V O L U ME 3 , ISS U E 12 DEC EMBER 2 0 1 9 MASS M ONTHLY A PPL ICATIO N S IN STRE N G TH SPO R T E R IC H E LMS | G R E G N UCK O LS | MIC HAEL ZO URDO S | ERIC T REXL E R The Reviewers Eric Helms Eric Helms is a coach, athlete, author, and educator. He is a coach for drug-free strength and physique competitors at all levels as a part of team 3D Muscle Journey. Eric regularly publishes peer-reviewed articles in exercise science and nutrition journals on physique and strength sport, in addition to writing for commercial fitness publications. He’s taught undergraduate- and graduate-level nutrition and exercise science and speaks internationally at academic and commercial conferences. He has a B.S. in fitness and wellness, an M.S. in exercise science, a second Master’s in sports nutrition, a Ph.D. in strength and conditioning, and is a research fellow for the Sports Performance Research Institute New Zealand at Auckland University of Technology. Eric earned pro status as a natural bodybuilder with the PNBA in 2011 and competes in the IPF at international-level events as an unequipped powerlifter. Greg Nuckols Greg Nuckols has over a decade of experience under the bar and a B.S. in exercise and sports science. Greg earned his M.A. in exercise and sport science from the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. He’s held three all-time world records in powerlifting in the 220lb and 242lb classes. He’s trained hundreds of athletes and regular folks, both online and in-person. He’s written for many of the major magazines and websites in the fitness industry, including Men’s Health, Men’s Fitness, Muscle & Fitness, Bodybuilding.com, T-Nation, and Schwarzenegger.com. Furthermore, he’s had the opportunity to work with and learn from numerous record holders, champion athletes, and collegiate and professional strength and conditioning coaches through his previous job as Chief Content Director for Juggernaut Training Systems and current full-time work on StrongerByScience.com. Michael C. Zourdos Michael (Mike) C. Zourdos, Ph.D., CSCS, has specializations in strength and conditioning and skeletal muscle physiology. He earned his Ph.D. in exercise physiology from The Florida State University (FSU) in 2012 under the guidance of Dr. Jeong-Su Kim. Prior to attending FSU, Mike received his B.S. in exercise science from Marietta College and M.S. in applied health physiology from Salisbury University. Mike served as the head powerlifting coach of FSU’s 2011 and 2012 state championship teams. He also competes as a powerlifter in the USAPL, and among his best competition lifts is a 230kg (507lbs) raw squat at a body weight of 76kg. Mike owns the company Training Revolution, LLC., where he has coached more than 100 lifters, including a USAPL open division national champion. Eric Trexler Eric Trexler is a pro natural bodybuilder and a sports nutrition researcher. Eric has a PhD in Human Movement Science from UNC Chapel Hill, and has published dozens of peer-reviewed research papers on various exercise and nutrition strategies for getting bigger, stronger, and leaner. In addition, Eric has several years of University-level teaching experience, and has been involved in coaching since 2009. Eric is the Director of Education at Stronger By Science. 2 Letter from the Reviewers C an you believe it’s the last issue of 2019? Neither can we, and we want to collectively take a moment to say thank you for being a MASS subscriber. While our success in growing the MASS subscriber base is something we each feel proud of, as it tells us we are providing value and utility, we are also incredibly encouraged that so many people value their continued education. This bodes well for the fitness industry, and we want to thank you for making it a better place. With that said, this final issue of 2019 is a very interesting one! Guest reviewer Anne-Kathrin Eiselt PhD, a neuroscientist, long-time trainer, and experienced CrossFit and powerlifting competitor, wrote an excellent review of how and why our brains make us hungry. Also, Greg discusses a seemingly science fiction-based intervention, as he reviews how lasers (AKA phototherapy) may actually help you make gains. Additionally, Dr. Trexler reviews the evidence on betaine as a body composition aid, and in his second article, the safety profile of sucralose. On top of that, Dr. Zourdos reviews a study that highlights the practical limitations of low load, high rep training, and also examines a study that compares various resistance training recovery-marker surrogates, including HRV. Greg also reviews a study on bench press ROM which suggests the efficacy of partial range of motion training is movement specific, and examines an analysis of the peak performance age for both powerlifters and weightlifters. To round the issue out, we have video content from both Dr. Helms and Dr. Zourdos. Eric covers how one of the possible regulatory systems which influences bodyweight could be total energy expenditure, a novel perspective given most hypotheses involve body fatness and energy intake. Mike, in his video, takes you through the competition-day elements of a powerlifting meet, covering both practical considerations and also a logical, tactical approach to attempt selection to ensure you end your day with the largest total possible. Once again, thank you. It’s been a pleasure and a privilege having your trust for the year, and we look forward to putting out more reviews and content in 2020! Sincerely, The MASS Team Eric Helms, Greg Nuckols, Mike Zourdos, and Eric Trexler 3 Table of Contents 6 BY G R EG NUCKOL S Improving Strength Endurance with Frickin Lasers It sounds like sci-fi, but using a low-power infrared or near-infrared laser on your muscles between sets may improve strength and strength endurance. 17 BY M I CHAEL C. ZOUR DOS Most People Find Low-Load Training to Failure Miserable You may have heard that recent evidence suggested that low-load training produces similar hypertrophy to more typical, moderate-load training. That may be true, but a new study shows that people find low-load training to be pretty miserable in comparison to moderate-load training. 25 BY E R I C T R EXL ER Betaine May Promote Body Composition Improvements When Combined With Resistance Training Betaine, which is found in beets, spinach, whole grains, and seafood, has been used to improve the body composition of livestock. To see if these effects also apply to humans, a recent meta-analysis sought to review the small body of literature evaluating betaine’s effects on fat loss in human subjects. Read on to find out if betaine might help you achieve your physique-related goals. 37 BY G R EG NUCKOL S Bench Press Range of Motion: An Exception to the Principle of Specificity? Much of what we know about range of motion specificity comes from single-joint studies and squat studies. When we branch out to the bench press, things get more complicated. 49 BY M I CHAEL C. ZOUR DOS The Usefulness of Heart Rate Variability in Resistance Training is Tenuous The potential benefit of heart rate variability in resistance training is its ability to track recovery and be used as a readiness indicator. However, does heart rate variability actually correlate with performance? This article reviews a recent study and examines the totality of the literature to provide some answers. 4 58 72 BY A NN E-KAT HR I N EI SELT How The Brain Controls Eating Behavior Many people find it difficult to follow a calorie-restricted diet. Is your brain working against you? This review of neuroscience research helps explain why dieting makes you feel lousy, why it is so easy to overeat hyperpalatable food, and how you can use this knowledge to your advantage. BY E R I C T R EXL ER More Good News for Artificial Sweeteners: No Effect of Short-Term Sucralose Ingestion on Glycemic Control or Gut Microbiome Some view artificial sweetener consumption as a safe and healthy way to cut calories, while others suspect that they’re too good to be true. A recent study found that one week of consuming highdose sucralose, also known as SPLENDA®, had no effect on glycemic control or the gut microbiome. Read on to get the scoop on the potential health effects of artificial sweeteners. 86 98 100 BY G R EG NUCKOL S What’s the Best Age to Dominate Strength Sports? If you want to maximize your competitiveness in powerlifting or weightlifting, at what age should you anticipate being at the peak of your prowess? It seems that weightlifting is a young person’s game, but many powerlifters are still improving well into their 30s (or even 40s). BY M I CHAEL C. ZOUR DOS VIDEO: Powerlifting Game Day MASS has many articles and videos on programming for powerlifting, but what can you expect at the actual competition? This video breaks down everything you need to prepare, and what you need to know about powerlifting game day as a coach and lifter. BY E RI C HEL MS VIDEO: New Perspectives on Activity and Bodyweight We are still uncovering the mechanisms of how humans regulate body weight. Typically, this is viewed from the perspective of our body attempting to maintain a certain level of adiposity or mass during an energy deficit or surplus. However, there are likely regulatory mechanisms related to total daily energy expenditure that influence body weight as well. 5 Study Reviewed: Muscular Preconditioning Using Phototherapy Improves the Physical Work Capacity of the Quadriceps When Applied Between Repeated Bouts of Resistance Exercise. Borsa et al. (2019) Improving Strength Endurance with Frickin Lasers BY G RE G NUC KO LS It sounds like sci-fi, but using a low-power infrared or nearinfrared laser on your muscles between sets may improve strength and strength endurance. 6 KEY POINTS 1. Phototherapy between sets of isokinetic knee extensions improved torque, power, and total work performed in a crossover study. 2. When digging into the literature, I was surprised to find that there’s actually a considerable amount of evidence in favor of phototherapy; it seems to reliably boost performance by improving recovery between sets, accelerating recovery from training. It may even lead to larger strength gains and more muscle growth over time. 3. Phototherapy works because infrared and near-infrared light interacts with protein complexes involved in aerobic metabolism. W hen we think about the stimuli our muscles can respond to, light probably isn’t the first thing that comes to mind. After all, barring a catastrophic injury, your muscles should reside safely under your skin, mostly existing in darkness. When we think about stimuli for our muscles, we generally think of mechanical stimuli, electrical stimuli, and chemical stimuli first, for good reason. However, certain protein complexes in your muscles can respond to light through a process called photobiomodulation. This responsiveness to light, specifically infrared and near-infrared light, opens up the possibility for lasers to impact muscle function (referred to as phototherapy). That may sound like science fiction, but actual science supports its efficacy. In the present study (1), subjects completed the same fatigue protocol consisting of four sets of isokinetic knee extensions twice, in a crossover fashion. One time subjects received phototherapy between sets, and one time they received a sham treatment. Peak torque, average torque, average power, and total work performed were greater in sets 2, 3, and 4 in the phototherapy condition. As it turns out, this finding is right at home in the rest of the literature on the topic. Purpose and Hypotheses Purpose The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of phototherapy, compared to a sham treatment, on neuromuscular performance of the quads when applied between successive sets of fatiguing training. Hypotheses The authors hypothesized that phototherapy would delay the onset and extent of fatigue compared to a sham treatment, allowing the subjects to maintain higher torque and work output. 7 Figure 1 Timeline of study Healthy volunteers n=20 Volunteers excluded n=1 Randomization for treatment order n=19 Healthy volunteers n=20 Familiarization training Phase 1 Phase 2 72 hours recovery 5 minute warm-up 5 minute warm-up Active phototherapy n=9 Sham phototherapy n=10 Fatigue protocol Fatigue protocol 5 minute warm-up 5 minute warm-up Active phototherapy n=10 Sham phototherapy n=10 Fatigue protocol Fatigue protocol 72 hours recovery Participants received a 72 hour recovery between familiarization training and start of phase 1, and between phase 1 and phase 2. All participants completed both phases of the study Subjects and Methods Subjects 20 subjects volunteered for the study (10 men and 10 women), with 19 successfully completing it. The only inclusion criteria were that the subjects needed to be injury-free and between the ages of 18 and 30 years old. Thus, it’s hard to know whether the subjects were predominantly trained or untrained. In the limitations section of the paper, the authors note that future studies should include highly competitive or elite athletes, so we can safely assume that such labels wouldn’t apply to the subjects in this study, but they don’t mention un- 8 Figure 2 Exercise protocol Pre-exercise Fatigue protocol: 4 bouts of 30 repetitions 4 minute recovery treatment (active/sham) Dose 1 5-minute warm-up on cycle ergometer Bout 1 4 minute recovery treatment (active/sham) Dose 2 Bout 2 4 minute recovery treatment (active/sham) Dose 3 Bout 3 Bout 4 Each subject completed a 5 minute warm-up on a cycle ergometer followed by the completion of four exercise bouts on an isokinetic dynamometer. There was a standardized passive 4-minute recovery period between each exercise bout during which active or sham PBMT was administered to the quadriceps muscle group. trained subjects as a limitation. The authors also note that they instructed the subjects to refrain from lifting weights for at least 48 hours prior to testing. This leads me to think that most (or at least some) of the subjects likely had some degree of prior training experience. Experimental Design The study used a standard crossover design. After familiarization, subjects were randomly assigned to complete a fatigue protocol while receiving either phototherapy between sets of knee extensions, or a sham treatment. After 72 hours of rest, the subjects completed the same fatigue protocol, receiving whichever treatment they did not receive during the first testing session. To the researchers’ credit, the famil- iarization was quite extensive. In each familiarization session (separated by at least 72 hours), the subjects completed the exact same fatigue protocol that was used during the experimental sessions. They performed familiarization sessions until their peak torque values differed by less than 10% session to session. Thus, all subjects had at least two familiarization sessions, with an average of 2.6. The fatigue protocol consisted of 4 sets of 30 maximal concentric isokinetic knee extensions at an angular velocity of 75° per second, with four minutes of rest between sets. Between sets, subjects either received phototherapy on their quads using a laser emitting near-infrared light, or a sham treatment using the same machine emitting a different frequency of light at a much lower power. The researchers 9 81 Normalized PT (Nm/BW) Figure 5 Normalized peak torque for the four exercise bouts (marginal means) 78 Sham phototherapy 2600 Active phototherapy 2500 * * 75 * 72 Total work (J) Figure 3 * 2400 2300 * 2200 * 2100 69 2000 1 2 Bout 3 1 4 Participants produced higher peak torque (PT) during bouts 2, 3, and 4 after receiving active phototherapy compared to sham phototherapy. * = statistically significant difference of p < 0.013 Figure 4 Average torque for the four exercise bouts (marginal means) 2 Bout 3 4 Participants produced more work during bouts 2, 3, and 4 after receiving active phototherapy compared to sham phototherapy. * = statistically significant difference of p < 0.013 Figure 6 Average power for the four exercise bouts (marginal means) 115 100 110 95 Average power (W) Average torque (Nm) Total work for the four exercise bouts (marginal means) * 90 * 85 * 105 100 * * 95 90 80 1 2 Bout 3 4 Participants produced more average torque during bouts 2, and 3 after receiving active phototherapy compared to sham phototherapy. * = statistically significant difference of p < 0.013 took the important (though depressingly uncommon) step of asking the subjects in both experimental sessions whether they thought they were getting the active treatment or the sham treatment; the subjects couldn’t tell them apart (p = 0.63), indicating that the sham treatment was applied successfully. During the fatigue protocol, the re- 1 2 Bout 3 4 Participants produced more power during bouts 2, 3, and 4 after receiving active phototherapy compared to sham phototherapy. * = statistically significant difference of p < 0.013 searchers assessed peak torque (normalized to body mass), average torque, total work, and average power. Findings During the first set of the fatigue protocol (before the first application of either treatment), normalized peak torque, 10 Figure 7 Comparison of phototherapy vs. placebo group Study or subgroup Mean difference SE Weight Mean difference IV, Random, 95% CI Mean difference IV, Random, 95% CI 3.3.1 Immediately dos Reis 2014 -8.9 4.41 7.7% Higashi 2013 2.5 1.88 18.1% 2.50 [-1.18, 6.18] 10.16 2.64 14.0% 10.16 [4.99, 15.33] Leal Junior 2008 Leal Junior 2009d 4.5 2 17.4% 4.50 [0.58, 8.42] 5 1.47 20.5% 5.00 [2.12, 7.88] 2.3 1.17 22.2% 2.30 [0.01, 4.59] Leal Junior 2010 Toma 2013 -8.90 [-17.54, -0.26] Total (95% CI) 100% Heterogeneity: Tau = 8.23; Chi = 16.89, df = 5 (P = 0.005); I = 70% 2 2 2 Test for overall effect: Z = 2.41 (P = 0.02) Test for subgroup differences: Not applicable 3.51 [0.65, 6.37] -10 -5 Favors placebo 0 5 10 Favors phototheraphy Outcome: exercise capacity (number of repetitions [count]) From Nampo et al. (2016) average torque, total work, and average power were similar between conditions, as one would hope. For every subsequent set, performance was maintained better in the phototherapy condition than the sham treatment condition. Specifically, in the phototherapy condition, there was either an increase (normalized peak torque and average torque) or a smaller decrease (total work and average power) in performance from set 1 to set 2 compared to the sham condition. After set 2, set-to-set decrements in performance were similar in both conditions, though absolute performance remained superior in the phototherapy condition. Interpretation I found this study fascinating. I was aware of the concept of phototherapy, but I didn’t realize how far the research had already progressed. As it turns out, I was pretty far behind on the research. Before I get ahead of myself, it’s worth explaining how phototherapy works (2, 3). You have protein complexes within your cells that react to light. That reactivity to light is known as photobiomodulation. Most importantly for exercise, one of the cytochrome complexes in your mitochondria, along with your hemoglobin (the molecule that carries oxygen in red blood cells) and myoglobin (the molecule that stores oxygen in your muscles) interact with red and infrared light. In your mitochondria, red and infrared light cause inhibitory factors to dissociate with cytochrome c oxidase. When hemoglobin and myoglobin inter- 11 act with red or infrared light, they release the oxygen they’re holding onto. As a result, your mitochondria work more efficiently, and you have more oxygen freely available for oxidative metabolism. Essentially, red and infrared light allow local aerobic energy production to hum along a bit more efficiently. As a result, strength endurance improves a bit, your muscle cells can move back toward homeostasis a bit faster after a metabolic stressor, and you experience a bit less total acidosis, leading to a bit less muscle damage. None of these things are night-and-day differences, but they can cumulatively make a noticeable impact. As an added bonus for strength-focused trainees, phototherapy also seems to increase intracellular calcium ion concentrations (2; though I’ll admit that I’m not sure I totally understand the mechanism by which this occurs). As I alluded to, I was pretty far behind on the research in this area. I assumed that the present study was one of the first few to examine the effects of phototherapy on exercise performance (1). As it turns out, there were at least 16 extant studies when a meta-analysis on the subject was published back in 2016 (4). The headline findings were that phototherapy improves rep performance, improves time to exhaustion, and attenuates blood lactate levels. An earlier systematic review also suggested that phototherapy attenuates muscle damage to some degree, leading to improved recovery after THERE ARE AT LEAST FIVE LONGITUDINAL STUDIES INVESTIGATING THE EFFECTS OF PHOTOTHERAPY ON HYPERTROPHY OR IMPROVEMENTS IN MUSCLE PERFORMANCE, AND ALL FIVE REPORTED POSITIVE EFFECTS IN FAVOR OF TRAINING WITH PHOTOTHERAPY. training (5). So, with all of that in mind, the results of the present study (1) are right in line with the rest of the literature on the subject. Phototherapy didn’t make a nightand-day difference in performance, but it did make a consistent, measurable difference. For all four measures, subjects performed ~7-10% better during sets two, three, and four in the phototherapy condition. Given its mechanism of action, it shouldn’t be too surprising that phototherapy improved average torque, total work, and average power. However, the striking thing to me is that it actually improved normalized peak torque 12 between sets 1 and 2, matching the 2016 meta-analysis which also found that phototherapy improved peak torque (3). This is likely due to phototherapy’s ability to increase intracellular calcium concentrations. Just as one final note about this particular study: the results of phototherapy are even more impressive, given that the subjects were resting four minutes between sets. That should be plenty of time to recover, especially from single-joint exercise (although 30 maximal reps is certainly challenging). If they only had a minute to recover, it would be reasonable to wonder whether phototherapy improved recovery, or if it just accelerated recovery. In other words, maybe one minute of rest with phototherapy would be equivalent to two minutes of rest without phototherapy, but one could still rest a bit longer and reach the same level of recovery before the next set, which would make one question the degree to which phototherapy would actually be useful and relevant in practical settings. However, if it’s able to delay fatigue when people are already resting four minutes between sets, that strikes me as an improvement that’s more meaningful and relevant. If phototherapy can improve strength endurance and recovery from training, and potentially even peak force, there must be some downside, right? Since it seems to attenuate muscle damage, my first though was that it may work similar- ly to NSAIDs or cold water immersion – decreasing inflammation and hastening recovery from training, while also attenuating longitudinal gains in strength and muscle mass. However, if anything, the opposite may be true. There are at least five longitudinal studies investigating the effects of phototherapy on hypertrophy or improvements in muscle performance, and all five reported positive effects in favor of training with phototherapy (6, 7, 8, 9, 10). However, there is a catch: I’m a little skeptical about whether consumer-grade phototherapy devices are effective. A cursory search revealed that research- or medical-grade phototherapy units cost between $10,000-$40,000 USD. However, there are consumer products retailing on Amazon for $100-$150. Immediately, I noticed that the device used in the present study (1) was considerably more powerful than the consumer products on Amazon (10W vs. 1-3W). However, other studies have used less powerful devices, which have also seemed to be effective. I know that research equipment is often massively overpriced (it’s generally purchased with grant money, and most organizations awarding grants have DEEP pockets), but a 100- to 400fold markup seems a little insane, assuming the consumer products are also effective. Do with that what you will. I could just be overly skeptical due to obscene price gouging, and the consumer products may be awesome. Or, 13 APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS Phototherapy seems to reliably improve acute training performance and accelerate recovery from training, and it may even lead to larger strength gains and more muscle growth over time. However, due to the massive cost disparity between research-grade and consumer-grade devices, I’m still a bit skeptical of the consumer products on the market. With that being said, a phototherapy device may be worth the splurge if you’re looking for an edge. the consumer products may be a sham that would be hard to detect (since phototherapy doesn’t cause any noticeable sensation). Seeing as consumer phototherapy devices are reasonably affordable, and since the research on phototherapy is so promising, investing in a phototherapy device may not be a bad investment if you’re looking for something to give you an edge. On one hand, it’s a fairly sizeable one-time purchase, but on the other hand, there are people who spend more money than that per month on supplements that are probably less effective. I don’t think a dose-response relationship for phototherapy is known, so your best bet would probably be to just use it on your prime movers between each set of your workout. A lot of the studies mention holding the device over each point to which it’s applied for 10+ seconds, so that would be my recommendation as well (since we know the experimental protocols are effective); if you were using it on your biceps, for example, instead of continually moving it over your biceps, hold it in place for 10-30 seconds over 3-4 spots along each of your biceps. Overall, phototherapy is an exciting technology with much more support than I realized. I’m still skeptical about the consumer products, but it’s an area of research you should keep your eye on. It likely won’t make a night-and-day difference, but for a one-time purchase, it seems to be more effective than most supplements, which would make a reliable phototherapy device a pretty good investment in the long-run for people who are willing to do a little extra in order to maximize their performance. Next Steps At this point, I’m pretty sold on phototherapy’s ability to improve acute performance and recovery, but I’d like to see more longitudinal research on whether it improves strength gains and hypertrophy over time. While there’s some research on that subject already, I’d just like to see more before I’d feel 14 comfortable recommending phototherapy with the same confidence I’d recommend something like creatine. I’d also like to see validation studies on some of the affordable consumer-grade products on the market. 