ARTICLE IN PRESS Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (2008) 122–129 www.elsevier.com/locate/brat Shorter communication Acceptance versus distraction: Brief instructions, metaphors and exercises in increasing tolerance for self-delivered electric shocks Jenny McMullena, Dermot Barnes-Holmesa, Yvonne Barnes-Holmesa, Ian Stewartb, Carmen Lucianoc, Andy Cochranea, a Department of Psychology, National University of Ireland, John Hume Building, Maynooth, Ireland b Department of Psychology, National University of Ireland, Galway, Ireland c University of Almeria, Almeria, Spain Received 18 April 2007; received in revised form 31 August 2007; accepted 10 September 2007 Abstract The current study compared the effects of an acceptance versus distraction rationale on coping with experimentally induced pain. Eighty participants were randomly assigned to one of five conditions: Full-Acceptance, Full-Distraction, Instruction-only-Acceptance, Instruction-only-Distraction and No-Instructions. Participants completed a simple matching task and were intermittently given the choice either to receive an electric shock and continue, or to avoid the shock and terminate the task. Only the Full-Acceptance strategy (that included experiential exercises and a metaphor) had a significant effect on task tolerance as measured by an increase in the number of shocks delivered post-intervention relative to baseline. In addition, the participants in both of the acceptance conditions showed lower levels of believability in that they were more likely to continue with the task even when reporting more pain. The results support the prediction that acceptance-based interventions work by undermining the behavioural-control functions of pain-related thoughts and feelings, and call for a systematic analysis of how metaphors and exercises work in analogue research. r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Experimental pain; Acceptance; Distraction; Instruction following; Electric shocks Introduction There are now a number of published analogue studies that have demonstrated that acceptance-based approaches lead to greater tolerance for experimentally induced physical stressors relative to strategies that focus on controlling the associated distress (McCracken, Vowles, & Eccleston, 2004). An early study examined the behavioural (task persistence) and subjective (self-report) effects of a control- versus an acceptance-based protocol, using a cold-pressor task (Hayes et al., 1999). The findings demonstrated that the acceptance rationale led to greater task persistence relative to the control protocol. Additionally, the participants in the Corresponding author. Tel.: +353 1 708 4765; fax: +353 1 708 4767. E-mail address: cochrane.andy@gmail.com (A. Cochrane). 0005-7967/$ - see front matter r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.09.002 ARTICLE IN PRESS J. McMullen et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (2008) 122–129 123 acceptance condition reported reduced believability of thoughts and feelings as reasons for discontinuing the task.1 Subsequent analogue studies have also demonstrated that acceptance appears to increase tolerance to aversive stimulation relative to cognitive control strategies (Gutiérrez, Luciano, Rodriguez, & Fink, 2004; Masedo & Esteve, 2007; Páez-Blarrina et al., in press). In one analogue study, however, neither acceptance nor control-based coping produced an increase in cold-pressor tolerance (Keogh, Bond, Hanmer, & Tilston, 2005). Unlike the other studies, however, the Keogh et al.’s analogue ‘‘interventions’’ involved relatively brief instructions with no experiential exercises or metaphors. To date, no attempt has been made to examine the effects of the method by which an acceptance- versus control-based intervention is delivered, that is, by instructions alone or in combination with the use of metaphor and exercises. The current study examined two methods of delivery: (1) a brief and simple instruction for accepting or distracting and (2) an instruction for accepting or distracting combined with a relevant experiential exercise and metaphor. The study compared the effects of each protocol on tolerance and self-reported pain produced by electric shock stimulation, using procedures derived from Gutiérrez et al. (2004). The interventions were presented as digitised video-clips, as part of a participant-controlled computer program, which aimed to reduce any inadvertent experimenter cuing. We predicted that only the acceptance condition with an exercise and metaphor would increase tolerance for self-delivered shocks. We also predicted that if acceptance increased tolerance this would occur primarily through a reduction in the believability of pain-related thoughts and feelings as a basis for discontinuing the aversive task. Method Participants A total of 97 individuals agreed to