Photo: Jeremy Newton, Jonathan Sturges, Spencer Greenberg and Alexander Fleiss of Rebellion Research. Source: Michael Rubenstein/Redux Perception and Individual Decision Making 6 Indecision may or may not be my problem. —Jimmy Buffett 165 166 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making into which AI makes them. Says an observer of one complex system—airline piloting, “When the person has no role in the task, there’s a much greater risk of complacency.” Sources: A. Shell, “Wall Street Traders Mine Tweets to Gain a Trading Edge,” USA Today (May 4, 2011), downloaded May 23, 2011, from www.usatoday.com/money/; “Soon, Your Computer Will Have Emotions Like You,” The Economic Times (April 20, 2011), downloaded May 23, 2011, from http://articles.economictimes.indiatimes.com; S. Patterson, “Letting the Machines Decide,” The Wall Street Journal (July 14, 2010), p. C1; and C. Negroni, “As Attention Wanders, Second Thoughts About the Autopilot,” The New York Times (May 18, 2010), pp. B1, B5. T he preceding example illustrates some of the issues that arise when people look for techniques to improve their decision making. One reason some like computerized decision making is that human decision makers can be incorrect or biased in many ways. This chapter will review some of these biases in human decision making, but it also explores how human decision makers can outperform machines, especially in the area of creativity. The Case Incident 1 (at the end of the chapter) considers computerized decision making further. In the following Self-Assessment Library, consider one perception—that of appropriate gender roles. S A L SELF-ASSESSMENT LIBRARY What Are My Gender Role Perceptions? In the Self-Assessment Library (available on CD or online), take assessment IV.C.2 (What Are My Gender Role Perceptions?) and answer the following questions. 1. Did you score as high as you thought you would? 2. Do you think a problem with measures like this is that people aren’t honest in responding? 3. If others, such as friends, classmates, and family members, rated you, would they rate you differently? Why or why not? 4. Research has shown that people’s gender role perceptions are becoming less traditional over time. Why do you suppose this is so? What Is Perception? 1 Define perception and explain the factors that influence it. Perception is a process by which individuals organize and interpret their sensory impressions in order to give meaning to their environment. However, what we perceive can be substantially different from objective reality. For example, all employees in a firm may view it as a great place to work—favorable working conditions, interesting job assignments, good pay, excellent benefits, understanding and responsible management—but, as most of us know, it’s very unusual to find such agreement. Why is perception important in the study of OB? Simply because people’s behavior is based on their perception of what reality is, not on reality itself. The world as it is perceived is the world that is behaviorally important. What Is Perception? 167 Factors That Influence Perception How do we explain the fact that individuals may look at the same thing yet perceive it differently? A number of factors operate to shape and sometimes distort perception. These factors can reside in the perceiver; in the object, or target, being perceived; or in the context of the situation in which the perception is made (see Exhibit 6-1). When you look at a target and attempt to interpret what you see, your interpretation is heavily influenced by your personal characteristics—your attitudes, personality, motives, interests, past experiences, and expectations. For instance, if you expect police officers to be authoritative or young people to be lazy, you may perceive them as such, regardless of their actual traits. Characteristics of the target also affect what we perceive. Loud people are more likely to be noticed in a group than quiet ones. So, too, are extremely attractive or unattractive individuals. Because we don’t look at targets in isolation, the relationship of a target to its background also influences perception, as does our tendency to group close things and similar things together. We often perceive women, men, Whites, African Americans, Asians, or members of any other group that has clearly distinguishable characteristics as alike in other, unrelated ways as well. Context matters too. The time at which we see an object or event can influence our attention, as can location, light, heat, or any number of situational factors. At a nightclub on Saturday night, you may not notice a young guest “dressed to the nines.” Yet that same person so attired for your Monday morning management class would certainly catch your attention (and that of the rest of the class). Neither the perceiver nor the target has changed between Saturday night and Monday morning, but the situation is different. Exhibit 6-1 Factors That Influence Perception Factors in the perceiver • Attitudes • Motives • Interests • Experience • Expectations Factors in the situation • Time • Work setting • Social setting Perception Factors in the target • Novelty • Motion • Sounds • Size • Background • Proximity • Similarity perception A process by which individuals organize and interpret their sensory impressions in order to give meaning to their environment. 168 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making Person Perception: Making Judgments About Others Now we turn to the application of perception concepts most relevant to OB—person perception, or the perceptions people form about each other. Attribution Theory 2 Explain attribution theory and list the three determinants of attribution. Nonliving objects such as desks, machines, and buildings are subject to the laws of nature, but they have no beliefs, motives, or intentions. People do. That’s why when we observe people, we attempt to explain why they behave in certain ways. Our perception and judgment of a person’s actions, therefore, will be significantly influenced by the assumptions we make about that person’s internal state. Attribution theory tries to explain the ways in which we judge people differently, depending on the meaning we attribute to a given behavior.1 It suggests that when we observe an individual’s behavior, we attempt to determine whether it was internally or externally caused. That determination, however, depends largely on three factors: (1) distinctiveness, (2) consensus, and (3) consistency. First, let’s clarify the differences between internal and external causation, and then we’ll elaborate on each of the three determining factors. Internally caused behaviors are those we believe to be under the personal control of the individual. Externally caused behavior is what we imagine the situation forced the individual to do. If one of your employees is late for work, you might attribute that to his partying into the wee hours and then oversleeping. This is an internal attribution. But if you attribute lateness to an automobile accident that tied up traffic, you are making an external attribution. Now let’s discuss the three determining factors. Distinctiveness refers to whether an individual displays different behaviors in different situations. Is the employee who arrives late today also one who regularly “blows off” commitments? What we want to know is whether this behavior is unusual. If it is, we are likely to give it an external attribution. If it’s not, we will probably judge the behavior to be internal. If everyone who faces a similar situation responds in the same way, we can say the behavior shows consensus. The behavior of our tardy employee meets this criterion if all employees who took the same route were also late. From an attribution perspective, if consensus is high, you would probably give an external attribution to the employee’s tardiness, whereas if other employees who took the same route made it to work on time, you would attribute his lateness to an internal cause. Finally, an observer looks for consistency in a person’s actions. Does the person respond the same way over time? Coming in 10 minutes late for work is not perceived in the same way for an employee who hasn’t been late for several months as it is for an employee who is late two or three times a week. The more consistent the behavior, the more we are inclined to attribute it to internal causes. Exhibit 6-2 summarizes the key elements in attribution theory. It tells us, for instance, that if an employee, Kim Randolph, generally performs at about the same level on related tasks as she does on her current task (low distinctiveness), other employees frequently perform differently—better or worse— than Kim on that task (low consensus), and Kim’s performance on this current task is consistent over time (high consistency), anyone judging Kim’s work will likely hold her primarily responsible for her task performance (internal attribution). Person Perception: Making Judgments About Others Exhibit 6-2 169 Attribution Theory Observation Attribution of cause Interpretation High Distinctiveness Low High Individual behavior Consensus Low High Consistency Low External Internal External Internal Internal External One of the most interesting findings from attribution theory research is that errors or biases distort attributions. When we make judgments about the behavior of other people, we tend to underestimate the influence of external factors and overestimate the influence of internal or personal factors.2 This fundamental attribution error can explain why a sales manager is prone to attribute the poor performance of her sales agents to laziness rather than to the innovative product line introduced by a competitor. Individuals and organizations also tend to attribute their own successes to internal factors such as ability or effort, while blaming failure on external factors such as bad luck or unproductive co-workers. People also tend to attribute ambiguous information as relatively flattering and accept positive feedback while rejecting negative feedback. This is the self-serving bias.3 A U.S. News & World Report study showed its power. Researchers asked one group of people “If someone sues you and you win the case, should he pay your legal costs?” Eighty-five percent responded “yes.” Another group was asked “If you sue someone and lose the case, should you pay his costs?” Only 44 percent answered “yes.”4 The evidence on cultural differences in perception is mixed, but most suggest there are differences across cultures in the attributions people make.5 One study found Korean managers less likely to use the self-serving bias—they tended to accept responsibility for group failure “because I was not a capable leader” instead of attributing failure to group members.6 On the other hand, Asian managers are more likely to blame institutions or whole organizations, whereas Western observers believe individual managers should get blame or praise.7 That probably explains why U.S. newspapers prominently report the names of individual executives when firms do poorly, whereas Asian media cover how the firm as a whole has failed. This tendency to make group-based attributions also explains why individuals from Asian cultures are more likely to make group-based stereotypes.8 Attribution theory was developed largely based attribution theory An attempt to fundamental attribution error The determine whether an individual’s behavior is internally or externally caused. tendency to underestimate the influence of external factors and overestimate the influence of internal factors when making judgments about the behavior of others. self-serving bias The tendency for individuals to attribute their own successes to internal factors and put the blame for failures on external factors. 