15 References 1. Borsa PA, Dale RB, Levine D, Crow JA. Muscular Preconditioning Using Phototherapy Improved the Physical Work Capacity of the Quadriceps when Applied between Repeated Bouts of Resistance Exercise. J Athl Enhanc 2019, 8:1. 2. de Freitas LF, Hamblin MR. Proposed Mechanisms of Photobiomodulation or Low-Level Light Therapy. IEEE J Sel Top Quantum Electron. 2016 May-Jun;22(3). 3. Ferraresi C, Huang YY, Hamblin MR. Photobiomodulation in human muscle tissue: an advantage in sports performance? J Biophotonics. 2016 Dec;9(11-12):1273-1299. 4. Nampo FK, Cavalheri V, Dos Santos Soares F, de Paula Ramos S, Camargo EA. Low-level phototherapy to improve exercise capacity and muscle performance: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lasers Med Sci. 2016 Dec;31(9):1957-1970. 5. Borsa PA, Larkin KA, True JM. Does phototherapy enhance skeletal muscle contractile function and postexercise recovery? A systematic review. J Athl Train. 2013 Jan-Feb;48(1):57-67. 6. Ferraresi C, de Brito Oliveira T, de Oliveira Zafalon L, de Menezes Reiff RB, Baldissera V, de Andrade Perez SE, Matheucci Júnior E, Parizotto NA. Effects of low level laser therapy (808 nm) on physical strength training in humans. Lasers Med Sci. 2011 May;26(3):349-58. 7. Vieira WH, Ferraresi C, Perez SE, Baldissera V, Parizotto NA. Effects of low-level laser therapy (808 nm) on isokinetic muscle performance of young women submitted to endurance training: a randomized controlled clinical trial. Lasers Med Sci. 2012 Mar;27(2):497-504. 8. Ferraresi C, Bertucci D, Schiavinato J, Reiff R, Araújo A, Panepucci R, Matheucci E Jr, Cunha AF, Arakelian VM, Hamblin MR, Parizotto N, Bagnato V. Effects of Light-Emitting Diode Therapy on Muscle Hypertrophy, Gene Expression, Performance, Damage, and Delayed-Onset Muscle Soreness: Case-control Study with a Pair of Identical Twins. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. 2016 Oct;95(10):746-57. 9. Baroni BM, Rodrigues R, Freire BB, Franke Rde A, Geremia JM, Vaz MA. Effect of low-level laser therapy on muscle adaptation to knee extensor eccentric training. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2015 Mar;115(3):639-47. 10. Toma RL, Vassão PG, Assis L, Antunes HK, Renno AC. Low level laser therapy associated with a strength training program on muscle performance in elderly women: a randomized double blind control study. Lasers Med Sci. 2016 Aug;31(6):1219-29. █ 16 Study Reviewed: Acute Effects of Different Training Loads on Affective Responses in Resistance-Trained Men. Ribeiro et al. (2019) Most People Find Low-Load Training to Failure Miserable BY MIC HAE L C . ZO URD O S You may have heard that recent evidence suggested that low-load training produces similar hypertrophy to more typical, moderate-load training. That may be true, but a new study shows that people find low-load training to be pretty miserable in comparison to moderate-load training. 17 KEY POINTS 1. This crossover design study compared subjects’ perceived exertion and discomfort during low-load training (25-30RM) and moderate-load training (8-12RM) on the bench press, hack squat, and lat pulldown. 2. Perceived exertion and perceived discomfort were both rated significantly higher following a training session with low loads versus high loads. Further, subjects noted more “displeasure” with the low-load training session versus the moderateload session. 3. Although low-load and moderate-load training can produce similar hypertrophy, serious practical limitations exist with using solely low-load training. This study points out that, on average, people simply perceive low-load training to failure to be more fatiguing and less enjoyable than more typical moderate-load training. W hen research reveals similar outcomes between training protocols, I often think, “cool, do what you do want.” Low-load training (25-30RM, or ~30-40% of one-repetition maximum [1RM]) versus moderate-load training (8-15RM, or ~60-80%) is one of those do-what-you-want scenarios, as a previous meta-analysis (reviewed by Greg) showed no difference in hypertrophy between low- and moderate-load training (2). However, there are practical limitations associated with low-load training, including a limited ability to progressively increase load over time and potentially greater acute fatigue and discomfort than moderate-load training. If training is more difficult, less enjoyable, and only produces similar outcomes to easier training, then why choose the more difficult option? This crossover design study (1) had 12 trained men perform the bench press, hack squat, and lat pulldown for 3 sets of 8-12RM in one session and 3 sets of 25-30RM in another session. Following both training sessions, researchers asked the participants to rate their perceived exertion, discomfort, and pleasure/displeasure on 10-point Likert scales. The low-load condition led to significantly greater exertion, discomfort, and displeasure than the moderate load condition. These results suggest that although two different protocols may produce similar hypertrophy, we should look deeper into what could affect long-term progress. This article will discuss the practical limitations of low-load training as a standalone strategy, and when lowload training may be useful. Purpose and Research Questions Purpose The purpose of this study was to com- 18 Table 1 Subject characteristics Subjects Age (years) Height (cm) Body mass (kg) Training experience (years) 12 men 26.7 ± 3.5 174.9 ± 9.9 85.1 ± 17.5 2.3 ± 0.9 Data are mean ± SD Subject characteristics from Ribeiro et al. 2019 (1) pare a lifter’s perception of exertion, discomfort, and pleasure/displeasure following 3 sets of 3 exercises using low loads (25-30RM) versus moderate loads (8-12RM). and 4, subjects performed 3 sets at the 8-12RM load on the bench press, hack squat, and lat pulldown on one day and 3 sets of the 25-30RM load on the other day. There were 120-second rest intervals between sets. The authors hypothesized that low-load training would lead to greater ratings of exertion, discomfort, and displeasure compared to moderate-load training. Perceptual Scales Hypotheses Subjects and Methods Subjects 12 men with 1-4 years of training experience participated. The available descriptive details of the subjects are in Table 1. Study Protocol All subjects completed this crossover design study over four lab visits separated by 48-72 hours. The first visit was for anthropometric testing (i.e. height, weight, body composition) and the second visit was to determine the 8-12RM and 25-30RM loads that would be used for each subject during the experimental visits (visit 3 and 4). During visits 3 The outcome measures in this study were three perceptual scales which were completed 15 minutes after training on visits 3 and 4. The scales rated perceived exertion, perceived discomfort, and perceived pleasure/displeasure for the entire training session. More details of the scales are in Table 2. Findings Very simply, the subjects rated exertion, discomfort, and displeasure all significantly higher in the low-load condition versus the moderate-load condition. In other words, subjects perceived the low-load, high-rep training to be more fatiguing, and they felt worse afterward. The mean values for each scale and effect sizes between conditions are in Table 3. The individual subject ratings for each scale are in Figure 1. 19 Table 2 Description of perceptual scales Session rating of perceived exertion scale Session rating of perceived discomfort scale Feelings of pleasure and displeasure An 11-point scale ranging from -5 to +5 The OMNI 0-10 scale was used 0 = resting 0 = neutral A 0-10 scale 0 = no discomfort at all 10 = maximum discomfort +1 to +5 = positive (pleasure ratings) -1 to -5 = negative (displeasure ratings) Protocol from Ribeiro et al. 2019 (1) Interpretation We’ve known for quite some time that when sets are equated between lowload (~30% of 1RM) and moderate-load (~60-80% of 1RM, called high-load by some) training, hypertrophy outcomes are similar in studies lasting at least six weeks, while strength unsurprisingly favors the moderate-load training (2). Oftentimes, some will use these previous results to state that you should feel free to use whichever strategy (low- or moderate-load) you wish. While on the surface that seems true, I have never been a believer of using solely low loads in the long term, and – in my opinion – the study reviewed here adds to that skepticism. Specifically, I believe low-load training has some important practical limitations over the long run. First, it seems as though progressive overload would be difficult over the long-term with a lowload strategy. While I cannot say that for certain, it logically seems more difficult to continually achieve progressive overload with low loads versus moderate loads over the very long term. Therefore, while low loads might produce similar hypertrophy to moderate loads in the short-term (i.e. 6-12 weeks), I would be surprised if continually using lower loads produced similar hypertrophy over the longer term (6 months to 1 year) for the limitations mentioned above. Another practical consideration with low loads is that, to date, all of the low-load versus moderate-load training studies have the low-load group training to failure, so we don’t yet know if training shy of failure with low loads can maximize hypertrophy. In contrast, with moderate loads, there is much more evidence to suggest that training shy of failure with moderate loads can maximize hypertrophy. Further, we know that training to failure elongates fatigue and the time course of recovery compared to submaximal training (3). Thus, if solely low loads are 20 Table 3 Results for each perceptual scale Scale Low-load Moderate-load sRPE 6.4 ± 0.7* 5.5 ± 1.0 1.06 Discomfort 8.7 ± 1.0* 6.7 ± 1.7 1.48 Pleasure/Displeasure -2.3 ± 1.9* 1.2 ± 1.3 2.19 *Significantly greater than the moderate-load condition; RPE = Session Rating of Perceived Exertion All effect sizes are large and suggest greater ratings in the low-load condition. used, training to failure may be necessary, which could in turn lead to a lower frequency and volume due to elongated recovery. However, the recovery piece aside, I think the current study reveals the biggest practical limitation to lowload failure training, which is that lowload training is pretty miserable for most lifters. The results presented in this article clearly show greater acute fatigue and discomfort in the low-load condition versus the high-load condition (Table 3). Further, Figure 1 shows that the greater exertion, discomfort, and displeasure following training occurred in almost every subject, and the difference between conditions was pretty drastic for some individuals. I’m not sure if the difference in the outcome measures would be this drastic if the low-load condition trained shy of failure; however, if you train shy of failure with low loads, then hypertrophy may be suboptimal. From a practical perspective, can you imagine completing such a miserable training session 2-3 times per week over the long-term? Probably not. Therefore, I would suspect that adherence could become an issue by solely using low-load training, even if you could overcome the progressive overload limitation. Table 4 provides a quick list of the key practical limitations of using low-load training exclusively. Despite the previous paragraph’s justified negativity toward low-load training, there may be an appropriate time and place to utilize low-load training. As with many training topics, people tend to look at low-load or moderate/ high-load training as a binary decision, when of course it is not. Perhaps you enjoy loading a bit lighter, but just not all the time. Perhaps you have trouble add- Table 4 Practical limitations of using solely low-load training for hypertrophy High levels of discomfort, displeasure, and fatigue Requires close proximity to failure 21 A Individual subject ratings in each condition B 10 8 Discomfort scale 8 6 sRPE C 10 Pleasure / displeasure scale Figure 1 4 2 6 4 2 0 0 Low-Load Moderate-Load 5 3 1 -1 -3 -5 Low-Load Moderate-Load Low-Load Moderate-Load From Ribeiro et al. (1) Data are ratings for each individual subject in each condition sRPE = Session RPE ing weight to the bar for a 30RM over the long term, but you still want to use low loads at times. In these cases, you could easily train a muscle group twice per week, once with low loads and once with moderate loads a la daily undulating programming. Also, you could alternate low-load training blocks and moderate-load training blocks. These basic strategies would mitigate some of the limitations but still allow you to perform low-load training if you wish. Of course, low-load training could still be used on assistance movements more often than on the main lifts. Doing sets of 25-30 on curls, triceps extensions, and rows is more feasible and enjoyable to do consistently than on the squat or deadlift. So, you can certainly train with more typical loads and rep ranges on the main lifts and lower loads with high reps on assistance movements. I bet a lot of people do that anyway. To that point, this study took perceptual measures of the entire session, which included two main movements (bench press and hack squat) and one true assistance movement (lat pulldown). Therefore, it is entirely possible that the displeasure from the session solely stemmed from the main movements, specifically the hack squat. We cannot know if results would have been the same if this study only used assistance movements. Besides, if it is necessary to take low loads to failure to maximize muscle growth, then it is more feasible to train to failure consistently on assistance movements than on the main lifts. 22 APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS 1. Training to failure at low loads leads to greater perceived exertion, discomfort, and displeasure, on average, compared to moderate-load training. 2. Although data do exist showing that hypertrophy is similar when low-load training is compared to moderate-load training over an eight-week study, coaches and lifters should be mindful of the practical limitations of low-load training. 3. The limitations of low-load training include difficulty implementing progressive overload in the long-term, along with the low enjoyment factor reported in the present study. Therefore, when programming for yourself or others, consider what will lead to greater long-term adherence, and use low-load training appropriately rather than as a full-time standalone training strategy. Lastly, low-load training may be easier on the joints for some, or may be useful when recovering from an injury. I’m sure some who are returning from an injury can load a barbell to a certain point without aggravating the issue, but as the weight goes up, the injury becomes a bit bothersome. In this case, a block of lowload training may be beneficial in the same way blood-flow restriction training could be beneficial to avoid heavy loading. These aren’t necessarily standalone training strategies; however, they have their place when the time is right. I also don’t think that you would have to take the low loads to failure when coming back from an injury. First, staying shy of failure would help to avoid aggravating the injury, and second, if you weren’t doing the movement at all and now you are doing it, you should still see some progress upon the initial return to training. Next Steps As with anything, a very long-term (i.e. 6 months to a year) low-load versus moderate-load study would be great. Also, in a long-term study, it would be necessary to gather the perceptual responses that were examined in the presently reviewed study over time. Just because low-load training is more miserable acutely doesn’t mean that the enjoyment can’t increase over time. To be clear, I don’t think the enjoyment would increase, but if enjoyment did improve, that would enhance the case to utilize low-load training. For a more direct follow-up to the specific data discussed in this article, I’d like to see the acute exertion, discomfort, and displeasure/ pleasure responses for single-joint assistance movements in response to lowand moderate-load training. 23 References 1. Ribeiro AS, dos Santos ED, Nunes JP, Schoenfeld BJ. Acute Effects of Different Training Loads on Affective Responses in Resistance-trained Men. International journal of sports medicine. 2019 Sep 9.. 2. Schoenfeld BJ, Grgic J, Ogborn D, Krieger JW. Strength and hypertrophy adaptations between low-vs. high-load resistance training: a systematic review and meta-analysis. The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research. 2017 Dec 1;31(12):3508-23. 3. Pareja-Blanco F, Rodríguez-Rosell D, Aagaard P, Sánchez-Medina L, Ribas-Serna J, Mora-Custodio R, Otero-Esquina C, Yáñez-García JM, González-Badillo JJ. Time Course of Recovery From Resistance Exercise With Different Set Configurations. Journal of strength and conditioning research. 2018 Jul. █ 24 Study Reviewed: Effect of Betaine on Reducing Body Fat—A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Randomized Controlled Trials. Gao et al. (2019) Betaine May Promote Body Composition Improvements When Combined With Resistance Training BY E RI C T RE X LE R Betaine, which is found in beets, spinach, whole grains, and seafood, has been used to improve the body composition of livestock. To see if these effects also apply to humans, a recent meta-analysis sought to review the small body of literature evaluating betaine’s effects on fat loss in human subjects. Read on to find out if betaine might help you achieve your physique-related goals. 25 KEY POINTS 1. The current meta-analysis (1) summarized the literature evaluating the effects of betaine (trimethylglycine) supplementation on fat loss. 2. Betaine did not significantly alter body weight, body mass index, or waist circumference, but significantly reduced fat mass (−2.25kg) and body-fat percentage (−2.44%) in comparison to placebo. 3. Betaine (2.5g/day for ≥ 6 weeks) may modestly facilitate fat loss and lean mass accretion when combined with resistance training, and effects may be larger in males than females. However, few studies are available, so definitive conclusions cannot be drawn yet. B etaine (trimethylglycine) finds its way into pre-workout supplements from time to time, but it isn’t a particularly well-known supplement. Betaine can be found in many foods, such as beets, spinach, whole grains, and seafood, and average daily betaine intake is around 100-400mg in adults (1). Betaine has been used as an additive in the feed of livestock (2), with evidence suggesting that it reduces fat storage and increases meat yield (3), but there is relatively minimal research evaluating its effects on human body composition. The current meta-analysis (1) sought to summarize the currently available literature investigating the effects of betaine supplementation on several fat-related parameters in humans. The search revealed only six qualifying studies, which were all published between 2002 and 2018. According to the analysis, betaine supplementation (2.0-9.9g/day, over a span of 10 days to 24 weeks) did not significantly alter body weight, body mass index (BMI), or waist circumference. In contrast, betaine supplementation significantly reduced both fat mass (−2.25kg; 95% CI: −3.96, −0.54kg) and body-fat percentage (−2.44%; 95% CI: −4.20, −0.68%). In addition, the largest effects were observed in the two studies that implemented resistance training programs in conjunction with supplementation. This article discusses what we currently know about betaine supplementation and its likelihood of being a worthwhile supplement for lifters. Purpose and Hypotheses Purpose The purpose of the current study was to “provide an up-to-date evaluation of the roles of betaine in obesity.” To that end, the authors performed a meta-analysis on the randomized controlled trials evaluating the effects of betaine supplementation on various measures of adi- 26 posity, such as body weight, BMI, waist circumference, fat mass, and body-fat percentage. Hypotheses The authors hypothesized that “betaine reduces body fat in humans.” This hypothesis is most directly related to the studies that reported fat mass as an outcome, but obviously closely relates to the other outcomes analyzed (body weight, BMI, waist circumference, and body-fat percentage). Subjects and Methods The authors systematically reviewed the betaine literature, and only included studies that met the following criteria (1): 1. The study used adult subjects. 