See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/260003216 Book Review: Deictic Conceptualisation of Space, Time and Person Article · January 2003 CITATION READS 1 89 1 author: Zouheir Maalej 110 PUBLICATIONS 586 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Motion verbs and verb- and satellite-framed languages View project The embodiment of fruit and vegetables View project All content following this page was uploaded by Zouheir Maalej on 04 February 2014. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. LINGUIST List 14.2209 Wed Aug 20 2003 Review: Semantics/Pragmatics: Lenz, ed. (2003) Lenz, Friedrich (ed.) (2003). Deictic Conceptualisation of Space, Time and Person. John Benjamins Publishing Company (Pragmatics & Beyond New Series 112). Zouhair Maalej, Department of English, University of Manouba-Tunis, Tunisia Purpose and contents The book under review is an edited collection of eleven papers distributed over the conceptualization of deixis as represented by space, time, and pronouns. PART I: SPACE Sergio Meira: “Addressee effects” in demonstrative systems: The cases of Tiriyo and Brazilian Portuguese (pp. 3-11) Working on two unrelated languages, Meira argues against the distinction made between distance and person-oriented systems of demonstratives, invoking the fact that even distance-oriented systems are essentially person-oriented. Konstanze Jungbluth: “Deictics in the conversational dyad: Findings in Spanish and some cross-linguistic outlines (pp. 13-40) Rejecting the distance and person-oriented systems for Spanish, Jungbluth offers a dyad-oriented system for demonstratives as an alternative, involving speaker and hearer. Claudio Di Meola: “Non-deictic uses of the deictic motion verbs kommen and gehen in German” (pp. 41-67) Di Meola describes non-deictic unrestricted motional uses of kommen and restricted uses of gehen in German. In its non-deictic use, kommen, for instance, has to do with the (i) overcoming of an obstacle, (ii) accidental movement, and (iii) passive movement, with the prepositional phrase of place or time becoming mandatory. Di Meola mentions the inherent link between the deictic and nondeictic uses of kommen and gehen as mediated through metonymic extention. Ellen Fricke: “Origo, pointing and conceptualization—What gestures reveal about the nature of the origo in face-to-face interaction” (pp. 69-93) Fricke investigates the relevance of speech-associated gestures to origo, which is differentiated into personal, local, and temporal. The personal is further differentiated hierarchically into primary (with the addresser) and secondary (with the addressee). PART II: TIME Christiane von Stutterheim, Mary Carroll, and Wolfgang Klein: “Two ways of construing complex temporal structures” (pp. 97-133) von Stutterheim et al investigate the way speakers of different languages (here English and German) convey temporal information in the construal of events. English and German were found to adopt different strategies of retelling events that are basically amenable to the formal linguistic features of the language in question. Thomas A. Fritz: “’Look here, what I am saying!’: Speaker deixis and implicature as the basis of modality and future tense” (pp. 135-151) Fritz explains modality and futurity by scalar implicature, which is derived from “saying” or deictically the amount of factuality or epistemic commitment to which speakers want to commit themselves. Tanja Mortelmans: “The ‘subjective’ effects of negation and past subjunctive on deontic modals: The case of German dürfen and sollen” (pp. 153-182) Mortelmans argues for a speaker-oriented conception of deontic modality, using negation and the past subjunctive as evidence. Mortelmans defends the view that dürfen (may) is pragmatically reactive and sollen (should?) is non-reactive. PART III: PERSON AND TEXT Johannes Helmbrecht: “Politeness distinctions in second person pronouns” (pp. 185-202) In a study of a hundred world languages, Helmbrecht isolates four classes of honorific types: (i) languages showing no politeness distinctions in their pronoun system, (ii) languages showing a binary politeness distinction, (iii) languages showing two or more politeness distinctions, and (iv) languages avoiding the use of second person polite address. These types show unequal geographic distribution, and are explained functionally and cross-linguistically as being affected by contact-induced borrowing. Katharina Kupfer: “Deictic use of demonstrative pronouns in the Rigveda” (pp. 203-321) Through elaborate syntactic argumentation, Kupfer presents a view of demonstratives as opaque deictic expressions whose deictic center has to be created by addresser and addressee in Rigveda, a collection of ancient Indian texts. Manfred Consten: “Towards a unified model of domain-bound reference” (pp. 223-248) Consten argues for a model of domain-bound reference where deixis and anaphora interact and can be direct or indirect, hoping that the two terms, deixis and anaphora, will not be part of the vocabulary of cognitive linguistics. Heiko Hausendorf: “Deixis and speech situation revisited” (pp. 249-269) Hausendorf offers a view of speech situation as involving participants in a mutually shared sensory perception in a socially and interactionally constructed discourse. Using Jakobson’s framework, Hausendorf phrases his view as follows: deixis “operates at the interface between code and message where sensory perception is required as a channel of communication in its own right” (p. 264). Critical evaluation Some critical comments are in good order. Mortelmans mentions in her paper that the concept of “projected reality” (p. 154) originates in Langacker (1991) and Achard (1998). If we abstract from the use of “world” instead of “reality”, we find that we owe the distinction between “real world” and “projected world” to Jackendoff (1985), who acknowledges that the distinction is at least as old as Kant, although for Mortelmans the “projected world” is part of irreality whereas for Jackendoff (1985: 28) it is the “experienced world” or “phenomenal world.” Mortelmans also talks about the possibility of including negation as a “modal deictic marker.” Invoking Lehmann (1991) in a footnote as arguing for the inclusion of negation as part of the modal system, Mortelmans seems to consider Lehmann as being at the origin of this idea. The author, however, might want to know that, in Halliday’s functional/systemic grammar (1985-1994), negation (as part of Polarity) is called “a Modal Adjunct” (1994: 90) within the Mood System (in the Hallidayian perspective, the clause as an exchange is divided into MOOD and RESIDUE). If negation is definitely modal in essence, it is not made clear by the author how it could have a deictic dimension. The inclusion of Mortelmans’s paper by the editor under the conceptualization of time is not where a paper on modality and negation should belong. In fact, Mortelmans hardly ever even alluded to time. To end this review, some tentative conclusions can be made about some of the findings arrived at in this volume: (i) (ii) (iii) Many of the papers in the collection use Bühler’s theory of deixis and the origo by either rehabilitating his work, or pointing out sources that pass over his theory, which scientifically counts as important acknowledgment. If, as Jungbluth found out, the Spanish demonstrative system can refer to parts of the body with proximal, medial, and distal pronouns, and is neither distance- nor person-oriented, the notion of deictic center should be revised as being not only egocentric but also hearercentric. A confirmation of this polycentric dimension of deixis is what Fricke discusses as a hierarchy of primary and secondary origos, and even more types of origos or centers. As von Stutterheim et al demonstrate, formal language-specific constraints can be invoked to explain differences in the construal of events cross-linguistically, whereby grammaticization seems to play a major role. With very few exceptions, the papers making up this book are insightful by shedding fresh cognitive light on the pragmatics of deixis. References Achard, Michel (1998). Representation of Cognitive Structures: Syntax and Semantics of French Sentential Complements. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Jackendoff, Ray (1985). Semantics and Cognition. Cambridge/London: The MIT Press. Langacker, Ronald W. (1991). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar (Vol.2): Descriptive Application. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Lehmann, Christian (1991). Startegien der Situationsperpektion. Sprachwissenschaft 16, 1-26. About the reviewer The reviewer is an assistant professor of linguistics. His interests include cognitive linguistics, metaphor, cognitive pragmatics, psycholinguistics, critical discourse analysis, etc. View publication stats