PATRICIA FUENTE GARCÍA ENGLISH IV: MORPHOLOGY INTRODUCTION SYNTAX: a: the way in which linguistic elements (as words) are put together to form constituents (as phrases or clauses) b: the part of grammar dealing with this MORPHOLOGY: a study and description of word formation (as inflection, derivation, and compounding) in language. WORDS: A single distinct meaningful element of speech or writing, used with others (or sometimes alone) to form a sentence and typically shown with a space on either side when written or printed WORD FORMATION: Words: units of meaning vs. units of sentence structure, pronounceable vs. abstract entities Word classes: lexical vs. functional categories, verbal morphology, inflectional classes Building words: morphemes, morphological processes, compounds, allomorphy 1. MORPHEMES & ALLOMORPHY MORPHEME VS MORPH Morpheme – Minimal unit of grammatical analysis, and also the smallest linguistic element capable of having a meaning. They have no internal structure other than phonological. Strictly speaking, however, morphemes are abstract elements of analysis (like lexemes and phonemes) and do not occur. What occurs is a phonetic (or orthographic) form which realizes the morpheme. When the phonetic (or orthographic) strings which realize morphemes are segmentable, these are termed ‘morphs’. A morph, therefore, can be defined as a segment of a word-form which represents a particular morpheme. ALLOMORPHY Besides morpheme and morph, a third term, ‘allomorph’ is required for morphological analysis. An allomorph is one of the variant pronunciations of a morpheme, among which the choice is determined by context (phonological, grammatical or lexical). For example, [z], [iz] and [s] are phonologically determined allomorphs of -s. The plural suffix, occurring respectively in cats, dogs and horses. A morpheme with only one pronunciation is sometimes said to have only one allomorph. The three different realizations of the plural morpheme -s are called allomorphs. In cases allomorphs are predicted by the phonological environment (this is relevant for the relation between morphology and phonology). Something very similar can be said for the past tense morpheme -ed: [id], [d], [t]. But not only phonology determines allomorphy: lexicon and grammar do as well. a. laugh, cliff — laughs, cliffs [s] b. wife, loaf — *wifes, *loafs — c. * [s] wives, loaves [z] Lastly, it must be pointed out that correlating morphemes with meaning is not always accurate. OTHER ALLOMORPHS: The shape of articles in English a. the indefinite article a b. the definite article the [ə] question [ən] answer [ðə] question [ði] answer [ə] book [ən] author [ðə] book [ði] author [ə] fence [ən] idea [ðə] fence [ði] idea in isolation: [ei] in isolation: [ði:] Roots: explain [ɪksˈpleɪn], explanation [ˌekspləˈneɪʃən], explanatory [ɪkˈsplænəˌtərɪ]. BOUND VS FREE A morph which can occur in isolation, i.e. can occur as an independent word, is called free morpheme. On the contrary, a morph which can only occur in a word-form in conjunction with at least one other morph is called a bound morpheme. While all affixes are bound morphemes, not all roots are free morphemes; some must be bound as well. There are a group of bound morphemes that cannot be assigned an independent meaning or a grammatical function, but nonetheless serve to distinguish one word from another. These are the so called cranberry morphemes and they occur in only one word. For example, in the word cranberry, ‘cran’ doesn’t have a meaning by itself but serves to distinguish that word from strawberry. More examples of cranberry morphemes: huckle-, gorm-, -mit (in remit, commit, transmit), -efer (refer, prefer, confer…), -duce (reduce, conduce, deduce), -voke (revoke, convoke, invoke), -serve (reserve, preserve, conserve, deserve). Bound morphemes which do not realize unanalyzable lexemes are affixes. Affixes can be divided intro prefixes, suffixes and infixes. In English prefixation is always derivational while suffixation may be either derivational of inflectional. For example, in the form ‘un-touch-able-s’, -able is a derivational suffix, un- is a derivational prefix, and -s is an inflectional suffix. 2. STEMS, ROOTS AND AFFIX FREEDOM ‘Root’ and ‘stem’ are terms used in literature to designate that part of the word that remains when al affixes have been removed. The root word is the primary lexical unit of a word, and of a word family (root is then called base word), which carries the most significant aspects of semantic content and cannot be reduced into smaller constituents. 