The objective value of subjective value: A multi-round negotiation study (Curhan et al., 2010) Does Subjective Value Matter? - Subjective value (Subjective Value (SV)): social, perceptual, and emotional consequences of a negotiation. o Has a valence in that it refers to the positivity or negativity of a social psychological outcome. - Subjective Value (Subjective Value (SV)) encompasses four factors: o Instrumental Subjective Value (SV): the subjective perception that the economic outcome is beneficial, balanced, and consistent with principles of legitimacy and precedent; o Self Subjective Value (SV): losing face versus feeling competent and satisfied that one has behaved appropriately; o Process Subjective Value (SV): the perception that one has been heard and treated justly, and that the process was efficient; o Relationship Subjective Value (SV): involves positive impressions, trust, and a solid foundation for working together in the future. - The third and fourth factors together form a broader construct of rapport. Relationship Marketing, which is fostering close relationships between buyers and sellers or channel partners, is economically advantageous. - Three Reasons Why Subjective Value (SV) can be Important to Negotiators: 1. Subjective Value (SV) can serve as a good in itself; 2. Subjective Value (SV) can influence learning and future behaviours; 3. Subjective Value (SV) resulting from one negotiation may feed back, positively or negatively, into future economic outcomes. Negotiation over Time - Participants who had reached an impasse in a prior simulation were more likely to reach an impasse in a subsequent simulation—even when initial impasses were assigned randomly. Results - Feelings about instrumental outcomes have a component of perceptual accuracy that reflects actual instrumental outcomes. - Divergence between objective and subjective value in that—despite the high positive correlation between a negotiator's objective score and instrumental Subjective Value (SV) for pairs of counterparts, their objective scores were highly negatively correlated, whereas their subjective feelings about instrumental outcomes were essentially independent. - The two parties agreed with each other in the form of positive correlations between negotiation partners in their Process Subjective Value (SV) and Relationship Subjective Value (SV). - There is substantial consistency in individual negotiators' skills. Discussion - Subjective Value (SV) is objectively valuable in negotiation. → Subjective impressions appear to pay off economically in subsequent negotiations. - Subjective Value (SV) can be inherently valuable, insofar as negotiators attempt to maximize socio-emotional rewards aside from instrumental rewards. - Individuals earned more objective value in the second negotiation if they had experienced greater Subjective Value (SV) in the first negotiation. - Negotiation dyads created more joint value in the second negotiation if partners had experienced greater Total Subjective Value (SV) in the first negotiation. o The effects were significant for Global, Instrumental, and Relationship Subjective Values (SV). - For Instrumental Subjective Value (SV), negotiators who felt that their first settlements were favorable, fair, and balanced may have benefited subsequently from greater motivation and effort. - Negotiators reported a greater desire to negotiate again with their counterparts after experiencing greater Subjective Value (SV). o Objective performance had no such predictive power, which is striking because, rationally, one should prefer to negotiate with counterparts against whom one has performed objectively well. Beyond negotiated outcomes: The hidden costs of anger expression in dyadic negotiation (Wang et al., 2012) Introduction - Negotiators who express anger tend to receive larger concessions and claim more value. o Negotiations may shape reputations: a critical asset that can significantly influence negotiators' future opportunities and outcomes. - Individuals care not only about the agreement that is reached, but also about the fairness of their treatment in the process. o When fairness is violated, individuals often engage in retaliatory behaviour (overt or covert). o Individuals often acquiesce to unfair treatment in public but pursue retribution through secretive and insidious means. Overt and Covert Effects of Anger Expression - Emotion as Social Information (EASI) model: expression of emotions through nonverbal (facial expression, body posture, or tone of voice) and/or verbal (language) channels can influence an observer through both an inferential and an affective mechanism. o Inferential influence occurs when individuals actively interpret others' expressions for meaning. Emotion-based inferences then inform and shape the observers' subsequent attitudes and behaviors. o The affective mechanism of reactions to expressed emotions in negotiation; emotional expressions elicit affective reactions in observers which may then influence observers' subsequent behaviors. - Rational-Choice arguments: negotiators are strategic actors who detect and decipher opponents' emotional expressions and act on that information to maximize their interests. - One negotiator's expressed anger often results in a negative emotional reaction in the other negotiator. Study 1 - An opponent's expression of anger influenced both a negotiator's overt concessions and a negotiator's covert retaliation towards the opponent. o Overtly, negotiators were more likely to reach agreement when their opponents expressed anger. o The same negotiators were also more likely to covertly retaliate against their opponents who expressed anger. - These negotiators also denied gratifying opportunities