Uploaded by Son Yisul

Exercises Week-1-sketch-answers (1)

advertisement
Sketch Answers to Exercises – Week 1
Note: these are only sketches of answers, not full-blown answers. These sketches serve to give you
the information that you need to write full answers.
Interpretation, Discussion, and Implementation
1. Think of an example for each of the following threats to the identification of causal effects
and explain it using your example. Find an example that was not mentioned in the lecture:
Note: try to come up with your own example – this is a good exercise to search for potential
sources of biases.
a. Reverse causality: Europeans in the Middle Ages believed lice to improve health.
Why? They observed that sick people do not have lice, whereas healthy people do.
However, causality is reversed: lice are sensitive to body temperature and leave sick
hosts.
b. Simultaneity: Excessive alcohol consumption raises the probability of becoming
depressive, but being depressive raises the probability of excessive alcohol
consumption. Hence, both cause each other. The correlation between alcohol
consumption and depression is thus reflecting both channels (and probably also other
channels, such as omitted variable bias and selection bias).
c. Omitted variable bias: Students who attend the tutorials have higher grades. So do
tutorials help in achieving higher grades? Probably yes, but also students who are
more motivated more often attend tutorials, and those who are more motivated also
put more effort in preparing for the exam, so they get better grades also because of
higher motivation. Students’ motivation thus is an omitted variable that, if not
controlled for, leads to omitted variable bias.
d. Selection bias: People who went to university have higher wages that those who
didn’t. Do they earn higher wages because of attending university? Probably yes, but
also there is selection into universities: Only those with sufficient skills make it to
university, i.e. these are a selected group. They would probably also earn higher
wages if they had not gone to university, because of their higher skills. So their higher
wages are only partly due to studying at a university. They are not comparable to
those who did not study, and that leads to selection bias. Note: Selection Bias and
Omitted Variable Bias are closely related concepts.
2. Discuss the following examples
a. You work in the marketing department a company, which has seen its market share
decline and responded by introducing a new marketing campaign. Your supervisor
wants to know whether the campaign was successful and asks you for the market
share before and after the campaign. You do not see a big difference in the market
share in the year before and the year after the introduction of the marketing
campaign. What do you conclude and why? Key question: what would have
happened in the absence of the introduction of the marketing campaign? If the firm
was on the decline, then the decline probably would have continued in the absence of
the campaign, so the fact that there is no difference in the market share before and
after the introduction of the marketing campaign could indicate that it helped to stop
the decline. But: we do not know, since we have no good indication of what would
have happened without the campaign.
b. You work for a hotel company. Your manager wants to reconsider the pricing
strategy and asks you to check the effect of prices on the occupancy rate of the
hotel. You have data on prices and occupancy for each day of the past 10 years. You
see a positive correlation between your hotels’ daily prices and occupancy rate, i.e.
days with high prices are also days with high occupancy. What do you conclude?
Omitted variable bias: the hotel sets prices high when demand is high, that is
probably why there is a positive correlation between prices and occupancy! It’s
unlikely that high prices lead to high occupancy, but it is more likely that high
demand leads to both, high occupancy and high prices. From the pure correlation, we
cannot determine the effect of prices on occupancy.
c. You work in an employment agency and want to know whether further training
measures for unemployed workers help them in finding jobs. Unemployed workers
can apply for the measures and the case workers decide who gets the training.
Discuss whether comparing the job finding rates of unemployed workers who
participated in a training to those who didn’t identifies the effect of the training.
Selection biases: the case workers decide who get’s the training. Depending on their
incentives, they might either pic the “good” or the “bad” ones, so that these are not
comparable to those who don’t get a training. Also, the unemployed decide
themselves whether to participate. Probably those participate who are more
motivated, and they are likely to have higher changes to find jobs, anyhow.
d. You want to know whether robots destroy jobs. You have data on the number of jobs
and the number of robots in the manufacturing industry for the Netherlands for the
past 20 years. You see that employment grew strong in industries that adopted
robots. Discuss whether this proves that robots do not destroy jobs. Omitted variable
bias: Firms are more likely to adopt robots if they grow. If demand for the output of
an industry rises, then the firms do both, employ more workers and invest in robots.
This tells us little about the effect of robots on the number of jobs, because both are
affected by an omitted variable. To know how the adoption of robots affected the
number of jobs, we need to control for the rise in demand.
3. In WW2, the Navy wanted to know where to improve armor on their planes to make sure
that they make it back home. They ran an analysis of where the planes got shot – illustrated
by the picture, below. What do you think, where should the Navy increase armor? Explain!
Selection bias: the data is only available for planes that returned. Since these planes survived,
the damage at the indicated places probably was not fatal. However, without additional
information, or additional assumptions, we cannot determine where planes are most
vulnerable. If we assume that planes are randomly hit at all places, then the fact that those
planes which returned do not show any hits at specific places (engines, cockpit) suggests that
these are the most vulnerable places. However, this conclusion is based on an assumption.
Repetition
4. What is a “counterfactual situation”? How does a counterfactual situation help in identifying
causal effects? And how does this relate to potential outcomes? The counterfactual situation
describes what would have happened to the treated unit had it not been treated. By
comparing the actual situation to the counterfactual situation, we identify the causal effect.
There are two potential outcomes for a unit, Y_0i describes the outcome that unit i would get
if it was not treated, Y_1i describes the outcome that unit i would get if it was treated. If unit i
receives the treatment, then Y_1i realizes. Y_1i then is the actual situation and Y_0i is the
counterfactual situation (and vice versa if unit i does not receive the treatment).
