Second Language Acquisition Theories and Teaching Practice: How Do They Fit? Author(s): Renate A. Schulz Source: The Modern Language Journal , Spring, 1991, Vol. 75, No. 1 (Spring, 1991), pp. 17-26 Published by: Wiley on behalf of the National Federation of Modern Language Teachers Associations Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/329831 JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at https://about.jstor.org/terms National Federation of Modern Language Teachers Associations and Wiley are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Modern Language Journal This content downloaded from 122.53.61.63 on Thu, 02 Jun 2022 14:28:00 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms Second Language Acquisition Theories and Teaching Practice: How Do They Fit? RENATE A. SCHULZ Department of German University of Arizona Tucson, AZ 85721 acquired a functional proficiency in two non- then a college senior - was diagnosed as sufprimary languages be suddenly considered as fering from Attention Deficit Disorder, a learnlacking in foreign language learning aptitude? ing disability characterized, in her case, A bycloser look at my daughter's second language short attention span, easy distractability, poor learning history may provide some explanation motor coordination, poor handwriting, and for the apparent inconsistency. inconsistent spelling.' Results of the Woodcock-My daughter acquired German as a "mother Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery indicated tongue." That is, the language spoken to her above average verbal ability and reading aptiby her mother up to about age seven was pretude, but a severe deficit in visual perceptual dominantly German. In addition to the input speed, and below average scores in math and she received from her mother in the US, she AT THE AGE OF TWENTY-TWO, MY DAUGHTER written language aptitude. All other abilities spent an average of about six weeks annually tested, such as broad cognitive ability, reasonin Germany during her early childhood. Since ing, memory, and knowledge aptitude, were the age of seven, English became with rare well within the average ranges. exceptions the exclusive language in the home. You may wonder about the relevance of my My daughter did, however, continue to spend intermittent summer vacations with German daughter's learning disability to the topic of this paper. Of interest is that, as a result of that relatives. The two times she attempted to learning disability diagnosis, my daughter re"learn" German as a foreign language in high ceived a waiver for requirements in matheschool and college she did not do particularly matics and foreign languages. Still more interesting is, however, that my daughter-- well. At the time I gave the fault for her mediocre performance to the teachers whowhose psychological test profile indicates in my opinion--were unable to deal with her apparently little talent for learning a foreign language--is functionally trilingual. She has native fluency in English, rates probably a "ter- minal two" on the ILR Scale in German, and about a 1 + in Spanish.2 When she was diag- nosed as lacking foreign language learning apti- tude, she had already fulfilled her language requirement and was enrolled in a third-year Spanish composition course - with which she did, however, have major problems. A superficial examination of the facts reported may lead one to doubt the construct superior conversational fluency within the constraints of a grammar-oriented classroom. My daughter's efforts to learn Spanish started in high school, but she dropped the course because she found the highly analytical grammatical approach boring. She then took the first two semesters at the university, completing both courses with a grade of C. After her year of elementary college Spanish she spent one summer in an intensive study pro- gram in Mexico, and the following summer holding summer employment in Costa Rica. and predictive validities of the psychological To make a long story short, my daughter has tests used to determine learning disabilities. been successful in acquiring three languages in How, after all, can an individual who has a predominantly natural acquisition environment but has considerable problems learning The Modern Language Journal, 75, i (1991) 0026-7902/91/0001/017 $1.50/0 ?1991 The Modern LanguageJournal a language in a formal school setting where the instructional goals, activities, and tests emphasize analysis and mastery of the grammatical code. Her experience has led me to reexamine This content downloaded from 122.53.61.63 on Thu, 02 Jun 2022 14:28:00 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms 18 The Modern Language Journal 75 (1991) theoretical assumptions and research pertainlectual processes; 4) those attempting a nativist ing to the differences betweenornaturalistic biological explanation,and emphasizing inborn, classroom language learning and toabilities; an attempt genetic and 5) those emphasizing the to reconcile the conflicts between prevalent lanlearner and learning strategies. Because of guage acquisition theories and teaching space constraints, practice. I will limit myself to reviewWhy can practically all "normal" ing five theoretical children, models which I believe most adolescents, and young adults relevant "acquire" a lan-1) Acculturation/ to FL educators: guage when immersed in the target Pidginizationlanguage Theory; 2) Linguistic Universals Theory, particularly it interfaces with Interculture, but so many fail to succeed in a asclassroom setting? Why do people who have language Theory; 3) Discourse Theory; 4) Cogacquired a second language in actual communi- nitive Theory; and 5) the Monitor Model. cative contexts remember the language much longer than individuals whose