2. The study was a randomized controlled trial. 3. Betaine was the only intervention that differed between groups in the study. 4. The study provided enough information to extract the values of interest. The authors ended up with six qualifying studies, with 195 total subjects. Study durations ranged from 10 days to 24 weeks, and betaine doses ranged from 2.0g/day to 9.9g/day. Notably, only two studies involved a training component, and both utilized well-designed resis- tance training protocols (4, 5). For the analysis, the authors compared changes observed in the betaine group to changes observed in the placebo group for each study. Notably, some key details appear to be omitted from the methods. With this type of meta-analysis, the authors typically need to calculate a change score standard deviation. This almost always requires the authors to make an educated guess about the correlation between pre-test and posttest values; as I noted in a recent MASS article, this estimate can have a meaningful impact on the results of the analysis. Theoretically, I could do a bunch of reverse-engineering to figure it out, but for reasons I’ll discuss later, that would provide us with some information that isn’t particularly valuable. Findings The analysis for body weight revealed no statistically significant effect from betaine supplementation (−0.29kg; 95% CI: −1.48, 0.89kg). Similarly, the effect on BMI was not statistically significant (−0.10kg/m2; 95% CI: −5.1, 0.31kg/m2), nor was the effect on waist circumference (0.68cm; 95% CI: −1.72, 3.09cm). Notably, these outcomes are all fairly indirect measures of adiposity, and there were only three studies for the BMI analysis and two studies for the waist circumference analysis. For the more direct measures of adi- 27 Figure 1 Effects of betaine supplementation on fat mass Total body fat mass Study ID WMD (95% CI) % Weight Cholewa, 2018 -1.30 (-8.94, 6.34) 4.91 Cholewa, 2013 -3.20 (-4.96, -1.44) 68.83 Favero, 2011 -0.60 (-5.19, 3.99) 13.13 Schwab, 2002 0.70 (-3.90, 5.30) 13.12 Overall (I-squared = 6.6%, p = 0.360) -2.25 (-3.96, -0.54) 100.00 -8.94 0 8.94 The weighted mean difference (WMD) for fat mass was a reduction of 2.25kg, which was statistically significant posity, the results were a bit more promising. Betaine supplementation led to significant reductions for both fat mass (−2.25kg; 95% CI: −3.96, −0.54kg; Figure 1) and body-fat percentage (−2.44%; 95% CI: −4.20, −0.68%; Figure 2). Still, it’s important to note that there were only four studies available for each of these analyses. In addition, it looks like the fat mass results that are reported in the abstract and the text of the results do not match the results reported in the figure. I am going to assume that the values in the figure are the accurate values, because the figure was probably auto- matically generated by their software (and therefore less prone to typos), and the reported values in the text yield an asymmetrical confidence interval. Interpretation There are a couple of different ways to view a meta-analysis. When a body of literature is large and well-developed, you can use a meta-analysis to form something of a working “conclusion” about whether or not a treatment works, and to identify some critical criteria that increase or decrease its efficacy. How- 28 Figure 2 Effects of betaine supplementation on body-fat percentage Body fat percentage Study ID WMD (95% CI) % Weight Schwab, 2002 0.30 (-4.18, 4.78) 15.42 Favero, 2011 -0.40 (-6.31, 5.51) 8.86 Cholewa, 2018 -1.70 (-8.21, 4.81) 7.29 Cholewa, 2013 -3.40 (-5.53, -1.27) 68.44 Overall (I-squared = 0.0%, p = 0.435) -2.44 (-4.20, -0.68) 100.00 -8.21 0 8.21 The weighted mean difference (WMD) for body-fat percentage was a reduction of 2.44 percentage points, which was statistically significant. ever, when a body of literature is small, it’s basically an update about the current state of the literature. For the current meta-analysis, the small number of studies would preclude us from making anything resembling a firm conclusion. Six total studies made the cut, with anywhere from 2-5 studies used in each individual analysis. When we look at the most direct body composition outcomes a lifting or athletic population would be interested in (fat mass and body-fat percentage), a couple of studies are particularly notable. For both outcomes, the studies by Cholewa in 2013 (4) and 2018 (5) show the largest effects, and these also happen to be the only studies done in young, healthy subjects performing resistance training while supplementing. More importantly, the meta-analysis is weighted, meaning that the studies do not all contribute equally to the overall analysis. For both fat mass and body-fat percentage, the studies by Cholewa collectively account for 70-75% of the weight of the analysis. So, from a theoretical perspective, we’d be most interested in the results of the Cholewa studies, as the sampled popu- 29 Figure 3 Effects of betaine on arm cross-sectional area (4) * 60 Arm CSA (m2) 50 40 Pre-treatment 30 Post-treatment 20 10 0 Placebo Betaine Values estimated via girth and skinfold measurements, in resistance-trained men lation and intervention are most relevant to the MASS readership. From a mathematical perspective, we’re still most interested in the results of the Cholewa studies, as they are largely driving the meta-analysis results for fat mass and body-fat percentage. I previously mentioned that I didn’t have a keen interest in reverse-engineering the meta-analysis statistics; this is because we’ve got more to gain from just directly looking at the Cholewa studies that are most relevant to us and that are pulling the weight of the analysis anyway. In the spirit of full disclosure, I’ve collaborated with Dr. Cholewa (but not on any of the studies in this article), and I consider him a friend. Nonetheless, I think I can give an unbiased summary of his studies, and my choice to focus on them is purely based on the study characteristics and mathematical considerations described above. So, let’s take a look at them. Key studies The first of the two Cholewa studies was published in 2013 (4), and the subjects were 23 resistance trained men between the ages of 18 and 35 who were able to bench press at least 100% of body weight and squat at least 125% of 30 Figure 4 Effects of betaine on lean body mass (4) 80 * 75 70 LBM (kg) 65 60 55 Pre-treatment 50 Post-treatment 45 40 35 30 Placebo Betaine Values estimated via skinfold measurements, in resistance-trained men body weight. Subjects were randomly assigned to consume betaine (2.5g/day) or placebo for six weeks, with groups matched based on training experience and body-fat percentage. Throughout the study, participants completed a periodized training program consisting of three, two-week microcycles that involved two upper-body and two lower-body sessions per week. The most notable outcome variables of interest included training volume, lean mass, fat mass, body-fat percentage, vertical jump, bench press one-rep max (1RM), squat 1RM, and cross-sectional area of the arm and thigh musculature. Body composition was estimated based on skinfold measurements, and cross-sectional areas were estimated using a method that combines skinfold measurements with limb circumferences. In terms of training volume, betaine did not consistently outperform placebo for bench press or squat. Betaine did not significantly alter thigh cross-sectional area, but arm cross-sectional area was significantly increased in the betaine group (Figure 3), as was lean body mass (Figure 4). The betaine group also had favorable (and statistically significant) fat loss results; the betaine group lost 2.9kg of fat 31 mass and 3.2 body-fat percentage points, compared to a gain of 0.3kg and 0.2 percentage points in the placebo group. Results were not significantly different for vertical jump, bench press 1RM, or squat 1RM. In summary, the study showed favorable effects of betaine on both lean mass and fat mass, but with pretty negligible effects on performance. The second of the Cholewa studies was published in 2018 (5). This study featured a sample of 36 untrained, college-aged female participants, with 23 of them completing the entire study. Subjects were randomly assigned to consume betaine (2.5g/day) or placebo for nine weeks. Throughout the study, subjects completed three days per week of progressive resistance exercise, with two lower-body days and one upper-body day per week. The program was split into two mesocycles, which were separated by a week of active rest during week five. Body composition was measured via air displacement plethysmography (BodPod), rectus femoris thickness was measured via ultrasound, and performance outcomes included vertical jump, bench press 1RM, and squat 1RM. For performance outcomes, results of the study indicated that all performance measurements improved over time, with no difference between betaine and placebo groups. In contrast, betaine significantly altered some of the body composition outcomes. The betaine group had IT WOULD APPEAR THAT BETAINE INFLUENCES BODY COMPOSITION MORE SO THAN PERFORMANCE, AND ITS EFFECTS ARE MORE LIKELY TO BE OBSERVED WHEN DOSED AT 2.5G/DAY OR MORE FOR AT LEAST SIX WEEKS. a larger reduction in fat mass (-2.0kg) and body-fat percentage (-3.3%) than the placebo group (-0.8kg and -1.7%), but betaine did not significantly alter changes in lean mass or rectus femoris muscle thickness. So, while betaine had favorable effects in this study (5), the effects were smaller in magnitude (in comparison to the placebo group) than the effects observed in the 2013 study with male subjects (4). To be clear, both of Cholewa’s studies utilized durations of six weeks or longer. Creatine can induce some pretty rapid effects with a short-term loading phase, but that’s not the case for betaine. Del Favero et al (6) investigated the effects of short-term (10-day) supplementation with betaine or creatine in untrained 32 subjects. Unsurprisingly, 20g/day of creatine increased muscle phosphocreatine content, along with squat and bench press strength and power. In contrast, 2g/day of betaine did not increase muscle phosphocreatine content, nor did it improve squat or bench press outcomes. It would appear that betaine influences body composition more so than performance, and its effects are more likely to be observed when dosed at 2.5g/day or more for at least six weeks, in conjunction with a well-structured resistance training program. Mechanisms While there is obviously value in observing outcomes in response to betaine supplementation, it’s important to identify the mechanisms that may underlie or explain the effect we’re observing. This is especially important when very few studies are available; if we observe something a couple of times, but can’t fathom a reason why, we’re more likely to have doubts that we’re observing a “real” effect. Authors of the current meta-analysis did a fantastic job laying out the potential mechanisms by which betaine could potentially reduce body-fat percentage (1). The proposed mechanisms include: 1. Betaine may increase lipolysis and reduce lipogenesis by influencing a ton of pathways associated with fat metabolism by influencing PPAR-α, SREBP-1c, acetyl-CoA carboxylase, carnitine accretion, fatty acid synthase, and more 2. Betaine may decrease the amount of triglycerides that fat cells take up from circulating lipoproteins by reducing the expression of lipoprotein lipase. 3. Betaine may increase mitochondrial content of liver cells and fat cells, and make white fat cells more like brown fat cells, which are more thermogenic. 4. Betaine may decrease levels of homocysteine via transmethylation to methionine; there is evidence to suggest that this may promote lipolysis and, possibly, the hypertrophic effects of betaine (5). 5. Betaine may promote protein synthesis (and, by extension, muscle hypertrophy) by increasing growth hormone secretion, increasing insulin-like growth factor 1 (IGF-1) secretion, stimulating the mTOR pathway, or reducing homocysteine thiolactone. At this point in time, we’ve got a couple of pretty relevant studies demonstrating beneficial effects of betaine on body composition, and a handful of pretty plausible mechanisms by which betaine might be favorably influencing fat loss and lean mass accretion. We’re far from having a conclusive body of literature, but for lifters with physique-oriented goals who like to experiment fairly lib- 33 erally with dietary supplements, there’s probably enough preliminary evidence to pique their interest. sibility of a sex-based difference cannot be automatically discounted. In fact, as explained by Cholewa et al (5), there are some plausible sex-linked mechanisms worth looking into. Activity of the enzyme that breaks down betaine to assist in the transmethylation of homocysteine to methionine is increased in the presence of estrogen (7). When the activity of this enzyme increases, it’s possible that less betaine will be available for muscle uptake (5). Compared to males, females appear to have lower plasma levels of betaine (8), so differential responses to betaine supplementation would not be implausible. Nonetheless, we shouldn’t get carried away; the effects in Cholewa’s male study were a bit larger than Cholewa’s female study, but betaine still had a beneficial effect in the female study, and there were several notable methodological differences between the studies. Potential sex differences Conclusions THE RESULTS OF THIS META-ANALYSIS, AND MORE SPECIFICALLY THE RESULTS OF THE STUDIES BY CHOLEWA ET AL SUGGEST THAT BETAINE MAY HAVE MODEST BUT FAVORABLE EFFECTS ON BODY COMPOSITION. It is noteworthy that the results from Cholewa’s 2013 study (4), in terms of fat loss and hypertrophy, were more favorable than Cholewa’s 2018 study (5). There are several methodological considerations that might explain this difference, including differences in the training programs, methods of body composition measurement, and the training status of participants, among others. However, the 2013 study was conducted with male subjects and the 2018 study was conducted with female subjects, so the pos- On the topic of dietary supplements, a prolific sports nutrition researcher named Dr. Ron Maughan has said, “If it works, it’s probably banned; if it’s not banned, it probably doesn’t work. But there may be a few exceptions.” My outlook on supplements isn’t quite as skeptical, particularly when it comes to performance, but I bring up this quote for a very specific application: If you’re looking for a dietary supplement that’s going to have massive effects on fat loss, and isn’t going to be unsafe and swiftly banned, you might 34 APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS Based on the small number of studies available, it’s hard to draw firm conclusions. Nonetheless, the limited evidence available suggests that 2.5g/day of betaine for ≥ 6 weeks may facilitate fat loss and lean mass gains when combined with resistance training. It is possible that the effects may be larger in males than females, but more research is needed to confirm this potential sex-based difference. want to temper your optimism. However, the results of this meta-analysis, and more specifically the results of the studies by Cholewa et al (4, 5), suggest that betaine may have modest but favorable effects on body composition. Results indicate that 2.5g/day for at least six weeks, in conjunction with a well-structured resistance training program, may facilitate increases in lean mass and reductions in fat mass. Effects on performance don’t seem to be as notable, and effects could potentially be smaller in females than males. However, given the very small number of studies available, these conclusions should all be viewed as very, very tentative. Hopefully we’ll be seeing more studies on betaine in the future, so we can either confirm or revise our current understanding. Finally, it’s prudent to note that you could feasibly increase your dietary betaine intake by 2.5g/day without using supplements. The average human dietary betaine intake is roughly 100-400mg/day (9), but a number of foods provide pretty substantial betaine doses per 100g serving (wheat bran = 1339mg, wheat germ = 1241mg, spinach = 600-645mg, beets = 114-297mg, pretzels = 237mg, shrimp = 219mg, and wheat bread = 201mg) (10). So, if you’re curious about exploring the potential benefits of betaine, but not curious enough to pay for a supplement, you could always try working some extra spinach and wheat germ into your diet (conveniently, both are pretty easy and painless additions to protein shakes). Next Steps At this point, we basically just need more data in general. We’ve still got plenty of work to do before we can conclusively say that betaine supplementation reliably enhances fat loss or muscle hypertrophy, let alone give confident recommendations for optimal dosing strategies. Ideally, more randomized controlled trials will be conducted in conjunction with resistance training programs, and it’d be great if they took a closer look at the possibility of more favorable responses in males than females. 35 References 1. Gao X, Zhang H, Guo X-F, Li K, Li S, Li D. Effect of Betaine on Reducing Body Fat-A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Randomized Controlled Trials. Nutrients. 2019 Oct 16;11(10). 2. Eklund M, Bauer E, Wamatu J, Mosenthin R. Potential nutritional and physiological functions of betaine in livestock. Nutr Res Rev. 2005 Jun;18(1):31–48. 3. Matthews JO, Southern LL, Higbie AD, Persica MA, Bidner TD. Effects of betaine on growth, carcass characteristics, pork quality, and plasma metabolites of finishing pigs. J Anim Sci. 2001 Mar;79(3):722–8. 4. Cholewa JM, Wyszczelska-Rokiel M, Glowacki R, Jakubowski H, Matthews T, Wood R, et al. Effects of betaine on body composition, performance, and homocysteine thiolactone. J Int Soc Sports Nutr. 2013 Aug 22;10:39. 5. Cholewa JM, Hudson A, Cicholski T, Cervenka A, Barreno K, Broom K, et al. The effects of chronic betaine supplementation on body composition and performance in collegiate females: a double-blind, randomized, placebo controlled trial. J Int Soc Sports Nutr. 2018 Jul 31;15:37. 6. del Favero S, Roschel H, Artioli G, Ugrinowitsch C, Tricoli V, Costa A, et al. Creatine but not betaine supplementation increases muscle phosphorylcreatine content and strength performance. Amino Acids. 2012 Jun;42(6):2299–305. 7. Finkelstein JD, Kyle W, Harris BJ. Methionine metabolism in mammals. Regulation of homocysteine methyltransferases in rat tissue. Arch Biochem Biophys. 1971 Sep;146(1):84–92. 8. Lever M, Atkinson W, George PM, Chambers ST. Sex differences in the control of plasma concentrations and urinary excretion of glycine betaine in patients attending a lipid disorders clinic. Clin Biochem. 2007 Nov;40(16–17):1225–31. 9. Obeid R. The Metabolic Burden of Methyl Donor Deficiency with Focus on the Betaine Homocysteine Methyltransferase Pathway. Nutrients. 2013 Sep 9;5(9):3481–95. 10. Craig SA. Betaine in human nutrition. Am J Clin Nutr. 2004 Sep 1;80(3):539–49. █ 36 Study Reviewed: Bench Press at Full Range of Motion Produces Greater Neuromuscular Adaptations than Partial Executions After Prolonged Resistance Training. Martínez-Cava et al. (2019) Bench Press Range of Motion: An Exception to the Principle of Specificity? BY G RE G NUC KO LS Much of what we know about range of motion specificity comes from single-joint studies and squat studies. When we branch out to the bench press, things get more complicated. 37 KEY POINTS 1. Subjects trained for 10 weeks, doing either full bench press reps or one of two partial ranges of motion (⅓ reps or ⅔ reps). They tested strength and velocity at all three ranges of motion pre- and post-training. 2. Unexpectedly, the full range of motion group tended to improve the most in all measures at all ranges of motion, not just the full range of motion measures. The ⅓ range of motion group tended to improve the least in all measures, even for the ⅓ range of motion tests. 3. While the principle of specificity has a tremendous amount of support, we need to remember that it’s a principle, not an iron-clad law of the universe. In the interpretation section, I’ll discuss when it may or may not apply. O ne of the first things you learn about when you start consuming strength training content is the principle of specificity. The principle of specificity has wide-reaching implications, but one of the well-known applications is range of motion specificity: you gain the most strength in the range of motion you train for. In other words, if I want to improve my deep squat, I’d want to do deep squats, but if I want to improve my half squat, I’d be better off doing half squats. However, we need to keep in mind that the principle of specificity is more of a strong heuristic rather than an ironclad law of the universe. Sometimes, it doesn’t apply. And when it doesn’t, we can learn something by thinking through the factors that may be able to “override” such an important principle. In the present study (1), three groups of subjects trained the bench press through either a full range of motion, a 2/3 range of motion, or a 1/3 range of motion, with strength and veloity testing for all three ranges of motion pre- and post-training. The full range of motion group improved the most for tests through all ranges of motion, while the 1/3 range of motion group got the worst results, including on the tests in the range of motion they were actually training. The interpretation section will dig into factors that may explain why the results of this study run counter to what we’d expect, given the principle of specificity. Purpose and Hypotheses Purpose The purpose of the study was to investigate the effects of bench press range of motion on strength and velocity adaptations. 