JASH 2000, Vol. 25, No.1, 18-28 copyright 2000 by The Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps Increasing Early Social-Communicative Skills of Preverbal Preschool Children With Autism Through Social Interactive Training Bogseon Hwang Ewha Woman's University Carolyn Hughes Vanderbilt University We evaluated the effects of a social interactive training program on early social-communicative skills (i.e., eye contact, joint attention, motor imitation) of three preverbal preschool children with autism. Training was conducted in the children's classroom during naturally occurring daily activities. Findings indicated that implementation of social interactive training components (i.e., contingent imitation, use of naturally occurring reinforcement, expectant look, environmental arrangement) was associated with increases in target behaviors across participants. In addition, generalization of eye contact and motor imitation behavior were found across an additional setting and different interactive partners, although joint attention rarely generalized. Social validation measures provided by 30 raters indicated that perceived positive changes in performance had occurred in child behaviors and that training procedures were considered acceptable. Results are discussed in terms of implications for research and practice. interpersonal interchange (Klinger & Dawson, 1992). Specifically, the exchange of early mutual gaze provides an opportunity for reciprocal interaction and turn taking experiences between mothers and infants (Stern, 1985). Nonverbal joint attention serves as a foundation for the subsequent development of referential communication and language skills (Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1990). Motor imitation functions as an aspect of interpersonal interaction, which provides a means of communicating and expressing a shared understanding with a partner (Rogers & Pennington, 1991). In addition, the imitative aspects of early mother-child play increase children's imitative responses, maintain mother-child interactions, and facilitate the development of attachment (Uzgiris, 1981). Children with autism, however, typically experience profound delays in these early social-communicative behaviors - the simplest reciprocal interactions that normally develop in the first few years of life (Zager, 1999). They use less eye contact or eye-to-face gaze with their interactive partners than do their counterparts without autism (Baker, Koegel, & Koegel, 1998; Phillips, Baron-Cohen, & Rutter, 1992). In addition, many rarely use nonverbal gestural joint attention or indicating behaviors to coordinate attention toward objects or events of interest with their caregivers (Sigman, Mundy, Sherman, & Ungerer, 1986) and other individuals (McEvoy, Rogers, & Pennington, 1993). Further, many children diagnosed with autism often display specific deficits in imitation of simple motor movements, such as clapping their hands, playing "patty cake,"or waving goodby (Stone, Ousley, & Littleford, 1997). These reports suggest that delays in social and communicative functioning are a distinguishing characteristic of the early development of children with autism. Given the importance of early social-communicative skills that have been associated with social reciprocity and communicative functioning, several early language DESCRIPTORS: autism, preschool children, socialcommunicative skills, social interactive training Over the past two decades, numerous studies have emphasized the importance of early development in social-communicative skills as a necessary foundation for subsequent development in social and communicative functioning (e.g., Guralnick, Connor, Hammond, Gottman, & Kinnish, 1996). Eye contact, joint attention, and motor imitation are regarded as early social-communicative skills that enable young children to engage in Supported in part by funding from the U.S. Department of Education, Office of Special Education and Rehabilitative Services (Student-Initiated Grant), and from a grant from the John Merck Foundation. Address all correspondence and requests for reprints to Dr. Bogseon Hwang, Department of Special Education, Ewha Woman's University, 11-1 Daehyun-Dong, Seodaemun-Gu, Seoul, South Korea 120-750. E-mail: bshwang@mm.ewha. ac.kr 18 19 Early Social-Communicative Skills intervention programs have been developed to address the interrelationship between social interaction and language development in children (e.g., Hepting & Goldstein, 1996; Klinger & Dawson, 1992; Koegel, 1995; McGee, Morrier, & Daly, 1999). Social interactive training programs incorporate naturally occurring patterns of early social interactions, which are developmentally appropriate and allow a child to take the active role of initiator in interactions. Interactive teaching techniques are used, such as contingent imitation, naturally occurring reinforcement, and expectant look. They provide instructor responses that are simple, predictable, and contingent on the child's initiations (Klinger & Dawson, 1992). In addition, the environment is arranged with rich materials to increase a child's interest in activities and the child's motivation to use social and communicative behaviors in meaningful ways. Recent studies indicate that social interactive strategies may promote early social and communicative skills in children with autism. Positive change has been reported in social behavior and affect, requesting assistance, greeting and social amenities, eye gaze, joint attention, and motor imitation (e.g., Charlop & Trasowech, 1991; Dawson & Galpert, 1990; Hwang & Hughes, 1995; Klinger & Dawson, 1992; Pierce & Schreibman, 1995). In addition, these studies suggest that combining social interactive strategies may increase the effectiveness of social-communicative skills training. For example, Klinger and Dawson (1992) systematically evaluated the effects of a "package" of social interactive strategies, which combined contingent imitation, naturally occurring reinforcement, expectant look, and