133 0.147 0.570 0.189 0.137 0.091 0.118 0.068 0.063 0.076 0.121 0.051 Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. HTMT metrics are evaluated based on two recommended guidelines; 0.85 for concepts considered to be measuring diverse constructs and 0.90 for similar constructs (Hair, Sarstedt, & Ringle, 2019). Discriminant validity measurements are affected by the removal of items to meet the composite reliability maximum cutoffs. Since extensive literature review is used, this research will use the less conservative HTMT value of < 1 when considering discriminant validity (Franke & Sarstedt, 2019). All HTMT values are acceptable (< 1.0; (Franke & Sarstedt, 2019). 5.3.7 Nomological Validity Another assessment of construct validity is nomological validity (Hair et al., 2021). Correlation of construct scores should be consistent with the theoretical direction, size, and significance of the correlations. Reviewing the latent variable correlations, all nomological relationships are consistent with theory as supported by the literature review in Chapter III. 66 5.4 Evaluation of the Structural Model The following steps, consistent with the second step of the CCA process (Hair et al., 2021), will be used to evaluate the structural model. Evaluation of collinearity, examination of size and significance of path coefficient, R2 of endogenous variables, ƒ2 effect size, predictive relevance Q2, and PLSpredict prediction errors. 5.4.1 Assessment of Collinearity Variance inflation factor (VIF) is used to assess collinearity issues. The rule of thumb for variance inflation factor (VIF) is constructs have collinearity issues above the cut-off of 3.00 (Hair et al., 2021). The VIF statistics for each latent variable are below in Table 5.8. Table 5.8. Variance Inflation Factor for each Latent Variable to Show There are no Collinearity Issues Variable DB EL GA LOC MI EL x MI DB EL GA 1.024 1.118 1.274 1.020 1.035 1.249 1.040 1.046 Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. 67 5.4.2 Path Coefficients and Significance The path coefficients were calculated using the SmartPLS algorithm. Through bootstrapping 5,000 subsamples and one-tail test, path statistical significance was ascertained (Hair et al., 2021). Path coefficients and significance (p values) are labeled in Figure 2. All path coefficients were statistically significant. The path coefficient for the moderating effect of EL on the MI → DB was not significant as it had a p value of 0.158. The path coefficient for the moderating effect of EL on the MI → GA was significant (p = 0.005). Table 5.9 and Figure 2 display the strength of the relationships and their significance for this one-tail test. Table 5.9. Size and Significance of the Path Coefficients Relationship DB → GA EL → DB EL → GA LOC → GA MI → DB MI → GA EL x MI → DB EL x MI → GA Original Sample (O) 0.106 0.241 0.419 -0.274 0.133 0.129 -0.090 -0.123 Sample Mean (M) 0.105 0.244 0.422 -0.270 0.142 0.133 -0.103 -0.125 Standard Deviation (STDEV) 0.053 0.068 0.048 0.058 0.077 0.045 0.089 0.047 T Statistics P Values (|O/STDEV|) 2.005 3.519 8.649 4.740 1.731 2.901 1.002 2.601 0.023 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.042 0.002 0.158 0.005 Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. 68 Figure 2. Structural model of one-tail test results showing the strength of the relationships and their significance. 5.4.3 Coefficients of Determination (R2) To assess the in-sample predictive power of the structural model, the coefficients of determination (R2) are determined after reliability and validity (Hair & Sarstedt, 2021). The larger the R2, the more variation of the endogenous variable is explained respectively to the independent variables. Measures closer to 1 have higher predictive power while those closer to 0 have lower. Table 5.10 below outlines both measures, R2 and R2 adjusted for the dependent variables for this one-tail test. 69 Table 5.10. Coefficients of Determination for the Dependent Variables to Show the Predictive Power of the DV Variable R-square R-square adjusted DB GA 0.086 0.397 0.077 0.388 Note. DB = dishonest behavior; GA = goal attainment; N = 342. This table shows that DB is relatively weak when measuring predictive power. GA is low moderate for the measurement of predictive power (Hair & Sarstedt, 2021). 5.4.4 Effect Sizes (ƒ2) The ƒ2 statistic measures the model with exogenous variables included than excluded to determine their effect sizes upon the model (Hair, Risher, et al., 2019). Less than .02 indicates no effect, between .02 and 0.15 are small effects, between 0.15 and 0.35 are medium effects, and greater than 0.35 are large effects (Cohen, 1988). Table 5.11 illustrates the effect sizes. Table 5.11. Effect Sizes of Constructs for the Structural Equation Model to Show the Effect Each Construct as on the Model Variable DB EL GA LOC MI EL x MI DB EL GA 0.062 0.017 0.201 0.019 0.010 0.100 0.021 0.024 Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. 70 These results show that the path LOC → GA has a moderate effect. The paths DB → GA and MI → GA have small effects. The moderation path of EL to MI → DB has a small moderation effect. The moderation path of EL to MI → GA has a moderate effect. 5.4.5 PLSpredict This study posits that in-sample prediction is adequate. Future studies in goal attainment and dishonest behavior may enhance a model assessment metric. SmartPLS software does apply the PLSpredict to help researchers calculate an out of sample prediction by training the model on a selected part of the sample and predicting the other data on a second holdout sample (Shmueli et al., 2019). Following recommendations on holdout sample size to be equal to or greater than 30, PLSpredict folds were set to 20 ensuring that holdout samples of the 342 respondents would be adequate (Hair, Black, et al., 2019). The mean absolute error (MAE) measures the average magnitude of the errors in a set of predictions without considering their direction (over or under). The MAE is thus the average absolute differences between the predictions and the actual observations, with all the individual differences having equal weight. Another popular prediction metric is the root mean squared error (RMSE), which is defined as the