– third variable that influences relationship between the IV and the DV. It is a mechanism that describes how two things are related. the process of inferring the causes of events or behaviors o Internal (dispositional) or External (situational) o Causal attributions – help people understand their social and political environments - Having to do with any of the 22 numbered pairs of chromosomes found in most human cells. Autosomal chromosomes are numbered 1-22 (Sex-linked & sexlimited) - X and Y chromosomes ONLY determine whether an individual is male or female and are not considered autosomal chromosomes. Inherited traits - genes that are present in both sexes of sexually reproducing species but are expressed in only one sex and have no penetrance or are simply 'turned off' in the other. i.e., facial hair in men or breasts in women – a measure of how well differences in people's genes account for differences in their traits. Inherited traits – experiences that could turn on or turn off activities of certain genes. the study of how your behaviors and environment can cause changes that affect the way your genes work. Unlike genetic changes, epigenetic changes are reversible and do not change your DNA sequence, but they can change how your body reads a DNA sequence. MARRIAGE & TESTOSTERONE ARTICLE When we talk about marriage, we cannot underestimate the contribution of the physiological factor, higher-than-normal testosterone levels. We are basically indicating one to two standard deviations above the mean pg. 464 There has been a well-established relationship between testosterone and _______ behavior antisocial More testosterone = aggressive & impulsive behavior, pleasure seeking (1-2 standard deviations above the mean) This article incorporated sociological (poor marriage material, socioeconomic status, and age) and psychological variables as they interact with physiological variable (testosterone) to examine or evaluate marital quality. pg. 465 A related sociological perspective suggests that low marital quality and stability come about because one or both of the partners is poor marriage material, which involves such factors as alcohol abuse, being in trouble with the law, and unemployment. Respondent accounts of their own divorces frequently cite such factors as instrumental in the dissolution of their marriages (Cleek & Pearson 1985; Kitson & Sussman 1982). Data indicate that men with high levels of testosterone are more apt to have irregular work histories, report encounters with the law, and suffer from alcohol abuse Dabbs & Morris 1990). In this case we are hypothesizing that any relation between testosterone and marital quality is entirely or in part accounted for by men's behavioral problems. The direct findings - Testosterone had a negative relation to marriage. More testosterone less likely to get married. pg. 467 For example, the likelihood of never marrying is 50% higher for men one standard deviation above the mean compared to those that are one standard deviation below the mean. Comparing men at the same two levels on other marital variables, we find that men at the higher level are 43% more likely to have divorced, 31% more likely to have separated because they were not getting along, 38% more likely to have had extramarital sex, and 12% more likely to have hit their wives. Age predicts lower testosterone which will be predicted better marriage IV AGE DV Better marriage quality 2 3 indirect findings age, SES (socioeconomic status), poor marriage material pg. 465 This change in behavior has been attributed to a decline in testosterone production, but we are not familiar with a study demonstrating that an age associated decline in testosterone production is followed by a decline in pugnaciousness. We hypothesize that the relation between age and higher marital quality is due to a decline in testosterone production. pg. 465 We begin our analysis by examining whether testosterone is related to the formation and dissolution of marriages and the quality of men's marriages. This is followed by an exploration of the nature of the relationship, whether any effects found occur only at very high levels of testosterone production, or whether the trend is linear. We then go on to explore how three sociological models of marital quality and stability may be informed by a knowledge of testosterone. How was poor marriage material operationally defined? alcohol abuse/substance abuse, poor work record Poor marriage material is the final model tested here. We propose that alcohol abuse, being in trouble with the law, and a poor work record (factors associated with testosterone production) mediate the effect of testosterone on marital success after controlling for age. A comparison of columns 1 and 3 in Table 4 indicate that this is the case; the testosterone-marital adversity coefficient is reduced by 28% when the marriage material