environmental arrangement. Results indicated that the social interactive training package was effective in promoting social-communicative skills among verbal and preverbal 5-year-olds with autism, including eye contact, joint attention, and motor imitation. Pierce and Schreibman (1995) investigated a peermediated interactive procedure that combined environmental arrangement and naturally occurring reinforcement among verbal elementary school children with autism. Treatment resulted in increases in joint attention by participants that generalized across settings and peers. Little information is available, however, regarding the effectiveness of social interactive training packages among preverbal children with autism who are functioning at early developmental levels (Dawson & Galpert, 1990). In addition, limited generalization of targeted skills has been demonstrated, particularly outside clinical settings and across additional interactive partners (Pierce & Schreibman, 1995). In order to determine if changes in social interactive behavior are durable, it is important to know if behaviors learned under training conditions generalize to children's interactions with the important others present in their daily environments (e.g., teachers, parents, peers). Therefore, further investigation of the generalized effects in everyday, nonclinical settings of social interactive training among children with autism at early developmental levels is needed. Therefore, we sought to demonstrate that early social-communicative skills of preverbal preschool children with autism could be increased through the use of social interactive strategies introduced in a natural classroom environment. The social interactive training package developed by Klinger and Dawson (1992; i.e., contingent imitation, naturally occurring reinforcement, expectant look, environmental arrangement) was adapted to promote the acquisition and generalization of early social-communicative skills among young children with autism (i.e., preverbal 3-year-olds at early developmental levels). Specifically, we sought to (a) increase the early social-communicative skills of the participants during naturally existing classroom activities and (b) promote generalization of intervention effects across an untrained situation and different interactive partners. In addition, we obtained social validation measures of perceived changes in participants' performance and appropriateness of training procedures. Method Participants Three young boys with autism who were enrolled in a university-based early intervention program for children with developmental delays participated in this study. A diagnosis of autism was made by community agencies according to criteria listed in the 4th ed. of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994). The boys (ages 32-43 months) were selected because they displayed low or nonexistent rates of eye contact, joint attention, and motor imitation, as determined by the first author through 2 months of direct observation conducted prior to taking baseline data. During observation, the children did not vocalize any meaningful words and expressed few communicative intents nonverbally and/or vocally. For example, they protested, greeted, or requested assistance only with gestures. They did not attend consistently to the classroom teacher, imitate body movements, request action or information, or make comments. Additional participant characteristics are found in Table 1. Interactive Partners Two graduate students in special education served as interactive partners for the participants during baseline and intervention sessions. During generalization sessions, two of the participants' classroom teachers who expressed interest and willingness served as interactive partners. The graduate students had at least 3 years of paid experience working with people with severe developmental disabilities and the teachers had over 5 years teaching experience with children with autism. 20 Hwang and Hughes Table 1 Participant Characteristics Participant, age, ethnicity, and gender Diagnosis" and cognitive development" Noel, 37 months, Caucasian male Autism, sensorimotor stages IV and V Expressive Receptive = 9 months 12 months Vocalized no meaningful words; occasionally showed temper tantrums that included making fists and shaking his body James, 43 months, African-American male Autism, sensorimotor stage V Expressive Receptive = = 12 months 12 months Expressed a combination of vocalization and gestures; was impulsive and easily distracted during structured and unstructured activities Louis, 32 months, Caucasian male Autism, sensorimotor stage V Expressive Receptive = = 8 months 12 months Did not yet produce any consistent word approximations; occasionally displayed arm flapping behaviors when he enjoyed playing with toys (e.g., spinning fans) Language development" = Communication and behavioral characteristics Note: Sensorimotor stages: Stage IV = 8- to 12-month age range; Stage V = 12- to 18-month age range (Uzgiris & Hunt, 1975). a Primary diagnosis of autism was identified by pediatricians or psychologists in the community. b Uzgiris-Hunt Ordinal Scales of Intellectual Development (Uzgiris & Hunt, 1975). C Sequenced Inventory of Communication Development-Revised (Hedrick, Prather, & Tobin, 1984). Settings The participants attended the early intervention program 3 days per week. All baseline and intervention sessions took place in a play area (3 m x 2 m) within the participants' classroom with one participant and one interactive partner present. Two teachers and four other children with developmental disabilities were present in another area of the classroom. Generalization sessions were conducted in a different classroom, which was used for individual and group speech therapy sessions. Only one participant and one interactive partner were present. Both settings contained a child sized table with several chairs and developmentally appropriate play materials. Tasks and Materials Partners interacted with the children