REPORTS Pollinator-Mediated Selection on Flower Color Allele Drives Reinforcement Robin Hopkins* and Mark D. Rausher Reinforcement is the process by which reduced hybrid fitness generates selection favoring the evolution of stronger prezygotic reproductive barriers between emerging species. Using common-garden field experiments, we quantified the strength of reinforcing selection in nature by demonstrating strong selection favoring an allele conferring increased pigment intensity in the plant Phlox drummondii in areas of sympatry with the closely related species Phlox cuspidata. Incomplete hybrid sterility between the two species generates selection for traits that decrease interspecies hybridization. In contrast, selection on this locus is undetectable in the absence of P. cuspidata. We demonstrate that reinforcing selection is generated by nonrandom pollinator movement, in which pollinators move less frequently between intensely pigmented P. drummondii and P. cuspidata than between lightly pigmented P. drummondii and P. cuspidata. R Department of Biology, Box 90338, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA. *To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: robin.hopkins@duke.edu 1090 (F3′5′h) alters the anthocyanin pigment composition of flowers and changes them from blue to red. At this hue locus, the ancestral “blue” allele (H) is dominant to the derived “red” allele (h). Up-regulation of an R2R3-Myb transcription factor increases the amount of pigments produced, resulting in increased color intensity. At this intensity locus, the derived “dark” allele (I) is dominant to the ancestral “light” allele (i). Western, allopatric P. drummondii populations are fixed for the i and H alleles, whereas eastern, sympatric populations are fixed or nearly fixed for the I and h alleles, and the two recombinant flower colors, light-red (iihh) and dark-blue (I-,H-), occur only near the boundary between allopatric and sympatric populations (15). Patterns of neutral genetic variation across the range of P. drummondii suggest extensive gene flow between allopatric and sympatric populations, indicating that natural selection and not genetic drift is likely responsible for the geographic pattern of flower color variation (15). To determine whether selection in sympatry is due primarily to environmental factors acting directly on flower color variation, rather than to effects of reinforcement, we performed a common-garden field experiment designed to detect selection in the absence of P. cuspidata. We measured average fitness of the four flower-color double-homozygote genotypes in their natural habitat. Three generations of crosses were performed to produce seeds of known flower-color genotype and to randomize the genetic background of loci unlinked to the two flower-color loci (19). For clarity, we will refer to the homozygous color genotypes by their corresponding flower color throughout the remainder of this paper. A total of 2720 seeds were planted in a randomized block design, with 170 individuals per genotype per block, at the University of Texas Stengl research station (Smithville, Texas). This station is located within the sympatric region of P. drummondii and P. cuspidata and contains natural populations of both species (19). No significant differences in survival or reproductive success among the flower-color genotypes were observed (table S2). We noted that 2 MARCH 2012 VOL 335 SCIENCE Fig. 1. Fitness components for each flower color genotype. (A) Survival probability (n = 1909). (B) Fruit production (n = 1240). (C) Fitness (n = 1909). (D) Relative hybridization rate (n = 931). Bars indicate 1 SE. www.sciencemag.org Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on August 8, 2020 einforcement is the evolution of increased prezygotic reproductive isolation due to selection favoring decreased hybridization between diverging groups of individuals or emerging species (1–4). A. R. Wallace first proposed that selection against hybrids might favor the evolution of novel prezygotic isolating barriers (subsequently termed the Wallace effect) in 1889 (5). Although this idea has been controversial, recent theoretical and empirical work suggests that reinforcement may often play an important role in increasing reproductive isolation in nature (1, 3, 4, 6, 7). However, the magnitude of reinforcing selection in nature is generally unknown, as are the genes upon which such selection acts. Theoretical models have demonstrated that direct environmental selection can be more effective in influencing trait evolution than reinforcing selection (6, 8–11), but previous investigations of reinforcement have rarely differentiated between these two types of selection [but see (12, 13)]. Flower color variation in Phlox drummondii has been hypothesized to be an example of reinforcement (14). The geographic range of this species partly overlaps with that of a congener, P. cuspidata, in eastern Texas. Both species have the light-blue (sometimes called violet or pink) flower color characteristic of most Phlox species in allopatric areas of their ranges, whereas P. drummondii has dark-red flowers in regions sympatric with P. cuspidata (15). In the region of sympatry, populations of the two species frequently grow in close proximity; produce hybrids in nature that have high, but not complete, ovule and pollen sterility; and exhibit some interspecific gene flow (16, 17). In P. drummondii, the difference between the ancestral light-blue flower color and the derived dark-red flower color is caused by mutations in the cis-regulatory regions of two genes (18). Down-regulation of the gene coding for the enzyme Flavonoid 3′5′-hydroxylase survival was slightly lower for two derived genotypes (dark-blue and dark-red), compared with the ancestral genotype (light-blue), whereas it was slightly higher for the derived genotype light-red (Fig. 1A). The number of fruits produced was slightly higher for all three derived genotypes compared with light-blue (Fig. 1B), but these differences were also not statistically significant (table S3). There were no detectable differences among genotypes for number of seeds per fruit (table S4). Female fitness, the product of survival and fruit production, was also slightly higher for the derived genotypes, compared with light-blue genotype (Fig. 1C), but again none of these differences were statistically significant (19). Overall, we did not detect environmental effects acting directly on flower color favoring the derived allele at either the hue or the intensity locus in the area of sympatry. To examine whether reinforcing selection generated by hybridization with P. cuspidata favors the derived allele at either flower-color locus, we established blocks consisting of 30 plants of one of the double-homozygous