participate, with a total of 80 individuals completing the study. The 17 participants who did not complete failed to meet various criteria for inclusion. All participants were recruited through word of mouth from students and ex-students of the National University of Ireland, Maynooth and University College, Dublin (46 female and 34 male) with a mean age of 28.5 (SE ¼ 7.03). Participants with a history of chronic pain, epilepsy or a medical condition that could be adversely affected by participation (e.g. metal joint replacement) were excluded. Participants were randomly assigned to each of the five conditions, after controlling for age, gender and number of shocks delivered during the first shock task (described below), until each condition contained 16 individuals. The participants did not receive any remuneration for their participation, and the study was conducted in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the Department of Psychology at the National University of Ireland, Maynooth. Materials and apparatus A portable personal computer was used to deliver the experimental phases including calibration of aversive stimulation, the two shock tasks, and the therapeutic interventions. A Lafayette 824151S isolated square wave stimulator (ISWS) was used to provide electrical stimulation via electrodes attached to the palmar surface of the forearm. Experimental sessions were conducted in a quiet room free from distraction. Believability measure Consistent with Gutiérrez et al. (2004), the believability of increasing levels of self-reported pain as a reason to stop the task was assessed by examining the relationship between this rating and shock tolerance. Specifically, this was measured by calculating the number of participants who reported increased pain levels post-intervention, relative to the first task, but still increased the number of self-delivered shocks received. The term believability is thus used here simply to denote a response-discordance between the rating of pain and shock tolerance. 1 The term ‘‘acceptance’’ embraces multiple interpretations, but for the purpose of the current study we defined acceptance as a technique that reduces the behavioural control functions of targeted thoughts and feelings by disconnecting them from overt behaviour. ARTICLE IN PRESS 124 J. McMullen et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (2008) 122–129 Procedure Participants were assigned to one of five conditions. The Full-Acceptance (F-ACC) and Full-Distraction (F-DIS) conditions contained instructions to accept or distract combined with a relevant experiential exercise and metaphor. The Instruction-based-Acceptance (I-ACC) and Instruction-based-Distraction (I-DIS) conditions contained brief instructions only (full scripts available from the second author on request). Finally, a no instruction group (No-I) was also included in which no strategy was provided. All participants received the same general instructions, and were told that the study involved the presentation of brief electric shocks that would be unpleasant but innocuous. They were also informed that they were free to discontinue the experiment at any stage. Participants were seated in front of the computer to start the calibration phase. Calibration of aversive stimulation The purpose of this phase was to determine a starting voltage for the subsequent two shock tasks. Two 10 mm diameter sensor electrodes were attached 2 cm apart on the palmar surface of a participant’s nondominant forearm. Participants wore headphones to amplify appropriate auditory stimuli and eliminate any distractions. The ISWS was located behind the participants so that they could not observe any manipulation. The experimenter sat behind the participant during this phase and subsequent shock tasks so that she could observe responses and deliver the shocks on schedule. On-screen instructions informed the participants that electric shocks of increasing intensity would be delivered to determine the voltage of shocks that would be used during subsequent stages of the experiment. The instructions reiterated that the shocks would be unpleasant but harmless and the participants were reminded that they could choose to terminate the experiment at any time. Participants used the mouse to click on an on-screen button to indicate their readiness to start, and then they were exposed to a series of shocks that gradually increased in voltage. Before each shock two buttons appeared on the screen, giving the participants the choice to either ‘‘Click here to end the experiment’’ or ‘‘Click here to receive a shock and continue.’’ If the participants chose to continue, a brief shock was delivered and they were asked to rate the painfulness of the shock on a Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) from 0 (no pain) to 100 (pain as bad as it could be). Once the participant had rated a shock at 50 or above, the calibration phase ended, and the voltage of the last shock received was set as the voltage level used for the MTS shock task, which followed immediately. First MTS shock task This phase was identical for all five conditions of the experiment. The participants were asked to follow the on-screen instructions and undertake a simple matching task. On each trial of the MTS, a single digit number appeared in a box at the top of the screen as a sample stimulus, and three single-digit numbers in similar boxes were presented along the bottom of the screen as comparisons. The participants selected the matching comparison by clicking on it with the mouse. A correct response was given feedback in the form of the word ‘correct’ appearing in the centre of the screen accompanied by an audible beep. An incorrect response produced the word ‘wrong’ without any sound. Correct responses were reinforced with the delivery of points on a VR 9 schedule. These points appeared in the top right corner of the screen. After approximately every 11th trial, the screen cleared and a red cross was presented along with two onscreen buttons. This configuration indicated that the participants now had the option to receive a shock and continue or avoid the shock and thereby end the task. The button in the left corner read ‘Click here to end’ while that on the right read ‘Click here to receive a shock and continue with the task’. If the participants chose to end the task they proceeded to the next stage of the experiment. If they chose to continue, the screen cleared for 1 s and they then received a shock. The participants were prompted to rate the painfulness of the shock using the VAS and then to click on a button at the bottom of the screen to continue with the task. The experimenter, according to a standardised schedule, gradually increased the frequency and duration of the shocks (protocol available from the authors); the voltage remained at the level set during the calibration stage. Unbeknown to the participants there was a preset maximum of 15 shocks during this phase. Participants who received all 15 were excluded from the remainder of the study and were thanked and ARTICLE IN PRESS J. McMullen et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (2008) 122–129 125 debriefed. Participants who opted to end the shock task before receiving this maximum proceeded to the intervention phase. Full acceptance and distraction interventions and second MTS shock task The participants were given a brief break of no more than 5-min before beginning the next stage of the experiment. Upon starting, they were asked to follow the on-screen instructions, and the experimenter left the room and remained outside until this phase was completed. Participants in both conditions were instructed to watch a series of video clips in a particular order; they were told that they could replay any clip by re-clicking on the on-screen button corresponding to that clip. At the end of each clip, participants were asked to write down a brief summary of what they had just been told and what they did. The video clips for the two full interventions differed in terms of content (i.e., acceptance versus distraction) but were closely matched in terms of duration, word-count and non-intervention-specific instructions (e.g., all participants were informed at regular intervals that the strategy they had been given would help them continue with the shock task). The first clip was identical for both the F-ACC and F-DIS conditions. Participants were first asked to write down three thoughts that they had when they decided to end the previous shock task. The second clip involved an experiential exercise, which differed across the two conditions. All of the participants were asked to open an envelope, which had been left beside the computer. The F-ACC group then removed a piece of paper on which was written ‘I cannot walk’. The participants were asked to repeat this phrase aloud while walking once around the room. The aim of the exercise was to teach the participants to disconnect what they say to themselves from what they actually do. In the F-DIS condition the participants found a blank piece of paper inside the envelope and were asked to imagine a pleasant scene on it. They were then asked to pick up one of the pieces of paper on which one of their own thoughts was written, and to repeat this thought aloud while walking once around the room. They were asked to distract themselves from the thought by imagining the pleasant scene. The aim of this version of the exercise was to teach the participants to distract themselves from shock-related thoughts and feelings. The third clip, identical across conditions, asked participants to summarise the exercise they had been given in Clip 2. In the fourth clip, the F-ACC group were asked to consider that during