language experi- ACCULTURATION/PIDGINIZATION THEORY ence was limited to a tutored setting? Are there insights from L2 acquisition theory which can The Acculturation/Pidginization Theor be applied to instruction to make the classroom vanced, among others, by Schumann, resemble more closely a natural acquisition that second language acquisition is part acculturation process and that the degree setting? Unfortunately, theoretical inquiry and guage proficiency is determined by the empirical research into areas related to lanto which a learner acculturates to the targ guage (TL) group. guage acquisition orient themselves most often This acculturation process is affected b to L1 (native language) acquisition or to L2 social and psychological "distance" betwe (second language) acquisition in a naturalistic home and the foreign cultures. These soc (non-tutored) setting. While scholars recognize the differences inherent in the acquisition envipsychological variables determine the eff guage learners will make to come into c ronment when language development depends on classroom instruction only, without the reinwith speakers of the TL and the degree to they are open to the input they receive forcement and support of a target language of the factors which, according to Schu setting and natural communicative constraints, are believed to be conducive to positive the psycholinguistic processes of acquiring a distance are the perceived social equa language (i.e., of gaining proficiency in a language) are believed to be similar, regardless of between the L1 and L2 groups, the simi between the native and TL cultures, low whether the language is acquired in a classsiveness by the "outsiders" as a cultural room, in a natural setting, or through classroom instruction with access to a natural within the TL culture (i.e., easy integ setting. Let me provide a thumbnail sketch of some and assimilation into the TL culture), po currently prevalent theories which try to explain, at least in part, how second or foreign languages are learned. McLaughlin discusses five of those theories: Interlanguage Theory, Linguistic Universal Theory, Acculturation/ Pidginization Theory, Cognitive Theory, and Krashen's Monitor Model. Ellis (2) adds several more: Accommodation Theory, Discourse Theory, the Variable Competence Model, and the Neurofunctional Theory.3 Wode points out that language acquisition theories fall into five general categories: 1) those attempting a behavioristic explanation, emphasizing the role of conditioning; 2) those attempting an interactionist explanation, emphasizing communicative/social need, purpose, and setting; 3) those attempting a cognitive explanation, emphasizing logical, intel- attitudes toward each other, and an ex tion by the L2 learner to stay in (or pos travel to?) the TL area for an extended Positive psychological distance is estab if learners encounter neither language n ture shock nor culture stress and if they high motivation and ego permeability task. Acculturation Theory suggests that social and psychological distance is great when attitudes toward the TL and its sp are negatively loaded and integrative m tion is lacking, learners will have diffic progressing beyond the early stages in la development, and the language will s pidginized (i.e., will fossilize in reduc simplified forms). Acculturation accounts mainly for natu tic L2 acquisition. However, we need t This content downloaded from 122.53.61.63 on Thu, 02 Jun 2022 14:28:00 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms Renate in A. mind vation Schulz 19 marked structures would need to occur much the import in the L2 more frequently in the input of the learner than a the less marked a ones to assure their acquisition. might play simila If, indeed, all natural languages are conlanguage learning.4 notion that affective factors determine the effort strained by universal principles inherent in our a student makes in and out of the classroom genetic make-up, and if these principles can be to obtain input and to use the language for arranged in a certain "accessibility hierarchy," communicative purposes. it follows that first and second language learners should make similar errors at similar stages in the acquisition process. This assumption is indeed supported by a number of studies, By examining surface featuresinvolving of a wide several different languages, examin- LINGUISTIC UNIVERSALS THEORY ing theconinterlanguages (the language output at range of human languages, linguists are tinuing to discover general sets of aprinciples particular stage of linguistic development) of variousof learners that apply to all languages. The theory Lin- in naturalistic as well as in learning situations. While error guistic Universals, or Universal classroom Grammar Theory, tries to explain language analyses acquisition indicate that interlanguages are influencedinnate, by a number of factors, studies have (L1 and L2) by hypothesizing a shared, a tendency for some errors to occur at biological, linguistic component in shown the genetic make-up of homo sapiens which accounts for particular stages of acquisition, regardless of the learner's mother tongue or age or the way the these universally shared linguistic features. Universal Grammar Theory holdslanguage "that the was acquired. In other words, the child starts with all the principles of Universal types of errors made by L2 as well as FL Grammar available" and that "the right learners enviare constrained by their universal ronmental input at the right time furthers grammar the (14: p. 98). Here is where Universal Theory interfaces with Interlanacquisition process" (14: pp. 93,Grammar 94). The theory posits that Universal Grammar guage becomes Theory. operative in L1 as well as L2 acquisition, in child language