Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 17(6):592– 603, Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Inc., Philadelphia © 2000 American Clinical Neurophysiology Society Neurophysiology and Functional Neuroanatomy of Pain Perception A. Schnitzler and M. Ploner Department of Neurology, Heinrich–Heine University, Düsseldorf, Germany Summary: The traditional view that the cerebral cortex is not involved in pain processing has been abandoned during the past decades based on anatomic and physiologic investigations in animals, and lesion, functional neuroimaging, and neurophysiologic studies in humans. These studies have revealed an extensive central network associated with nociception that consistently includes the thalamus, the primary (SI) and secondary (SII) somatosensory cortices, the insula, and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Anatomic and electrophysiologic data show that these cortical regions receive direct nociceptive thalamic input. From the results of human studies there is growing evidence that these different cortical structures contribute to different dimensions of pain experience. The SI cortex appears to be mainly involved in sensory-discriminative aspects of pain. The SII cortex seems to have an important role in recognition, learning, and memory of painful events. The insula has been proposed to be involved in autonomic reactions to noxious stimuli and in affective aspects of pain-related learning and memory. The ACC is closely related to pain unpleasantness and may subserve the integration of general affect, cognition, and response selection. The authors review the evidence on which the proposed relationship between cortical areas, pain-related neural activations, and components of pain perception is based. Key Words: Pain representation–Somatosensory cortex–SI–SII–Insula–Anterior cingulate cortex–Functional neuroimaging–Neurophysiology. Pain is an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage. It is mostly accompanied by the desire to stop and to avoid stimuli causing it. Pain can be modulated by cognitive factors like previous experiences and attention. The interaction of sensory, affective, and cognitive factors, and the indispensability for the physical integrity of the individual distinguish pain from other sensory experiences. From the peripheral receptor to the cerebral cortex, noxious stimuli are processed by specialized neural pathways termed the nociceptive system. Peripherally, noxious stimuli activate specific receptors (nociceptors) of thinly myelinated A␦- and unmyelinated C-fibers terminating in the spinal cord dorsal horn (Raja et al., 1999). In the dorsal horn, neurons responding specifically to noxious stimuli (nociceptive specific [NS]) or to both noxious and innoxious stimuli (wide dynamic range [WDR]) are found. NS neurons are mainly located in the superficial aspects of the dorsal horn (lamina I) whereas WDR neurons are located predominantly in the deep dorsal horn (laminae IV–V) (Craig and Dostrovsky, 1999). The axons of both types of neurons cross the midline within one or two segments and ascend in the spinothalamic tract (STT). In the lateral STT, most axons originate from lamina I neurons whereas axons in the anterior STT originate from deep dorsal horn neurons. Supported by grants from the Volkswagen-Stiftung (I/73240), the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (SFB 194), and the Ute-HunekeStiftung. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Alfons Schnitzler, Department of Neurology, Heinrich–Heine University, Moorenstrasse. 5, D-40225 Düsseldorf, Germany; e-mail: schnitza@uni-duesseldorf.de 592 PAIN PERCEPTION These STT neurons project to the thalamus, with the latter terminating mainly in lateral nuclei, whereas medial nuclei receive input predominantly from lamina I neurons. Cells in these thalamic nuclei in turn project to various distributed areas of the cortex. Medial and lateral thalamic nuclei and their particular cortical projection targets are summarized as medial and lateral pain systems respectively. Obviously, the nociceptive system consists of different, partially independent pathways ascending in parallel from the spinal cord dorsal horn to the cortex. At all levels these ascending parts of the nociceptive system can be modulated by descending projections. Involvement of the cerebral cortex in pain processing has been doubted for many decades. Henry Head observed that “pure cortical lesions cause no increase or decrease of sensibility to measured painful stimuli” (Head and Holmes, 1911, p. 154). Accordingly, electrical stimulation of the human cortex only rarely elicited painful sensations (Penfield and Boldrey, 1937). These historical findings were taken as evidence against a participation of the cerebral cortex in human pain processing. Later, various lesion studies contradicted these findings (for reviews see Kenshalo and Willis [1991] and Sweet [1982]). However, the observed clinical deficits were inconsistent, partly because information about lesion extent or presence of additional lesions was often scant before high-resolution structural brain imaging became available. More unequivocal evidence for an involvement of the cerebral cortex in pain processing came from animal experiments. Anatomic studies revealed parallel nociceptive thalamocortical projections to various cortical areas where corresponding neurophysiologic studies demonstrated pain-evoked neuronal responses. Eventually, these findings were complemented by human lesion studies using computed tomography and MRI, and by functional neuroimaging studies using single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), positron emission tomography (PET), and functionally MRI (fMRI). These studies noninvasively demonstrated involvement of distributed cerebral areas in human pain processing comprising a variety of subcortical and cortical regions. More recently, the topographic aspects of pain representations in the cerebral cortex have been extended to the temporal domain by results of neurophysiologic studies using whole-scalp magnetoencephalography (MEG) and EEG. Analysis of both temporal and spatial aspects of cortical activity allows inferences on the hierarchical organization of pain processing. Moreover, new stimuli and refined experimental paradigms allow one to investigate 593 the nociceptive system selectively, without concurrent stimulation of tactile afferents, and to dissociate different aspects of the pain experience. In some aspects, the results of these studies are not yet unequivocal. However, concerning the relevance of some essential cortical areas there is growing consensus between human and experimental animal studies. These areas are the primary (SI) and secondary (SII) somatosensory cortices of the lateral pain system, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) of the medial pain system, and the insular cortex, which cannot be assigned clearly to one of these systems. The current review summarizes the main findings indicating participation of these areas in the processing of pain. In addition, the relationship between the function of these areas and different aspects of pain perception is demonstrated. PRIMARY SOMATOSENSORY CORTEX The role of SI in pain perception has long been in dispute. Lesions of SI have been reported to cause either hypoalgesia or hyperalgesia, or to affect pain perception not at all (for reviews see Kenshalo and Willis [1991] and Sweet [1982]). In contrast, results from experimental animal studies clearly indicate participation of SI in pain processing. Anatomic studies revealed nociceptive projections from lateral thalamic nuclei, particularly from the ventral posterior lateral nucleus (VPL), to SI (Gingold et al., 1991; Kenshalo et al., 1980). Electrophysiologically, single cell recordings in rats (Lamour et al., 1983), and anesthetized (Chudler et al., 1990; Kenshalo and Isensee, 1983) and awake (Kenshalo et al., 1988) monkeys demonstrated neurons in SI that responded to noxious