variables are added to the equation. As with the other 3 sociological models tested, marriage material and testosterone continue to have independent effects on marital adversity. Using a large sample of former servicemen, our analysis shows that men with higher testosterone production report being less likely to marry and more likely to divorce. Once married this type of individual is more likely to leave home because he is having trouble getting along with his wife, more likely to have extramarital sex, more likely to report hitting or throwing things at his wife, and more likely to experience a lower quality of spousal interaction. The negative relationship between testosterone and marital success is a simple linear gradient. There was no evidence of a threshold effect. Three sociological correlates of marital success were examined to see if knowledge of them is extended with information about testosterone. It is clear that testosterone has direct effects as well as indirect effects through the correlates of marital success and that both of these have independent effects in the sense that they make statistically significant and unique contributions to marital quality and stability. We conclude that leaving testosterone out of the analysis not only results in lower levels of explained variance but may cause models to be estimated incorrectly. Sociologists who seek to understand marital success should be aware of the effects of testosterone. What would be a reason for young couples to divorce more? high testosterone, pleasure seeking unable to keep exclusive relationship. All the proposed marriage-testosterone models rest on the assumption that testosterone-stimulated behavior is relatively uncontrolled. As creatures of culture, successful members of society control their impulses. Testosterone related impulses are no exception. Since testosterone levels are established early in life, we propose that the control of testosterone related impulses is learned early in life. The research shows very consistently that offspring of parents who provide a great deal of support and a moderate amount of control have a stronger internal locus of control, have more advanced moral development, and exhibit more considerate and altruistic behavior than offspring of parents who do not (see, e.g., Maccoby & Martin 1983 for a review). On the basis of this research, we would expect high-testosterone individuals who received extensive parental support and who were subject to moderate parental control to have greater marital success than high-testosterone individuals who did not. If their parents exerted control effectively, high-testosterone individuals may have very successful marriages. Certainly, many high-testosterone men in our sample were happily married. Perhaps testosterone-stimulated aggressive impulses may become initiative and sensation seeking may become efforts to broaden knowledge and experience. High-testosterone individuals whose parents provided extensive support and moderate control may take an even more active role in being 4 thoughtful and caring spouses than low-testosterone individuals, a possibility that must await another study for exploration. BREAST CANCER ARTICLE What % of cancer is due to heritability? 5-10% What genes are associated with breast cancer? BRCA1 & BRCA2 If you have the genetic mutation what is your lifetime risk of getting breast or ovarian cancer? mutation in a breast – chances of BRCA1(2) 43-84% CBC (contralateral breast cancer) - (contralateral prophylactic mastectomy) BRCA1 & BRCA2 (genes) to protect you from getting breast cancer PSYCHOSOCIAL FACTORS women younger than 50 elected (age was a factor) <50 o if you have a family history of breast cancer and/or ovarian cancer that will increase the likelihood of choosing mastectomy o Education was a predictor o educational level • Women remove the opposite breast (CPM) because there is a 65% lifetime risk of contracting breast cancer in opposite breast • 314 total had breast cancer of those 70 elected preventative surgeries (23%) Gay Gene Article 5 Genetic essentialism fosters a strong link between genes and behavior, and that association implies predetermined, immutable behavior. Many supporters of gay rights evoke immutability arguments, implying homosexuals have no choice and thus cannot be morally denounced. This line of argument generates favorable feelings toward homosexuals and support for gay marriage (Haider-Markel and Joslyn, 2008). However, there are occasional rumblings within the gay and lesbian community about these tactics and an awareness that some biologically caused traits can change (Halley, 1994). In addition, an important implication of immutability beliefs is the absence of a remedy for perceived biological differences (Suhay and Jayaratne, 2012). As Dar-Nimrod and Heine (2010) observed: “If the genes are present, the outcome is expected.” In this way genetic determinism could, in some instances, strengthen the status quo or produce unanticipated consequences (Hegarty, 2002). 