Research on Professional Responsibility and Ethics in Accounting The Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education: Another Look David J. Burns James A. Tackett Fran Wolf Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Article information: To cite this document: David J. Burns James A. Tackett Fran Wolf . "The Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education: Another Look" In Research on Professional Responsibility and Ethics in Accounting. Published online: 16 Oct 2015; 149-180. Permanent link to this document: http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/S1574-076520150000019015 Downloaded on: 14 February 2016, At: 02:00 (PT) References: this document contains references to 0 other documents. To copy this document: permissions@emeraldinsight.com The fulltext of this document has been downloaded 22 times since NaN* Users who downloaded this article also downloaded: P. Nithiarasu, T. Sundararajan, K.N. Seetharamu, (1998),"Finite element analysis of transient natural convection in an odd-shaped enclosure", International Journal of Numerical Methods for Heat &amp; Fluid Flow, Vol. 8 Iss 2 pp. 199-216 http:// dx.doi.org/10.1108/09615539810201839 Aisyah Abdul-Rahman, Raudha Md Ramli, (2015),"Islamic Cross Currency Swap (ICCS): hedging against currency fluctuations", Emerald Emerging Markets Case Studies, Vol. 5 Iss 4 pp. 1-18 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/EEMCS-09-2014-0215 Alex Maritz, Colin Jones, Claudia Shwetzer, (2015),"The status of entrepreneurship education in Australian universities", Education + Training, Vol. 57 Iss 8/9 pp. 1020-1035 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/ET-04-2015-0026 Access to this document was granted through an Emerald subscription provided by emerald-srm:232872 [] For Authors If you would like to write for this, or any other Emerald publication, then please use our Emerald for Authors service information about how to choose which publication to write for and submission guidelines are available for all. Please visit www.emeraldinsight.com/authors for more information. About Emerald www.emeraldinsight.com Emerald is a global publisher linking research and practice to the benefit of society. The company manages a portfolio of more than 290 journals and over 2,350 books and book series volumes, as well as providing an extensive range of online products and additional customer resources and services. Emerald is both COUNTER 4 and TRANSFER compliant. The organization is a partner of the Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) and also works with Portico and the LOCKSS initiative for digital archive preservation. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) *Related content and download information correct at time of download. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) THE EFFECTIVENESS OF INSTRUCTION IN ACCOUNTING ETHICS EDUCATION: ANOTHER LOOK David J. Burns, James A. Tackett and Fran Wolf ABSTRACT This study examines the effectiveness of instruction in accounting ethics as measured by the impact of that instruction on the incidence of student plagiarism in a college writing assignment. This study avoids the potential problems inherent in measuring Machiavellianism via a psychological questionnaire by using a “reverse methodology,” whereby Machiavellianism is assessed directly from behavior. The results support past research suggesting that traditional collegiate ethical education may not affect students’ ethical choices. The findings also suggest that increasing penalties for ethical failures may be an effective means of deterring students and business professionals from engaging in inappropriate activities. This study supports the use of a behavioral measure of Machivellianism as a means of evaluating the effectiveness of alternative instructional Research on Professional Responsibility and Ethics in Accounting, Volume 19, 149 180 Copyright r 2015 by Emerald Group Publishing Limited All rights of reproduction in any form reserved ISSN: 1574-0765/doi:10.1108/S1574-076520150000019015 149 150 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. methods in ethics. This behavioral approach is superior to the traditional questionnaire methodology because Machivellianism is judged based on actual behavior rather than having students respond to hypothetical and often stereotyped ethical cases, whereby the student can provide an artificial response that will be viewed favorably by the evaluator. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) The findings suggest that higher education needs to recognize the relevance of factors beyond mere ethical education when preparing students for the ethical challenges they will face in the business world. This paper employs a unique “reverse methodology” to measure Machiavellianism. This reverse methodology has greater external validity in quasi-experimental ethical studies because the results can be extrapolated to real-world scenarios where there is a cost to behaving ethically. Keywords: Ethics education; Machiavellianism; ethical failures; accounting education; academic dishonesty Virtually all collegiate business programs require extensive ethical preparation in their curricula. Such widespread adoption of business ethics education has resulted primarily from the requirements of the Association to Advance Collegiate Schools of Business (AACSB The primary business accrediting agency), which now mandates that business education include an ethics component (Swanson & Frederick, 2005). The drive to include ethics instruction in business education has also risen from external sources. Some view the ultimate cause of many of the recent business scandals to be weaknesses in the collegiate education of those involved (e.g., Nonis & Swift, 2001; Smith, Davy, Rosenberg, & Haight, 2002). In response, business practitioners and society are demanding increased and more effective instruction in business ethics (Evans & Weiss, 2008; Luthar & Karri, 2005; Waples, Antes, Murphy, Connelly, & Mumford, 2009). This drive also assumes that making students aware of alternative ethical frameworks and providing training on how to analyze ethical situations will translate into ethical behavior when these students become business professionals. Within business, accounting appears to be an area that deserves particular attention (Liu, Yao, & Hu, 2012). A number of recent high-profile accounting scandals has led to an increased desire for accounting controls (e.g., Sarbanes-Oxley legislation) and for Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education 151 accounting ethics education (Holder-Webb & Cohen, 2007; Yang & Wu, 2009; Young & Annisette, 2009). The assumption that ethics education alone will positively affect the ethical behavior of students, however, is not necessarily valid. Students may be trained to perform well on examinations regarding questions of ethical conduct or recognizing ethical issues (Gautschi & Jones, 1998), but extrapolating those results into real-world ethical dilemmas where the price for ethical behavior must be paid in the form of decreased productivity or efficiency presents external validity issues (Williams & Dewett, 2005). As Bloodgood, Turnley, and Mudrack (2010) hypothesized, providing college training in ethics may not improve ethical behavior in students whose personalities predispose them to act in their own self-interest. If this hypothesis is correct, then to a large degree, the ethical preparation provided by many business schools may not be successful in altering the behavior of future executives. From this perspective, students who are predisposed to behave ethically will do so, and students who are predisposed toward unethical conduct will do so regardless of the ethical theories or analytic training that is presented to them in college. The counterargument to Bloodgood et al.’s hypothesis says that ethics courses cause students to think more carefully about the social consequences of unethical behavior, thereby modifying their attitudes and increasing the likelihood that they will avoid such behavior (French, 2006). Currently, persuasive evidence indicating that taking college courses in business ethics results in reduced unethical behavior when students migrate into the business world appears to be absent (Williams & Dewett, 2005), supporting Bloodgood et al.’s hypothesis. Indeed, several recent studies report that business ethics instruction has little or no effect on the subsequent choices made by students (e.g., Jewe, 2008; Natale & Sora, 2010; Peppas, 2003; Peppas & Diskin, 2001; Ritter, 2006). Seshadri, Broekemier, and Nelson (1998) concluded that classes in business ethics have no impact on the ethicality of students’ subsequent decision making. Consequently, business students remain the most prolific cheaters on college campuses (McCabe, Butterfield, & Trevino, 2006) An examination of accounting ethics education research shows similar results, although there is disagreement over whether ethical education ultimately produces accountants who will act more ethically (Tweedie, Dyball, Hazelton, & Wright, 2013). Some evidence exists suggesting that ethics education may affect accounting students’ moral sensitivity (e.g., Dellaportas, Leung, & Jackling, 2006), ethical standards (e.g., Keller, Smith, & Smith, 2007), and their cognitive moral capability (e.g., Jones, Massey, & Thorne, Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) 152 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. 