stimuli. They were found much less frequently than cells responsive to tactile stimuli and were localized at the borders between the cytoarchitectonic areas 3b and 1, and 1 and 2 respectively (Chudler et al., 1990; Kenshalo and Isensee, 1983). Most of these nociceptive SI neurons were arranged somatotopically and had restricted receptive fields (Chudler et al., 1990; Kenshalo and Isensee, 1983; Kenshalo et al., 1988; Lamour et al., 1983). The activity of these neurons correlated with duration and intensity of the stimulus (Chudler et al., 1990; Kenshalo et al., 1988; Kenshalo and Isensee, 1983; Lamour et al., 1983) as well as with the intensity of stimulus perception (Kenshalo et al., 1988). Psychophysical data suggest that these findings most probably also apply to humans (Chudler et al., 1990). Thus, functionally, nociceptive SI neurons are predestinated to encode sensory-discriminative aspects of pain. In contrast to these animal studies but corresponding J Clin Neurophysiol, Vol. 17, No. 6, 2000 594 A. SCHNITZLER AND M. PLONER to the inconsistent clinical observations of human lesion studies, the first functional neuroimaging studies published in the beginning of the 1990s have produced greatly different results demonstrating pain-evoked increase, decrease, or no change of SI activity. By using PET and repeated heat stimuli presented to six spots on the arm, Talbot et al. (1991) found a notable activation focus in SI contralateral to the stimulated arm. By using SPECT, Apkarian et al. (1992) found that submerging fingers in hot water led to a decrease in SI activity. Finally, by using similar heat stimuli, but presented to a single spot on the dorsal hand, Jones et al. (1991a) failed to observe notable activation in SI. However, more recent PET, fMRI, and MEG/EEG studies (Andersson et al., 1997; Casey et al., 1994; Coghill et al., 1994, 1999; Craig et al., 1996; Derbyshire et al., 1997; Iadarola et al., 1998; Ploner et al., 1999b, 2000; Rainville et al., 1997; Tarkka and Treede, 1993) have provided convergent evidence for participation of SI in human pain processing. Furthermore, the results of several investigations point to a specific role of SI in sensory-discriminative functions of pain perception, such as spatial discrimination (Andersson et al., 1997; Tarkka and Treede, 1993) and intensity coding (Coghill et al., 1999; Porro et al., 1998). Tarkka and Treede (1993) reported results consistent with a somatotopic organization of cerebral responses along the central sulcus in a study modeling evoked potentials arising from laser pulses of painful heat. Using PET, Andersson et al. (1997) demonstrated a similar somatotopic organization of activations along the central sulcus during sustained pain on the dorsum of the hand or foot elicited by intracutaneous injection of capsaicin. Somatotopic arrangement of pain in SI strongly suggests that this region is involved in localization of painful stimuli on the body surface. In agreement with this notion, a recent case report demonstrated that localization of painful stimuli may be grossly impaired when SI is damaged (Ploner et al., 1999a). In that study painful laser stimuli were delivered to a patient with a right postcentral stroke as documented by high-resolution MRI. When the dorsum of the affected left hand was stimulated, the patient reported an intense unpleasant sensation that was only poorly localized anywhere in the left upper limb. Studies combining brain imaging with psychophysical assessment of graded painful stimuli have revealed positive correlations between perceived pain intensity and activation of a functionally diverse group of brain regions, including the contralateral SI (Coghill et al., 1999; Porro et al., 1998). These results indicate that the intensity of different aspects of pain may be processed prefJ Clin Neurophysiol, Vol. 17, No. 6, 2000 erentially in different regions. For example, SI activation correlates with the intensity of pain sensation (Bushnell et al., 1999) but not with the intensity of pain unpleasantness (Rainville et al., 1997). Taken together, these findings indicate an essential role of SI for the sensory-discriminative pain component. Nevertheless, the inconsistency of results of early lesion and functional imaging studies remains remarkable. This may be due in part to different net effects of excitation and inhibition within SI (Apkarian, 1995; Bushnell et al., 1999): Neurophysiologic recordings revealed that some nociceptive neurons in SI had very large receptive fields (Kenshalo and Isensee, 1983; Lamour et al., 1983; Mountcastle and Powell, 1959), did not code stimulus intensity or duration, or were even suppressed by noxious stimuli (Chudler et al., 1990; Kenshalo et al., 1988; Lamour et al., 1983). Such inhibitory effects within SI have also been demonstrated simultaneously with (Tommerdahl et al., 1996, 1998) as well as after excitation (Backonja et al., 1991). Inhibition has been observed within (Tommerdahl et al., 1996, 1998) as well as outside the somatotopically appropriate regions of SI (Derbyshire et al., 1997). These inhibitory effects on the neurophysiologic level may account for pain-induced impairments in tactile perception on the behavioral level (Apkarian et al., 1994), and it has been proposed that inhibition within SI optimizes the utilization of SI processing circuits for pain perception. Regardless of the functional role of inhibitory phenomena, the net effect of exciting some neurons and inhibiting the spontaneous activity of others could have different effects on regional cerebral blood flow (as measured by PET) or on venous blood oxygenation (as measured by fMRI), depending on such variables as timing, duration, location, and intensity of the painful stimulus (Bushnell et al., 1999). Another confounding variable that may account for inconsistent pain-evoked SI activations is its modulation by cognitive factors. Attention to the sensory aspects of pain has been shown to yield increases in SI activity (Bushnell et al., 1999) as well as in the intensity of stimulus perception (Bushnell et al., 1985, 1999). Furthermore, anticipation of a painful stimulus yields decreases in blood flow in areas of SI outside the representation of the anticipated stimulus (Drevets et al., 1995). Thus, neurons that show inhibition to noxious stimuli and do not encode stimulus intensity or location may participate in cognitive modulation of SI activity and pain perception. Anatomically, SI consists of four cytoarchitectonically defined areas, each of which contains a separate representation of the body surface with a, at least partially, PAIN PERCEPTION 595 FIG. 1. Comparison of tactile and nociceptive responses in a single subject. Tactile and nociceptive afferents supplying the dorsum of the hand were stimulated. Tactile afferents were stimulated with 0.3-msec constant voltage pulses delivered to the superficial branch of the radial nerve just proximal to the wrist. Cutaneous nociceptive afferents were stimulated selectively by 1-msec pulses of a Tm:YAG laser. (A) Magnetic field patterns at 30, 49, and 68 msec (tactile), and 145 msec (nociceptive); corresponding current sources; and source strength waveforms. The helmet-shaped sensor array is viewed from the upper left. Shaded areas indicate fields directed into the head. Arrows represent location and direction of current sources; corresponding source strengths as a function of time are shown on the right of the sensor arrays. (B) Location of sources superimposed on the subject’s brain surface rendering. Note that, along the postcentral gyrus, the nociceptive primary somatosensory cortex source is located more medially than the tactile area 3b response. Its location corresponds well to the tactile area 1 source. Note also that tactile stimuli elicited a response arising from the posterior parietal cortex, whereas no corresponding activity was recorded to nociceptive stimuli. Adapted from Ploner M, Schmitz F, Freund H-J, Schnitzler A. Differential organization of touch and pain