6 For decades, scientists offered theories about the hereditary or congenital nature of homosexuality. Hereditary theories were reasonably popular in the early 20th century, but then diminished somewhat in the 1970s and 1980s. However, in the 1990s, a flurry of scientific studies revived interest in the biological basis of homosexuality and the nature versus nurture debate generally. The most prominent study by Hamer et al. (1993) claimed to have identified a genetic marker that accounted for male homosexuality. The specific genetic marker (Xq28), which contains hundreds of genes, became known as the “gay gene”, and attracted widespread media attention. The news media were cautiously optimistic, noting the research needed replication but also highlighting the potential implications that “naturalizing” homosexuality could bring such as less discrimination (see Conrad and Markens, 2001 for review of media coverage). Gay rights activists were lifted by the science, asserting that if sexual orientation could be shown to be genetic, then homosexuality was natural, not chosen, and therefore the individual would not immediately be blamed for his or her behavior (Weiner, Perry, and Magnusson, 1988; Weiner, Osborne, and Rudolph, 2011). A few writers expressed concern that a gay gene might invite tinkering with the genetic code or testing for such genes during pregnancy. Alternative perspectives underscored the impact and reach of the gay gene science in public discourse and the immutability implied by genetic causes. However, environmental causes of homosexuality, while apparent in public debate, did not appear to stir the political and social context as did the emergence of genetic evidence (Dar-Nimrod and Heine, 2010). In summary, it appears that a biological basis for homosexuality enhances favorable attitudes and behaviors toward gays and lesbians. Public debate about sexual orientation turns on biology and choice. Because genetics is contrasted with choice, where choice would imply blame for a stigmatized condition, biological accounts function as a rationale for reducing hostility toward gays and lesbians and increase positive perspectives. We therefore anticipate respondents who endorse genetic attributions for sexual orientation are more likely to perceive homosexuality as an immutable characteristic and utilize stereotypic beliefs to describe homosexual behavior. In explaining sexual orientation, genetic attributions should also encourage positive stereotypes of homosexuals and deter negative ones. Table 1 shows the data from 2003 and 2012 Pew Center Surveys. In 2003, 32.2 percent selected genetics, 13.7 percent one’s upbringing, and choice was the highest cause at 39.8 percent. So the biological attribution represents nearly a third of respondents, and nearly 54 percent attribute homosexuality to situational causes—upbringing and choice. In 2012, about 44 percent chose the “born with” statement, 12 percent believed that one’s upbringing was responsible, and approximately 32 percent attributed homosexuality to personal choice. Thus, among those in 2012 willing to assign a cause to homosexuality, disregarding the Don’t Know category, respondents split nearly evenly as 49.8 percent believed genetics was responsible and 50.2 percent attributed homosexuality to environmental factors—choice and upbringing. Sample differences between 2003 and 2012 reflect the growing popularity of the genetic attribution, which increased by approximately 11 percent. The logistic estimates in Table 3 support our theoretical expectations. First, independent of other individual-level demographics and relevant beliefs, genetic attribution is a significant predictor of 7 immutability (b = 2.62, p < 0.01). Those respondents who believe genetics is the cause of homosexuality are substantially more likely to also believe that homosexuality cannot be changed. Second, the marginal effects column, which represents the influence of a discrete change in an independent variable from 0 to 1 on the dependent variable, while holding other variables at their modal/median level, underscores the relative strength of genetic attribution. The logistic estimates in Table 3 support our theoretical expectations. First, independent of other individual-level demographics and relevant beliefs, genetic attribution is a significant predictor of immutability (b = 2.62, p < 0.01). Those respondents who believe genetics is the cause of homosexuality are substantially more likely to also believe that homosexuality cannot be changed. Second, the marginal effects column (dy/dx), which represents the influence of a discrete change in an independent variable from 0 to 1 on the dependent variable, while holding other variables at their modal/median level, underscores