170 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making on experiments with U.S. and Western European workers. But these studies suggest caution in making attribution theory predictions in non-Western societies, especially in countries with strong collectivist traditions. Differences in attribution tendencies don’t mean the basic concepts of attribution and blame completely differ across cultures, though. Self-serving biases may be less common in East Asian cultures, but evidence suggests they still operate across cultures.9 Recent studies indicate Chinese managers assess blame for mistakes using the same distinctiveness, consensus, and consistency cues Western managers use.10 They also become angry and punish those deemed responsible for failure, a reaction shown in many studies of Western managers. This means the basic process of attribution applies across cultures, but that it takes more evidence for Asian managers to conclude someone else should be blamed. Common Shortcuts in Judging Others 3 Identify the shortcuts individuals use in making judgments about others. The shortcuts we use in judging others are frequently valuable: they allow us to make accurate perceptions rapidly and provide valid data for making predictions. However, they are not foolproof. They can and do get us into trouble when they result in significant distortions. Selective Perception Any characteristic that makes a person, an object, or an event stand out will increase the probability we will perceive it. Why? Because it is impossible for us to assimilate everything we see; we can take in only certain stimuli. This explains why you’re more likely to notice cars like your own, or why a boss may reprimand some people and not others doing the same thing. Because we can’t observe everything going on about us, we engage in selective perception. A classic example shows how vested interests can significantly influence which problems we see. Dearborn and Simon asked 23 business executives (6 in sales, 5 in production, 4 in accounting, and 8 in miscellaneous functions) to read a comprehensive case describing the organization and activities of a steel company.11 Each manager was asked to write down the most important problem in the case. Eighty-three percent of the sales executives rated sales important; only 29 percent of the others did so. The researchers concluded that participants perceived as important the aspects of a situation specifically related to their own unit’s activities and goals. A group’s perception of organizational activities is selectively altered to align with the vested interests the group represents. Because we cannot assimilate all that we observe, we take in bits and pieces. But we don’t choose randomly; rather, we select according to our interests, background, experience, and attitudes. Selective perception allows us to speed-read others, but not without the risk of drawing an inaccurate picture. Seeing what we want to see, we can draw unwarranted conclusions from an ambiguous situation. We find another example of selective perception in financial analysis. From 2007 to 2009, the U.S. stock market lost roughly half its value. Yet during that time, analysts’ sell ratings (typically, analysts rate a company’s stock with three recommendations: buy, sell, or hold) actually decreased slightly. Although there are several reasons analysts are reluctant to put sell ratings on stocks, one is selective perception. When prices are going down, analysts often attend to the past (saying the stock is a bargain relative to its prior price), rather than the future (the downward trend may well continue). As one money manager noted, “Each time the market went down was a new opportunity to buy the stock even cheaper.”12 That much is true, of course, but it shows the dangers of selective perception: by looking only at the past price, analysts were relying on a false reference point and failing to recognize that what has fallen can fall further still. Person Perception: Making Judgments About Others 171 glOBalization! Chinese Time, North American Time W e realize just how much we take our perceptions of the world for granted when we try to see through the eyes of someone who grew up in a culture totally different from our own. For instance, people see the passage of time quite differently in different cultures. Some cultures tend to focus more on the past, others on the future. Li-Jun Ji and colleagues investigated how Chinese perceive events relative to Canadians’ perceptions. Participants read a description of a theft, along with descriptions of events that occurred in the distant past, recent past, or present. When attempting to solve the case, Chinese participants were more likely to rely on events from the distant past and recent past, whereas Canadians were more attentive to recent events. Even the way we visualize the passage of time differs across cultures. Lera Boroditsky, Orly Fuhrman, and Kelly McCormick from Stanford University examined how American English speakers and Mandarin (Chinese) speakers differed in their perception of time. Because English uses phrases like “looking forward to” events or “putting the past behind us,” English speakers tend to think about time as a horizontal timeline. Mandarin however uses words like shàng (up) and xià (down) to refer to time, so events accumulate in a stack. Could this difference in language structure explain why Chinese speakers pay more attention to history when thinking about events? Do Chinese think more about events “piling up” on top of one another (making them more relevant), whereas North Americans think about events moving away in time so that what is in the past is over and done? Further research will have to examine whether this is the case, but it remains an interesting possibility. Sources: L. Ji, Z. Zhang, and D. Messervey, “Looking Into the Past: Cultural Differences in Perception and Representation of Past Information,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96, no. 4 (2009), pp. 761–769; L. Boroditsky, O. Fuhrman, and K. McCormick, “Do English and Mandarin Speakers Think about Time Differently?” Cognition 118, no. 1 (2011), pp. 123–129; and A. J. Shipp, J. R. Edwards, and L. S. Lambert, “Conceptualization and Measurement of Temporal Focus: The Subjective Experience of Past, Present, and Future,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 110, no. 1 (2009), pp. 1–22. Halo Effect When we draw a general impression about an individual on the basis of a single characteristic, such as intelligence, sociability, or appearance, a halo effect is operating.13 If you’re a critic of President Obama, try listing 10 things you admire about him. If you’re an admirer, try listing 10 things you dislike about him. No matter which group describes you, odds are you won’t find this an easy exercise! That’s the halo effect: our general views contaminate our specific ones. The reality of the halo effect was confirmed in a classic study in which subjects were given a list of traits such as intelligent, skillful, practical, industrious, determined, and warm and asked to evaluate the person to whom those traits applied.14 Subjects judged the person to be wise, humorous, popular, and imaginative. When the same list was modified to include “cold” instead of “warm,” a completely different picture emerged. Clearly, the subjects were allowing a single trait to influence their overall impression of the person they were judging. Contrast Effects An old adage among entertainers is “Never follow an act that has kids or animals in it.” Why? Audiences love children and animals so much that you’ll look bad in comparison. This example demonstrates how a contrast effect can distort perceptions. We don’t evaluate a person in isolation. Our reaction is influenced by other persons we have recently encountered. selective perception The tendency to selectively interpret what one sees on the basis of one’s interests, background, experience, and attitudes. halo effect The tendency to draw a general impression about an individual on the basis of a single characteristic. contrast effect Evaluation of a person’s characteristics that is affected by comparisons with other people recently encountered who rank higher or lower on the same characteristics. 172 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making In a series of job interviews, for instance, interviewers can make distortions in any given candidate’s evaluation as a result of his or her place in the interview schedule. A candidate is likely to receive a more favorable evaluation if preceded by mediocre applicants and a less favorable evaluation if preceded by strong applicants. These young women are taking part in a running test for employment in a police force in Peshawar, Pakistan. But women in America and European, Eastern European, Asian, and Latin American countries report that gender stereotyping makes it difficult for them to enter the profession because it is largely regarded as a job strictly for men. Even in countries like the United States where the law requires hiring police officers without regard to gender, the stereotypical view of women inaccurately generalizes them as lacking the mental, physical, and emotional fitness required to perform police work. But women who want to do police work contend that they satisfy the fitness requirements and even bring special qualities to the job such as compassion and good communication skills. Source: Pakistan Press International Photo/Newscom Stereotyping When we judge someone on the basis of our perception of the group to which he or she belongs, we are using the shortcut called stereotyping.15 We rely on generalizations every day because they help us make decisions quickly; they are a means of simplifying a complex world. It’s less difficult to deal with an unmanageable number of stimuli if we use heuristics or stereotypes. For example, it does make sense to assume that Tre, the new employee from accounting, is going to know something about budgeting, or that Allie from finance will be able to help you figure out a forecasting problem. The problem occurs, of course, when we generalize inaccurately or too much. In organizations, we frequently hear comments that represent stereotypes based on gender, age, race, religion, ethnicity, and even weight (see Chapter 2):16 “Men aren’t interested in child care,” “Older workers can’t learn new skills,” “Asian immigrants are hardworking and conscientious.” A growing body of research suggests stereotypes operate emotionally and often below the level of conscious awareness, making them particularly hard to challenge and change.17 Stereotypes can be deeply ingrained and powerful enough to influence life-and-death decisions. One study, controlling for a wide array of factors (such as aggravating or mitigating