in human primary somatosensory cortex. J Neurophysiol 2000;83:1770 – 6. serial information flow and hierarchical organization in the tactile modality (for review see Iwamura [1998]). By comparing neuromagnetic responses to nociceptive and tactile stimuli delivered to the same skin site of the dorsum of the hand, Ploner et al. (2000) studied the precise representation of pain within the parietal cortex in a group of normal subjects. In agreement with previous anatomic and physiologic investigations (for review see Iwamura [1998]), they found serial activations of three parietal sources to tactile stimuli: Brodmann area 3b, representing the first cortical stage of a processing cascade, followed by activation of area 1 and the posterior parietal cortex. By contrast, nociceptive stimuli activated only a single parietal source, which was located at the site of the tactile area 1 source (Fig. 1). This finding agrees with results of monkey studies (Chudler et al., 1990; Kenshalo and Isensee, 1983), and suggests that nociceptive processing does not share the elaborate and hierarchical organization of tactile processing that probably evolved during evolution in parallel with an improvement in tactile capacities of the hands. Taken together, a considerable amount of evidence suggests that SI has a pivotal role in sensory-discriminative aspects of pain. Within SI, pain processing appears to be less hierarchically organized than tactile processing. Cognitive factors can alter the perceived intensity of pain and, accordingly, can modulate SI activity in functional imaging studies. Physiologically, inhibition of nociceptive neurons and neurons with nonsensory-discriminative response characteristics may be involved in this cognitive modulation and in the interaction of pain and touch. J Clin Neurophysiol, Vol. 17, No. 6, 2000 596 A. SCHNITZLER AND M. PLONER SECONDARY SOMATOSENSORY CORTEX The second somatosensory area is located in the parietal operculum in the upper bank of the Sylvian fissure and its existence has been known since the early cortical stimulation studies during epileptic surgery (Penfield and Jasper, 1954). Several clinical observations of patients with opercular lesions suggest that SII is relevant to the perception of pain. Patients with lesions in the parietal operculum have mainly been reported to show impaired pain thresholds (Greenspan and Winfield, 1992; Greenspan et al., 1999; Kenshalo and Willis, 1991; Sweet, 1982). However, macroscopically, SII and the insular cortex are difficult to separate. Thus, based on lesion studies alone, inferences about the role of SII in pain are not unequivocal. Anatomic studies indicate that SII receives nociceptive projections from lateral thalamic nuclei (Friedman and Murray, 1986; Stevens et al., 1993). These projections originate mostly from the ventral posterior inferior (VPI) thalamic nucleus (Friedman and Murray, 1986; Stevens et al., 1993), whereas nociceptive projections to SI originate predominantly from the VPL (Gingold et al., 1991; Kenshalo et al., 1980). Differences in spinal input and response characteristics of VPL and VPI neurons (Apkarian and Hodge, 1989; Apkarian and Shi, 1994; Dong et al., 1989; Kenshalo and Willis, 1991) indicate anatomic and functional segregation of nociceptive pathways from the spinal cord to SI and SII. Correspondingly, contrary to most SI cells, nociceptive neurons in SII including the neighboring area 7b and the retroinsular cortex have mostly large, bilateral receptive fields (Dong et al., 1989, 1994; Robinson and Burton, 1980; Whitsel et al., 1969) and encode stimulus intensity poorly (Dong et al., 1989, 1994; Robinson and Burton, 1980). However, only very few neurons have been found in these areas, mainly in the caudal part of SII, that responded to noxious stimuli (Dong et al., 1989, 1994; Robinson and Burton, 1980; Whitsel et al., 1969). Some of these nociceptive neurons have multisensory properties and respond additionally to stimuli from other modalities, particularly to threatening visual stimuli from locations corresponding to the nociceptive receptive field (Dong et al., 1994; Whitsel et al., 1969). Thus, although SII receives appropriate spinothalamic projections, evidence from single cell recordings for nociceptive neurons in this area is sparse. The paucity of nociceptive SII neurons in animal studies contrasts with results of human neurophysiologic and neuroimaging studies in which SII is one of the regions that is found most consistently to be activated. SII has been implicated in a number of PET and fMRI J Clin Neurophysiol, Vol. 17, No. 6, 2000 studies on experimental pain (Casey et al., 1994; Coghill et al., 1994, 1999; Craig et al., 1996; Davis et al., 1998; Derbyshire et al., 1997; Gelnar et al., 1999; Iadarola et al., 1998; Rainville et al., 1997; Talbot et al., 1991) as well as in neuromagnetic recordings of cortical activity (Hari et al., 1983, 1997; Huttunen et al., 1986; Kakigi et al., 1995; Ploner et al., 1999b, 2000) or scalp-evoked potentials (Bromm and Chen, 1995; Spiegel et al., 1996; Tarkka and Treede, 1993) using different painful stimuli such as thermal heat or laser-radiant heat. Generally, activations are observed bilaterally in the upper bank of the Sylvian fissure. Recordings of laser-evoked potentials from subdural grids (Lenz et al., 1998) confirm the results of these noninvasive studies. A recent investigation using stereotactically implanted depth electrodes (Frot et al., 1999) suggests that the generator of laserevoked potentials may be located at the inner surface of the parietal operculum on the outer bank of the circular sulcus of the insula, which would be outside the classic SII area. If the nociceptive area was separate from the tactile area, then the discrepancy between animal and human studies could have a simple explanation: Because electrophysiologic studies in SII usually use mechanical search stimuli, they may have missed the nociceptive neurons systematically. In humans, the temporal activation pattern of SI and SII strongly supports direct thalamocortical distribution of nociceptive information to SII: MEG recordings revealed nearly simultaneous activation onsets of SI and SII to selective nociceptive stimuli indicating parallel activation of these areas (Ploner et al., 1999b) (Fig. 2). This contrasts with the sequential activation of SI and SII after innocuous tactile stimuli (Hari et al., 1993; Ploner et al., 2000; Schnitzler et al., 1999), which reflect a serial organization of these cortices in human tactile processing (for review see Iwamura [1998]). Thus, temporal activation patterns suggest fundamentally different processing modes in humans for tactile and nociceptive information, the former following a serial processing scheme from thalamus via SI to SII and the latter underlying parallel processing in SI and SII. Interestingly, this parallel organizational mode of SI and SII in human pain processing corresponds to the parallel cortical organization of tactile processing in lower primates and nonprimates (Garraghty et al., 1991; Turman et al., 1992), which has shifted to serial processing during evolution in higher primates and humans. Obviously, the basic mammalian parallel organizational scheme has been preserved in human pain processing. Assuming that the hierarchical organization of somatosensory cortices in human tactile processing allows for sophisticated discriminative capacities, these appear to be of less importance in human pain PAIN PERCEPTION FIG. 2. Cortical responses to cutaneous laser stimuli (see Fig. 1) applied to the right hand. (A) Location of cortical sources superposed on MR images. (Left) Axial slice through the primary somatosensory cortex (SI) hand area viewed from above. (Middle, right) Coronal and axial slice through the secondary somatosensory cortex (SII). (B) Source strength as a function of time. Group mean (⫾standard error of the mean) source activities across six subjects are shown. Note the virtually simultaneous activation of contralateral SI and SII, indicating parallel thalamocortical organization of nociceptive information flow. Contra, contralateral; ipsi, ipsilateral. Adapted from Ploner M, Schmitz F, Freund H-J, Schnitzler A. Parallel activation of primary and secondary somatosensory cortices in human pain processing. J Neurophysiol 1999b;81:3100 – 4. processing than in tactile processing. Instead, preservation of direct access to SII emphasizes the relevance of SII in human pain processing. The anatomic connections of SII and inferences from its proposed function in tactile processing suggest an important role of SII in pain-associated learning and memory. SII projects via the insula to the temporal lobe limbic structures (Friedman et al., 1986; Shi and Cassell, 1998a). These corticolimbic projections have been proposed to subserve tactile learning and memory (Friedman et al., 1986; Mishkin, 1979). Similarly, SII may play a key role in relaying nociceptive information to the temporal lobe limbic structures (Dong et al., 1989; Lenz et al., 1997). 