Ouch! You are getting into your bed when you stub the big toe of your right foot against the middle leg of the bed base. ‘Ouch!’ you exclaim, or you shout some expletive that would be inappropriate to repeat in this essay. But it hurts! You can feel the pain in your toe, however the pain is not in your toe, so where is it? Pain is a perception; that is, it’s really all in your head. Whether we’re talking about a burn from a flame, a hammer blow, or the pain from that bed leg you just stubbed your toe on, there’s nothing inherently painful in any given stimulus. Instead, it’s all about how your brain reads, and reacts to, the information it receives from a given stimulus.1 Nevertheless, you are conscious of the pain and it seems to be in your toe, but is The path that a “this feels bad” signal travels going from a stubbed toe to the brain, where it is processed and made into the feeling of pain. Source: http://neurowiki2014.wikidot.com/group:pain the consciousness of the pain in your toe, in your toe? Or is the consciousness of the pain in your toe in your brain, or is the consciousness of the pain in your toe in your mind? Hmmm these are loaded questions so we’ll take a step back and analyse what happens in your body from the moment you stub your toe on the bed post. The stubbing When you stub your toe, on the leg of the bed base say, free nerve endings, called nociceptors,2 in the skin of your toe, send signals through the peripheral nervous system (PNS) to the central nervous system (CNS) via axons. So, your toe, like some other parts of your body, is designed to sense pain. There are generally three main stages in the perception of pain. The first stage is sensing what is going to cause the pain, that is pain sensitivity. In the second stage the signals are transmitted from the cause of the pain, via the peripheral nervous system (PNS), to the dorsal horn (DH), which is located in the spinal cord. Lastly, the third stage performs the transmission of the signals to the higher brain via the central nervous system (CNS). Typically, there are two routes for signal transmissions to be conducted: the ascending and descending pathways. The pathway that goes upward carrying sensory information from the body via the spinal cord towards the brain is defined as the ascending pathway, whereas 1 the nerves that goes downward from the brain to the reflex organs via the spinal cord is known as the descending pathway.3 As far as you are concerned, the pain is in your toe, but your ‘Ouch!’ reaction is not in your toe, so the message must have been transmitted from your toe to your brain and/or your mind to make you conscious of the pain, but what is this consciousness? Nociceptors/free nerve endings, and the fibres carrying the pain sensation from the nociceptors to the spinal cord. So, where are we now in following the pain? Pain Briefly, as we have seen, stubbing your big toe has stimulated many nociceptors in your toe, which have stimulated electrical impulses in axons–nerve fibres–to neurons in your brain, but the question remains; what is it that makes you conscious of the pain? In this essay, we’ll follow the path of the pain and, hopefully, at the end of the essay you will have a credible suggestion for the answer to the question, what is it that makes you conscious of the pain. There are two types of neuronal fibres that carry the pain signal through the PNS. The faster fibres–myelinated A-fibres–elicit a sharp pain sensation and the slower fibres–unmyelinated Cfibres–elicit a more burning pain sensation.4 A closer look Now, we are going to follow the path of the pain to your brain in much greater detail to try and see how you become conscious of the pain. As you can see in the diagram below, neurons are made up of a cell body, also called soma, which contains the nucleus of the cell which controls the cell and keeps it alive, as well as branching treelike fibres known as the dendrites, which receive information in the form of action potentials from other cells via long, segmented fibres, known as axons. If a certain threshold potential is reached the conical axon hillock generates an impulse which enters the axon to be transferred to other cells via the dendrites to axon terminals which end in synapses. 2 Looking more closely at the nerve fibres or axons which conduct the pain signal as action potentials, we see that the Schwann cells constitute the myelinated axon which has nodes of Ranvier, to allow diffusion of ions, Conduction of the action potential between cells In a myelinated axon, the myelin sheath forces the current to travel down the nerve fibre through the unmyelinated nodes of Ranvier, which have a high concentration of ion channels. Upon stimulation, these ion channels propagate the action potential to the next node. In this way the action potential jumps along the fibre as it is regenerated at each node, a process called saltatory conduction. In an unmyelinated axon, the action potential is slower because is propagated along the entire membrane without this help. enabling them to conduct the action potential much faster than the unmyelinated axons to the dendrites which then connect the axon to multiple cells. In addition to carrying information away from the cell body toward other neurons, axons also transmit to the muscles and glands,5 and the brain. As mentioned above, neurons communicate with each other via electrical events called ‘action potentials’. This is important to note: the signals are transmitted electrically as action potentials. These electrical action potentials travel along the axons and dendrites