38 Table 1 Subject characteristics Number Age (years) Height (cm) Weight (kg) Body fat (%) 1RM smith machine bench press (kg) Bench press relative to body mass 49 males 24.0 ± 4.7 176.2 ± 8.4 73.4 ± 9.9 10.6 ± 4.3% 71.8 ± 14.2 0.98 ± 0.18 Hypotheses No hypotheses were given. Subjects and Methods Subjects 49 young men who had been benching 2-4 times per week for at least 6 months completed the study. More details about the subjects can be seen in Table 1. Experimental Design Before any performance testing, the subjects underwent nine familiarization sessions: three sessions of 1/3 bench reps, three sessions of 2/3 bench press, and three sessions of full bench reps. After familiarization was completed, the subjects underwent three testing sessions. In each session, they completed a load-velocity profile and a 1RM test with one bench press range of motion (i.e. 1/3 reps, 2/3 reps, or full reps). The order of the testing sessions was randomized for each subject, and the same order was repeated for post-testing. The load-velocity testing started at 20kg, with loads increased by 10kg per set until mean propulsive velocity (2) fell below 0.5 m/s, after which time loads increased by 2.5-5 kg per set until a 1RM was reached. Subjects performed 3 reps per set with light loads (<50% 1RM), 2 reps per set with moderate loads (5080% 1RM), and 1 rep per set with heavy loads (>80% 1RM). After the initial testing sessions were completed, subjects were assigned to one of four groups in a counterbalanced fashion based on pre-training bench press strength. One group trained doing 1/3 reps, one group trained doing 2/3 reps, one group trained doing full reps, and a control group didn’t train at all. The three experimental groups trained twice per week for 10 weeks, using a linearly periodized training program. Loads were selected and adjusted using velocity targets that were intended to correspond with the target intensity for the day. Details of the training program can be seen in Table 2. After 10 weeks of training, the performance tests were repeated. The researchers did a good job of standardizing as many aspects of the study as possible. The subjects trained using a Smith machine (this probably wasn’t necessary, but it does make velocity data a little more accurate since all of the movement is completely vertical, though this accuracy comes at the cost of a bit of ecological validity), grip 39 Table 2 Descriptive characteristics of the resistance training programs Scheduled Week 1 Week 2 Week 3 Week 4 Week 5 Week 6 Week 7 Week 8 Week 9 Week 10 %1RM ~60 ~60 ~65 ~65 ~70 ~70 ~75 ~75 ~80 ~80 4x8 5x8 4x8 5x8 4x6 5x6 4x5 5x5 4x4 5x4 Group BPFull 0.76 0.76 0.68 0.68 0.61 0.61 0.54 0.54 0.47 0.47 Group BP2/3 0.64 0.64 0.57 0.57 0.51 0.51 0.45 0.45 0.40 0.40 Group BP1/3 0.48 0.48 0.43 0.43 0.38 0.38 0.34 0.34 0.30 0.30 Sets x reps Target MPV (m•s ) -1 BPFull = full bench press; BP2/3 = two-thirds bench press; BP1/3= one-third bench press Target MPV = maximal intended velocity repetition performed at the end of each session’s warm-up to ensure that the load (kg) to be used matched the velocity associated with the intended % 1RM width was standardized (5-7 cm outside of shoulder width), and safety bars were used to ensure that range of motion was appropriate and consistent for each rep. The subjects lowered the bar to safety pins and paused for two seconds before pressing each rep, making the demarcation between the eccentric and concentric crystal clear, thus theoretically improving velocity measurements and ensuring a consistent range of motion. Furthermore, subjects were instructed to maintain a velocity of 0.45-0.65m/s for their eccentrics (with the aid of visual and audio feedback from the velocity device used in the study), and to press each concentric as explosively as possible. Findings Surprisingly, the group doing full reps tended to improve the most in all measures, and the 1/3 reps group tended to improve the least. According to the principle of specificity, one would have expected that the full reps group would improve the most at full reps, the 2/3 reps group would improve the most at 2/3 reps, and the 1/3 reps group would improve the most at 1/3 reps. However, that was not the case. Figure 1 tells the story. Note that not all differences between groups were statistically significant (and there are 72 potential pairwise comparisons; it’s not worth going through all of them one by one), but the overall pattern is crystal clear. Interpretation Upon reading the title of this article, I thought it was going to be a straightforward article that I could review in my sleep. “There’s this thing called the ‘principle of specificity,’ here are all of the studies backing it, and the present study adds one more to the pile.” In and out, easy peasy. However, this study does not fit that narrative. Training through a full range of motion was best for improving performance through a full range of motion, but training through a partial range of motion wasn’t best for improving 40 Figure 1 Changes in relative strength ratio (A) and velocity developed against all (B) low (C) and high (D) loads common to pre-test and post-test 1RM/BM MPVALL Training group assigned BPFULL ES = 0.8 - 1.2 BP2/3 ES = 0.30 - 0.6 BP1/3 CON ES = 0.1 - 0.4 BPFULL ES = -0.3 - -0.5 0.20 25 20 *#† #† *#† 15 #† #† 10 #† 5 † † † 0 -5 MPV Change (m·s-1) 1RM/BM Change (%) 30 Training group assigned 0.15 BP2/3 ES = 0.7 - 2.0 BPFULL A #† #† 0.10 #† † BPFULL BP2/3 † † 0.00 BP2/3 BP1/3 Exercise Tested MPV>50% 1RM BP2/3 Training group assigned BP1/3 ES = 0.3 - 0.8 CON ES = 0.3 - 0.6 *#† #† *#† #† BPFULL ES = -0.1 - -0.2 *#† 0.10 † 0.05 0.00 -0.05 0.20 MPV Change (m·s-1) BPFULL ES = 1.5 - 0.7 BP1/3 Exercise Tested B MPV<50% 1RM MPV Change (m·s-1) #† † 0.05 Training group assigned -0.10 C ES = 0.0 - -0.4 *#† -15 0.15 CON ES = 0.4 - 0.7 -0.05 -10 0.20 BP1/3 ES = 0.4 - 0.7 0.15 0.10 0.05 BP2/3 ES = 1.0 - 2.4 BP1/3 ES = 0.9 - 1.7 CON ES = 0.3 - 0.0 ES = -0.9 - -0.6 *#† #† #† #† † † #† #† † 0.00 -0.05 -0.10 BPFULL BP2/3 BP1/3 Exercise Tested D BPFULL BP2/3 BP1/3 Exercise Tested BPFULL = full bench press; BP2/3 = two-thirds bench press; BP1/3 = one-third bench press; CON = control; * = significant differences when compared with the BP2/3 group; # = significant differences when compared with the BP1/3 group; † = signficiant differences when compared with the CON group; ES = range of effect size for each resistance training group Data are presented as mean ± SEM performance through a partial range of motion. In fact, the 1/3 range of motion group had the worst gains though the 1/3 range of motion in three of the four measures. So, I can’t just type out my stock “principle of specificity” spiel and call it a day. However, while that makes this article more difficult to interpret, it also makes it much more fun. Before I get ahead of myself, let’s just recap the results: a longer range of motion seems to be the way to go for … everything, at least in this study. When I thought about it for a moment, I realized that the key studies people generally cite to support the concept of range 41 TRAINING THROUGH A FULL RANGE OF MOTION WAS BEST FOR IMPROVING PERFORMANCE THROUGH A FULL RANGE OF MOTION, BUT TRAINING THROUGH A PARTIAL RANGE OF MOTION WASN’T BEST FOR IMPROVING PERFORMANCE THROUGH A PARTIAL RANGE OF MOTION. of motion specificity use the squat (3) or single joint exercises (4), rather than the bench press. What about the bench press specifically? There are three other relevant studies, but only one of them tested strength through multiple ranges of motion. Clark et al compared the effects of benching through a full ROM against benching through a variable ROM (a combination of full reps, 1/4 reps, 1/2 reps, and 3/4 reps; 5). Neither group experienced significant increases in force output through a full range of motion (which calls into question the usefulness of the training program used in the study), but the variable ROM group had a larger increase in force output through a 1/2 rep ROM. Two studies by Massey et al were nearly identical, except that one of the studies used male participants (6) while the other used female participants (7). Both studies had three groups: one trained through a full range of motion, one group trained through a partial range of motion (not lowering the bar below the sticking point), and one group did half of their sets through a full ROM and half though a partial ROM. In both studies, full-ROM bench press strength was the only outcome measure. In the study on males, the full and partial ROM groups had similar increases in strength, while the group doing both full and partial reps tended to gain a bit less strength. In the study on females, on the other hand, the full ROM group had the largest increase in 1RM strength, while the partial ROM and mixed ROM groups had slightly smaller increases. Putting all four of these studies together, it becomes clear that doing full ROM training is important for building strength through a full ROM on bench press (benching through a full ROM built strength through a full ROM or through the bottom of the ROM as well as or better than all other conditions in all studies). However, ROM specificity isn’t as clearly supported. In the present study (1), benching through a full ROM built more strength through a partial ROM than benching through a partial ROM did. In the Clark study, while par- 42 Figure 2 Theoretical differences in squat and bench press strength curves Force output relative to the weakest point in the lift Bench strength Squat strength 200% 175% 150% 125% 100% 0% 20% 40% 60% 80% Percentage of concentric completed tials built more strength through the top part of the ROM, benching through a full ROM failed to increase strength off the chest to a greater degree than doing partials. In the Massey study on males, benching through a full ROM and doing partials proved equally effective for building strength through a full ROM. And finally, in the Massey study on females, the group using mixed ROMs (and thus still doing some training through a full ROM) failed to increase full ROM strength more than the group only doing partials. All in all, it’s a murky picture. So, what might explain these results? If the principle of specificity is so well-supported, why does ROM speci- ficity in the bench press look so iffy? My first thought is that the magnitude of strength fluctuations throughout a full range of motion probably has an impact. In other words, are you 20% stronger at the top of a rep than at the bottom of a rep, or are you 100% stronger? In the case of squats, it’s not terribly uncommon to be able to lift WAY more for partials than for full reps. Speaking from experience, I’ve done quarter squats with 1000+lb when my 1RM through a full ROM was closer to ~650, and I’m sure that disparity would be even larger if I trained quarter squats with as much focus as I put into full ROM squats. With bench, on the other hand, there’s 43 maybe a 15% difference between my full-ROM bench press and the heaviest weight I could use for high pin presses or board presses. The exact ratios may differ for you, but I’d be surprised if that same principle didn’t apply to almost everyone reading this: there’s a bigger gap between partial-ROM strength and full-ROM strength in the squat than in the bench press. By extension, one would then assume that full-ROM bench press would have a larger effect on partial-ROM bench press strength than full-ROM squatting would have on partial-ROM squat strength. If an appropriate load for full-ROM bench press is 200lb and an appropriate weight for partials is 230, doing reps at 200 is still probably heavy enough to do something for partial-ROM strength. However if an appropriate load for full-ROM squats is 400lb and an appropriate load for half squats is 650, doing full-ROM reps with 400 probably isn’t doing much to improve partial-ROM strength directly (beyond simply building more muscle mass). The opposite principle may also be true – you may get better carryover from partial-ROM training to full-ROM strength when the strength curve of a movement is flatter. Half reps on bench press feel more similar to full-ROM bench press than half reps on squat feel when compared to full-ROM squats. If you’ve ever converted an athlete from partial-ROM training to full-ROM training, you’ve probably seen this firsthand – their first session benching through a full ROM is a little humbling because they need to take some weight off the bar, but they still perform reasonably well, and the weights they can handle are at least somewhat comparable to the loads they were using before for partials, unless they were previously using a very partial ROM. With squats, on the other hand, shifting from half squats to full squats often requires a complete rebuild of their squatting mechanics, and necessitates slashing their training weights at least in half. In the present study (1), another factor in play is how the subjects actually performed their reps. To keep ranges of motion consistent, the subjects benched to pins, allowing the bar to briefly rest on the pins between sets. This is a great way to ensure that the range of motion was consistent, but it possibly decreases ecological validity a bit. When you switch between the eccentric and concentric portions of a lift, if there’s not a physical impediment to help you decelerate the bar (i.e. the floor when deadlifting), you get a rather large spike in force output when transitioning between lowering the bar and explosively lifting the bar. Lowering the bar to pins negates that spike in force output. That does mirror the way that many people would do partial bench press reps (i.e. pin press or board press), but it may not capture all of the ways that someone could apply partial range of motion training when bench pressing (namely, simply revers- 44 ing each rep yourself without touching your chest). It’s plausible that partial ROM exercises that require the athlete to actively decelerate and reverse the load build more partial ROM strength than exercises, like pin presses, that allow another physical object to decelerate the load. Finally, it’s important to think about these results conceptually, rather than simply accepting one study as the final word on the topic. We have good research indicating that longer ranges of motion tend to lead to more muscle growth (3, 8). Since the subjects weren’t very well-trained, they likely still had plenty of room to grow more muscle. Hypertrophy wasn’t assessed in this study, but I don’t think we’d be unjustified to assume that that full range of motion group likely experienced the most muscle growth. That could be enough to explain the superior gains in performance, even through partial ranges of motion. Reasoning by analogy, two studies on squatting and jump performance immediately come to mind. In one study on untrained lifters (3), deep squats led to greater improvements in jump height than half squats (the positioning of half squats more closely mirrors jumping mechanics than the positioning of full squats). In another study on high level athletes (9), half squats led to larger improvements in jump height than full squats. In my opinion, the most likely explanation is that for untrained or semi-trained athletes, more hypertrophy IS IT POSSIBLE THAT OPTIMIZING TECHNIQUE FOR SHORT-TERM PERFORMANCE COULD ACTUALLY LIMIT LONG-TERM DEVELOPMENT, ASSUMING YOU DO MOST OF YOUR BENCH PRESS TRAINING WITH A COMPETITIONSTYLE SET-UP? I CERTAINLY THINK IT’S POSSIBLE. can occur, and that muscular development can lead to robust performance improvements. For highly trained lifters, on the other hand, much less hypertrophy can occur, so optimizing for movement specificity rather than hypertrophy leads to larger performance improvements. Thus, if this study was repeated on trained lifters, I would expect that specificity would apply to a greater degree, with the full ROM group having the largest strength gains through a full ROM, the 2/3 ROM group having the largest strength gains through a 2/3 ROM, and the 1/3 ROM group having the largest strength gains through a 1/3 ROM. 45 APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS While the principle of specificity is a cornerstone of training theory, it’s important to remember that it’s a principle, not an iron-clad law. Specifically, range of motion specificity may not hold up quite as well in the bench press as in the squat. For longterm strength development, benching through a longer range of motion than your competition-style setup may be worth a shot if you plateau. Finally, I’d just like to touch on something the authors of the present study mention in their discussion (1). They propose that powerlifters are actually training the bench press through a partial range of motion, due to arching, taking a wide grip, and specifically aiming to minimize range of motion, and that they may be able to gain more strength if they did more of their training through a purposefully longer range of motion. I think that’s an idea worth at least considering. Based on competition definition, a “full” range of motion is any range of motion that allows the bar to touch your chest and lock out, as long as your grip width doesn’t exceed 81cm. But is that REALLY a full range of motion biomechanically? I’d argue that it isn’t. For example, a close grip bench with a smaller arch involves more elbow flexion, and a greater combination of shoulder extension and horizontal abduction than a typical competition-style bench, so your prime movers clearly aren’t going through their full range of motion with a wide-grip, arched bench press. Is it possible that optimizing technique for short-term performance could actually limit long-term development, assuming you do most of your bench press training with a competition-style set-up? I certainly think it’s possible. For what it’s worth, that matches my experience (I always tend to make better bench progress when I’m doing a lot of cambered bar bench or close-grip bench with a smaller arch). It matches the anecdote of Mike MacDonald, who may be the most successful bench presser of all time; he simultaneously held the bench press records in four different weight classes at one point and swore by cambered bar bench press. More recently, Josh Bryant’s lifters have been very successful on the bench press, while primarily benching with pretty narrow grip widths (at least by powerlifting standards). Jeremy Hoornstra and Julius Maddox are his two most successful lifters, owning the all-time bench press records at 242, 275, and superheavyweight. At the very least, if your bench press is plateaued, I think it’s worth considering doing some of your weekly bench press training with a technique that allows for a longer range of motion. 46 Next Steps I’d like to see more research looking at range of motion specificity in a wider array of exercises and in more advanced lifters. I’d also like to see a training study in powerlifters comparing a training program consisting solely of wide grip bench against a training program with pressing volume split evenly between wide-grip bench and close-grip bench. 47 References 1. Martínez-Cava A, Hernández-Belmonte A, Courel-Ibáñez J, Morán-Navarro R, González-Badillo JJ, Pallarés JG. Bench Press at Full Range of Motion Produces Greater Neuromuscular Adaptations Than Partial Executions After Prolonged Resistance Training. J Strength Cond Res. 2019 Sep 26. 2. Mean propulsive velocity is very similar to mean concentric velocity. The difference is that mean propulsive velocity trims off the end of each concentric when the bar decelerates prior to lockout before calculating average velocity, whereas mean concentric velocity is a measure of velocity for the entire concentric portion of each rep. At high loads, mean propulsive velocity and mean concentric velocity are similar. At low loads, mean propulsive velocity is a bit faster than mean concentric velocity since the bar needs more deceleration prior to lockout. For the purposes of this study, the differences don’t really matter, since both are valid and reliable measures and convey nearly identical information. 3. Bloomquist K, Langberg H, Karlsen S, Madsgaard S, Boesen M, Raastad T. Effect of range of motion in heavy load squatting on muscle and tendon adaptations. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2013 Aug;113(8):2133-42. 4. Valamatos MJ, Tavares F, Santos RM, Veloso AP, Mil-Homens P. Influence of full range of motion vs. equalized partial range of motion training on muscle architecture and mechanical properties. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2018 Sep;118(9):1969-1983. 5. Clark RA, Humphries B, Hohmann E, Bryant AL. The influence of variable range of motion training on neuromuscular performance and control of external loads. J Strength Cond Res. 2011 Mar;25(3):704-11. 6. Massey CD, Vincent J, Maneval M, Moore M, Johnson JT. An analysis of full range of motion vs. partial range of motion training in the development of strength in untrained men. J Strength Cond Res. 2004 Aug;18(3):518-21. 7. Massey CD, Vincent J, Maneval M, Johnson JT. Influence of range of motion in resistance training in women: early phase adaptations. J Strength Cond Res. 2005 May;19(2):409-11. 8. McMahon GE, Morse CI, Burden A, Winwood K, Onambélé GL. Impact of range of motion during ecologically valid resistance training protocols on muscle size, subcutaneous fat, and strength. J Strength Cond Res. 2014 Jan;28(1):245-55. 9. Rhea, M., Kenn, J., Peterson, M., et al. Joint-Angle Specific Strength Adaptations Influence Improvements in Power in Highly Trained Athletes. Human Movement, 2016, 17(1), pp. 4349. █ 48 Study Reviewed: Heart Rate Variability, Neuromuscular and Perceptual Recovery Following Resistance Training. Flatt et al. (2019) The Usefulness of Heart Rate Variability in Resistance Training is Tenuous BY MIC HAE L C . ZO URD O S The potential benefit of heart rate variability in resistance training is its ability to track recovery and be used as a readiness indicator. However, does heart rate variability actually correlate with performance? This article reviews a recent study and examines the totality of the literature to provide some answers. 49 KEY POINTS 1. This study examined if the recovery pattern of heart rate variability (HRV) was related to the recovery of perceived recovery, soreness, movement velocity, and vertical jump power following a damaging training session. 2. Recovery of HRV did not correlate with any other metric. Therefore, it does not seem wise to use these recovery metrics as proxies for each other. 