3) untouchables’ a. un-touch-able-s 4) a. ‘disagreement’: dis+agree+ment b. dis- agree dis-agree-ment c. agree -ment dis-agree-ment (5) a. leg -ible, audi-ence, magn -ify (associated with Romance roots ) b. cran -berry, huckle -berry, gorm -less (cranberry morphemes ) AFFIXATION Prefixes precede a base, suffixes follow it. We may also talk about infixes, which break up a base (possibly English intensifiers like –friggin’– as in unbefrigginlievable), and circumfixes, which surround it (en–BASE–en for enlighten?). These kids are frigging awesome Inflectional affixes don’t change a base’s category but simply specify it for the inflectional property (person, number, gender, or case as well as tense, for example). Derivational affixes may (but need not) change the category and create a new word. a. de+, re+, sub+, in+, con+, pre+, en+, be+… prefixes b. +ion, +ity, +y, +al, +ic, +ate, +ous, +ive… suffixes 3. MONOMORPHEMIC, COMPLEX AND POLYMORPHEMIC WORDS So far, we have only encountered complex words that are created by concatenation, i.e. by linking together bases and affixes as in a chain. There are, however, also other, i.e. non-concatenative, ways to form morphologically complex words. CONVERSION Conversion is a kind of word transformation in which a new word is created (belonging to a new word class) from an existing word (belonging to a different word class) without any change in form: To walk – a walk; to throw- a throw; to book- a book; to father- father The verb water is derived from the noun water by adding to the base noun water a zero-morph with the meaning ‘apply X.’. The exact status of conversion within word formation is unclear. That is why it is frequently called zero-derivation. TRUNCATION OR CLIPPING (derivation process) It is a word formation process which consists in the reduction of a word to one of its parts but retaining both its original meaning and its word class. It is different from back-formation; back formation may change the part of speech or the word’s meaning, whereas clipping creates shortened words from longer words, but does not change the part of speech of the meaning of the word. Examples: Ronald > Ron; laboratory > lab; mathematics > math; examination > exam. There are 3 types of clipping: 1. BACK CLIPPING: ad (advertisement), doc (doctor), exam (examination), gym (gymnasium) 2. FORE CLIPPING: chute (parachute), phone (telephone), varsity (university) 3. MIDDLE CLIPPING: flu (influenza), fridge (refrigerator) BLENDS (derivation process) We also find so-called blends, which are amalgamations of parts of different words, such as smog (← smoke/fog) or modem (← modulator/demodulator). In other words, blends are new lexemes formed from parts of two other words in such a way that there is no transparent analysis into morphs. Blends based on orthography are called acronyms, which are coined by combining the initial letters of compounds or phrases into a pronounceable new word (NATO, UNESCO, RAM, ANZAC, AIDS etc.). The use of capital letters in the spelling of some of these words reflects the fact that speakers are aware of some of their acronyms status. If the conventional way of reading the string is by pronouncing the name of each letter in turn, as with USA, UK or RP, then it is not an acronym but an abbreviation. ANALOGY Sometimes new complex words are derived without an existing word-formation rule but formed on the basis of a single (or very few) model words. For example, earwitness ‘someone who has heard a crime being commited’ was coined on the basis of eyewitness, cheeseburger on the basis of hamburger, and air-sick on the basis of sea-sick. The process by which these words came into being is called analogy. BACK FORMATION Back formation is the process of creating a new lexeme, usually by removing actual or supposed affixes. - Verb “edit” from “editor” - Verb “donate” from “donation” Derivation: affixation, non-affixation, prefixation, suffixation, infixation, conversion, truncation, blending. 4. INFLECTION Inflection morphology deals with the inflected forms of words, that is the kind of variation that words exhibit on the basis of their grammatical context. Lexeme – word seen as an abstract grammatical entity, represented concretely by one or more different inflected word forms according to the grammatical context. Where the distinction is important, lexemes are conventionally represented in small capitals while word forms are in italics. For example, the verb lexeme PERFORM has four inflected word forms: perform, performs, performing and performed. Word form- word viewed as a pronounceable entity, representing concretely a lexeme in some grammatical context. One-word form may be shared by more than one lexeme; for example, [rouz] is shared by the noun ‘line of objects’ (as its plural form), the noun (as its basic, or singular, form), the verb ‘propel with oars’ (as its third person singular present tense form), and the verb (as its past tense form). One lexeme may be represented by two (or more) quite distinct root morphemes (not allomorphs). The term given to this phenomenon is suppletion; go and went are said to be distinct roots (and hence distinct morphemes), standing in a suppletive relationship as representatives, in different grammatical contexts, of one lexeme. INFLECTION OF VERBS In English, a verb lexeme has at most five distinct forms: 1. Third person singular present tense: gives e.g. Mary gives a lecture every year. 2. Past tense: gave e.g. Mary gave a lecture last week. 3. Progressive participle: giving e.g. Mary is giving a lecture today. 