the next task they could notice shock-related thoughts and feelings, but continue with the task, regardless of the content of those thoughts. The ‘walk round the room’ exercise was used as an example of a possible disconnection between thoughts and behaviour. The F-DIS group were asked to consider that during the next task, they could distract themselves from the shock-related thoughts and feelings, and continue with the task, by imagining their pleasant scene, as they had during the ‘walk round the room’ exercise. In effect, both groups were encouraged to continue with the shock task, but the F-ACC group were asked to do so regardless of their thoughts about the shocks whereas the F-DIS group were asked to employ a distraction strategy to cope with the shock-related thoughts in order to continue. The fifth clip introduced a metaphor relevant to the particular intervention. Both groups were asked to imagine that continuing with the shock task was comparable to crossing a muddy swamp. The F-ACC group were told that the best way to cross the swamp was just to notice any unpleasant thoughts and feelings and carry them with them as they continued to the other side of the swamp, because they could have those thoughts and act differently to what they thought or felt. The F-DIS group were told that the best way to get across the swamp was to think of more pleasant images while continuing to cross the swamp, because removing the unpleasant thoughts and replacing them with more pleasant and positive things would help them to continue with the shock task. The sixth and seventh clips provided the participants with a rationale for the study. Both groups were told that the aim of the study was to help people suffering from chronic disabling pain and therefore it was important that they continue with the next shock task for as long as possible. The experimental task was compared to a real life situation in which they suffered from chronic pain. In this imaginary scenario, the matching task was a boring job that they had to do in order to earn money to support their family; the electric shocks were like bouts of chronic pain that had to be endured in order to do the job. The eighth video clip informed participants that if they did not wish to replay any video clip they should report to the experimenter. ARTICLE IN PRESS 126 J. McMullen et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (2008) 122–129 The second MTS shock task was identical to the first for the F-ACC and F-DIS groups, except for the inclusion of video-based reminders of the relevant intervention. The reminder video-clips differed in content (i.e. either acceptance- or distraction-based), but were matched for word count and duration (approximately 52 s). The video-clips appeared in the top left corner of the screen at the same time as the red cross. The participants could replay the clip if they wished and then continue the task as before by choosing whether or not to receive the next shock. Instruction-based acceptance and distraction interventions and second MTS pain task Participants assigned to these conditions were asked to remain in the room after the first shock task for a short break. They were provided with magazines to read, while the experimenter remained outside. After 30 min had elapsed (i.e., duration matched with the ‘‘Full’’ intervention groups) the experimenter re-entered the room and gave the participants written instructions explaining the next stage of the experiment. Participants were informed that during the next shock task a video clip would appear on screen at the same time as the red cross. The instructions on the video clip would advise them of the best way to cope with the shocks and they were asked to use this information as best as they could. The second shock task was identical to the first task except for the inclusion of the video-based interventions. The video clips were presented in the top left corner of the screen alongside the red cross. The clips for the two interventions were again matched for word count, duration (approximately 30 s) and nonintervention-specific information. The message conveyed in the I-ACC condition informed the participants that the best way to deal with the task was to accept that the shock was going to hurt and to simply notice the pain and continue with the task as best as possible. The I-DIS participants were informed that the best way to deal with the task was to distract from the pain and continue with the task as best as possible. After the video clip had been played the task continued as described for the first shock task, with the participants choosing whether to receive the next shock or discontinue the experiment. No-instructions condition and second MTS pain task Participants in this condition remained in the room for 30 min after the first task. They were provided with some magazines to