genotypes (“focal plants”), as well as 105 light-blue plants of a stock line. We followed 30 of the light-blue stock individuals as “reference plants” to control for environmental variation among blocks. In addition, we planted 115 P. cuspidata plants in each block. We collected fruits from reference and focal plants and randomly chose 100 to 150 seeds from each REPORTS likely resulted in the subsequent spread of the dark allele throughout the sympatric region. To our surprise, there appeared to be no effect of the red allele at the hue locus on hybridization, even though this allele is fixed in sympatric populations. Taken together with the lack of difference in fitness among genotypes in the first experiment, the difference in hybridization rates between dark and light plants supports the hypothesis that reinforcing selection is responsible for the fixation of the dark allele in sympatric populations. Manual pollen transfers indicate that there is no difference between light and dark flowered plants in fertilization success of P. cuspidata pollen (14). These data suggest that dark-flowered individuals are not less compatible with P. cuspidata pollen than light-flowered individuals. It is therefore likely that nonrandom patterns of pollinator visitation between Phlox species with different flower colors explain our observed variation in hybridization. We examined patterns of pollinator visitation in experimental arrays to test this hypothesis. We constructed three arrays containing light-blue plants, P. cuspidata plants, and either light-red, dark-red, or dark-blue focal plants (19). We observed a total of 181 pollinators making a total of 2301 transitions between plants. The primary visitors to both Phlox species in these arrays, as in natural populations, were Battus philenor butterflies (108 individuals observed) and various species of skippers (Lepidoptera, family Hesperiidae) (73 individuals observed) (table S6a). Both types of pollinators displayed Table 1. The percentage of transitions by pollinators across flower types. (A) Pollinator movement between colors and species within arrays containing light-red P. drummondii. (B) Pollinator movement between colors and species within arrays containing dark-blue P. drummondii. (C) Pollinator movement between colors and species within arrays containing dark-red P. drummondii. The percentages of transitions between P. cuspidata and P. drummondii plants are in bold. A Pollinators in arrays with the light-red P. drummondii moved To (%) From Light-red (252) Light-blue (219) P. cuspidata (185) Light-red Light-blue P. cuspidata 32 50 34 44 22 31 24 28 35 B Pollinators in arrays with the dark-blue P. drummondii moved From Dark-blue (222) Light-blue (198) P. cuspidata (274) Dark-blue To (%) Light-blue P. cuspidata 51 42 7 38 18 32 11 40 61 Dark-red To (%) Light-blue P. cuspidata 37 37 9 50 25 33 13 38 58 C Pollinators in arrays with the dark-red P. drummondii moved From Dark-red (255) Light-blue (335) P. cuspidata (378) www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 335 similar movement patterns and visited both Phlox species extensively (table S6b). In arrays with lightred plants, there is no evidence of pollinator constancy, as measured by the Bateman’s Constancy Index (19, 20) (table S6c). Pollinators were equally likely to visit light-blue and light-red plants after visiting P. cuspidata (Table 1A) (19). This pattern is consistent with finding no difference in hybridization rates between these two genotypes. In addition, pollinators were equally likely to visit a P. cuspidata plant after visiting a light-blue or a light-red plant, which suggested that pollen wastage through interspecific fertilization does not differ between these two genotypes (Table 1A) (19). In contrast, pollinators in arrays containing either dark-blue or dark-red plants exhibited a significantly higher species-level Bateman Constancy Index for dark-flowered genotypes compared with light-flowered genotypes (table S6c). In particular, pollinators were half as likely to visit dark plants as light-blue plants after visiting a P. cuspidata—a pattern that explains the reduced hybridization observed in plants with dark pigmentation (Table 1, B and C) (19). Pollinators were also substantially less likely to visit a P. cuspidata after visiting a dark-blue or dark-red plant than after visiting a light-blue plant (Table 1, B and C) (19), which suggested that darkly pigmented plants waste less pollen on interspecific pollination. Although we did not directly measure male fitness in our field experiments, this observation indicates that the dark allele may significantly increase male fitness, in addition to female fitness, relative to the light allele. It is possible that pollinators could be responding to pleiotropic effects of the intense allele (e.g., nectar volume or concentration), but this seems unlikely given the visual orientation of the primary pollinator, B. philenor (21). Our investigations provide no explanation for why sympatric populations of P. drummondii have evolved red flowers. Although previously we used patterns of genetic variation at this locus and at neutral markers to show that natural selection drove the fixation of the red (h) allele in the region of sympatry (15), in the current study, we detected neither fitness differences nor differences in levels of interspecific hybridization between genotypes at the hue locus. One possible explanation for these contrasting results is that selection due to environmental factors favors the red allele in sympatry but that the magnitude of this selection is too small to detect given the power of our analysis (as may be evidenced by Fig. 1C). A second possibility is that environmental selection operates only intermittently on this locus and was absent during our experiments. Finally, a third possibility is that a selective agent, such as another type of pollinator, generated selection in the past on the hue locus but is no longer present. Although hitchhiking by the red allele in a selective sweep involving a closely linked gene is a formal possibility, this seems unlikely because it would have required that the favored mutation arose on a rare haplotype carrying the red allele. 