learning as well as INTERLANGUAGE in that for THEORY adults. While initially it was believed that this "mental language organ," or languageSelinker acquisi- defines interlanguage as a sep linguistic system, constructed by the lea tion device (LAD), atrophies with the onset of theno result of five central cognitive proce puberty, a number of studies indicate qualilanguage transfer from the mother ton transfer of training, resulting from spec child learner, except in pronunciation ability.5 In fact, adults -because of increased channel tures of instruction; 3) second language le 4) second language commun capacity due to maturational factors - mightstrategies; be strategies; and 5) overgeneralization of th the more efficient foreign language learners, of the target language. Through error a particularly if exposure time and input are of speech and writing samples of learn limited to that of a traditional language course. various stages, researchers have found In a totally naturalistic setting the child coninterlanguages reflect systematic patter tinues to be superior, not because of a better error and communication strategies. Man functioning LAD, but, it is now believed, these errors are developmental and will because of differences (in quantity and quality) tually disappear if the learner receives suf in the available input.6 appropriate input. What is of interest to us is that Linguistic Universals Theory posits an inherent hierarchyInterlanguage forms found in early lan acquisition can also be found in pidgi of difficulty among the universal "rules" which guages. The speakers of a pidgin lang are dependent on the "degree of markedness," at a relatively early stage of in or complexity, of a certain structure.' Itfossilize is believed that those structures which fall under guage development because, it is believed receive insufficient input and lack the m the universal core grammar are less marked tion or need to perfect their language skill and more easily acquired than the structures tative differences between the adult and the their limited communication needs can be satisidiosyncratic to a particular language (peripheral grammar). The more highly factorily fulfilled without grammatical accu- This content downloaded from 122.53.61.63 on Thu, 02 Jun 2022 14:28:00 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms 20 The Modern Language Journal 75 (1991) racy. Continued comprehensible input, however exposed most frequently]; (and, I assume, continued efforts by the acquires lan- commonly b) the learner guage learner to approximate theoccurring standard of and then later formulas the target language), can help learners over-these into their [stress added] analyses come that stage and continue to component move toward parts; closer approximation of the target language. c) the learner is helped to construct senWhat are the implications of Interlanguage tences vertically [i.e., by borrowing Theory for FL teaching? Extended comprehensible parts of speech of preceding discourse, input helps learners shape their output an also knownto as "scaffolding"] . increasingly closer approximation of the TL COGNITIVE norm. Formal instruction (i.e.,THEORY grammar analysis and discrete-point grammar practice) Rather than stressing innate, universal l can temporarily improve performance on disguistic processes, affective factors, input, crete-point tests, but apparently has relatively interaction as causative factors for second lan- little influence on spontaneous language use. guage development, Cognitive Theory sees second language learning as a mental process, leading through structured practice of various Discourse theory posits that learners developsubskills to automatization and component competence in a second language not simply integration of linguistic patterns. While Disby absorbing input, but by actively participatcourse Theory posits that language is available DISCOURSE THEORY ing in communicative interaction, by after it has been acquired or fori.e., analysis negotiating meaning and filling information routinized, Cognitive Theory maintains that gaps. Ellis (2) states a main hypothesis of automatic or routinized only after skills become processes. Controlled analytical proDiscourse Theory, which applies toanalytical L1 as well as L2 acquisition: "The development of the- forcesses including, of course, structured pracmal linguistic devices for realizing basic tice - are lanseen as "stepping stones" for automatic guage function grows out of the interpersonal processes use (14: p. 135). [stress added] to which language is put" Rather than positing a hierarchical develop(p. 259). ment of linguistic structures, such as suggested Like other theories mentioned, Discourse by Interlanguage Theory, Cognitive Theory Theory addresses L2 acquisition in a naturalis- posits a hierarchy of complexity of cognitive tic setting. We might nevertheless want to subskills which lead from controlled practice to examine the principles advanced by Hatch and automatic processing of language. As the summarized by Ellis (2: pp. 259-60) for impli- learner develops increasing degrees of mastery, cations for foreign language learning: he or she engages in a constant process of 1) SLA follows a "natural" route in syntacrestructuring to integrate new structures with tical development. [Hatch believes this those previously learned. Cognitive learning "natural" route is determined by the pre- dictable discourse--which, of course, thus is seen to consist of several different phases where the learning tasks become refined, restructured, and consolidated.8 learners engage.] The notion that analysis and structured prac2) Native speakers adjust their speech in tice foster automatic processing of language and order to negotiate meaning with non- are essential to foreign language development native speakers [intuitively they speak in a classroom setting is not new. Increasingly, more slowly, louder, use shorter sentences however, researchers question whether L2 includes predictable input - in which L2 and less complex structures.] 