using a variety of play materials, including toys (e.g., bubbles, toy fruit, wind-up toy), household objects (e.g., bowl, mirror, toothbrush), art supplies (e.g., crayon, paper, play dough), and musical instruments (e.g., drum, music box, toy piano). During baseline and generalization sessions, two to five play materials from a pool of materials typically used in early intervention classrooms were selected by the partners. The same materials were used in baseline and generalization sessions. During social interactive training, play materials were chosen from the same pool of materials based on participants' preference according to procedures recommended by Koegel, Dyer, and Bell (1987) and Cronin, Hemmeter, and Kaiser (1990). First, the partner presented (e.g., handed or demonstrated) to each child an object typically effective at facilitating social interaction (vs. parallel play) used during interactive play in early intervention classrooms. An object was considered a preferred play ma- terial if the child oriented himself toward or manipulated the object or expressed enjoyment nonverbally (e.g., eye gaze, laughing, smiling, clapping). By repeating this process, a pool of preferred play materials was identified for each child, from which items were introduced during social interactive training. If a child lost or showed no interest in a play item (e.g., the child did not play with the item or pushed it away), it was "removed and replaced with another item from the child's pool of preferred play materials. The total pool of preferred play materials was 28 for Noel, 30 for James, and 27 for Louis. Outcome Measures Three dependent measures were used to assess participants' social-communicative responses across all observation sessions: (a) frequency of eye contact, (b) frequency of joint attention, and (c) frequency of motor imitation. As a measure of fidelity of treatment, social interactive training strategies performed by partners were also measured across all observation sessions. Measures were defined as follows (coding manual available from the first author). Eye contact. The child looked at the interactive partner's eyes or face for at least 2 continuous sec without a prompt (e.g., the partner speaking the child's name or saying "look at this" or "see"; Klinger & Dawson, 1992). Eye gaze directed toward any other part of the partner's body (e.g., hands) was not included (Wetherby & Prizant, 1993). Joint attention. The child directed the attention of the partner toward an object or event (Wetherby & Prizant, 1993). Participant behaviors scored as joint attention included (a) referential looking at the interactive partner and an object (e.g., the child alternated Early Social-Communicative Skills looking at a mechanical toy and at the partner's face while manipulating the toy on the table); (b) pointing to an object within reach (e.g., when the child spilled juice, he pointed to the juice while looking at the interactive partner); and (c) showing objects to the interactive partner (e.g., the child held up a toy car and smiled while looking at the partner; Mundy et aI., 1990). Motor imitation. When the interactive partner modeled a simple familiar action with objects or body movements (e.g., the partner pushed a toy car across the table), the child imitated the action within 5 sec without prompting (Koegel et aI., 1987; Stone et aI., 1997). Motor imitation was scored as partial imitation if it approximated the partner's action or as complete imitation if the entire action was performed. Social interactive training strategies. Social interactive training was composed of four strategies implemented by partners: contingent imitation, naturally occurring reinforcement, expectant look, and environmental arrangement. See "Social interactive training" below for definitions. Experimental Design and Conditions A multiple baseline across participants design (Kazdin, 1982) was used to evaluate the effects of the social interactive training package. There were two experimental conditions: baseline and social interactive training. Ten minute observation sessions were conducted twice weekly across both experimental conditions during the 30 weeks of the study. In addition, throughout the study, a multiple probe technique (Horner & Baer, 1978) was used to collect intermittent generalization data every 2-3 weeks. Baseline. During baseline, partners interacted with the participants the same as teachers in the early intervention program typically did. However, if the participants did not interact, the partners did not prompt them to do so. During baseline, two to five play materials were available to each participant. Baseline continued until stability in level and trend of the dependent measures was obtained. Social interactive training. Following baseline, social interactive training was introduced to Noel, with training for the other two participants following sequentially. At the beginning of each training session, partners brought participants to the play area and presented play materials from each participant's pool of preferred play materials. Next, partners followed participants' leads by playing with toys in which participants indicated an interest (e.g., by facial expression or gestures). When participants lost interest in play items, they were removed and replaced with other items from the participants' pool. Social interactive training was implemented by partners as they interacted with participants and play materials. Training consisted of the following four strategies: contingent imitation of participants' actions (e.g., 21 interactions with toys) within the participants' field of vision immediately following participants' interactions (Klinger & Dawson, 1992); naturally occurring reinforcement (i.e., participants gained access to objects or events in which they demonstrated an interest by use of facial expressions, vocalizations, or gestures; Koegel & Johnson, 1989); presenting an expectant look for 5 sec before responding to participants' actions or requests (i.e., before