circumstances), showed that the degree to which black defendants in murder trials looked “stereotypically black” essentially doubled their odds of receiving a death sentence if convicted.18 Another experimental study found that students who read scenarios describing leaders tended to assign higher scores for leadership potential and effective leadership to Whites than to minorities even though the content of the scenarios was equivalent, supporting the idea of a stereotype of Whites as better leaders.19 Person Perception: Making Judgments About Others 173 One problem of stereotypes is that they are widespread and often useful generalizations, though they may not contain a shred of truth when applied to a particular person or situation. So we constantly have to check ourselves to make sure we’re not unfairly or inaccurately applying a stereotype in our evaluations and decisions. Stereotypes are an example of the warning “The more useful, the more danger from misuse.” Specific Applications of Shortcuts in Organizations People in organizations are always judging each other. Managers must appraise their employees’ performances. We evaluate how much effort our co-workers are putting into their jobs. Team members immediately “size up” a new person. In many cases, our judgments have important consequences for the organization. Let’s look at the most obvious applications. Employment Interview Few people are hired without an interview. But interviewers make perceptual judgments that are often inaccurate20 and draw early impressions that quickly become entrenched. Research shows we form impressions of others within a tenth of a second, based on our first glance.21 If these first impressions are negative, they tend to be more heavily weighted in the interview than if that same information came out later.22 Most interviewers’ decisions change very little after the first 4 or 5 minutes of an interview. As a result, information elicited early in the interview carries greater weight than does information elicited later, and a “good applicant” is probably characterized more by the absence of unfavorable characteristics than by the presence of favorable ones. Performance Expectations People attempt to validate their perceptions of reality even when these are faulty.23 The terms self-fulfilling prophecy and Pygmalion effect describe how an individual’s behavior is determined by others’ expectations. If a manager expects big things from her people, they’re not likely to let her down. Similarly, if she expects only minimal performance, they’ll likely meet those low expectations. Expectations become reality. The self-fulfilling prophecy has been found to affect the performance of students, soldiers, and even accountants.24 Performance Evaluation We’ll discuss performance evaluations more fully in Chapter 17, but note for now that they very much depend on the perceptual process.25 An employee’s future is closely tied to the appraisal—promotion, pay raises, and continuation of employment are among the most obvious outcomes. Although the appraisal can be objective (for example, a sales-person is appraised on how many dollars of sales he generates in his territory), many jobs are evaluated in subjective terms. Subjective evaluations, though often necessary, are problematic because all the errors we’ve discussed thus far— selective perception, contrast effects, halo effects, and so on—affect them. Ironically, sometimes performance ratings say as much about the evaluator as they do about the employee! stereotyping Judging someone on the basis of one’s perception of the group to which that person belongs. self-fulfilling prophecy A situation in which a person inaccurately perceives a second person, and the resulting expectations cause the second person to behave in ways consistent with the original perception. 174 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making The Link Between Perception and Individual Decision Making Explain the link between perception and decision making. MyManagementLab For an interactive application of this topic, check out this chapter’s simulation activity at www.mymanagementlab.com. Delta Airlines management made a decision in reaction to the problem of negative publicity resulting from a growing number of customer complaints about poor service. To improve service, Delta reinstated the personal assistance of its elite Red Coat airport agents that it started in the 1960s but eliminated in 2005 due to budget cuts. The primary mission of the Red Coats, such as Charmaine Gordon shown here helping customers at Kennedy International Airport in New York, is to fix customer problems. Today, Delta has about 800 Red Coats who walk around airports and use handheld devices to give one-on-one, on-the-spot help to customers in everything ranging from printing boarding passes to directing passengers to the right concourse. Individuals in organizations make decisions, choices from among two or more alternatives. Top managers determine their organization’s goals, what products or services to offer, how best to finance operations, or where to locate a new manufacturing plant. Middle- and lower-level managers set production schedules, select new employees, and decide how to allocate pay raises. Nonmanagerial employees decide how much effort to put forth at work and whether to comply with a boss’s request. Organizations have begun empowering their nonmanagerial employees with decision-making authority historically reserved for managers alone. Individual decision making is thus an important part of organizational behavior. But the way individuals make decisions and the quality of their choices are largely influenced by their perceptions. Decision making occurs as a reaction to a problem.26 That is, a discrepancy exists between the current state of affairs and some desired state, requiring us to consider alternative courses of action. If your car breaks down and you rely on it to get to work, you have a problem that requires a decision on your part. Unfortunately, most problems don’t come neatly labeled “problem.” One person’s problem is another person’s satisfactory state of affairs. One manager may view her division’s 2 percent decline in quarterly sales to be a serious problem requiring immediate action on her part. In contrast, her counterpart in another division, who also had a 2 percent sales decrease, might consider that quite acceptable. So awareness that a problem exists and that a decision might or might not be needed is a perceptual issue. Every decision requires us to interpret and evaluate information. We typically receive data from multiple sources and need to screen, process, and interpret them. Which data are relevant to the decision, and which are not? Our perceptions will answer that question. We also need to develop alternatives and evaluate their strengths and weaknesses. Again, our perceptual Source: Jessica Ebelhar / The New York Times/ Redux Pictures 4 Decision Making in Organizations 175 process will affect the final outcome. Finally, throughout the entire decisionmaking process, perceptual distortions often surface that can bias analysis and conclusions. Decision Making in Organizations 5 Apply the rational model of decision making and contrast it with bounded rationality and intuition. Business schools generally train students to follow rational decision-making models. While models have considerable merit, they don’t always describe how people actually make decisions. This is where OB enters the picture: to improve the way we make decisions in organizations, we must understand the decisionmaking errors people commit (in addition to the perception errors we’ve discussed). Next we describe these errors, beginning with a brief overview of the rational decision-making model. The Rational Model, Bounded Rationality, and Intuition Rational Decision Making We often think the best decision maker is rational and makes consistent, value-maximizing choices within specified constraints.27 These decisions follow a six-step rational decision-making model.28 The six steps are listed in Exhibit 6-3. The rational decision-making model relies on a number of assumptions, including that the decision maker has complete information, is able to identify all the relevant options in an unbiased manner, and chooses the option with the highest utility.29 As you might imagine, most decisions in the real world don’t follow the rational model. People are usually content to find an acceptable or reasonable solution to a problem rather than an optimal one. Choices tend to be limited to the neighborhood of the problem symptom and the current alternative. As one expert in decision making put it, “Most significant decisions are made by judgment, rather than by a defined prescriptive model.”30 What’s more, people are remarkably unaware of making suboptimal decisions.31 Exhibit 6-3 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. Steps in the Rational Decision-Making Model Define the problem. Identify the decision criteria. Allocate weights to the criteria. Develop the alternatives. Evaluate the alternatives. Select the best alternative. decisions Choices made from among rational Characterized by making rational decision-making model A two or more alternatives. problem A discrepancy between the current state of affairs and some desired state. consistent, value-maximizing choices within specified constraints. decision-making model that describes how individuals should behave in order to maximize some outcome. 176 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making Bounded Rationality Our limited information-processing capability makes it impossible to assimilate and understand all the information necessary to optimize.32 So most people respond to a complex problem by reducing it to a level at which they can readily understand it. Also many problems don’t have an optimal solution because they are too complicated to fit the rational decision-making model. So people satisfice; they seek solutions that are satisfactory and sufficient. When you considered which college to attend, did you look at every viable alternative? Did you carefully identify all the criteria that were important in your decision? Did you evaluate each alternative against the criteria in order to find the optimal college? The answers are probably “no.” Well, don’t feel bad. Few people made their college choice this way. Instead of optimizing, you probably satisficed. Because the human mind cannot formulate and solve complex problems with full rationality, we operate within the confines of bounded rationality. We construct simplified models that extract the essential features from problems without capturing all their complexity.33 We can then behave rationally within the limits of the simple model. How does bounded rationality work for the typical individual? Once we’ve identified a problem, we begin to search for criteria and alternatives. But the criteria are unlikely to be exhaustive. We identify choices that are easy to find and highly visible and that usually represent familiar criteria and tried-andtrue solutions. Next, we begin reviewing them, focusing on alternatives that differ little from the choice currently in effect until we identify one that is “good enough”—that meets an acceptable level of performance. That