3. Before we can confidently implement HRV as a recovery metric or readiness indicator, there needs to be more data showing that changes in HRV are actually correlated with resistance training performance. M any different metrics are thrown around to track recovery; however, the metrics are often conflicting, making it hard to choose one. It is also often accepted that the more technical a metric is, the better it is at gauging recovery. An example of this is heart rate variability (HRV), which measures the variability of the time between heart beats. As reviewed by Eric Helms in a previous MASS issue, when used as a readiness indicator to implement a flexible training template, HRV-guided training did not yield greater strength or hypertrophy compared to a fixed training template (2). That study doesn’t negate the possibility that HRV could have some benefit, but even if HRV is useful, it has some practical limitations. Thus, if it can be reliably correlated with a more practical metric, then that practical metric could be used as an HRV proxy. This observational study (1) had 10 trained men perform 6 sets to failure with 90% of a 10-repetition maximum (RM) on the squat, bench press, and lat pulldown. Then, recovery was tracked immediately post-training and 24 and 48 hours post-training with HRV and various other metrics (vertical jump power, squat and bench press velocity, perceived recovery, and perceived soreness). Recovery of HRV did not correlate with any other metrics. Further, the rate of recovery for each metric varied considerably between individuals. Based on these findings, it does not seem that any practical recovery metric can be used interchangeably with HRV. This article will not only examine the present findings, but will also discuss why a recovery metric must actually correlate with performance to be used as a readiness indicator, and what metrics fit these criteria in the literature. Purpose and Hypotheses Purpose The purpose of this study was to examine if the time course of HRV recov- 50 Table 1 Subject characteristics Subjects Age (years) Body Mass (kg) Height (cm) Training experience 10 men 24.4 ± 4.5 94.8 ± 21.4 180.7 ± 6.7 > 1 year Data shown are group means ± SD Subject characteristics from Flatt et al. 2019 (1) ery was correlated with various other recovery metrics following a damaging training session. Hypotheses No hypotheses were provided. Subjects and Methods Subjects 10 men with at least 1 year of training experience participated. All subjects played rugby in the surrounding area (Savannah, Georgia, USA). The available descriptive details of the subjects are in Table 1. Study Protocol This study was conducted over four visits to the lab. Each visit is detailed below. Visit 1: Baseline HRV and 10RM testing on the squat, bench press, and lat pulldown. Visit 2: Occurred 5 days after visit 1. This visit involved a damaging training session with recovery metrics taken immediately before and after training with the exception of HRV, which was as- sessed immediately upon waking up in the morning. The recovery metrics used were the perceived recovery status and soreness scales (0-10 scales), vertical jump, and average concentric velocity against a load that corresponded to a velocity of 1.0 m/s when the lifter was fresh. The training session, which was 6 sets to failure on the squat, bench press, and lat pulldown, was performed at a load of 90% of a 10RM. If we assume that a 10RM is generally about 75% of 1RM, then the load used in this study would have been ~67.5% of 1RM. Visits 3 and 4: All recovery metrics were tested to assess the time course of recovery on each day. HRV was again tested after waking in the morning, and the other recovery metrics were tested at the same time as they were tested at pre-training in visit 2. Heart Rate Variability (HRV) Eric has covered HRV before in depth, but before we move onto the findings and interpretation, I want to provide a refresher. In short, HRV is the variability of the time between each heartbeat. In general, a lower HRV will be seen when someone is under-recovered. In the reviewed study, HRV was measured in 51 Table 2 Load used and reps performed Exercise Load used (90% of 10RM) (kg) Average total reps performed per subject Squat 94.7 ± 12.7 62.7 ± 10.3 Bench press 81.1 ± 11.6 38.8 ± 4.5 Lat Pulldown 50.5 ± 6.6 42.2 ± 7.6 Data shown are group means ± SD Subject characteristics from Flatt et al. 2019 (1) both the standing and supine (lying on your back) position at each time point. Findings Table 2 shows the average load used on each exercise and the average reps performed by each subject. Time Course of Recovery All metrics demonstrated significant fatigue immediately post-training. At 24 hours post-training, both HRV metrics were no longer significantly different from baseline (p>0.05). At 48 hours, all metrics except for perceived recovery and soreness were no longer significantly different when compared to pre-training levels. The time course of recovery for each recovery metric is shown in Table 3. Correlations Between Recovery Metrics There was no significant correlation between the change in either HRV metric and the change in any other recovery metric at any time point. The specific r-values of the correlations are in Table 4. Interpretation On the surface, these results don’t tell us much other than the fact that all recovery metrics don’t track along the same exact time course. The premise of this study, however, is that HRV is an acceptable proxy for readiness to train. In other words, the rationale for this study is that when implementing a flexible template (read more here), HRV can be used to guide what training day to do, or when to train again following a damaging session. The problem is that little data exist showing HRV as an effective readiness indicator for resistance training, or even showing that 52 Table 3 Time course of recovery for each metric Recovery metric Pre IP 24h 48h Supine HRV 4.38 ± 0.74 2.32 ± 0.48* 4.18 ± 0.81 4.31 ± 0.59 Standing HRV 3.45 ± 0.32 1.83 ± 0.56* 3.38 ± 0.47 3.40 ± 0.48 VJ peak power (W) 4877 ± 432 4375 ± 404* 4636 ± 321* 4754 ± 427 Squat V1.0 (m•s-1) 1.00 (0.00) 0.90 (0.07)* 0.95 (0.07)* 0.93 (0.11) Bench press V1.0 (m•s-1) 1.00 (0.00) 0.90 (0.13)* 0.94 (0.05)* 0.95 (0.09) Perceived soreness (au) 1.00 (1.25) 5.50 (4.00)* 5.50 (3.25)* 6.50 (3.00)* Perceived recovery (au) 8.50 (2.00) 4.00 (3.00)* 5.00 (1.50)* 6.50 (3.25)* From Flatt et al. (1) *Significantly worse (more fatigue) than Pre (pre-training); IP = Immediately post-training; 24 h = 24 hours post-training; 48 h = 48 hours post-training V1.0 = A load that corresponded to a velocity of 1.0 m/s at pre-training; AU = Arbitrary units; HRV = heart rate variability; VJ = Vertical Jump. HRV correlates with resistance training performance. Indeed, using HRV as a readiness metric to implement a flexible template has been evaluated only once, in a study previously reviewed by Eric. In that study from de Oliveira et al (2), one group of untrained men performed 20 total training sessions in a fixed strategy of three times per week (Monday, Wednesday, Friday), and one group performed 20 total training sessions and only performed the next session when HRV had recovered to baseline. Hypertrophy and strength adaptations after the 20 sessions were similar between groups, with the only difference being that the HRV group took only ~5 weeks to complete all 20 sessions compared to the 7 weeks in the fixed group. On the surface, that looks positive, in that HRV allowed subjects to gain the same strength in a shorter time frame. However, I would argue that we can’t actually infer much from that study. I would bet that untrained individuals in the de Oliveira study simply progressed at a rate in which they didn’t need that much rest; thus, the fixed group could have probably performed the sessions within 5 weeks in a fixed time frame without issue. In fact, Greg previously reviewed a study in MASS that found that a group of untrained subjects who did progressive resistance training 3 days in a row each week for 12 weeks versus a group who trained on 3 nonconsecutive days per week experienced the same strength gains (3). Further, when implementing a flexible template, it is probably important to have one of these two factors present to see a greater benefit than a normal fixed order training protocol: 1) A very busy lifestyle in which stress or sleep is probably suboptimal, 2) A really demanding training block in which high levels of muscle damage and/or fatigue are consistently present, or both. In the de Oliveira study, the training was pret- 53 Table 4 All results Recovery metric ΔHRV Supine (%) ΔHRV Standing (%) 24h 48h 24h 48h ΔSquat V1.0 (%) r = 0.58 r = 0.04 r = 0.63 r = -0.38 ΔBench Press V1.0 (%) r = -0.06 r = 0.45 r = -0.08 r = -0.23 ΔVJ Peak Power (%) r = 0.04 r = 0.39 r = 0.35 r = 0.36 ΔPerceived recovery (au) r = -0.01 r = -0.36 r = 0.50 r = 0.06 ΔPerceived soreness (au) r = -0.01 r = -0.37 r = -0.58 r = -0.47 From Flatt et al. (1) All values are r values derived from bivariate correlations examining the percentage ∆ (change) in HRV (heart rate variability) metrics with all other recovery metrics. None of the relationships in this table were statistically significant (p values ranged from 0.052 to 0.978). 24 h = 24 hours post-training; 48 h = 48 hours post-training; V1.0 = A load that corresponded to a velocity of 1.0 m/s at pre-training; AU = Arbitrary units; VJ = Vertical Jump. ty standard and probably didn’t warrant flexibility, especially since each training session was the same. In a flexible template, training sessions typically differ throughout the week (i.e. hypertrophy, power, strength or heavy, moderate, and light), which makes more sense to use flexibility than when sessions are the same. Another question to ask before using a flexible template: How much does readiness really matter? I think it matters if you feel absolutely terrible, but probably not so much if you feel decent (compared to amazing). For example, if you take the 0-10 perceived recovery status scale (4), where 0 = “Very poorly recovered / Extremely tired” and 10 = “Very well recovered / Highly energetic,” it likely matters if you are a 0 or 1 versus a 9 or 10. However, if you rate your recovery a 6 versus an 8, does that really matter? Probably not, especially if your training sessions are pretty standard. So, I’m not sure the design in the de Oliveira study allowed for a lot of insight into the usefulness of HRV in resistance training. In research, it is customary to go from point A to point B rather than from A to C. However, with HRV, I think we have gone from A to C in the resistance training literature. For example, if HRV is to be used as a readiness indicator, we should first examine if changes in HRV are indeed indicative of changes in performance. Watkins et al did this a few years ago with vertical jump performance. Specifically, in 2017, Watkins et al (5) tested vertical jump height on trained men and women and then had them perform 4 sets to failure at 80% on the squat. Then, 48 hours later, they retested vertical jump performance and had all lifters perform the squat workout again. The researchers found a significant decrease in both vertical jump height (-2.5 cm and -8.4%) and squat reps performed (-5.6 reps and -28.3%%) and a significant correlation between decreases in vertical jump height and squat volume (r=0.65). 54 Now that’s going from point A to point B. To go to point C with vertical jump, the next step would be to carry out a flexible training study and use vertical jump as the readiness indicator. With HRV, we simply don’t have data showing that it is truly an effective readiness indicator for the main lifts. The closest we’ve come to going from point A to B with HRV in resistance training is a 2011 study from Chen and colleagues (6). Chen did observe that both HRV and squat performance declined in the days following a damaging training session in weightlifters. However, in the Chen study, the authors did not conduct correlations between HRV changes and performance declines. Further, Chen and colleagues also found that muscle pain and creatine kinase recovery also followed the same pattern as HRV, so it would be inappropriate to pick out HRV as the best metric of the bunch in the absence of a statistical analysis demonstrating its superiority over the other measures. We’ll get back to this in the “Next Steps,” but it would be nice to see a replication of the Watkins study with HRV added as a readiness indicator. Although there were no significant correlations between HRV and other recovery metrics in the reviewed study (1), there were some r-values that represented moderate correlations and a p-value of 0.052 (just shy of significance) for the relationship between squat velocity and standing HRV at 24 hours post-training I WOULD URGE YOU TO BE CAUTIOUS ABOUT USING HRV AS YOUR GOLD STANDARD FOR RECOVERY FROM LIFTING. (r=0.63, Table 4). With only 10 subjects in this study, it is very possible that this correlation could be significant with more subjects (although the p-value could also increase); however, the problem still remains that we don’t know if either squat velocity at such a light load or standing HRV are actually indicative of true performance changes. Although vertical jump power and HRV statistically recovered at different times (HRV at 24 hours and vertical jump at 48 hours), the percentage changes weren’t that different. Vertical jump power was 4.9% lower than baseline at 24 hours, while supine HRV and standing HRV were 4.6% and 2.0% lower respectively, at 24 hours. However, it’s important to reiterate that although mean changes were similar, these metrics were not correlated on an individual level. Therefore, at the individual level, it is possible that if HRV was used to implement a flexible template, it would have stipulated more training at 24 hours; however, that would have likely been at odds with vertical jump for some individuals. 55 APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS 1. Following a damaging training session, recovery of HRV does not correlate with recovery of various neuromuscular performance metrics or simple Likert scales. 2. Importantly, the overall picture surrounding HRV is quite thin in regards to resistance training. It is necessary to see a study examining if HRV is truly indicative of performance on the main lifts before recommending it as a readiness indicator. 3. Vertical jump has more support than HRV in the existing literature as a readiness indicator. Further, vertical jump is quick and easy to test and likely useful in this regard. However, there is still no longitudinal data examining vertical jump to dictate training in a flexible template. Overall, this study provides more questions than answers. However, I would urge you to be cautious about using HRV as your gold standard for recovery from lifting and would be even more cautious about letting it dictate what you actually do. Might HRV eventually be good in this regard? Maybe. It has been an effective autoregulatory tool in endurance training (7), but we are years into HRV research now and the evidence for it in resistance training just isn’t that strong. It is possible that some metrics are more indicative of recovery and performance in some people than in others, so perhaps some trial and error on what metrics seem to be useful for you are in order. Lastly, I would also urge you to not go overboard with recovery metrics and readiness, in general. For example, as discussed above, don’t go crazy over minor differences in your readiness. Rather, keep your threshold for changing the day’s workout between feeling absolutely terrible and amazing. If you have minor fluctuations in readi- ness, just use RPE or velocity to autoregulate the load on the barbell and get your workout in. Next Steps As discussed above, I’d love to see the Watkins et al study (5) replicated with HRV as a readiness indicator alongside vertical jump. With this design, we could see if HRV correlated with squat performance and if HRV and vertical jump height are correlated with each other. If HRV holds up to vertical jump, then a longitudinal flexible training study could be conducted with one fixed order group, one group using vertical jump to determine the specific training day, and another group using HRV as a readiness indicator. 56 References 1. Flatt AA, Globensky L, Bass E, Sapp BL, Riemann BL. Heart Rate Variability, Neuromuscular and Perceptual Recovery Following Resistance Training. Sports. 2019 Oct;7(10):225. 2. de Oliveira RM, Ugrinowitsch C, Kingsley JD, da Silva DG, Bittencourt D, Caruso FR, Borghi-Silva A, Libardi CA. Effect of individualized resistance training prescription with heart rate variability on individual muscle hypertrophy and strength responses. European journal of sport science. 2019 Jan 30:1-9. 3. Yang Y, Bay PB, Wang YR, Huang J, Teo HW, Goh J. Effects of consecutive versus non-consecutive days of resistance training on strength, body composition, and red blood cells. Frontiers in physiology. 2018 Jun 18;9:725. 4. Laurent CM, Green JM, Bishop PA, Sjökvist J, Schumacker RE, Richardson MT, Curtner-Smith M. A practical approach to monitoring recovery: development of a perceived recovery status scale. The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research. 2011 Mar 1;25(3):620-8. 5. Watkins CM, Barillas SR, Wong MA, Archer DC, Dobbs IJ, Lockie RG, Coburn JW, Tran TT, Brown LE. Determination of vertical jump as a measure of neuromuscular readiness and fatigue. The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research. 2017 Dec 1;31(12):3305-10. 6. Chen JL, Yeh DP, Lee JP, Chen CY, Huang CY, Lee SD, Chen CC, Kuo TB, Kao CL, Kuo CH. Parasympathetic nervous activity mirrors recovery status in weightlifting performance after training. The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research. 2011 Jun 1;25(6):1546-52. 7. Vesterinen V, Nummela A, Heikura I, Laine T, Hynynen E, Botella J, Häkkinen K. Individual endurance training prescription with heart rate variability. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2016;48. █ 57 Concept Review How the Brain Controls Eating Behavior BY ANNE-K AT HRI N E I S E LT Many people find it difficult to follow a calorie-restricted diet. Is your brain working against you? This review of neuroscience research helps explain why dieting makes you feel lousy, why it is so easy to overeat hyperpalatable food, and how you can use this knowledge to your advantage. 58 KEY POINTS 1. Your brain has a finely tuned system to keep a healthy body weight, but this does not mean a shredded physique. To achieve that, you’ll most likely have to fight your internal hunger signals. 2. Hunger signals are generated by the activation of AGRP neurons, which will induce a negative feeling and initiate food-seeking activities. 3. Eating highly palatable food makes you feel good because of the connection between the consumption and reward systems, but this “high” is only short-lived. 4. The simplest way to prevent overeating is to avoid highly rewarding food, as these foods override your satiety signals. 5. Low-reward (i.e. non-hyperpalatable) foods are hard to overeat and will make it easier to maintain or lose body fat. E volutionarily conserved brain circuits control our eating behavior – especially when and how much we eat. This control is exerted by three distinct but interconnected circuits: the hunger circuit, the consumption circuit, and the satiety circuit. Dieting and weight loss cause constant activation of the hunger circuit, which is also associated with negative feelings (aka feeling “hangry”) to motivate an individual to consume more food. Eating highly pleasurable foods that are high in fat and sugar will over-activate the reward system that is coupled to the consumption circuit, making it easy to overeat. At the same time, eating these foods will override satiety signals, which is why you can become uncomfortably stuffed after a Thanksgiving dinner. Relying on mostly non-hyperpalatable food will appropriately engage all three circuits to keep a stable and healthy body weight without feeling hangry; for most individuals, though, more effort is needed to achieve a six-pack physique. Guest Reviewer: Anne-Kathrin Eiselt, PhD, CPT Anne-Kathrin Eiselt is a neuroscientist at the Howard Hughes Medical Institute with a research focus on how nutrition and exercise affect the brain. She studied psychology with a focus on human decision-making at the University of Potsdam, Germany and received her Ph.D. in neurobiology in 2013 from the University of Tuebingen, Germany where she studied how the brain computes decisions. Anne-Kathrin competed in powerlifting and CrossFit and has more than 10 years of experience in coaching clients as a certified fitness professional with a special focus on behavioral change, lifestyle modification, sports nutrition, and preand postnatal fitness. 59 Background Eating is a very complex behavior that is influenced by many different factors like what time of day it is, where you are, if you are alone or with others, your emotional state, the time and macronutrient profile of your last meal, and so forth. Eating behavior is hard to study because it’s so individualized and variable based on your genetics, how you grew up, your lifestyle, and your surroundings. There are a couple of things that are pretty much the same for all of us, though – not only as humans, but as animals in general. Eating is a behavior that is controlled by different brain processes that are highly conserved and similar across mammalian species. Thanks to tremendous progress in the development of new technologies and tools over the last 10 years, neuroscientists can finally answer more questions about what is going on in the brain when we reach for that cookie. The Three Brain Circuits That Control Eating The control of eating can be separated into three distinct but interconnected neural circuits that are anatomically located in different brain regions: the hunger circuit, the consumption circuit, and the satiety circuit (1). The Hunger Circuit What’s a concept review? A written concept review is similar to our signature video reviews. The aim of this article type is to review a cornerstone topic in physiology or applied science research. The whole process of looking for food starts with the hunger (or more accurately called food-seeking) circuit. This neural circuit is mainly centered on neurons in the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus, a very unique brain structure at the bottom of your brain that is anatomically positioned to get access to peptides and hormones that can cross the blood brain barrier (see Figure 1). What kind of hormones and peptides? This is where the familiar names of leptin, ghrelin, and insulin, among others, come into play. These hormones constantly circulate in your blood and the arcuate nucleus tightly monitors their levels, whether they are high or low (2). If you haven’t eaten anything all day, then your insulin levels will be moderately low (since your pancreas does not produce a lot of insulin when you are fasting), but your ghrelin levels – a hormone that is secreted by your stomach in response to fasting – will be high. The arcuate nucleus, specifically neurons called AGRP (agouti-related peptide) and POMC (pro-opiomelanocortin) neurons, will notice and respond to these levels of different hormones and peptides. These 60 neurons detect short- and long-term energy stores, as well as other hormonal signals and work via a yin-yang principle to encourage or discourage eating behavior. POMC neurons work on a longer timescale and don’t have immediate influence over behavior, whereas AGRP neurons do. Therefore, we will now focus on AGRP neurons. AGRP neurons are generally pretty quiet, but they become active when they detect signs of low energy stores. When these neurons are active, they start a whole cascade of responses that make you active and motivated to find food (hence food-seeking circuit). This is why it is really hard to just sit on the couch and do nothing when you are hungry. AGRP neurons make you want to get up and move around, preferably toward the fridge or pantry. It also explains why it is much easier to stick to a diet when you can distract yourself throughout the day or when you are running around to get things done. The activation of this hunger circuit corresponds to the typical feeling of hunger you experience when you haven’t eaten for a while. Interestingly, we recently discovered that these food-seeking neurons also make you feel lousy and unhappy (3). For example, when you are on a calorie-restricted diet or haven’t eaten in a while, the activity of the AGRP neurons will cause an unpleasant feeling. How do we know this? In the lab, we can specifically and artificially activate AGRP neurons in Figure 1 The hunger circuit Arcuate nucleus Ghrelin Insulin Leptin PYY The hunger circuit is mainly controlled by neurons in the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus that regulate short- and long-term appetite and metabolism through peripheral hormones and peptides produced by the body. the brains of mice and monitor how the animals behave. Not only do these mice actively seek and voraciously consume food (even if they just ate and shouldn’t be hungry at all), but they also avoid the areas and places that are associated with this artificial neuron activation. We call this “place avoidance.” Mice do not like to have these neurons activated. It is un- 61 pleasant – a feeling we want to get rid of as soon as we can. Evolutionarily, this makes a lot of sense. The brain needed some kind of mechanism to motivate an animal to get up and find food, especially since finding food is often risky compared to staying put in a secure cave. This unpleasant feeling of hunger ensured that the animal would get active to find some calories, instead of lazily sitting in his cave and accidentally starving to death. It also explains the common feeling of being “hangry” – it’s literally no fun to be on a diet. Hence, this hunger circuit makes you very motivated to stop this unpleasant feeling by finding and eating food. As soon as you eat something (and even before, just as you have the food right in front of you), these neurons will become silent within a couple of seconds – also something we can test and visualize in the lab – and the “hangry state” is over. At this point, the consumption circuit takes over. But before we leave the hunger circuit, a word about leptin. Leptin Leptin, a hormone secreted by your adipose tissue (i.e. fat cells) informs the arcuate nucleus (and other brain areas) about your energy reserves in the form of fat. Lots of body fat means lots of circulating leptin, whereas very little fat stores means lower circulating leptin levels. It is of course more complicated than this, since circulating leptin levels are also influenced by a host of other factors and hormones, but for simplicity, you could say that