and imposed significant obstacles for their opponent after the negotiation. → Anger expression leads to retaliatory behavior intended to harm angry opponents. - Feelings of mistreatment mediated the effect of opponents' expression of anger on focal negotiators' covert retaliation. → Negotiators sabotaged those who expressed anger because they felt offended by the anger. Study 2 - Power in negotiations can be understood as the degree of perceived dependence between the negotiators. - Given the lack of desirable alternatives to a negotiated agreement, low-power negotiators are more likely to appease their angry opponents by making larger concessions during the negotiation. - Since high-power negotiators are less dependent on their low-power opponents, there is less pressure for them to concede to the demands of their opponents even when anger is expressed. o High-power negotiators demanded a large portion of the value regardless of whether or not their low-power opponents expressed anger. o Power influenced negotiators' value claiming behavior in response to opponents' anger expression through perceived risk of adopting a demanding stance overtly. - Both low-power and high-power negotiators covertly retaliated against angry opponents. o High-power negotiators struck back as vigorously as did low-power negotiators after their opponent expressed anger. General Discussion - If focusing solely on claiming value, expressing anger indeed appears to be helpful when negotiating with others who are lower or equal in power. - Covert retaliation in two forms in these studies: o Withholding important resources; o Creating obstacles for a negotiator. The communication orientation model: Explaining the diverse effects of sight, sound, and synchronicity on negotiation and group decision-making outcomes (Swaab et al., 2012) - Face-to-face contact can hinder as well as help the ability to craft and achieve mutually satisfactory negotiation agreements and high-quality decision-making outcomes. - Communication orientation model: proposes that the presence of visual, vocal, and synchronous communication channels: o Help communicators with a neutral orientation achieve efficient and effective negotiation and group decision-making outcomes; o Does not affect the achievement of high-quality outcomes for communicators with a cooperative orientation; o But hurts the outcomes of communicators with a noncooperative orientation. Negotiation and Group Decision-Making - A high-quality outcome is defined in two ways: o Successfully reaching an agreement when there are one or more possible agreements that make both parties better off versus declaring an impasse and walking away from the table, which would leave both parties worse off than if a deal had been reached; o Involves reaching an agreement that expands the total amount of resources available to both parties, often referred to as joint profit. - To achieve high joint profit the parties must make mutually beneficial tradeoffs or identify and optimize compatible interests. - A high-quality group decision-making outcome can be defined in three ways: o Whether decision-makers select an objectively right answer; o A decision that has been identified as superior by outside experts; o A group's ability to reach consensus on a judgment task when agreement among group members is superior to no agreement. - Sharing and integrating information is considered to be a precondition for high-quality outcomes. The Link Among Sight, Sound, Synchronicity, and Outcome Quality: Theoretical Approaches and Empirical Inconsistencies - On one hand, the presence of communication channels (sight, sound, and synchronicity) may increase the likelihood that communicators will develop incipient levels of trust necessary to discover and integrate information for mutual benefit. - On the other hand, eliminating these communication channels may increase the probability of high-quality agreements. o Not being able to see, hear, or directly respond to others may shroud antagonistic behaviors that can be roadblocks to information sharing and integration. - Richness approach: communication settings differ along a continuum of richness, where richness is based on the ability of communication channels to provide communication synchronicity and carry visual or vocal information exchange. o Social presence theory; o Information richness theory; o Cluelessness model; o Reduced social cues approach. - The key proposition of the richness approach is that the presence of sight, sound, and synchronicity makes social interactions more personal, and as a result the capacity of any a communication channel to convey multiple communication cues determines whether people will be able to work effectively with each other, especially when they are faced with an ambiguous task. - The ability of communication channels to carry visual channels or vocal channels or provide rapid feedback through synchronous communication facilitates rapport building and information exchange. - Eye contact can fulfill an important role in building rapport and signaling respect to the counterpart. — Presence of visual channels is a key determinant of cooperation and information integration. - The level, range, and variability of voice pitch, as well as the loudness and tempo of voice, are all thought to be helpful to interpret emotions accurately. — Misunderstandings are more likely to occur over email than in phone conversations and the inability to speak to and hear others decreases the use of cooperative tactics in negotiations. - Communication synchronicity facilitates the ease with which people can socially validate each other's opinion, which then increases the exchange of information. — Communication