ends our search. So the solution represents a satisficing choice—the first acceptable one we encounter—rather than an optimal one. Satisficing is not always a bad idea—a simple process may frequently be more sensible than the traditional rational decision-making model.34 To use the rational model in the real world, you need to gather a great deal of information about all the options, compute applicable weights, and then calculate values across a huge number of criteria. All these processes can cost time, energy, Source: Imagechina/AP Images Top managers of Nike, Inc. operated within the confines of bounded rationality in making a decision about its operations in China. To reinforce its future development and rapid growth in China, Nike decided to invest $99 million to build the China Logistics Center, a new distribution facility in Jiangsu for the company’s footwear, apparel, and equipment products. With China overtaking Japan as Nike’s second largest market after the United States, the new distribution center is expected to reduce product delivery times by up to 14 percent to the more than 3,000 Nike retail stores in China. Decision Making in Organizations 177 and money. And if there are many unknown weights and preferences, the fully rational model may not be any more accurate than a best guess. Sometimes a fast-and-frugal process of solving problems might be your best option. Returning to your college choice, would it really be smarter to fly around the country to visit dozens of potential campuses, paying application fees for all these options? Can you really even know what type of college is “best” for you when you’re just graduating from high school, or is there a lot of unknown information about how your interests are going to develop over time? Maybe you won’t major in the same subject you started with. It might be much smarter to find a few colleges that match most of your preferences and then focus your attention on differentiating between those. Intuition Perhaps the least rational way of making decisions is intuitive decision making, an unconscious process created from distilled experience.35 It occurs outside conscious thought; it relies on holistic associations, or links between disparate pieces of information; it’s fast; and it’s affectively charged, meaning it usually engages the emotions.36 While intuition isn’t rational, it isn’t necessarily wrong. Nor does it always contradict rational analysis; rather, the two can complement each other. But nor is intuition superstition, or the product of some magical or paranormal sixth sense. As one recent review noted, “Intuition is a highly complex and highly developed form of reasoning that is based on years of experience and learning.”37 For most of the twentieth century, experts believed decision makers’ use of intuition was irrational or ineffective. That’s no longer the case.38 We now recognize that rational analysis has been overemphasized and, in certain instances, relying on intuition can improve decision making.39 But we can’t rely on it too much. Because it is so unquantifiable, it’s hard to know when our hunches are right or wrong. The key is neither to abandon nor rely solely on intuition, but to supplement it with evidence and good judgment. Common Biases and Errors in Decision Making 6 List and explain the common decision biases or errors. Decision makers engage in bounded rationality, but they also allow systematic biases and errors to creep into their judgments.40 To minimize effort and avoid difficult trade-offs, people tend to rely too heavily on experience, impulses, gut feelings, and convenient rules of thumb. These shortcuts can be helpful. However, they can also distort rationality. Following are the most common biases in decision making. Exhibit 6-4 provides some suggestions for how to avoid falling into these biases and errors. Overconfidence Bias It’s been said that “no problem in judgment and decision making is more prevalent and more potentially catastrophic than overconfidence.”41 When we’re given factual questions and asked to judge the probability that our answers are correct, we tend to be far too optimistic. When people say they’re 90 percent confident about the range a certain number might take, their estimated ranges contain the correct answer only about 50 percent of the time— and experts are no more accurate in setting up confidence intervals than are bounded rationality A process of making decisions by constructing simplified models that extract the essential features from problems without capturing all their complexity. intuitive decision making An unconscious process created out of distilled experience. 178 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making Exhibit 6-4 Reducing Biases and Errors Focus on Goals. Without goals, you can’t be rational, you don’t know what information you need, you don’t know which information is relevant and which is irrelevant, you’ll find it difficult to choose between alternatives, and you’re far more likely to experience regret over the choices you make. Clear goals make decision making easier and help you eliminate options that are inconsistent with your interests. Look for Information That Disconfirms Your Beliefs. One of the most effective means for counteracting overconfidence and the confirmation and hindsight biases is to actively look for information that contradicts your beliefs and assumptions. When we overtly consider various ways we could be wrong, we challenge our tendencies to think we’re smarter than we actually are. Don’t Try to Create Meaning out of Random Events. The educated mind has been trained to look for cause-and-effect relationships. When something happens, we ask why. And when we can’t find reasons, we often invent them. You have to accept that there are events in life that are outside your control. Ask yourself if patterns can be meaningfully explained or whether they are merely coincidence. Don’t attempt to create meaning out of coincidence. Increase Your Options. No matter how many options you’ve identified, your final choice can be no better than the best of the option set you’ve selected. This argues for increasing your decision alternatives and for using creativity in developing a wide range of diverse choices. The more alternatives you can generate, and the more diverse those alternatives, the greater your chance of finding an outstanding one. Source: S. P. Robbins, Decide & Conquer: Making Winning Decisions and Taking Control of Your Life (Upper Saddle River, NJ: Financial Times/Prentice Hall, 2004), pp. 164–168. novices.42 When people say they’re 100 percent sure of an outcome, they tend to be 70 to 85 percent correct.43 Here’s another interesting example. In one random-sample national poll, 90 percent of U.S. adults said they expected to go to heaven. But in another random-sample national poll, only 86 percent thought Mother Teresa was in heaven. Talk about an overconfidence bias! Individuals whose intellectual and interpersonal abilities are weakest are most likely to overestimate their performance and ability.44 There’s also a negative relationship between entrepreneurs’ optimism and the performance of their new ventures: the more optimistic, the less successful.45 The tendency to be too confident about their ideas might keep some from planning how to avoid problems that arise. Investor overconfidence operates in a variety of ways.46 Finance professor Terrance Odean says “people think they know more than they do, and it costs them.” Investors, especially novices, overestimate not just their own skill in processing information, but also the quality of the information they’re working with. Test your own confidence level with investments: compare the long-term returns of your stock market picks relative to index funds. You’ll find an overall index performs as well as, or better than, carefully hand-picked stocks. The main reason many people resist index funds is that they think they’re better at picking stocks than the average person, but most investors will actually do only as well as or only slightly better than the market as a whole. Anchoring Bias The anchoring bias is a tendency to fixate on initial information and fail to adequately adjust for subsequent information.47 It occurs because our mind appears to give a disproportionate amount of emphasis to the first information it receives. Anchors are widely used by people in professions in which persuasion skills are important—advertising, management, politics, real estate, and law. Assume two pilots—Jason and Glenda—have been laid Decision Making in Organizations 179 off their current jobs, and after an extensive search their best offers are from Delta Airlines. Each would earn the average annual pay of Delta’s narrow-body jet pilots: $126,000. Jason was a pilot for Pinnacle, a regional airline where the average annual salary is $82,000. Glenda was a pilot for FedEx, where the average annual salary is $200,000. Which pilot is most likely to accept, or be happiest with, Delta’s offer? Obviously Jason, because he is anchored by the lower salary.48 Any time a negotiation takes place, so does anchoring. When a prospective employer asks how much you made in your prior job, your answer typically anchors the employer’s offer. (Remember this when you negotiate your salary, but set the anchor only as high as you realistically can.) Finally, the more precise your anchor, the smaller the adjustment. Some research suggests people think of making an adjustment after an anchor is set as rounding off a number. If you suggest a target salary of $55,000, your boss will consider $50,000 to $60,000 a reasonable range for negotiation, but if you mention $55,650, your boss is more likely to consider $55,000 to $56,000 the range of likely values.49 Confirmation Bias The rational decision-making process assumes we objectively gather information. But we don’t. We selectively gather it. The confirmation bias represents a specific case of selective perception: we seek out information that reaffirms our past choices, and we discount information that contradicts them.50 We also tend to accept at face value information that confirms our preconceived views, while we are critical and skeptical of information that challenges them. Therefore, the information we gather is typically biased toward supporting views we already hold. We even tend to seek sources most likely to tell us what we want to hear, and we give too much weight to supporting information and too little to contradictory. Interestingly, we are most prone to the confirmation bias when we believe we have good information and strongly believe in our opinions. Fortunately, those who feel there is a strong need to be accurate in making a decision are less prone to the confirmation bias. Availability Bias More people fear flying than fear driving in a car. But if flying on a commercial airline really were as dangerous as driving, the equivalent of two 747s filled to capacity would crash every week, killing all aboard. Because the media give much more attention to air accidents, we tend to overstate the risk of flying and understate the risk of driving. The availability bias is our tendency to base judgments on information readily available.51 Events that evoke emotions, are particularly vivid, or are more recent tend