2003), but such effects may be short-lived (LaGrone, Welton, & Davis, 1996). Similarly, Liu et al. (2012) found that ethics instruction appears to have at least a temporary effect on several ethics-related perceptions, such as sensitivity and moral reasoning, but these effects appear to have limited influence on the ethicality of choices actually made during one’s educational experience (e.g., academic dishonesty) or on subsequent decision making in the workplace. Indeed, Thomas (2012) noted that the observational effects in past studies have been relatively weak. Loeb (1988) suggested that the goals of ethics education in accounting should be more modest, focusing on issues such as helping students to recognize ethical issues in accounting and to develop a sense of moral obligation instead of directing behavior. Tweedie et al. (2013) noted that education is unlikely to achieve even these more modest goals. Interestingly, although Thomas (2012) observed that a college education has a positive effect on students’ deliberative reasoning (when comparing freshmen and senior accounting students), Josien and Broderick (2013) observed that the rate of academic dishonesty increases as students progress through their education. Abdolmohammadi and Baker (2007) observed that students’ moral reasoning is inversely related to their plagiarism activity. The relationship between ethical reasoning and academic dishonesty, therefore, does not seem to be clear. Bay and Greenberg (2001) observed that knowing the right thing to do does not necessarily lead to ethical choices. Similarly, Lampe and Engleman-Lampe (2012) suggested that individuals often make choices not consistent with their beliefs and attitudes of what may be the “right” course of action when they deal with the subsequent dissonance through rationalization. To further complicate matters, Ferguson, Collison, Power, and Stevenson (2011) suggested that the way that accounting is taught may predispose accountants to act in unethical fashions regardless of ethics education. Determining whether a student is predisposed toward unethical behavior is a difficult question. Josien and Broderick (2013) recognized that the largest limitation to research on academic dishonesty is the reliance on selfreported data. The limitations of self-report data are evident in three areas. First, Burrus, McGoldrick, and Schuhmann (2007) noted that students often do not have a clear picture of what cheating is. Without first giving students a clear definition of academic dishonesty, students’ self-reports of their cheating behavior may be inconsistent. Second, unethical individuals with even modest intelligence would likely not willingly provide evidence of their dishonesty, and would likely fabricate answers to questions or ethical cases to conform to their professor’s expectations. Third, the use of survey Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education 153 instruments to measure personal predispositions raises similar concerns. Bloodgood et al. (2010), for instance, used the Christie and Geis (1970) Machiavellianism scale, a survey instrument to categorize students on whether they exhibit Machiavellian attitudes. Here, savvy students may recognize the variables that are being manipulated, and that these variables contain features that are contrary to the educational expectations of college student behavior. The anonymity of student responses to such questionnaires provides some assurance about the legitimacy of the data; yet, students recognize that their responses may alter the professor’s view of the class as a whole, and thereby indirectly impact their grades. Accordingly, the validity of measuring Machiavellianism via surveys in educational settings may be open to question. Addressing the issue of how prior student attitudes impact the effectiveness of college ethics courses is challenging, and the problem needs to be broken down into smaller issues. Ideally, student attitudes and behaviors need to be measured under conditions where students are unaware that they are being observed. The focus of this study is to accomplish this by identifying students’ Machiavellian attitudes via actual behavior within the educational environment and relate them to their beliefs that additional ethics instruction and/or punishment will have the greatest effect on limiting unethical activity. The effects of additional ethical instruction in accounting are also examined. This study employs a commercial plagiarism search engine as a surrogate measure for Machiavellianism, and then compares plagiarism activity in an extensive writing assignment across three variables: the presence or absence of enhanced ethical training; student evaluations of whether more ethical training would have prevented fraud in a given ethics case; and student evaluations of whether harsher penalties would have prevented fraud in the same ethics case. First, we review the present state of research in student ethics. Second, we explore the Machiavellian connection to ethics. Third, we develop and test hypotheses. Finally, we draw conclusions. THE PRESENT STATE OF RESEARCH IN STUDENT ETHICS The quantity of literature on academic dishonesty will overwhelm any first time viewer of it with the prevalence and depth of cheating by students in high school, college, and graduate school. Research shows that over 70 Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) 154 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. percent of high school students have engaged in active cheating on their assignments and at least 50 percent have plagiarized writing assignments using the Internet (McCabe, 2005). Even worse, 77 percent of students in the McCabe (2005) survey believe that Internet plagiarism is not a serious issue, and the rate of cheating has been increasing over the past several decades (Galloway, 2012; Murdock, Miller, & Kohlhardt, 2004). As would be expected, the situation in higher education is not much better. The rate of reported cheating in college is in the 40 80 percent range in most studies (e.g., Klein, Levenburg, McKendall, & Mothersell, 2007), with Brown, Weible, and Olmosk (2010) observing a high of 100 percent in an undergraduate management class. The rate of cheating in higher education has also been increasing over the past several decades (Chapman, Davis, Toy, & Wright, 2004; Jones, 2011; MacGregor & Stuebs, 2012). Internet plagiarism is viewed as one of the most serious cheating problems (Comas & Sureda, 2010). Although the use of the Internet has created an explosion in student plagiarism (Granitz & Loewy, 2007; Quah, Stewart, & Lee, 2012), many students may not perceive Internet plagiarism to be cheating (Gabriel, 2010). Lawson (2004), however, observed that students who have cheated on exams are more likely to plagiarize, allowing him to conclude that plagiarism is viewed by students to be similar to other forms of academic dishonesty. Abdolmohammadi and Baker (2007) believed that plagiarism is particularly important to accounting students since students’ degree of learning is lessened. They further suggest that acts of plagiarism during one’s education may lead to an increased tendency to engage in unethical activities once students find themselves in the accounting profession. The extensive ethical preparations of collegiate business programs do not appear to lessen the plague of college cheating. Harris (1989), Smyth, Davis, and Knoncke (2009), and Wood, Longenecker, and Moore (1988) noted that business students appear to have lower ethical values than non-business students. One prevailing theory on why cheating is more common among business students rests on the notion that business is a selfinterested endeavor and, accordingly, students with an interest in business will have stronger egoism values (Williams & Dewett, 2005). Several studies support this theory, observing differences between business students and those pursuing degrees in other areas. McLean and Jones (1992), for instance, observed that students pursuing business education are much more Machiavellian than students pursuing other majors. Similarly, Westerman, Bergman, Bergman, and Daly (2012) observed that business students are similarly more narcissistic than students pursuing other Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education 155 majors, specifically those pursuing psychology majors. Sautter, Brown, Littvay, Sautter, and Bearnes (2008), however, observed no difference between the narcissism of entering freshman business and nonbusiness majors, suggesting that the differences observed in students’ narcissism may be due to the academic environment faced by business school students. Indeed, they observed supporting evidence the narcissism of business students appeared to increase during their college years. (It should be noted, however, that they did not control for students changing majors.) The overwhelming prevalence of college cheating in business schools is inconsistent with the professed emphasis placed by these schools on ethics. The Aristotelian logic is simple: all cheaters behave unethically when they cheat; many business students are cheaters; ergo, many business students (students with extensive training in ethics) behave unethically. This logic is consistent with Bloodgood et al. (2010), who found that taking a business ethics course did not significantly alter student views on the ethicality of academic dishonesty. Others provide evidence suggesting that a business education itself may adversely affect students’ moral character (e.g., Frank, Gilovich, & Regan, 1993; Schneider & Prasso, 2002). Interestingly, Tang and Tang (2010) reported that students with ethical intentions may become more ethical as the result of ethical instruction whereas students with unethical intentions may become less ethical as the result of ethical instruction. Similarly, Bloodgood et al. (2010) observed that for students scoring low on Machiavellianism, taking a business ethics course resulted in greater acceptance of certain forms of college cheating. Ethics education, therefore, does not appear to affect all individuals in the same way. Indeed, for some, ethics education may lead to results contrary to those sought some students may act less ethically as a result of ethics education. A possible reason for this surprising effect may be that ethics education provides some students with a greater ability to rationalize or justify their actions. Rationalizations allow individuals to alleviate their moral anxiety by developing explanations (Zyglidopoulos, Fleming, & Rothenberg, 2009) which make unethical activities appear acceptable. MacGregor and Stuebs (2012) observed that students who are better at rationalizing their choices are more likely to engage in academic dishonesty, including cheating. As a result of rationalizations, “corrupt individuals tend not to view themselves as corrupt” (Anand, Ashforth, & Joshi, 2004, p. 40). Moreover, Zyglidopoulos et al. (2009) suggested that the rationalizations given by individuals often are excessive, providing a basis for more unethical actions in the future, or what Lowell (2012) referred to as a downward ethics spiral. This indicates that academia may need to reflect Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) 156 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. carefully on this ethics anomaly and take a more realistic appraisal of what college ethics courses accomplish. Research suggests that student ethics may vary based on gender (Crown & Spiller, 1998). Klein et al. (2007) observed that in about half of the studies they examined, males cheat more than females, while no relationships were observed in the other half. Given the lack of