square root of the average of the squared differences between the predictions and the actual observations. As the RMSE squares the errors before averaging, the statistic assigns a greater weight to larger errors, which makes it particularly useful when large errors are undesirable – as is typically the case in business research applications (Hair, Sarstedt, & Ringle, 2019). Using Shmueli et al.’s (2019) evaluation guidelines, all Q2 metrics were above zero as shown in Table 5.12. Since the prediction errors are highly symmetrically 71 distributed, the root mean squared error (RMSE) metrics for the PLS-SEM model and the naïve (linear) model were compared. The PLS-SEM model has a medium predictive power since a majority of indicators for the endogenous variables have less error compared to the naïve LM model (Manley et al., 2021; Shmueli et al., 2019). Table 5.12. PLSpredict Summary of Items to Show Comparison of the Error Measurement in the Predictive Power Item Q² predict PLS-SEM_RMSE PLS-SEM_MAE LM_RMSE LM_MAE DB1 DB5 DB6 DB7 DB8 GA1 GA2 GA6 0.012 0.046 0.024 0.012 0.015 0.192 0.115 0.27 0.622 0.389 0.522 0.326 0.324 0.678 0.71 0.663 0.433 0.151 0.285 0.126 0.134 0.519 0.54 0.535 0.631 0.391 0.525 0.335 0.332 0.693 0.726 0.672 0.43 0.2 0.306 0.159 0.165 0.538 0.563 0.525 GA7 0.277 0.682 0.529 0.683 0.523 Note. LM = linear model; RMSE = root mean squared error; MAE = mean absolute error; DB = dishonest behavior; GA = goal attainment; N = 342. Table 5.13. PLSpredict Summary of Constructs Variable Q²predict RMSE MAE DB GA 0.039 0.359 1.037 0.807 0.499 0.646 Note. DB = dishonest behavior; GA = goal attainment; RMSE = root mean squared error; MAE = mean absolute error; N = 342. 72 5.5 Overview of Hypotheses Results Hypothesis one posited that locus of control is positively associated with goal attainment. While the coefficient was significant, the sign was in the opposite direction (negative). Therefore, H1 was not supported. Hypothesis 2 posited that monetary incentive is positively associated with goal attainment. This hypothesis was supported. Hypothesis 3 posited that employee loyalty negatively moderates the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship such that, when employee loyalty is high, monetary incentives have less of an impact on goal attainment. This hypothesis was supported. Hypothesis 4 posited that monetary incentive is positively associated with dishonest behavior. This hypothesis was supported. Hypothesis 5 posited that dishonest behavior increases goal attainment. This hypothesis was supported. Hypothesis 6 posited dishonest behavior mediates the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship. This hypothesis was supported. Hypothesis seven posited that employee loyalty negatively moderates the monetary incentive to dishonest behavior relationship, such that, when employee loyalty is high, there is less dishonest behavior. This hypothesis was not supported as the path coefficient is negative the p-value is 0.158 (> 0.05) and therefore not significant. The path coefficients and the effect sizes for the proposed hypotheses were assessed. In addition, mediation was examined following the PLS-SEM procedure, which is superior to the PROCESS method (Sarstedt et al., 2020). The path beta coefficients and significance are shown in Table 5.14 below. 73 Table 5.14. Path Relationship, Betas, and Significance for the Proposed Hypotheses in This Study Path Relationship Beta T statistics P values DB → GA EL → DB EL → GA LOC → GA MI → DB MI → GA 0.106 0.241 0.393 -0.274 0.133 0.115 2.005 3.519 7.946 4.740 1.731 2.643 0.023 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.042 0.004 EL x MI → DB EL x MI → GA -0.090 -0.113 1.002 2.461 0.158 0.007 Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. 5.5.1 Assessment – Moderation and Mediation In the model, LOC was hypothesized to have a positive effect on goal attainment (GA). Results show LOC does not have a positive effect on GA and that relationship is significant (t = 4.740, p < 0.05). Therefore, these results show that LOC does not contribute positively to attaining the goal. Monetary incentive (MI) was hypothesized to have a positive effect on GA and in this study, MI does have a positive effect on GA (t = 2.643, p < 0.05). Therefore, these results show that MI does contribute positively to attaining the goal. Employee loyalty (EL) was hypothesized to have a negative moderating effect on the MI to GA relationship, the findings indicate moderation does exist and EL does reduce the MI to GA relationship and is significant (t = 2.461, p < 0.05) as shown by Graph A in Figure 3 below. These results show that EL does contribute to reducing the monetary incentive to attain the goal. MI was hypothesized to have a positive effect on 74 dishonest behavior (DB). In this study, MI does have a positive effect on DB but slightly and barely significant as the p value < 0.05 at 0.042 and the t = 1.731. Similarly, DB was hypothesized to have a positive effect on GA, and it does have a positive effect on GA and is significant (t = 2.005, p < 0.05). In addition, DB was hypothesized to mediate the relationship of MI to GA, and does mediate that relationship as MI has a positive effect on DB and in turn, DB has a positive effect on GA. Both of those effects are significant. EL was hypothesized to have a negative moderating effect on the MI to DB relationship. Findings indicate MI is negatively associated related in the MI to DB relationship, but that effect is not significant as the p value is 0.158. This is demonstrated by Graph B in Figure 4 below. These results show that EL does have an impact on reducing dishonest behavior when a monetary incentive is involved but that moderating relationship is not significant. Table 5.15 displays the results of the mediation and moderation outcomes on the path relationships of all hypotheses Table 5.15. Outcomes of Mediation and Moderation on Path Relationship Hypotheses Hypothesis Path Relationship Outcome Hypothesis 1 Hypothesis 2 Hypothesis 3 Hypothesis 4 LOC → GA MI → GA EL → moderates MI → GA MI → DB Not Supported Supported Supported Supported Hypothesis 5 Hypothesis 6 Hypothesis 7 DB → GA DB → mediates MI → GA EL → moderates MI → DB Supported Supported Not Supported Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. 