to be more available in our memory, leading us to overestimate the chances of unlikely events such as an airplane crash. The availability bias can also explain why managers doing performance appraisals give more weight to recent employee behaviors than to behaviors of 6 or 9 months earlier, or why credit-rating agencies such as Moody’s or Standard & Poor’s may issue overly positive ratings by relying on information presented by debt issuers, who have an incentive to offer data favorable to their case.52 anchoring bias A tendency to fixate confirmation bias The tendency to availability bias The tendency for on initial information, from which one then fails to adequately adjust for subsequent information. seek out information that reaffirms past choices and to discount information that contradicts past judgments. people to base their judgments on information that is readily available to them. 180 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making Escalation of Commitment Another distortion that creeps into decisions is a tendency to escalate commitment.53 Escalation of commitment refers to staying with a decision even when there is clear evidence it’s wrong. Consider a friend who has been dating someone for several years. Although he admits things aren’t going too well, he says he is still going to marry her. His justification: “I have a lot invested in the relationship!” Individuals escalate commitment to a failing course of action when they view themselves as responsible for the failure.54 They “throw good money after bad” to demonstrate their initial decision wasn’t wrong and to avoid admitting they made a mistake.55 In fact, people who carefully gather and consider information consistent with the rational decision-making model are more likely to engage in escalation of commitment than those who spend less time thinking about their choices.56 Perhaps they have invested so much time and energy in making their decisions that they have convinced themselves they’re taking the right course of action and don’t update their knowledge in the face of new information. Many an organization has suffered because a manager determined to prove his or her original decision right continued to commit resources to a lost cause. Randomness Error Most of us like to think we have some control over our world and our destiny. Our tendency to believe we can predict the outcome of random events is the randomness error. Decision making suffers when we try to create meaning in random events, particularly when we turn imaginary patterns into superstitions.57 These can be completely contrived (“I never make important decisions on Friday the 13th”) or can evolve from a reinforced past pattern of behavior (Tiger Woods often wears a red shirt during a golf tournament’s final round because he won many junior tournaments wearing red shirts). Superstitious behavior can be debilitating when it affects daily judgments or biases major decisions. Risk Aversion Mathematically, we should find a 50–50 flip of the coin for $100 to be worth as much as a sure promise of $50. After all, the expected value of the gamble over a number of trials is $50. However, nearly everyone but committed gamblers would rather have the sure thing than a risky prospect.58 For many people, a 50–50 flip of a coin even for $200 might not be worth as much as a sure promise of $50, even though the gamble is mathematically worth twice as much! This tendency to prefer a sure thing over a risky outcome is risk aversion. Risk aversion has important implications. To offset the risks inherent in a commission-based wage, companies pay commissioned employees considerably more than they do those on straight salaries. Risk-averse employees will stick with the established way of doing their jobs, rather than taking a chance on innovative or creative methods. Sticking with a strategy that has worked in the past does minimize risk, but in the long run it will lead to stagnation. Ambitious people with power that can be taken away (most managers) appear to be especially risk averse, perhaps because they don’t want to lose on a gamble everything they’ve worked so hard to achieve.59 CEOs at risk of being terminated are also exceptionally risk averse, even when a riskier investment strategy is in their firms’ best interests.60 Because people are less likely to escalate commitment where there is a great deal of uncertainty, the implications of risk aversion aren’t all bad.61 When a risky investment isn’t paying off, most people would rather play it safe and cut their losses, but if they think the outcome is a sure thing, they’ll keep escalating. Risk preference is sometimes reversed: people prefer to take their chances when trying to prevent a negative outcome.62 They would rather take a 50–50 gamble on Decision Making in Organizations 181 Myth or Science? Creative Decision Making Is a Right-Brain Activity O ne article of faith in creativity research and practice is that whereas the left brain governs analytical, rational thinking, the right brain underlies creative thinking. However, judging from a recent review of neuropsychology research, this accepted wisdom is false. Neuropsychologists study creativity by asking people to engage in creative thinking, which they measure in different ways. In the Remote Associates Test, individuals indicate what word links a series of three words (such as Falling Actor Dust; Salt Deep Foam). Other tests ask individuals to compose creative stories, write captions for cartoons, or provide unique solutions to unusual hypothetical problems. While participants are thinking creatively, the researchers assess their brain activity using various techniques, including MRI. A recent review of 72 studies found right brain activity was not associated with creative thinking. The authors conclude, “Creativity, or any alleged stage of it, is not particularly associated with the right brain or any part of the right brain.” Indeed, the review showed it was difficult to isolate creative thinking in any one region of the brain. Another review of 45 studies reached the same conclusion, noting that the diverse ways in which creativity and brain activity were measured made generalizations difficult. These results do not discourage all neuropsychologists. One neuroscientist, Oshin Vartanian, summed up the literature as follows: “Initially, a lot of people were looking for the holy grail. They were searching for the creativity module in the brain. Now we know it is more complicated.” Sources: A. Dietrich and R. Kanso, “A Review of EEG, ERP, and Neuroimaging Studies of Creativity and Insight,” Psychological Bulletin 136, no. 5 (2010), pp. 822–848; R. Ardena, R. S. Chavez, R. Grazioplene, & R. E. Jung, “Neuroimaging Creativity: A Psychometric View,” Behavioural Brain Research 214, no. 2 (2010), pp. 143–156; and A. McIlroy, “Neuroscientists Try to Unlock the Origins of Creativity,” Globe and Mail (January 28, 2011), downloaded May 20, 2011, from www.theglobeandmail.com/. losing $100 than accept the certain loss of $50. Thus they will risk losing a lot of money at trial rather than settle out of court. Trying to cover up wrongdoing instead of admitting a mistake, despite the risk of truly catastrophic press coverage or even jail time, is another example. Stressful situations can make these risk preferences stronger. People will more likely engage in risk-seeking behavior for negative outcomes, and risk-averse behavior for positive outcomes, when under stress.63 Hindsight Bias The hindsight bias is the tendency to believe falsely, after the outcome is known, that we’d have accurately predicted it.64 When we have accurate feedback on the outcome, we seem pretty good at concluding it was obvious. Over the past 10 years, the home video rental industry has been collapsing fast as online distribution outlets have eaten away at the market.65 Hollywood Video declared bankruptcy in May 2010 and began liquidating its assets; Blockbuster filed for bankruptcy in September 2010. Some have suggested that if only these organizations had leveraged their brand and distribution resources escalation of commitment An increased commitment to a previous decision in spite of negative information. randomness error The tendency of individuals to believe that they can predict the outcome of random events. risk aversion The tendency to prefer a sure gain of a moderate amount over a riskier outcome, even if the riskier outcome might have a higher expected payoff. hindsight bias The tendency to believe falsely, after an outcome of an event is actually known, that one would have accurately predicted that outcome. 182 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making effectively and sooner to develop web-based delivery, as Netflix does, and lowcost distribution in grocery and convenience stores, which Redbox offers, they could have been avoided failure. While that might seem obvious now, many experts with good information failed to see these two major trends that would upend the industry. Of course, after the fact, it is easy to see that a combination of automated and mail-order distribution would outperform the traditional brick-and-mortar movie rental business. Similarly, former Merrill Lynch CEO John Thain—and many other Wall Street executives—took blame for failing to see what now seems obvious (that housing prices were inflated, too many risky loans were made, and the values of many “securities” were based on fragile assumptions). Though the criticisms may have merit, things are often all too clear in hindsight. As Malcolm Gladwell, author of Blink and The Tipping Point, writes, “What is clear in hindsight is rarely clear before the fact.”66 The hindsight bias reduces our ability to learn from the past. It lets us think we’re better predictors than we are and can make us falsely confident. If your actual predictive accuracy is only 40 percent but you think it’s 90, you’re likely to be less skeptical about your predictive skills. When the U.S.-based global financial services firm Lehman Brothers failed, many people blamed Richard Fuld, the company’s chief executive, accusing him of overconfidence, confirmation, and anchoring biases and a lack of knowledge about complicated financial investment instruments. Even as Lehman Brothers continued to post billiondollar losses, Fuld exuded confidence that the investment bank was sound, and he rebuffed criticism of the bank’s failure to value its assets accurately. Comments by former employees and passersby blaming Fuld for the bankruptcy were recorded on an artists’ rendering of him placed in front of Lehman’s offices in New York City. Source: Louis Lanzanop/AP Images Application: Financial Decision Making This discussion of decision making errors may have you thinking about how organizations and individuals make financial decisions. Did decision errors influence capital markets and even lead to crises like the financial meltdown of 2008? How are financial decisions affected by errors and biases? Experts have identified several ways this can occur.67 One of the core problems that created the financial crisis was that large loans were made to individuals who could not repay them, and finance companies purchased these bad debts without realizing how poor the prospects of repayment were. Thus, overconfidence bias by both lenders and borrowers Decision Making in Organizations OB Poll 183 Gloomy Perceptions Return Percent Indicating Economy Is in Recession or Depression 80 Depression 70 33% 60 50 40 30 Recession 29% 12% 33% 36% 20 26% 10 0 February, 2008 September, 2008 April, 2011 Source: Based on C. Merkle and M. Weber, “True Overconfidence—The Inability of Rational Information Processing to Account for Apparent Overconfidence” (March 2009). Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract⫽1373675 about the ability to pay back loans was clearly a major factor. Most studies suggest that people are more willing to buy on credit and spend more money when they feel confident. Although experts were no more accurate at predicting financial outcomes than were people without knowledge or skills in finance, they were more confident in their predictions. Unfortunately, as confidence decreases in the face of poor economic data, businesses and consumers become more conservative in their spending. This further decreases demand for products and services, which deepens the economic crisis in a vicious cycle. Overconfidence isn’t the only decision error implicated in the financial crisis. Investors deliberately avoid negative information about investments, an example of the confirmation bias. Lenders may have overlooked potential problems with borrowers’ accounts when making loans, and stock traders may have ignored information about potential problems with complex derivatives when making purchasing decisions. Once a loan has been paid off, lenders also selectively ignore the negative effects of debt, making them more likely to make unwise loans in the future. What might prevent these situations from occurring in the future? Both investors and consumers may need to more carefully consider whether their confidence level is aligned with their actual future ability to pay. It is also always a good idea to seek information that goes against your initial inclinations, to ensure you’re getting the whole picture. Be careful not to commit the hindsight bias and conclude after financial crises have dissipated that it should have been obvious problems were about to occur. S A L SELF-ASSESSMENT LIBRARY Am I A Deliberate Decision Maker? In the Self-Assessment Library (available on CD or online), take assessment IV.A.2 (Am I a Deliberate Decision Maker?). Would it be better to be a more deliberate decision maker? Why or why not? 184 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making Influences on Decision Making: Individual Differences and Organizational Constraints We turn here to factors that influence how people make decisions and the degree to which they are susceptible to errors and biases. We discuss individual differences and organizational constraints. Individual Differences 7 Explain how individual differences and organizational constraints affect decision making. Decision making in practice is characterized by bounded rationality, common biases and errors, and the use of intuition. In addition, individual differences create deviations from the rational model. In this section, we look at two such differences: personality and gender. Personality The little research so far conducted on personality and decision making suggests personality does influence our decisions. Let’s look at conscientiousness and self-esteem (both discussed in Chapter 5). Specific facets of conscientiousness—rather than the broad trait itself— may affect escalation of commitment (above).68 Two such facets—achievement striving and dutifulness—actually had opposite effects. Achievement-striving people were more likely to escalate their commitment, whereas dutiful people were less likely. Why? Generally, achievement-oriented people hate to fail, so they escalate their commitment, hoping to forestall failure. Dutiful people, however, are more inclined to do what they see as best for the organization. Second, achievement-striving individuals appear more susceptible to the hindsight bias, perhaps because they have a greater need to justify their actions.69 Unfortunately, we don’t have evidence on whether dutiful people are immune to this bias. Finally, people with high self-esteem are strongly motivated to maintain it, so they use the self-serving bias to preserve it. They blame others for their failures while taking credit for successes.70 Gender Research on rumination offers insights into gender differences in decision making.71 Rumination refers to reflecting at length. In terms of decision making, it means overthinking problems. Twenty years of study find women spend much more time than men analyzing the past, present, and future. They’re more likely to overanalyze problems before making a decision and to rehash a decision once made. This can lead to more careful consideration of problems and choices. However, it can make problems harder to solve, increase regret over past decisions, and increase depression. Women are nearly twice as likely as men to develop depression.72 Why women ruminate more than men is not clear. One view is that parents encourage and reinforce the expression of sadness and anxiety more in girls than in boys. Another theory is that women, more than men, base their selfesteem and well-being on what others think of them. A third idea is that women are more empathetic and more affected by events in others’ lives, so they have more to ruminate about. By age 11, girls are ruminating more than boys. But this gender difference seems to lessen with age. Differences are largest during young adulthood and smallest after age 65, when both men and women ruminate the least.73 Influences on Decision Making: Individual Differences and Organizational Constraints 185 An Ethical Choice Whose Ethical Standards to Follow? A s we note in the chapter, different standards identify ethical behavior in different cultures. Managers who work in international contexts frequently run into complex problems when behavior acceptable for the home office is unacceptable in local cultures, and vice versa. How difficult is it to arrive at a global policy for ethical decision making? Consider these examples. Individuals from countries higher in Hofstede’s dimension of power distance, like those in Latin America, were more likely to report that bribery was acceptable than were individuals from lower power distance countries, like the United States and much of Europe. An international scandal ensued after German corporation Siemens set aside money for bribes when working in Africa, but company executives initially defended the actions as consistent with business practices in those countries. Such differences don’t mean business ethics are “higher” or “lower” in different countries. Although U.S. executives might frown on bribery, they may view it as ethical and necessary to lay workers off during poor economic times, whereas other cultures view layoffs as a betrayal of the relationship between workers and employers. So how are managers to decide which ethical standards to observe? Is it better to act consistently with your own culture, or adopt the ethical standards of the countries where you operate? This question isn’t easy to answer. Ethics experts suggest leaders should adhere to universal values of human life and welfare as the core of their international codes of ethics, and they note there are few cases where values are directly in conflict (no culture actively encourages bribery as moral; it’s just that some societies view it as less of a problem than others do). Source: Based on L. J. Thompson, “The Global Moral Compass for Business Leaders,” Journal of Business Ethics 93, no. S1 (2010), pp. 15–32; C. Baughn, N. L. Bodie, M. A. Buchanan, and M. B. Bixby, “Bribery in International Business Transactions,” Journal of Business Ethics 92, no. 1 (2010), pp. 15–32; and T. Patel and A. Schaefer, “Making Sense of the Diversity of Ethical Decision Making in Business: An Illustration of the Indian Context,” Journal of Business Ethics 90, no. 2 (2009), pp. 171–186. Mental Ability We know people with higher levels of mental ability are able to process information more quickly, solve problems more accurately, and learn faster, so you might expect them also to be less susceptible to common decision errors. However, mental ability appears to help people avoid only some of these.74 Smart people are just as likely to fall prey to anchoring, overconfidence, and escalation of commitment, probably because just being smart doesn’t alert you to the possibility you’re too confident or emotionally defensive. That doesn’t mean intelligence never matters. Once warned about decision-making errors, more intelligent people learn more quickly to avoid them. They are also better able to avoid logical errors like false syllogisms or incorrect interpretation of data. Cultural Differences The rational model makes no acknowledgment of cultural differences, nor does the bulk of OB research literature on decision making. But Indonesians, for instance, don’t necessarily make decisions the same way Australians do. Therefore, we need to recognize that the cultural background of a decision maker can significantly influence the selection of problems, the depth of analysis, the importance placed on logic and rationality, and whether organizational decisions should be made autocratically by an individual manager or collectively in groups.75 Cultures differ in their time orientation, the importance of rationality, their belief in the ability of people to solve problems, and their preference for collective decision making. Differences in time orientation help us understand why managers in Egypt make decisions at a much slower and more deliberate pace than their U.S. counterparts. While rationality is valued in North America, that’s not true elsewhere in the world. A North American manager might make an important decision intuitively but know it’s important to appear to proceed 186 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making in a rational fashion because rationality is highly valued in the West. In countries such as Iran, where rationality is not as paramount as other factors, efforts to appear rational are not necessary. Some cultures emphasize solving problems, while others focus on accepting situations as they are. The United States falls in the first category; Thailand and Indonesia are examples of the second. Because problem-solving managers believe they can and should change situations to their benefit, U.S. managers might identify a problem long before their Thai or Indonesian counterparts would choose to recognize it as such. Decision making by Japanese managers is much more group-oriented than in the United States. The Japanese value conformity and cooperation. So before Japanese CEOs make an important decision, they collect a large amount of information, which they use in consensus-forming group decisions. In short, there are probably important cultural differences in decision making, but unfortunately not yet much research to identify them. Organizational Constraints Organizations can constrain decision makers, creating deviations from the rational model. For instance, managers shape their decisions to reflect the organization’s performance evaluation and reward system, to comply with its formal regulations, and to meet organizationally