agreement on the existence of a gender effect, studies examining academic dishonesty should control for gender. On the other hand, students’ propensity to cheat appears to vary with their GPA. Burrus et al. (2007) and Straw (2002), for instance, observed that students with lower GPAs tend to be more likely to cheat. Straw attributes the difference in tendencies to differences in importance individuals with lower GPAs have more to lose if they acquire a lower grade in a class. An alternative explanation, however, may be that students with lower GPAs may have earned the lower grades because they have less motivation, use less effort or have lower abilities and rely on cheating to overcome these shortcomings. Hence, studies examining student cheating activity should also control for students’ GPA. THE MACHIAVELLIAN CONNECTION If higher education is to understand and reduce student dishonesty, it needs to place greater focus on the causative factors of such behavior, rather than simply espousing the opinion that more or better training in ethics is the solution. One factor that has been studied in the literature is the personality trait known as Machiavellianism. Machiavellianism describes individuals with personalities that show a low regard for conventional morality (Jones & Kavanagh, 1996). Generally, individuals with strong Machiavellian characteristics (high Machs) are manipulative in their dealings with others and focus on achieving their desired ends by any available means (Christie & Geis, 1970). A “Machiavellian administrator is one who employs aggressive, manipulative, exploiting, and devious moves in order to achieve personal and organizational objectives” (Calhoon, 1969, p. 211). This definition is, to some degree, a misnomer. Niccolò Machiavelli’s most famous work, The Prince (1513/2009), was written as a guide for 16th-century monarchs and rulers, focusing on using deception, manipulation, and traditionally amoral actions to achieve the greatest overall good for the citizens of the kingdom. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education 157 Machiavelli’s unconventional ideas were arguably never intended to be used for self-gain or personal enrichment, but rather as a rational means of choosing the lesser of evils to achieve a greater good for society as a whole. Nevertheless, the term, Machiavellianism, has since been applied to individuals who “do not take a moralistic view of themselves, others, or of interpersonal relations” (Geis, Christie, & Nelson, 1970, p. 76) and who welcome the use of guile, deceit, and other similar means to accomplish their objectives (Forgas, 1985; Winter, Stylianou, & Giacalone, 2004) without regard for the well-being of others (Tang & Tang, 2010). High Mach individuals engage in unethical behavior not as an end in itself, but rather as a means to achieving some particular economic benefit or avoiding some type of punishment (Tang & Chen, 2008). Generally, high Mach individuals are more accepting of unethical behavior, and have a greater propensity for engaging in cheating activity (Granitz, 2003; O’Fallon, & Butterfield, 2005; Wilson, Near, & Miller, 1996). Indeed, within an academic setting, Quah et al. (2012) observed that high Mach students hold more positive attitudes toward plagiarism than low Mach students and Menon and Sharland (2011) observed a strong relationship between Machiavellianism and attitudes toward academic dishonesty. As discussed previously, using a questionnaire or survey to identify high Mach individuals may lead to incorrect results. High Machs are, by their nature, duplicitous and may decide to provide false information when the circumstances show a potential advantage to using deception (Jones & Paulhus, 2009). Similarly, using behavior is also superior to using students’ expressed rationale (or rationalization) for engaging in plagiarism after they are caught (as used by Granitz & Loewy, 2007) since the expectation is that high Machs would be unlikely to provide accurate responses if they perceive such responses as possibly leading to negative consequences. Another limitation on the validity of assessing Machiavellian personality characteristics by means of a questionnaire concerns the lack of economic consequences. Asking subjects a hypothetical question about whether they would return a lost wallet filled with money, for instance, provides little insight into what they would do if they actually found such a wallet. Recent research raises additional concerns in attempting to predict unethical behavior. Measures that assess Machiavellianism via a questionnaire or survey are no exception. Research suggests that individuals’ predictions of their behavior, including the ethicality of their actions, often differ from the choices actually made (Epley & Dunning, 2000). Indeed, most individuals believe that they are more ethical than their subsequent behavior suggests (Tenbrunsel, 1998; Tyson, 1992). Tenbrunsel, Diekmann, Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) 158 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. Wade-Benzoni, and Bazerman (2010) suggested that individuals possess a different frame when they actually make decisions than when they are predicting their behavior due to “ethical fading” and other decision biases, individuals often do not realize the ethical implications of decisions when they actually make them, permitting the pursuit of less ethical behavior. Similarly, Bazerman and Tenbrunsel (2011) referred to “bounded awareness,” where individuals do not perceive all of the pertinent information (e.g., information of an ethical nature) when making decisions. Consequently, examining actual behavior is the preferable method of assessing the ethics of individuals’ choices. Rather than identifying high Machs with a conventional psychological questionnaire, an alternative would be to flag individuals as Machiavellian when they engage in duplicitous behavior to achieve a desirable objective. Thus, students displaying a clear pattern of blatant academic dishonesty could be classified as high Machs. Such an approach has its drawbacks because adverse personal circumstances could entice a student to cheat on an exam or writing assignment, when, in fact, such a digression would be out of their normal character. Nevertheless, this approach to identifying students with Machiavellian personalities does not suffer from the disinformational drawbacks of simply asking students whether they are deceptive or manipulating individuals. The strength of a “dishonest students are ‘high Machs’” approach is the ability to collect or observe other additional information that would, a priori, be associated with Machiavellian personalities. For example, Machiavellians’ engaging in cheating would undoubtedly consider the consequences of being caught, and would formulate an excuse or argument to reduce the impact of any adverse penalty to their dishonesty. Therefore, any student attitudes or actions that mitigate the consequences of dishonesty could be viewed as additional evidence of a Machiavellian personality. HYPOTHESES The overall conclusion of Bloodgood et al. (2010) focuses on the observation that individual personality characteristics play a larger role than ethics instruction in determining student attitudes toward academic dishonesty. Although research seems to support Bloodgood et al.’s (2010) hypothesis, the study used a 20-item scale developed by Christie and Geis (1970) to classify students according to their inclination for Machiavellian behavior. As mentioned earlier, however, high Machs can be expected to engage in Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education 159 duplicitous behavior to achieve a desirable objective, behavior that could include replying to a questionnaire in a fashion to obtain possible desirable outcomes instead of providing honest responses. The current study, therefore, avoids the use of a survey and instead assesses level of student plagiarism as measured by a similarity index (SI) and footnote disclosures as a surrogate measure for Machiavellianism. These metrics are then compared across two different student groups, one group being exposed to a lecture on ethical theories and frameworks and the other group simply receiving a factual presentation of ethics rules as developed by the American Institute of Certified Public Accountants. Given that the research explored earlier suggests that ethics education may not have a positive effect on subsequent behavior, the following hypothesis is tested: H1. Exposure to a discussion on ethics does not affect students’ subsequent incidence of academic dishonesty. Specifically, no relationship exists between the level of student plagiarism in a college accounting writing assignment and the type of training in ethical theories and frameworks students receive. Furthermore, the lack of a relationship remains after controlling for the effects of gender and GPA. As discussed above, classifying all cheating students as high Machs is questionable. High Machs have a consistent and pervasive preference toward manipulative opportunism, and some students may cheat only when facing severe time constraints for studying or preparing assignments. One would expect high Machs to more carefully consider the consequences of being caught than students who cheated out of desperation, and, consequently, high Machs would have more formally developed and articulated defensive strategies. Such strategies would be unobservable for research purposes; yet, surrogates for defensive strategies and attitudes are available. In this study, one of the requirements for the writing assignment was for students to point out whether the ethical failure in the examined case could have been best prevented or mitigated with either additional ethical education and/or more severe punishment of offenders since they are two of the most referenced mechanisms for lessening the incidence of cheating activity by students. Punishment is often viewed to be a deterrent to undesirable behavior, including cheating behavior. Several studies (e.g., Burns, Manolis, & Ahuja, 2006; Chapman et al., 2004; Smith et al., 2002) observed that students’ cheating tendencies drop dramatically if students view that the perceived costs of cheating are high (a significant punishment with a high risk of being caught and experiencing the punishment). This is Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) 160 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. consistent with deterrence theory, which was originally developed as a way to reduce the occurrence of undesired behavior (Cheng, Li, Zhai, & Smyth, 2014). Deterrence theory assumes that disincentives can influence behavior, where individuals make choices to avoid negative results, including punishment (Wenzel, 2004). Deterrence theory assumes that individuals are adequately rational to consider consequences when deciding courses of action (Kennedy, 2010). Paternoster (2010) provided a summary of deterrence research focusing on the deterrence of criminal activity. He notes that the effect of deterrence on individuals’ choices does not seem to be as strong as is commonly believed, but he suggests one reason for this lies in the temporal distance between an action and the associated punishment. Deterrence researchers, however, are increasingly recognizing that the effectiveness of deterrents on influencing choices varies across a number of individual characteristics (Worrall, Els, Piquero, & TenEyck, 2014). Piquero, Paternoster, Pogarsky, and Loughran (2011) termed the differences in the ways that deterrents affect individuals “differential deterrability.” Differences in Machivellianism logically should affect how individuals respond to deterrents or the types of possible punishment. (Some deterrents have the opposite effect on behavior, however, not deterring the questionable behavior, but facilitating it; Bazerman & Tenbrunsel, 2011. Hence, the presence of deterrents themselves cannot guarantee positive changes in behavior. Instead, the focus must be determining and implementing the “right” deterrents.) A priori, based on the characteristics of Machiavellianism, one would expect high Machs to favor added ethical education rather than punishment. (This would be the opportunistic approach.) High Machs are likely not to favor anything that could possibly be used against them (like punishment), which could impede them from reaching their goals. Ethics education, however, is relatively harmless and could be viewed as beneficial since its assumed effectiveness could lead to an environment with less oversight, facilitating questionable activities. (If instructors believe that ethical education can be effective in minimizing cheating, they may be less likely to provide oversight that has the goal of minimizing cheating activity.) Moreover, as discussed earlier, ethics education appears to give students the tools needed to rationalize academically dishonest behavior, so added ethics education could provide high Machs with additional insight into facilitate academic cheating. On the other hand, one would expect low Machs (honest students) to have a greater preference for punishing cheaters as a means for dissuading unethical activity since their ethical behavior precludes punishment. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education 161 Instead, cheaters would be more likely to be appropriately punished. Low Machs, however, are less likely to favor increased ethics education. They will likely see little reason for ethics education since it will likely be viewed as having no effect on them (since their choices already tend to be more ethical). Instead, the increased education would likely be viewed as merely an additional activity to endure with little personal benefit. Students who favor neither added ethics education nor increased punishment and students who favor both increased education and increased punishment would appear to lie between high Machs and low Machs (moderate Machs) in terms of their relative Machiavellian tendencies. Some moderate Machs (students who may be involved in cheating, but not to the extent of high Machs) may favor neither education nor punishment they would prefer the lack of punishment so that it will not affect any unethical behavior in which they may partake. They, however, do not possess the desire to possibly facilitate cheating activities (which may be the outcome of less oversight due to ethics education) or be as focused on their cheating activities to seek out ways to rationalize their choices. Similarly, other moderate Machs likely favor both ethics education and punishment. Although the preferences are opposite of the other moderate Machs mentioned above, the rationale is similar. For these students, the presence of punishment will likely be viewed as a constraint against “excessive” cheating by high Machs (which would put moderate Machs at performance disadvantage). The ethics education may lead to decreased oversight, as mentioned above, likely making it easier for some unethical activity to occur. Moreover, the ethics education may increase students’ abilities to rationalize their unethical choices. Consequently, favoring both punishment and ethics education will likely have appeal to some moderate Machs, while others will likely favor neither punishment nor ethics education. Consequently, Hypotheses 2 seems to be appropriate: H2. Students who express that ethical failures can be best prevented through additional ethical education are more likely to cheat than students who believe that ethical failures can best be prevented through increased punishment. In other words, students who express that the ethical failure in the examined case could have been best prevented or mitigated with additional ethical education (hypothesized to be high Machs) possess higher similarity indexes and include lower numbers of references in their class project than students who express that the ethical failure could have been prevented with increased punishment (hypothesized to be low Machs). 162 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) METHODOLOGY The above hypotheses are tested using a 2 × 2 × 2 full factorial, between subjects design with 157 accounting seniors (93 males and 64 females) from a Midwestern, AACSB accredited college of business. Separate sections of the same upper division accounting class were used as cluster samples, with each cluster serving as part of the experimental or control groups. In a true experimental design, students would have been assigned randomly between the experimental and control groups. This, however, was not practical since students registered according to their own scheduling needs. Therefore, a quasi-experimental design was employed. Any student who had previously taken the course was removed from the study so each participant was viewing the lecture and presentation for the first time. Random assignment was used to determine which student cluster received the enhanced ethics lecture (treatment variable) and which received the stock lecture on the ethical rules for accountants as determined by the American Institute of Certified Public Accountants (control variable). The enhanced ethics lecture presented persuasive arguments that students and business professionals benefit most when they act in a manner that promotes ethical behavior in themselves and others. Two different litmus tests of ethical behavior were presented. First, the ideas of John Stuart Mill’s Utilitarianism were examined. Second, Immanuel Kant’s Categorical Imperative model (Universalization) was discussed and analyzed. Mill’s and Kant’s models were presented as thought experiments in both business and student ethics contexts, and they were stripped of jargon to make the material readily understandable to persons without a philosophical background in ethics. Students were informed to use these alternative models of ethical thought to navigate their way through ethical dilemmas. The convergence of both models on a particular course of action increases the likelihood that the action is not only ethical, but is also in accordance with their own long-run best interest. Seventy-one of the students were exposed to the enhanced ethics lecture and 86 students were presented with only a factual presentation of the professional code. The final levels in the design focused on categorizing student responses in a term paper covering two classic ethical failures in the field of accounting. Part of the assignment included the requirement that students identify whether added ethical training or harsher punishment would have been effective in preventing or mitigating the ethical failures that were depicted in the cases. The cases were complex and described the factual circumstances and details surrounding several ethically ambiguous scenarios where student Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education 163 judgment would be needed in identifying exactly where and why ethical digressions occurred. Thus, the design of the study involves: (type of ethical lecture given to the student) × (the student’s choice of whether additional ethical education would have been effective in preventing or mitigating the ethical failures depicted in the cases) × (student choice of whether harsher punishment would have been effective in preventing or mitigating the ethical failure depicted in the cases). Since relationships have been observed between academic performance and ethical attitudes (e.g., Klein et al., 2007; Lawson, 2004), student grade point average as reported by means of their academic transcript served as a covariate. Furthermore, since ethical attitudes have been observed to vary by student gender (e.g., Lawson, 2004; Smyth et al., 2009), student gender was treated as a control variable. To ensure that they possessed a clear understanding of what is regarded as plagiarism, students were given detailed instructions for preparing their term paper. These instructions included the need for proper academic referencing of sources used with specific examples of acceptable and unacceptable referencing procedures in the course syllabus. Students were also given a copy of the University Code of Student Conduct, which provided a detailed description of their obligation to avoid academic misconduct and plagiarism. The course syllabus contained a statement that any plagiarism or other academic misconduct would result in a failing grade for the course. The student papers were submitted electronically and then sent to a commercial plagiarism detection service in a fashion similar to that used by Abdolmohammadi and Baker (2007). The output from this service displayed a similarity index (SI) for each student. The SI metric represented the proportion of the paper that was taken from Internet sources or from published works from their proprietary database, including books, journal articles, and manuscripts from other students. In addition to the SI metric, the plagiarism detection service provided an HTML copy of the student’s paper with highlighted sections and notes to show exactly from where the suspected plagiarized material was taken. The student papers were examined in light of this information, and