read. After 30 min had elapsed the participants started the second shock task that was procedurally identical to the first shock task. Final measures When the participants chose to discontinue the second shock task (or reached the maximum of 15 shocks), the headphones and electrodes were removed. All participants (except No-I group) were asked to rate on an 11-point scale how useful (0 ¼ not useful 10 ¼ very useful) the assigned strategy was, and how difficult (0 ¼ very easy 10 ¼ very difficult) it was to use during the shock task. They were also asked to summarise the strategy in their own words. Results The data met the assumptions of normal distribution (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test) and thus parametric tests were employed in subsequent analyses. Pre-Intervention group differences The means (+SE) are presented in Table 1. Three one-way ANOVAs confirmed that the five groups did not differ significantly in terms of age, number of shocks received and pain ratings during the first MTS task (all p’s4.09). Shock tolerance The means (+SE) for the number of shocks delivered in both tasks are presented in Table 1. The F-ACC group showed the largest and the No-I group the smallest increase in number of shocks received from task 1 to ARTICLE IN PRESS J. McMullen et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (2008) 122–129 127 Table 1 Mean (+SE) number of shocks delivered and subjective pain ratings Age First task Shocks Pain rating Second task Shocks Pain rating F-ACC F-DIS I-ACC I-DIS No-I 29.69 (2.13) 27.13 (.61) 29.06 (2.13) 27.69 (1.84) 28.81 (1.78) 5.44 (.99) 48.67 (6.369) 3.56 (.94) 53.18 (7.618) 3.44 (.758) 41.86 (7.294) 4.75 (.86) 52.37 (.8.263) 2.31 (.72) 46.34 (8.425) 8.38 (1.13) 42.60 (6.19) 4.69 (1.14) 51.64 (7.96) 3.75 (.77) 43.64 (6.05) 2.56 (.73) 55.09 (8.02) 2.50 (.88) 41.00 (8.85) Note: Pain rating: 0 ¼ no pain, 100 ¼ pain as bad as it could be. Table 2 Means (+SE) for ratings of usefulness and difficulty of assigned strategy F-ACC F-DIS I-ACC I-DIS Usefulness Difficulty 4.40 3.75 3.38 3.81 5.87 5.38 4.75 5.75 (.74) (.65) (.43) (.51) (.78) (.79) (.67) (.57) Note: Useful: 0 ¼ not useful, 10 ¼ very useful; Difficulty: 0 ¼ very easy, 10 ¼ very difficult. task 2, with only the I-DIS group showing a decrease. A 2 5 mixed repeated measures ANOVA was conducted with the time of testing (first versus second shock task) as the within-participant variable and the five conditions as the between-participant variable. The ANOVA yielded a significant main effect for condition (F (4, 75) ¼ 4.089, p ¼ .0047, Z2p ¼ .18), and an interaction between condition and time of testing (F (4, 75) ¼ 7.165, po.0001, Z2p ¼ .27). Given the significant interaction, separate repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted for each of the five conditions. The ANOVAs indicated that tolerance increased significantly for F-ACC (F (1, 15) ¼ 14.48, p ¼ .0017, Z2p ¼ .49), and decreased significantly for I-DIS (F(1, 15) ¼ 5.618, p ¼ .032, Z2p ¼ .026). None of the other conditions yielded a significant change in tolerance (all p’s4.1). The F-ACC effect remained significant (i.e., po.01) after a Bonferroni correction, but I-DIS did not. VAS pain and believability The mean VAS pain ratings associated with the shocks during the first and second tasks are presented in Table 2. The mean ratings given by the F-ACC, F-DIS and No-I groups were lower overall (i.e. less pain) for the second relative to the first shock task, whereas the ratings given by the two instruction only conditions increased. A 2 5 mixed repeated measures ANOVA yielded no significant effects (all p’s4.4). The relationship between the self-reported pain ratings and shock tolerance was used as a behavioural measure of the believability of pain. Insofar as the acceptance intervention served to disconnect thoughts and feelings from action, it was predicted that participants would increase the number of self-delivered shocks even if they reported increased pain levels relative to the first task. Across the five conditions, the ratios of participants who self-delivered more shocks even though they reported more pain were as follows: F-ACC, 6/6 participants; F-DIS, 2/4; I-ACC, 7/9; I-DIS, 2/7; No-I, 0/5. To analyse the differential effects of the interventions, the two acceptance groups were combined, as were the two distraction groups. The relation between self-reported pain ratings and shock tolerance was statistically significant (w2 (1, 26), 7.095, p ¼ .008). In other words, a greater percentage of participants in the two acceptance conditions who reported an increase in level of pain from pre- to post-intervention, also increased the number of self-delivered shocks. ARTICLE IN PRESS 128 J. McMullen et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (2008) 122–129 Adherence measures The participants in the four intervention groups