to synapses, which are essentially chemical neurotransmitters that in turn trigger an electrical signal to allow the electrical action potential to propagate through to the next cell. 6 Some physics: all electrical events produce an electromagnetic field (emf), and ‘… it has been Electrochemical transmission of a nerve impulse at the synapse. The synapse, also called a neuronal junction, is the site of the transmission of electric nerve impulses between two nerve cells–neurons–or between a neuron and a gland or muscle cell. A synaptic connection between a neuron and a muscle cell is called a neuromuscular junction. The arrival of the nerve impulse at the presynaptic terminal stimulates the release of neurotransmitter into the synaptic gap. The binding of the neurotransmitter to receptors on the postsynaptic membrane stimulates the regeneration of the action potential in the postsynaptic neuron. known for more than a century that the brain generates its own electromagnetic (em) field, a fact that is widely utilised in brain scanning techniques.’7 3 This is a clue to what consciousness is. There are two main reasons that substantiate the idea that specific areas of the brain are related both physically and electrically to specific body parts and, motor inputs and sensory inputs. These are illustrated in the two diagrams below. 4 patterns If our brains are connected to a sensing device such as an electroencephalogram–EEG; functional magnetic resonance imaging–fMRI; positron emission tomography–PET; precision ultrasound– magnetic resonance-guided focused ultrasound surgery–MRgFUS, or event-related potentials– ERPs, it becomes obvious that different parts of the body are permanently associated with the same parts of the brain so it will have the same pattern of stimulus, that is, we can say that the emf produced by a particular stimulus will always have the same pattern. In our example of the stubbed toe, the emf pattern of which is stored in your memory, will stir that memory of previous stubbings. Similarly, every time you see the colour red, you will be conscious of it as the colour red because of the electromagnetic pattern you have memorised for red. That is, from now on, observing something that is red in colour will stimulate the particular emf pattern that you are conscious of as being red. [The subject of memory, is important to consciousness, but there it is not so well understood as it seemed to be until fairly recently, so we’ll take a brief look at both the main current theories and their failings and leave it to the experts to do further work … later.] Since you will be receiving a large number of stimuli at any given moment—sights, smells, sounds, tastes, thoughts, maybe pain—they will produce a large number of patterns which will accumulate and combine to form a constantly changing complex electromagnetic field. It is proposed that it is this constantly changing complex electromagnetic field that we perceive as being consciousness, but let’s not get ahead of ourselves.8 But how was this electrical stimulation achieved? This question was left unanswered until recent work by Thibaud Parpaite and Bertrand Coste on ‘Piezo channels’, revealed that the neurons are stimulated by the actions of piezo9 proteins. Piezo proteins constitute a family of excitatory ion channels directly gated by mechanical forces. These ion channels are involved in cell mechanotransduction—the conversion of mechanical forces into biological signals. 5 Mechanotransduction is important because all living organisms are subjected to mechanical forces from their environment and rely on mechanotransduction for their survival. For instance, our senses of touch, mechanical pain, proprioception,10 hearing and balance depend on mechanically-activated channels.11 Piezo-dependent mechanotransduction. Various mechanical stimuli exerted on cells induce changes in the membrane tension, resulting in the piezo channel opening. In the cemi field theory,12 a conscious being is aware of the information contained within the cemi field, that is, qualia—the subjective feel of particular mind states13—must correspond to particular configurations of the cemi field. The qualia for the colour red will thereby correspond to the em field perturbations that are generated whenever our neurones are responding to red light in our visual field.14 Relevant work in 2019 is reported under the heading, ‘What happens in our brain when we do complex tasks?’; Have you ever sat down to complete your morning crossword or Sudoku and wondered about what’s happening in your brain? Somewhere in the activity of the billions of neurons in your brain lies the code that lets you remember a key word, or apply the logic required to complete the puzzle. Given the brain’s intricacy, you might assume that these patterns are incredibly complex and unique to each task. But recent research suggests things are actually more straightforward than that. It turns out that many structures in your brain work together in precise ways to coordinate their activity, shaping their actions to the requirements of whatever it is that you’re trying to achieve.15 What we are interested here is the mention of patterns, which indicates that the same parts of the brain always perform the same tasks and would, therefore, produce the same electromagnetic fields. However, since at the level of the brain’s em field, sensory information may be combined with neuronal information acquired through learning, the ensuing field modulations would be expected to correlate not with the sensory stimuli alone, but with the meaning of