2 MARCH 2012 Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on August 8, 2020 focal and reference genotype in each block to genotype and determine whether the paternal parent was P. drummondii or P. cuspidata (19). Using this paternity test, we calculated the hybridization rate for focal and reference genotypes within each block. Across the four blocks, the hybridization rate (proportion of seeds sired by P. cuspidata) varied between 28 and 44% for the light-blue reference plants, which indicated substantial overall interspecific hybridization. The hybridization rates of the light-blue and light-red focal plants were similar to those of their respective reference plants (Fig. 1D and table S5, a and b). In contrast, the hybridization rates of dark-blue and dark-red focal plants were more than 50% lower than the reference plants (Fig. 1D and table S5, a and b). Thus, we conclude that the dark allele (I) at the intensity locus significantly decreases hybridization between P. drummondii and P. cuspidata. Given conservative empirical estimates of hybrid sterility of ~90% (17) and the average light-blue reference plant hybridization rate of 0.43, the reduction in hybridization translates into a selection coefficient of 0.32 favoring the dark allele (19). The two species of Phlox are commonly found in intermixed populations with spatial proximity similar to that in our experiments. In these populations, the strong reinforcing selection documented here would increase the frequency of the dark allele rapidly to fixation. Extensive gene flow between P. drummondii populations (15), including those without nearby P. cuspidata, has 1091 REPORTS 1092 Although reinforcement has been studied primarily in animals (3, 7), our work indicates that it may also be an important contributor to speciation in plants. If so, this phenomenon may provide a partial explanation for the tremendous diversity of floral color, floral morphology, and inflorescence structure that characterize flowering plants. References and Notes 1. R. Butlin, Trends Ecol. Evol. 2, 8 (1987). 2. T. Dobzhansky, Genetics and the Origin of Species (Columbia Univ. Press, New York, 1937). 3. D. J. Howard, in Hybrid Zones and the Evolutionary Process, R. G. Harrison, Ed. (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1993), pp. 46–69. 4. M. R. Servedio, M. A. F. Noor, Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 34, 339 (2003). 5. A. R. Wallace, Darwinism: An Exposition of the Theory of Natural Selection, with Some of Its Applications (Macmillan, London, 1889). 6. M. Kirkpatrick, V. Ravigné, Am. Nat. 159 (suppl. 3), S22 (2002). 7. D. Ortiz-Barrientos, A. Grealy, P. Nosil, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1168, 156 (2009). 8. M. Kirkpatrick, Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 267, 1649 (2000). 9. M. Kirkpatrick, M. R. Servedio, Genetics 151, 865 (1999). 10. M. R. Servedio, Evolution 55, 1909 (2001). 11. M. R. Servedio, Evolution 58, 913 (2004). 12. A. Y. K. Albert, D. Schluter, Evolution 58, 1099 (2004). 13. P. Nosil, B. J. Crespi, C. P. Sandoval, Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 270, 1911 (2003). 14. D. A. Levin, Evolution 39, 1275 (1985). 15. R. Hopkins, D. A. Levin, M. D. Rausher, Evolution 66, 469 (2012). 16. D. A. Levin, Am. J. Bot. 54, 1122 (1967). 17. L. G. Ruane, K. Donohue, Evol. Ecol. 22, 229 (2008). 18. R. Hopkins, M. D. Rausher, Nature 469, 411 (2011). 19. Material and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online. 20. N. M. Waser, Am. Nat. 127, 593 (1986). 21. M. D. Rausher, Science 200, 1071 (1978). 22. H. D. Bradshaw Jr., D. W. Schemske, Nature 426, 176 (2003). 23. D. R. Campbell, N. M. Waser, E. J. Melendez-Ackerman, Am. Nat. 149, 295 (1997). 24. M. E. Hoballah et al., Plant Cell 19, 779 (2007). 25. L. Chittka, J. D. Thomson, N. M. Waser, Naturwissenschaften 86, 361 (1999). 26. M. Caisse, J. Antonovics, Heredity 40, 371 (1978). 27. J. Felsenstein, Evolution 35, 124 (1981). 28. L. W. Liou, T. D. Price, Evolution 48, 1451 (1994). 29. M. R. Servedio, Evolution 54, 21 (2000). Acknowledgments: We thank M. Kirkpatrick, S. Otto, M. Whitlock, D. Des Marais, and members of the Rausher and Kirkpatrick laboratory group for advice on this manuscript and S. Scarpino for statistical consultation. We thank the University of Texas Stengl Research Station for field experiment support. This work was supported by NSF grant 0841521 to M.D.R. and a NSF Doctoral Dissertation Improvement Grant to R.H. and M.D.R. R.H. was supported by the NSF Graduate Research Fellowship Program. All data presented here are available in the supporting material. Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/science.1215198/DC1 Materials and Methods Figs. S1 and S2 Tables S1 to S9 References 12 October 2011; accepted 12 January 2012 Published online 2 February 2012; 10.1126/science.1215198 Generation of Leaf Shape Through Early Patterns of Growth and Tissue Polarity Erika E. Kuchen,1* Samantha Fox,1* Pierre Barbier de Reuille,2 Richard Kennaway,2 Sandra Bensmihen,1 Jerome Avondo,1 Grant M. Calder,1 Paul Southam,2 Sarah Robinson,1 Andrew Bangham,2† Enrico Coen1† A major challenge in biology is to understand how buds comprising a few cells can give rise to complex plant and animal appendages like leaves or limbs. We address this problem through a combination of time-lapse imaging, clonal analysis, and computational modeling. We arrive at a model that shows how leaf shape can arise through feedback between early patterns of oriented growth and tissue deformation. Experimental tests through partial leaf ablation support this model and allow reevaluation of previous experimental studies. Our model allows a range of observed leaf shapes to be generated and predicts observed clone patterns in different species. Thus, our experimentally validated model may underlie the development and evolution of diverse organ shapes. T he shapes of many plant and animal appendages are thought to develop under the influence of orthogonal organizing 1 John Innes Centre, Norwich Research Park, Norwich, NR4 7UH, UK. 2School of Computing Sciences, University of East Anglia, Norwich Research Park, Norwich, NR4 7TJ, UK. *These authors contributed equally to this work. †To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: enrico.coen@jic.ac.uk (E.C.); a.bangham@uea.ac.uk (A.B.) 2 MARCH 2012 VOL 335 SCIENCE systems (i.e., systems with axes that intersect at right angles) (1–4). However, it is unclear how these orthogonal systems lead to changes in tissue shape and how shape changes may themselves feed back to deform the organizing systems. Consider a square piece of tissue that deforms during growth (Fig. 1A). The tissue has an initial linear orthogonal system that organizes the pattern of morphogenesis (Fig. 1B, arrows). We might en- www.sciencemag.org Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on August 8, 2020 By measuring reinforcing selection acting on the dark flower–color allele in P. drummondii under natural sympatric conditions and by quantifying selection in the absence of P. cuspidata, we were able to compare the relative strengths of direct selection by other environmental factors and by reinforcing selection on a trait conferring increased premating isolation in a region of sympatry. The absence of detectable fitness differences among flower color genotypes in the absence of