high leptin levels signal that energy reserves are high (4). High leptin levels reduce the activity of ARGP neurons while activating POMC neurons, making you less hungry and less motivated to find food, while increasing your metabolic rate. This is an evolutionary mechanism to keep your body weight somewhat stable. Enough energy stores in the form of fat means no need to find more food. In lay terms: if you are a little chubby, your brain will make you less hungry and increase the amount of calories you burn just by sitting around, so you are less likely to get any fatter. On the other hand, when fat energy stores get low – for example, while you are dieting to reach a very lean body composition – those low leptin levels will constantly activate your AGRP neurons, making you hangry and motivated to get up and find something to eat, while at the same time reducing your energy expenditure (5). It’s a pretty nifty system to ensure that your bodyweight stays in homeostasis – not too fat and not too lean. But of course, this evolutionary system doesn’t care that you want visible six-pack abs; it only cares about maximizing your survivability. The tools it employs are increasing hunger, increasing food reward (increasing how good something tastes), and slowing your metabolic rate. No wonder it is so hard to stay lean and shredded, right? Your brain is working against you. 62 Figure 2 The consumption circuit Lateral hypothalamus Dorsal Striatum Ventral tegmental area Insulia Substantia Nigra Amygdala Nucleus Accumbens The consumption circuit. Simplified schematic of the brain areas activated in response to palatable food. At the center of the consumption circuit is the lateral hypothalamus, which regulates the rewarding responses to palatable food to drive and sustain food consumption. The lateral hypothalamus is highly interconnected with the reward system of the brain. Another obvious question then is: Why do people become obese when you should have a mechanism that theoretically prevents you from becoming too fat? Since its discovery in 1994 (6), researchers were hoping leptin was the holy grail for solving the obesity epidemic. However, only in some rare genetic cases do obese patients bene- fit from leptin supplementation, while most other obese patients don’t lose a lot of weight in clinical studies (7). The reason? Beyond a certain point, increased leptin production does little to curb your appetite or increase your metabolism. In fact, obesity is often coupled with leptin resistance (similar to insulin resistance) (4,8). These obese individuals have 63 enough leptin in their system; the problem is that the brain is not able to sense and detect all that leptin. Where leptin treatment might play a potential role is in weight loss maintenance to counteract the effect of decreased leptin levels with less body fat, which will cause increased hunger and energy intake and decreased energy expenditure (7). It is hypothesized that it could prevent “yo-yo” dieting by restoring the leptin balance; however, this is still under investigation and needs to be proven. Overall, the hunger circuit and various changes in hormones and peptides help explain what kicks off your hunger and motivates you to find food. But what happens after you start eating? That brings us to the consumption circuit. The Consumption Circuit The onset of eating, and even the mere sight of food in front of you, will silence the hunger circuit by reducing AGRP neuron activity. It will stay inactive until it detects another energy deficit. Now it is time for the consumption circuit to take the lead. This neural circuit is mainly based in the lateral hypothalamus, another small but evolutionarily preserved region in the middle of the brain. It is strongly connected to the reward centers of the brain, including the ventral tegmental area, ventral striatum, substantia nigra, and nucleus accumbens (see Figure 2) (9). These are areas that are well-known for playing a fundamental role in reward and pleasure signaling. When you consume food, neurons in the lateral hypothalamus will be activated and signal to those reward centers that good stuff is happening. Of course, the more palatable (aka tasty) the food is, the higher the reward response will be – especially if it is high in carbs and fat (think cookies or pizza). Interestingly, artificial sweeteners like sucralose do not activate the reward system to the same extent as sugar. Palatable, caloric food is so powerful that rodents prefer it even over cocaine and are willing to expose themselves to extreme or painful conditions just to get access to shortcake, Coca-Cola, or M&M’s, even if they are not hungry (9). The main reason is how these overly tasty items stimulate the brain reward centers, especially the dopamine and opioid systems. Some researchers argue that overeating in obesity is similar to excessive drug use in addiction and that obesity should be considered a brain disorder. The concept of food addiction is still highly debated (10, 11), but there is mounting evidence supporting the idea that obesity and a high-fat-high-sugar diet change the brain and might make it harder for individuals to stop binge eating. The consumption of food, especially highly palatable food, has also been shown to enhance your mood, which is another feature of activating the lateral 64 hypothalamus and the connected reward system. And here is the double whammy: high-calorie, palatable food activates these reward centers much more when you are hungry or dieting (12). So, the hungrier you are, the more you will like and want that tasty cookie. This is encapsulated in the old adage “Hunger is the best sauce.” Thus, it is not surprising that periods of dieting increase self-reported ratings of the power of palatable food and cravings for ‘‘tempting’’ foods (13). This is another way your brain is seemingly working against you on your path to a ripped physique. Due to the rewarding nature of this circuit, its activity becomes self-sustaining and operates almost like an eating loop. The main molecular players causing the positive feelings associated with eating are dopamine, serotonin, opioids, and cannabinoids. In short, eating releases chemicals that make us feel good and happy, which is also the reason why people often eat when stressed or to cope with negative feelings or emotions (14, 15, 16, 17). This comfort/emotional eating is typically directed toward sweet, fatty, or highly processed food and is thought to help, at least transiently, with mood improvement or to distract from negative emotions. If you catch yourself snacking when you are not hungry, the “feel-good” characteristics of the consumption and reward circuits are to blame. In fact, you would not stop eating unless the last circuit (the satiety cir- Figure 3 The satiety circuit PBN LH NTS Blood circulation N. Vagus Adipose tissue Leptin Stomach Ghrelin Leptin Pancreas Amylin Insulin PP Small intestine Large intestine PYY CCK GLP-1 The satiety circuit is activated by caloric and volume feedback from ingested food. Peripheral peptides and hormones as well as signals from the intestines will be transmitted via the vagus nerve and processed in the hindbrain (first in the NTS, then in the PBN). The hindbrain also receives information from circulating blood levels. Once information that enough calories have been ingested is received, the hindbrain will send the stop signal to the consumption circuit. LH = lateral hypothalamus; NTS= nucleus of the solitary tract; PBN = parabrachial nucleus. cuit) stepped in, as it conveys the stop signal to cease eating. Satiety Circuit The satiety circuit is activated by the caloric and volume feedback from the ingested food. A variety of signals will 65 communicate the current state of energy availability (mainly via the vagus nerve) to the hindbrain, where the satiety circuit is located (Figure 3, 18). These signals include ghrelin, intestinal peptides (like peptide tyrosine tyrosine-PYY, cholecystokinin-CCK, and glucagon-like peptide-1-GLP-1), insulin, amylin (an enzyme that helps with protein digestion), and leptin. Recently, scientists discovered exactly what these satiety circuit neurons are (19). They are called CGRP (calcitonin-gene-related peptide) neurons and are located in the parabrachial nucleus in the hindbrain. These CGRP neurons transmit the stop signal to the consumption circuit to stop eating. Interestingly, some experiments unintentionally revealed that if you get rid of these satiety neurons, mice will consume food until it comes out of their nose or until their stomach bursts. Without these powerful neurons, you would not know when enough volume and calories are consumed to stop eating (20). CGRP neurons are also active when you are nauseated and sick, which is why you don’t want to eat even the most delicious meal in these situations. A related concept is taste aversion; if you eat something and get sick from it, these neurons will “remember” that taste and associate it with feeling sick. The next time you encounter that specific food or taste, you might feel disgusted or repelled by it (21). Powerful neurons, indeed. There are, of course, many other factors that play a role when it comes to optimizing your satiety signals, like the amount of fiber you ingest, the macronutrient content of the meal, the water content in your stomach, how lean you are, and so forth. In general, eating a meal that is high in fiber, with a sufficient amount of water content, and a balanced mix of macronutrients will swiftly activate the satiety circuit to send the appropriate “stop eating” signal. It should also leave you satisfied for hours until your hunger circuit detects the next energy deficit or bout of prolonged fasting. These three interconnected systems (the hunger, consumption, and satiety circuits) work best when you eat a well-balanced diet with lower-reward foods. Once your stomach is filled and some nutrients and calories are absorbed, you stop eating and feel satisfied. Overall, your brain is monitoring your energy stores and hormones to make sure you are fit and healthy with enough energy reserves for a potential famine. The Problem: An Obesogenic Environment This finely balanced system is not adapted to an environment of readily available high-reward, high-calorie foods. Unfortunately, most of us are living in such an obesogenic environment where we are surrounded by easily accessible food options, many of which are 66 Figure 4 Cat The prefontal cortex (PFC) Dog Man In humans, the prefrontal cortex covers almost 30% of the cortex, making us the most cognitive and goal-directed species. processed or engineered to circumvent our body’s normal satiety response. Why does it affect our brain so much to promote overeating? After we are born, our brain circuits learn the cues and stimuli in our environment (like the smell and sight of bacon) and predict the positive survival-enhancing effect of ingesting those calories and the good feelings we get from eating tasty food. The connection between eating specific foods and the amount of energy it will provide is learned, and cues that predict the availability of these foods become able to activate the same brain reward system as the eaten food itself (22). This happens through the postingestive consequences of consuming: your brain learns to associate the good feeling of eating pizza with the sight, smell, and location of where you ate that pizza. Next time you drive by that pizza place or smell pizza, you might feel a craving or desire to eat. Interestingly, after learning such an association, the smell or sight of pizza can influence, for example, the release of hormones like ghrelin (23). This can trigger an almost automatic response to crave the pizza, even though you might not need the calories at all. Another problem with consuming high calorie foods is that they not only over-activate your reward systems, but 67 they also overwrite the incoming satiety signals, leading you to massively overeat beyond any homeostatic needs. Evolutionarily, your brain wants to ensure that those precious calorie resources are consumed and saved for future famines; this was especially relevant in times when high calorie food was not an everyday thing. In modern-day, affluent societies, this system now contributes to our increasing waistlines, and famines are almost nonexistent. We are surrounded by high-calorie food cues and stimuli 24/7, and our brain is constantly trying to make us eat those precious calories to prepare for a potential, but improbable, famine. Prefrontal Cortex – Control Over Goal-Directed Behavior In situations where we have to actively fight the urge to eat pizza and cookies, we have to engage yet another brain region. It’s called the prefrontal cortex. This is the area located right behind your forehead. The prefrontal cortex is the area that is most developed in humans compared to all other animal species (see Figure 4) (24). Not surprisingly, we are the only species able to plan our diet and behavior in accordance with goals like a powerlifting or bodybuilding competition. This brain region also takes the longest to completely mature in humans and won’t be fully wired up until adolescence ends. If you have ever heard of impulse control and the marshmallow test (where toddlers can either have one marshmallow immediately or they can wait a couple of minutes to receive two), it is exactly this region supporting those functions to delay immediate reward and focus on long-term goals. An athlete dieting for a competition needs to engage their prefrontal cortex a lot to delay the immediate reward of a tasty and satisfying treat for the longterm goal of having a specific amount of body fat on competition day. There are considerable amounts of interesting research and insights about the prefrontal cortex (so much that we’ll have to save them for a future review), but it’s worth mentioning that you need this brain region to stick to your diet (25), especially when things are starting to get harder and distractions to derail your diet are everywhere. To make it easier on you and your prefrontal cortex, building healthy habits that you don’t need to think about is your best bet for long-lasting diet success. Conclusion With weight management in mind, we can use the knowledge about how the brain controls appetite and eating behavior to our advantage and trick the consumption and satiety circuit to function in our favor for fat loss. The secret: eating simple, low-reward foods that contain enough fiber and water. You 68 APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS 1. Highly rewarding food will over-activate the reward system and overwrite satiety signals. 2. Dieting and weight loss will be easier if you focus on mostly non-hyperpalatable food, which will appropriately activate the satiety circuit, so you feel full without eating too many calories. 3. If you have trouble gaining weight, include food items that are less bland and higher in palatability to be able to consume a larger amount of calories. have probably heard it before and it may not be an exciting revelation, but neuroscience research supports this notion. There are many ways to lose fat, but if you mainly consume meals that don’t over-activate the reward system, you will be more likely to succeed and stick with it. Your brain circuits will work in accordance with your goals – eat only when hungry, and stop when you’ve ingested enough. The less rewarding the diet, the sooner your satiety circuit will tell you to stop eating. It’s really hard to overeat on chicken and broccoli. French fries and strawberry cheesecake, on the other hand, not so much. 69 References 1. Sternson SM, Eiselt AK (2017) Three pillars for the neural control of appetite. Annual Review Physiology, 79: 401-423. 2. Marx J (2003) Cellular warriors at the battle of the bulge. Science, 299:846-849. 3. Betley JN, Shengjin X, Huang Cao ZF, Gong R, Magnus CJ, Yu Y, Sternson SM (2015) Neurons for hunger and thirst transmit a negative-valence teaching signal. Nature, 521:180-185. 4. Li MD (2011) Leptin and beyond: an odyssey to the central control of body weight. Yale J Biol Med 84:1-7. 5. Pandit R, Beerens S, Adan RAH (2017) Role of leptin in energy expenditure: the hypothalamic perspective. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol, 312:R938-R947. 6. Zhang Y, Proenca R, Maffei M, Barone M, Leopold L, Friedman JM (1994) Positional cloning of the mouse obese gene and its human homologue. Nature, 372:425-432. 7. Kelesidis T, Kelesidis I, chou S, Mantzoros CS (2010) Narrative review: the role of leptin in human physiology: Emerging Clinical Applications. Ann Intern Med, 152:93-100. 8. Myers MG, Leibel RL, Seeley RJ, Schwartz MW (2011) Obesity and leptin resistance: Distinguishing cause from effect. Trend Endocrinol Metab, 21:643-651. 9. Kenny PJ (2011) Reward mechanisms in obesity: new insights and future directions. Neuron, 69:664-679. 10. Finlayson G (2017) Food addiction and obesity: unnecessary medicalization of hedonic overeating. Nat Rev Endocrinol, 13:493-498. 11. Fletcher PC, Kenny PJ (2018) Food addiction: a valid concept? Neuropsychopharmacology, 43:2506-2513. 12. Goldstone AP, Prechtl de Hernandez CG, Beaver JD, Muhammed K, Croese C, Bell G, Durighel G, Hughes E, Waldman AD, Frost G, Bell JD (2009) Fasting biases brain reward systems towards high-calorie foods. Eur J Neurosci 30:1625-1635. 13. Hofmann W, van Koningsbruggen GM, Stroebe W, Ramanathan S, Aarts H (2010). As pleasure unfolds: Hedonic responses to tempting food. Psychol Sci. 21:1863–1870. 14. Meye FJ, Adan RA (2014) Feelings about food: the ventral tegmental area in food reward and emotional eating. Trends Pharamacol Sci, 35:31-40. 15. Gibson EL (2012) The psychobiology of comfort eating: implications for neuropharmacological interventions. Behav Pharmacol, 23:442-460. 16. Frayn M, Livshits S, Knauper B (2018) Emotional eating and weight regulation: a qualitative study of compensatory behaviors and concerns. J Eat Disord, 6:23. 17. Kemp E, Bui M, Grier S (2013) When food is more than nutrition: understanding emotional eating and overconsumption. J Consum Behav, 12:204-213. 70 18. Posovszky C, Wabitsch M (2015) Regulation of appetite, satiation, and body weight by enteroendocrine cells. Part 2: therapeutic potential of enteroendocrine cells in the treatment of obesity. Horm Res Paediatr, 83:11-18. 19. Carter ME, Soden ME, Zweifel LS, Palmiter RD (2013) Genetic identification of a neural circuit that suppresses appetite. Nature, 503:111-114. 20. Campos CA, Bowen AJ, Schwartz MW, Palmiter RD (2016) Parabrachial CGRP neurons control meal termination. Cell Metab, 23:811-820. 21. Chen JY, Campos CA, Jarvie BC, Palmiter RD (2018) Parabrachial CGRP neurons establish and sustain aversive taste memories. Neuron, 100:891-899. 22. Johnson AW (2013) Eating beyond metabolic need: how environmental cues influence feeding behavior. Trends Neurosci, 36:101-109. 23. Sinha R, Gu P, Hart R, Guarnaccia JB (2019) Food craving, cortisol and ghrelin responses in modeling highly palatable snack intake in the laboratory. Physiol Behav, 208: 112563. 24. Fuster JM (2002) Frontal lobe and cognitive development. J Neurocytol, 31:373-385. 25. Eiselt AK, Chen J, Arnold J, Kim T, Sternson SM (2019) Medial prefrontal cortex guides decision making about hunger and thirst. bioRxiv preprint 620690. █ 71 Study Reviewed: Short-Term Impact of Sucralose Consumption on the Metabolic Response and Gut Microbiome of Healthy Adults. Thomson et al. (2019) More Good News for Artificial Sweeteners: No Effect of Short-Term Sucralose Ingestion on Glycemic Control or Gut Microbiome BY E RI C T RE X LE R Some view artificial sweetener consumption as a safe and healthy way to cut calories, while others suspect that they’re too good to be true. A recent study found that one week of consuming high-dose sucralose, also known as SPLENDA®, had no effect on glycemic control or the gut microbiome. Read on to get the scoop on the potential health effects of artificial sweeteners. 72 KEY POINTS 1. The current study (1) sought to determine if sucralose (also known as SPLENDA®), a particularly common non-nutritive sweetener, affects glycemic control and the gut microbiome. 2. Compared to placebo, seven days of consuming 780mg/day of sucralose (equivalent to about 20 diet sodas per day) did not significantly affect glycemic control or the gut microbiome. 