responding to participants, partners waited and looked expectantly at participants for 5 sec (MeGee et aI., 1999; Zager, 1999); and environmental arrangement (e.g., providing materials of interest to participants, presenting two or more options to allow participants to choose, removing materials that were of no interest to the child; McGee et aI., 1999). The four strategies were implemented in response to partners' behavior rather than on the basis of a predetermined schedule. Social interactive strategies were implemented as follows. First, to promote participants' eye contact, partners imitated participants' actions using toys, body movements, and vocalizations simultaneously or as soon as possible after a participant initiated an action. Partners performed actions within view of participants in order to engage their attention (e.g., partner kept her face near toy with which she was interacting). In addition, they smiled and expressed enjoyment when participants looked at them. Second, to promote joint attention and spontaneous communication, partners arranged the environment to require participants to use gestures or sounds to draw partners' attention toward objects (e.g., partner removed a piece of a toy). To provide opportunities for participants to comment or act, partners waited and looked expectantly for at least 5 sec. In addition, partners followed participants' line of vision to look in the direction of objects or events at which participants were looking. Third, to increase the children's motor imitation, partners waited until participants were looking at them and then modeled a simple, familiar action that was already in a participant's behavioral repertoire. The action was one that a participant had performed recently (e.g., pushing a toy car, blowing bubbles, clapping hands). Fourth, partners prompted participants to respond if participants did not interact with partners within 30 sec. Generalization probes. Generalization data were collected in the speech therapy classroom during which only a participant and another partner, the child's classroom teacher, were present. Social interaction trainers were not present. As during baseline, social interactive training strategies were not implemented. The classroom teachers interacted with participants in the same manner in which they typically did during individual speech therapy sessions. Two to five play materials were chosen by the classroom teacher from the same pool of materials present in baseline rather than from the pool of preferred materials used during training. 22 Hwang and Hughes Generalization probes were taken on randomly chosen days every 2-3 weeks for a minimum of 150/0 of each participant's sessions across all experimental conditions. Partner Training During the first 2 weeks of the social interactive training condition and using a group format, partners were trained by the first author to implement the social interactive strategies. Training sessions lasted 2 hours and occurred 5 days a week. Training consisted of discussions of the social interactive strategies, role play, and in vivo training with a child not participating in the study. Following a brief discussion of the four social interactive strategies (i.e., contingent imitation, naturally occurring reinforcement, expectant look, and environmental arrangement), the first author modeled each strategy and the partners practiced using the strategy with the first author and each other. The first author provided feedback on the correctness of the partners' performance and partners were given opportunities to critique each other. During in vivo practice sessions, partners practiced until they were able to demonstrate correct independent use of the strategies on three consecutive opportunities. Observers and Observer Training Two graduate students in special education served as the primary and the reliability observers, respectively. To limit sources of observer bias, observers did not interact with participants. Prior to collecting data, both observers read and discussed the definitions of the dependent and independent measures and observation and recording procedures. They then practiced data recording procedures for each of target behaviors by watching videotapes of adult-child social interaction. Observer training continued until the observers reached a criterion of 80% interobserver agreement as calculated on a point-by-point basis (Kazdin, 1982) for both dependent and independent measures for three consecutive practice sessions before beginning baseline observations. Observation and Recording Procedures A video camera was used to record partnerparticipant interactions during each 10 minute observation session. During baseline and training, the video camera was placed in the play area. During generalization, the camera was hidden behind a one way mirror. The videotaped sessions were subsequently analyzed using the Multiple Option Observation System for Experimental Studies (MOOSES) software program (Tapp, Wehby, & Ellis, 1995). MOOSES is designed for MS-DOS compatible computers and allows for simultaneous recording of event and duration measures based in real time. Cumulative duration of event behaviors is also collected because the termination of each event is signaled by entering a new code (or a code for "no target behavior occurring"). At the onset of each observation, the observer recorded the participant's and partner's name, session number, and date. During the session, the observer recorded the occurrence of all outcome measures (i.e., participants' eye contact, joint attention, and motor imitation and partners' use of social interactive strategies). Play materials used and activities engaged in were also recorded. Interobserver Agreement Interobserver agreement on all outcome measures was assessed by having the two observers record target behaviors simultaneously, but independently, while watching the videotapes of the participants. To reduce reactivity due to the presence of a second observer, observations of the