P. cuspidata indicates that another agent of selection is unlikely to be involved in flower color divergence in P. drummondii. Although we cannot rule out small, statistically undetectable differences in survival or reproductive success favoring these genotypes, such differences would be of minor importance compared with the strong reinforcing selection acting on the intensity locus. Many plants have evolved premating reproductive isolation by switching pollinator types (e.g., from bees to hummingbirds) (22–24). Our work suggests that increased reproductive isolation can also be achieved by a single pollinator species through constancy of individual pollinators. In particular, if pollinators transition between flowers with similar phenotypes more frequently then between flowers with unlike phenotypes, this will decrease gene flow between unlike flowers. Constancy is commonly studied in bumble bees but rarely investigated in butterfly pollinators (20, 25). That the primary pollinator Battus philenor exhibits this type of constancy is not surprising, given that females of this species exhibit constancy for leaf shape when searching for oviposition sites (21). Theoretical models indicate that the likelihood of successful reinforcement is greater when selection is strong, because this will counteract gene flow and recombination, which tend to reduce premating isolation (26–28). Our results indicate that, at least in some cases, very strong reinforcing selection may act on a single allele and lead to increased reproductive isolation. Theory also indicates that reinforcement is more easily achieved by a one-allele mechanism (4, 29), but empirical assessment of this prediction has been difficult because the genetic basis of reinforcement is understood in few systems (7). Our current demonstration of reinforcing selection acting on the dark allele indicates that reinforcement in P. drummondii involves a twoallele reinforcement mechanism. The intensity locus causes reproductive isolation only if the dark allele is present in P. drummondii and the light allele is present in P. cuspidata. Consistent with theory, we find that strong selection and high levels of hybrid sterility cause the spread of the dark allele through sympatric P. drummondii populations. We suspect all reinforcement mechanisms involving different floral phenotypes to which pollination vectors must respond will be two-allele assortative mating mechanisms, because pollinators must be able to discriminate between the novel phenotype in one species and the ancestral phenotype in both species. Pollinator-Mediated Selection on Flower Color Allele Drives Reinforcement Robin Hopkins and Mark D. Rausher Science 335 (6072), 1090-1092. DOI: 10.1126/science.1215198originally published online February 2, 2012 ARTICLE TOOLS http://science.sciencemag.org/content/335/6072/1090 SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS http://science.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2012/02/02/science.1215198.DC1 REFERENCES This article cites 25 articles, 3 of which you can access for free http://science.sciencemag.org/content/335/6072/1090#BIBL PERMISSIONS http://www.sciencemag.org/help/reprints-and-permissions Use of this article is subject to the Terms of Service Science (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published by the American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. The title Science is a registered trademark of AAAS. Copyright © 2012, American Association for the Advancement of Science Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on August 8, 2020 The Constant Pollinator In ecology, reinforcement is the process by which species prevent hybridization and maintain species boundaries, but the underlying genetic mechanisms are unclear. Hopkins and Rausher (p. 1090, published online 2 February) examined reinforcement between two species of a wild flowering plant called Phlox that show incomplete hybrid sterility. Down-regulation of a flavonoid gene produces red flowers and operates in concert with a color intensity locus to adjust flower color and tone. A distinct geography of flower color has emerged in which it appears that dark coloration causes less hybridization between the species because the butterfly pollinators tend to favor light-blue flower color variants. If pollinators visit flowers with similar phenotypes more frequently than flowers with dissimilar phenotypes, this will decrease gene flow between the unlike flowers. www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/science.1215198/DC1 Supporting Online Material for Pollinator-Mediated Selection on Flower Color Allele Drives Reinforcement Robin Hopkins* and Mark D. Rausher *To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: robin.hopkins@duke.edu Published 3 February 2012 on Science Express DOI: 10.1126/science.1215198 This PDF file includes Materials and Methods Figs. S1 and S2 Tables S1 to S6 Full References Other Supporting Online Material for this manuscript includes the following: (available at www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/science.1215198/DC1) Table S7. Field Experiment 1 – Fruit-Set and Survival (Excel) Table S8. Field Experiment 2- Hybridization (Excel) Table S9. Pollinator Observation (Excel) Materials and Methods Growing conditions In order to induce germination, we soaked all Phlox seeds in 500ppm gibberellic acid solution for 48 hours, planted them in Metro-Mix 360 (Sun Gro Horticulture, Bellevue, WA) and stratified them for 7 days at 4°C. Once seeds germinated we grew the plants in the Duke University Greenhouses (Durham, NC) or the University of Texas at Austin greenhouses (Austin, TX). We planted experimental seedlings into the field once the plants started growing true leaves. Breeding design We performed a series of crosses to produce seeds of known genotype at the flower color loci (figure S1). This crossing design randomized the genetic background of unlinked loci and ensured that experimental seeds of the four double homozygote genotypes were equally outbred. Seeds from each of the two field experiments were created from slightly different crossing designs as described below. All genotyping occurred as described by Hopkins and Rausher (18). For the first field experiment, in which we tested for ecological selection acting on flower color alleles, seeds were collected from natural populations in the spring of 2006. P. drummondii seeds were collected from two populations with dark-red flowers (Poc2104 and 80S1) and two populations with light-blue flowers (Dog95 and 466E7) (table S1). Individuals from population Dog95 were paired with individuals from population Poc2104, and individuals from 466E7 were paired with individuals from 80S1. Pairs of individuals were crossed to create F1 individuals and