3. Current literature has failed to consistently show unfavorable effects of sucralose on glycemic control or the gut microbiome, and has also failed to demonstrate that sucralose increases body weight, energy intake, or cancer risk. More research is needed, particularly on its effects on the gut microbiome, but sucralose appears to be a suitable sugar replacement that can be safely used to facilitate weight management. T he term “non-nutritive sweetener” describes a group of diverse sweetening agents that have one thing in common: they can deliver the same sweetness as a typical sugar dose, while contributing virtually no calories. They accomplish this task by being sweeter than sugar — way sweeter. For example, saccharin is about 500x sweeter than sugar, aspartame and acesulfame potassium are about 200x sweeter, and sucralose is about 600x sweeter. There are some naturally occurring non-nutritive sweeteners, such as stevia, but many of them, including sucralose, are synthetically produced. When people hear that their diet soda tastes the same, has zero calories, and that an “unnatural” substance made it possible, many assume that it’s too good to be true, resulting in skepticism and concerns about potential downsides. Common concerns relate to the effects of non-nutri- tive sweeteners on cancer risk, appetite, glycemic control, and the gut microbiome. The current study (1) evaluated the effects of sucralose (also known as SPLENDA®), a particularly common non-nutritive sweetener, on glycemic control and the gut microbiome. Results of this study indicated that seven days of high-dose sucralose ingestion did not significantly alter the gut microbiome or glycemic control, which contradicts some previous findings. Read on to find out what these new results mean for our growing understanding of sucralose and other non-nutritive sweeteners. Purpose and Hypotheses Purpose The purpose of this study was to determine if short-term (one week), daily 73 Table 1 Baseline subject characteristics from Thomson et al Placebo (n=14) Sucralose (n=16) Age (years) 23.5 ± 2.9 [18.2 - 29.3] 22.8 ± 3.0 [18.7 - 30.2] 0.51 Weight (kg) 77.0 ± 8.3 [57.9 - 88.0] 73.2 ± 6.9 [60.9 - 83.7] 0.19 Height (m) 1.73 ± 0.04 [1.67 - 1.80] 1.75 ± 0.07 [1.63 - 1.90] 0.31 Body mass index (kg/m2) 25.7 ± 2.9 [20.8 - 28.9] 23.8 ± 1.7 [21.1 - 26.6] 0.04 Glycemia (mg/dl) 84 ± 8 [67 - 95] 85 ± 6 [72 - 94] 0.50 Cholesterol (mg/dl) 173 ± 23 [134 - 219] 147 ± 22 [106 - 184] <0.01 Despite using standard randomization protocols, the placebo and sucralose groups had significantly different body mass index and cholesterol levels at baseline. However, these differences probably didn’t have a meaningful effect on the study results. sucralose intake alters glycemic control or the gut microbiome. Just to clarify, “glycemic control” refers to the body’s ability to efficiently manage blood glucose levels throughout the day; a person with poorly controlled diabetes has poor glycemic control, and a person with high insulin sensitivity has excellent glycemic control. Common research outcomes related to glycemic control include fasting glucose and insulin levels, glucose and insulin responses to glucose or food ingestion, glycated hemoglobin (HbA1c), and more. has been shown to adversely affect glycemic control and that the effect could feasibly be related to sucralose-induced changes in the gut microbiome. However, they also presented some human evidence suggesting that sucralose does not influence glycemic control. They were clearly testing the hypothesis that daily sucralose ingestion alters glycemic control by causing gut microbiome changes, but it’s unclear if they suspected (or doubted) that this effect would be observed. Hypotheses Subjects and Methods The authors did not clearly state a hypothesis. In the introduction, they presented evidence indicating that sucralose Subjects The researchers recruited 34 74 weight-stable, healthy men between 18 and 50 years of age, with a body mass index (BMI) of 20-30kg/m2. Participants were not trained and did not regularly participate in intense physical activity for three months prior to the study. Four subjects dropped out, so the study finished with 16 subjects in the sucralose group and 14 in the placebo group. Subject characteristics are presented in Table 1. day for a 70kg individual. The daily dose used in the current study is equivalent to about 20 diet sodas per day. Methods Throughout the study, body weight remained stable; the sucralose group and placebo group both experienced non-significant, negligible increases in body mass (+0.21 ± 1.17kg and +0.16 ± 0.74kg, respectively). Participants arrived at the first visit to get their glycemic control tested using an oral glucose tolerance test. A baseline blood sample was taken, followed by ingestion of 75g of glucose, then additional blood draws at 30, 60, 90, and 120 minutes after glucose ingestion. At each time point, glucose and insulin were measured, which allows the researchers to understand how efficiently each subject clears glucose from their bloodstream. Subjects completed this test in a fasted state and brought a fecal sample with them (or collected one in the lab) to allow for gut microbiome profiling. After this visit, the seven-day supplementation period began. Subjects either took a sucralose capsule, or a placebo capsule of similar appearance, three times per day. The total daily dose of sucralose was 780mg/day. To put that into perspective, the European Union lists 15 mg/kg/day as the acceptable daily intake for sucralose, which would be 1,050mg/ Following the seven-day supplementation period, the oral glucose tolerance test was repeated, and another fecal sample was collected to allow for gut microbiome profiling. Findings The researchers observed and calculated many different indices pertaining to blood levels of glucose and insulin and their changes in response to glucose intake. The results of these outcomes are presented in Table 2. If the treatments (placebo versus sucralose) caused differential responses, this would be reflected by a significant group × time interaction; as can be seen in Table 2, none of these interaction effects were statistically significant. In addition to the outcomes presented in Table 2, the authors calculated the area under the curve for glucose and insulin responses to the oral glucose tolerance test in each group, both before and after the seven-day intervention period. As can be seen in Figure 1, these val- 75 Table 2 Glycemic control variables before and after intervention (mean ± SD) Placebo (n=14) Sucralose (n=16) Before After Change Before Glycemia (mg/dl) 82 ± 5 79 ± 4 -2.2 ± 5.0 82 ± 5 Insulinemia (μU/ml) 12 ± 5 11 ± 4 -1.0 ± 3.3 9±4 HOMA-IR 2.4 ± 1.1 2.1 ± 0.8 -0.3 ± 0.7 1.9 ± 0.9 After p Change Group Time Group x Time 82 ± 5 0.0 ± 6.1 0.22 0.31 0.29 8±4 -0.9 ± 4.6 0.07 0.22 0.96 1.7 ± 0.9 -0.2 ± 1.1 0.13 0.21 0.77 Fasting After oral glucose Glycemia (mg/dl)** 115 ± 17 112 ± 21 -3.0 ± 17.4 107 ± 21 113 ± 21 6.2 ± 18.6 0.65 0.63 0.17 Insulinemia (μU/ml)** 81 ± 38 87 ± 51 5.6 ± 38.1 63 ± 40 78 ± 41 15.4 ± 30.5 0.35 0.11 0.44 ISI-Composite 4.1 ± 2.1 4.7 ± 3.0 0.5 ± 2.4 7.8 ± 10.0 5.9 ± 4.2 -1.9 ± 7.7 0.21 0.52 0.29 ** = Mean of responses over the 2-hr time period following glucose ingestion ues were not significantly altered by sucralose or placebo. In terms of the gut microbiome, a baseline difference was observed; the placebo group had a relatively higher abundance of phylum Firmicutes before the study began, and this persisted throughout the intervention (Figure 2). However, treatment (sucralose versus placebo) did not induce any meaningful changes throughout the study in either group. The researchers ran a few more in-depth analyses pertaining to the gut microbiome, which assessed indices of intra-individual microbiome alterations. These additional analyses also indicated that the gut microbiome was largely unaffected by the intervention in the current study. Finally, the researchers split the sample into “responders” and “non-responders” based on whether their values increased or decreased following the intervention, regardless of which treatment they received. These exploratory analyses are not central to the research question, so I won’t spend a lot of time focusing on them, but they generally found that responders and non-responders had different gut microbiome compositions. While this doesn’t suggest that sucralose induced any changes in gut microbiome composition or glycemic control, it does reinforce the general relationship between the gut microbiome and glycemic control. Interpretation The current study provides more data to help answer an important (and valid) question: Do artificial sweeteners unfavorably affect the gut microbiome or glycemic control? As reviewed by Pepino (2), there are legitimate reasons to investigate this question. Previous studies have suggested that non-nutritive sweeteners (including sucralose) can significantly alter learned responses that relate to glycemic control and energy intake, significantly alter the gut microbiome, and significantly alter glycemic 76 Figure 1 Changes in metabolic response to glucose consumption before and after the intervention Glycemic response (AUC, x 103) A After 16 14 12 10 B Insulinemic response (AUC, x 103) Before 18 n = 14 n = 16 Placebo Sucralose Treatment p-value = 0.57 20 Before After 15 10 5 0 n = 14 n = 16 Placebo Sucralose Treatment p-value = 0.73 AUC = Total area under curve for each group, both before and after the intervention period and insulinemic responses to glucose intake (2, 3). In fact, one rodent study found that while sucralose impaired oral glucose tolerance, this effect was blocked by antibiotics and was transferable to other mice via fecal transplant (4). These observations would appear to imply (but not prove) that the microbiome might play a causative role in the observed changes in glycemic responses. While the overwhelming majority of this evidence comes from either in vitro models or rodent studies, there are also some human trials lending support to some of these theoretical downsides of sucralose. For example, one study suggested that sucralose elicited a cephalic phase insulin response in a subset of 77 Figure 2 Gut microbiome compositions before and after intervention Mean relative abundance p = 0.05 Actinobacteria p = 0.02 1.0 Bacteroidetes Firmicutes 0.8 Proteobacteria 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 Before After Placebo (14) “responders,” particularly when ingested as a food rather than a beverage (5). A separate trial suggested that consuming sucralose (48mg) 10 minutes before an oral glucose tolerance test led to unfavorable effects on glycemic responses (6), and another found that 14 days of sucralose ingestion (36mg/day) led to a significant reduction in insulin sensitivity (7). In contrast, the current study (1) did not identify meaningful effects of sucralose ingestion on glycemic control or the gut microbiome. So, what gives? In a fairly recent study, Grotz et al (8) summarized the existing literature pertaining to the effects of sucralose on glycemic responses. They presented a big table containing 16 studies; some involved single-dose sucralose inges- Before After Sucralose (14) tion, while others included repeated sucralose ingestion over a period of weeks or months. All 16 studies included some measure of blood glucose or blood insulin levels, and only one of the 16 studies reported a statistically significant effect on either parameter. A couple of additional studies have come out in the time since Grotz et al published this summary table, but the general idea remains the same: the majority of human trials find no significant impact of sucralose ingestion on glycemic control. When we look at the studies suggesting that sucralose impairs various outcomes related to glycemic control, a few patterns emerge. For example, a couple of them are unblinded designs lacking a placebo condition (6, 7). A couple of 78 them openly questioned the real-world impact of their own results, as one study’s results were dependent upon responder/nonresponder stratification and distinguishing between solid versus liquid sources of sucralose (5), and the other used a relatively small sample and did not collect baseline data, which would have facilitated interpretation of the study results (9). All four of these studies recruited participants with relatively low habitual intake of non-nutritive sweeteners, and also utilized samples that were mostly (64-88%) female, with mean ages suggesting that the majority of the participants were premenopausal. This is important, because it has been speculated that habitual users and non-users of non-nutritive sweeteners may have differential responses to short-term interventions, and outcomes related to glycemic control and insulin release can be influenced by menstrual cycle phase (10). As the literature currently stands, it would appear that the majority of studies fail to observe significant effects of acute or chronic sucralose consumption on outcomes related to glycemic control. Of the studies that do report significant differences, there may be some key design limitations and sample characteristics underlying the observed effects. In addition, it is important to distinguish between statistically significant effects and clinically or practically significant effects; if an intervention slightly alters AS THE LITERATURE CURRENTLY STANDS, IT WOULD APPEAR THAT THE MAJORITY OF STUDIES FAIL TO OBSERVE SIGNIFICANT EFFECTS OF ACUTE OR CHRONIC SUCRALOSE CONSUMPTION ON OUTCOMES RELATED TO GLYCEMIC CONTROL. insulin or glucose kinetics after glucose consumption but doesn’t appear to have a meaningful impact on an individual’s overall health or body composition, it’s hard to view that as a good enough reason to avoid sucralose altogether. Given that previous studies have identified some characteristics that may alter glycemic responses to sucralose and stratified between responders and non-responders, it would seem that individual responses may vary. If you’re concerned about your glycemic response to acute or chronic sucralose consumption, you could easily do a little experiment with a home glucose monitor (thanks to Greg for suggesting this practical recommendation). Notably, there are a couple of fairly long, well-designed studies that can give 79 us some confidence in sucralose. In one such study, 128 diabetic subjects were randomly assigned to receive either sucralose (667mg/day) or a placebo for 13 weeks. The study measured a number of outcomes related to glycemic control and general safety, with no significant effects observed. Similarly, the same research group randomly assigned 47 healthy male volunteers to receive either sucralose (1000mg/day) or a placebo for 12 weeks. Again, the study found no change in outcomes related to glycemic control, such as fasting glucose, fasting insulin, c-peptide, glycated hemoglobin (HbA1c), or responses to an oral glucose tolerance test. Taken together, it would seem that there is insufficient evidence to warrant major concern about sucralose inducing unfavorable effects on glycemic control. When it comes to effects on the microbiome, we really don’t have much human evidence to go off of. A recent review from 2019 sought to describe the effects of several non-nutritive sweeteners on the gut microbiome (3). While some sweeteners, such as aspartame and saccharin, had several studies documenting their effects on the gut microbiome, only a few rodent studies (and zero human studies) were reviewed for sucralose. It’s way too early to determine if sucralose significantly alters the gut microbiome in humans. However, several studies have provided large, daily doses of sucralose for weeks at a time, and these studies have failed to consistently yield serious adverse effects, weight gain, or clinically meaningful alterations in glycemic control. We can’t discount the possibility that long-term sucralose ingestion may alter the human gut microbiome, or that this alteration could have physiologically or clinically meaningful effects. However, the current evidence doesn’t seem to hint at any particularly disastrous effects, even with fairly high doses of sucralose ingested for up to three months (8, 11). Effects of non-nutritive sweeteners on other outcomes While we’re on the subject of non-nutritive sweeteners, it makes sense to briefly address some of the other common concerns that people worry about. As MASS readers will remember from Volume 3, Issue 10, Dr. Helms covered a recent study indicating that artificially sweetened diet soda has a neutral, if not positive, effect on cravings and food intake. Along the same lines, a meta-analysis found that non-nutritive sweeteners tend to reduce energy intake and body weight when they’re used to replace sugar calories and have a positive or neutral effect when compared to plain water (12). While many folks fear that some non-nutritive sweeteners may increase cancer risk, a recent review found no evidence linking sucralose to cancer, even at intake levels substantially higher than would be anticipated (13). Taken 80 WE CAN’T DISCOUNT THE POSSIBILITY THAT LONGTERM SUCRALOSE INGESTION MAY ALTER THE HUMAN GUT MICROBIOME, BUT THE CURRENT EVIDENCE DOESN’T SEEM TO HINT AT ANY PARTICULARLY DISASTROUS EFFECTS, EVEN WITH FAIRLY HIGH DOSES. together, there’s insufficient evidence to conclude that sucralose has a negative impact on the gut microbiome or glycemic control, and plenty of evidence suggesting that regular consumption of sucralose is not linked to weight gain, appetite alterations that result in overeating, or elevated cancer risk. It’s not uncommon for people to distrust organizations that provide oversight, and I wouldn’t be shocked if some people doubted that such organizations would actually apply bans or change policy in response to unfavorable data. However, when it comes to non-nutritive sweeteners, there is actually precedent that lends evidence to the contrary. Cyclamate used to be an approved sweetener in the United States, but it was banned in 1969 in response to some rodent data linking cyclamate to bladder cancer (14). Other countries were less convinced by this evidence and opted not to ban cyclamate, but national and international agencies put a great deal of thought into which food additives and sweetening agents are safe to consume, and these decisions are updated (and sometimes reversed) when new evidence becomes available. For now, the sucralose data seem to range from neutral to positive, and sucralose is regarded as a safe sweetening agent by organizations all over the world. Non-nutritive sweeteners: a very broad term Before I wrap things up, I want to emphasize a point that sometimes seems to get lost in the artificial sweetener conversation. I’ll frequently get asked questions like, “Do artificial sweeteners increase cancer risk?” or “Do artificial sweeteners mess up your gut microbiome?” In reality, the underlying premise of these questions is flawed; non-nutritive sweeteners are just a bunch of things that are really, really sweet, with extremely low caloric content per serving. They can be natural or synthetic in origin, and they are wildly different compounds with totally different structures and properties. We can’t lump them together as one cohesive category, 81 APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS A lot of people are wary of non-nutritive sweeteners, possibly because they seem too good to be true. However, the current evidence does not suggest that sucralose has meaningful effects on glycemic control or the human gut microbiome, although more data on the gut microbiome are certainly needed. Overall, sucralose appears to be a safe sugar replacement and can be used as a tool to support successful weight management. which means we can’t project positive attributes of sweetener X onto sweetener Y, and we can’t assume that sweetener Z is harmful because a new study reported some unfavorable effects of sweetener Y. Unfortunately, this means we need a ton of research to evaluate all of the outcomes we’re concerned about, using all of the sweeteners that find their way into our diets. Conclusions The results of the current study indicate that one week of ingesting high doses of sucralose, equivalent to about 20 diet sodas per day, does not significantly affect glycemic control or the gut microbiome. While there isn’t much evidence about the human microbiome to compare these results to, the findings are in line with a number of studies showing no effect or negligible effects of sucralose on glycemic control. Of course, it’s important to note that the current results do not conclusively rule out the possibility that sucralose might influence these outcomes in some cir- cumstances. Limitations of the current study include the use of a fairly small sample size and an intervention with a relatively short duration. The studies that do report statistically significant effects tend to indicate that responses vary substantially from person to person, and the exact reasons for this variability are not currently known. Based on the limited data available, sucralose does not significantly affect glycemic control or the gut microbiome, nor does it appear to increase body weight, energy intake, or cancer risk. Overall, sucralose looks like a suitable sugar replacement that can facilitate weight management within the context of a well-constructed diet. Next Steps Right now, we simply don’t have much research examining the effect of sucralose on the human gut microbiome. In addition, much of the research evaluating glycemic control has utilized pretty short study durations, ranging from single-dose designs to only a week 82 or two of daily consumption in many cases. Finally, studies on both glycemic responses and microbiome changes in response to sucralose intake have suggested that inter-individual responses vary greatly. A great next step would be to design a randomized controlled trial evaluating longitudinal changes in response to sucralose intake. Ideally, such a study would be long enough in duration to ensure that any sucralose-induced changes would be observed by the time of post-testing, and it would have a large enough sample to identify characteristics that might reliably separate responders from nonresponders. It seems that menstrual cycle phase and habitual intake of non-nutritive sweeteners might possibly be influential characteristics, but many other possibilities exist. Finally, this area of research will eventually have to perform similar trials with other common non-nutritive sweeteners, as the results of sucralose would not necessarily generalize to all other non-nutritive sweeteners. 83 References 1. Thomson P, Santibañez R, Aguirre C, Galgani JE, Garrido D. Short-term impact of sucralose consumption on the metabolic response and gut microbiome of healthy adults. Br J Nutr. 2019 Oct 28;122(8):856–62. 2. Pepino MY. Metabolic effects of non-nutritive sweeteners. Physiol Behav. 2015 Dec 1;152:450–5. 3. Ruiz-Ojeda FJ, Plaza-Díaz J, Sáez-Lara MJ, Gil A. Effects of Sweeteners on the Gut Microbiota: A Review of Experimental Studies and Clinical Trials. Adv Nutr Bethesda Md. 2019 Jan 1;10:S31–48. 4. Suez J, Korem T, Zeevi D, Zilberman-Schapira G, Thaiss CA, Maza O, et al. Artificial sweeteners induce glucose intolerance by altering the gut microbiota. Nature. 2014 Oct 9;514(7521):181–6. 5. Dhillon J, Lee JY, Mattes RD. The cephalic phase insulin response to nutritive and low-calorie sweeteners in solid and beverage form. Physiol Behav. 2017 Nov 1;181:100–9. 6. Pepino MY, Tiemann CD, Patterson BW, Wice BM, Klein S. Sucralose affects glycemic and hormonal responses to an oral glucose load. Diabetes Care. 2013 Sep;36(9):2530–5. 7. Romo-Romo A, Aguilar-Salinas CA, Brito-Córdova GX, Gómez-Díaz RA, Almeda-Valdes P. Sucralose decreases insulin sensitivity in healthy subjects: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2018 Sep 1;108(3):485–91. 8. Grotz VL, Pi-Sunyer X, Porte D, Roberts A, Richard Trout J. A 12-week randomized clinical trial investigating the potential for sucralose to affect glucose homeostasis. Regul Toxicol Pharmacol. 2017 Aug 1;88:22–33. 9. Lertrit A, Srimachai S, Saetung S, Chanprasertyothin S, Chailurkit L-O, Areevut C, et al. Effects of sucralose on insulin and glucagon-like peptide-1 secretion in healthy subjects: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Nutrition. 2018;55–56:125–30. 10. Sheu WH. Alteration of insulin sensitivity by sex hormones during the menstrual cycle. J Diabetes Investig. 2011 Aug 2;2(4):258–9. 11. Grotz VL, Henry RR, McGill JB, Prince MJ, Shamoon H, Trout JR, et al. Lack of effect of sucralose on glucose homeostasis in subjects with type 2 diabetes. J Am Diet Assoc. 2003 Dec;103(12):1607–12. 12. Rogers PJ, Hogenkamp PS, de Graaf C, Higgs S, Lluch A, Ness AR, et al. Does low-energy sweetener consumption affect energy intake and body weight? A systematic review, including meta-analyses, of the evidence from human and animal studies. Int J Obes 2005. 2016 Mar;40(3):381–94. 13. Berry C, Brusick D, Cohen SM, Hardisty JF, Grotz VL, Williams GM. Sucralose Non-Carcinogenicity: A Review of the Scientific and Regulatory Rationale. Nutr Cancer. 2016 Dec;68(8):1247–61. 84 14. Yang Q. Gain weight by “going diet?” Artificial sweeteners and the neurobiology of sugar cravings. Yale J Biol Med. 2010 Jun;83(2):101–8. █ 85 Study Reviewed: Peak Age and Performance Progression in World-Class Weightlifting and Powerliting Athletes. Solberg et al. (2019) What’s the Best Age to Dominate Strength Sports? BY G RE G NUC KO LS If you want to maximize your competitiveness in powerlifting or weightlifting, at what age should you anticipate being at the peak of your prowess? It seems that weightlifting is a young person’s game, but many powerlifters are still improving well into their 30s (or even 40s). 