any flagged material or paraphrased statements with proper references were noted and removed from the calculated SI. Thus, the instructor-adjusted SI represented quotations or paraphrasing from other sources that were not properly referenced by the student. In addition to the SI measurement, the number of verifiable academic references was recorded for each student. The lack of academic references is not, in itself, evidence of student plagiarism; however, one would expect, a priori, an inverse relationship between the level of student plagiarism and 164 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) the number of academic references. Accordingly, students who were properly citing their sources would have more listed academic references than students who were engaging in “cut and paste” plagiarism. A pilot experiment was conducted in a prior semester to verify that the cases and instructions were clear, and that students understood the assignment requirements. No problems were noted. The assignment was listed on the course syllabus as a graded component with a weight equal to about 15 percent of their course grade. Factors and Dependent Variables The three factors evaluated in this study are the type of ethical lecture, students’ beliefs that ethical education will prevent or mitigate the incidence of ethical lapses, and students’ beliefs that increased penalties for ethical violations will prevent or mitigate the incidence of ethical lapses as discussed above. The dependent variables are the adjusted SI score from the assigned term papers and the number of verifiable references cited by each student. Student GPA was used as a covariate, and student gender was treated as a categorical control variable. Multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was used to examine effects of the three factors on the joint relationship between SI and the number of student references. (Since student gender is a categorical variable, it cannot be directly employed as a covariate. Computationally, student gender was entered as a factor since doing so is mathematically equivalent to including it as a covariate in the form of a dummy variable.) In a secondary analysis, analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to separately evaluate SI and the number of student citations. Both MANCOVA and ANCOVA are used for the sake of completeness; a significant MANCOVA result does not imply a significant ANCOVA, and vice versa. Students were then classified into four groups based on their beliefs that ethical education will lower the incidence of ethical lapses and on their beliefs that increased penalties for ethical violations will lower the incidence of ethical lapses. RESULTS Table 1 provides summary statistics. Table 2 displays associations between factors and variables. As expected, the two dependent variables are strongly correlated (r = −.3385, p < .0005) indicating that as students 165 Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education Table 1. Summary Statistics. Parametric Statistics Similarity index (SI) Number of references GPA Mean Standard Deviation Minimum Maximum 13.17 2.54 3.24 16.01 2.159 .471 0 0 2.21 87 11 4.00 Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Frequencies Action to Prevent Ethical Lapse Increased ethical instruction Increased penalties Table 2. Perceived to Be Effective Perceived Not to Be Effective 83 84 74 73 Association Correlation Matrix of Factors and Variables. Similarity Number of Ethical Index (SI) References Instruction Similarity index (SI) Number of r = −.3385 references p = .000 Ethical r = −.2441 instruction p = .002 Increase r = −.3240 penalties p = .000 Ethics lecture r = .0375 p = .641 Gender r = .2539 p = .001 GPA r = −.2564 p = .001 Increase Penalties Ethics Lecture Gender GPA r = .227 p = .004 r = .2377 χ2 = .036 p = .003 p = .874 r = .0772 χ2 = 1.239 χ2 = .106 p = .336 p = .335 p = .751 r = .2937 χ2 = −.1255 χ2 = −.0977 χ2 = .095 p = .000 p = .117 p = .224 p = .760 r = .2139 r = −.0004 r = .1482 r = −.0270 r = −.0949 p = .007 p = .996 p = .064 p = .737 p = .237 Notes: Pearson correlation and biserial correlations are reported when ratio measure(s) are examined. Chi-squares are reported when both measures are nominal. Significant (at the .05 level) relationships are indicated in bold. Ethical Instruction = Students’ beliefs that ethical education will lower the incidence of ethical lapses. Increase Penalties = Student’s belief that increased penalties for ethical violations will lower the incidence of ethical lapses. Ethics Lecture = Exposure to enhanced ethics lecture. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) 166 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. increasingly engage in plagiarism, they include fewer references. The relationships between the dependent variables and students’ GPA and gender produce results consistent with past research. Students with higher GPAs appear to plagiarize less as indicated by a lower SI score (r = −.2564, p = .001) and a higher number of references (r = .2139, p = .007) than students with lower GPAs. Similarly, female students appear to plagiarize less by having a lower SI score (t = 3.465, p = .001) and a higher number of references (t = 3.514, p = .001) than male students. Finally, although the summary statistics on students’ beliefs on providing ethical instruction or increasing penalties for the unethical activity depicted in the cases are virtually identical, students’ responses were not observed to be statistically associated (χ2 = −.036, p = .874). MANCOVA results are reported in Table 3. The overall model is significant (at the .05 level). When the results for the individual variables are examined, as expected, significant effects were noted for both GPA and gender. A significant effect, however, was not observed for whether students were exposed to the enhanced ethics lecture. This result supports H1 exposure to the enhanced ethics lecture does not appear to affect students’ plagiarism activities after controlling for the effects of gender and GPA. Significant effects were observed for the other two factors students’ beliefs on whether ethical instruction and/or whether increased penalties could have prevented or mitigated the ethical lapses depicted in the case. None of the interactions between the factors and covariates was significant. For additional insight, individual ANCOVAs were conducted for the two dependent variables independently. ANCOVA results are reported in Table 4. The results for both ANCOVAs were consistent with the results observed for the MANOVA. To test the second hypothesis, the sample was split into four groups based on their responses to whether they believe that the ethical failure in the examined case could have been best prevented or mitigated with additional ethical education or with increased punishment. The groups and summary statistics are displayed in Table 5. Group 3, the group believed to consist of high Machs, displayed the highest mean SI and the lowest mean number of references as expected. Similarly, Group 2, the group believed to consist of low Machs, displayed the lowest SI and the highest number of references, also as expected. The significances of the differences were tested. The results of a MANOVA and individual ANCOVAs comparing the groups while controlling for GPA and age and appropriate post hoc tests are displayed in Table 6. 167 Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Table 3. Multivariate Analysis of Covariance (MANCOVA) Results. Intercept GPA Provide ethical instruction Increase penalties Enhanced ethics lecture Gender Provide ethical instruction × Increase penalties Provide ethical instruction × Enhanced ethics lecture Provide ethical instruction × Gender Increased penalties × Enhanced ethics lecture Increase penalties × Gender Enhanced ethics lecture × Gender Provide ethical instruction × Increase penalties × Enhanced ethics lecture Provide ethical instruction × Increase penalties × Gender Provide ethical instruction × Enhanced ethics lecture × Gender Increase penalties × Enhanced ethics lecture × Gender Interaction term including all Four factors Wilks’ Lambda F Effect df Error df Significance .896 .923 .920 .900 .996 .920 .986 8.101 5.834 6.005 9.354 .281 6.003 .964 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 139 139 139 139 139 139 139 .000 .004 .003 .001 .755 .003 .384 .991 .654 2 139 .522 .984 1.115 2 139 .331 .981 1.314 2 139 .272 .995 .376 2 139 .687 .995 .348 2 139 .707 .983 1.220 2 139 .298 .991 .605 2 139 .547 .996 .286 2 139 .752 .998 .167 2 139 .847 .994 .398 2 139 .673 Notes: Significant (at the .05 level) relationships are indicated in bold. Ethical instruction = Students’ belief that ethical education will lower the incidence of ethical lapses. Increase penalties = Student’s belief that increased penalties for ethical violations will lower the incidence of ethical lapses. Ethics lecture = Exposure to enhanced ethics lecture. 168 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. Table 4. Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) Results. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Similarity Index Intercept GPA Provide ethical instruction Increase penalties Enhanced ethics lecture Gender Provide ethical instruction × Increase penalties Provide ethical instruction × Enhanced ethics lecture Provide ethical instruction × Gender Increased penalties × Enhanced ethics lecture Increase penalties × Gender Enhanced ethics lecture × Gender Provide ethical instruction × Increase penalties × Enhanced ethics lecture × Provide ethical instruction × Increase penalties × Gender Provide ethical instruction × Enhanced ethics lecture × Gender Increase penalties × Enhanced ethics lecture × Gender Interaction term including all Four factors F df 16.230 6.707 7.014 11.930 .261 4.991 .256 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 .968 Number of References Significance F df Significance .000 .011 .009 .001 .610 .027 .614 .122 6.920 7.010 5.884 .215 8.922 1.855 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 .728 .009 .009 .017 .643 .003 .175 1 .327 .181 1 .671 .198 1 .657 1.801 1 .182 .277 1 .600 2.573 1 .111 .514 1 .475 .139 1 .709 .670 1 .414 .093 1 .760 1.429 1 .234 1.420 1 .235 .636 1 .393 1 .532 .113 1 .738 .382 1 .537 .093 1 .761 .191 1 .662 .001 1 .981 .774 1 .381 ,427 Notes: Significant (at the .05 level) relationships are indicated in bold. Ethical instruction = Students’ belief that ethical education will lower the incidence of ethical lapses. Increase penalties = Student’s belief that increased penalties for ethical violations will lower the incidence of ethical lapses. Ethics lecture = Exposure to enhanced ethics lecture. 