Copyright 1984 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1984, Vol. 46, No. 1,44-56 Causes and Effects of Causal Attribution Reid Hastie Northwestern University What types of events instigate causal reasoning and what effects does causal reasoning have on the subsequent use of information stored in memory? Three experiments are reported that address these questions. The essential conclusions of the research are that unexpected events elicit causal reasoning and that causal reasoning produces relatively elaborate memory representations of these events. When subjects reason about why events occurred, they are more likely to remember those events than events that did not elicit causal reasoning. The implications of these findings for theories of causal reasoning and social memory are discussed. Figure 1 is a heuristic summary of the attribution process that serves as an organizational structure for research studying the question, "When do we ask why?" Four types of conditions that elicit attributions have been studied: the asking of a "why" question, the occurrence of events that are unexpected by the perceiver, the dependence of the perceiver on others for hedonically relevant outcomes, and the perceiver's own failure to perform a well-defined task satisfactorily. Enzle and Schopflocher (1978) demonstrated that an instruction to evaluate an actor's dispositional qualities produced attributional reasoning that did not occur as frequently when such an instruction was not put to the subject. The critical dependent variable in this research was a rating of an actor's attractiveness at the end of an experimental session. Subjects who did not make the earlier dispositional evaluation were not sensitive to information about the actor's disposition. Thus, the authors concluded that subjects only performed attributional reasoning when requested to (implicitly) by the experimenter. Several studies support the conclusion that unexpected events instigate attributional processing. Pyszczynski and Greenberg (1981) and Wong and Weiner (1981) showed that a subject's information-seeking responses were oriented toward explanation-relevant information when unexpected events occurred. This tendency to seek explanation-relevant information was relatively weaker when events were expected. Lau and Russell (1980) found thart sports writers were likelier to engage in explanatory analyses when the outcome of a When we perceive events we are almost irresistibly drawn to seek their sources or causes. This tendency is especially strong when the events are the actions of other people (Heider, 1958;Schank&Abelson, 1977;Zajonc, 1980). But, although this tendency is strong and pervasive, it is not inevitable. Psychologists have studied countless stimulus events that do not elicit causal reasoning: light flashes, visual patterns, acoustic tones, lists of common English words, and so on, including virtually all of the conventional experimental laboratory materials. Even social events do not inevitably elicit causal attribution. Some commentators (Manis, 1977) have claimed that the emphasis on attribution processes in recent social psychology is exaggerated. And, of course, most of the interesting literature in Western civilization would be reduced to boring chronicles if protagonists were in the habit of constant attribution. Hamlet, Lady Macbeth, Lear, and Richard of Gloucester might all have ended their plays in good health had they paused more often to wonder why. The point is obvious: Sometimes we engage in extensive causal reasoning, and sometimes we do not. The critical question is, under what conditions do we attempt to explain why an act has occurred? The author would like to thank Joan Dobrof, John Porter, and Barbara Signer for their assistance conducting the research. Requests for reprints should be sent to Reid Hastie, Psychology Department, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60201. 44 CAUSAL ATTRIBUTION sports event was unexpected as compared to when it was expected. Clary and Tesser (in press) asked subjects to retell stories in which a protagonist acted in a manner inconsistent with one of his personal characteristics. A content analysis of the subjects' retellings of the stories found that they tended to spontaneously supply explanations for the characterdiscrepant actions. Dependence on another person for desired outcomes appears to instigate causal reasoning. Berscheid, Graziano, Monson, and Dermer (1976) found that men and women who were about to go out on a date with a stranger were likelier to engage in causal reasoning about the stranger's disposition than were subjects who did not expect to date the stranger. The dependent variables in this research included measures of attention focus, recall and recognition, and extremity of dispositional trait ratings. All of these measures were sensitive to the date^-nondate independent variable manipulation in a manner that was consistent with the conclusion that causal reasoning is likelier to occur when one is dependent on another. Harvey, Yarkin, Lightner, and Town (1980) varied the extent to which the observer of a conversation empathized with the individuals conversing or took a detached role while observing the conversationalists. They found that the number of attributions that appeared in 45 observers' written reports summarizing the conversation was affected by their role; empathizing observers included more attributions in their reports than did detached observers. These researchers also found that recall for information from the conversations was higher for observers who took the empathetic role than for detached observers. The results were interpreted as evidence that the empathetic set stimulated subjects to become involved and to try to understand the causes of the events they observed in the conversations. Observers' efforts to understand the events led them to wonder why they occurred. This interpretation is closely analogous to the suggestion that hedonic relevance or outcome dependency will stimulate subjects to reason causally about outcome-relevant events. A recent article by Monson, Keel, Stephens, and Genung (1982) is also consistent with the general notion that hedonic relevance or outcome dependency affects the nature of causal reasoning about another person's actions. These researchers found that when subjects anticipated future interaction with another person, their attributions tended to be more valid, or at least dependent on the available causally relevant information, than when future interaction was not anticipated. Wong and Weiner (1981) and Diener and Dweck (1978) provide evidence that when a subject fails a task (typically an academic EXPLICIT QUESTION i UNEXPECTED/ EVENT OUTCOME IDEPENDENCY/ TASK FAILURE Figure ]. Hypothetical sequence of conditions and processing stages that are involved in causal reasoning. 