75 Figure 3. Graph A showing the moderating effects of employee loyalty on the relationship of monetary incentive to goal attainment. Above, the graph also shows the moderating effect EL has on the MI to GA relationship. This graphs the effect EL has on reducing the impact of MI on GA relationship and the effect is significant. This graph shows the positive effect EL has on reducing the need for monetary incentive to attain the goal. 76 Figure 4. Graph B showing the moderating effects of employee loyalty on the relationship of monetary incentive to dishonest behavior. Above is the simple slope analysis of the moderating effect of EL on the MI to DB relationship. The graph displays the slight effect EL has on moderating the MI to DB relationship. However, this is not significant and shows that EL has minimal effect reducing the MI to DB relationship. 77 CHAPTER VI DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS 6.1 Discussion In this research, it was shown that monetary incentives do have a positive effect on goal realization. This research also found that there are other factors or byproducts of monetary incentives when used for goal attainment. Monetary incentive was found to have a positive effect on dishonest behavior which in turn positively contributed to goal attainment. Employee loyalty was found to moderate the relationships that involved monetary incentives. This research found that employee loyalty negatively impacted the relationships that involved monetary incentives to dishonest behavior, although not significant, and the monetary incentives to goal attainment. This can be used to show that employee loyalty can decrease dishonest behavior and help decrease the need for monetary incentives for goal realization in the organization. In previous research, Corgnet et al. (2015), found that monetary incentives had a detrimental effect on workers’ motivation. Gneezy and Rustichini (2000) found as well that small monetary incentives undermined employee performance and did not enhance performance. Glaser et al. (2017) found that high incentives spurred intense aggression with employees and negatively impacted the firm. Alternatively, Khan et al. (2020) found that monetary incentives increase job performance and motivation. In their study, monetary incentives are shown to increase goal attainment and increase dishonest behavior which also enhance goal attainment. This study used dishonest behavior as a mediator for the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship and found that 78 dishonest behavior increases goal attainment and is increased by monetary incentives. This means that management should be careful with monetary incentives and be aware that dishonest behavior can be increased by the monetary incentive and create goal realization that may not be accurate. Murali et al. (2017) found in their study that loyalty increases performance. Pandey and Khare (2012) found that employee loyalty decreases organizational costs. Mowday et al. (1979) found in their study that employee loyalty creates an increased acceptance of organizational goals and values. In this study, employee loyalty was used as a moderator to the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship. This study found that as shown in Figure 2B, employee loyalty does decrease that relationship and shows that employee loyalty would allow for goal attainment with fewer monetary incentives. This means that management should enhance employee loyalty and that would decrease the amount of monetary incentive needed for goal realization. This study also measured the moderation effect employee loyalty has on the monetary incentive to dishonest behavior relationship. The results found employee loyalty does moderate that relationship and decreases the effect monetary incentive has on dishonest behavior. This means that management should enhance employee loyalty and the result would be a decrease in the dishonest behavior. This study enhances the previous studies and finds that employee loyalty not only reduces dishonest behavior but also decreases the need or amount of monetary incentive for goal attainment. These results can help show how employee loyalty can be used in organizations to help enhance goal attainment and realization. Organizations can concentrate on increasing the loyalty of employees and use in turn fewer monetary 79 incentives to help realize the organization’s goals. This study also found that monetary incentive does increase goal attainment and organizations can use monetary incentives to help attain the goals of the organization. 6.1.1 Theoretical Implications The implications of this research can show that expected utility theory is still a factor for goal attainment when monetary incentives are added. As shown in this research, as monetary incentive is added for goal attainment there is a positive effect on that relationship. This would still defend the EUT theory that employees will perform the needed tasks for the goal if there is a benefit for the employees. Employee loyalty in this research shows an impact on the relationships of MI to GA and MI to DB. This relates to the agency theory where the employee is the agent and acts for the benefit of themselves while at the same time performing for the benefit of the organization. As the employee becomes more loyal, they buy into the goals and culture of the organization and in turn can attain the goals and benefit from the monetary incentives. 