169 Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education Table 5. Student’s Belief That Ethical Education or Increased Penalties for Ethical Violations Will Lower the Incidence of Ethical Lapses. Frequencies Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Student’s Belief That Increased Penalties for Ethical Violations Will Lower the Incidence of Ethical Lapses Students’ belief that ethical education will lower the incidence of ethical lapses No Yes No Group 1 37 Group 3 38 Yes Group 2 37 Group 4 45 Summary Statistics Similarity Index Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 Group 4 Mean Number of References Mean Std. deviation Mean Std. deviation 13.78 4.30 22.53 12.04 16.773 4.551 21.989 10.57 2.41 3.70 1.82 2.29 2.153 2.209 1.814 2.074 The overall MANCOVA is significant (at the .05 level) indicating significant differences across the four groups. For further insight, the results from individual ANCOVAs (where SI and number of references were viewed separately) are provided. The results from both ANCOVAs were consistent with the results observed for the MANOVA. Post hoc test was conducted for each ANCOVA. Since disagreement exists over the appropriateness of using Tukey tests when ANCOVAs are used (as opposed to ANOVAs), Šidák tests were utilized. The results of post hoc Šidák tests are also displayed in Table 6. For SI, a significant (at the .05 level) difference was observed for only one group comparison. The significant difference was observed between Group 2 and Group 3, supporting H2. Specifically, a significant difference was observed between the SIs for the groups viewed to represent high Mach and low Mach individuals high Mach students possess significantly higher SIs than low Mach individuals, signifying higher rates of plagiarism. For number of references, significant (at the .05 level) differences were observed for two group comparisons. First, a significant difference was again observed between Group 2 and Group 3, supporting the H2. Specifically, a significant difference was observed between the number of references cited for the groups believed to represent high Mach and low 170 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. Table 6. Analysis of Covariance Results. MANCOVA Wilks’ Lambda Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Intercept GPA Group Gender Gender × Group .897 .937 .830 .910 .965 F Effect df Error df Significance 2 2 6 2 6 147 147 294 147 294 .000 .009 .000 .001 .515 8.436 4.911 4.774 7.276 .874 ANCOVAS Similarity Index Intercept GPA Group Gender Gender × Group Number of References F df Significance F df Significance 16.322 6.623 6.796 7.185 .530 1 1 3 1 3 .000 .011 .000 .008 .662 .005 5.039 4.844 10.073 1.123 1 1 3 1 3 .945 .026 .003 .002 .342 Group 1 Group 2 Šidák pairwise comparisons Similarity index Group 1 Group 2 −8.246 .088 Group 3 7.119 15.365 .215 .000 Group 4 −.601 7.644 .999 .113 Šidák pairwise comparisons Number of references Group 1 Group 2 1.021 .162 Group 3 −.651 −1.673 .673 .003 Group 4 −.300 −1.322 .986 .022 Notes: Significant (at the .05 level) relationships are indicated in bold. Group 3 −7.720 .127 .351 .972 Group 4 Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education 171 Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Mach individuals high Mach students cited significantly fewer references than low Mach individuals. A significant difference was also observed between Group 2 (low Machs) and Group 4 (students who prescribed both more ethical education and increased penalties). This suggests that low Machs may be particularly committed to referencing materials or “doing the right thing.” In other words, they may be more focused on avoiding having their actions interpreted as being unethical. Support, therefore, is observed for H2. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION Today’s business students are tomorrow’s business leaders. Consequently, students’ views of ethical behavior will logically influence their choices when they enter the business world. Lawson (2004) observed a strong relationship between students’ ethical choices in academia and in the business world suggesting “students who cheat on exams or who plagiarize papers were more likely to be accepting of the need for unethical behavior in the workforce than those who do not engage in academic dishonesty” (p. 195). Brown and Choong (2005) and Nonis and Swift (2001) observed similar relationships. Hence, academic cheating is often used as an indicator for future ethical behavior on the job (Smyth et al., 2009). Consequently, the academic dishonesty of collegiate business students should be an important concern for businesses (Klein et al., 2007). The paradox between the mandated ethical preparation of collegiate business students and the noted high levels of academic misconduct by those same students is troubling. The results of this study support past research suggesting that ethical education may not positively affect students’ ethical choices (Bloodgood et al., 2010). Many believe that the solution to ethical failures in business is increased ethical education. Ethics research, however, does not seem to support this solution, although it needs to be recognized that the current study examined only the effect of a single ethics-enhanced lecture. Higher education needs to recognize that factors beyond mere ethical education and training may determine whether students and business professionals will behave ethically. The results are consistent with Tenbrunsel et al. (2010), who raise the possibility that ethics education may increase individuals’ perceptions of their own ethicality but may not have a positive effect on actual decision making. They suggest that individuals “unwittingly engage in behaviors that they would condemn upon further reflection or awareness” (p. 155). Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) 172 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. Hence, it appears that it is preferable to examine students’ actual cheating behavior and the context in which is occurs than their ethical perceptions. The primary methodological strength of this study rests on using actual incidences of unethical conduct (plagiarism) to establish Machiavellianism rather than using a student questionnaire, which respondents can manipulate. The questionnaire approach has stronger internal validity in terms of accurately measuring the precise features of Machiavellian personality types; however, it lacks the external validity of extrapolating those results into real-world ethical dilemmas. Indeed, research suggests that a disconnect exists between one’s ethical attitudes and one’s behavior, a disconnect that can be aggravated by the use of rationalizations. By reversing the normal diagnostic paradigm (by measuring actual behavior based on deviant behavior (plagiarism) in an academic setting instead of utilizing a questionnaire to establish students’ Machiavellianism), the study provides external validity to Bloodgood et al.’s finding that Machiavellianism is positively associated with the view that college cheating is acceptable. As a part of this reversed experiment, high Mach individuals were not merely identified as individuals who engaged in plagiarism. A priori, based on the characteristics of Machiavellianism, one would expect high Machs to favor added ethical education rather than punishment (this would be the opportunistic approach), and one would expect low Machs (honest students) to have a greater preference for punishing cheaters. Students who favor neither added ethical education nor increased punishment and students who favor both would appear to lie between high Machs and low Machs. The results support the above methodology. Students who expressed the belief that increased punishment (low Machs) would be able to prevent or mitigate the ethical failures depicted in the cases were less likely to have been involved in plagiarism and provided more references on the written assignment, whereas students who expressed that added education (high Machs) would be able to prevent or mitigate the ethical failures in the cases were more likely to have been involved in plagiarism and provided fewer references. Besides providing insight into Machiavellianism, these results provide clues into how institutions of higher education and businesses may be able to best attempt to prevent or mitigate ethical failures. The results suggest that students who are most likely to be involved in ethical failures (high Machs) do not favor the use of increased penalties as a way to prevent to mitigate ethical failures. They may view the increased penalties to be a significant hindrance to continued ethical failures. Hence, although ethics education may not be effective in positively affecting behavior, the Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education 173 study suggests that increasing specific penalties for ethical failures may be effective. This conclusion is consistent with past research on deterrents to academic dishonesty. Deterrence theory suggests that the incidence of cheating activity will depend in part on the perceived costs of engaging in the activity (McCabe & Trevino, 1997; Tibbetts, 1999). Part of the problem with ethical failures, therefore, may be the lack of sufficient deterrents (penalties). Indeed, several studies (e.g., Chapman et al., 2004; Smith et al., 2002) have observed that students’ cheating tendencies drop dramatically if students view that the perceived costs of cheating are high (a high risk of being caught). Research on the effectiveness of honor codes on the incidence of cheating behavior supports this conclusion. McCabe Trevino and Butterfield (2001) observed that the existence of an honor code in and of itself is insufficient to have a significant effect on students’ cheating behavior it must be enforced to be effective. In other words, effective deterrents (penalties) may be needed. In conclusion, although the study does not suggest that ethics instruction is not worthwhile, it does support the research that suggests that ethics education may not positively affect the ethics of many students. What should be the focus of ethics research then? The results suggest that a more fruitful approach for future research may be to focus more attention on the relative effectiveness of various forms of deterrents to ethical failures. Since high Machs are logically those who are most likely to engage in cheating activity, deterrents to unethical activity may be able to affect the choices of these