46 REID HASTIE achievement task) he or she is likelier to engage in attributional reasoning to explain the failure than when he or she does not fail. Wong and Weiner used information-seeking measures (requests for additional information) in the context of a role-play study, whereas Diener and Dweck used a concurrent talk-aloud procedure to study what was on their primary school subjects' minds while performing an academic task. Figure 1 can also help us organize the empirical research according to the substages of the attribution process that are referenced by dependent variable measures in each study. The Wong and Weiner (1981) and Pyszczynski and Greenberg (1981) experiments attempted to tap the attention and information-seeking substage of the process. The other studies focused on the products of the attribution process studying disposition trait ratings of actors or the summary of attributional reasoning in a newspaper article. The Diener and Dweck (1978) study is difficult to classify, but their measures of attribution contents in talk-aloud protocols were counts of the number of attribution conclusions that each subject reached. Thus, their measure also captures a product of the entire process. Figure 1 indicates tha^ the products of attribution will be stored in long-term memory some of the time. Smith and Miller (1979) and Hastie (1980) have suggested that memory traces may provide information concerning attribution processes. Smith and Miller suggested that responses to questions concerning the causes of the behavior would be answered more quickly if a proposition summarizing attribution for the product had been previously stored in memory as compared to an answer based on mental computation at the time the question was presented. Smith and Miller (1979) and Sherman and Titus (1982) have reported empirical results that support the notion that causal attribution does frequently occur during normal comprehension processes. These researchers also hypothesized that the product of this causal reasoning, inferences about causes and future effects, would be stored in memory representations along with the original stimulus information. A number of researchers including Harvey et al. (1980), Sherman and Titus (1982), Bower and Masling (1980), Stevenson (1981), Fisher and Craik (1980), Bradshaw and Anderson (1982), and Black and Bern (1981) have all hypothesized that causally significant propositions will be highly elaborated when stored in memory with other related event information. Black and Bower (1980) and Graesser, Robertson, Lovelace, and Swinehart (1980) have suggested that information about plans and goals will tend to be stored in relatively superordinate locations within general hierarchical knowledge structures used to encode narrative information. Uncompleted plans involving blocked goals will not be accorded such priority in mental storage structures, and they will be relatively poorly remembered compared to information directly relevant to completed plans. Crocker, Binns, and Weber (1983) have suggested that the type of causal attribution that is provided for an act will determine whether or not the act is well remembered. Their suggestion, supported by their empirical results, was that an unexpected act would be relatively well remembered, but only if it was explained with reference to causal information about the actor. On the other hand, if the unexpected action was explained with reference to the situation in which it occurred, it was not better remembered than an expected act. Hastie (1980) has suggested that attribution processes involve information seeking both in the environment and in memory. He suggested that the review of relevant information in memory would also produce associative links between new information (e.g., an unexpected action by an actor) and old information already stored in memory (e.g., past actions of the actor). Thus, causal reasoning would produce relatively richly linked constellations of memory traces concerning an individual or event that was central in the attribution process. Experiment 1 in the present study attempts to test three hypotheses. A direct measure of attributional processing at the time an event is perceived is used to measure the occurrence of attributional thinking, and two hypotheses concerning the conditions instigating attribution will be tested: (a) Unexpected or incongruent actions of an actor are likelier to elicit attributional processing than expected or congruent actions of the actor, (b) Socially unacceptable, evaluatively negative actions of CAUSAL ATTRIBUTION an actor will elicit attributional processing to a greater extent than socially acceptable actions. The effect of attributional processing on memory for information about an actor will be studied by asking subjects, at the end of the experimental session, to recall all the information that has been presented about each actor. The third hypothesis under test is that events that receive attributional processing at acquisition will be better recalled than events that do not elicit causal reasoning. Experiment 1 Method Overview. Subjects were shown descriptions of behaviors performed by hypothetical characters. For each phrase describing a behavior, the subject was asked to write a short continuation of the phrase in his or her own words. Subjects had been informed that the continuation task was part of a study of grammatical usage in extemporaneous writing. After performing the continuation task for several characters, the subjects were surprised by a request to recall as many of the behavior description phrases as a they could remember. A measure of subjects' likelihood II of attempting to explain an action was obtained by having if judges rate the subjects' written continuations for explan/1 atory content. Memory measures were obtained from the 'I recall task. Subjects. The subjects were 24 undergraduate university students. They were paid $1.50 each for their participation in the experiment. Approximately equal numbers of male and female subjects participated in all of the research reported in this article. In no case was there a statistically reliable effect of the sex variable on measures of memory or on the causal attribution measure. Design. Each subject studied, wrote continuation phrases,.and recalled sentences from six lists. Each list contained behavior descriptions that were attributed to a single hypothetical character. Three list lengths were studied, a three-description list, a six-description list, and a nine-description list, and subjects saw two lists of each length. Each list was associated with a personality trait adjective (e.g., friendly, intelligent, dishonest), md within each list two thirds of the behavior descriptions were congruent with the trait adjective and one third were incongruent. One half of the lists were associated with a congruent personality trait that was evaluatively positive or good, the other half with a trait that was evaluatively negative or bad. Twelve personality traits were used to construct the lists and this set was divided into two six-trait sets creating two replications Of a basic experimental design. Within each replication, congruent traits (lists) were paired with each of the three possible list lengths (three items, six items, or nine items). This created three sets of List Length X Congruent Trait Pairings (defined by six-description trait sets) within each of the two