5. “The problem of identifying causal effects is basically a problem of missing data.” – Explain
this statement. What data is missing? Explain whether it is possible to solve the problem by
collecting the missing data. (see also answer to last exercise) We only observe one of the
potential outcomes, but never both. We observe the actual situation, but never the
counterfactual situation. The counterfactual situation is the missing data. It is impossible to
observe the counterfactual situation, because a unit is either treated or not, but never both.
We therefore can identify causal effects only by developing a credible counterfactual
situation to which we can compare the actual situation.
6. Why is it impossible to identify the causal effect of a treatment for an individual unit of
observation (e.g. for a specific person)? What can you do instead? A unit (e.g. a person) is
either treated, or not, but never both. We therefore cannot estimate the treatment effect for
an individual unit. What we can do is to is to try to find a suitable group of untreated units
that serves as a good comparison group for treated units. The average differences between
these treated and comparable untreated units then is a good estimate of the average causal
effect for those units (individuals). This is the average causal effect. However, it is based on
the assumption that the control group is in fact comparable.
7. Explain the following equation:
𝐴𝑣𝑔𝑛 [𝑌1𝑖 |𝐷𝑖 = 1] − 𝐴𝑣𝑔𝑛 [𝑌0𝑖 |𝐷𝑖 = 0] = 𝛥 + 𝐴𝑣𝑔𝑛 [𝑌0𝑖 |𝐷𝑖 = 1] − 𝐴𝑣𝑔𝑛 [𝑌0𝑖 |𝐷𝑖 = 0]
a. In particular, explain the meaning of each element of the equation separately (hint:
you have to distinguish between potential outcomes and observed outcomes)
𝑌1𝑖 – potential outcome of unit i if it would be treated
𝑌0𝑖 – potential outcome of unit i if would not be treated
𝐷𝑖 = 1 – unit i is treated
𝐷𝑖 = 0 – unit i is not treated
𝐴𝑣𝑔𝑛 [𝑌1𝑖 |𝐷𝑖 = 1] – Average of the potential outcome if treated for those individuals
which are also treated in reality (i.e. average observed outcome of treated units)
𝐴𝑣𝑔𝑛 [𝑌0𝑖 |𝐷𝑖 = 0] – Average of the potential outcome if untreated for those
individuals which are also not treated in reality (i.e. average observed outcome of
untreated units)
𝛥 – average causal effect
𝐴𝑣𝑔𝑛 [𝑌0𝑖 |𝐷𝑖 = 1] – Average of the potential outcome if untreated for those
individuals which in reality did receive the treatment (counterfactual situation)
b. And explain the overall meaning of the equation in general.
The equation states that the average differences in observed outcomes between
treated and untreated units reflects the sum of the average causal effect as well as
the selection bias (i.e. the differences in outcomes between treated and untreated
units that would exist even without any treatment, i.e. genuine differences between
treated and untreated units that are not caused by the treatment).
8. What is the Stable Unit Treatment Value Assumption (SUTVA)? What does the SUTVA imply
for the interpretation of causal effects?
The Stable Unit Treatment Value Assumption (SUTVA) is the assumption that the treatment
effect does not depend on who else receives the treatment. The important implication is that
causal effects that were estimated under this assumption (which applies to most estimates of
causal effects) only have a marginal or local interpretation. The background is that in reality
scaling up of a treatment often does cause feedback effects. Think for example of a
marketing campaign: if only a few firms implement it, then these firm likely profit a lot. But if
everybody does it, then the effects for each individual firm likely become weaker because
firms marketing campaigns might cannibalize each other. The treatment effect of the
campaign thus are only valid “locally” or “marginally” for few firms, but not if many firms
adopt it.
9. Explain the difference between applied data analysis I & visualization vs. applied
microeconometric methods using the regression model:
Population Model:
𝑌 = 𝛽0 + 𝛽1 𝑋 + 𝜀
Estimated Model in Sample:
̂0 + 𝛽
̂1 𝑋 + 𝑒 ,
Y=𝛽
̂0 + 𝛽
̂1 𝑋, 𝑌 = 𝑌̂ + 𝑒
𝑌̂ = 𝛽
Make use of the difference (which?) between the population model and the estimated model
in the sample inf your explanation. What is the key challenge in applied microeconometrics?
Applied data analysis I & visualization focuses on prediction, i.e. on the 𝑌̂. That is, the aim is
to get a good prediction of the outcome variable. In applied microeconometrics, we instead
̂1 . In Applied data analysis I & visualization, we
try to estimate the effect of X on Y, which is 𝛽
can see how well we do by comparing the estimated outcome 𝑌̂ to the true outcome Y. In
̂1 to the true effect 𝛽1 ,
applied microeconometrics, we cannot compare the estimated effect 𝛽
̂1 under the assumption that 𝜀 is uncorrelated with
because 𝛽1 is unobservable. We estimate 𝛽
𝑋, but that assumption cannot be tested, because we do not observe the error term 𝜀. We
only observe the realized residuals 𝑒. The realized residuals 𝑒 are always uncorrelated with 𝑋,
but that is what we have assumed in the first place. But we never know whether that also
holds true for the error term 𝜀. Hence, the main problem in applied microeconometrics is that
we never can test whether we truly estimate the causal effect. All we can do is to choose our
models such that it is as plausible as possible that 𝜀 is uncorrelated with 𝑋.
Download