6.1.2 Managerial Implications This research shows that MI impacts GA in a positive manner. This can help managers and organization stakeholders understand the need to incentivize employees to help goal realization. Employee loyalty is also shown to have an impact on the MI to GA and the MI to DB relationships in a positive manner. This shows that managers and stakeholders need to help employees become loyal so that goal realization of the firm can be accomplished with less MI and possibly less DB within the firm. 80 6.2 Conclusions The purpose of this study is to research the effects monetary incentives have on the final realization of the goal. The research seeks out to see the effects that locus of control has on goal attainment. Other factors studied in this research are employee loyalty and the effect that employee loyalty has on goal attainment when measured against the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship and the monetary incentive to dishonest behavior relationship. The idea is to see if highly loyal employees still regard a monetary incentive as a major driving force for goal realization. Also researching the effect employee loyalty has on dishonest behavior and if highly loyal employees are less likely to partake in dishonest behaviors to attain the goal. An additional factor affecting goal realization is studied as well to see the effect that dishonest behavior has on goal attainment when monetary incentives are involved. One hypothesis is not supported, and those findings should lead to future research to breakdown the employees by demographics and possibly identify groups that do support the hypothesis even though the overall respondent’s data does not. Having 342 respondents allowed this study to have enough respondents to separate out the data for future research. 6.3 Limitations There are several limitations with this study. The respondents are from several different companies in the financial sales industry. This gives a generic perception and not very specific to one firm. Management has several different styles and, since this study includes numerous employees from different firms, the results are generic in nature 81 and not very specific to a single firm. Using this model and scales in a specific firm will be much more precise to help management make better decisions for goal realization. A second limitation is that most of the scales used for these constructs are very dated. Due to our economic changes over the past twenty years, some of the scales could be updated and help be more precise in research. Employees change over time as new hires enter the workforce and different generations of employees affect the firm. The third limitation of this study is it is primarily quantitative. Interviews with employees at a specific company could help have a better understanding of how monetary incentives affect employees and their work behaviors and intentions. This research is left to rely on previous literature and scales to construct models and interpret results. Although this research supported most of the hypotheses, exploring more possibilities with employees and updating the scales to narrow the constructs could help the understanding of the concepts that play a part in employees work ethics and wants or needs that are external to the firm that affect the employees’ actions for goal realization. 6.4 Future Research Since locus of control was not supported when measured to increase goal attainment, that would be a significant area to further the research and look at demographics or length of employment to see if there is a factor that does positively increase GA. For example, looking at the data set and one LOC question, it was noticed that a majority of respondents with high school and associates degree rated luck high when asked about promotions whereas, the majority of respondents with Master and higher degrees rated hard work high for that question. 82 There are several areas where the constructs could be more precise. Monetary incentives can be researched in the future to see if there is a level of that incentive which influences employees to work harder to attain the goal. Another area for future research is similar to LOC with employee loyalty. Employee loyalty can be researched to see how much of an impact long term employees have on GA as well as breaking out the demographics of EL to see if there is a group that measures more positively to GA or more negatively to DB for example. As for dishonest behavior, there needs to be future research for that construct. Based on literature, it is stated that respondents are not likely to admit their dishonesty even in anonymous surveys. Future research can possibly test better scales to make the DB construct a little narrower and possibly get a better measure of how DB affects goal realization in the workplace. Additional future research can be done on the employee loyalty construct. Employee loyalty should be researched to see how much of an impact that has on goal realization and then use the demographics of that construct to look at the highly loyal employees. If it can be determined the most significant positive effect EL has on goal realization, then firms can look at their workforce and spend resources helping their workforce become much more loyal and use less MI to enhance goal realization. 6.5 Research Conclusion and Summary The summary of this research shows that, based on the 342 respondents from the financial sales industry, monetary incentives positively impact goal realization. Within that model, factors such as employee loyalty enhances goal attainment. Employee loyalty 83 moderates the monetary incentive to goal attainment relationship and moderates the monetary incentive to dishonest behavior relationship. This research also shows that monetary incentives positively impact dishonest behavior and in turn that dishonest behavior positively impacts goal attainment. To summarize, monetary incentive and employee loyalty positively impacts goal realization. 84 REFERENCES Al-Belushi, F. Y., & Khan, F. R. (2017). Impact of monetary incentives on employee’s motivation: Shinas College of Technology, Oman - A case study. International Journal of Management, Innovation and Entrepreneurial Research, 3(1), 01-11. Antoncic, J. A., & Antoncic, B. (2011). Employee loyalty and its impact on firm growth. International Journal of Management and Information Systems, 15(1), 81-88. Bakker, A. B., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2008). Positive organizational behavior: Engaged employees in flourishing organizations. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 29(2), 147-154. Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process, 50(2), 248-287. Barsky, A. (2008). Understanding the ethical cost of organizational goal setting: A review and theory development. Journal of Business Ethics, 81(1), 63–81. Becker, G. S. (1968). Crime and punishment: An economic approach. Journal of Political Economy, 76(2), 169-217. Bennett, R., & Robinson, S. (2000). Development of a measure of workplace deviance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(3), 349–360. Bernoulli, D. (1738/1954). Exposition of a new theory on the measurement of risk (English Translation, 1954). Econometrica, 22, 23-36. Bilal, H., Waseem, M., & Ali, S. (2020). Pragmatic impact of loyalty on deviant workplace behavior among banking sector employees. Journal of Accounting and Finance in Emerging Economies, 6(2), 407–414. 85 Bonner, S. E., & Sprinkle, G. B. (2002). The effects of monetary incentives on effort and task performance: Theories, evidence, and a framework for research. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 27(4–5), 303–345. Bradach, J. & Eccles, R. (1989). Price, authority, and trust. Annual Review of Sociology, 15, 97-118 Bulmer, M. G. (1979). Principles of statistics. Courier Corporation. Chin, W. W. (1998). The partial least squares approach to structural equation modeling. Modern Methods for Business Research, 295(2), 295-336. Cobb-Clark, D. A. (2015). Locus of control and the labor market. IZA Journal of Labor Economics, 4(1), 1–19. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Erlbaum. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155. Corgnet, B., Gómez-Miñambres, J., & Hernán-González, R. (2015). Goal setting and monetary incentives: When large stakes are not enough. Management Science, 61(12), 2926–2944. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2014.2068 Daramola, A. A., & Daramola, L. (2019). Does monetary incentives have stronger influence on workers’ productivity other than any form of motivational incentives? International Journal of Business and Management Future, 3(2), 38– 45. Doty, D., & Glick, W. (1998). Common methods bias: Does common methods variance really bias results? Organizational Research Methods, 1(4), 374-406. 86 Dutta, T., & Dhir, S. (2021). Employee loyalty: Measurement and validation. Global Business Review, 097215092199080. https://doi.org/10.1177/0972150921990809 Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 109–132. Eisenhardt, K. M. (1989). Agency theory: An assessment and review. Academy of Management Review, 14(1), 57–74. https://doi.org/10.2307/258191 Elegido, J. M. (2013). Does it make sense to be a loyal employee? Journal of Business Ethics, 116(3), 495–511. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief. Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research, 50(2), 179-221. Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1), 39-50. Franke, G., & Sarstedt, M. (2019), Heuristics versus statistics in discriminant validity testing: a comparison of four procedures, Internet Research, 29(3), 430-447. https://doi.org/10.1108/IntR-12-2017-0515 Galvin, B., Randel, A., Collins, B., & Johnson, R. (2018). Changing the focus of locus (of control): A targeted review of the locus of control literature and agenda for future research. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 39(7), 820–833. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2275 Gerlach, P., Teodorescu, K., & Hertwig, R. (2019). The truth about lies: A meta-analysis on dishonest behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 145(1), 1–44. 87 Glaser, D., van Gils, S., & van Quaquebeke, N. (2017). Pay-for-performance and interpersonal deviance: Competitiveness as the match that lights the fire. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 16(2), 77–90. https://doi.org/10.1027/18665888/a000181 Gneezy, U. (2005). Deception: The role of consequences. American Economic Review, 95(1), 384-394. Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2000). Pay enough or don’t pay at all. Quarterly Journal Economy, 115(3):791-810. https://doi: 10.1162/003355300554917 Graham, J., Meindl, P., Beall, E., Johnson, K. M., & Zhang, L. (2016). Cultural differences in moral judgment and behavior, across and within societies. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 125-130. Grover, S. L. (1993). Why professionals lie: The impact of professional role conflict on reporting accuracy. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 55, 251–272. Hair, J., Black, W., Babin, B., & Anderson, R. (2019). Multivariate data analysis (8th ed.). Cengage. Hair, J. F., Celsi, M. W., & Money, A. H., Samuel, P., & Page, M. J. (2015). Essentials of business research methods (2nd ed.). Routledge. Hair Jr, J. F., Howard, M. C., & Nitzl, C. (2020). Assessing measurement model quality in PLS-SEM using confirmatory composite analysis. Journal of Business Research, 109, 101-110. Hair Jr, J. F., Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C. M., & Sarstedt, M. (2021). A primer on partial least squares structural equation modeling: PLS-SEM (3rd ed.). Sage. 88 Hair, J. F., Hult, G. T., Ringle, C. M., Sarstedt, M., & Thiele, K. O. (2017). Mirror, mirror on the wall: A comparative evaluation of composite-based structural equation modeling methods, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 45, 616-632. Hair, J. F., Page, M., & Brunsveld, N. (2020). Essentials of business research methods (4th ed.). Routledge. Hair, J., Ringle, C., & Sarstedt, M. (2011). PLS-SEM: Indeed, a silver bullet. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 19(2), 139–152. Hair, J. F., Risher, J. J., Sarstedt, M., & Ringle, C. M. (2019). When to use and how to report the results of PLS-SEM. European Business Review, 31(1), 2-24. Hair, J. F., Sarstedt, M. & Ringle, C. M. (2019). Rethinking some of the re-thinking of partial least squares. European Journal of Marketing, 53(4), 566-5. Hair, J. F., & Sarstedt, M. (2020). Factors versus composites: Guidelines for choosing the right structural equation modeling method. Project Management Journal, 50(6), 619–624. Hair, J. F., & Sarstedt, M. (2021). Explanation plus prediction – The logical focus of project management research. Project Management Journal, 52(4), 319-322. https://doi.org/10.1177/8756972821999945. Hart, D. W., & Thompson, J. A. (2007). Untangling employee loyalty: A psychological contract perspective. Business Ethics Quarterly, 17(2), 297-323. Henseler, J., Ringle, C. M., & Sarstedt, M. (2015). A new criterion for assessing discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 43(1), 115-135. 