86 KEY POINTS 1. When analyzing competition results from 4000+ athletes at world championships and the Olympics, it appears that powerlifters peak at an age of 35 ± 7, while weightlifters attain their best performances at 26 ± 3 years old. 2. Notice the standard deviations: if you want to be a world-class weightlifter, you realistically need to start training young enough that you can peak sometime in your 20s. However, while some powerlifters also reach their peak performance at a young age, there are some world-class lifters who are still improving into their 40s. 3. These differences are probably driven by the differing demands of the sports (weightlifting is more based on power, and powerlifting is more based on sheer force production), along with differences in overall competitiveness and talent pools. A fter genetics, training age may be the most important factor explaining strength differences between individuals. However, biological age clearly plays a role as well. For every sport, there’s an age at which the best of the best are the most competitive. You don’t see many basketball players or footballers dominating in their 40s, after all. So, what is the prime age for strength sports? A recent study examined competition results in world championships and the Olympics and found that world-class weightlifters tend to peak at around 26 years old, while powerlifters peak around 35. The standard deviation was also wider for powerlifters (7 years) than weightlifters (3 years), giving powerlifters a wider competitive window. These differences may be driven by several factors, including the age at which people begin training for each sport, the different physical requirements of the sports, and possibly changes in equipment over time. Purpose and Hypotheses Purpose The purpose of this study was to identify the age of peak performance in elite weightlifters and powerlifters, as well as the magnitude of the performance improvement in the preceding years. Hypotheses No hypotheses were stated. Subjects and Methods Subjects This was retrospective research, meaning the authors analyzed data that already existed, instead of actually recruiting 87 Figure 1 Theoretical representation of chnages in total over time 850 Total 825 800 775 750 30 32 34 36 Year These are theoretical totals at different ages for a fictitious athlete, fitted with a polynomial trendline. The trendline peaks at 33.7 years old. In this example, the athlete hit their best total at age 34. As such, this method for estimating peak age probably gives a reasonably close approximation, unless an athlete had a ton of random variance in their year-to-year results. subjects and running a study. As such, the “subjects” were everyone who had competed in the IPF powerlifting single ply world championships between 20032017, and the IWF weightlifting world championships or Olympics between 1998-2017, totaling 4385 competitors. Data Analysis The authors made an interesting decision when determining each athlete’s age of peak performance. Personally, I would have just recorded their age when they hit their highest total. Instead, the authors fit a quadratic curve to each of the athletes’ performance trends over time, with the assumption that peak performance occurred at the top of the curve. An example is provided below as Figure 1. As such, only the athletes with at least three world championship appearances could be analyzed. This was a somewhat odd choice, in my opinion, but as we’ll see, it probably didn’t matter a whole lot. Rather than just looking at totals, the authors also split things out by lift, by sex, and by performance level (medalists 88 Table 1 Predicted peak age (in years) and weight lifted (in kilograms) for powerlifting events Squat Event (n) Age Bench press Weight Age Deadlift Weight Age Weight Men 59 kg (8) 30.7 (5.4) 260 (18) 32.1 (4.0) 170 (20) 32.1 (4.2) 243 (19) 66 kg (11) 34.9 (7.0) 281 (27) 34.6 (4.4) 186 (30) 33.5 (6.6) 277 (26) 74 kg (8) 33.9 (3.6) 295 (40) 34.2 (3.6) 196 (30) 33.8 (4.2) 288 (21) 83 kg (10) 33.7 (7.6) 331 (25) 36.4 (6.9) 229 (30) 32.8 (5.8) 307 (21) 93 kg (12) 36.2 (6.1) 339 (21) 35.8 (6.8) 240 (27) 35.0 (6.6) 316 (24) 105 kg (8) 33.8 (5.1) 376 (25) 33.4 (4.8) 269 (25) 31.0 (5.2) 342 (25) 120 kg (5) 34.6 (10.0) 382 (34) 33.7 (10.5) 253 (26) 36.1 (10.1) 355 (31) >120 kg (8) 35.0 (5.2) 397 (29) 34.1 (7.5) 306 (43) 33.6 (5.3) 327 (23) Women 47 kg (7) 34.2 (7.9) 169 (25) 35.5 (8.0) 98 (19) 35.4 (7.8) 162 (18) 52 kg (10) 36.3 (7.0) 171 (26) 35.4 (7.3) 99 (17) 36.5 (7.1) 168 (15) 57 kg (8) 35.2 (5.6) 197 (19) 35.5 (5.8) 114 (19) 36.9 (6.2) 189 (8) 63 kg (5) 35.3 (6.6) 220 (15) 34.8 (5.2) 154 (19) 32.7 (5.2) 211 (21) 72 kg (8) 34.5 (9.1) 233 (18) 35.7 (8.4) 160 (16) 33.7 (8.3) 222 (16) 84 kg (5) 38.3 (7.8) 230 (31) 40.2 (6.3) 168 (20) 35.0 (8.5) 202 (22) >84 kg (5) 37.9 (7.3) 263 (38) 40.6 (5.8) 177 (21) 37.7 (7.3) 223 (20) Data shown are group means ± SD vs. non-medalists). Furthermore, they analyzed the rates at which lifters improved their performance prior to their peak, and whether moving up or down weight classes improved athletes’ placing at competitions. Since weight classes changed a few times in both sports during this time span, the authors treated similar weight classes as if they were the same weight class for athletes who competed before and after each weight class realignment (i.e. if you competed in the 75kg class before the IPF’s new weight classes, and the 74kg class after the new weight classes, you were considered to be in the same class). Findings Peak age was 35 ± 7 for powerlifters and 26 ± 3 for weightlifters. That 9-year gap was a statistically significant difference. Within each sport, peak age for all three powerlifts was similar, as was the peak age for both the snatch and clean and jerk for weightlifters. The study 89 Table 2 Predicted peak age (in years) and weight lifted (in kilograms) for weightlifting events Snatch Event (n) Age Clean and jerk Weight Age Weight Men 56 kg (26) 25.9 (3.6) 122 (9) 26.2 (3.4) 151 (10) 62 kg (21) 27.2 (4.1) 133 (9) 27.6 (5.1) 164 (10) 69 kg (17) 26.4 (2.6) 146 (9) 26.1 (3.1) 177 (9) 77 kg (19) 26.2 (3.1) 159 (7) 25.8 (2.7) 193 (9) 85 kg (19) 26.5 (3.2) 166 (10) 24.7 (2.6) 202 (9) 94 kg (17) 25.1 (2.7) 173 (6) 24.9 (2.7) 212 (8) 105 kg (21) 25.7 (2.5) 185 (8) 24.7 (2.2) 220 (9) >105 kg (25) 28.0 (3.0) 193 (15) 28.4 (3.5) 232 (15) Women 48 kg (19) 24.7 (3.4) 82 (6) 24.8 (3.1) 103 (9) 53 kg (13) 25.8 (3.0) 88 (67) 24.3 (2.7) 110 (8) 58 kg (13) 25.6 (3.3) 96 (5) 26.0 (4.2) 123 (8) 63 kg (10) 27.2 (2.8) 103 (8) 26.5 (3.1) 128 (10) 69 kg (12) 24.3 (4.7) 108 (11) 24.4 (4.7) 136 (13) 75 kg (12) 26.7 (3.1) 111 (11) 27.1 (3.4) 136 (14) >75 kg (21) 25.1 (3.8) 122 (13) 25.7 (3.8) 154 (16) Data shown are group means ± SD provides a couple of tables of peak age for all lifts, all weight classes, and both sexes, which you can see below in Tables 1 and 2. Personally, I think it’s more granular than is useful, but it’s here if you’re interested. In weightlifting, peak age was slightly younger for medalists than non-medalists, though the difference wasn’t significant for powerlifting. Peak age was slightly older for males in weightlift- ing and for females in powerlifting. All of those differences were pretty small (<1.5 years) and likely not meaningful; when you have almost 4500 subjects, just about everything will be statistically significant. In the five years preceding peak performance, competitors in both sports improved by ~10%, though there was a huge range. The upper and lower bounds aren’t given, but the standard deviations 90 Figure 2 Improvements in world-class strength athletes prior to peak performance Men Women Improvement (%) 30 20 *** * 10 0 Weightlifting Powerlifting Likelihood of clear substantial sex differences: * = possibly *** = very likely were ~7-10%, meaning that anything between an incredibly small increase and a 20% increase in performance in the five years before peak performance wouldn’t be outside of the norm. Interestingly, in both sports, female lifters improved more than male lifters in the five years preceding peak performance (2.7 ± 3.8% more in powerlifting, and 3.3 ± 1.6% more in weightlifting). Powerlifters made a bit more progress than weightlifters in the 5 years prior to peak performance (12 ± 10% vs. 9 ± 7%), and within powerlifting, rates of improvement were greater for the squat and bench press than the deadlift. Within both sexes, changing weight classes was generally beneficial. Males who moved down a weight class improved their placing by three spots, on average, while males who moved up a weight class and females who moved either up or down improved their placing by one spot, on average. However, 91 Figure 3 Relative strength gains by age Monthly gains in allometrically scaled strength 8.00% 7.00% 6.00% 5.00% 4.00% 3.00% 2.00% 1.00% 0.00% 7.5 20 32.5 45 57.5 70 -1.00% -2.00% 10th percentile 25th percentile 50th percentile 75th percentile 90th percentile Percentiles of strength improvements or decrements as a function of age in all raw, drug-tested lifters in the Open Powerlifting database who competed at least twice. changing a weight class was relatively uncommon. Only 733 of the 4385 athletes went up or down a weight class (16.7%). Of the people who changed weight classes, moving up a class was more common than moving down a class (534 vs. 199; almost 73% of the lifters who changed weight classes went up a weight class, while only ~27% moved down a weight class). Interpretation This was a neat article that confirms what strength sport fans have known for a long time, or at least strongly suspected: Weightlifting is a young person’s game, while powerlifters can improve much deeper into their careers than athletes in most other sports. On the surface, powerlifting and weightlifting look like pretty similar sports, but the make-or-break factors in elite competition separate them. Namely, in powerlifting, you’re not penalized (much) by being slow. In weightlifting, on the other hand, speed is the name of the game. If you can still produce just as much force, but you can’t generate as high of velocities at the end of your second pull, or you can’t drop under the bar as fast to receive a clean or snatch, or you can’t generate as much power in the blink of an eye on your jerk drive, 92 Figure 4 Summary of findings Effect size range (Cohen’s D) 1 0.5 0 -0.5 Direct Hypertrophy Indirect Hypertrophy Older strength gains Strength gains 20+ weeks Lower body strength gains Overall strength gains Young strength gains Upper body strength gains Positive values mean larger relative gains for women, while negative values mean larger relative gains for men Diamonds = Effect size 95% CI; Black = not significant; Red = significant difference The white region represents trivial effects, light green is small effects, darker green is medium effects and darkest green is large effects your performance suffers. In powerlifting, on the other hand, generating higher velocities right out of the hole on a squat could theoretically give you a little more leeway when grinding out a max, but the overall impact is MUCH smaller; there have always been successful powerlifters who don’t move the bar particularly fast. That distinction helps explain why weightlifters peak at a younger age than powerlifters: The ability to generate velocity and power drop off faster due to age than the ability to generate force (2, 3). There are a couple of reasons for this: the elastin content of tendons decreases (which decreases the efficiency of the stretch shortening cycle), and neural impulses travel at a slightly slower speed (which decreases active rate of force development; 4). So, while trained lifters can still add muscle mass and increase their force output into their 30s or even 40s, power output and velocity are much more subject to the aging process, and continued training can only slow down their decline. However, I don’t know that the differences between the sports can be chalked up solely to physiology. Weightlifting is also a much more competitive sport worldwide. Most powerlifters don’t start training for powerlifting until high school at the earliest, whereas internationally competitive weightlifting programs begin developing weightlifters from a very young age. Thus, the training age of a 26-year-old internationally competitive weightlifter is, on average, much higher than a 26-year-old internationally competitive powerlifter. The present study (1) had no way of accounting for training age, but I strongly 93 suspect that it’s an important factor. If powerlifters started training for powerlifting at the same age that weightlifters start training for weightlifting, it’s possible that we’d discover that the physiological peak age for powerlifting is, say, 30 years old rather than 35. Maybe the average 30-year-old powerlifter simply isn’t at an advanced enough training age to be at peak performance, and so they wind up peaking in the sport a few years after they “should” have peaked physiologically. One final factor to consider is that this study examined world-class single ply powerlifters. Since the resurgence of raw lifting is a relatively recent phenomenon, the authors had to focus in on single ply competition in order to have enough years of data to analyze. During the time period the authors analyzed, it’s my understanding that single ply, IPF-approved powerlifting gear has improved considerably. Thus, it’s possible that a fair amount of the lifters in the sample put up their best totals a few years after their actual strength peaked, as improved equipment allowed them to keep improving their competition numbers. When considering the effects of lifting gear and training age, I think we can firmly conclude that weightlifters do likely reach their peak in the sport younger than powerlifters do, but I think the average of 35 years old for powerlifters is a little higher than it “should” be. In other words, that may have been the average age at which the lifters in their sample actually attained their best performance, but I’m not fully convinced that it’s actually the “optimal” age for powerlifters. I want to make one thing clear, however: Just because internationally competitive weightlifters peak at 26 and internationally competitive powerlifters peak at 35, on average, you shouldn’t assume that’s when you’ll peak. I carried out an analysis of powerlifting meet results last year (on all raw competitors, not just internationally competitive lifters) and found that people of all ages are still making gains, on average. Now, there’s clearly some self-selection bias – lifters who started competing in their 20s don’t keep getting stronger into their 70s, after all. It’s more likely that people simply stop competing (or competing as often) once their strength starts waning. However, of the people who do compete in powerlifting, we can see that the median rate of progress is positive well into the Masters divisions (40 years old and above). One common fear I hear from middle-aged folks who start lifting is that the progress they’ll be able to make may be very modest on account of their age. The data don’t really bear that out. While it’s true that people in their teens and early 20s make progress at a faster rate than Masters and Submasters athletes, median rates of progress only drop by about 50% between 30 and 60 94 APPLICATION AND TAKEAWAYS For both powerlifting and weightlifting, getting into the sport at an early age can help ensure that your competitive peak can coincide with your physiological peak. However, if you don’t have a time machine, you can still improve powerlifting performance (and, more generally, the ability to build muscle and increase your capacity to generate force) well into your 30s and beyond. Time catches up with everyone eventually, but the impact of aging on force generation starts later and proceeds more gradually than the impact of aging on velocity and power generation. years old (Figure 3). So, if you’re just getting into lifting in your 40s or 50s, your rate of progress may not be quite as fast as someone in their late 20s or early 30s, but on the flip side, new lifters in their late 20s or early 30s can generally make a LOT of progress pretty quickly. You’ll probably also be able to make very substantial progress, just at a somewhat slower rate. An interesting finding of the present study (1) is that athletes tended to improve their placing by changing weight classes. However, I’d caution you about getting too trigger happy on jumping to a new weight class, assuming it will improve your competitiveness. Since this was a study on internationally competitive lifters, most of the lifters shifting weight classes probably did so because they knew they could move into a less competitive weight class. In other words, we don’t know that they actually became more competitive in a vacuum (i.e. their Wilks or Sinclair scores improved). Rather, it’s likely they moved into a new weight class because they knew that the weight class above or below their own was likely to be less competitive in the upcoming year due to injuries, retirements, etc. I do think it’s worth noting that the majority of athletes that changed weight classes tended to move up a weight class, though. In general, you should be the most competitive in the heaviest weight class you can fill out while maintaining pretty good body composition. The fact that way more of the internationally competitive lifters in this sample moved up a weight class rather than down a weight class bears that out. There’s one more observation I’d like to bring your attention to: The female lifters in this sample made more progress (on a percentage basis) than the male lifters in the five years prior to peak performance. That matches trends I’ve noted in both the published literature (mostly in studies on untrained lifters) and in powerlifters more generally (not just world-class lifters): On a relative basis, female lifters seem to progress faster than male lifters. I’ll be hon- 95 est: I’m not sure why that’s the case. In untrained lifters, differences in starting points could certainly play a role (i.e. “untrained” female lifters may be more untrained, relatively speaking, than “untrained” male lifters, perhaps due to differences in sport participation or jobs involving manual labor). However, I’d assume that any of those baseline differences would be washed out by the time people felt sufficiently trained to step on a powerlifting platform, and certainly before they became world-class lifters. I’m really not sure why relative rates of strength progress would be higher in females than males, but it’s an observation that’s shown up several times now, in lifters ranging from untrained to internationally competitive. I hope we see some future research on physiological factors underpinning this observation. Next Steps In general, I’d just like to see a repeat of this study in 10 years with raw lifters used as the powerlifting sample. The sport is growing so much, and more youth lifters are getting into powerlifting, so I’ll be interested to see if peak age stays in the mid-30s, or if it trends a bit younger over time. 96 References 1. Solberg PA, Hopkins WG, Paulsen G, Haugen TA. Peak Age and Performance Progression in World-Class Weightlifting and Powerlifting Athletes. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2019 Oct 7:1-7. 2. Reid KF, Fielding RA. Skeletal muscle power: a critical determinant of physical functioning in older adults. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2012 Jan;40(1):4-12. 3. It’s almost necessarily true that power will decrease faster than force output. Since power is force multiplied by velocity, if force output goes down 10%, then power must also decrease by at least 10%. If any other changes occur that impact velocity, power necessarily decreases at a faster rate than force does. 4. In sedentary folks, type II fibers also atrophy due to aging at a faster rate than type I fibers, but that shouldn’t be a major factor in elite strength athletes █ 97 VIDEO: Powerlifting Game Day BY MIC HAE L C . ZO URD O S MASS has many articles and videos on programming for powerlifting, but what can you expect at the actual competition? This video breaks down everything you need to prepare, and what you need to know about powerlifting game day as a coach and lifter. Click to watch Michael's presentation. 98 References 1. Pritchard, HJ and Morton, RH. Powerlifting: Success and Failure at the 2012 Oceania and 2013 Classic World Championships. J Aust Strength Cond 23: 67–70, 2015. 2. Gary, M. A Powerlifter’s Guide to Attempt Selection. Maryland Powerlifting, 2009. 3. USA Powerlifting. Lifter’s Handbook. 2016. 4. International Powerlifting Federation. Technical Rules Book. 2019. █ 99 VIDEO: New Perspectives on Activity and Bodyweight BY E RI C HE LMS We are still uncovering the mechanisms of how humans regulate body weight. Typically, this is viewed from the perspective of our body attempting to maintain a certain level of adiposity or mass during an energy deficit or surplus. However, there are likely regulatory mechanisms related to total daily energy expenditure that influence body weight as well. Click to watch Eric's presentation. 100 References 1. Melby CL, Paris HL, Sayer RD, Bell C, Hill JO. Increasing Energy Flux to Maintain Diet-Induced Weight Loss. Nutrients. 2019 Oct 21;11(10). 2. Speakman JR. The evolution of body fatness: trading off disease and predation risk. The Journal of experimental biology. 2018 Mar 7;221(Pt Suppl 1). 3. Beaulieu K, Hopkins M, Blundell J, Finlayson G. Homeostatic and non-homeostatic appetite control along the spectrum of physical activity levels: An updated perspective. Physiology & behavior. 2018 Aug 1;192:23-9. 4. Pontzer H. Energy Constraint as a Novel Mechanism Linking Exercise and Health. Physiology (Bethesda, Md.). 2018 Nov 1;33(6):384. 5. Mountjoy M, Sundgot-Borgen JK, Burke LM, Ackerman KE, Blauwet C, Constantini N, Lebrun C, Lundy B, Melin AK, Meyer NL, Sherman RT. IOC consensus statement on relative energy deficiency in sport (RED-S): 2018 update. British journal of sports medicine. 2018 Jun;52(11):687. █ 101 Just Missed the Cut Every month, we consider hundreds of new papers, and they can’t all be included in MASS. Therefore, we’re happy to share a few pieces of research that just missed the cut. It’s our hope that with the knowledge gained from reading MASS, along with our interpreting research guide, you’ll be able to tackle these on your own. 1. Schwarz et al. A Comparison of Machine versus Free-Weight Squats for the Enhancement of Lower-Body Power, Speed, and Change-of-Direction Ability during an Initial Training Phase of Recreationally-Active Women 2. Bennett et al. A randomised controlled trial of movement quality-focused exercise versus traditional resistance exercise for improving movement quality and physical performance in trained adults 3. Wilke et al. Acute Effects of Foam Rolling on Range of Motion in Healthy Adults: A Systematic Review with Multilevel Meta-analysis 4. Huebner eet al. Age-associated Performance Decline and Sex Differences in Olympic Weightlifting 5. Delgado et al. Comparison Between Back Squat, Romanian Deadlift, and Barbell Hip Thrust for Leg and Hip Muscle Activities During Hip Extension 6. Schoenfeld et al. Does Training to Failure Maximize Muscle Hypertrophy? 7. Mielgo-Ayuso et al. Effect of Caffeine Supplementation on Sports Performance Based on Differences Between Sexes: A Systematic Review 8. Barrón-Cabrera et al. Epigenetic Modifications as Outcomes of Exercise Interventions Related to Specific Metabolic Alterations: A Systematic Review. 9. Baptista et al. Exercise Dependence: An Updated Systematic Review 10. Armour et al. Exercise for dysmenorrhoea. 11. Kristiansen et al. Inter- and intra-individual variability in the kinematics of the back squat 12. Kasovic et al. Kinematic Differences Between the Front and Back Squat and Conventional and Sumo Deadlift 13. Moro et al. Low skeletal muscle capillarization limits muscle adaptation to resistance exercise training in older adults 14. Hyatt et al. Muscle-Specific Sensitivity to Voluntary Physical Activity and Detraining 15. Tung et al. Physiological and Biochemical Effects of Intrinsically High and Low Exercise Capacities Through Multiomics Approaches 102 16. Barreto et al. Protective Effect Conferred by Isometric Preconditioning Against Slowand Fast-Velocity Eccentric Exercise-Induced Muscle Damage 17. Martínez-Cava et al. Range of Motion and Sticking Region Effects on the Bench Press Load-Velocity Relationship 18. Tungate. The Bench Press: A Comparison Between Flat-Back and Arched-Back Techniques 19. García-Ramos et al. The load-velocity profiles of three upper-body pushing exercises in men and women 20. Pareja-Blanco et al. Time course of recovery from resistance exercise before and after a training program 21. Chapman et al. Using Perceptual and Neuromuscular Responses to Estimate Mechanical Changes During Continuous Sets in the Bench Press 103 Thanks for reading MASS. The next issue will be released to subscribers on January 1, 2020. Graphics by Kat Whitfield, and layout design by Lyndsey Nuckols. 104