4. Perfect or passive participle: given e.g. Mary has given a lecture today. The lecture is always given by Mary. 5. basic form (used everywhere else): give e.g. Mary may give a lecture. The contrast between present at (1) and past at (2) is a contrast of tense. The other dimensions of contrast manifested in (1) are person (third person versus the rest) and number (singular versus plural, just as for nouns and pronouns). However, because only one-word form (gives) exhibits these contrasts, they play a much smaller inflectional role in modern English verbs than in Old English verbs. Other verbs or verb-like words whose behaviour belongs to grammar rather than word-formation are the auxiliaries, such as BE and HAVE, and modals, such as CAN, MUST, MAY. Their various forms distinguish an unusually small or large range of grammatical words. Instead of the usual verbal maximum of five forms, modals distinguish only two (e.g. can, could) or even just one (e.g. Must), while BE distinguishes eight (am, is, are, was, were, being, been, be). INFLECTION OF NOUNS Most countable nouns in English have two-word forms: a singular and a plural. Inflectionally, for any noun lexeme X, there are just two grammatical words, ‘singular of X’ and ‘plural of X’, contrasting in number. -s is the regular suffix for forming plurals. Irregular suffixes expressing plurality include -i, -ae and -a (as in cacti, formulae, phenomena) found with some relatively learned words borrowed from Latin or Greek; the suffix -(r)en that shows up only in oxen, children and brethren; and a very few others such as the Hebrew -im in cherubim and kibbutzim. There are also some countable nouns that express their plural with no suffix at all (teeth, men) where there is a change in the vowel of the root – or, more precisely, an allomorph of the root with a different vowel from the singular. There are also some whose plurals display not even a vowel change: for example, sheep, Fish (sometimes called ‘zero-plural’ nouns, if they are analysed as carrying a ‘zero suffix’). they all denote animals, birds or fish that are either domesticated (SHEEP) or hunted (DEER), usually for food (TROUT, COD). There are a few nouns such as SCISSORS and PANTS which exist only in an -splural form, and which appear only in plural syntactic contexts, even though they denote single countable Entities: a. Those scissors belong in the top drawer. b. Your pants have a hole in the seat. For the singular form, there is a conventional circumlocution or periphrastic form: pair of pants and pair of scissors (as in That pair of scissors belongs in the top drawer). 'Apostrophe-s' form: pianist’s, man’s, child’s, children’s etc. – do not count as further inflected forms of the lexemes PIANIST, MAN, CHILD as ‘possessive’ forms. What -’s attaches itself to is not a morphological unit such as noun root (e.g. man) but a syntactic unit, namely a noun phrase. So -’s belongs in the study of syntax, not morphology. PRONOUNS AND DETERMINERS There is a distinction between this and these, as in this pianist and these pianists. These are the singular and plural forms of the determiner lexeme THIS. Other determiners include THE but only one other determiner exhibits a singular–plural contrast: THAT, with singular and plural forms that and those. The relationship between nominative and accusative forms in pronouns is consistently suppletive, as in I/me, she/her, we/us, and they/them, except that for YOU the two forms are identical (you). This is consistent with the fact that pronouns are very common, and suppletion affects only very common words such as GO. FORMS OF ADJECTIVES Many English adjectives exhibit three forms, for example GREEN here: - Grass is green. - The grass is greener now than in winter. - The grass is greenest in early summer. The grammatical words that green, greener and greenest express are the positive, comparative and superlative of GREEN, contrasting on the dimension of comparison. Better and best are suppletive of GOOD. Broadly speaking, the suffixes -er and -est appear on adjectives whose basic form has one syllable, or two provided that the second