89 Heywood, J. S., Jirjahn, U., & Struewing, C. (2017). Locus of control and performance appraisal. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 142, 205-225. Hildreth, J., & Anderson, C. (2018). Does loyalty trump honesty? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 79, 87–94. Idowu, H., Soyebo, K., & Adeoye, E. (2019). Incentives as correlates of employees loyalty towards management in organization. African Journal of Business Management, 13(12), 407-414. https://doi: 10.5897/AJBM2017.8434. Johnson, R. E., Rosen, C. C., Chang, C. H., & Lin, S. H. (2015). Getting to the core of locus of control: Is it a core evaluation of the self or the environment? Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 1568-1578. Johnson, R. E., Rosen, C. C., & Levy, P. E. (2008). Getting to the core of core selfevaluations: A review and recommendations. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 29, 391-413. Kajackaite, A., & Gneezy, U. (2017). Incentives and cheating. Games and Economic Behavior, 102, 433–444. Kaptein, M. (2008). Developing a measure of unethical behavior in the workplace: A stakeholder perspective. Journal of Management 34(5), 978–1008. Kaushik, M., Singh, V., & Chakravarty, S. (2022). Experimental evidence of the effect of financial incentives and detection on dishonesty. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 2680. Khan, M., Daniyal, M., & Ashraf, M.Z. (2020). The relationship between monetary incentives and job performance: Mediating role of employee loyalty. International Journal of Multidisciplinary and Current Educational Research, 2(6), 12–21. 90 Khazanov, G. K., Ruscio, A. M., & Forbes, C. N. (2020). The positive valence systems scale: Development and validation. Assessment, 27(5), 1045–1069. Kline, R. B. (2011). Convergence of structural equation modeling and multilevel modeling. In M. Williams & W. P. Vogt (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of innovation in social research model. SAGE. https://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781446268261.n31 LaMalfa, K. (2007). The top 11 ways to increase your employee loyalty. Business Week Technology Research. White paper. Lawler, E. E., & Suttle, J. L. (1973). Expectancy theory and job behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 9(3), 482–503. Le Maux, B., Masclet, D., & Necker, S. (2021). Monetary incentives and the contagion of unethical behavior, Freiburger Diskussionspapiere zur Ordnungsökonomik, No. 21/03, Albert-Ludwigs-Universität Freiburg, Institut für Allgemeine Wirtschaftsforschung, Abteilung für Wirtschaftspolitik und Ordnungsökonomik, Freiburg i. Br. ZEW - Centre for European Economic Research Discussion Paper No. 21-025. Lengwiler, Y. (2009). The origins of expected utility theory. Vinzenz Bronzin’s Option Pricing Models: Exposition and Appraisal, i(June), 535–545. Locke, E. A. (2004). Linking goals to monetary incentives. Academy of Management Executive, 18(4), 130–133. https://doi.org/10.5465/ame.2004.15268732 Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting & task performance. Prentice-Hall, Inc. 91 Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57(9), 705–717. Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2004). What should we do about motivation theory? Six recommendations for the twenty-first century. Academy of Management Review, 29(3), 388-403. Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2006). New directions in goal-setting theory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(5), 265–268. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00449.x Logan, G. M. (1984). Loyalty and a sense of purpose. California Management Review, 27(1), 149-156. Manley, S. C., Hair, J. F., Williams, R. I., & McDowell, W. C. (2021). Essential new PLS-SEM analysis methods for your entrepreneurship analytical toolbox. International Entrepreneurship and Management Journal. 17(1), 1-21. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11365-020-00687-6 Mazar, N., Amir, O. & Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. Journal of Marketing Research 45(6), 633-644. Melnikoff, D. E., & Bailey, A. H. (2018). Preferences for moral vs. immoral traits in others are conditional. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(4), E592-E600. Mitra, A., & Shahriar, Q. (2020). Why is dishonesty difficult to mitigate? The interaction between descriptive norm and monetary incentive. Journal of Economic Psychology, 80, Article 102292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2020.102292 92 Mongin, P. (1997). Expected utility theory. In J. Davis, W. Hands & U. Maki (Eds.), Handbook of Economic Methodology (pp. 342-350). Edward Elgar. Mowday, R. T., Porter, L. W., & Steers, R. M. (1979). The measurement of organizational commitment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 14, 224-227. Mowday, R. T., Porter, L. W., & Steers, R. M. (1982). Employee-organization linkages. Academic Press. Murali, S., Poddar, A., & Seema, A. (2017). Employee loyalty, organizational performance and performance evaluation: A critical survey. Journal of Business and Management, 19(8), 62–74. https://doi.org/10.9790/487X-1908036274 Nagin, D. S., Rebitzer, J. B., Sanders, S., & Taylor, L. J. (2002). Monitoring motivation and management: The determinants of opportunistic behavior in a field experiment. American Economic Review, 92(4), 850-873. Nawaz, M., Hassan, M., Hassan, S., Shaukat, S., & Asadullah, M. (2014). Employee training and empowerment. Middle East Journal of Scientific Research, 19(4), 593–601. Nnubia, A. L. (2020). MI and employee performance. International Journal of Innovative Finance and Economics Research, 8(1), 10–22. Padmanabhan, S. (2021). Locus of control and job satisfaction. Behavioral Sciences, 2, 1–6. Pandey, C., & Khare, R. (2012). Impact of job satisfaction and organizational commitment on employee loyalty. International Journal of Social Science and Interdisciplinary Research, 1(8), 26-41. 93 Pascual-Ezama, D., Prelec, D., & Dunfield, D. (2013). Motivation, money, prestige, and cheats. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 93, 367–373. Perneger, T. V., Courvoisier, D. S., Hudelson, P. M., & Gayet-Ageron, A. (2015). Sample size for pre-tests of questionnaires. Quality of Life Research, 24(1), 147– 151. Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879–903. Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2012). Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 539–569. Pokorny, K. (2008). Pay-but do not pay too much. An experimental study on the impact of incentives. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 66(2), 251–264. Ponta, L., Cainarca, G. C., & Cincotti, S. (2020). Monetary incentives in Italian public administration: A stimulus for employees? An agent-based approach. Complexity, 2020, Article 6152017. https://doi.org/10.1155/2020/6152017 Ponta, L., Delfino, F., & Cainarca, G. (2020). The role of monetary incentives: Bonus and/or stimulus. Administrative Sciences, 10(1), 8. Reichheld, F. (2006). The microeconomics of customer relationships. MIT Sloan Management Review, 47(2), 73–81. Rishipal, H. (2019). Employee loyalty and counterproductive work behaviour among employees in the Indian hospitality sector. Worldwide Hospitality and Tourism Themes, 11(4), 438-448. 94 Robinson, M., & Farkas, M. (2021). The effect of monetary incentives on task attractiveness, effort, and performance. Journal of Applied Accounting Research, 22(5), 761-779. Robinson, S. L., & Bennett, R. J. (1995). A typology of deviant workplace behaviors: A multidimensional scaling study. Academy of Management Journal, 38(2), 555– 572. Rotter, J. B. (1954). Social learning and clinical psychology. Prentice-Hall, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1037/10788-000 Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80(1), 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0092976 Saleem, S. (2011). The impact of financial incentives on employees commitment. European Journal of Business and Management, 3(4), 258-266. Sapre, N. (2021). Revisiting the expected utility theory and the consumption CAPM. Munich Personal RePEc Archive, Paper No. 106668, 1–11. Sarstedt, M., Hair, J. F., Nitzl, C., Ringle, C. M., & Howard, M. C. (2020). Beyond a tandem analysis of SEM and PROCESS: Use PLS-SEM for mediation analyses! International Journal of Market Research, 62(3), 288-299. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470785320915686 Shmueli, G., Sarstedt, M., Hair, J. F., Cheah, J., Ting, H., Vaithilingam, S., & Ringle, C. M. (2019). Predictive model assessment in PLS-SEM: Guidelines for using PLSpredict. European Journal of Marketing, 53(11), 2322-2347. https://doi.org/10.1108/EJM-02-2019-0189 95 Spano, A., & Monfardini, P. (2018). Performance-related payments in local governments: Do they improve performance or only increase salary? International Journal of Public Administration, 41(4), 321–334. https://doi.org/10.1080/01900692.2016.1265982 Spector, P. E. (1982). Behavior in organizations as a function of employee’s locus of control. Psychological Bulletin, 91(482–497. https://doi.org/1.1037/00332909.91.3.482 Spector, P. E. (1988). Development of the work locus of control scale. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 61(4), 335-340. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.20448325.1988.tb00470.x Stigler, G. J. (1950). The development of Utility Theory II. Journal of Political Economy, 58(5), 373-396. https://doi.org/10.1086/256980 Vroom, V. (1964). Work and motivation, Wiley. Wang, Q., Bowling, N. A., & Eschleman, K. J. (2010). A meta-analytic examination of work and general locus of control. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(4), 761– 768. Williams, L., & Brown, B. (1994). Method variance in organizational behavior and human resources research: Effects and correlation, path coefficients, and hypothesis testing. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 57(2), 185-209. Williamson, O. E. (1993). Opportunism and its critics. Managerial and Decision Economics, 14, 97-107. 96 Yaqub, M. Z., Saz, G., & Hussain, D. (2009). A meta-analysis of the empirical evidence on expected utility theory. European Journal of Economics, Finance, and Administrative Sciences, 15(15), 117–133. Zhang, H., Ge, X., Liu, Z., & Wei, L. (2020). Goal related unethical behaviors. Journal of Research in Personality, 87, 1–9. 97 APPENDICES Appendix A Original Item Factor Loadings Table A1. Original Items – Factor Loadings for all Constructs Item DB DB1 0.744 DB2 DB3 0.653* 0.532* DB4 DB5 DB6 DB7 DB8 EL1 EL10 0.688* 0.868 0.812 0.724 0.799 EL LOC 0.740 0.827 EL2 EL3 EL4 EL5 EL6 EL7 EL8 0.867 0.770 0.818 0.736 0.775 0.846 0.857 EL9 GA1 GA2 0.674* GA3 GA4 GA5 GA6 GA7 GA8 GA 0.782 0.742 0.555* 0.692* 0.644* 0.752 0.728 0.639 98 MI EL x MI Table A1 cont. LOC1 LOC2 LOC3 LOC4 LOC5 LOC6 LOC7 LOC8 0.317* 0.748 0.822 0.743 0.662* 0.589* 0.650* 0.756 LOC9 MI1 MI2 MI3 MI4 MI5 MI6 MI7 MI8 MI9 0.592* 0.418* 0.393* 0.431* 0.502* 0.559* 0.796 0.711 0.828 0.642* Note. LOC = locus of control; GA = goal attainment; MI = monetary incentive; DB = dishonest behavior; EL = employee loyalty; N = 342. Items marked with asterisk (*) were removed after initial screening. 99 Appendix B Invitation to Participate You are invited to voluntarily participate in a research project on goal attainment and loyalty in your workplace. The purpose of the study is to better understand the measurement of goals and loyalty in the workplace. This will take no longer than 8 to 11 minutes to complete. Participation will remain anonymous, and no identifying data will be collected. You will be given a survey of about 60 items, most of which will be on a 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree) answer format. You have the right to refuse to answer any questions that you do not wish to complete and/or answer. The results of this study may be potentially advantageous in future society as a result of information gathered in the study. While unlikely, it is possible that a loss of confidentiality may occur. Be aware, however, that no identifying information is recorded for the current study, and all responses will be saved on a password protected online account. Even if a loss of confidentiality may occur, the data will not have indicators to track responses to individual participants. Also, if a publication is produced from the data obtained in the current study, all results will be presented as overall findings – no direct information about particular responses will be provided. All answers will be deleted after all data has been collected and three years have passed after any eventual publication. All information will be used for research purposes only. 100 If you agree to participate, you must be at least 25 years of age and proficient in the English language. You can withdraw at any time without consequence. Please contact me at G1822@Jagmail.SouthAlabama.edu or the Institutional Review Board at the University of South Alabama at 251-460-6308 if you have questions about your rights as a research subject. 