3) The conversational strategies used to negotiate meaning, and the resulting adjusted input, influence the rate and route of SLA in a number of ways: acquisition is a skill- similar to driving a car or playing the piano--that can be mastered exclusively through controlled operations of subskills which lead eventually to their auto- matic processing, i.e., to spontaneous com- a) the learner learns the grammar of the municative language use. Cognitive Theory L2 in the same order as the frequency with a sprinkling of Discourse Theory and order of the various features in the behaviorist conditioning- seems to account input [i.e., the learner masters first most closely for what foreign language teachers those structures to which he or she is and current textbooks try to accomplish in This content downloaded from 122.53.61.63 on Thu, 02 Jun 2022 14:28:00 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms Renate A. Schulz 21 The Input Hypothesis, in Krashen's words, classroom instructi refers to his belief that "humans acquiredo lanmatical syllabus through guage analysis an in only one way --by understanding messages, or by receiving 'comprehensible processing) to autom albeit limited pract input' .. ." (12: p. 2). Two corollaries of the of Cognitive Theory Input Hypothesis (12: p. 2) state: ciently reflected in t 1) Speaking is a result of acquisition and not books. Cognitive The its cause. Speech cannot be taught directly continuing restruc but "emerges" on its own as a result of through various rec building competence via comprehensible other theories whic input. acquisition, Cognitive 2) If input is understood, and there is enough tain spiral or cyclica of it, the necessary grammar skills, where the is automatiint cally provided. The language teacher dent permits continu need not attempt deliberately to teach the approximation to the next structure along the natural orderroom teaching and te it will be provided in just the right quansufficiently recogniz tities and automatically reviewed if the refinement with con Our student receives a sufficient amount of expectations mastery theory. comprehensible input. are not of supp The Monitor Hypothesis holds that formal learning has no effect on acquisition except that THE MONITOR MODEL it can serve as a monitor or editor for the learner's output, provided 1) there is sufficient The most ambitious and widely known - the as focus of the interaction is on form time; 2) well as presently the most controversial - theory rather than meaning; and 3) the learner knows theFL rule in question. which attempts to account for L2 and acquisition is Krashen's Monitor Model. This Thetheory Affective Filter Hypothesis posits a mental screen between the learner and the environis also the only one from which direct pedagogi- cal extrapolations have been madement in the sowhich is activated by affective factors called Natural Approach (13). Since the self-confidence, etc.) and which (e.g., anxiety, Monitor Model has received extensive atten- controls the amount of input a student is tion (laudatory and critical) in the professional exposed to and the amount of input a student literature, I provide only a brief summary ofconverts into intake. A high affective filter its five main tenets. inhibits acquisition, a low affective filter proKrashen's Acquisition/Learning Hypothesis motes it. In Krashen's words (12: p. 33): maintains that adult or adolescent language". .. comprehensible input and the strength of learners have two processes at their disposal tothe filter are the true causes [stress added] of help them in developing language fluency. Onesecond language acquisition." is acquisition, the other, learning. Acquisition Krashen's Monitor Model has been criticized is subconscious and takes place through natural on a number of points. Of major interest to us language interactions, similar to those available are the criticisms levied against his acquisition/ to children when they acquire their motherlearning dichotomy and his view of comprehentongue. Learning, on the other hand, requires sible input as sole explanatory factor for second conscious thought and analysis and takes place language acquisition. Clearly, we can all attest predominantly in formal instruction. Accord-from personal experience that skills which at ing to Krashen, only language that has beenone time were learned consciously through acquired is available for use in spontaneoussegmentation and analysis can eventually communication. become automatic through practice and be The Natural Order Hypothesis, inspired byavailable for spontaneous use. To what extent Universal Grammar and Interlanguage Theory, this conscious analysis is "necessary" or helpful maintains that we acquire grammatical strucfor foreign language learning when sufficient tures in a predictable order not determined and by appropriate comprehensible input is not the order in which they are taught (12: p. 1). available remains a major question. This content downloaded from 122.53.61.63 on Thu, 02 Jun 2022 14:28:00 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms 22 The Modern Language Journal 75 (1991) FROM THEORY TO PRACTICE on many. And because of the nature of learn- ing, in general, which proceeds on a highly individualistic Language acquisition - be it first, second, basis, or students are frequently at foreign - is an extremely complex different process, levels parof language development, even ticularly difficult to penetrate since itthey cannot though are in the same