these F1’s were selffertilized to create F2 populations. We created a total of four F2 populations from the population cross 466E7X80S1 and six F2 populations from the population cross Dog95XPoc2104. F2 individuals were genotyped at markers in the hue locus (F3’5’h) and the intensity locus (R2-R3 Myb) in order to identify individuals homozygous at single nucleotide polymorphisms differentiating the two parents at each locus. F2 populations created from the same parental source populations were paired for a total of 2 pairs from F2 populations 466E7X80S1 and 3 pairs from Dog95XPoc2104. Within each pair of F2 populations, individuals with the same homozygous genotype at both flower color loci were crossed to each other. There were a total of four possible homozygous genotype combinations within each F2 population pair: light-blue (iiHH), dark-blue (IIHH), lightred (iihh), and dark-red (IIhh). Crosses between paired F2 populations created the seeds used in the field experiment. This crossing design created five sets of outbred seed families from two sets of source populations, with known homozygous genotype at the flower color loci and with randomized genetic background. The crossing design for the field experiment in which we measured hybridization had a similar design to the one described above. Individuals from the following populations were crossed to create F1 individuals: 466E7XPoc2104 and Dog95X80S1 and Dog95XPoc2104. The F1 individuals were self-fertilized to create F2 populations. F3 experimental seeds were created by crossing F2 individuals homozygous at the flower color loci from the 466E7XPoc2104 cross to both the Dog95X80S1 F2s, and the Dog95XPoc2104 F2s. Light-blue stock lines used as standards in the hybridization experiment were created by randomly crossing five individuals collected from population 696N1 for two generations. Offspring from these plants were randomly distributed between blocks. P. cuspidata seeds that were collected from population 2104circ. Plants were self fertilized in the greenhouse for two generations and seeds were randomly distributed across blocks. Planting Procedures For the first field experiment, which tested for direct selection on flower color, experimental seeds were planted in a randomized block design across four spatial blocks. Within each block, an equal number of seeds for each flower color genotype within each source population were fully randomized. Seeds were allowed to germinate and produce the first true leaf in the University of Texas at Austin greenhouses (Austin, TX). In early December 2008, seedlings were planted into four spatially separated blocks at the University of Texas Stengl research station (Smithville, TX). Within each block, seedlings were planted 15 cm apart in 8 rows. Seedlings were given supplementary water every three days for two weeks to enhance transplant survival, after which plants were left to survive under natural conditions. The Stengl Field Station (http://www.bfl.utexas.edu/stengl) is located in the sympatric range of the two Phlox species. This 208 acre preserve is composed of natural pine forests, and meadows which are managed to encourage the growth of native herbaceous species. The common garden field experiments were performed in fenced plots within some of the meadows. Both species of Phlox are found naturally on the field station property as well as neighboring properties and roadsides. Pollinators have been observed visiting natural Phlox populations at the station. Experimental plants overwintered as small vegetative rosettes, grew vegetatively through February, then began to produce reproductive shoots and flower. They flowered and set fruit through the end of June at which point they dried-up and died. We surveyed for overwinter survival at the end of January and continued to monitor survival weekly for the remainder of the experiment. We counted and removed fruits as they ripened on each plant and collected a subset of fruits in order to calculate average seed set per fruit. When almost completely ripe, four to ten fruits were collected on 8 plants of each flower color genotype from each block for a total of 128 plants, 547 fruits, and 1290 seeds The second field experiment measuring hybridization rate was planted in a split plot design with four blocks containing one of the four flower color genotypes as focal color for the block. Within each block, we planted reference light-blue plants and P. cuspidata plants as well as focal color plants in a fully randomized design. We planted individuals 15 cm apart in four rows within each block. All other methods were similar to those described above. We germinated seeds in the University of Texas at Austin greenhouses and allowed them to produce the first true leaves prior to transplanting them in the field in late January, 2010. We provided supplemental water to enhance transplant survival. Plants were regularly surveyed for survival and fruits were collected to determine paternity. Analysis of survival To determine whether flower color genotypes differed in probability of survival to flowering, we performed a linear analysis using the procedure Catmod in the SAS v.9.2 statistical package (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC) with survival to flower as the dependent variable. The independent factors were: (i) block (ii) L vs. D at the intensity locus (iii) B vs. R at the hue locus (iv) source population Parameters in the model were estimated using maximum likelihood. The generalized Wald statistic, which is approximately chi-square distributed, is computed to test hypotheses about linear combinations of parameters. The results are presented in table S2. Neither the effect of the hue locus nor the effect of the intensity locus nor the effect of their interaction on survival was significant, indicating no evidence of an effect of flower color genotype on survival to flowering. Analysis of fruit production Fruit production was analyzed using a mixed model Analysis of Variance with the PROC MIXED procedure in SAS using the method of restricted maximum likelihood. The fixed-effects in the analysis were intensity genotype, hue genotype, and the interaction. The random-effects were block and source population (source) and all two and three-way interactions involving fixed effects. Fruit number was log-transformed before analysis. Untransformed analysis produced similar results. Neither intensity genotype, nor hue genotype nor their interaction had a significant effect on fruit number (table S3a). Significance of random effects was determined by using a likelihood ratio χ2 statistic. First, the complete model was run and second a model was run without one of the random effects. The difference in the log likelihoods between the two models was calculated and used to test