particular stimuli. ???? Herex First time stubbing, no pain sensed at first, what? no memory, where is memory? Follow the pain from the stubbing to the brain, feeling 6 Back emf role?? The role of consciousness in memory16 It is well established that conscious awareness or attention appears to a prerequisite to laying down long-term memories and for learning complex tasks (although unconscious or subliminal learning may be possible for some tasks), but the mechanism remains obscure. However, in the cemi field theory, memory and learning are inevitable consequences of conscious attention. As described above, the influence of the cemi field in the brain (consciousness) may provide a fine control over motor tasks — a small push or pull on the probability of neurone firing. However, if the target neurones for em augmentation are connected by Hebbian synapses,17 then the influence of the brain’s em field will tend to become hard-wired into either increased (long-term potentiation, LTP) or decreased (long-term depression, LTD) neural connectivity. After repeated augmentation by the brain’s em field, conscious motor actions will become increasingly independent of em field influences. The motor activity will be ‘learned’ and may thereafter be performed unconsciously, without the em influence on the neural networks involved. Similarly, in the absence of any motor output, the cemi field may be involved in strengthening synapses to ‘hard-wire’ neurones and thereby lay down long-term memories. Before we go on, it’s important to clear up the controversial aspects of what consciousness is. Since it will come up early in any discussion about consciousness, we’ll start with the “hard problem”, first conjectured in 1995 by David Chalmers: It is widely agreed that experience arises from a physical basis, but we have no good explanation of why and how it so arises. Why should physical processing give rise to a rich inner life at all? It seems objectively unreasonable that it should, and yet it does.18 This confusion leads to a definition of consciousness which is controversial, as is delineated in this Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy entry.19 herex 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Brian Gardiner, ‘Why Does Stubbing Your Toe Hurt So Damn Much?’, Wired Science, 7 April, 2015: https://www.wired.com/2015/04/stubbing-toe-hurt-damn-much/ Nachum Dafny, ‘Pain Principles’, Neuroscience Online, the Open-Access Neuroscience Electronic Textbook: https://nba.uth.tmc.edu/neuroscience/m/s2/chapter06.html Yam Mun Fei, et al., ‘General Pathways of Pain Sensation and the Major Neurotransmitters Involved in Pain Regulation’, International Journal of Molecular Sciences: https://www.mdpi.com/1422-0067/19/8/2164/htm Tatiana F Almeida, Suely Roizenblatt, and Sergio Tufik, ‘Afferent pain pathways: a neuroanatomical review’, Brain Research, 1000.1, 2004, 40–56. Charles Stangor and Jennifer Walinga, ‘Chapter 4. Brains, Bodies, and Behaviour, 4.1 The Neuron Is the Building Block of the Nervous System’, Introduction to Psychology – 1st Canadian Edition: https://opentextbc.ca/introductiontopsychology/chapter/31-the-neuron-is-the-building-block-of-the-nervous-system/ The Editors of Encyclopaedia Britannica, ‘Neuronal junction, Synaptic junction’, Britannica: https://www.britannica.com/science/neuroplasticity Johnjoe McFadden, ‘Synchronous Firing and Its Influence on the Brain’s Electromagnetic Field: Evidence for an Electromagnetic Field Theory of Consciousness’, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9, No. 4, 2002, p 24. For more detailed explanation of the ‘Electromagnetic Theory of Consciousness’, see Johnjoe McFadden, YouTube, especially around 30 minutes: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=pRaRYX3bBRE Older readers may be familiar with the crystal pick-up on a gramophone which relies on the distortion of the crystal by the ripples in the grooves on the record to produce, by the piezo effect, an electric potential which is converted into music by the gramophone circuitry. proprioception is the perception or awareness of the position and movement of the body. Thibaud Parpaite and Bertrand Coste, ‘Piezo channels’, Current Biology 27, 3 April 3, 2017 © 2017 Elsevier Ltd, R243–R258: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.01.048 conscious electromagnetic information (CEMI) field theory 7 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 Michael Tye, ‘Qualia’, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2021 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.): https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2021/entries/qualia. Johnjoe McFadden, ‘Synchronous Firing and Its Influence on the Brain’s Electromagnetic Field: Evidence for an Electromagnetic Field Theory of Consciousness’, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9, No. 4, 2002, p 43. Shine, J. M., Breakspear, M., Bell, P.T. et al. ‘Human cognition involves the dynamic integration of neural activity and neuromodulatory systems’, Nature Neuroscience 22, 289–296 (2019): https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0312-0 Johnjoe McFadden, p 41. D O Hebb, The Organization of Behavior, New York: Wiley & Sons, 1949. David J Chalmers, ‘Facing Up to the Problem of Consciousness’, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2(3):200–19, 1995. Van Gulick, Robert, ‘Consciousness’, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2021 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.): https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2021/entries/consciousness/ 8