imposed time constraints. Precedent can also limit decisions. Performance Evaluation Managers are strongly influenced by the criteria on which they are evaluated. If a division manager believes the manufacturing plants under his responsibility are operating best when he hears nothing negative, we shouldn’t be surprised to find his plant managers spending a good part of their time ensuring that negative information doesn’t reach him. Reward Systems The organization’s reward system influences decision makers by suggesting which choices have better personal payoffs. If the organization rewards risk aversion, managers are more likely to make conservative decisions. From the 1930s through the mid-1980s, General Motors consistently gave promotions and bonuses to managers who kept a low profile and avoided controversy. These executives became adept at dodging tough issues and passing controversial decisions on to committees. Formal Regulations David Gonzalez, a shift manager at a Taco Bell restaurant in San Antonio, Texas, describes constraints he faces on his job: “I’ve got rules and regulations covering almost every decision I make—from how to make a burrito to how often I need to clean the restrooms. My job doesn’t come with much freedom of choice.” David’s situation is not unique. All but the smallest organizations create rules and policies to program decisions and get individuals to act in the intended manner. And of course, in so doing, they limit decision choices. System-Imposed Time Constraints Almost all important decisions come with explicit deadlines. A report on new-product development may have to be ready for executive committee review by the first of the month. Such conditions often make it difficult, if not impossible, for managers to gather all the information they might like before making a final choice. Historical Precedents Decisions aren’t made in a vacuum; they have a context. In fact, individual decisions are points in a stream of choice. Those made in the What About Ethics in Decision Making? 187 Source: Newscom Formal regulations shape employee decisions at McDonald’s restaurants throughout the world. McDonald’s standardizes the behavior of restaurant crew members such as the employee shown here preparing the company’s specialty coffee, McCafe Mocha. McDonald’s requires employees to follow rules and regulations for food preparation and service to meet the company’s high standards of food quality and safety and reliable and friendly service. For example, McDonald’s requires 72 safety protocols to be conducted every day in each restaurant as part of a daily monitoring routine for restaurant managers. past are like ghosts that haunt and constrain current choices. It’s common knowledge that the largest determinant of the size of any given year’s budget is last year’s budget.76 Choices made today are largely a result of choices made over the years. What About Ethics in Decision Making? 8 Contrast the three ethical decision criteria. Ethical considerations should be an important criterion in all organizational decision making. In this section, we present three ways to frame decisions ethically.77 Three Ethical Decision Criteria The first ethical yardstick is utilitarianism, which proposes making decisions solely on the basis of their outcomes, ideally to provide the greatest good for the greatest number. This view dominates business decision making. It is consistent with goals such as efficiency, productivity, and high profits. Another ethical criterion is to make decisions consistent with fundamental liberties and privileges, as set forth in documents such as the Bill of Rights. An emphasis on rights in decision making means respecting and protecting the basic rights of individuals, such as the right to privacy, free speech, and due process. This criterion protects whistle-blowers when they reveal an organization’s unethical practices to the press or government agencies, using their right to free speech. utilitarianism A system in which whistle-blowers Individuals who decisions are made to provide the greatest good for the greatest number. report unethical practices by their employer to outsiders. 188 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making A third criterion is to impose and enforce rules fairly and impartially to ensure justice or an equitable distribution of benefits and costs. Union members typically favor this view. It justifies paying people the same wage for a given job regardless of performance differences and using seniority as the primary determination in layoff decisions. Each criterion has advantages and liabilities. A focus on utilitarianism promotes efficiency and productivity, but it can sideline the rights of some individuals, particularly those with minority representation. The use of rights protects individuals from injury and is consistent with freedom and privacy, but it can create a legalistic environment that hinders productivity and efficiency. A focus on justice protects the interests of the underrepresented and less powerful, but it can encourage a sense of entitlement that reduces risk taking, innovation, and productivity. Decision makers, particularly in for-profit organizations, feel comfortable with utilitarianism. The “best interests” of the organization and its stockholders can justify a lot of questionable actions, such as large layoffs. But many critics feel this perspective needs to change.78 Public concern about individual rights and social justice suggests managers should develop ethical standards based on nonutilitarian criteria. This presents a challenge because satisfying individual rights and social justice creates far more ambiguities than utilitarian effects on efficiency and profits. However, while raising prices, selling products with questionable effects on consumer health, closing down inefficient plants, laying off large numbers of employees, and moving production overseas to cut costs can be justified in utilitarian terms, that may no longer be the single measure by which good decisions are judged. Improving Creativity in Decision Making 9 Define creativity and discuss the three-component model of creativity. MyManagementLab For an interactive application of this topic, check out this chapter’s simulation activity at www.mymanagementlab.com. Although the rational decision-making model will often improve decisions, a rational decision maker also needs creativity, the ability to produce novel and useful ideas.79 These are different from what’s been done before but appropriate to the problem presented. Creativity allows the decision maker to more fully appraise and understand the problem, including seeing problems others can’t see. L’Oréal puts its managers through creative exercises such as cooking or making music, and the University of Chicago requires MBA students to make short movies about their experiences. Creative Potential Most people have useful creative potential. But to unleash it, they have to escape the psychological ruts many of us fall into and learn how to think about a problem in divergent ways. Exceptional creativity is scarce. We all know of creative geniuses in science (Albert Einstein), art (Pablo Picasso), and business (Steve Jobs). But what about the typical individual? Intelligent people and those who score high on openness to experience (see Chapter 5) are more likely to be creative.80 Other traits of creative people are independence, self-confidence, risk taking, an internal locus of control, tolerance for ambiguity, a low need for structure, and perseverance.81 Exposure to a variety of cultures can also improve creativity.82 So taking an international assignment, or even an international vacation, could jump-start your creative process. A study of the lifetime creativity of 461 men and women found fewer than 1 percent were exceptionally creative.83 But 10 percent were highly creative and about 60 percent were somewhat creative. This reinforces that most of us have creative potential; we just need to learn to unleash it. What About Ethics in Decision Making? 189 Three-Component Model of Creativity What can individuals and organizations do to stimulate employee creativity? The best answer lies in the three-component model of creativity,84 which proposes that individual creativity essentially requires expertise, creative thinking skills, and intrinsic task motivation. Studies confirm that the higher the level of each, the higher the creativity. Expertise is the foundation for all creative work. Film writer, producer, and director Quentin Tarantino spent his youth working in a video rental store, where he built up an encyclopedic knowledge of movies. The potential for creativity is enhanced when individuals have abilities, knowledge, proficiencies, and similar expertise in their field of endeavor. You wouldn’t expect someone with minimal knowledge of programming to be very creative as a software engineer. The second component is creative-thinking skills. This encompasses personality characteristics associated with creativity, the ability to use analogies, and the talent to see the familiar in a different light. A meta-analysis of 102 studies found positive moods increase creativity, but it depends on what sort of positive mood was considered.85 Moods such as happiness that encourage interaction with the world are more conducive to creativity than passive moods such as calm. This means the common advice to relax and clear your mind to develop creative ideas may be misplaced. It would be better to get in an upbeat mood and then frame your work as an opportunity to have fun and experiment. Negative moods also don’t always have the same effects on creativity. Passive negative moods such as sadness doesn’t seem to have much effect, but avoidance-oriented negative moods such as fear and anxiety decrease creativity. Feeling threatened reduces your desire to try new activities; risk aversion increases when you’re scared. Active negative moods, such as anger, however, do appear to enhance creativity, especially if you are taking your task seriously. Being around creative others can make us more inspired, especially if we’re creatively “stuck.”86 One study found that having “weak ties” to creative people—knowing them but not well—facilitates creativity because the people are there as a resource if we need them but not so close as to stunt our own independent thinking.87 Analogies allow decision makers to apply an idea from one context to another. One of the most famous examples was Alexander Graham Bell’s observation that it might be possible to apply the way the ear operates to his “talking box.” Bell noticed the bones in the ear are operated by a delicate, thin membrane. He wondered why a thicker and stronger membrane shouldn’t be able to move a piece of steel. From that analogy, the telephone was conceived. Thinking in terms of analogies is a complex intellectual skill, which helps explain why cognitive ability is related to creativity. Demonstrating this effect, one study found children who got high scores on cognitive ability tests at age 13 were significantly more likely to have made creative achievements in their professional lives 25 years later.88 Some people develop creative skills because they see problems in a new way. They’re able to make the strange familiar and the familiar strange.89 For instance, most of us think of hens laying eggs. But how many of us have considered that a hen is only an egg’s way of making another egg? creativity The ability to produce novel and useful ideas. three-component model of creativity The proposition that individual creativity requires expertise, creative thinking skills, and intrinsic task motivation. 