for the importance of the random effect in the model using a χ2 distribution with degrees of freedom equal to the difference in the number of covariance parameters between the two models. This was done for each of the random effects in succession. Block was the only significant random effect in the model (table S3b). Analysis of seeds per fruit We analyzed number of seeds per fruit on a subset of fruits using a mixed-model Analysis of Variance as described for the fruit set data above. Neither intensity genotype, nor hue genotype nor their interaction had a significant effect on number of seeds per fruit (table S4a). None of the random covariance estimates significantly contributed to the model fit (table S4b). Calculation of Fitness We calculated mean fitness of each genotype by multiplying the probability of survival by the number of fruits produced. We calculated the variance of mean fitness from the standard formula for the variance of a product of two random variables: Var[X· Y] = E[X]2 Var[Y] + E[Y]2 Var[X] + Var[X] Var[Y] The standard error of fitness is the square root of the variance. The standard errors overlapped and we therefore judged the mean fitnesses to not be significantly different. Genotyping hybrids We germinated seeds collected from the experimental plants in the common garden in the Duke University greenhouse. We collected tissue from each individual and extracted DNA using a modified CTAB extraction protocol (18). An intron length polymorphism in the gene Anthocyanin synthase (ANS) differentiates the two Phlox species with P. drummondii having a 10-12 base pair smaller intron than P. cuspidata. A portion of this intron was amplified using the following two primers: Forward: GCGGGACATGTCGATTTGGC Reverse: CCCGTATGGGGAATACATTC The forward primer was fluorescently labeled with FAM allowing the intron size to be scored using capillary electrophoresis and fragment analysis on an ABI 3730x1 DNA Analyzer. Intron size was scored by eye using the program GENE MARKER (SoftGenetics, 2005, State College, PA). All offspring contained a maternal allele from P. drummondii and a paternal allele from either P. drummondii or P. cuspidata. Analysis of hybridization rate We analyzed variation in hybridization rates between different flower color genotypes using bootstrapping. The following method accounted for variation in mother and offspring sample size and for between-block variation in hybridization rate. We first estimated hybridization rate of each genotype in each block and then tested specific hypotheses about variation in hybridization rates. Estimating hybridization rates. Using bootstrapping we calculated standard errors around our estimates of hybridization rate for each genotype. This involved sampling with replacement mothers from within each genotype and block. Once mothers were sampled we sampled offspring from that mother with replacement. We then calculated the hybridization rate for that genotype to create a single bootstrap replicate estimate. We repeated this 10,000 times and determined the standard error by calculating the standard deviation of the bootstrap replicate estimates. We performed this for all 8 genotypes (4 focal colors and 4 reference) (table S5a.). In order to control for between-block variation, we standardized the focal plant hybridization rates by dividing by the hybridization rate of the reference plants in that block. Specifically, we divided the vector of 10,000 bootstrap replicate estimates of the focal color by the vector of 10,000 bootstrap replicate estimates of the corresponding reference for the block. This gave us 10,000 bootstrap replicate estimates of the standardized hybridization of each flower color genotype. We calculated a new standard error for this estimate of relative hybridization (table S5a). Hypothesis testing. We used bootstrap re-sampling to test specific hypotheses about the effects of the flower color loci on hybridization rate. Specifically, we tested whether genotype at the intensity locus affected hybridization, if genotype at the hue locus affected hybridization, and if there was an interaction between genotypes at the hue and intensity locus. We first calculated the observed value for each of these comparisons. In order to control for variation between blocks we again standardized the hybridization rates of the focal plants by dividing by that of the reference plants. The first comparison of interest is the effect of the intensity locus: Main Effect of Intensity (MI) = ½ [(DB + DR) – (LB + LR)] (1) (DB=relative hybridization of dark-blue, DR=relative hybridization of dark-red, LB=relative hybridization of light-blue, LR=relative hybridization of light-red.) The second comparison estimates the effect of the hue locus: Main Effect of Hue (MH) = ½ [(DB + LB) – (DR + LR)] (2) And the final comparison estimates the effect of the interaction between loci. Interaction Effect (IE) = ½ [(DB + LR) – (DR + LB)] (3) For each of these comparisons we calculated the observed value from the field collected data. We then performed re-sampling with replacement from pooled data to determine the distribution under the null hypothesis that there is no effect of either locus nor an interaction. In order to do this we first created a “focal pool” with all the focal mother plants from all of the four blocks and a “reference pool” with all the reference mothers from all of the four blocks. From these pools we drew our re-sampled dataset. This involved 3 steps: 1. Sample random mothers (with replacement) for each focal color from the “focal pool”. The sample size was the same as the observed sample size for each focal genotype. 2. Sample random mothers (with replacement) for the standards from the “standard pool”. Again, the sample sizes for each standard set was the same as observed in each block. 