class. What, then, are some common tenets, shared by the theories be directly observed. None of the theories discussed,explawhich do have implications for discussed offers a complete and coherent nation. Most attempt to explain how a second teaching? Even considering language is learned by examining only one of the currently rather inconclusive state of aL2 acquisition theory and the many contributing factors. Eventually, research, inputwill and interaction clearly play a more complete theory of L2 acquisition major role in language learning, in- or outside have to account for the biological/innate, the the classroom. Motivation also clearly affects social/interactive, the cognitive, and the both the amount of input students seek and the behaviorist aspects of language learning. And number of communicative interactions a sound pedagogy will, in addition, have to which they are willing to engage. keep in mind the many possible individual in learner factors which facilitate or inhibit second language development in a classroom setting. Probably wisely, few psycholinguists venture into the pedagogical implications of current theories. I am, however, first and foremost a practitioner who sees the prime value of theory and research in their potential for leading us to possible practical implications and applications to improve teaching and learning. Unfor- tunately, those of us who are FL teachers do not have the luxury of waiting around for the definitive theory and its verification by research before deciding on what to do in the classroom. THE INPUT FACTOR According to Ellis (2: p. 276): Input comprises (1) the inherent properties of target language system, and (2) the formally a interactionally adjusted features found in foreig and teacher talk. Input constitutes the data u which the learner strategies work, but also the i is itself in part determined by the learner's us communication strategies. Thus the relationship tween input and learner processes is an interac one. The implications of the input facto Let me, therefore, be foolhardy enough to attempt to find some pedagogical implications siderable for foreign language teach in the theories discussed. they point to the need for language on the part of the teacher who is fre Extrapolating from naturalistic child lanonly "live" source of input (other tha guage acquisition to adult or adolescent foreign vided by other learners) available to language learning in a classroom is difficult because, clearly, major differences exist This does not mean that all teachers must have between these modes: differences in the physinative speaker competence. But it does mean that teachers must be able to speak a language cal, psychological, and intellectual maturity fluently and accurately enough to feel comfortbetween both groups of learners, in situations able in using it as exclusive means of communiand settings in which interaction occurs, in the cation, whether for instructional purposes, type and amount of input available, in the types of communicative acts that occur and their classroom management, or social interaction. underlying purposes, in available language-useA rating of "advanced" on the ACTFL/ETS opportunities, in personal motivation to availoral proficiency scale would indicate the minioneself of such opportunities, etc. As a prac- mally required competence.9 tical example, input and interaction opportuni- The importance of input has implications for ties available in the classroom differ from thoseinstructional time. Regardless of the quantity and quality of input provided in the classroom, encountered on the playground or in a local bar. And the Chinese student hoping to studythe time available in a conventional foreign language program - for the majority of students in the US is likely to make a greater effort finding target language texts and speakers tolimited to one or two years - is simply inadeinteract with than the American student who is taking Chinese to fulfill a language requirement. Obviously, naturalistic language learning takes place one-on-one; classroom learning, one quate, if we hope to have them develop any meaningful, lasting communicative proficiency. The importance of input has further implications for developers of instructional software, including textbooks. I invite you to count the This content downloaded from 122.53.61.63 on Thu, 02 Jun 2022 14:28:00 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms Renate A. Schulz 23 tice, i.e., that thetextboo learners' general know pages in your puter software, or th of the language governs the quantity of in which they take part. audiotape tice programs t Practically speaking, this means si tha ing, linguistically, should and not plan to teach comparative a authentic appr "input" is tives, but provided rather to compare the world's and countries as to their size, population the classroom. We ne prevalent "frontal sity, living standard, and use of natur instruction sources. We (i.e., should not focus whe on teaching of most input and th subjunctive - but provide situations where tion) goals. is Language learning- regardless of theoretical orientation - necessitates frequent recycling of lexical and grammatical structures in different contexts. While we pay lip service to the cyclical nature of language learning, indicating at least an awareness that the frequency in which vocabulary and grammatical patterns are encountered in the input contributes to their eventual retention and use, a large percentage of the words and structures we expect in the students' active command appear only once or twice in the textbook. (Recycling should, of course, not just be limited to receptive skill modalities. But appropriate written, oral, and visual input - that showing target-culture specific settings and