suffers when people cannot directly respond to one another in real time. - Criticism richness approach: the approach is technologically deterministic and ascribes too much influence to the presence or absence of communication channels and too little attention to the role of the social context in which communication takes place. - Social information processing theory (SIP): communicators strive to develop positive and meaningful relationships and social cues and information are indispensable for the development of such relationships. - Developing meaningful relationships in the absence of visual contact, vocal contact, or synchronicity, takes more time. o The negative effects of non face-to-face interactions on the establishment of social relationships diminish over time. - Social identity model of deindividuation effects (SIDE): the salience of social identities moderates the effects of communication channels on group processes. The presence of communication channels can either intensify or diminish a group process depending on existing social identities. o Cognitive component: intragroup discussions and describes how the salience of social identity moderate the effects of visual contact. It argues that conditions of anonymity may either lead to depersonalized perceptions of self and others and strengthen the salience of social identities that are already in place or amplify personal identity and independence in contexts in which no shared identity exists from the outset. o Strategic component: describes how the absence or presence of individuating information enhances or diminishes influence in intergroup interactions. Conditions of anonymity can increase a low-power party's strategic freedom and influence when the communication environment obscures and shrouds their lack of power. - Strong social identities can overcome the absence of communication channels. - The theories directly contradict the richness approach by showing that the absence of visual, vocal, and synchronous communication channels does not necessarily deteriorate social interactions. - None of the previous theories provide a full explanation for why the presence of communication channels may sometimes have negative consequences for negotiation and group decision-making outcomes. - Neither SIP nor SIDE provides a comprehensive model that covers both negotiations and group decision-making. The Communication Orientation Model - Communication orientation model: the main orientation with which people approach their interaction holds the key to theoretical and empirical parsimony. - Sharing and integrating information is a critical process by which these orientations interact with communication channels to affect negotiation and group decision-making outcomes. - The presence of paraverbal and nonverbal cues such as tone of voice, facial expression, and gesture allows communicators with unknown orientations to learn more about the other side and potentially trust them enough to share and integrate information. o The presence of communication channels help establish rapport. - People with cooperative communication orientations are more likely to value joint welfare, share information about priorities and preferences, and interpret others' actions as efforts to coordinate, which, in turn, increases the quality of their interaction outcomes. - Differs from SIP and SIDE in focusing on how a cooperative communication orientation based on strong interpersonal ties and shared social identities leads individuals to share and integrate information regardless of the presence of communication channels. - Using a noncooperative lens to define the situation, people may interpret each other's actions as efforts to dominate and exploit, which then inspire competitive tactics to defend and protect one's own interests. - The presence of communication channels can create mutually reinforcing competitive tactics and intensify negative emotions that decrease the chances of disclosing accurate information about underlying interests for communicators with noncooperative orientations. - Information sharing and successful integration for communicators with noncooperative orientation are more likely to be achieved in the absence of communication channels. Cross-Cultural Differences as a Test of the Communication Orientation Model - Individuals in interdependent cultures adopt a cooperative approach to social relations—at least with ingroup members. - Individuals from independent cultures tend to think of themselves as autonomous individuals separate from others, give personal goals priority over ingroup goals, and approach their social world with a less cooperative approach to interdependent decision making. Negotiation Meta-Analysis - The presence of communication channels improved negotiation outcome quality for neutrally oriented negotiators, had no effect on outcome quality for cooperatively oriented negotiators, and decreased outcome quality for negotiators with a noncooperative orientation. - The positive effects of communication channels were more pronounced in Western cultures, in which people presumably enter into negotiations with a neutral orientation, than in Eastern cultures, in which people likely enter into negotiations with a more cooperative orientation. Group Decision-Making Meta-Analysis - The positive impact of the presence of communication channels on group decision making was lower when people held a cooperative orientation compared to when they had a neutral orientation for both intellective and judgment decision tasks. - Culture moderated the findings for people with a neutral orientation such that the positive effects of visual channels were more pronounced in Western cultures than in Eastern cultures.