3. For each mother in each group of focal and standard samples we re-sampled the offspring with replacement. Each sampled offspring maintained its identity as a hybrid or not. Re-sampled offspring sample sizes were the same as observed for each mother. Once we had a re-sampled dataset for each of the 4 focal and 4 reference genotypes, we calculated a sample relative hybridization rate for each focal color. Sample comparisons (equations 1-3 above) were calculated with the sample relative hybridization rates. We re-sampled the pooled dataset 10,000 times. Each time we calculated the above three comparisons to create the null distribution for each comparison. Finally we determined where the observed value for each comparison fell in the null distribution and calculated a P-value as the proportion of bootstrap values that were greater than or equal to the observed value (table S5b). Because we are testing the null hypothesis against the a priori alternative hypothesis that the derived allele has a lower hybridization rate, the test is one-tailed. Selection. We calculated the magnitude of selection on allelic variation at the intensity locus from the hybridization rates in the dark-red and the dark-blue focal color plots. Previous work has shown that hybrids are approximately 90% sterile (16, 17), therefore fitness was estimated as (1-0.9hkk), where hkk equals the hybridization rate for genotype kk. Average hybridization of light-blue reference plants across both dark intensity plots equaled 0.43, and average hybridization of focal plants in both dark plots equaled 0.21. The selection coefficient was calculated as (1-0.9 hII)/(1-0.9 hii) -1 = (1 – 0.9x0.21)/(1 – 0.9 x 0.43) - 1 = 0.32. Pollinator Array Experiments Array Design The pollinator observation arrays consisted of three plant types each: P. cuspidata individuals were alternated with P. drummondii individuals and the P. drummondii individuals, alternated light-blue flower color genotype with one focal color (a derived flower color genotype). Each array had 20 P. drummondii individuals (10 of each of two flower color genotypes), and 35 P. cuspidata plants (figure S.2). In total we had three arrays, one included light-red individuals (iihh) to quantify the effect of the hue locus on pollinator movement, one included dark-blue individuals (IIHH) to quantify the effect of the intensity locus on pollinator movement, and the third included dark-red individuals (IIhh) to determine if there is an interaction between loci effecting pollinator movement between Phlox species. Each day the total number of open flowers for each P. drummondii genotype were manipulated to be equal within each array. There were approximately the same number of P. cuspidata flowers as P. drummondii flowers (Mean ratio of cuspidata/drummondii=0.988, SD=0.285). We observed movement of 181 pollinators making a total of 2301 transitions between plants, and used these data to calculate the transition rate between species for each flower color genotype. Observations were performed over 10 days during May 2011, between 10am and 3pm on sunny or mostly-sunny days. This time of year corresponded to when natural Phlox populations experienced high concentrations of blooms. Although butterflies were seen foraging earlier then 10am and later then 3pm, most activity tended to be concentrated in the middle of the day. Analysis of Pollinator Movements The number of transitions between different genotypes in the pollinator arrays are given in table S6a. Two comparisons were performed: (1) proportion of visits to lightblue vs. focal color (light-red, dark-red, or dark-blue) following a visit to P. cuspidata; and (2) proportion of visits from light-blue vs. light-red, dark-red, or dark-blue to P cuspidata. (1) Proportion of visits to light-blue vs. focal plants following a visit to P. cuspidata. We first tested for heterogeneity between Battus philenor and skippers in the proportion of visits to light-blue and to focal plants using a G-test (table S6b). There was no significant heterogeneity for either the light-red or dark-blue arrays, so for these arrays data for Battus and skippers were combined for analysis. There was significant heterogeneity for the dark-red array, likely due to the small number of skippers observed visiting dark-red from P. cuspidata; therefore, Battus and skippers were analyzed separately. To test whether the proportion of visits to light-blue vs. focal color differed following a visit to P. cuspidata we used a G-test to ask whether the numbers of visits from P. cuspidata differed from equality using maximum likelihood. We compared the expected number of visits to focal color and light-blue from P cuspidata to the predicted number of visits given equal visitation to both colors. In the light‐red arrays, pollinators were equally likely to visit light‐blue and light‐red after visiting a P. cuspidata plant (χ21 = 0.207, n.s.). In the dark‐blue arrays, pollinators were significantly less likely to visit a dark‐blue than a light‐blue after visiting a P. cuspidata plant (χ21 = 46.22, P<0.001). In the dark‐red arrays, both Battus and skippers were significantly less likely to visit a dark‐red than a light‐blue after visiting a P. cuspidata plant (χ21 = 30.3, P<0.001 and χ21 = 29.84, P<0.001, respectively). (2) Proportion of visits from light-blue vs. focal plant to P cuspidata. We first tested for heterogeneity between Battus philenor and skippers using a 3way G-test (table S.6c). In this analysis, the three factors were (i) pollinator (Battus vs. skippers), (ii) initial visit to light-blue vs. focal color (light-red, dark-red, or dark-blue), and (iii) Number of visits to P. drummondii (light-blue plus focal color) vs. to P. cuspidata. Significant heterogeneity is indicated by the 3-way interaction term. There was no significant heterogeneity for any of the three arrays, so data for Battus and skippers were combined for analysis. To test whether the proportion of visits from light-blue vs. focal color to P cuspidata differed, we used G-tests on the combined pollinator data. Visits to light-blue and focal color were pooled because they represent transitions within P. drummondii and we are interested in comparing the proportions of intra-specific visits to interspecific visits. The two factors in this test were thus (i) number of visits to light-blue and focal color vs. number of visits to P. cuspidata, and (ii) initial visit to light-blue vs. focal color. In the light-red arrays, pollinators were equally likely to move from light-blue vs. light-red to P. cuspidata (χ21 = 0.7, n.s.). By contrast, in the dark‐blue and dark‐red arrays, pollinators were significantly less likely to move from dark‐blue or dark‐red to P. cuspidata than to move from light‐blue (χ21 = 48, P<0.001 and χ21 = 17.4, P<0.001, respectively). Bateman’s Constancy Index. In order to quantify constancy within each plot we calculated Bateman’s Constancy Index (BCI). This index varies from ‐1, indicating movement only between unlike plants, to 1, indicating movement only between like plants, with zero indicating random movement of pollinators. Within each array type we calculated two BCI’s one for the focal color and P. cuspidata and one for the light‐blue plants and P. cuspidata. Transition tables were constructed as illustrated below and the BCI was calculated using the standard formula: FROM TO P. drummondii P. cuspidata P. drummondii A B P. cuspidata C D BCI = [(AD)½ – (BC)½]/[(AD)½ + (BC)½] Fig. S1. Crossing design for field experiments. Field collected parents with light-blue and darkred genotypes were crossed to create F1’s which were self fertilized to create F2 families. Experimental seeds were created by crossing F2 individuals with the same homozygous genotype between families. Fig. S2. Schematic