situations - can provide for most dents have to make appropr polite requests or conj about the future of humanity if we conti the present course of polluting and explo our environment. And yes, incidental grammatical pattern exists that can be us many polite requests. ... In other wor advocate a content and problem-solving appro FL instruction. ESL instructors have known for a long time that even those students lacking in academic language learning aptitude and who are unsuccessful in analyzing and reciting grammatical paradigms can benefit greatly from content-based language instruction. Again, the importance of interaction holds implications for our instructional materials as well as classroom activities. I invite you to count textbook activities which require students to interact with other living beings in or out- much of the needed recycling.) side the classroom in a communicative context THE INTERACTION FACTOR and information has to be obtained. From the table of contents of textbooks and the number where meaning actually has to be negotiated As for the importance of interaction, we need of pages devoted to it, can one doubt that gramto examine the amount and type of practice we the real content of present FL mar remains instruction? provide in and outside the classroom. While folk wisdom tells us that practice makesYet perfect, general agreement exists among theoreticians and researchers that the textbook does not it may not be the quantity of practice but the kind of practice that enhances acquisition. determineNot the order of grammatical mastery, all practice may be equally effectiveand for thatlearngrammatical grading and sequencing - such as for we encounter in most instructional ing a foreign language. Ellis (3: p. 32), instance, conjectures that "controlled" ornot necessary for language acquisitexts - are tion in or outside the classroom. I mentioned "focused" practice (i.e., practice that focuses learner attention on a discrete linguistic feature) earlier that several studies indicate that, regardmight not be as effective as "free practice" less of how or students have acquired their lan"unfocused performance" (i.e., communicative guage fluency, they develop predictable practice that focuses learner attention on sequences in an the second language. This "natural exchange of information). Based on a review sequence of development" may in part be based of available research on the practiceonvariable, an innate "universal grammar" which makes some rules easier to learn than others and he questions whether any grammar learning takes place in controlled practice and concludes requires that certain structures be acquired that "correct responses merely indicate that the can be integrated. Or it may be before others learner has accessed the appropriate based cognitive on the frequency of certain structures in strategies for reproducing the target structure; the input to which the students are exposed they do not show that learning is taking place." (Hatch); or on the frequency of need for cerEllis suggests not so much that practice causes tain structures in basic human interaction; or acquisition, but that acquisition causes pracin part on all of these and yet additional This content downloaded from 122.53.61.63 on Thu, 02 Jun 2022 14:28:00 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms 24 The Modern Language Journal 75 (1991) unknown factors. It is certainly not based onthrough real-life inpu proficiency mainly interaction. Her motivation to avail hersel the sequence chosen by well-intentioned textbook authors. that input and engage in language intera What are possible implications for FL teachhas been particularly great with Spanish ing? Grammatical analysis and extendedevery time she visited a Spanish-speaking pattern practice may well enable some students try I was faced with the possibility of a Sp to pass a discrete-point grammar test, but it isspeaking son-in-law. unlikely that students will use specific construc- That my daughter's German profici tions correctly in real communicative interacappears to have become arrested some dis tions immediately after having been "covered."from grammatical accuracy I blame main Our teaching and testing practices have to insufficient input. Also, her anxiety to b reflect the fact that "covering" and "teaching" taken for German may have played a are not synonymous with learning, acquiring,Already as a small child, resisting th or mastering. permissive, highly structured environme Research in the near future will probably not her grandmother's household, she would yield a dependable acquisition order of gram- test loudly when she was mistaken for Ge and would thrive on the attention which her matical structures to guide our anticipation of mastery. Our articulation and placement efforts "foreign" status brought her. Since by name and will doubtless continue to be plagued by indi-physical appearance she can be easily mistaken vidual variability in language proficiency for German, she might have subconsciously acquired as a result of one, two, or more years resisted error correction, lest it would threaten of classroom study. As McLaughlin (p. 149) her identity as American. points out, "there seems to be considerable indiCONCLUSIONS vidual variation in how learners acquire second languages due to different learning, perIn the last decade, FL learning has regain formance, and communication strategies." increasing attention, and enrollments are Individual learners will continue to acquire specific structures, lexical items, or com-again on the rise. Even FL requirements in vogue again. The ACTFL-initiated pr municative functions at different rates in spite ciency movement has done much to reinvi of common instruction. Particularly, our testate the profession with