101 Appendix C Survey Statements Goal Attainment: 5 points 1 = Not At All; 2 = Rarely; 3 = Sometimes; 4 = Usually; 5 = Totally I have made considerable progress toward attaining my goal for the company. I accomplished what I set out to do for my goals for the company. I achieve my personal goals. I regularly achieve goals set by my company. I acquire the ability to perform the tasks to reach my goal for the company. I want to accomplish the goals set for myself by my company. I feel pleased when I reach the goal set by my company. I expect to master the tasks I undertake to reach my company goal. Dishonest Behavior: 5 points 1 = Never; 2 = Rarely; 3 = Sometimes; 4 = Often; 5 = Almost Always I would falsify work data to benefit myself. I would make false or misleading claims to the customer to attain the sale. Please select often for this question. I accept inappropriate gifts, favors, entertainment, or kickbacks from customers. I would engage in false or deceptive sales and marketing practices (i.e., creating unrealistic expectations) to gain the sale. I would falsify or manipulate financial reporting information to attain the goal. I would falsify time and expense reports to attain more money. I would breach computer, network, or database controls to obtain private information. I would steal or misappropriate assets to benefit me financially (e.g., money, equipment, materials). 102 Monetary Incentive: 5 points 1 = Strongly Agree; 2 = Agree; 3 = Neutral; 4 = Disagree; 5 = Strongly Disagree Monetary incentives are important to me. I place importance on monetary incentives. Monetary incentives are of no value to me. Monetary incentives on my job match my work effort. Monetary incentives at my company are not up to my expectations. Financial incentives increase employee job satisfaction. Monetary incentives increase employee loyalty at my company. Monetary incentives help to develop employee attitudes towards organizational success at my company. If I attain the goal for the monetary incentive, I know I will receive it from my company. Please select Agree for this question. Employee Loyalty: 5 points 1 = Strongly Agree; 2= Agree; 3 = Undecided; 4 = Disagree; 5 = Strongly Disagree I speak positively about my company when talking to customers. I speak positively about my company when talking to friends and relatives. I can recommend the products and services of my company to others. I would like to stay with this company in the future. I would not change immediately to another company if I got an offer. I enjoy discussing my organization to others. I would recommend this company to friends. I am committed to the company’s success. I take pride in my work. I have a strong belonging to my organization. 103 Locus of Control: 6 points 1 = Disagree Very Much; 2 = Disagree Moderately; 3 = Disagree Slightly; 4 = Agree Slightly; 5 = Agree Moderately; 6 = Agree Very Much Promotions are usually a matter of good fortune. How my life takes course is dependent on me. Success is gained through hard work. If you know what you want out of a job, you can find a job that gives it to you. Getting the job you want is mostly a matter of luck. Making money is primarily a matter of good fortune. In order to get a really good job, you need to have family members or friends in high places. People who perform their jobs well generally get rewarded for it. I have little control over things that happen in my life. It takes a lot of luck to be an outstanding employee on most jobs. 104 Appendix D IRB Approval 105 BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH Name of Author: Lawrence E Goehrig Graduate and Undergraduate School Attended: University of South Alabama, Mobile, Alabama Nova Southeastern University, Fort Lauderdale, Florida University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida Degrees Awarded: Doctor of Philosophy in Business Administration, 2022, Mobile, Alabama Master of Business Administration Certificate, 2014, Fort Lauderdale, Florida Master of Business Administration, 2013, Fort Lauderdale, Florida Bachelor of Science, 1986, Gainesville, Florida Publications: Merkle, A. C., Hessick, C., Leggett, B. R., Goehrig, L., & O’Connor, K. (2020). Exploring the components of brand equity amid declining ticket sales in Major League Baseball. Journal of Marketing Analytics, 8(3), 149–164. Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Mehta, R., & Babin, B. J. (2021). Sales force management: Building customer relationships and partnerships (2nd ed.). Wiley. [Contributor, Case Study: Midwest Auto Parts, February 2021, pp. 283-284]. 106 ProQuest Number: 30249713 INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality and completeness of this reproduction is dependent on the quality and completeness of the copy made available to ProQuest. Distributed by ProQuest LLC ( 2023 ). Copyright of the Dissertation is held by the Author unless otherwise noted. This work may be used in accordance with the terms of the Creative Commons license or other rights statement, as indicated in the copyright statement or in the metadata associated with this work. Unless otherwise specified in the copyright statement or the metadata, all rights are reserved by the copyright holder. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code and other applicable copyright laws. Microform Edition where available © ProQuest LLC. No reproduction or digitization of the Microform Edition is authorized without permission of ProQuest LLC. ProQuest LLC 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, MI 48106 - 1346 USA