primary observer were monitored throughout the study. Interobserver agreement was taken for a minimum of 28 % of sessions per participant per experimental condition. To assess percentage agreement for all outcome measures, the point-bypoint agreement method (Kazdin, 1982) was computed by dividing the number of agreements by the number of agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 100. In addition, to examine agreement on occurrence and nonoccurrence of target behaviors corrected for chance agreement, kappa (Cohen, 1960) was calculated using the following formula: agreement on occurrences and nonoccurrences minus the expected agreement divided by the total number of agreements and disagreements minus the expected agreement. Overall mean interobserver agreement across participants on the frequency of social-communicative behaviors was 920/0 for eye contact (range: 670/0-100 %),860/0 for joint attention (range: 67 %-100 %), and 92 % for motor imitation (range: 75%-1000/0). Kappa values were .87 for eye contact (range: .63-1.0), .79 for joint attention (range: .50-1.0), and .88 for motor imitation (range: .71-1.0). Overall mean interobserver agreement on fidelity of treatment measures across partners was 900/0 for contingent imitation (range: 75 %-1000/0), 870/0 for naturally occurring reinforcement (range: 71 % 1000/0), 91% for expectant look (range: 86 %-1000/0), and 90% for environmental arrangement (range: 82%1000/0). Kappa values were .87 for contingent imitation (range: .62-1.0), .83 for naturally occurring reinforcement (range: .69-.93), .81 for expectant look (range: .73-.92), and .86 for environmental arrangement (range: .67-1.0). Social Validation To assess the social validity (Wolf, 1978) of the social interactive training package, 30 individuals were recruited by asking for volunteers to participate in a social skills study. Respondents represented three groups who had experience with children with autism (i.e., 10 special education teachers or educational assistants, 10 23 Early Social-Communicative Skills Table 2 Mean and Standard Deviation of Respondents' Ratings on Social Validation Measures Video 1: baseline Items The child frequently uses eye contact to respond to the interactive partner's social/communicative exchanges. The child frequently uses joint attention behavior to direct the interactive partner's attention toward an object or event. The child frequently imitates familiar actions with toys and body movements made by the interactive partner. The training is appropriate for facilitating the child's social-communicative skills, such as eye contact, joint attention, and motor imitation. The training and materials appear to be positive and interesting to the child. Mean score a Video 2: intervention M SD M SD 1.1a .3 4.3 .5 1.5 .6 4.4 .7 2.0 .8 4.6 .5 1.8 .6 4.6 .5 2.3 1.7 .7 .4 4.7 4.5 .5 .4 1 = strongly disagree; 2 = mildly disagree; 3 = do not feel strongly either way; 4 = mildly agree; 5 = strongly agree. parents of children with developmental disabilities, 10 graduate students in hearing and speech science or special education). To minimize bias, respondents were not informed of the purpose of the study. We developed two 3-minute videotapes, each of which was composed of randomly selected segments from each child's complete set of videotapes. One videotape contained a 1minute segment taken from the middle of a randomly selected 10-minute observation session of each child's performance during baseline. The other videotape contained a J-minute segment derived in the same fashion from observation sessions of each child's performance during training. The order of the children's performance on the baseline tape was random; however, this same order was followed for the training tape. All respondents viewed both the baseline and training videotapes; however, the order of presentation of the videotapes to respondents was randomized. The 30 respondents observed the segments and completed a 5-item, 5-point, Likert-type rating scale (1, strongly disagree; 5, strongly agree; see Table 2). Social validation data analysis included descriptive and inferential statistical tests. To summarize findings, means and standard deviations were computed for responses to the five quantitative questions. In addition, a two way split plot analysis of variance was conducted to test for significant differences among respondents' mean ratings for the two videotaped segments. To determine the extent of interjudge agreement, Kendall's coefficient of concordance (W) was calculated (Siegel, 1956). Results Changes in Social-Communicative Skills Table 3 represents the means and ranges of frequency of social-communicative behaviors displayed by participants during baseline and training conditions. The frequency of social-communicative behaviors per session during baseline and training is displayed in the upper panels for each participant in Figure 1. As shown Table 3 Mean and Range of Frequency of Social-Communicative Behaviors During Baseline and Training Conditions Eye contact Participant and condition Noel Baseline Training James Baseline Training Louis Baseline Training Joint attention Motor imitation M M M (range) (range) (range) 2.1 (0-6) 9.7 (4-25) 0 (0) 4.8 (0-12) 0 (0) 6.1 (0-14) 0.9 (0-3) 3.7 (0-8) 0.1 (0-1) 4.4 (2-9) 0.4 (0-3) 7.6 (3-14) 0.6 (0-3) 7.5 (3-13) 0.1 (0-1) 5.0 (1-10) 0.2 (0-2) 9.7 (3-19) in Table 3 and Figure 1, participants rarely engaged in eye contact, joint attention, or motor imitation behaviors with their partners during baseline. In contrast, during training, all participants increased the frequency of their social-communicative behaviors, although increases in joint attention were generally less pronounced than increases in eye contact or motor imitation across participants. Generalization of Treatment Effects Table 4 shows the means and ranges of frequency of social-communicative behaviors displayed by participants during generalization probes. The frequency of social-communicative behaviors per session during generalization probes is shown in the lower panels for each participant in Figure 1. As can be seen, changes in behavior from baseline to training were similar to training effects, although increases for joint attention were more modest. 