597 For the tactile system, there is evidence that areas in the parietal operculum may be involved in feature extraction such as roughness discrimination and object size detection (Ledberg et al., 1995). Single neurons in SII often exhibit weak responses to passively presented tactile stimuli and larger receptive fields than those in SI, but display specificity for spatial patterns such as gratings and are activated specifically during active touch tasks (Sinclair and Burton, 1993). As such, one of the functions of SII may be tactile object recognition. For this function, most of the input to SII is assumed to derive from SI, and the output is directed toward the insular cortex and the parahippocampal gyrus (Friedman et al., 1986). This pattern of connections is similar to that of the inferior temporal cortex, which is implicated in the visual discrimination of objects (Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982). By analogy, one of the functions of SII in pain perception may be the recognition of the noxious nature of a stimulus. Consistent with this idea, patients with lesions in the parietal operculum have both impaired tactile object recognition (Caselli, 1993) and deficits in pain perception (Biemond, 1956; Greenspan and Winfeld, 1992; Greenspan et al., 1999; Ploner et al., 1999a). The case study by Ploner et al. (1999a) represents striking support in favor of this notion. They report a patient with an ischemic lesion of the left SII (and SI). Besides being grossly impaired in localizing a painful laser stimulus delivered on the affected hand, this patient was totally unable to recognize the noxious nature of the stimulus even when appropriate terms were presented to him. When asked to pick terms that might describe his sensation from a list that included “hot,” “burning,” and “pain,” the patient chose none. However, he perceived an intense unpleasantness and showed appropriate motor reactions and avoidance behavior. In summary, neurophysiologic and functional imaging data clearly indicate participation of SII in human pain processing. Preserved, direct thalamic access of nociceptive information to SII, supported by anatomic thalamocortical connections and by parallel activation of somatosensory cortices, suggests a particular relevance of this area in pain processing. Functionally, SII may be involved in recognition, learning, and memory of painful events. By contrast, in animal studies only very few SII neurons respond to noxious stimuli. The relevance of this conflict currently remains uncertain. INSULA Until recently, evidence for participation of the insula in human pain processing was scarce. However, during J Clin Neurophysiol, Vol. 17, No. 6, 2000 598 A. SCHNITZLER AND M. PLONER the last decade this has changed dramatically. Lesion studies have shown that damage restricted to the insula apparently reduces pain affect and appropriate reactions to painful and threatening visual and auditory stimuli but does not influence pain threshold (Berthier et al., 1988; Greenspan et al., 1999). In addition, functional imaging studies in particular demonstrated consistently pain-related activations of the insula (Andersson et al., 1997; Binkofski et al., 1998; Casey et al., 1994, 1996; Coghill et al., 1994, 1999; Craig et al., 1996; Davis et al., 1998; Derbyshire et al., 1997; Gelnar et al., 1999; Iadarola et al., 1998; Rainville et al., 1997; Talbot et al., 1991; Tolle et al., 1999). These results motivated complementary investigations in experimental animals. Indeed, single cell recordings and local field potentials in rats (Hanamori et al., 1998; Ito, 1998) and monkeys (Dostrovsky and Craig 1996; Robinson and Burton, 1980; Zhang et al., 1999) revealed nociceptive responses in the insula. These nociceptive neurons had large receptive fields (Robinson and Burton, 1980; Zhang et al., 1999) and responded partially to multimodal stimulation, in particular to visceral stimuli (Hanamori et al., 1998; Ito, 1998; Zhang et al., 1999). Together, these results clearly indicate participation of the insula in the processing of pain. The response characteristics of nociceptive neurons in the insula point against sensory-discriminative functions. So far, considerations of the functional importance of the insula in pain processing have to rely mainly on indirect evidence. The insula consists of anatomically and functionally different areas (for reviews see Augustine [1996] and Mesulam and Mufson [1985]). Thalamic and cortical connectivity as well as results from physiologic studies suggest that the posterior granular parts of the insula are related mainly to auditory, visual, and somatosensory functions, whereas the anterior dysgranular parts of the insula are related predominantly to limbic, olfactory, gustatory, and viscero-autonomic functions (Augustine, 1996; Mesulam and Mufson, 1985). Thus, the anterior insula processes predominantly intrapersonal information, and the posterior insula processes mainly extrapersonal information. Most functional imaging studies agree that pain-related activations are located in the anterior parts of the insula (Andersson et al., 1997; Binkofski et al., 1998; Casey et al., 1994, 1996; Coghill et al., 1994, 1999; Craig et al., 1996; Davis et al., 1998; Derbyshire et al., 1997; Gelnar et al., 1999; Iadarola et al., 1998; Rainville et al., 1997; Talbot et al., 1991; Tolle et al., 1999). In contrast, tactile activations are located more posteriorly (Burton et al., 1993; Coghill et al., 1994). Thus, painrelated activity appears to be spatially distinct from J Clin Neurophysiol, Vol. 17, No. 6, 2000 tactile activity and to be located in parts of the insula where mainly intrapersonal information is processed. This is supported by single cell recordings showing nociceptive neurons in the mid/anterior insula (Dostrovsky and Craig, 1996). However, an earlier investigation that did not explore the anterior insula showed nociceptive neurons in the posterior insula (Robinson and Burton, 1980). Therefore, further investigations are needed to clarify the distribution of nociceptive neurons and their spatial relationship to neurons concerned with other functions. Recently, the mid/anterior insula has been shown to receive afferents from a distinct posterior thalamic nucleus termed the posterior portion of the ventral medial nucleus (VMpo) (Craig, 1995). This newly described thalamic nucleus has been shown to contain almost exclusively thermoreceptive and nociceptive neurons, and to receive predominantly afferents from lamina I of the spinal cord dorsal horn (Craig et al., 1994). Thus, a nociceptive and thermoreceptive spinothalamocortical pathway from lamina I of the spinal cord dorsal horn via VMpo to the insula has been proposed (Craig et al., 1994). This pathway has been suggested to mediate enteroceptive information (i.e., information on the physiologic condition of the body itself, including the specific sensations of pain and temperature) (Craig, 1996). Disruption of this pathway has been proposed