syllable ends in a vowel (e.g. tidy, yellow), while longer adjectives usually require a periphrasis (more, the most). CONCLUSION Some words (lexemes) have more than one-word form, depending on the grammatical context or on choices that grammar forces us to make (for example, in nouns, between singular and plural). This kind of word formation is called ‘inflectional’. Inflection affects nouns, verbs, adjectives and a few adverbs, as well as the closed classes of pronouns, determiners, auxiliaries and modals. However, the maximum number of distinct inflected forms for any open-class lexeme is small: Nouns: 2 e.g. cat, cats Verbs: 5 e.g. gives, gave, giving, given, give Adjectives: 3 e.g. green, greener, greenest Adverbs: 3 e.g. soon, sooner, soonest 5. DERIVATION In the previous section we discussed the words perform, performs, performed and performance. I argued that perform, performs and performed were grammatically conditioned variants of one lexemePERFORM, but performance was not one of these variants. The reason was that, whereas there are grammatical factors that determine the choice between perform, performs and performed (in appropriate contexts), there is no grammatical factor that requires specifically the presence of -ance on performance. To put it another way: there are contexts where, if any verb appears, it must carry the third person singular suffix -s, but there are no contexts where, if a noun appears, it must carry the suffix -ance. The suffix -ance is not one of the small class of suffixes (so-called ‘inflectional’ suffixes) whose use is tightly determined by grammar. What sort of suffix is it, then? A short answer is that, not being inflectional, it must be derivational, since the term ‘derivation’ is used for all aspects of wordstructure involving affixation that is not inflectional. Since performance is not a variant of the lexeme PERFORM, it must belong to some other lexeme, which may itself have more than one form. What lexeme could this be? This question is easy to answer when we notice that, alongside performance, there is a plural form performances. Just as cat and cats are the two forms (singular and plural) of the lexeme CAT, it makes sense to regard performance and performances as the two forms of a lexeme PERFORMANCE. This tells us something about the relationship between perform and performance: it is a relationship not between word forms but rather between lexemes. Thus, derivational morphology is concerned with one kind of relationship between lexemes. A. Derivation B. Inflection worker (she) works useless (the) workers untruthfulness (is) colonializing interview (we) picked curiosity (the) children passivize terrorism Derivation versus Inflection – is not restricted – suffixational (English) to suffixation – can occur inside derivation – occurs outside all derivation – encodes lexical meaning – encodes grammatical categories – often changes the part – does not change it of speech – is often restricted – highly / fully productive in its productivity – often semantically opaque a. ∗walk-ive – is rarely semantically opaque exhaust→exhaustive ∗read-ive operate→operative ∗surprise-ive assault→assaultive b. ∗computer-al colony→colonial ∗desk-al phrase→phrasal ∗child-al ADVERBS DERIVED FROM ADJECTIVES Most of the adverbs are created by the addition of suffix -ly: careful-> carefully Some others are created by conversion: FAST (as in The car was driven fast) and HARD (as in They worked hard), derived from the adjective FAST (as in a fast car) and HARD (as in hard work) There are simple of monomorphemic adverbs, though few in number, that include some very common words (OFTER, SELDOM, NEVER, SOON). Other adverbs are morphologically complex without containing -ly (NOWHERE, EVERYWHERE, TODAY, YESTERDAY) NOUNS DERIVED FROM MEMBERS OF OTHER WORD CLASSES NOUNS FROM ADJECTIVES (‘property of being X’): (A) -ness, e.g. goodness, tallness, fierceness, sensitiveness (B) -ity, e.g. purity, equality, ferocity, sensitivity (C) -ism, e.g. radicalism, conservatism NOUNS FROM VERBS (“activity or result of Xing”) (A) -ance, -ence, e.g. performance, ignorance, reference, convergence (B) -ment, e.g. announcement, commitment, development, engagement (C) -ing, e.g. painting, singing, building, ignoring (D) -((a)t)ion, e.g. denunciation, commission, organisation, confusion (E) -al, e.g. refusal, arrival, referral, committal (F) -er, e.g. painter, singer, organiser, grinder (1) Change in the position of the stress, e.g. nouns PÉRMIT, TRÁNSFER alongside verbs PERMÍT, TRANSFÉR (2) Change in the final consonant, e.g. nouns BELIEF, PROOF, DEFENCE alongside verbs BELIEVE, PROVE, DEFEND (3) Change in a vowel, e.g. nouns SONG, SEAT alongside verbs SING, SIT. NOUNS DERIVED FROM NOUNS (A) ‘inhabitant