190 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making Creative people often love their work, to the point of seeming obsession. The final component in the three-component model of creativity is intrinsic task motivation. This is the desire to work on something because it’s interesting, involving, exciting, satisfying, or personally challenging. It’s what turns creativity potential into actual creative ideas. Environmental stimulants that foster creativity include a culture that encourages the flow of ideas; fair and constructive judgment of ideas; rewards and recognition for creative work; sufficient financial, material, and information resources; freedom to decide what work is to be done and how to do it; a supervisor who communicates effectively, shows confidence in others, and supports the work group; and work group members who support and trust each other.90 International Differences There are no global ethical standards,91 as contrasts between Asia and the West illustrate.92 Because bribery is commonplace in countries such as China, a Canadian working in China might face a dilemma: should I pay a bribe to secure business if it is an accepted part of that country’s culture? A manager of a large U.S. company operating in China once caught an employee stealing. Following company policy, she fired him and turned him over to the local authorities. Later, she was horrified to learn the employee had been summarily executed.93 Although ethical standards may seem ambiguous in the West, criteria defining right and wrong are actually much clearer there than in Asia, where few issues are black and white and most are gray. Global organizations must establish ethical principles for decision makers in countries such as India and China and modify them to reflect cultural norms if they want to uphold high standards and consistent practices. S A L SELF-ASSESSMENT LIBRARY How Creative Am I? In the Self-Assessment Library (available on CD or online), take assessment I.A.5 (How Creative Am I?). MyManagementLab Now that you have finished this chapter, go back to www.mymanagementlab.com to continue practicing and applying the concepts you’ve learned. Summary and Implications for Managers Perception Individuals base their behavior not on the way their external environment actually is but rather on what they see or believe it to be. ● ● Whether a manager successfully plans and organizes the work of employees and actually helps them to structure their work more efficiently and effectively is far less important than how employees perceive the manager’s efforts. Employees judge issues such as fair pay, performance appraisals, and working conditions in very individual ways. To influence productivity, we need to assess how workers perceive their jobs. Summary and Implications for Managers 191 Checklists Lead to Better Decisions POINT W hile life and lives sometimes turn on the basis of big decisions, it’s often the little ones that matter more. Our failure to follow routine, everyday protocols makes the world a more dangerous place for ourselves, and for others. A few examples . . . Nearly 100,000 U.S. patients are killed every year by the failure of doctors and nurses to follow simple instructions. Really. Hospital-acquired infections kill that many people every year, and nearly all those deaths are entirely preventable. Most airline crashes occur because pilots ignore the rules. Pilot failure to follow protocols is a primary contributing factor to the majority of incidents and accidents. An important way of attacking these errors is to use checklists. Support for a checklist approach is provided by a new book, The Checklist Manifesto. In it, the author, Harvard Medical School surgeon Atul Gawande notes, “The volume and complexity of what we know has exceeded our ability to deliver its benefits correctly, safely, or reliably.” Unless, of course, we use checklists. Dr. Peter Pronovost, a critical care specialist at Johns Hopkins, developed his own operating room checklist, which included some “no brainers” such as wash your hands with soap, put drapes over entire patient, and put sterile dressing over incisions. Within 1 year of the checklist’s adoption at Johns Hopkins, the post-op infection rate went from 11 percent to zero. According to Gawande, in using checklists to improve decisions, we should keep the following guidelines in mind: ● ● ● ● ● Include all “stupid but critical” tasks so they’re not overlooked. Make it mandatory for team members to inform others when an item on the list is completed (or not). Empower team members to question superiors about the checklist. Allow for improvisation in unusual circumstances. Thoroughly test-drive the checklist before implementing it. Gawande notes that checklists aren’t important only for medical decision making. Engineering, business, technology, safety, and transportation are all industries that would benefit from greater development and use of checklists in everyday decision making. As a project manager noted, “Successful checklists detail both the sequence of necessary activities as well as the communication checkpoints to ensure dialog among project participants.” COUNTERPOINT C hecklists work well, except when they don’t. Checklists have a paradox that makes them of dubious usefulness: the more complex the decision making, ostensibly the more important the checklist. But the more complex the decision making, the less likely that a checklist can or should be followed. Checklists can take an impractical amount of time to follow. Driving an automobile is a routine but complex process. Do you keep a checklist in your car for every time you get behind the wheel? Moreover, by their very nature, complex tasks can pose problems that fall outside the scope of the checklist. The last thing we need to solve unanticipated or complicated problems is rote allegiance to a checklist that is poorly suited to the problem at hand. Indeed, a problem with many poor decisions is that heuristics are too often followed, with little thought to whether the assumptions behind them still hold true. If we have learned anything from the financial crisis, it is that a model or heuristic is only as good as its assumptions. Assume housing prices are properly valued and likely to continue to increase, and it makes all the sense in the world to be aggressive in making loans. Countrywide and Fannie Mae had all sorts of rules, protocols, and checklists they followed in making catastrophically bad loan decisions. As for the medical decision making, as another physician and author, Sandeep Jauhar, noted, advocates of checklists often ignore the unintended consequences. Insurers compensate doctors for ticking off boxes on checklists—like prescribing antibiotics—even when there is no evidence they are warranted. Because this protocol encourages the growth of antibiotic-resistant bacteria, we all are endangered by this checklist-adhering behavior. We want to think we live in a world where decisionmaking errors can be easily solved. We can mitigate some decisions by learning more about decision-making errors, but one of the main learning points is that we need a healthier respect for the degree to which we’re susceptible to errors. Checklists provide a false sense of security and an ignorance about when they cause more problems than they solve. Sources: S. Jauhar, “One Thing After Another,” New York Times Book Review (January 24, 2010), p. 7; C. Arnst, “Make a List. Check it Twice,” Bloomberg Businessweek (February 22, 2010), pp. 78–79; J. Ross, “The Checklist Manifesto and the Digital Divide,” Forbes (July 27, 2010), downloaded on May 7, 2011, from www.forbes.com. 192 CHAPTER 6 Perception and Individual Decision Making ● ● Absenteeism, turnover, and job satisfaction are also reactions to an individual’s perceptions. Dissatisfaction with working conditions and the belief that an organization lacks promotion opportunities are judgments based on attempts to create meaning in the job. The employee’s conclusion that a job is good or bad is an interpretation. Managers must spend time understanding how each individual interprets reality and, when there is a significant difference between what someone sees and what exists, try to eliminate the distortions. Individual Decision Making Individuals think and reason before they act. This is why an understanding of how people make decisions can be helpful for explaining and predicting their behavior. In some decision situations, people follow the rational decision-making model. But few important decisions are simple or unambiguous enough for the rational model’s assumptions to apply. So we find individuals looking for solutions that satisfice rather than optimize, injecting biases and prejudices into the decision process, and relying on intuition. What can managers do to improve their decision making? We offer four suggestions. ● ● ● ● Analyze the situation. Adjust your decision-making approach to the national culture you’re operating in and to the criteria your organization evaluates and rewards. If you’re in a country that doesn’t value rationality, don’t feel compelled to follow the rational decision-making model or to try to make your decisions appear rational. Similarly, organizations differ in the importance they place on risk, the use of groups, and the like. Adjust your decision approach to ensure it’s compatible with the organization’s culture. Second, be aware of biases. Then try to minimize their impact. Exhibit 6-4 offers some suggestions. Third, combine rational analysis with intuition. These are not conflicting approaches to decision making. By using both, you can actually improve your decision-making effectiveness. As you gain managerial experience, you should feel increasingly confident in imposing your intuitive processes on top of your rational analysis. Finally, try to enhance your creativity. Actively look for novel solutions to problems, attempt to see problems in new ways, and use analogies. Try to remove work and organizational barriers that might impede your creativity. QUESTIONS FOR REVIEW 1 6 2 7 3 8 What is perception, and what factors influence our perception? What is attribution theory? What are the three determinants of attribution? What are its implications for explaining organizational behavior? What shortcuts do people frequently use in making judgments about others? 4 What is the link between perception and decision making? How does one affect the other? 5 What is the rational model of decision making? How is it different from bounded rationality and intuition? What are some of the common decision biases or errors that people make? What are the influences of individual differences, organizational constraints, and culture on decision making? Are unethical decisions more a function of an individual decision maker or the decision maker’s work environment? Explain. 9 What is creativity, and what is the three-component model of creativity?