replications. Within each list-length/congruent-trait cell of the design four subjects studied lists from that cell; the order of presentation of the lists for each subject was scrambled at random. Thus, there were three between-subjects factors 47 in the experimental design: replication (two levels), listlength/congruent-trait pairings (three levels), and list order (nested within pairing, four levels). The within-subject design also included three factors: trait valence (good vs. bad), list length (three, six, or nine items), and item type (congruent vs. incongruent). Furthermore, care was taken to see that each item type occurred equally often in all possible serial positions within a list, Materials. The experimental plan required sets of sentences describing behaviors characteristic of a representative sample of personality traits. Twelve traits were chosen from the 80 traits studied by Rosenberg and Sedlak (1972) in their multidimensional scaling analysis. These 12 traits were combined to form six pairs of "opposite-meaning" traits: intelligent-unintelligent, honest-liar, conscientiousirresponsible, friendly-hostile, shy-aggressive, and naivecynical. The pairs were presented to 12 pretest subjects with an instruction of the form, "consider a person who is very intelligent; you would expect to see him (her). . ." The resulting lists of pretest subject-generated behaviors were edited to yield three- to five-word behavior descriptions for each of the traits. Some examples of behaviors generated as congruent with the intelligent trait are "won the chess tournament" and "attended the symphony concert." All behavior descriptions were written to describe specific, easy-to-picture events. Each list of behaviors associated- with a trait included two-thirds congruent behaviors and one-third incongruent behaviors. Incongruent behaviors for each list were selected by sampling from the set of behaviors associated with the trait opposite in meaning to the trait for the congruent behaviors. Thus, for example, incongruent behaviors for the intelligent trait were selected to be congruent with the unintelligent trait. The list-generation plan counterbalanced congruent and incongruent traits, thus providing a control for idiosyncratic characteristics of particular trait-related sentences. Congruent sentences for one character were presented as incongruent sentences for other characters to subjects in the other replication of the between-subjects design. Procedure. Experimental sessions lasted approximately 40 min. Subjects were tested in groups of from two to five participants. They were instructed that the experiment was part of a research program studying psycholinguistics. Their task would be to generate extemporaneous continuations of short phrases that would be, analyzed to determine their grammatical structures. They were also instructed that the written materials would be comprised of sentences describing behaviors of a series of hypothetical characters and that they were to attempt to form a clear personality impression of each character before spontaneously generating a sentence continuation for each behavior description. The instructions indicated that, just as in real life, the characters described by the experimental sentences might not be perfectly consistent. However, the subjects' task was to attempt to form as clear an impression as possible and then to write the continuations. When each of the six lists of sentences was presented, subjects were instructed to perform two tasks: (a) read through the set of three, six, or nine sentences to form an impression of the character, and (b) write a short continuation of each sentence starting with the first sentence and proceeding through the end of the list. The experimenter paced subjects through these two tasks by in-, strutting them when to read all descriptions to form an impression (30 s) and when to write each sentence con- 48 REID HASTIE tinuation (20 s per sentence). Each list was presented on a single page of a six-page booklet and included a linedrawn portrait of the hypothetical character, the character's name (selected from a list of six common male American names), and an occupation label (e.g., attorney, engineer), as well as the list of sentences, each followed by a space for the subject's continuation. After completing the sentence continuation task for each of the six characters, the subject was given another booklet of six pages and each page was headed with one character's name, picture, and occupation label. At this time, subjects were instructed to recall as many of the sentence descriptions as they could in any order in which they occurred to them. They were to write these sentences on the appropriate recall page for each character. Subjects were given 18 min to perform the recall task. When the recall task was concluded subjects were debriefed, paid, and dismissed from the experiment. Results The results will be presented in three parts: analyses of data summarizing the contents of subjects' written continuations of behavior description phrases, data summarizing subjects' recall performance, and statistics summarizing the relationship between recall and continuation measures. Sentence continuation contents.. Subjects' written continuations averaged seven words in length. Less than 2% of the continuations were missing or illegible and no continuations included more than one explanation. Judges, comparable in age and background to the subjects, read the written continuations of each behavior description and classified each continuation into one of three coding categories: (a) An explanation answered the question, "Why was the act performed?" (b) An elaboration answered the question, "What were the circumstances when the act occurred?" (c) A temporal succession answered the question, "What happened after the act occurred?" The judges were further instructed that when they were uncertain as to whether an action was or was not an explanation, they should classify it as an explanation. If their uncertainty concerned classification in the elaboration or temporal succession categories they were to call it an elaboration. The three types of continuations did not differ reliably in length. If we consider the behavior description, "won the chess tournament," we can see examples of each of the three types of continuation: explanation, ". . . because he had studied the game for five years"; elaboration, ". . . in the YMCA gameroom"; and temporal succession, ". . . and celebrated his victory with his girlfriend." Agreement among the three judges who performed the coding task was high, with percent agreement statistics all above 80%. Initial efforts to make fine discriminations among types of attributions (person vs. situation) or explanations (causal antecedent, goal orientation, etc.; see Lehnert, 1978) appearing in subjects' continuations were thwarted by relatively low levels of interrater agreement. Overall, 24% of the continuations were classified as explanations, 69% as elaborations, and 7% as temporal successions. The focus of the present theoretical analysis was on the probability that a behavior description would be continued with an explanation. The proportion of descriptions continued by explanations is the dependent variable in Figure 2. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) showed that only one effect reached conventional levels of statistical significance; this was a test of the difference between proportion explained for congruent versus incongruent items, F( 1,18) = 8.12, p < .01, MSe = . 131. An incongruent act was likelier to elicit an explanation as a continuation than was a congruent act (18 of the 24 subjects were likelier to continue an incongruent item with an explanation as compared with a congruent item). This finding is consistent with findings obtained by Wong and Weiner (1981), Pyszczynski and Greenberg (1981), and Lau and Russell (1980). Recall performance. Subjects had been instructed to recall the behavior descriptions presented by the experimenter (not their own continuations of those phrases). First, a lenient scoring criterion was employed: An item was scored as correctly recalled if the general sense of the description was preserved (i.e., any changes from the original were substitutions of synonymous words). The data to be reported in this article were tabulated following this criterion. Second, a strict scoring criterion was adopted: An item was scored as correctly recalled if it differed from the original in at most one important word (and if this deviation did not affect the meaning of the phrase). Analyses were repeated with this criterion, and the general pattern of the results remained the same as for the lenient criterion. However, application of the strict criterion resulted in very 49 CAUSAL ATTRIBUTION low memory performance; frequently the overall proportion recalled for a cell in the experimental design fell below. 10. Thus, some of the effects of independent variable factors were no longer significant in an analysis on the strict recall criterion data. The proportions of behavior descriptions recalled are displayed in Figure 2, broken down by list length (three-, six-, or nine-item lists), good or bad evaluation of the congruent trait, and congruent or incongruent item within the list. Overall, the relevant analyses reproduce effects reported in earlier work on person memory. There is a significant difference between the proportions of congruent and incongruent items recalled, with congruent items being recalled at a lower level than incongruent items, F(\, 18) = 11.38,p< .003, MS; = .650 (19 of the 24 subjects recalled a greater proportion of incongruent than congruent items). This effect is a replication of similar effects reported by Hastie and Kumar (1979), Srull (1981), and others. Incongruent item recall tended to be higher than congruent item recall when set sizes were unequal such that incon- gruent items were less frequent than congruent items (as they were in all of the present experimental materials). There is a second effect of list length such that items are likelier to be recalled if they were presented in shorter lists than if they were presented in longer lists, F(2, 36) = 4.98, p < .01, MSe = .109. This result is probably also best interpreted as a set size or list length effect familiar from research on verbal learning (Murdock, 1960;Tulving&Pearlstone, 1966). A final effect of theoretical interest, the comparison between recall for behaviors attributed to a good person and behaviors attributed to a bad person did not reach conventional levels of statistical significance, F(l, 18) = 1.68, ns, MSe = .215. A further exploration of the good-bad difference examined the relative recall of behavior descriptions of bad acts as compared to good acts. This compared, for example, congruent acts attributed to a good character and incongruent acts attributed to a bad character (both good acts) with congruent acts attributed to a bad character and incongruent acts attributed to a good .40 ,10 s 3 ,30 ,30 ,20 ,20 2 § a. T 2/1 1/2 T 6/3 2/1 4/2 6/3 LIST TYPE ( NO. CONGRUENT ACTS / NO, INCONGRUENT ACTS ) Figure 2. Recall memory and sentence continuation task results from Experiment 1. (Open circles represent results for incongruent items and filled circles for congruent items.) 50 REID HASTIE character (both bad acts). This comparison of good to bad behavior descriptions (.32 vs. .29 proportions of acts recalled, respectively) also failed to reveal a reliable good-bad difference. Relationship between recall and explanation. We have now demonstrated an effect of expectedness of information on both recall of the information and attributional reasoning concerning the information. Hastie (1980; see also Hastie & Kumar, 1979) has suggested that there is a causal relationship between attributional processing and memory for information. One condition for the truth of the hypothesis that causal attribution has a causal influence on memory for information is that there be a correlation between the memory and attribution measures in the present experiment. To test this hypothesis a correlational index, the phi-coefficient (Siegel, 1956), was calculated for each list for each subject in the experiment (six phi-coefficients per subject). Within a list, each behavior description was classified into a four-cell matrix with explained/not explained and recalled/not recalled as marginal categories. Thus, the phicoefficient summarized the correlational relationship between explanation and recall for each list. These phi-coefficients were entered as the dependent variable in an ANOVA to determine whether the coefficient was significantly different from zero. Although the overall average for the phi-coefficient was small in magnitude (+.15), it was significantly different from zero, F(l, 18) = 17.56, p < .001, MS"e = .02. None of the independent variables in the experimental design had a statistically significant effect on the phi-coefficient measure. In summary, Experiment 1 has yielded several results with theoretical and methodological significance. First, we have replicated the finding that incongruent behavior descriptions, when relatively infrequent in a list of behavior descriptions, are better recalled than congruent behavior descriptions. Second, in the present experiment this result has been obtained under conditions where the list was not preceded by an ensemble of congruent trait adjectives and under conditions where the subjects did not expect to have their memory tested. Recall instructions were presented only after all lists had been presented and without an earlier warning instruction. Third, an examination of the continuations generated by subjects at the time lists were studied demonstrated that attributional or causal reasoning was likelier to occur for incongruent items than for congruent items within lists of behavior descriptions attributed to a single character. Fourth, a correlation was found between the probability of explanation and probability of recall measures. This correlation is consistent with the hypothesis that causal attribution influences recall. However, the result is as yet only correlational and does not strongly support an argument for causal influence. Experiment 2 was designed to provide a stronger test of the