renewed commitm ing procedures need to reflect that while we to developing students' communicative pro hope to raise an awareness of morphological ciency. Scholarly and research activities and syntactical patterns, we do not expect their abound; in fact, L2/FL acquisition and teachimmediate mastery. ing are emerging as separate fields of inquiry, I am not arguing for the elimination of gram- interdisciplinary in nature, at a number of mar instruction. What I am arguing for is that institutions. grammar should not play the main role, but As we examine and revise our curricula in a supportive role only, clearly limited in the amount of time we devote to it and in the response to this renewed interest and try to ful- fill a national mandate to develop usable lanweight we allot to it in formal teaching and testing. THE MOTIVATION FACTOR guage skills in our students, we can all benefit by critically examining the implicit and explicit assumptions which guide our teaching in light of recent theoretical and research developIf we succeed in providing sufficient high ments. Based on the present state of L2 acquisi- interest input and practice activities tion which focus theory and research, I recommend that our on content and human interaction, curriculum the third planning and teaching activities be guided by three basic questions: prerequisite to foreign language learning-- motivation - might take care of itself. 1) I How am can conwe supply students with the optivinced that students will be more willingmum to seek amount of interesting, comprehenand use opportunities for foreign language sible input? "practice" if this practice is not limited 2) toWhat gramcan we do to provide students with matical manipulation. opportunities to interact in the language in realin communicative contexts and with Before I conclude, you may be interested how all of this applies to my daughter. real Obcommunicative purposes? 3) What can we do to increase students' viously, she acquired her foreign language This content downloaded from 122.53.61.63 on Thu, 02 Jun 2022 14:28:00 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms Renate A. Schulz 25 motivation soresponses that I predict that satisfactory to these additional input questions will improve our success rate in t an tunities and teaching. In other words,continu student motivation, the year or two of language input, and communicative interaction which convention c may well be the most important factors in FL becoming learning and communi may, in the final analysis, decide guage? our students' level of language proficiency. 10 of a language. Each language differs in certain unique aspects (peripheral grammar). NOTES 7For instance, it is believed that the difficulty level, when relativizing a particular noun phrase, proceeds from subject 'A revised version of a keynote address presented relativization at the as least difficulty via direct object, indirect object, object of a preposition, and genitive to the relativiMLJ/Ohio State Univ. Symposium on Research Perspectives in Adult Language Learning and Acquisition, zationColumof the object of a comparative as most difficult. bus, Ohio, November 1989. 8Interestingly, error analysis is used to support both the 2The descriptor "terminal two" was coined by Theodore nativist/biological and the cognitive explanation of language V. Higgs and Ray Clifford to refer to the phenomenon of acquisition. While adherents to Universal Grammar Theory so-called "street learners" who acquire a second language interpret the learners' transitional grammars to be evidence in a natural setting, without formal instruction. These indi- of the activation of innate principles, more cognitively viduals demonstrate relatively sophisticated vocabulary oriented researchers interpret them to be evidence of cogusage with, however, certain faulty, fossilized grammati- nitive procedural strategies intended to restructure their cal patterns which appear difficult if not impossible to internal representation of the TL. Such strategies include, correct. The "two" refers to the oral proficiency scale, for instance, during the initial stages of language learning, ranging from zero to five, originally developed by the Forsimplifying, regularizing, overgeneralizing, and reducing eign Service Institute and now used by all members of ILR, redundancy. Inferencing and hypothesis testing strategies i.e., all agencies of the federal government involved in for- are more prevalent at later stages. eign language instruction. 9For a description of the ACTFL/ETS Oral Proficiency 3For a short overview of current L2 hypotheses, see entry 4 in the Bibliography. 4For a review of research on the role of attitudes and moti- vation in L2 learning, see entry 5 in the Bibliography. 5For a review of research dealing with the age factor in L2 learning, see entry 6 in the Bibliography. 6"Universal Grammar," or core grammar, should, however, not be expected to be a set of specific grammatical rules in the traditional sense. Rather, it consists of general, shared features in all natural languages. Also, we should not expect Universal Grammar to account for all features Scale see entry 1 in the Bibliography. That many teachers are not "advanced" speakers of the target language was brought home by a Texas study (see 10) which indicated that teachers' oral competence is often considerably below this level. This sad fact is not necessarily an indictment of the teachers, but rather of our system of teacher training, which does not provide easy and affordable access to study abroad opportunities. 'ODuring the 1990-91 academic year, the author's address is: Department of Foreign Languages, US Air Force Academy, CO 80840. 5. Gardner, Robert C. Social Psychology and Second Language BIBLIOGRAPHY Learning: The Role of Attitudes and Motivation. Balti- more: Arnold, 1985. 