the relative strength of genetic attribution. The unfavorable stereotypical judgments referencing unhappiness (b = −0.46, p < 0.00) and unstable relationships (b = −0.31, p < 0.00) are inversely related to genetic attributions. That is, respondents who report genetic attributions are less likely to favor these negative stereotypic judgments and more likely to report gays and lesbians are “just like other people.” Yet, the favorable stereotype, concerning the style of gay men, is a preferred description among respondents attributing homosexuality to genetics (b = 0.69, p < 0.00). Respondents who believe genetics is the cause of homosexuality are evidently willing to differentiate gay men from heterosexual men on style, a favorable stereotype about the group, but not so for instability of relationships or unhappiness, which are less favorable categorizations. Although it is undoubtedly true that genetic research is pushing the boundaries of what we in fact know about ourselves, it appears equally true that genetics plays a large role in shaping how we view others. In this article, we posited that people’s understanding about genetics as a cause for group behaviors affects perceptions of immutability and stereotypical judgments about such groups. In particular, we examined causal beliefs about genetic influences on sexual orientation. We hypothesized that adoption of genetic attributions would shape stereotypical judgments about homosexuals and beliefs about whether homosexuality can or cannot be changed. We tested hypotheses by employing individual-level data from three nationally representative surveys. The analyses led to several important conclusions. First, when individuals report a genetic attribution, they are significantly more likely to believe there is a predetermined, immutable outcome. The vast majority of individuals who reported a genetic explanation also believed homosexuality could not be changed. And among those respondents who reported environmental factors caused homosexuality, most felt sexual orientation could be changed. Even after controlling for educational attainment, gender, ideological belief, age, race, and religiosity, the relationship between genetic attribution and immutability remained strong. This finding is consistent with recent research that suggests deterministic thinking increases when subjects are exposed to genetic arguments (Dar-Nimrod and Heine, 2010). 8 Second, our findings show that genetic attributions influence stereotypic thinking. In the case of homosexuality, a genetic attribution seems to remove the locus of control from individual choice. This makes the behavior that was conventionally perceived as problematic (morally questionable) less so, thereby changing attitudes about the group behavior; in our case increasing positive stereotypes about gays and lesbians while also decreasing negative stereotypes. Our findings are consistent across three different stereotypic judgments about gays and lesbians. And we were able to replicate these findings with a relatively new survey. Finally, and more broadly, our results point to the powerful framing influence that a genetic attribution can have on the way people think about individuals and groups. Although scientific and medical advances in genetics can give us great hope for the future, the public’s understanding of biology, and the role of genetics in sexual orientation, appears crucial to maintaining and increasing support for gays and lesbians. 1. Define attributions and what are the 2 different types of attributions o the process of inferring the causes of events or behaviors. o Internal (dispositional) or External (situational) o Causal attributions – help people understand their social and political environments 2. Identify the gene that has been associated with homosexuality. o Xq28 (X chromosome came from mother) 3. What role does attribution play in explicating the origin of Homosexuality? o It’s a key precursor for attitudes toward homosexuality. 4. What is the relationship between stereotypes (favorable vs. unfavorable) and attribution of homosexuality? o In general, researchers discovered that if respondents believed homosexuality had a genetic basis, prejudice declined, tolerance and positive feelings toward gays increased, and policies supportive of gays rights were favored o In contrast to choose, where the person bears responsibility and any moral connotations attached to the behavior, genetics evoke nature and therefore discrimination and prejudice are less evident. 5. What is the implication of making internal (genetic) attributions of homosexuality in relation to stereotypes? o To assert that genetics is the cause not that it is a choice. “I was born this way” That we cannot anymore change our sexual orientation or our eye color. 