of X’: -er, -(i)an e.g. Londoner, New Yorker, Texan, Glaswegian (B) ‘devotee of or expert on X’: -ist, -ian e.g. contortionist, Marxist, logician, historian (C) ‘state of being an X’: -ship, -hood e.g. ladyship, motherhood, priesthood (D) ‘female X’: -ess, -ine e.g. waitress, princess, heroine (E) ‘small X’: -let, -ette, -ie e.g. droplet, booklet, cigarette, doggie ADJECTIVES DERIVED FROM ADJECTIVES In this category, prefixes predominate. The prefix un- meaning ‘not’ is extremely widespread: for example, UNHAPPY, UNSURE, UNRELIABLE, UNDISCOVERED. This does not mean, however, that un- can be prefixed to all adjectives quite freely; we do not find, for example, UNGOOD with the meaning ‘bad’. Another negative prefix is in-, with allomorphs indicated by the variant spellings il-, ir- and im-, as in INTANGIBLE, ILLEGAL, IRRESPONSIBLE and IMPOSSIBLE. It is more restricted than un-. eatable/uneatable edible/inedible readable/unreadable legible/illegible lawful/unlawful legal/illegal touchable/untouchable tangible/intangible The only suffix of note is -ish, meaning ‘somewhat X’, as in GREENISH, SMALLISH, REMOTISH ‘rather remote’. ADJECTIVES DERIVED FROM MEMBERS OF OTHER WORD CLASSES a. a not very interesting book b. The party-goers sounded very drunk. c. The car seemed more damaged than the lamp-post. FROM VERBS -able ‘able to be Xed’: breakable, readable, reliable, watchable -ent, -ant ‘tending to X’: repellent, expectant, conversant -ive ‘tending to X’: repulsive, explosive, speculative FROM NOUNS -al, e.g. original, normal, personal, national -ful, e.g. joyful, hopeful, helpful, meaningful -less, e.g. joyless, hopeless, helpless, meaningless -ish, e.g. boyish, loutish, waspish, selfish VERBS DERIVED FROM VERBS Most prominent are re- and the negative or ‘reversive’ prefixes un-, de- and dis-, as in the following examples: (A) paint, enter repaint, re-enter (B) tie, tangle untie, untangle (C) compose decompose sensitise desensitise (D) entangle disentangle believe disbelieve Also, worth mentioning here is the relationship between the verbs in the left and right columns in: Intransitive Transitive LIE (past lay) LAY (past laid) RISE (past rose) RAISE (past raised) FALL (past fell) FELL (past felled) VERBS DERIVED FROM MEMBERS OF OTHER WORD CLASSES FROM NOUNS NOUNS BATH VERBS BATHE BREATH BREATHE HOUSE […s] HOUSE […z] FROM NOUNS AND ADJECTIVES de-, e.g. debug, deforest, delouse -ise, e.g. organise, patronise, terrorise -(i)fy, e.g. beautify, petrify en- (with its allomorph em-) forms verbs meaning ‘cause to become X’ or ‘cause to possess or enter X’ from a few adjectives and nouns: ENFEEBLE, ENSLAVE, EMPOWER, ENRAGE, ENTHRONE, ENTOMB. With the adjectives BOLD and LIVE as bases, the prefix en- is combined with a suffix -en: EMBOLDEN, ENLIVEN. This suffix usually occurs without the prefix, however, and does so quite widely (e.g. TIGHTEN, LOOSEN, STIFFEN, WEAKEN, WIDEN, REDDEN, DEEPEN, TOUGHEN). These verbs have either an intransitive meaning, ‘become X’, or a transitive one, ‘cause to become X’. 6. COMPOUNDING A compound is a word that consists of two elements (binary structure), the first of which is either a root, a word or a phrase, the second of which is a root or a word. We can say that compounds usually exhibit what is called a modifier–head structure. Their head usually occurs on the right-hand side. Examples: Film society, Parks commissioner, Deep-fry, Knee-deep The compound as a whole inherits most of its semantic and syntactic information from its head. Thus, if the head is a verb, the compound will be a verb (e.g. deep-fry), if the head is a count noun, the compound will be a count noun (e.g. beer bottle), if the head has feminine gender, the compound will have feminine gender (e.g. head waitress). Another property of the compound head is that if the compound is pluralized the plural marking occurs on the head, not on the non-head: park commissioner – park commissioners. COMPOUND VERBS Verbs formed by compounding are much less usual than verbs derived by affixation. Nevertheless, a variety of types exist which may be distinguished according to their structure: (6) verb–verb (VV): stir-fry, freeze-dry (7) noun–verb (NV): hand-wash, air-condition, steam-clean (8) adjective–verb (AV): dry-clean, whitewash (9) preposition–verb (PV): underestimate, outrun, overcook Only the PV type is really common, however, and some compounds with under-, over- and out- do not need to be classed as lexical items. For example, out- can create a transitive verb meaning ‘outdo in Xing’ from any verb denoting a competitive or potentially competitive activity (e.g. outsail, outsing, outswim), while new words with over- can also be created freely (e.g. overpolish, overcriticise, overbleach). COMPOUND ADJECTIVES (10) noun–adjective (NA): sky-high, coal-black, oil-rich (11) adjective–adjective (AA): grey-green, squeaky-clean, red-hot (12) preposition–adjective (PA): underfull, overactive As with verbs, it is the type with the preposition over as its first element that seems most productive, in that new adjectives of this type, with the meaning ‘too X’, are readily acceptable: for example, overindignant, oversmooth. COMPOUND NOUNS It is with nouns that compounding really comes into its own as a word forming process in English. (13) verb–noun (VN): swearword, drophammer, playtime (14) noun–noun (NN): hairnet, mosquito net, butterfly net, hair restorer (15) adjective–noun (AN): blackboard, greenstone, faintheart (16) preposition–noun (PN): in-group, outpost, overcoat All of these have the main stress on the left – an important characteristic for distinguishing compound nouns from noun phrases. While noun phrases tend to be stressed phrase-finally, i.e. on the last word, noun compounds tend to be stressed on the first element. This systematic difference is captured in the so-called nuclear stress rule (‘phrasal stress is on the last word of the phrase’) and the so-called compound stress rule (‘stress is on the lefthand member of a Compound’). PRIMARY AND SECONDARY COMPOUNDS In morphology, a primary compound, or root compound, is a compound construction in which the head element (the right-hand one) is not derived from a verb and whose interpretation is therefore not precisely predictable on a purely linguistic basis. Examples of this kind of compounds are: hairnet, mosquito net, windmill, blackboard, toy factory. In morphology, a secondary compound, or a synthetic compound, is a type of compound in which the first element is interpreted as the object of the verb contained within the second. Examples of this kind are: hair restorer, slum clearance, beer drinker, car driver, bookseller, church-goer, pasta-eating, windowcleaning, shop clearance, soccer-playing. ENDOCENTRIC (HEADED) AND EXOCENTRIC (HEADLESS) COMPOUNDS Endocentric compounds are those regarded as having an internal nucleus inside the word itself, i.e. that some of the words integrating the compound convey the meaning of the word. Ex: blackbird, greenhouse, car seller. Exocentric compounds are those that are not determined by any element inside them, they do not have an internal nucleus inside but outside the word; i.e. the meaning does not depend on the words that integrate the compound. Ex: redneck (pelirrojo), pickpocket. Phrasal verbs are also considered to be exocentric: takeoff, sell-out, wrap-up, sit-in. Some adjectives consisting of a preposition and a noun are included in this category as well: overland, in-house, with-profits, offshore, downmarket, upscale, underweight, over-budget. COPULATIVE COMPOUNDS Copulative compounds are those in which no member is semantically prominent, but the two members equally contribute to the meaning of the compound. a. singer-songwriter b. the doctor-patient gap scientist-explorer a modifier-head structure poet-translator the mind-body problem hero-martyr Each form in (a) refers to one entity that is characterized by both members of the compound. A poet-translator, for example, is a person who is both a poet and a translator. This type of copulative compound is sometimes called an appositional compound. By contrast, the copulative compounds in (b) denote two entities that stand in a particular relationship with regard to the following noun. The particular type of relationship is determined by the following noun. The doctor-patient gap is thus a gap between doctor and patient. This second type of copulative compound is also known as a coordinative compound. COMBINING FORMS The vocabulary of English, especially in scientific and technical areas, includes a huge repertoire of compounds that are made up of bound roots, known as combining forms. Here are just a few: anthropology, sociology, plantigrade, retrospect, retrograde, cardiogram, electrocardiogram These elements are lexemes that are originally borrowed from Latin or Greek, but their combinations are of modern origin (hence the term neoclassical elements). PRHASAL WORDS There are also complex items that function as words, yet whose internal structure is that of a clause or phrase rather than of a compound. There is no standard term for these items; we can use the concept phrasal words. Jack-in-the-box, couldn’t-care-less (as in a couldn’t-care-less attitude). We know when we are dealing with phrasal words because they are pluralized by adding -s at the and and their structure is that of a phrase rather than a word. On the contrary, noun phrases such as brother-in-law or lady-in-waiting are pluralized by adding -s to the first word (brothers/ladies).