of the dark-red pollinator observation array. The light-red and dark-blue arrays were a similar design. Supporting Tables Table S1. Seed source populations, locations, and flower color Population Species Color Dog95 P. drummondii light-blue 466_E7 P. drummondii light-blue Poc2104 P. drummondii dark-red 80S1 P. drummondii dark-red 696N1 P. drummondii light-blue 2104circ P. cuspidata light-blue Location W -97.3423 N 30.2926 W -97.7291 N 29.5049 W -97.0871 N 30.0813 W -97.6613 N 29.6403 W -97.2902 N 30.3236 W -97.0878 N 30.0707 Table S2. Maximum likelihood analysis of variance for survival differences among flower color genotypes. Source DF Chi Square intercept intensity hue source block intensity*hue intensity*source intensity*block hue*source hue*block intensity*hue*source intensity*hue*block intensity*source*block hue*source*block intensity*hue*source*block 1 1 1 1 3 1 1 3 1 3 1 3 3 3 3 493.17 0 0 304.1 518.26 0.01 0.01 9.95 0 1.74 0 2.06 8.1 2.16 1.24 (Wald score) P <.0001 0.9657 0.9509 <.0001 <.0001 0.9396 0.9404 0.019 0.9646 0.6286 0.9585 0.56 0.0441 0.5389 0.7434 Table S3. Mixed model analysis of variance for fruit-set. a. No significant fixed effects of intensity, hue or their interaction. b. Covariance of random effects reveal a significant block effect. a. Fixed Effect Variance Parameter DF F P Hue Intensity Hue X Intensity 1 1 1 0.08 0.03 0.08 0.8194 0.8944 0.8294 Random Effect Covariance Parameter Estimate Chi Square DF Source Block Source X Block Hue X Source Hue X Block Intensity X Source Intensity X Block Hue X Intensity X Source Hue X Intensity X Block Residual 0 0.7439 0 0 0 0.006614 0.002892 0.02256 0.004686 1.2721 0 9.8 0 0 0 0 0 2.2 0.2 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 full model log likelihood 3852.3 b. P 1 0.0017* 1 1 1 1 1 0.138 0.6547 Table S4. Mixed model analysis of variance for number of seeds-per-fruit. a. No significant fixed effects of intensity, hue, or their interaction. b. No significant random effects. a. Fixed Effect Variance Parameter Hue Intensity Hue X Intensity DF F P 1 1 1 0.94 0.46 5.32 0.5106 0.6191 0.2605 Estimate Chi Square DF 0 0.00335 0 0 0.002264 0 0.007688 0 0 1.2721 0 0.1 0 0 0.1 0 0.7 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 b. Random Effect Covariance Parameter Source Block Source X Block Hue X Source Hue X Block Intensity X Source Intensity X Block Hue X Intensity X Source Hue X Intensity X Block Residual full model log likelihood 59.5 P 1 0.751 1 1 0.751 1 0.402 1 1 Table S5. Hybridization data for each genotype (focal and reference). (a) Bootstrap standard errors indicated in parentheses. The two light genotypes show a relative hybridization rate not significantly different from 1 indicating these genotypes behaved similar to the light-blue reference plants. The two dark genotypes had half the hybridization rate of the reference light-blue plants. (LB=light-blue, LR=light-red, DB=dark-blue, DR=dark-red). (b) Results from hypotheses tests indicating an effect of the intensity locus but no effect of the hue locus or a genetic interaction. We re-sampling with replacement from pooled data in order to calculated a null distribution against which we tested the observed data . a. Block color Hybrids LB 28 LR 48 DB 21 DR 21 Focal NonHybridization hybrids rate 0.301 65 (0.069) 0.421 66 (0.104) 0.207 80 (0.061) 0.212 78 (0.063) b. Effect P Intensity 0.0477 Hue 0.5118 Interaction 0.4927 Hybrids 46 31 50 69 LB reference NonHybridization hybrids rate 0.275 121 (0.072) 0.373 52 (0.121) 0.427 67 (0.106) 0.439 88 (0.083) Relative hybridization rate 1.09 (0.472) 1.13 (0.824) 0.486 (0.220) 0.482 (0.184) Table S6. Analysis of pollinator transitions. a. Number of pollinator transitions in the three array types. b. G-test for heterogenetity between pollinator types in proportions of transitions both to and from P. cuspidata. c. Bateman’s Constancy Index within each of the three pollinator arrays. BCI was calculated for transitions within and between P. cuspidata and each P. drummondii focal color and light-blue in each array. Array is indicated by column title. a. Pollinator Array Focal Color From Light-Blue To Battus philenor Skippers Total LR DB DR LR DB DR LR DB DR lightblue 43 29 73 6 7 9 49 36 82 focal color 90 57 120 18 25 4 108 82 124 Cusp 30 16 90 32 64 39 62 80 129 From focal color lightblue 92 63 122 20 21 5 112 84 127 b To P. cuspidata From P. cuspidata Array Type LR DB DR LR DB DR G-value 0.01 1.61 3.9 0.02 0 0.01 DF 1 1 1 1 1 1 c. focal color light-blue LR 0.162 0.0829 DB -0.035 0.752 DR -0.024 0.622 P ns ns 0.05 ns ns ns focal color 75 101 95 5 12 0 80 113 95 Cusp 28 7 29 32 18 4 60 25 33 From P. cuspidata lightblue 27 15 87 31 73 39 58 88 126 focal color 30 6 29 33 14 5 63 20 34 Cusp 16 7 52 48 159 166 64 166 218 References and Notes 1. R. Butlin, Trends Ecol. Evol. 2, 8 (1987). 2. T. Dobzhansky, Genetics and the Origin of Species (Columbia Univ. Press, New York, 1937). 3. D. J. Howard, in Hybrid Zones and the Evolutionary Process, R. G. Harrison, Ed. (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1993), pp. 46-69. 4. M. R. Servedio, M. A. F. Noor, Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 34, 339 (2003). 5. A. R. Wallace, Darwinism: An exposition of the theory of natural selection, with some of its applications (Macmillan, London, 1889). 6. M. Kirkpatrick, V. Ravigne, Am. Nat. 159, S22 (2002). 7. D. Ortiz-Barrientos, A. Grealy, P. Nosil, Year in Evolutionary Biology 2009 1168, 156 (2009). 8. M. Kirkpatrick, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 267, 1649 (2000). 9. M. Kirkpatrick, M. R. Servedio, Genetics 151, 865 (1999). 10. M. R. Servedio, Evolution 55, 1909 (2001). 11. M. R. Servedio, Evolution 58, 913 (2004). 12. A. Y. K. Albert, D. Schluter, Evolution 58, 1099 (2004). 13. P. Nosil, B. J. Crespi, C. P. Sandoval, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 270, 1911 (2003). 14. D. A. Levin, Evolution 39, 1275 (1985). 15. R. Hopkins, D. A. Levin, M. D. Rausher, Evolution 10.1111/j.15585646.2011.01452.x (2011). 16. D. A. Levin, Am. J. Bot. 54, 1122 (1967). 17. L. G. Ruane, K. Donohue, Evol. Ecol. 22, 229 (2008). 18. R. Hopkins, M. D. Rausher, Nature 469, 411 (2011). 19. Material and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online. 20. N. M. Waser, Am. Nat. 127, 593 (1986). 21. M. D. Rausher, Science 200, 1071 (1978). 22. H. D. Bradshaw, D. W. Schemske, Nature 426, 176 (2003). 23. D. R. Campbell, N. M. Waser, E. J. MelendezAckerman, Am. Nat. 149, 295 (1997). 24. M. E. Hoballah et al., Plant Cell 19, 779 (2007). 25. L. Chittka, J. D. Thomson, N. M. Waser, Naturwissenschaften 86, 361 (1999). 26. M. Caisse, J. Antonovics, Heredity 40, 371 (1978). 27. J. Felsenstein, Evolution 35, 124 (1981). 28. L. W. Liou, T. D. Price, Evolution 48, 1451 (1994). 29. M. R. Servedio, Evolution 54, 21 (2000).