9 What were the 3 dependent variables that defined stereotypical judgements? relationship stability, sense of style, happiness Genetic attribution is a significant predictor of _______ Favorable attributions – the extent to which people believe that they themselves as opposed to external forces have control over the outcome of events in their lives. -predictors attribution of the causes of an event to sources internal or external to the self, which may influence subsequent behavior in relation to that event What is the difference between locus of control and locus of causality? Locus of control refers to the determinants of outcomes, whereas Locus of causality refers to the determinants of behaviors. Most of your behavior is influenced by the interaction between experiences (environment) and genetic predisposition. GENES - DNA is made up of GCAT (guanine, cytosine, adenine, and thiamine – Are alteration in the genetic change in the code. (mutation = alteration) – Physical characteristic/observable trait – Genetic information that predicts that trait – Genes code for single traits or multiple traits? 10 both. (it’s rare) most traits are the interaction between many genes. are segments of DNA are made up of DNA (23 PAIRS 46 TOTAL) - forms of genes - requires presence of only 1 copy for it to be expressed (it always wins) only exerts its characteristics in the homozygous same genes different genes Examples of epigenetic changes Learning new things aids in branching of new dendrites and axons While environment plays a bigger role in determining your behavior and your overall personality despite genetic predisposition, heredity does play a role. What evidence do we have as a field of bio psych to prove that some behaviors are due to heritability? Twin studies (For example, adopted children) Even if you inherited that specific gene (i.e., alcoholism) and you were raised in a family that is very restrictive or religious, what do you think will happen? 11 Chances are you will be significantly less likely to become an alcoholic. Because of your environment. What if we are all raised in the same condition, and someone turns out to be an alcoholic. (FYI exposed to same environmental factors, trait was implicated in heritability) Evidence for heritability What is a sex-linked gene? A gene on either the X or Y chromosome. ________ changes alter genetic activity without replacing any gene or any code. Epigenetic Musicians tend to have larger temporal lobe, what is the percentage? 30% – responsible for nerve growth factor, aids in survival of neurons function: branching out of dendrites A family of proteins that aids in survival, growth, and function of neurons by aiding in the impulse process, branching out more dendrites, therefore a better explanation for depression (recently) low levels BDF. Low levels of BDF have been indicated in depression (not just serotonin) Prefrontal and Hippocampus still maturing in early age (risky and impulsive behavior) biological (male or female) psychological/social factors prescribed to men and women by society 12 The differences are largely accounted by internalized stereotypes, socialization, and cultural expectations. In lay terms the differences are learned not innate. There are two school of thoughts that explain the definitions for the difference between women and men. One school of thought is social constructionism. says women and men are different not because of anatomy, it is by what is constructed by society. Gender is a thing it’s not something you were born with; it is something that you learn. Believe gender is fluid (Liberals) –the theory that women and men are innately, radically, psychologically, and biologically different. Not molded by society (Conservative) What are these ideologies about? They attempt to explain gender difference. One school of thought which is social constructionism believes gender is fluid – a specific characteristic of an individual. Traits can be determined by genes, environmental factors or by a combination of both. dichotomy for example black vs white. it is mutually exclusive to gender i.e., women are emotional – precursors to female internal structures – precursors to male internal structures (i.e., gonads that will either be testes or ovaries) – sex determining region on the Y chromosome gene helps the gonads to become testes – small tube that transports sperm (MIH) – Testes produce this. plays a significant role in sexual differentiation. allows for normal female structure or development of fallopian tubes, ovaries, etc. – hormones in males help -> gonads (i.e., Testosterone) sex limited 13 – hormones in females help -> ovaries (i.e., Estradiol) sex limited The differentiation of the external anatomy is largely determined by the levels of Testosterone? True or False TRUE - plays an important role in anatomy development, menstruation, and sex drive in women. crucial in sexual desire in women Erection is dependent on largely by the stimulation of ____ and ____ receptors? Dopamine D2 Which region of the brain is responsible for sexual orgasm and erection in men? Medial pre-optic area/anterior hypothalamus (MPOA/AH) Which neurotransmitter plays a role in prolonging erection? Nitric Oxide – critical to sexual development and a healthy reproductive system – growth of egg that will ovulate (highest level FSH) at the end of your menstrual cycle - triggers ovulation P329 both ER and PR present the surge of FSH and LH so you don’t ovulate and thicken the mucus of the cervix, makes it harder for 14 sperm to reach egg. if it does reach it prevents it from reaching the egg. Estradiol is primarily responsible for sexual desire True At what time in a woman’s menstrual cycle do her estradiol levels increase? Pre-ovulatory days When does estradiol decline? At the end of the cycle Ovulation 15th day Men look for age (young women) Women look for stability – normalized homosexuality and challenged our perception of homosexuality. – primary stress hormone overdevelopment of the adrenal gland. The adrenal glands produce important hormones, including Cortisol, which regulates the body's response to illness or stress. Excessive production of testosterone in girls 15 – these individuals are boys, genetically (XY) but they lack androgen receptors (derives testosterone) enables to activate male development as a result they look like a female What is another name for androgen insensitivity? Testicular feminization syndrome Stress coupled with ______ will increase the probability that the son will be homosexual. Alcohol/drinking Stress + Alcohol in prenatal women = GAY BABY Stress releases two (2) chemicals that antagonize the production of Testosterone Cortisol & endorphins -Interconnects the amygdala and connect the left and right hemisphere // if it interconnects it plays a function or a role in memory, emotions, speech, hearing, smell play a key role in pain perception or sensation. – above the optic chiasma, thought to be concerned with the regulation of physiological circadian rhythms. Larger in homosexual men –the sexually dimorphic nucleus of humans. smaller in homosexual men fewer antigen receptors & produce less testosterone 16 EMOTIONS DURING SEXUAL ACTIVITY ARTICLE – Anxious, overwhelming feeling – lack of happiness rooted in your attachment style with your parents The purpose of the present study was to assess the emotional reactions to a list of automatic thoughts presented to sexually functional and dysfunctional men and women during sexual activity. Cognitive Schema of Depression Pg 492 Regarding the impact of mood, research consistently suggests that depressive affect is negatively related to sexual arousal. Previous research using the AT sub-scale has shown that men and women with sexual dysfunction had significantly more negative automatic thoughts during sexual activity compared to men and women without sexual difficulties Specifically, failure and disengagement thoughts, sexual abuse thoughts, and lack of erotic thoughts were more often endorsed by women with sexual dysfunction relative to controls whereas erection concern thoughts, failure anticipation thoughts, and lack of erotic thoughts were more often endorsed by men with sexual dysfunction compared to men without sexual difficulties Previous research shown that women and men with sexual dysfunction had significantly more negative thoughts including all of the following except ______. 17 She may throw in something that was not discussed (see below for what WAS discussed) Thoughts associated with each sex with sexual dysfunction: Men – erection concern thoughts (am I going to be able to achieve or maintain an erection) failure and anticipation (I am not going to be able to please her, etc.) lack of erotic thoughts were more often endorsed by men with sexual dysfunction. Women – failure, disengagement, sexual abuse, and lack of erotic thoughts pg. 492 We predicted that sexually dysfunctional men and women would report significantly more negative emotions and less pleasure and satisfaction to automatic thoughts that occurred during sexual activity compared to men and women without sexual dysfunction. A total of 376 subjects (MAY ASK THIS) International index of erectile dysfunction KNOW FOR TEST!! MALE (5 factors) sexual functioning, sexual desire, orgasmic function, intercourse satisfaction, and overall satisfaction FEMALE (6 factors) sexual interest/desire, sexual arousal, lubrication, orgasm, sexual satisfaction, and sexual pain pg. 495 Results section Table 2 Sexually dysfunctional women reported, on average, significantly higher scores on the emotional responses of sadness, disillusion, guilt, and anger pg. 495 1st column & 2nd column (findings for women) In order to examine the contribution of each emotional response to distinguish sexually functional from dysfunctional women, a discriminant analysis was performed. The results confirm the power of the emotional response during sexual activity in discriminating (differentiating) women with and without sexual dysfunction. (Whether you are functional or not) pg. 496 1st column 2nd paragraph (findings for men) 18 Sexually dysfunctional men had significantly higher scores on the emotional responses of sadness, disillusion, and fear, and significantly lower scores on pleasure and satisfaction. Confirming that the emotional response to automatic thoughts presented during sexual activity significantly differentiated sexually dysfunctional from functional men. (Meaning healthy men showed different emotions compared to unhealthy men) 2nd column (very last sentence before discussion) This finding supports the strong power of the emotional response during sexual activity to discriminate between men with and without sexual dysfunction. pg. 496 1st paragraph in the discussion section recaps what they studied, purpose and hypothesis. The purpose of the present study was to assess the emotional reactions to a list of automatic thoughts presented to sexually functional and dysfunctional men and women during sexual activity. Data supported the important role played by emotional response on sexual dysfunctional processes. The results showed that both men and women with sexual dysfunction endorsed significantly more negative emotions and less positive emotions to a list of automatic thoughts during sexual activity. Men with sexual dysfunction had significantly more emotions of sadness, disillusion, and fear and less pleasure or satisfaction compared to sexually healthy men. Female data pointed to similar differences, with women with sexual dysfunction reporting less pleasure and satisfaction, and more sadness, disillusion, guilt, and anger. pg. 497 last paragraph Overall, data on emotional responses showed a certain uniformity in men and women. These findings might have important implications both conceptually and clinically. Emotions associated with depressive mood and not with anxiety appear to be most strongly associated with sexual dysfunction; thus, treatment approaches oriented to reduce anxiety (relaxation and systematic desensitization) might be less efficient than interventions aimed at elevating mood during sexual activity. Research comparing these two therapeutic strategies would be very valuable in testing this hypothesis. What is the clinical implication of the study, why is this important? What is the takeaway. It pinpoints a specific emotion associated with sexual dysfunction. emotions that are related to depression not related to an anxiety. – an intent to act • • 19 IM - intrinsic motivation (activities you engage in that allow you to grow) (maximize for your own wellbeing) EX – extrinsic motivation (minimize as many as possible) – a future event or circumstance which is possible but cannot be predicted with certainty. hinders you from growth (trying to fulfill one contingency to the next) – a complex state that is produced by the interplay of physiological arousal, psychological factors. (i.e., physiological heart is racing because you are excited) - gives purpose or direction to behavior and operates in humans at a conscious or unconscious level – emotions are a result of physiological arousal S (stimulus) —> physiological arousal —> emotions According to James Lang theory of emotion, what was the primary requirement for you to feel an emotion Physiological emotion What is the biggest drawback of this theory? 1. underestimating the role of cognitive labeling 2. two completely different emotions can illicit the same exact physiological response (i.e., Happiness and fear will cause your heart to beat faster (2 contradictory emotions) 3. secondary emotions that are things or feelings that you may experience that may not illicit a physiological response. emotions that distinguish humans from animals (i.e., hate, pride, guilt, curiosity) (ignored cognitions) Stimulus physiological Emotions 20 Simultaneous - criticized Lang – what we think you see a snake, and you experience the bodily sensations & fear at the same time (left perception out) (A) – SCHACHTER – SINGER (2 Factor theory) Stimulus physiological —> RUN or FEAR Cognitive label - emotional response of fear is a result of the interaction between physiological arousal (see a snake your heart races) and cognitive labeling (why is my heart racing? I guess I am scared.) (B) Cognitive – mediation theory Stimulus Appraisal (primary) Physiological arousal - before you even feel the racing heart, you have to go through the appraisal process (often unconscious and immediate) following the stimulus (i.e., if you lose your job, your response to your job loss may be different than mine. primary process (perception of severity) + secondary process, coping skills) 21 Know the differences between emotions and motivations (from book) Stop at slide 7 on PPT slides for Chapter 11 22