provided a written summary of the assigned strategy at the end of the experiment. Two independent raters were asked to categorise these summaries as being consistent with either acceptance- or distraction-based strategies. All the participants in the two acceptance groups were categorised as reporting an acceptance strategy, and all participants in the two distraction groups were categorised as reporting a distraction strategy. Finally, the participants in the four intervention groups were asked to rate their assigned strategy in terms of usefulness and difficulty. No obvious differences emerged (Table 2). Two one-way ANOVAs indicated that participants’ reports of usefulness and difficulty did not differ significantly across the four interventions: Usefulness (F (3, 59) ¼ .505, p ¼ .7); Difficulty (F (3, 59) ¼ .507, p ¼ .7). Discussion The main findings of the current study are consistent with earlier research (e.g., Gutiérrez et al., 2004; Hayes et al., 1999) in that only the F-ACC participants showed a significant increase in the number of self-delivered shocks during the second task. Furthermore, the data indicate that participants from both of the acceptance conditions showed lower levels of believability relative to the other groups. The F-DIS condition also improved pain tolerance (but not significantly), whereas I-DIS reduced tolerance (but again non-significantly when correcting for multiple tests). Interestingly, the No-I condition produced no effect in terms of tolerance, which therefore suggests that the I-DIS intervention may have had a negative effect. Strictly speaking, of course, neither distraction condition produced a significant effect, unlike the F-ACC condition, which is consistent with previous research and current experimental predictions. The five groups in the current study did not differ in terms of self-reported pain levels. In contrast, Gutiérrez et al. (2004) reported a significant reduction in self-reported pain for the Distraction group. It is unclear why this disparity emerged, but it may be related to procedural differences. For example, a possibly important difference is that the protocols were implemented using video-clips in the current study, whereas Gutiérrez et al. involved direct social interaction with a researcher–therapist. Interestingly, earlier research employing the same video-based interventions as the current study also failed to find any changes in pain reports across the groups (Johnson et al., 2004). The present study is the first to show that experiential exercises and metaphors play a critical role in the effectiveness of experimental analogues of acceptance-based interventions. Of course, the current findings are restricted to a relatively artificial pain-induction task and a relatively small, non-clinical sample, but the results do call for a careful and systematic analysis of how exercises and metaphors work in future analogue research. Acknowledgements The preparation of this paper was supported by the Irish Research Council for the Humanities and Social Sciences through a Government of Ireland Scholarship to the last author. References Gutiérrez, O., Luciano, C., Rodriguez, M., & Fink, B. C. (2004). Comparison between an acceptance-based and a cognitive-control-based protocol for coping with pain. Behaviour Therapy, 35, 767–783. Hayes, S. C., Bisset, R., Korn, Z., Zettle, R. D., Rosenfarb, I., Cooper, L. I., et al. (1999). The impact of acceptance versus control rationales on pain tolerance. Psychological Record, 49, 33–47. Johnson, B., Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Luciano, C., Wilson, K. (2004). Doing what you do not want to do to achieve valued outcomes: Acceptance and valuing in the face of aversive stimulation. Paper presented at the symposium: Behavior therapy: Process oriented experimental analyses, held at the annual conference of the Psychological Society of Ireland, Cork Ireland, November 2004. Keogh, E., Bond, F. W., Hanmer, R., & Tilston, J. (2005). Comparing acceptance- and control-based coping instruction on the coldpressor pain experiences of healthy men and women. European Journal of Pain, 9(5), 591–598. ARTICLE IN PRESS J. McMullen et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (2008) 122–129 129 Masedo, A. I., & Esteve, M. R. (2007). Effects of suppression, acceptance and spontaneous coping on pain tolerance, pain intensity and distress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 199–209. McCracken, L. M., Vowles, L. M., & Eccleston, C. (2004). Acceptance of chronic pain: component analysis and a revised assessment method. Pain, 107, 159–166. Páez-Blarrina, M., Luciano, C., Gutiérrez-Martı́nez, O., Valdivia, S., Rodrı́guez, M., Ortega, J. (in press). Coping with pain in the motivational context of values: Comparison between an acceptance-based and a cognitive-control-based protocol. Behavior Modifcation.