individuals. Research, therefore, indicates that developing appropriate penalties may be successful to prevent or mitigate ethical failures. The study is significant for several reasons. First, the results provide classroom instructors with insight into the causes of Internet plagiarism; that is, Internet plagiarism may originate in the nature of the students involved instead of a lack of ethical instruction. The study also supports previous studies which similarly suggest that the effects of ethical instruction may not affect students’ ethical behavior. Second, through the means used to assess students’ Machiavellianism, the study was able to bring the role of deterrence (penalties) into the discussion as a possible substitute to ethics education as a way to prevent or mitigate ethical failures. The results suggest that classroom instructors may be able to effectively reduce academic dishonesty by increasing deterrents to academic dishonesty instead of expanding ethical instruction. Third, past research suggests that unethical behavior in an academic setting appears to lead to unethical behavior in business. Consequently, the Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) 174 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. study suggests that unethical behavior in business may arise at least in part from the qualities of individual business persons some individuals may have more Machiavellian tendencies than others. If true, this suggests that a focus on hiring may be an area where significant change can be enacted. It further suggests that the extent of potential hirees’ ethical education may not be the best evidence to examine to predict their likely future choices. Instead, the findings suggest that potentially more fruitful examination will focus on evidence of the relative Machiavellianism of potential hires as identified through their past ethical failures. Lastly, the study employed a “reverse methodology,” where students’ Machiavellianism was assessed directly from behavior instead of via a scale or via measuring attitudes. By assessing actual behavior, external validity can be enhanced and several measurement shortcomings can be overcome. Limitations The study possesses a number of limitations which may limit the generalizability of the results. First, the study included students attending advanced accounting classes at a single university. The generalizability of the results to students attending other academic institutions or those pursuing other majors has not been assessed. Second, the study classified students as high Machs based on only one form of academic dishonesty. The generalizability of the findings to other forms of academic dishonesty has not been unequivocally established. Third, although past research examining the effects of ethics education tended to observe only short-term effects, this study only examines the short-term effects of the ethics lecture. Fourth, students in the experimental group were only exposed to only a single enhanced ethics lecture. The results may be different if ethics were integrated throughout the course or throughout the accounting curriculum. Finally, although past research suggests a strong relationship between ethics within an academic environment and ethics in the workplace, the generalizability of the findings of this study to ethics in the workplace has not been examined. In addition to expanding the study to include different groups of students studying accounting at different types of universities and at different stages of their education, further research can provide additional insights into unethical academic behavior. Longitudinal studies examining the longterm effects of ethics instruction, for instance, would be insightful. Furthermore, extending the study to address different types of academic Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education 175 dishonesty besides Internet plagiarism would provide further knowledge of academic dishonesty. Additional insight can be gathered by further examining the measurement of Machiavellianism. For instance, how does the Machiavellianism as measured in this study relates to Machiavellianism as measured by a survey? Such examination may provide additional insight into the disconnect observed to often exist between ethical attitudes and individuals’ actual behavior. Specifically, what factors may affect the size of this disconnect? For instance, are high Mach individuals more accepting of unethical activities (possess lower ethical attitudes)? Burns, Smith, and Lanasa (1993) provided evidence that they are not, suggesting that Machivellianism and ethical attitudes may not differ. If true, does this mean that there is a more profound disconnect between ethical attitudes and behavior for high Mach individuals (are high Mach individuals more likely to experience ethical fading, bounded awareness)? Does this suggest that high Mach individuals are more likely to employ rationalizations? REFERENCES Abdolmohammadi, M. J., & Baker, C. R. (2007). The relationship between moral reasoning and plagiarism in accounting courses: A replication study. Issues in Accounting Education, 22(1), 45 55. Anand, V., Ashforth, B. E., & Joshi, M. (2004). Business as usual: The acceptance and perpetuation of corruption in organizations. Academy of Management Executive, 18(2), 39 53. Bay, D., & Greenberg, R. (2001). The relationship of the DIT and behavior: A replication. Issues in Accounting Education, 16(3), 367 381. Bazerman, M. H., & Tenbrunsel, A. E. (2011). Blind spots: Why we fail to do what’s right and what to do about it. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Bloodgood, J. M., Turnley, W. H., & Mudrack, P. E. (2010). Ethics instruction and the perceived acceptability of cheating. Journal of Business Ethics, 95(1), 23 37. Brown, B. S., & Choong, P. (2005). An investigation of academic dishonesty among business students at public and private United States universities. International Journal of Management, 22(20), 201 214. Brown, B. S., Weible, R. J., & Olmosk, K. E. (2010). Business school deans on student academic dishonesty: A survey. College Student Journal, 44(2), Pt. A, 299 309. Burns, D. J., Manolis, C., & Ahuja, R. D. (2006). An analysis of some factors affecting students’ perceived costs of cheating and academic dishonesty. In J. Chapman (Ed.), Enriching theoretical and practical understanding of marketing (pp. 148 149). Muncie, IN: Association of Marketing Theory and Practice. Burns, D. J., Smith, P. W., & Lanasa, J. M. (1993). Machiavellianism and ethical perceptions of retail situations. In D. M. Levy & D. Grewal (Eds.) Developments in marketing science (pp. 251 255). Miami, FL: Academy of Marketing Science. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) 176 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. Burrus, R. T., McGoldrick, K., & Schuhmann, P. W. (2007). Self-reports of student cheating: Does a definition of cheating matter? Research in Economic Education, 38(1), 3 16. Calhoon, R. P. (1969). Niccolo Machiavelli and the twentieth century administrator. Academy of Management Journal, 12(2), 205 212. Chapman, K. J., Davis, R., Toy, D., & Wright, L. (2004). Academic dishonesty in the business school environment: I’ll get by with a little help from my friends. Journal of Marketing Education, 26(3), 236 249. Cheng, L., Li, W., Zhai, Q., & Smyth, R. (2014). Understanding personal use of the Internet at work: An integrated model of neutralization techniques and general deterrence theory. Computers in Human Behavior, 38(1), 220 228. Christie, R., & Geis, F. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. New York, NY: Academic Press. Comas, R., & Sureda, J. (2010). Academic plagiarism: Explanatory factors from students’ perspective. Journal of Academic Ethics, 8(3), 217 232. Crown, D. F., & Spiller, M. S. (1998). Learning from literature on collegiate teaching: A review of empirical research. Journal of Business Ethics, 17(6), 683 700. Dellaportas, S., Leung, P., & Jackling, B. (2006). IES 4 Ethics education revisited. Australian Accounting Review, 16(38), 4 12. Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2000). Feeling “holier than thou”: Are self-serving assessments produced by errors in self- or social prediction? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 861 875. Evans, F. J., & Weiss, E. J. (2008). Views on the importance of ethics in business education: Survey results from AACSB deans, CEOs, and faculty. In D. L. Swanson & D. G. Fisher (Eds.), Advancing business ethics education (pp. 43 66). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing. Ferguson, J., Collison, D., Power, D., & Stevenson, L. (2011). Accounting education, socialisation and the ethics of business. Business Ethics: A European Review, 20(1), 12 29. Forgas, J. P. (1985). Interpersonal behavior: The psychology of social interaction. Sydney: Pergamon Press. Frank, R. H., Gilovich, T., & Regan, D. T. (1993). Does studying economics inhibit cooperation? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 7(2), 159 171. French, W. (2006). Business ethics training: Face-to-face and at a distance. Journal of Business Ethics, 66(1), 117 126. Gabriel, T. (2010). Plagiarism lines blur for students in digital age. The New York Times, August 10, p. A1. Galloway, M. K. (2012). Cheating in advantaged high schools: Prevalence, justifications, and possibilities for change. Ethics & Behavior, 22(5), 378 399. Gautschi, F. H., & Jones, T. M. (1998). Enhancing the ability of business students to recognize ethical issues: An empirical assessment of the effectiveness of a course in business ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 17(2), 205 216. Geis, F., Christie, R., & Nelson, C. (1970). In search of the Machiavelli. In R. Christie & F. L. Geis (Eds.), Studies in Machiavellianism (pp. 77 95). New York, NY: Academic Press. Granitz, N. A. (2003). Individual, social and organizational sources of sharing and variation in the ethical reasoning of managers. Journal of Business Ethics, 42(2), 101 124. Granitz, N. A., & Loewy, D. (2007). Applying ethical theories: Interpreting and responding to student plagiarism. Journal of Business Ethics, 72(3), 293 306. Harris, J. (1989). Ethical values and decision processes of male and female business students. Journal of Education for Business, 64(5), 234 238. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education 177 Holder-Webb, L., & Cohen, J. R. (2007). The association between disclosure, distress, and failure. Journal of Business Ethics, 75(3), 301 314. Jewe, R. D. (2008). Do business ethics courses work? The effectiveness of business ethics education: An empirical study. Journal of Global Business Issues, 2, 1 6. Jones, D. L. R. (2011). Academic dishonesty: Are more students cheating? Business Communication Quarterly, 74(2), 141 150. Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2009). Machiavellianism. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 257 273). New York, NY: Guilford Press (Chap. 7). Jones, G. E., & Kavanagh, M. J. (1996). An experimental examination of the effects of individual and situational factors on unethical behavioral intentions in the workplace. Journal of Business Ethics, 15(5), 511 523. Jones, J., Massey, D., & Thorne, L. (2003). Auditors’ ethical reasoning: Insights from past research and implications for the future. Journal of Accounting Literature, 22, 45 103. Josien, L., & Broderick, B. (2013). Cheating in higher education: The case of multi-methods cheaters. Academy of Educational Leadership Journal, 17(3), 93 105. Keller, A., Smith, K., & Smith, L. (2007). Do gender, education level, religiosity, and work experience affect the ethical decision-making of U.S. accountants? Critical Perspectives on Accounting, 18(3), 299 314. Kennedy, D. M. (2010). Deterrence and crime prevention: Reconsidering the prospect of sanction. New York, NY: Routledge. Klein, H. A., Levenburg, N. M., McKendall, M., & Mothersell, W. (2007). Cheating during the college years: How do business school students compare? Journal of Business Ethics, 72(2), 197 206. LaGrone, R. M., Welton, R. E., & Davis, J. R. (1996). Are the effects of accounting ethics interventions transitory or permanent? Journal of Accounting Education, 14(3), 259 276. Lampe, M., & Engleman-Lampe, C. (2012). Mindfulness-based business ethics education. Academy of Educational Leadership Journal, 16(3), 99 111. Lawson, R. A. (2004). Is classroom cheating related to business students’ propensity to cheat in the real world? Journal of Business Ethics, 49(2), Pt. 2, 189 199. Liu, C., Yao, L. J., & Hu, N. (2012). Improving ethics education in accounting: Lessons from medicine and law. Issues in Accounting Education, 27(3), 671 690. Loeb, S. (1988). Teaching students accounting ethics: Some crucial issues. Issues on Accounting Education, 3(2), 316 329. Lowell, J. (2012). Managers and moral dissonance: Self justification as a big threat to ethical management? Journal of Business Ethics, 105(1), 17 25. Luthar, H. K., & Karri, R. (2005). Exposure to ethics education and the perception of linkage between organizational ethical behavior and business outcomes. Journal of Business Ethics, 61(4), 353 368. MacGregor, J., & Stuebs, M. (2012). To cheat or not to cheat: Rationalizing academic impropriety. Accounting Education: An International Journal, 21(3), 265 287. Machiavelli, N. (1513/2009). The prince (W. K. Marriottt, Trans.). Toronto: Prohyptikon. McCabe, D. L. (2005). Levels of cheating and plagiarism remain high. Clemson, SC: Center for Academic Integrity. McCabe, D. L., Butterfield, K. D., & Trevino, L. K. (2006). Academic dishonesty in graduate business programs: Prevalence, causes and proposed action. Academy of Management Learning & Education, 5(3), 294 305. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) 178 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. McCabe, D. L., & Trevino, L. K. (1997). Individual and conceptual influences on academic dishonesty: A multi-campus investigation. Research in Higher Education, 38(3), 522 538. McCabe, D. L., Trevino, L. K., & Butterfield, K. D. (2001). Cheating in academic institutions: A decade of research. Ethics & Behavior, 11(3), 219 232. McLean, P. A., & Jones, D. B. (1992). Machiavellianism and business education. Psychological Reports, 71(1), 57 58. Menon, M. K., & Sharland, A. (2011). Narcissism, exploitative attitudes, and academic dishonesty: An exploratory investigation of reality versus myth. Journal of Education for Business, 86, 50 55. Murdock, T. B., Miller, A., & Kohlhardt, J. (2004). Effects of classroom context variables on high school students’ judgments of the acceptability and likelihood of cheating. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(4), 765 777. Natale, S. M., & Sora, S. A. (2010). Exceeding our grasp: Curricular change and the challenge to the assumptive world. Journal of Business Ethics, 92(1), 79 85. Nonis, S., & Swift, C. O. (2001). An examination of the relationship between academic dishonesty and workplace dishonesty: A multicampus investigation. Journal of Education for Business, 77(2), 69 77. O’Fallon, M. J., & Butterfield, K. D. (2005). A review of the empirical ethical decision-making literature: 1996 2003. Journal of Business Ethics, 59(4), 375 413. Paternoster, R. (2010). How much do we really know about criminal deterrence? Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 100(3), 765 823. Peppas, S. C. (2003). Attitudes toward codes of ethics: The effects of corporate misconduct. Management Research News, 26(6), 77 90. Peppas, S. C., & Diskin, B. A. (2001). College courses in ethics: Do they really make a difference? The International Journal of Educational Management, 15(7), 347 354. Piquero, A. R., Paternoster, R., Pogarsky, G., & Loughran, T. (2011). Elaborating the individual difference component in deterrence theory. Annual Review of Law and Social Sciences, 7, 335 360. Quah, C. H., Stewart, N., & Lee, J. W. C. (2012). Attitudes of business students toward plagiarism. Journal of Academic Ethics, 10(3), 185 199. Ritter, B. A. (2006). Can business ethics be trained? A study of the ethical decision-making process in business students. Journal of Business Ethics, 68(2), Pt. 1, 153 164. Sautter, J. A., Brown, T. A., Littvay, L., Sautter, A. C., & Bearnes, B. (2008). Attitude and divergence in business students: An examination of personality differences in business and non-business students. Electronic Journal of Business Ethics and Organization Studies, 13(2), 70 78. Schneider, M., & Prasso, S. (2002). How an MBA can bend your mind. Business Week, April 1, p. 12. Seshadri, S., Broekemier, G. M., & Nelson, J. W. (1998). Business ethics To teach or not to teach? Teaching Business Ethics, 1(3), 303 313. Smith, K. J., Davy, J. A., Rosenberg, D. L., & Haight, G. T. (2002). A structural modeling investigation of the influence of demographic and attitudinal factors and in-class deterrents on cheating behaviors among accounting majors. Journal of Accounting Education, 20(1), 45 65. Smyth, L. S., Davis, J. R., & Knoncke, C. O. (2009). Students’ perceptions of business ethics: Using cheating as a surrogate for business situations. Journal of Education for Business, 84(4), 229 239. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Effectiveness of Instruction in Accounting Ethics Education 179 Straw, D. (2002). The plagiarism of generation “why not? Community College Week, 14, July 8, pp. 4 7. Swanson, D. L., & Frederick, W. C. (2005). Denial and leadership in business ethics education. In R. A. Peterson & O. C. Ferrell (Eds.), Business ethics: The new challenge for business schools and corporate leaders (pp. 222 240). Armonk, NY: Sharpe. Tang, T. L.-P., & Chen, Y. (2008). Intelligence vs. wisdom: The love of money, Machiavellianism, and unethical behavior across college major and gender. Journal of Business Ethics, 82(1), 1 26. Tang, T. L.-P., & Tang, T. L. N. (2010). Finding the lost sheep: A panel study of business students’ intrinsic religiosity, Machiavellianism, and unethical behavior intentions. Ethics& Behavior, 20(5), 352 379. Tenbrunsel, A. E. (1998). Misrepresentation and expectations of misrepresentation in an ethical dilemma: The role of incentives and temptation. The Academy of Management Journal, 41(3), 330 339. Tenbrunsel, A. E., Diekmann, K. A., Wade-Benzoni, K. A., & Bazerman, M. H. (2010). The ethical mirage: A temporal explanation as to why we aren’t as ethical as we think we are. Research in Organizational Behavior, 30, 153 173. Thomas, S. (2012). Ethics and accounting education. Issues in Accounting Education, 27(2), 399 418. Tibbetts, S. G. (1999). Differences between men and women regarding decisions to commit test cheating. Research in Higher Education, 40(3), 323 341. Tweedie, D., Dyball, M. C., Hazelton, J., & Wright, S. (2013). Teaching global ethical standards: A case and strategy for broadening the accounting ethics curriculum. Journal of Business Ethics, 115(1), 1 15. Tyson, T. (1992). Does believing that everyone else is less ethical have an impact on work behavior? Journal of Business Ethics, 11(9), 707 717. Waples, E. P., Antes, A. L., Murphy, S. T., Connelly, S., & Mumford, M. D. (2009). A metaanalytic investigation of business ethics instruction. Journal of Business Ethics, 87(1), 133 151. Wenzel, M. (2004). The social side of sanctions: Personal and social norms as moderators of deterrence. Law and Human Behavior, 28(5), 547 567. Westerman, J. W., Bergman, J., Bergman, S., & Daly, J. (2012). Are universities creating millennial narcissistic employees? An empirical examination of narcissism in business students and its implications. Journal of Management Education, 36(1), 5 32. Williams, S. D., & Dewett, T. (2005). Yes, you can teach business ethics: A review and research agenda. Journal of Leadership and Organizational Studies, 12(2), 109 120. Wilson, D. S., Near, D., & Miller, R. R. (1996). Machiavellianism: A synthesis of the evolutionary and psychological literatures. Psychological Bulletin, 119(2), 285 299. Winter, S. J., Stylianou, A. C., & Giacalone, R. A. (2004). Individual differences in the acceptability of unethical information technology practices: The case of Machiavellianism and ethical ideology. Journal of Business Ethics, 54 (3), Pt. 2, 279 301. Wood, J., Longenecker, J., & Moore, C. (1988). Ethical attitudes of students and business professionals: A study of moral reasoning. Journal of Business Ethics, 7(4), 249 257. Worrall, J., Els, N., Piquero, A., & TenEyck, M. (2014). The moderating effects of informal social control in the sanctions-compliance nexus. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 39(2), 341 357. 180 DAVID J. BURNS ET AL. Downloaded by UMEA UNIVERSITY At 02:00 14 February 2016 (PT) Yang, H. L., & Wu, W. P. (2009). The effect of moral intensity on ethical decision making in accounting. Journal of Moral Education, 38(3), 335 351. Young, J. J., & Annisette, M. (2009). Cultivating imagination: Ethics, education and literature. Critical Perspectives on Accounting, 20(1), 93 109. Zyglidopoulos, S., Fleming, P., & Rothenberg, S. (2009). Rationalization, overcompensation and the escalation of corruption in organizations. Journal of Business Ethics, 84(1), 65 73.