24 Hwang and Hughes Baseline ~ 30 Social Interactive Training 25 20 15 10 5 o ~ 30 25 20 15 30 IJamesl 25 20 15 10 5 o 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 30 25 o Eye Contact 20 o Joint Attention 15 6. Motor Imitation ~_-----~-------o_----- 10 _--- 5 __-==: ~&----~----------~~------D~--- 0 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 __ ~ ~-------b------~-D _------G---___ ~ 40 45 50 __D 55 60 Observation Sessions Figure 1. Frequency of early social-communicative behaviors (eye contact, joint attention, motor imitation) during baseline and training sessions (upper panels) and generalization probes (lower panels) for participants. Dotted lines in upper panels indicate participant absence. 25 Early Social-Communicative Skills Table 4 Mean and Range of Frequency of Social-Communicative Behaviors During Generalization Probes Participant and condition Noel Baseline Training James Baseline Training Louis Baseline Training Eye contact M (range) Joint attention M (range) Motor imitation M (range) 1.5 (1-2) 9.7 (4-15) 0 (0) 1.0 (0-2) 1.0 (0-2) 4.1 (1-9) 2.3 (2-3) 5.2 (2-9) 0 (0) 2.0 (0-4) 0 (0) 7.5 (4-11) 0.3 (0-1) 8.2 (7-11) 0 (0) 1.4 (0-3) 0 (0) 4.4 0-7) Social Interactive Training Strategies Implemented (Fidelity of Treatment) Figure 2 shows the number of social interactive strategies implemented correctly by partners per session during baseline and training. Table 5 displays the 60 means and ranges of social interactive training strategies implemented per baseline, training, and generalization conditions. During baseline, partners did not implement contingent imitation, naturally occurring reinforcement, or expectant look. They implemented environmental arrangement at low levels. Partners' implementation of all strategies during baseline averaged 1.5 times with Noel, 2.6 times with James, and 2.1 times with Louis per 10-minute session. During training, implementation of all strategies increased to an average of 31.1 times with Noel, 37.7 times with James, and 42.9 times with Louis per session. During generalization probes, classroom teachers implemented social interactive strategies with partners at relatively low levels, although mean implementation of strategies was slightly above baseline levels (i.e., means for all strategies implemented per session were 5.6 times with Noel, 5.8 times with James, and 4.2 times with Louis). Social Validation Table 2 displays the means and standard deviations for respondents' ratings to each of the five Likert-type questions for two videotaped segments (i.e., one baseline and one training). Low standard deviations associated with mean ratings reflected little variability across the respondents. In addition, Kendall's coefficient of Social Interactive Training Baseline []2ill 50 40 30 20 10 o ~L.-J---"''-'''__WiliI. L , I 60,... I I I I 50 f-IJamesl I I I I 40 I 30 20 10 I- 40 30 -- I ---I • T ... _- I I: I I I -------------, I I I I I I '''' I I o Contingent Imitation 1m Naturally Occurring Reinforcement • Expectant Look • Environmental Arrangement 20 10 oOb-a.....IAJ....I..af=--.JII-DI..-..-..I.....- - i a . l 5 10 15 ~. .._............l..aJULll.l1...llJl,l.l..lULll..1.UULlllJL.l.l,.lL.ll...ll.Jl.ll,)LlUL.ll..l;UL1U1.J....L....., 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 Observation Sessions Figure 2. Frequency of social interactive strategies (contingent imitation, naturally occurring reinforcement, expectant look, environmental arrangement) implemented correctly by partners during baseline and training sessions. 26 Hwang and Hughes Table 5 Mean and Range for Frequency of Social Interactive Training Strategies Across Conditions for Each Participant Participant and social interactive training strategy Noel Contingent imitation Naturally occurring reinforcement Expectant look Environmental arrangement Total James Contingent imitation Naturally occuring reinforcement Expectant look Environmental arrangement Total Louis Contingent imitation Naturally occuring reinforcement Expectant look Environmental arrangement Total Baseline M (range) Training M (range) 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0) 1.5 (0-4) 1.5 (0-4) 6.7 (0-17) 5.9 (0-15) 8.2 (1-16) 0.05 (0-1) 0.3 (0-2) 0.7 (0-3) 1.9 (0-4) 2.6 (0-6) 9.5 (4-17) 8.0 (3-15) 0.04 (0-1) 0.2 (0-3) 0 (0) 1.8 (0-3) 2.1 (0-6) lOA (2-17) 31.1 (8-51) Generalization probes M (range) 004 (0-3) 1.3 (0-6) 1.9 (0-6) 1.9 (1-6) 5.6 (6-16) (5-18) 37.7 (9-52) 1.2 (0-6) 2.1 (0-6) 104 (0-5) 1.0 (0-4) 5.8 (3-16) 11.3 (6-16) 9.3 (6-14) 11.3 (7-16) 11.7 (7-18) 42.9 (35-49) 0 (0) 0.9 (1-6) 1.9 (0-6) 104 (1-4) 4.2 (3-15) lOA (4-17) lOA concordance (W) was calculated as 1, which was significant at p < .00001. This high value of W indicated that all 30 respondents employed a similar standard in the evaluation of the two videotaped performances. To test for significant differences among respondents' mean ratings for the two video segments, a twoway split plot ANOVA was conducted. A significant difference between mean ratings was found (F = 744.9, df = 1, p < .01). There was no significant group effect among three respondent groups on mean video ratings. These findings suggest that increases in three socialcommunicative skills of the participants after implementation of social interactive strategies were associated with higher ratings by respondents. Discussion Findings in this study corroborated previous efforts, indicating that social interactive training can increase early social-communicative skills of children with autism (e.g., Klinger & Dawson, 1992; Pierce & Schreibman, 1995). In addition, social validation measures in- dicated that a variety of judges perceived that positive performance changes had occurred in child behaviors and that training procedures were acceptable. This investigation extends prior research in several important ways. First, we