to disinhibit activity in a parallel nociceptive pathway to the ACC and, by this means, to be involved in the generation of central pain (Craig, 1998). However, these findings remain to be integrated with results from previous anatomic, physiologic and functional imaging studies. Particularly the correspondence between an insular region defined by connectivity and single cell responses in monkeys to pain-related activations of the anterior insula in humans has not yet been demonstrated unequivocally. Nevertheless, connections of the insula to widespread thalamic and cortical regions related to all sensory modalities and autonomic and limbic functions suggest a supramodal integrative role of the insula subserving appropriate, particularly autonomic, responses to stimuli from various modalities including pain. In addition, the insula receives afferents from SII and projects to the amygdala and hippocampal formation, and has thus been proposed to subserve tactile and pain-related learning and memory (Friedman et al., 1986; Lenz et al., 1997; Mesulam and Mufson, 1985; Shi and Cassell, 1998a, b). Therefore, the insula may integrate pain-related input from SII and the thalamus with contextual information from other modalities before relaying this information to the temporal lobe limbic structures. In summary, numerous functional imaging studies PAIN PERCEPTION indicate participation of anterior insular cortex in human pain processing. Experimental animal studies suggest involvement of the insula in a wide variety of functions with anatomic and functional differences between anterior and posterior insula. Recent findings indicating a dedicated pain and temperature pathway to the insula need to be integrated with previous experimental animal and functional imaging data. Functionally, the insula may be involved in autonomic reactions to noxious stimuli and in pain-related memory and learning. ANTERIOR CINGULATE CORTEX Traditionally the cingulate gyrus has been regarded as part of the limbic system, and was thus associated with the motivational-affective component of pain. Accordingly, surgical lesions of the cingulate cortex (Hurt and Ballantine, 1974) and the neighboring white matter (Foltz and White, 1962) reduce the emotional value and the motivation to avoid painful stimuli but do not impair detection of painful stimuli. Such effects have also been observed in experimental animals (Vaccarino and Melzack, 1989). Thus, these results confirm the relative independence of different aspects of pain perception as well as of the corresponding anatomic substrates. However, the cingulate cortex is an anatomically and functionally heterogenous area. Cytoarchitectonically, it is divided into the ACC, which has an agranular appearance similar to motor cortex, and a posterior part consisting of granular layers II and IV typical of areas receiving primary afferent input (Brodmann, 1909). In general, the ACC receives little afferent supply from other cortical areas whereas the posterior cingulate has extensive input from the frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes. Nevertheless the two cingulate parts are well interconnected. Differences in input are especially striking in connections with the amygdala, which projects reciprocally to the anterior but not to the posterior cingulate. Because a major role of the amygdala is the storage of emotional memory, its connections with the ACC may serve to relate ongoing with past emotional experience. Anterior and posterior cingulate are also functionally distinct (Vogt et al., 1992). Particularly, the ACC appears to be involved in pain processing: This part receives input mainly from the medial thalamic nuclei (midline and intralaminar nuclei, central, parafascicular, reuniens, and mediodorsal nuclei) (Baleydier and Mauguiere, 1980; Sikes and Vogt, 1992; Vogt et al., 1979, 1987), which in turn receive afferents from the STT (Albe-Fessard et al., 1985; Apkarian, 1995; Dong et al., 1978). 599 Single cell recordings in the ACC of animals (Koyama et al., 1998; Sikes and Vogt, 1992; Yamamura et al., 1996) revealed nociceptive neurons that are not organized somatotopically and that have large receptive fields that often include the entire animal (Sikes and Vogt, 1992; Yamamura et al., 1996). A substantial proportion of neurons has nociceptive-specific characteristics whereas others behave like polymodal nociceptors in that they respond to noxious heat and noxious pressure. The only innocuous stimulus that activates these neurons is tap stimuli. The large receptive fields and activation by innocuous tap are similar to nociceptive neurons in the medial thalamic nuclei (Bushnell and Duncan, 1989; Dong et al., 1978), which suggests that ACC nociceptive activity arises from these nuclei. This is corroborated further by the finding that activity of nociceptive neurons in the ACC can be almost completely blocked with lidocaine injections into the medial and intralaminar thalamic nuclei (Sikes and Vogt, 1992). In a recent study, single-neuron recordings and microstimulation were carried out in the ACC of patients under local anesthesia who underwent cingulotomy for the treatment of psychiatric disease (Hutchison et al., 1999). In agreement with animal data neurons were identified that responded selectively to painful thermal and mechanical stimuli (Figs. 3B and C), thus providing direct support, on the single-neuron level, for a role of the ACC in human pain sensation. Most neurons were activated and very few were inhibited. None of these cells responded to innocuous mechanical or thermal stimuli. Receptive fields were usually large and often bilateral. Surprisingly, electrical stimulation even with high currents at sites in the ACC, where pain-sensitive neurons were recorded, failed to elicit painful or unpleasant sensations. As possible explanations the authors propose that pain may be perceived only with simultaneous activation of other cortical regions (for example, SI, insula) or that it requires bilateral activation of the ACC. Furthermore, electrical activation may not mimic normal pain-related firing patterns adequately. Additionally, it is conceivable that pain-related activity in the ACC may also represent descending modulation of pain rather than perception of pain. The location of pain-related neurons in the study of Hutchison et al. (1999) is in good agreement with a number of functional neuroimaging studies showing consistent ACC activation during application of noxious stimuli to various parts of the body (Figs. 3A and B) (Binkofski et al., 1998; Casey et al., 1994; Coghill et al., 1994, 1999; Craig et al., 1996; Davis et al., 1997; Derbyshire et al., 1994, 1998; Gelnar et al., 1999; Hsieh et al., 1995; Iadarola et al., 1998; Jones et al., 1991a; J Clin Neurophysiol, Vol. 17, No. 6, 2000 600 A. SCHNITZLER AND M. PLONER FIG. 3. Activation of human anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) by painful stimuli. (A) Changes in pain-related positron emission tomographic (PET) activity associated with hypnotic suggestions of high unpleasantness. Sagittal slice through the ACC. The red circle indicates the location and size of the activated ACC area. The image represents the subtraction of PET data recorded when the hand was submerged in thermally neutral water (35°C) from data recorded when the hand was submerged in painfully hot water (47°C). PET data were averaged across 11 experimental sessions, and are illustrated against the average MRI for that subject group (Reprinted with permission from American Association for the Advancement of Science. Source: Rainville P, Duncan GH, Price DD, Carrier B, Bushnell MC. Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science 1997;277:968 –71.) (B, C) Responses and locations of ACC neurons recorded from patients during neurosurgical treatment (cingulotomy) for psychiatric illness. (Adapted from Hutchison WD, Davis KD, Lozano AM, Tasker RR, Dostrovsky JO. Pain-related neurons in the human cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci 1999;2:403–5.) (B) Diagram showing the locations of pain-related neurons (red circles) on a sagittal map 3 mm lateral to the midline. The dashed outline shows the region in which neurons were tested. VAC, vertical line through the ACC; VPC, vertical line through the posterior commissure. (C) ACC neuron from B responding to thermal stimuli in the noxious range. Note that stimuli reported by the patient as warm but nonpainful (44°C, 46°C) did not activate the unit. (Top trace) Thermode temperature. (Below) Firing-rate histogram (bin width, 200 msec). The location of the thermode was on the contralateral volar forearm. Porro et al., 1998; Rainville et al., 1997; Silverman et al., 1997; Talbot et al., 1991; Tolle et al., 1999; Vogt et al., 1992, 1996). It is also consistent with the location of pain-evoked potentials in humans obtained both from intracranial (Lenz et al., 1998) and extracranial recordings (Bromm et al., 1995; Tarkka and Treede, 1993; Valeriani et al., 2000). In addition to the clinical reports that patients with cingulotomies sometimes still feel pain but report it as less distressing or bothersome, and that there is a high level of opioid binding in the ACC (Jones et al., 1991b; Vogt et al., 1995), there is now also convincing evidence that pain affect (i.e., the unpleasantness of pain) is encoded in the ACC. By using hypnosis, Rainville et al. (1997) selectively altered the unpleasantness of noxious thermal stimuli without changing the perceived pain intensity. Analysis of cerebral activation pattern as measured by PET revealed that the modulation of pain affect was paralleled by activation changes in the ACC but not in other brain areas (Rainville et al., 1997) (Fig. 3A). However, it should be mentioned that the ACC cannot be considered a mere pain center because it is also activated by various other conditions such as during performance of the Stroop task (Bench et al., 1993; Pardo et al., 1990) and other cognitively challenging tasks (Corbetta et al., 1991; Petersen et al., 1988). Moreover, the ACC appears to be involved in regulation of autonomic functions and response selection and initiation as well as in attention (for review see Devinsky et al. [1995]). Therefore, pain-associated activations may represent a nonpain-specific effect (for example, of attenJ Clin Neurophysiol, Vol. 17, No. 6, 2000 tion). However, direct comparison of pain- and attentionrelated activations revealed that these activations do not correspond spatially to each other (Davis et al., 1997; Derbyshire et al., 1998). Thus, pain-related activations of the ACC are unlikely to be the result, exclusively, of attention effects. On the other hand, some studies showed more than one activation focus in the ACC (Derbyshire et al., 1998; Tolle et al., 1999; Vogt et al., 1996). This and the proximity of the nociceptive, motor, and cognitive regions of the ACC suggests possible local interconnections that may allow the output of the ACC pain area to command immediate behavioral reactions. CONCLUSIONS During the last decade, advances in functional brain imaging techniques have led to the identification of neuroanatomic substrates of pain perception. SI and SII, the insula, and the ACC are involved essentially in cortical processing of painful stimuli. Conceptual and methodological refinements have allowed for characterization and selective investigation of different components of pain perception. Taken together, accumulating evidence suggests that each of the previously mentioned cortical areas is concerned with processing of specific aspects of pain. On the behavioral level, these different aspects of pain perception are partly independent of each other. Accordingly, on the anatomic level, the painrelated areas and pathways are organized predominantly in parallel. However, well-orchestrated cooperation between these distributed areas is required to unify the PAIN PERCEPTION different aspects of pain perception into the unique and indispensable experience of pain. The advances in our understanding of normal pain perception have created the basis for studying the pathophysiology of abnormal pain states (e.g., chronic pain syndromes) that may result from imbalances within the pain-related cerebral network. REFERENCES Albe-Fessard D, Berkley KJ, Kruger L, Ralston HJ, Willis WD Jr. Diencephalic mechanisms of pain sensation. Brain Res 1985;356: 217–96. Andersson JL, Lilja A, Hartvig P, et al. Somatotopic organization along the central sulcus, for pain localization in humans, as revealed by positron emission tomography. Exp Brain Res 1997;117:192–9. Apkarian AV. Functional imaging of pain: new insights regarding the role of the cerebral cortex in human pain perception. Semin Neurosci 1995;7:279 –93. Apkarian AV, Hodge CJ. Primate spinothalamic pathways: II. The cells of origin of the dorsolateral and ventral spinothalamic pathways. J Comp Neurol 1989;288:474 –92. Apkarian AV, Shi T. Squirrel monkey lateral thalamus. I. Somatic nociresponsive neurons and their relation to spinothalamic terminals. J Neurosci 1994;14:6779 –95. Apkarian AV, Stea RA, Bolanowski SJ. Heat-induced pain diminishes vibrotactile perception: a touch gate. Somatosens Mot Res 1994; 11:259 – 67. Apkarian AV, Stea RA, Manglos SH, Szeverenyi NM, King RB, Thomas FD. Persistent pain inhibits contralateral somatosensory cortical activity in humans. Neurosci Lett 1992;140:141–7. Augustine JR. Circuitry and functional aspects of the insular lobe in primates including humans. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 1996;22: 229 – 44. Backonja M, Howland EW, Wang J, Smith J, Salinsky M, Cleeland CS. Tonic changes in alpha power during immersion of the hand in cold water. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1991;79:192– 203. Baleydier C, Mauguiere F. The duality of the cingulate gyrus in monkey. Neuroanatomical study and functional hypothesis. Brain 1980;103:525–54. Bench CJ, Frith CD, Grasby PM, et al. Investigations of the functional anatomy of attention using the Stroop test. Neuropsychologia 1993;31:907–22. Berthier M, Starkstein S, Leiguarda R. Asymbolia for pain: a sensory– limbic disconnection syndrome. Ann Neurol 1988;24:41–9. Biemond A. The conduction of pain above the level of the thalamus opticus. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 1956;75:231– 44. Binkofski F, Schnitzler A, Enck P, et al. Somatic and limbic cortex activation in esophageal distention: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Ann Neurol 1998;44:811–5. Brodmann K. Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grohirnrinde. Leipzig: Barth, 1909. Bromm B, Chen AC. Brain electrical source analysis of laser evoked potentials in response to painful trigeminal nerve stimulation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1995;95:14 –26. Burton H, Videen TO, Raichle ME. Tactile-vibration-activated foci in insular and parietal-opercular cortex studied with positron emission tomography: mapping the second somatosensory area in humans. Somatosens Mot Res 1993;10:297–308. Bushnell MC, Duncan GH. Sensory and affective aspects of pain perception: is medial thalamus restricted to emotional issues? Exp Brain Res 1989;78:415– 8. Bushnell MC, Duncan GH, Dubner R, Jones RL, Maixner W. Attentional influences on noxious and innocuous cutaneous heat detection in humans and monkeys. J Neurosci 1985;5:1103–10. 601 Bushnell MC, Duncan GH, Hofbauer RK, Ha B, Chen J, Carrier B. Pain perception: is there a role for primary somatosensory cortex? Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1999;96:7705–9. Caselli RJ. Ventrolateral and dorsomedial somatosensory association cortex damage produces distinct somesthetic syndromes in humans. Neurology 1993;43:762–71. Casey KL, Minoshima S, Berger KL, Koeppe RA, Morrow TJ, Frey KA. Positron emission tomographic analysis of cerebral structures activated specifically by repetitive noxious heat stimuli. J Neurophysiol 1994;71:802–7. Casey KL, Minoshima S, Morrow TJ, Koeppe RA. Comparison of human cerebral activation pattern during cutaneous warmth, heat pain, and deep cold pain. J Neurophysiol 1996;76:571– 81. Chudler EH, Anton F, Dubner R, Kenshalo DR Jr. Responses of nociceptive SI neurons in monkeys and pain sensation in humans elicited by noxious thermal stimulation: effect of interstimulus interval. J Neurophysiol 1990;63:559 – 69. Coghill RC, Sang CN, Maisog JM, Iadarola MJ. Pain intensity processing within the human brain: a bilateral, distributed mechanism. J Neurophysiol 1999;82:1934 – 43. Coghill RC, Talbot JD, Evans AC, et al. Distributed processing of pain and vibration by the human brain. J Neurosci 1994;14:4095–108. Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Dobmeyer S, Shulman GL, Petersen SE. Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography. J Neurosci 1991;11:2383– 402. Craig AD. Supraspinal projections of lamina I neurons. In: Besson JM, Gilbaud G, Ollat H, eds. Forebrain areas involved in pain processing. Paris: Libbey, 1995:13–25. Craig AD. Pain, temperature, and the sense of the body. In: Franzén O, Johansson R, Terenius L, eds. Somesthesis and the neurobiology of the somatosensory cortex. Basel: Birkhäuser, 1996:27–39. Craig AD. A new version of the thalamic disinhibition hypothesis of central pain. Pain Forum 1998;7:1–14. Craig AD, Bushnell MC, Zhang ET, Blomqvist A. A thalamic nucleus specific for pain and temperature sensation. Nature 1994;372: 770 –3. Craig AD, Dostrovsky JO. Medulla to thalamus. In: Wall PD, Melzack R, eds. Textbook of pain. New York: Churchill Livingstone. 1999:183–214. Craig AD, Reiman EM, Evans A, Bushnell MC. Functional imaging of an illusion of pain. Nature 1996;384:258 – 60. Davis KD, Kwan CL, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ. Functional MRI study of thalamic and cortical activations evoked by cutaneous heat, cold, and tactile stimuli. J Neurophysiol 1998;80:1533– 46. Davis KD, Taylor SJ, Crawley AP, Wood ML, Mikulis DJ. Functional MRI of pain- and attention-related activations in the human cingulate cortex. J Neurophysiol 1997;77:3370 – 80. Derbyshire SW, Jones AK, Devani P, et al. Cerebral responses to pain in patients with atypical facial pain measured by positron emission tomography. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994;57:1166 –72. Derbyshire SW, Jones AK, Gyulai F, Clark S, Townsend D, Firestone LL. Pain processing during three levels of noxious stimulation produces differential patterns of central activity. Pain 1997;73: 431– 45. Derbyshire SW, Vogt BA, Jones AK. Pain and Stroop interference tasks activate separate processing modules in anterior cingulate cortex. Exp Brain Res 1998;118:52– 60. Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA. Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain 1995;118:279 –306. Dong WK, Chudler EH, Sugiyama K, Roberts VJ, Hayashi T. Somatosensory, multisensory, and task-related neurons in cortical area 7b (PF) of unanesthetized monkeys. J Neurophysiol 1994;72:542– 64. Dong WK, Ryu H, Wagman IH. Nociceptive responses of neurons in medial thalamus and their relationship to spinothalamic pathways. J Neurophysiol 1978;41:1592– 613. Dong WK, Salonen LD, Kawakami Y, Shiwaku T, Kaukoranta EM, Martin RF. Nociceptive responses of trigeminal neurons in SII-7b cortex of awake monkeys. Brain Res 1989;484:314 –24. J Clin Neurophysiol, Vol. 17, No. 6, 2000 602 A. SCHNITZLER AND M. PLONER Dostrovsky JO, Craig AD. Nociceptive neurons in primate insular cortex [abstract]. Soc Neurosci Abstr 1996;22:111. Drevets WC, Burton H, Videen TO, Snyder AZ, Simpson JR, Raichle ME. Blood flow changes in human somatosensory cortex during anticipated stimulation. Nature 1995;373:249 –52. Foltz EL, White LE. Pain. “relief” by frontal cingulotomy. J Neurosurg 1962;19:89 –100. Friedman DP, Murray EA. Thalamic connectivity of the second somatosensory area and neighboring somatosensory fields of the lateral sulcus of the macaque. J Comp Neurol 1986;252:348 –73. Friedman DP, Murray EA, O’Neill JB, Mishkin M. Cortical connections of the somatosensory fields of the lateral sulcus of macaques: evidence for a corticolimbic pathway for touch. J Comp Neurol 1986;252:323– 47. Frot M, Rambaud L, Guenot M, Mauguiere F. Intracortical recordings of early pain-related CO2-laser evoked potentials in the human second somatosensory (SII) area. Clin Neurophysiol 1999;110: 133– 45. Garraghty PE, Florence SL, Tenhula WN, Kaas JH. Parallel thalamic activation of the first and second somatosensory areas in prosimian primates and tree shrews. J Comp Neurol 1991;311:289 –99. Gelnar PA, Krauss BR, Sheehe PR, Szeverenyi NM, Apkarian AV. A comparative fMRI study of cortical representations for thermal painful, vibrotactile, and motor performance tasks. Neuroimage 1999;10:460 – 82. Gingold SI, Greenspan JD, Apkarian AV. Anatomic evidence of nociceptive inputs to primary somatosensory cortex: relationship between spinothalamic terminals and thalamocortical cells in squirrel monkeys. J Comp Neurol 1991;308:467–90. Greenspan JD, Lee RR, Lenz FA. Pain sensitivity alterations as a function of lesion location in the parasylvian cortex. Pain 1999; 81:273– 82. Greenspan JD, Winfield JA. Reversible pain and tactile deficits associated with a cerebral tumor compressing the posterior insula and parietal operculum. Pain 1992;50:29 –39. Hanamori T, Kunitake T, Kato K, Kannan H. Responses of neurons in the insular cortex to gustatory, visceral, and nociceptive stimuli in rats. J Neurophysiol 1998;79:2535– 45. Hari R, Karhu J, Hamalainen M, et al. Functional organization of the human first and second somatosensory cortices: a neuromagnetic study. Eur J Neurosci 1993;5:724 –34. Hari R, Kaukoranta E, Reinikainen K, Huopaniemie T, Mauno J. Neuromagnetic localization of cortical activity evoked by painful dental stimulation in man. Neurosci Lett 1983;42:77– 82. Hari R, Portin K, Kettenmann B, Jousmaki V, Kobal G. Righthemisphere preponderance of responses to painful CO2 stimulation of the human nasal mucosa. Pain 1997;72:145–51. Head H, Holmes G. Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain 1911;34:102–254. Hsieh JC, Belfrage M, Stone–Elander S, Hansson P, Ingvar M. Central representation of chronic ongoing neuropathic pain studied by positron emission tomography. Pain 1995;63:225–36. Hurt RW, Ballantine HTJ. Stereotactic anterior cingulate lesions for persistent pain: a report on 68 cases. Clin Neurosurg 1974;21: 334 –51. Hutchison WD, Davis KD, Lozano AM, Tasker RR, Dostrovsky JO. Pain-related neurons in the human cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci 1999;2:403–5. Huttunen J, Kobal G, Kaukoranta E, Hari R. Cortical responses to painful CO2 stimulation of nasal mucosa: a magnetoencephalographic study in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1986;64:347–9. Iadarola MJ, Berman KF, Zeffiro TA, et al. Neural activation during acute capsaicin-evoked pain and allodynia assessed with PET. Brain 1998;121:931– 47. Ito SI. Possible representation of somatic pain in the rat insular visceral sensory cortex: a field potential study. Neurosci Lett 1998;241: 171– 4. J Clin Neurophysiol, Vol. 17, No. 6, 2000 Iwamura Y. Hierarchical somatosensory processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1998;8:522– 8. Jones AK, Brown WD, Friston KJ, Qi LY, Frackowiak RS. Cortical and subcortical localization of response to pain in man using positron emission tomography. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1991a;244:39 – 44. Jones AK, Qi LY, Fujirawa T, et al. In vivo distribution of opioid receptors in man in relation to the cortical projections of the medial and lateral pain systems measured with positron emission tomography. Neurosci Lett 1991b;126:25– 8. Kakigi R, Koyama S, Hoshiyama M, Kitamura Y, Shimojo M, Watanabe S. Pain-related magnetic fields following painful CO2 laser stimulation in man. Neurosci Lett 1995;192:45– 8. Kenshalo DR Jr, Chudler EH, Anton F, Dubner R. SI nociceptive neurons participate in the encoding process by which monkeys perceive the intensity of noxious thermal stimulation. Brain Res 1988;454:378 – 82. Kenshalo DR Jr, Giesler GJ Jr, Leonard RB, Willis WD. Responses of neurons in primate ventral posterior lateral nucleus to noxious stimuli. J Neurophysiol 1980;43:1594 – 614. Kenshalo DR Jr, Isensee O. Responses of primate SI cortical neurons to noxious stimuli. J Neurophysiol 1983;50:1479 –96. Kenshalo DR, Willis WD. The role of the cerebral cortex in pain sensation. In: Peters A, Jones EG, eds. Cerebral cortex. Vol. 9. New York: Plenum Press, 1991:153–212. Koyama T, Tanaka YZ, Mikami A. Nociceptive neurons in the macaque anterior cingulate activate during anticipation of pain. Neuroreport 1998;9:2663–7. Lamour Y, Willer JC, Guilbaud G. Rat somatosensory (SmI) cortex: I. Characteristics of neuronal responses to noxious stimulation and comparison with responses to non-noxious stimulation. Exp Brain Res 1983;49:35– 45. Ledberg A, O’Sullivan BT, Kinomura S, Roland PE. Somatosensory activations of the parietal operculum of man. A PET study. Eur J Neurosci 1995;7:1934 – 41. Lenz FA, Gracely RH, Zirh AT, Romanoski AJ, Dougherty PM. The sensory–limbic model of pain memory. Pain Forum 1997;6:22– 31. Lenz FA, Rios M, Zirh A, Chau D, Krauss G, Lesser RP. Painful stimuli evoke potentials recorded over the human anterior cingulate gyrus. J Neurophysiol 1998;79:2231– 4. Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ. The insula of Reil in man and monkey. In: Peters A, Jones EG, eds. Cerebral cortex. Vol. 4. New York: Plenum Press, 1985:179 –226. Mishkin M. Analogous neural models for tactual and visual learning. Neuropsychologia 1979;17:139 –51. Mountcastle VB, Powell TPS. Neural mechanisms subserving cutaneous sensibility, with special reference to the role of afferent inhibition in sensory perception and discrimination. Bull Johns Hopkins Hosp 1959;105:201–32. Pardo JV, Pardo PJ, Janer KW, Raichle ME. The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1990;87:256 –9. Penfield W, Boldrey E. Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain 1937;60:389 – 443. Penfield W, Jasper H. Epilepsy and the functional anatomy of the human brain. Boston: Little, Brown and Company, 1954. Petersen SE, Fox PT, Posner MI, Mintun M, Raichle ME. Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of singleword processing. Nature 1988;331:585–9. Ploner M, Freund H-J, Schnitzler A. Pain affect without pain sensation in a patient with a postcentral lesion. Pain 1999a;81:211– 4. Ploner M, Schmitz F, Freund H-J, Schnitzler A. Parallel activation of primary and secondary somatosensory cortices in human pain processing. J Neurophysiol 1999b;81:3100 – 4. Ploner M, Schmitz F, Freund H-J, Schnitzler A. Differential organiza- PAIN PERCEPTION tion of touch and pain in human primary somatosensory cortex. J Neurophysiol 2000;83:1770 – 6. Porro CA, Cettolo V, Francescato MP, Baraldi P. Temporal and intensity coding of pain in human cortex. J Neurophysiol 1998; 80:3312–20. Rainville P, Duncan GH, Price DD, Carrier B, Bushnell MC. Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science 1997;277:968 –71. Raja SN, Meyer RA, Ringkamp M, Campbell JN. Peripheral neural mechanisms of nociception. In: Wall PD, Melzack R, eds. Textbook of pain. New York: Churchill Livingstone. 1999:11–57. Robinson CJ, Burton H. Somatic submodality distribution within the second somatosensory (SII), 7b, retroinsular, postauditory, and granular insular cortical areas of M. fascicularis. J Comp Neurol 1980;192:93–108. Schnitzler A, Volkmann J, Enck P, Frieling T, Witte OW, Freund H-J. Different cortical organization of visceral and somatic sensation in humans. Eur J Neurosci 1999;11:305–15. Shi CJ, Cassell MD. Cascade projections from somatosensory cortex to the rat basolateral amygdala via the parietal insular cortex. J Comp Neurol 1998a;399:469 –91. Shi CJ, Cassell MD. Cortical, thalamic, and amygdaloid connections of the anterior and posterior insular cortices. J Comp Neurol 1998b; 399:440 – 68. Sikes RW, Vogt BA. Nociceptive neurons in area 24 of rabbit cingulate cortex. J Neurophysiol 1992;68:1720 –32. Silverman DH, Munakata JA, Ennes H, Mandelkern MA, Hoh CK, Mayer EA. Regional cerebral activity in normal and pathological perception of visceral pain. Gastroenterology 1997;112:64 –72. Sinclair RJ, Burton H. Neuronal activity in the second somatosensory cortex of monkeys (Macaca mulatta) during active touch of gratings. J Neurophysiol 1993;70:331–50. Spiegel J, Hansen C, Treede RD. Laser-evoked potentials after painful hand and foot stimulation in humans: evidence for generation of the middle-latency component in the secondary somatosensory cortex. Neurosci Lett 1996;216:179 – 82. Stevens RT, London SM, Apkarian AV. Spinothalamocortical projections to the secondary somatosensory cortex (SII) in squirrel monkey. Brain Res 1993;631:241– 6. Sweet WH. Cerebral localization of pain. In: Thompson RA, Green JR, eds. New perspectives in cerebral localization. New York: Raven Press, 1982:205– 42. Talbot JD, Marrett S, Evans AC, Meyer E, Bushnell MC, Duncan GH. Multiple representations of pain in human cerebral cortex. Science 1991;251:1355– 8. Tarkka IM, Treede RD. Equivalent electrical source analysis of painrelated somatosensory evoked potentials elicited by a CO2 laser. J Clin Neurophysiol 1993;10:513–9. Tolle TR, Kaufmann T, Siessmeier T, et al. Region-specific encoding of sensory and affective components of pain in the human brain: 603 a positron emission tomography correlation analysis. Ann Neurol 1999;45:40 –7. Tommerdahl M, Delemos KA, Favorov OV, Metz CB, Vierck CJ Jr, Whitsel BL. Response of anterior parietal cortex to different modes of same-site skin stimulation. J Neurophysiol 1998;80: 3272– 83. Tommerdahl M, Delemos KA, Vierck CJ Jr, Favorov OV, Whitsel BL. Anterior parietal cortical response to tactile and skin-heating stimuli applied to the same skin site. J Neurophysiol 1996;75: 2662–70. Turman AB, Ferrington DG, Ghosh S, Morley JW, Rowe MJ. Parallel processing of tactile information in the cerebral cortex of the cat: effect of reversible inactivation of SI on responsiveness of SII neurons. J Neurophysiol 1992;67:411–29. Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M. Two cortical visual systems. In: Ingle DG, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJW, eds. Analysis of visual behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1982:549 – 86. Vaccarino AL, Melzack R. Analgesia produced by injection of lidocaine into the anterior cingulum bundle of the rat. Pain 1989;39: 213–9. Valeriani M, Restuccia D, Barba C, Le Pera D, Tonali P, Mauguiere F. Sources of cortical responses to painful CO(2) laser skin stimulation of the hand and foot in the human brain. Clin Neurophysiol 2000;111:1103–12. Vogt BA, Derbyshire S, Jones AK. Pain processing in four regions of human cingulate cortex localized with co-registered PET and MR imaging. Eur J Neurosci 1996;8:1461–73. Vogt BA, Finch DM, Olson CR. Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: the anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. Cereb Cortex 1992;2:435– 43. Vogt BA, Pandya DN, Rosene DL. Cingulate cortex of the rhesus monkey: I. Cytoarchitecture and thalamic afferents. J Comp Neurol 1987;262:256 –70. Vogt BA, Rosene DL, Pandya DN. Thalamic and cortical afferents differentiate anterior from posterior cingulate cortex in the monkey. Science 1979;204:205–7. Vogt BA, Watanabe H, Grootoonk S, Jones AKP. Topography of diprenorphine binding in human cingulate gyrus and adjacent cortex derived from coregistered PET and MR images. Hum Brain Map 1995;3:1–12. Whitsel BL, Petrucelli LM, Werner G. Symmetry and connectivity in the map of the body surface in somatosensory area II of primates. J Neurophysiol 1969;32:170 – 83. Yamamura H, Iwata K, Tsuboi Y, et al. Morphological and electrophysiological properties of ACCx nociceptive neurons in rats. Brain Res 1996;735:83–92. Zhang ZH, Dougherty PM, Oppenheimer SM. Monkey insular cortex neurons respond to baroreceptive and somatosensory convergent inputs. Neuroscience 1999;94:351– 60. J Clin Neurophysiol, Vol. 17, No. 6, 2000