relationship between causal explanation and recall. Experiment 2 Method Overview. Subjects, materials, and procedure were similar to those employed in Experiment 1 with one major alteration of the instructions to subjects. In Experiment 2 subjects were not allowed to extemporaneously generate continuations for each behavior description. Rather, the experimenter provided a written instruction preceding each behavior description that indicated which type of continuation was to be generated by the subject. Thus, type of continuation was manipulated by the experimenter and became the focal independent variable in the experimental design. An effect of continuation instruction on recall of behavior descriptions would establish the causal priority of attributional reasoning on memory. Subjects. As in Experiment 1, 24 undergraduate university students served as subjects in the experiment and were paid for their participation. Design. Each subject studied, wrote continuation phrases, and recalled 'sentences from six lists. Each list contained nine behavior descriptions that were attributed to a single hypothetical character, Six of the descriptions within each list were consistent with one personality trait adjective, and three descriptions were consistent with an opposite-meaning trait adjective (incongruent items). Within the set of six congruent descriptions in a list; two items were preceded by an instruction code indicating that the subject was to continue each of those items with an explanation phrase, two were preceded by an instruction code indicating that an elaboration continuation was required, and two were preceded by a code indicating that a succession continuation was required. For the three incongruent descriptions, one was preceded by the explanation continuation code, one by the elaboration continuation code, and one by the succession continuation code. Thus, for each list and for each item type (congruent or incongruent) within the list explanation, elaboration, and succession continuations were generated by subjects. As in Experiment 1, one half of the lists presented to a subject were associated with a congruent personality trait that was evaluatively positive and the other half with a trait that was evaluatively negative. Again, as in Experiment 1, 12 personality traits were used to construct the 51 CAUSAL ATTRIBUTION lists and this set was divided into two six-trait replications. Within each replication, counterbalancing plans ensured that lists for each (congruent) trait occurred equally often in each of the six presentation positions during an experimental session, that continuation instructions were paired with specific behavior descriptions in a random fashion, and that descriptions were randomly ordered within a list attributed to a character in two different random sequences across all subjects in the experimental design. Thus, there were three between-subjects factors in the experimental design: replication (two levels), continuation-instruction/behavior-description assignment (two levels), and list order (nested within pairing, six levels). The within-subject design included four factors: congruent trait evaluation (good vs. bad), congruent trait (three levels; dependent on replication), continuation instruction (explain, elaborate, or succeed), and item type (congruent vs. incongruent). Materials. The materials used in Experiment 2 were identical to those used in Experiment 1. The sole change was the use of nine-description lists only. Procedure. There was only one change from the procedure used in Experiment 1. Subjects were instructed that each behavior description phrase would be preceded by a single capitalized word to indicate the type of continuation they were to produce for that phrase. Subjects were given the following instructions: The task you are to perform involves reading each action description and writing a continuation for each one. A continuation is a short phrase (written by you) that provides a plausible extension of the original typed description. In the experiment you will be asked to write three types of continuations, "EXPLAIN," "SUCCEED," and "ELABORATE." The type of continuation which you are to write for an item is indicated in capital letters before the item. Try to use your imagination and write a continuation that is satisfying to you for each item. An "EXPLAIN" continuation is one which tells why an action occurred; that is, it gives a reason for the action. (Examples of each kind of continuation follow these instructions.) A "SUCCEED" continuation is one which describes activity which might follow the behavior or occur after it in time. An "ELABORATE" continuation would give more details or further description of the behavior and its setting. You should feel free to use varied and imaginative continuations and should write continuations that make sense to you for each item. Other instructions for the continuation task were identical to those presented in Experiment 1, and the instructions for the recall task were identical as well. Results The dependent variable of interest in Experiment 2 is the probability of recall of items presented under the various instruction sets. These data are summarized in Figure 3. First, there is a clear main effect of item type with incongruent items (set size three out of nine) .3D ,10 EXPLAIN ELABORATE SUCCEED CONTINUATION INSTRUCTION Figure 3. Recall memory task results from Experiment 2. (Unfilled bars represent results for congruent items and crosshatched bars represent results for incongruent items.) recalled with a higher probability than congruent items, J^l, 20) = 10.21, p< .01, MSe = .16 (16 of 24 subjects recalled a greater proportion of incongruent than congruent items). There is also a main effect for the continuation instruction variable, F(2,40) = 8.35, p < .001, MSe = .23, with items continued by an explanation best remembered and items continued with an elaboration or succession phrase less well remembered (15 of the 24 subjects achieved their highest level of recall for items that were continued with an explanation). Figure 3 shows an apparent interaction pattern between the item type and continuation instruction factors such that there is almost no difference in the-relative probability of recall of a congruent or incongruent item under exr planation continuation instructions but quite a large difference between congruent and incongruent item recall under succession instructions. However, this interaction effect did not reach conventional levels of statistical significance, F(2, 40) = 1.93, ns, MSe = .19. Again, as in Experiment 1, no effect of evaluative valence (good or bad items) was found. The simple hypothesis that continuation 52 REID HASTIE type would have an effect on probability of recall is clearly supported. Furthermore, the prediction that a causal continuation would provide for a more retrievable memory trace than other types of continuation was also confirmed. Experiment 3 A control experiment was conducted to determine whether or not the continuation task required different amounts of time under the different continuation instructions. The experiment was relevant to issues raised by a concern that total time of stimulus exposure (during the sentence continuation task) would be correlated with the type of continuation required and would serve as an explanatory construct to account for subsequent performance on the recall test. We thought it was important to explore the implications of the simple total-time account for recall results before considering alternate accounts in terms of type of processing. We will not present a thorough outline of the Method section for this experiment because the results are straightforward and doubtless quite general. Twelve undergraduate student subjects were asked to perform the continuation task on six lists each containing 18 behavior descriptions. Each list contained a majority of 12 (congruent) items and 6 (incongruent) items. For each list subjects were asked to explain, elaborate, and write temporal successions for some of the descriptions. A majority (12) of these continuation instructions were from one of the three types of continuation. This meant that for each subject two of the lists included instructions to explain 12 of the items, elaborate 3 of the items, and write temporal successions for 3 of the items. Similarly, two of the lists included a majority of the elaborate instructions, and two a majority of the succession instructions. For each subject, total time to complete the list could serve as a term in an algebraic equation, and three equations could be set up—one for the explain-, one for the elaborate-, and one for the succession-majority condition. With three equations and three unknowns (time taken to perform the explanation, elaboration, and succession continuations), it was possible to solve for the unknowns yielding estimates of the time to write one explanation, one elaboration, and one succession for each subject. These estimates—time taken to complete an explanation, an elaboration, or a succession continuation—were entered as dependent variables in an ANOVA. The mean estimates were quite close to one another (23.9 s, 25.3 s, and 20.8 s, respectively), and the ANOVA did not yield a significant effect of continuation type, F(2, 33) = 1.72, ns, MSe = 36.76. Thus, there is no empirical evidence that total time to write the continuations was responsible for differences in memorability of items subjected to the three types of continuation. General Discussion The major contribution of the present research is to our understanding of the relationships between the occurrence of unexpected events, causal reasoning, and memory. Previous research by the author and other psychologists (e.g., Hastie et al., 1980) introduced and popularized the paradigm used in the present experiments. In brief, the paradigm involves setting subjects an impression formation or social judgment task, presenting information about a target character in the form of trait attributions and sentences describing the character's actions, and testing the subjects' memory for information about the character. One phenomenon obtained using this paradigm that has received considerable attention in the social memory literature is the relatively high recall of unexpected or incongruent behavior descriptions that can be observed under some experimental conditions (e.g., Crocker et al., 1983; Hamilton, Katz, & Leirer, 1980; Hastie & Kumar, 1979; Hemsley & Marmurek, 1982;Srull, 1981; Wyer& Gordon, 1982). The present empirical results make a significant contribution to our understanding of this phenomenon by strongly implying that causal reasoning, instigated by the occurrence of an unexpected event, is one determinant of subsequent superior recall of that event. The present results reinforce earlier conclusions that the occurrence of an unexpected event elicits causal reasoning. There are certain conditions in the present experimental paradigm that are probably necessary to produce this result. First, subjects are reasoning about social entities, and it is normal and perhaps CAUSAL ATTRIBUTION even adaptive to consider another person's motives, intentions, or circumstances when their actions surprise us. In contrast, we would not expect subjects to react to the occurrence of a surprising word presented in a list of other common English words by reasoning about the causal properties of that word. Second, subjects were instructed to study the information presented about each hypothetical character with the goal of integrating that information to form a sensible impression of the target character. Two aspects of this instruction are probably important in producing the incongruity-memory effect: (a) forming an impression by integrating (not discounting) all available information, and (b) making sense out of information about an individual person (rather than a group, or other less integral individual). Third, the pacing of the experimental continuation task was relatively leisurely, allowing subjects to optionally allocate their attention to various aspects of the stimulus materials or to alternative strategic subtasks. Fourth, the nature of the incongruity, defined with reference to the relationships between behaviors and personality traits, was dramatic and obvious. The incongruent items were always in a minority in the full lists of descriptions attributed to a character and they were inconsistent with the majority items and the personality traits on both evaluative and descriptive grounds. Pretests of experimental materials and tasks in our own laboratory and communications with other colleagues suggest that all of these conditions taken jointly may be necessary to produce the moderately high rates of causal reasoning observed in our experiments. Our failure to observe differential rates of causal reasoning for evaluatively good versus bad behavior descriptions suggests that one type of unexpectedness, violations of the norm that socially undesirable acts occur with relatively low frequencies in everyday life, does not instigate causal reasoning. Some researchers (Kanouse & Hanson, 1972) have speculated that socially undesirable acts that are relatively infrequent are more informative and have relatively large impacts on social judgments. If informativeness is taken in an information theory sense, it should be related to how surprising or unexpected an act is. However, no relationship was obtained between how sur- 53 prising a socially desirable or undesirable act is and the occurrence of causal reasoning. The fact that exactly half of the presented behavior descriptions were socially undesirable, an unnaturally high proportion, may limit the generality of this conclusion. In defense of the generality of the present results to naturally occurring situations in which a person reviews his or her memories of casual acquaintances, we would cite the variety of conditions under which similar results have been obtained. The basic memory result—high probability of recall of a small set of impression-incongruent events—has been obtained (a) in dozens of experiments using various list lengths and impression types, (b) for trait and behavior descriptions, (c) when presented in auditory, visual, or film modes, and (d) when acquired under incidental or intentional learning conditions (e.g., Hastie, 1980; Hastie & Kumar, 1979; Srull, 1981). Current research in our laboratory using staged interactions between true subjects and the experimenter's confederates has also obtained similar results. At present we do not have evidence for the instigation of causal reasoning outside of the present research and the results cited in the introduction to the present article. Nonetheless, these results have been obtained in a wide variety of social judgment tasks. Taken together, all of these findings provide a strong prima facie case for the generality of the present conclusions to at least some analogous natural social-acquaintance situations. The notion that unexpected events elicit causal reasoning appears to be conceptually the most basic of the conditions that have been hypothesized to instigate causal reasoning, in the sense that other specific conditions (with the possible exception of outcome dependence) may be seen as subordinate to the unexpectedness condition. Furthermore, empirical research has provided the most direct and substantial evidence for the role of the unexpectedness condition in instigating causal reasoning. Some philosophical analyses of the perception of causation (e.g., Hart & Honore, 1959; Mackie, 1974) have also emphasized the significance of unexpectedness or deviation from a normal course of events as a condition for causal reasoning and for the identification of causes. One speculation, consistent with these philosophical analyses, is that causal rea- 54 REID HASTIE soning is elicited by changes in the state of the world, particularly when these changes are unexpected. Thus, perhaps it is more reasonable to talk of to-be-explained deviations or changes in conditions rather than to-be-explained events. The implication is that when a social perceiver attempts to explain a state of affairs, both the current state and its conceivable alternatives taken together define the to-be-explained effect. Furthermore, expectations about other normal states of the world will determine which factors are selected as causal or explanatory events. As the philosophers have put it, "the cause . . . is a difference in the normal course which accounts for the difference in the outcome" (Hart & Honore, 1959, p. 27), or "both cause and effect are seen as differences within a field" (Mackie, 1974, p. 35). The second major contribution of the present research is to characterize the role of causal reasoning in social memory tasks. The results are consistent with the outline of an information processing model introduced by Hastie and Kumar (1979; see also Hastie, 1980) and extended by Srull (1981). In summary, that theoretical analysis included the following postulates: 1. During the acquisition stage of a memory task (corresponding to the stage of the experiment during which subjects generated continuations for behavior-description sentences in the present experiments), prepositional representations of behaviors attributed to each hypothetical character are formed in memory. 2. If the subject is attempting to form an impression of the hypothetical character to whom the behavior descriptions are attributed, behaviors that are unexpected or incongruent with reference to this impression will receive special attention. This attention may take the form of special processing, for example, attempts to explain why the surprising or unexpected act was performed or simply additional processing. The product of the extra processing or special processing is an increased number of associative, links between the incongruent behavior and other behaviors attributed to that character. 3. During the retention interval, following the acquisition task, forgetting occurs as associative links between propositions and within propositions are lost at the same rate for in- congruent acts and for other acts attributed to the character. 4. During the retrieval stage, corresponding to the point in the present experiments at which the subject is instructed to attempt to recall as much information as possible about each character, a search process occurs in which the subject examines information in memory by tracing down pathways in the network of associations linking information. The probability of retrieving a particular behavior description is directly related to the number of associative paths that link that behavior to other information about the character. Ineongruent events, by virtue of their longer or deeper processing, will be linked to other information about a character stored in memory. Thus, they will be likelier to be recalled than behaviors that were not incongruent at the time of study. Other cognitive psychologists have hypothesized that unexpected or atypical events will be processed differently than unsurprising events and that memory for the unexpected will be superior to memory for the mundane. Graesser and Nakamura (1982) have suggested that atypical events will be linked to relevant knowledge structures such as scripts (Abelson, 1981) by distinctive associative links called tags. Tagged events will be especially well remembered and discriminated from similar but nonoccurring atypical items on recall and recognition tests. Schank (1982) has stated similar hypotheses about memory processes and structures. He argued that learning that occurs after expectations are disconfirmed, "failuredriven memory," is the most important memory process in everyday life. The present experiments were not designed to discriminate between models that hypothesize distinctive tags and models that hypothesize elaboration of associative links to explain the relative high recall of incongruent behaviors. Both types of accounts are plausible for the present results. The results of the present experiments support predictions that incongruent events will be likelier to receive special processing during acquisition in the form of causal attribution and that these events will be better recalled than events that are not incongruent. The results of the third control experiment reported in the present article imply that the increased memory for an event that is asso- CAUSAL ATTRIBUTION elated with causally explaining why the event occurred does not require more processing time than other types of memory encoding or elaboration processes. Our speculation is that causal reasoning leads to the creation of many links between items in memory but without requiring extra processing time for this elaboration to occur. Bradshaw and Anderson (1982) apply a similar interpretation to the effects of causal reasoning on memory trace structure. Final conclusions concerning causal reasoning and person memory can be summarized succinctly. When unexpected behaviors are attributed to a person, the perceiver is relatively likely to engage in causal reasoning to understand why these behaviors occurred. One product of this causal reasoning is a more elaborate memory representation for the unexpected information and more links between the memory trace for that event and other traces in memory, leading to relatively high levels of recall for information about unexpected events. References Abelson, R. P. (1981). Psychological status of the script concept. American Psychologist, 36, 715-729. Berscheid, E., Graziano, W., Monson, X, & Dertner, M. (1976). Outcome dependency: Attention, attribution, and attraction. Journal of Personality and Social PsyL chohgy, 34, 978-989. / Black, J. B., & Bern, H. (1981). Causal coherence and \ memory for events in narratives. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 267-275. Black, J. B., & Bower, G. H. (1980). Story understanding f~ .as problem solving. Poetics, 9, 223-250. / Bower, G. H., & Masting, M. (1980). Causal explanations ( as mediators for remembering correlations. Unpublished ^— manuscript, Stanford University. Bradshaw, G. L., & Anderson, J. R. (1982). Elaborative encoding as an explanation of levels of processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 165174. Clary, E. G., & Tesser, A. (in press). Reaction to discrepant information: The naive scientist and plausible alternative explanations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. Crocker, J., Binns, D., & Weber, R. (1983). Person memory and causal attributions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 55-66.' Diener, C. T, & Dweck, C. S. (1978). An analysis of learned helplessness: Continuous changes in performance, strategy, and achievement cognitions following failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 451-462. Enzle, M. E., & Schopflocher, D. (1978). Instigation of attribution processes by attributional questions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 595-599. Fisher, R. P., & Craik, R I. M. (1980). The effects of 55 elaboration on recognition memory. Memory & Cognition, 8, 400-404. Graesser, A.C., & Nakamura, G. V. (1982). The impact of a schema on comprehension and memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 16, pp. 60-109). New York: Academic Press. Graesser A. C., Robertson, S. P., Lovelace, E. R,, & Swinehart, D. M. (1980). Answers to why-questions expose " the organization of story plot and predict recall of actions. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 110-119. Hamilton, D. L., Katz, L. B., & Leirer, V. O. (1980). Organizational processes in impression formation. In R. Hastie, T. M. Ostrom, E. B. Ebbesen, R. S. Wyer, Jr., D. L. Hamilton, & D. E. Carlston (Eds.), Person memory: The cognitive basis of social perception (pp. 121-153). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hart, H. L. A,, & Honore, A. M. Causation in the law. London: Oxford University Press, 1959. Harvey, J. H., Yarkin, K. L., Lightner, J. M., & Town, J. P. (1980). Unsolicited interpretation and recall of interpersonal events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 551-568. Hastie, R. (1980). Memory for behavioral information that confirms or contradicts a personality impression. In R. Hastie, T. M. Ostrom, E. B. Ebbesen, R. S. Wyer, Jr., D. L. Hamilton, & D. E. Carlston (Eds.), Person memory: The cognitive basis of social perception (pp.' 155-177). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hastie, R., & Kumar, P. A. (1979). Person memory: Personality traits as organizing principles in memory for behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 25-38. Hastie, R., Ostrom, T. M., Ebbesen, E. B., Wyer, R. S., Jr., Hamilton, D. L., & Carlston, D. E. (Eds.). (1980). Person memory: The cognitive basis of social perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley. Hemsley, G. D., & Marmurek, H. H. C. (1982). Person memory: The processing of consistent and inconsistent person information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 433-438. Kanouse, D. E., & Hanson, L. R. (1972). Negativity in evaluations. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 47-62). Morris,town, NJ: General Learning. Lau, R. R., & Russell, D. (1980), Attributions in the sports pages: A field test of some current hypotheses in attribution research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 29-38. Lehnert, W. (1978). The process of answering questions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Mackie, J. L.. (1974). Cement of the universe. London: Oxford University Press. Manis, M. (1977). Cognitive social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 3, 550-566. Monson, T. C, Keel, R., Stephens, D., & Genung, V. (1982). Trait attributions: Relative validity, covariation with behavior, and prospect of future interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 1014-1024. Murdock, B. B., Jr. (1960). The immediate retention of unrelated words. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 222-234. 56 REID HASTIE Pyszczynski, T. A., & Greenberg, J. (1981). Role of disconfirmed expectancies in the instigation of attributional processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 31-38. Rosenberg, S., & Sedlak, A. (1972). Structural representations of implicit personality theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 235-297). New York: Academic Press. Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory. New York: Cambridge University Press. Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Sherman, R. C., & Titus, W. (1982). Covariation infor-J, mation and cognitive processing: Effects of causal im-f plications on memory. Journal of Personality andSoda| Psychology, 42, 989-1000. Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistics for the behavion sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill. Smith, E. R., & Miller, F. D. (1979). Salience and the \ cognitive mediation of attribution. Journal of Personality \ and Social Psychology, 37, 2240-2252. Srull, T. K. (1981). Person memory: Some tests of associative storage and retrieval models. Journal of Exper- imental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 440-563. Stevenson, R. J. (1981). Depth of comprehension, effective elaboration, and memory for sentences. Memory & Cognition, 9, 169-176. Tulving, E., & Pearlstone, Z. (1966). Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 381391. Wong, P. T. P., & Weiner, B. (1981). When people ask "why" questions, and the heuristics of attributional search. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 650-663. Wyer R. S., Jr., & Gordon, S. E. (1982). The recall of information about persons and groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 128-164. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151175. Received December 30, 1982 Revision received June 23, 1983 Search Opens for Editor of Contemporary Psychology The Publications and Communications Board has opened nominations for the editorship of Contemporary Psychology for the years 1986-1991. Donald Foss is the incumbent editor. Candidates must be members of APA and should be available to start receiving and processing books for review in early 1985 to prepare for issues published in 1986. To nominate candidates, prepare a statement of one page or less in support of each candidate. Submit nominations no later than February 1, 1984, to: Anne D. Pick ATTN: Journal Search Committee Institute for Child Development University of Minnesota 51 East River Road Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455