introduced the social interactive training program to a novel population: 3-year-old preschool children with autism who were functioning at early developmental levels. Previous investigations included school-aged (5-10 year old) participants with verbal language abilities. Our findings indicated that eye contact, joint attention, and motor imitation could be increased among preverbal 3-year-olds with autism by implementing a combination of social interactive strategies (i.e., contingent imitation, naturally occurring reinforcement, expectant look, environmental arrangement). These findings are important in light of the role that early social reciprocity and social-communicative skills play in the development of children's subsequent language acquisition (Yoder & Warren, 1993). To minimize cumulative effects of language delay over time, one solution may be to introduce social interactive training during the initial stages of social-communicative development of preverbal children with autism. Second, we introduced social interactive training in the children's everyday classroom rather than in a clinical setting, as occurred in previous studies (e.g., Dawson & Galpert, 1990; Klinger & Dawson, 1992). Participants were able to request, reject, comment on, and interact with objects and events with which they came in contact on a daily basis. If children are removed from their everyday environments and placed in highly structured training settings, the functional utility of acquired communication skills is questionable (Dattilo & Camarata, 1991). Introducing social interactive training in the children's everyday classroom setting may have promoted the acquisition of social-communication skills observed in this study because the acquired skills likely were functional in the children's daily environments. In addition, training in the children's everyday classroom may have promoted the generalization of skills across settings and partners observed in this study. Unlike in the Pierce and Schreibman (1995) study, however, we found only modest generalization of joint attention behavior. Differential effects across the two studies may exist because children in the previous study were older (10 years) and verbal. Participants in our study may have been less proficient at attending simultaneously to an object and an interactive partner. We found that even during training, increases were more modest for joint attention than for eye contact or motor imitation. It may be that joint attention is a more complex skill that develops more slowly and requires additional training than other social-communicative skills. Third, only two prior investigations of early social interactive training assessed fidelity of treatment of interactive partners' implementation of prescribed strategies (Charlop & Trasowech, 1991; Wolery, Gast, Kirk, 27 Early Social-Communicative Skills & Schuster, 1988). This is unfortunate because social interactive training consists of multiple strategies that require precise implementation by an interactive partner in order to support the child as initiator of interactive play. In our study, interactive partners' implementation of training strategies was assessed in real time from videotaped performance. This allowed us to investigate the use of the strategies in relation to changes in children's social-communicative behaviors. During training, partners were found to implement strategies as prescribed. In addition, the onset of implementation of strategies across participants was related to observed increases in participants' social-communicative behaviors. Several limitations to the study are worth noting. First, although increases in targeted skills did occur across participants, considerable variability occurred in children's performance. James, for example, increased his eye contact less than did his peers. Participants' characteristics may account for some of this variability. James, for example, was more distractible than his peers. An additional factor relating to this variability could be that training was provided only twice weekly. Social-communicative skills typically develop as an ongoing process of children's daily interactions with important others in their environment rather than through infrequent, short-term efforts (Rowland & Schweigert, 1993). Changes in participants' behavior may have been more consistent if training had been provided across the day during different activities, in multiple settings with a variety of interactive partners. Second, increases in targeted behaviors occurred at a slower rate than anticipated, perhaps because of the infrequency of training. Consequently, the 7 month duration of the study ended before training could be withdrawn. Therefore, maintenance of training effects was not assessed, although durability of behavior change is a critical criterion for evaluating the efficacy of intervention efforts. Future researchers should anticipate the delayed effects that may occur when teaching early socialcommunicative skills to young, preverbal children with autism. Ample time should also be allowed to assess the maintenance of targeted skills over time. In summary, this study provides both empirical and subjective support of social interactive training in increasing acquisition and generalization of early socialcommunicative skills of preverbal preschoolers with autism. A critical question, however, warrants further investigation: Does early social-communicative skills training promote later language development in children with autism? The ultimate demonstration of the long-term value of the social interactive approach will be established through repeated assessments of its effect on children's language development over time as observed during functional, reciprocal interactions in children's everyday lives. References American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Baker, M. J., Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (1998). Increasing the social behavior of young children with autism using their obsessive behaviors. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 23, 300-308. Charlop, M. H., & Trasowech, J. E. (1991). Increasing autistic children's daily spontaneous speech. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 747-761. Cohen, R. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Education and Psychological Measurement, 20, 3746. Cronin, P., Hemmeter, M. L., & Kaiser, A. P. (1990). The environmental rating scale. Unpublished manuscript, Vanderbilt University. Dattilo, J., & Camarata, S. (1991). Facilitating conversation through self-initiated augmentative communication treatment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 369-378. Dawson, G., & Galpert, L. (1990). Mothers' use of imitative play for facilitating social responsiveness and toy play in young autistic children. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 151-162. Guralnick, M. J., Connor, R. T., Hammond, M. A., Gottman, J. M., & Kinnish, K. (1996). The peer relations of preschool children with communication disorders. Child Development, 67, 471-489. Hedrick, D., Prather, E., & Tobin, A. (1984). Sequenced inventory of communication development-Revised. Seattle: University of Washington Press. Hepting, N. H., & Goldstein, H. (1996). What's natural about naturalistic language intervention? Journal of Early Intervention, 20, 249-265. Horner, R. D., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Multiple-probe technique: A variation of the multiple baseline. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 189-196. Hwang, B., & Hughes, C. (1995). Effects of social interactive strategies on early social-communication skills of a preschool child with developmental disabilities. Education and Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 30, 336-349. Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single-case research designs: Methods for clinical and applied settings. New York: Oxford University Press. Klinger, L. G., & Dawson, G. (1992). Facilitating early social and communicative development in children with autism. In S. F. Warren & J. Reichle (Eds.), Causes and effects in communication and language intervention (Vol. 1, pp. 157-186). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Koegel, L. K. (1995). Communication and language intervention. In R. L. Koegel & L. K. Koegel (Eds.), Teaching children with autism: Strategies for initiating positive interactions and improving learning opportunities (pp. 17-32). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Koegel, R. L., Dyer, K., & Bell, L. (1987). The influence of child-preferred activities on autistic children's social behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 243-252. Koegel, R. L., & Johnson, J. (1989). Motivating language use in autistic children. In G. Dawson (Ed.), Autism: Nature, diagnosis, and treatment (pp. 310-325). New York: Guilford. McEvoy, R. E., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1993). Executive function and social communication deficits in young autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 563-578. McGee, G. G., Morrier, M. J., & Daly, T. (1999). An incidental teaching approach to early intervention for toddlers with 28 Hwang and Hughes autism. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24, 133-146. Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115-128. Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1992). The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evidence from normal infants and children with autism or mental handicap. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 375-383. Pierce, K., & Schreibman, L. (1995). Increasing complex social behaviors in children with autism: Effects of peerimplemented pivotal response training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 285-295. Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137-162. Rowland, C., & Schweigert, P. (1993). Analyzing the communication environment to increase functional communication. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 18, 161-176. Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill. Sigman, M., Mundy, P., Sherman, T., & Ungerer, J. (1986). Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children and their caregivers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 647-656. Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books. Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., & Littleford, C. D. (1997). Motor imitation in young children with autism: What's the object? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 475-485. Tapp, J. T., Wehby, J. H., & Ellis, D. N. (1995). A multiple option observation system for experimental studies: MOOSES. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 27, 25-31. Uzgiris, I. D. (1981). Two functions of imitation during infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 1-12. Uzgiris, I. D., & Hunt, J. M. (1975). Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Wetherby, A. M., & Prizant, B. M. (1993). Communication and symbolic behavior scales manual: Normed edition. Chicago: Riverside. Wolery, M., Gast, D. L., Kirk, K., & Schuster, J. (1988). Fading extra-stimulus prompts with autistic children using time delay. Education and Treatment of Children, 11, 29-44. Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: The case for subjective measurement or how applied behavior analysis is finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 203-214. Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (1993). Can developmentally delayed children's language development be enhanced through prelinguistic intervention? In A. P. Kaiser & D. B. Gray (Eds.), Enhancing children's communication: Research foundations for intervention (Vol. 2, pp. 35-61). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Zager, B. D. (Ed.). (1999). Autism: Identification, education, and treatment (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Article received: June 17,1999 Final acceptance: January 6, 2000 Editor in charge: Joseph R. Scotti