Uploaded by KPODO GODWIN_ wan hive

ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR

advertisement
NAME: KPODO GODWIN YAO SETSOAFIA
INDEX NUMBER: UEB3707618
COURSE: ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR
ASSIGNMENT 1
BIRDS
In 1951 Gustav Kramer discovered the sun compass. He performed his experiments by placing
European Starlings in orientation cages and then used mirrors to shift the apparent location of the
sun. In response, the birds shifted their migratory restlessness to match the compass direction
indicated by the apparent new position of the sun.
Further research revealed that the bird’s sun compass is tied to its circadian rhythm. It seems
birds have a time compensation ability to make allowances for changes in the sun’s position over
the course of the day. This theory is supported by another experiment in which pigeons were
placed in a closed room with an altered cycle of light and dark. Over a period of a few days, their
circadian rhythm was reset. The birds were then released on a sunny day. Because their “internal
clock” had been reset, they misinterpreted the position of the sun and made a predictable error in
their homing direction. The pigeons actually ignore the position of the sun relative to its position
in the sky, relying on its azimuth direction, i.e., the compass direction at which a vertical line
from the sun intersects the horizon.
Further study has also revealed that pigeons have to learn the sun’s path to use it in navigation.
Young pigeons allowed to see the sun only in the morning lack the ability to use the sun for
navigation in the afternoon.
Birds can determine the compass direction from the sun (or from sun-related cues like the
skylight polarization pattern) by compensating, through their circadian clock sense, for the sun’s
apparent daily movement in azimuth.
One possibility, aside from the escape response documented here, is that the sun plays a similar
role in homing and migratory behavior as it does in birds. Birds use geomagnetic cues during
migration over long distances, where the sun’s arc (and the compensation schedule) would
constantly change, but use a sun compass for orientation over shorter distances, such as the
directed movement within a familiar home range where the azimuthal changes over the course of
a day can be learned. Wiltschko and Wiltschko (2009) hypothesize that many birds actually learn
two compensatory schedules for changes in the sun azimuth: one for foraging and/or breeding
sites in the temperate zone, and another for overwintering areas in the tropics. This hypothesis
may also apply to both terrestrial and marine turtles that for at least some extended periods
occupy familiar home ranges (Makowski et al., 2006). Birds migrating by day use the Sun to
navigate, adjusting their angle to the Sun as the Sun’s position moves from east to west.
Some birds, like robins, use Earth’s magnetic field to assist in migration. It is believed that they
have magnetic crystals near their nostrils to help them sense the field and orient themselves.
Birds also use landmarks such as islands, trees, and buildings, as well as sounds and smells,
when they search for nesting grounds in spring.
MONARCH BUTTERFLIES
Monarch butterflies navigate by a sun compass, meaning they use the position of the sun to
determine which way is south. Monarch butterflies use the Earth’s magnetic field and a ‘sun
compass’ in their antenna as navigational tools for their long-distance migration, scientists say.
Monarchs use a time-compensated sun compass in their antenna to help them make their longdistance migratory journey to overwintering sites. Monarchs use a light-dependent inclination
magnetic compass to help them orient southward during migration.
Monarch butterflies have a keen sense of direction, even on cloudy days. This is because they
have a magnetic compass to direct their migration in addition to navigating by the position of the
sun.
The magnetic compass was ultraviolet light-sensitive and an essential orientation mechanism to
aid migration when directional daylight cues are unavailable. The monarchs are using the sun as
a celestial cue.
Since monarchs migrate during the day, the sun is the celestial cue most likely to be useful in
pointing the way to the overwintering sites. This proposed mechanism is called a sun compass.
Monarchs may use the angle of the sun along the horizon in combination with an internal body
clock (like a circadian rhythm) to maintain a southwesterly flight path. The way this would work
is illustrated below. For example, if a monarch’s internal clock reads 10:00 AM, then the
monarch will fly to the west of the sun to maintain a southern flight direction. When the
monarch’s internal clock reads noon (12:00 PM), the monarch’s instincts tell it to fly straight
toward the sun, while later in the day the monarch’s instincts tell it to fly to the east of the sun.
Scientists have suggested that monarchs may use a magnetic compass to orient, possibly in
addition to a sun compass or as a “back-up” orientation guide on cloudy days when they cannot
see the sun. Studies of migratory birds have indicated that they register the angle made by the
earth’s magnetic field and the surface of the earth. These angles point south in the Northern
Hemisphere and north in the Southern Hemisphere.
Migratory monarchs indeed possess a magnetic compass that aids in orienting migrants south
towards their overwintering grounds during fall migration. Remarkably, the use of the magnetic
compass requires short wave UV-light (previous magnetic compass experiments failed to
account for light at this range). With UV-light being allowed to enter the flight simulator, eastern
migratory monarchs consistently oriented themselves south. The light-sensitive magneto sensors
reside in the adult monarch’s antennae. While the expert consensus remains that the sun compass
is the monarch’s primary compass for navigation.
HONEY BEES
Honey bees use the sun as a reference point in navigation and communication. Experiments have
shown that bees have an internal representation of the sun's movement through the sky and
suggest that this representation is innate, but is tailored by experience.
Research into this pattern shows that the bees navigate relative to the sun. Since the sun is so far
from the earth, over a short time, the bee cannot see the sun's motion, even if the bee moves.
(Humans often see a similar effect when driving; distant objects seem to move less than the signs
near the road.) The bee can, therefore, use the sun as a fixed point and orient itself by
maintaining a fixed angle between its line of flight and the line to the sun.
The sun, however, plays a greater role in the food gathering cycle. The dance language, which
bees use to communicate, is also based on the location of the sun. When bees return from a food
source, they perform a ``waggle dance'' on the vertical comb nearest the entrance to the hive. The
dancing bee makes a short, straight run while waggling its abdomen, then circles back and
repeats the action several times. The bee orients its dance so that the angle between the direction
of the straight run and the ray opposite gravity is the same as the angle between the food source
and the position of the sun. In this scheme, dancing straight-up means ``fly toward the sun,'' and
dancing straight down means ``fly away from the sun.'' Given this angle, other bees can orient
themselves to the sun and locate the food source.
The bee, having some innate sense of the sun's movement, flies out to search for food. She has
probably flown a few times before and has refined her sense to great precision. When she finds
food, she returns straight home, and, because she has a sense of the sun's direction, she will be
able to dance at the proper angle to tell the other bees where to fly. Since they too know where
the sun is, even though they may not be able to see it due to cloud cover, they are able to
navigate to the food source.
Honey bees have the ability to detect the Earth’s magnetic field and the suspected
magnetoreceptors are the iron granules in the abdomens of the bees.
Download