6. Harley, Birgit. Age in Second Language Acquisition. San 1. American Council on the Teaching of Foreign LanDiego: College-Hill, 1986. guages. "ACTFL Proficiency Guidelines 1986." 7. Hatch, Evelyn. "Discourse Analysis and Second Lan- Defining and Developing Proficiency: Guidelines, guage Acquisition." Second Language Acquisition. Ed. Implementations and Concepts. Ed. Heidi Byrnes Evelyn&Hatch. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, 1978: 401-35. Michael Canale. Lincolnwood, IL: National Textbook, 1987: 15-24. 8. - . "Discourse Analysis, Speech Acts and Second 2. Ellis, Rod. Understanding Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press, 1986. 3. - . "The Role of Practice in Classroom Learning." AILA Review 5 (1988): 20-39. 4. Ferguson, Charles A. & Thom Huebner. "Foreign Language Instruction and Second Language Acquisition Research in the United States." NFLC Occasional Language Acquisition." Second Language Acquisition Research. Ed. W. Ritchie. New York: Academic, 1978. 9. Higgs, Theodore V. & Ray Clifford. "The Push toward Communication." Curriculum, Competence, and the For- eign Language Teacher. Ed. Theodore V. Higgs. Skokie, IL: National Textbook, 1982: 57-79. Papers. Washington: National FL Center, Johns 10. Hiple, David V. &Joan H. Manley. "Testing How Well Hopkins Univ., 1989. Foreign Language Teachers Speak: A State Man- This content downloaded from 122.53.61.63 on Thu, 02 Jun 2022 14:28:00 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms 26 The Modern Language Journal 75 (1991) date." Foreign Language Annals 20for (1987): 147-53. Second Language Acquisition. Rowl 11. Krashen, Stephen. Principles and Practices Second1978. Lanburyof House, guage Acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon, 1982. 16. Selinker, Larry. "Interlanguage." 210-31. Implications. 12. . The Input Hypothesis: Issues and London: Longman, 1985. 17. Wode, Henning. Psycholinguistik. Eine Einfiihrung in die 13. - & Tracy Terrell. The Natural Approach: Lehr- und Lernbarkeit Language von Sprachen. Munich: Hueber, Acquisition in the Classroom. Oxford: 1988. Pergamon, 1983. 14. McLaughlin, Barry. Theories of Second-Language 18. Woodcock, Richard W.Learn& Mary Bonner Johnson. ing. London: Arnold, 1987. Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery. Allen, 15. Schumann, John H. The Pidginization TX:Process: DLM Teaching A Resources, Model 1978. Information about The Modern Language Journal Major Focus: a refereed publication devoted primarily and tables); leave a 2" left margin, 1 2" elsewhere; to research in methods, pedagogy, and applied submit original and two clear copies (only original linguistics pertaining to modern languages including will be returned). TESL; publishes articles, reports, teaching tips, Manuscript Length: twenty pages preferred (excluding news, book reviews, professional advertisements, and bibliography, tables, notes); longer acceptable, deoccasional essays on the state of the profession. Circulation: approximately 7000. pending on merit. Manuscript Style: MLA Style Manual (1985). Frequency of Publication: four issues per year (approxi- Multiple Submissions: manuscripts submitted simulmately 150 pages each). taneously for publication elsewhere not considered; Evaluation: manuscripts refereed by at least two author(s) must inform editor at time of submission specialist readers. of similar/related versions of manuscript that have Time from Submission to Decision: normally sixty days; appeared or are being considered elsewhere. longer during academic vacation periods; time from Special Requirements: 1) indicate full name of instituacceptance to publication usually no longer than one tion where (each) author is employed; 2) include a self-addressed 9 x 12 manila envelope and US $5.00 year. Acceptance Rate: ten to thirteen percent of manuscripts in loose postage or thirteen international postal submitted are accepted. coupons (non-US submissions $10.00 or twenty-five Articles Published per Volume: thirty to forty. international postal coupons if air mail return desired); 3) prepare cover sheet containing the title Ownership: The Modern Language Journal is owned and of the manuscript, separate short title (see 4 below) copyrighted by the National Federation of Modern for identification, name, address, and both office and Language Teachers Associations and published by home telephone numbers of author(s); 4) select one it at the University of Wisconsin Press. The MLJ or two key words from the title of the manuscript has no legal relationship to the Modern Language to function as identification markers; type those Book Reviews per Volume: approximately 200. Association of America (MLA). words at the top right of all pages of the manuscript Payment: authors of articles receive two complimentary followed by the appropriate page number; 5) refer copies of the issue in which their work is published; book reviewers receive one gratis copy. Offprints.: available at cost, but must be purchased directly from the reprint service at the time author reads galley proofs. Language of Publication: English. to your own previous publications in the third person-not "as I said in . . .," but "as Jane Smith out (including bibliography, notes, citations, figures, entry are indented four spaces. noted . . . "; 6) refer to an institution where research was conducted or where the author teaches as "insti- tution X" during the refereeing process; 7) number consecutively each entry in the "Bibliography," being Manuscript Format: typewritten, double-spaced through- sure that the second and subsequent lines of each This content downloaded from 122.53.61.63 on Thu, 02 Jun 2022 14:28:00 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms