Uploaded by desirprus skela

Psyc 216 Exam 2 Study Guide

advertisement
Module 7
Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development
● Piaget
○ First to study/experiment with child cognitive development
○ Tasks that were easy/replicable with children around the world
○ Did systematic observations and experimental testing of children
Children’s perceptual world is not fundamentally different from adults as the
organs needed for perception develop at an early age.
Cognition may be fundamentally different however.
● Paiget’s (created cognitive development) 3 radical claims
○ 1. Constructivist view
■ Children are active agents in learning and knowledge
acquisition (children are not a vessel where society pours
knowledge into (Against Freudian view))
■ Children are not passive entities shaped by rewards and
punishment like balls of clay (Against Skinner view)
○ 2. Children are fundamentally different thinkers than adults (Stage
view)
■ Major qualitative discontinuities in cognitive development
○ 3. No innate cognitive basis to development
■ There are general tendencies for adaptation and
organization that are shared with all other animals
■ Piaget emphasized nurture, not nature, in cognitive
development; as they interact with the world they create their
own cognitive structures
Stage Theory
● Universal sequence of 4 stages (based off tasks children can complete)
■ Sensorimotor (0-2)
● No object permanence
■ Preoperational (2-7)
● Learn to think about things in more symbolic ways,
still egocentric
■ Concrete Operational (7-12) (mental representations are
used but abstract thought is limited)
● Think and reason logically about concrete objects
(Operational thought)
■ Formal Operational (12+) (abstract concepts are acquired)
● Think About abstract concepts and logically test
hypotheses
● Development is parallel (development proceeds at same rate across all
cognitive areas, ex. Preoperational in one = Preoperational in all)
● Development is discontinuous (each stage has qualitatively different
cognitive structures) Example. A tree versus a butterfly
○ Implications are that children undergo a “mental metamorphosis”
Preoperational Stage
Sensorimotor stage
● The infant has intelligence, but is incapable of thought
● The infant perceives and acts on the world in more intelligent ways,
ex. Grasping and throwing objects
● The infant is incapable of sophisticated mental representation (and
hence incapable of thought, lives in the here and now).
Preoperational stage
● Emergence of sophisticated mental representation
○ Mental representation: The capacity to have one thing stand for or
represent another thing
○ Manifestation of mental representations
■ Delayed (deferred) imitation
● Mental images that stand for past events
■ Anticipation in problem solving
● Mental images that stand for future events
■ Pretend play
● Objects and actions that stand for other things. Ex.
dressing up as a frog knowing you are not actually a
frog but a representation of a frog.
■ Language
● Words that stand for objects, people, places, actions,
events, etc.
● Comparing Sensorimotor and Preoperational stage
○ Sensorimotor
■ Only perception and action
■ No representation, no thought
○ Preoperational
■ Mental representation starts, so thoughts begin
● Can remember past
● Anticipate future
● Can imagine a different reality
● Not just a physical world but not a different perception
of that physical world begins
Main achievement of Preoperational Stage
● Main achievement
○ internalization of actions (child learns to carry out (represent)
actions mentally)
● Main limitation
○ Internalized actions are not yet organized in flexible, reversible
systems.
■ Piaget called reversible actions Operations
■ Hence, Preoperational thinking
● Consequences
○ Rigid, inflexible thinking, as shown in a wide range of tasks
How can we test children’s stages?
● Conservation tasks
○ Preoperational children cannot focus on two aspects of a display
simultaneously (centered)
○ Number conservation 5 pennies for child and experimenter, then
asked who has more pennies. However if the experimenter
lengthens the penny lines the child says the experimenter has
more
● Class-inclusion tasks
○ Preoperational children cannot focus on two aspects of a display
simultaneously (centered)
○ More yellow flowers or more flowers
● Seriation tasks
○ Preoperational children cannot focus on two aspects of a display
simultaneously (centered)
○ Children are asked to order sticks in order of length but cannot
● Transitive-inference tasks
○ Jane is taller than sue, sue is taller than anne. Is Jane taller or
shorter than Anne? Preop. Children cannot answer
● Three-mountains task
○ Preop. Children cannot think of the appearance and reality of an
object simultaneously.(perspective taking).
○ Three different colored mounds, child is placed in front of green and
a doll in front of red. Then asked “what does the doll see?” Preop.
Children cannot answer. (Egocentrism)
Piaget Revisited
● Is development actually parallel? (does development proceed at the same
rate across all cognitive areas.
● To test this we would give battery of Piaget’s tasks to 5-10 year olds.
○ See if we get 2 distinct groups: preop should fail, concrete op
should succeed
● In practicality, not as clear as Piaget predicted. For example, children
would fail number conservation and succeed in three mountains and vice
versa.
● Children do not develop in a parallel way
● Is development discontinuous (ladybug and larvae)
○ Answer is no, development is more continuous and gradual.
■ Evidence: modified piagetian tasks, novel tasks
■ Quantifier spreading (modified number conservation),
children might not understand if experimenter is asking for
who has more pennis “number-wise” or “length-wise”, failure
in pragmatic reasoning.
■ McGarrigle and Donaldson
● 4-6 year children, “terrible teddy” comes in and
stretches line accidentally. Then children succeed in
this task.
■ Rose and Blank
● First grades (mean age 6), began with unequal
stretched out lines first, preop. Children began by
judging the 2 sets as equal.
■ Ellen Markman
● 4-5 year olds
● Two conditions: Standard - unit labels - soldiers
● Modified - set labels - army
● My soldiers are your soldiers - fail, my army your
army - Pass
● These findings suggest that children could not understand what the
relevant question was.
● Demand characteristics - the task isn't difficult cognitively but the language
makes it confusing
● Modified piagetian task - visual egocentrism
○ Problem with three mountains task, mountains look the same from
all angles
■ Helene Borke study
● 3-4 year olds
● Modified condition: rich display (house, lake, boat,
animals) as opposed to 3 mountains
● Rationale: with richer display, easier to compute doll’s
perspective..
● This caused kids who failed three mountains task to
pass this modified task
● Conclusions
○ General development appears more gradual or continuous than
Piaget suggested
○ Young children appear more competent that Piaget suggested
○ Researchers became more skeptical of stage view.
● The competent young child
○ To explain the gradual improvement in young childrens
performance in cognitive tasks, researchers evoked
■ Changes in information processing
■ Changes in experience and knowledge acquisition
Module 8
Early physical reasoning: Part 1 - Categorization
● Can infants categorize objects, people, and things? (lump things that are
similar in one umbrella)
○ Critical for knowledge acquisition
○ Quinn, Eimas, and Rosenkrantz study
■ 3 and 4 month olds, can they differentiate between a dog or
a cat?
■ Familiarization trials
● On each trial, 2 color photos of cats OR dogs
● 6 total trials with all different cats OR dogs
○ See 12 cats or dogs total
■ Test trials
● Novel cat and Novel dog photos
● See which one they stare at longer
● Familiarized with cats, stared at dogs longer
● Familiarized with dogs, stared at cats longer
○ Behl-Chadha study
■ Also tested 3-4 month olds using the same method
■ Non-animal categories
● Familiarized with chairs, stared at couch or bed
● Familiarized with couches, stared at chair or bed
○ Superordinate categories (Broader categories)
■ Familiarized to 8 different 4-legged animals, stared at bird,
fish or chair.
● Habituation and Dishabituation
○ Initial presentation creates an orienting response
○ Repeated presentation of same stimuli until boredom (Habituation)
○ Change property of interest and measure looking response
■ Increase in attention = noticed the change (dishabituation)
■ Continued boredom = didn't notice the change
● Arterberry and Bornstein, 2001 study
○ Habituated 3-month olds to either vehicles or animals
○ Then presented with a new stimulus vehicle or animal
○ Research question can infants categorize
○ Found that infants looked longer at an item from a new category
than an item from an old category
● Categorization conclusions
○ Infants are capable of perceptual categorization (categorization
based off perceptual features of the stimulus
○ Infants can categorize at different levels (superordinate vs
subordinate)
Object Permanence
● Piaget was an empiricist
● Piaget believed that children are fundamentally different thinkers than
adults
● Piaget's 3 claims review
○ True - Children are active participants in their own knowledge
acquisition
○ False - Major qualitative discontinuities across development
○ False - No innate cognitive abilities
● Researchers next began to study infants and soon discovered rich
competencies that hinted at innate cognitive abilities
● Core knowledge hypothesis
○ Humans have a rich set of innate cognitive abilities
○ These core abilities, combined with our general ability to remember
and learn, form the foundation for human cognition
● Causal reasoning - making sense of events in the world
○
○
○
○
Physical reasoning (objects and other physical entities)
Psychological reasoning (agents)
Sociomoral reasoning (individuals with moral status)
Guided by an innate skeletal framework
■ Consists of abstract principles and thoughts that help infants
understand the world and reason about events
■ Becomes more elaborate with experience
● Physical reasoning
○ Object permanence
■ Objects are permanent entities in the sense that they still
exist when out of sight. Out of sight, out of mind
● Before 8-9 months old, no object permanence, piaget
would hide toys from kids and see if they search for it
● This finding was easily replicated, and had a huge
impact
■ Over time, researchers began to consider the possibility that
maybe young infants have OP, but have a problem with
search.
■ Test: need non-search task to access OP.
○ New tests of object permanence
■ Violation of Expectation (VoE) method
● Show infants two events: Possible event vs
Impossible event
● See which event they look at longer
● Infants and adults make predictions about upcoming
information as events unfold.
● Assumption: Infants look longer at events they find
surprising or unexpected
● Infants have a set of core expectations about the
world and are surprised when these are violated
● Perez and Feigenson (2019) study
○ 12 months old
○ Violation of Solidity expectation
○ Researchers showed a car on a ramp, for the
possible event the car would stop when it hits a
wall at the bottom of the ramp, impossible
event will be when the car goes through that
wall at the bottom of the ramp
○ Infants looked longer at the impossible
outcome.
■ Then show infants the wall, either a
solid wall or a tunnel wall.
■ If shown solid wall, infants explore the
car more
■ If shown wall with tunnel, infants explore
the ball more
■ Conclusion: infants are looking for an
explanation
● Renee Baillargeon
○ 8 and 6.5 month old infants
○ Used VoE to show infants two events
■ Possible event - consistent with OP
■ Impossible event- non-consistent with OP
■ If infants look longer at impossible events they understand
object permanence
● Habituation event - car with ramp and a screen,
screen goes up showing nothing behind it. Screen
goes down and car goes through screen
● Possible event - car goes through screen and nothing
is behind screen
● Impossible event - car goes through screen but
before, it is shown that there is something blocking
the ramp behind the screen
● Both 8 and 6.5 month old infants looked at impossible
event much longer
○ Tested again with 5.5 and 4.5 month olds
■ Image behind a screen
■ Possible event, screen tries to fall down but is stopped by
image behind it
■ Impossible event, screen tries to fall down and does so
successfully
■ Both 5.5 and 4.5 looked reliably longer at impossible event
over possible event
○ Aguiar and Baillargeon, 3.5 and 3 month olds
■ Habituation event, minnie mouse doll moving behind a
screen
■ Possible event - doll goes behind screen and you can see
doll peaking out of the screen with a cutout
■ Impossible event - doll goes behind screen but you cant see
doll peaking out from the cutout of the screen
● 3 month olds looked reliably longer at impossible
event
● 3.5 month olds looked at both equally, why?
○ They guess that there is a trick being played
here thinks that maybe there are 2 mice
● 3.5 month olds - Lower screen, only 1 mouse - look
reliably longer at impossible
● 3 month olds - lower screen, show 2 mice, look
equally at 2 events.
■ Interpretation
● At 3.5 months old
○ Know mouse exists when hidden
○ Detect violation in impossible event
○ Can generate explanation for the violation
● At 3 months old
○ Know mouse exists when hidden
○ Detect violation in impossible event
○ Cannot generate explanation for the violation
(unless shown 2 mice).
● Why do infants fail to search?
○ Processing load account (Bushnell and Keen)
■ Infants have limited processing resources
■ Infants fail search tasks because their processing resources
are overwhelmed by the combined load of:
● Representing the hidden object
● Planning and executing an appropriate sequence of
actions to retrieve the object
○ VoE tasks are easier than search tasks because infants have to
represent and reason about the hidden object, but they do not have
to plan search actions to retrieve them.
■ No combined load
○ Processing load account prediction - infants should succeed at
search tasks if the actions are made simpler.
■ Test - have infants search for an object in the dark
■ Logic - Need only do a simple reach (easy to plan) to get the
object
○ Hood and Willats
■ >5 months, success
Early Physical Reasoning
● Current evidence suggests that infants are born with several physical
principles that guide their reasoning around objects
○ Continuity - Objects exist continuously in time and space; they
cannot spontaneously appear or disappear.
○ Solidity - Objects are solid entities that cannot pass through other
objects
○ Gravity - Objects cannot remain stable in midair
● 2.5 month olds can detect continuity violations
○ Knows that a duck cannot spontaneously disappear after a screen
covers it.
● 2.5 month olds can detect solidity violations
○ Knows that an object cannot pass through the sides of a container,
putting a smaller can into a bigger can, lifting the bigger can and
the smaller can is still seen outside of the can
● 2.5 months olds can detect gravity violations
○ Knows that a cylinder cannot remain suspended midair
● Stahl and Feigenson (2015)
○ 11 month olds
○ Show infants possible and impossible events
○ Impossible events violate continuity, solidity, or gravity
○ Then teach infants something new about the object involved in the
event
○ At test, give them two objects (one from event and one not from
event) and see which they explore more.
○ Using the car/ramp method again.
■ Teach infants about a hidden property of the car, it makes
noise.
■ Test trial make car go up and down while noise plays
■ Make ball and car go up and down while noise plays
■ After watching the impossible event, they looked more at the
toy car, showing that they learned object-sound mapping.
■ After watching possible event: no learning.
■ Conclusion: infants spend more time exploring objects that
are involved in continuity, solidity, or gravity violations than
they do other objects.
Learning about the physical world
● Event categories
○ Types of causal interactions between objects. These include:
■ Occlusion (an object can hide another object
■ Containment (container contains containee)
■ Support (supporter supports suportee)
■ Collision (hitter hits hitee)
○ Infants identify the variables that help better predict outcomes
within the category
○ Example: Containment events
■ As early as 2.5 months of age, infants represent an object
hidden in a container
■ But do they pay attention to the objects width? Or height? Or
color?
■ Infants learn to pay attention to these variables one by one
through experience
■ 4 month olds infants have identified width as a containment
variable: they detect a violation when a wide object is
lowered into a narrow container.
■ By 7.5 months of age infants have identified height as a
containment variable: they detect a violation when a tall
object becomes hidden inside a shorter container
■ By 14 months, color is identified as well. If a purple object is
hidden in a container and a orange object is pulled out,
infants will detect a violation
○ Infants learn separately about each category
■ Example: the variable height
● Is identified at about 3.5 months old in occlusion
events
● But is not identified until about 7.5 months in
containment events
■ 3.5 month olds know you cannot occlude tall objects behind
short objects
Module 9
Early psychological reasoning
● When an adult observes an agent act in a scene, they attempt to make
sense of the agents actions by inferring the mental state that underlie
these actions
○ This ability is referred to by many names
■ Naive psychological reasoning
■ Theory of mind
■ Mental state reasoning
■ Mind reading
●
●
●
●
●
■ Mentalizing
An agent is an entity that has control over its own actions (decides when
and how to act)
○ People, animals, fictional characters
What types of mental states are there?
○ Motivational states
■ What motivation the agent has in the scene
● Goal
● Preference
○ Epistemic states
■ What knowledge the agent has/lacks in a scene
● Knowledge
● Ignorance
○ Counterfactual states
■ What false information the agent has in a scene
● False beliefs
● False perceptions
The principle of rationality
○ After they infer an agent's mental state, adults use these mental
states with a principle of rationality, to predict the agents actions as
the scene changes
■ Rationality:
● All other things being equal, agents act rationally
○ Rationality has two main corollaries
■ Consistency
● Agents act in a manner consistent with their mental
state
● If I told you I liked apples and didn't like oranges. If
you offer me an apple and an orange, i would reach
for the apple not the orange
■ Efficiency
● Agents pursue their goals in an efficient manner
What about infants?
○ Infants possess rich expectations about objects, do they also
possess rich expectations about agents
Motivational states: Goals
○ Can infants attribute a goal to an agent? Do they expect the agent
to pursue this goal efficiently?
○ Detour task, Gergely, Csibra and colleagues (1995, 1999)
■ 6, 9, and 12 month olds
■ Experimental and control conditions
■ VoE method
■ Habituation event, experimental condition: rectangle barrier
between left ball and right ball, ball jumps over barrier to
reach right ball
■ Habituation event, control conditions: Rectangle barrier is
behind right ball, so no need to jump over to reach the right
ball, but does so anyways
■ Test event, old action: the ball jumps over and reaches
bigger ball, but there is no rectangle
■ Test event, new action: the ball does not jump but moves in
a straight line to the left ball
■ Results of experiment
● 9-12 months
○ Experimental conditions: looked reliably longer
at the old action than at the new action: there is
no need to jump over but still does so infants
are confused
○ Control conditions: looked equally at the 2
events
● 6 months
○ No difference in looking time at all
● Interpretation
○ 9-12 month olds identify the circle as an agent
○ Attributed to the small circle the goal of
reaching the bigger circle
○ Infants expect small circle to pursue goal
efficiently and go to large circle in a straight line
when barrier is removed
○ Control condition: infants cannot form an
explanation for the small circles actions. Why is
it jumping when there is no barrier?
○ Philips and Wellman (2005)
■ 12 month olds detour task
■ Experimental and control conditions
■ Experimental condition
● Habituation
○ Researcher reaches over an obstacle to grab
a ball
○ Direct reach test event: no more obstacle so
they reach directly for it
○ Indirect reach test event: no more obstacles
but they still reach over.
■ Control condition
● Same as experimental but there is no ball. (no target,
no goal)
■ Results
● Experimental condition
○ Infants looked longer at at the old action than
the new action
● Control condition
○ Equally looked at both
● Infants can attribute a wide variety of goals to an agents including:
○ Obtaining, picking up, displacing, opening, throwing, storing,
tearing, breaking, reaching, chasing, helping, comforting, hitting,
sharing with, stealing from agents.
● Sticky mittens experiment Skerry, Carey and Spelke (2013)
○ Sometimes young infants have trouble understanding an agents
goal but they can succeed with a little help
○ Detour task with an agent wearing a grey mitten
○ Video taped events
○ 3 month olds
■ Same as the philips and wellman study but agent wears a
grey mitten
■ Initial results were negative: infants looked equally at two
scenarios
● Most likely they weren't sure what the goal the agent
was trying to pursue as they have no experience in
picking things up
● Infants given velcro mittens allowing them to pick up
other velcro objects
● Infants then looked reliably longer at old action rather
than new action after being trained with velcro mittens
● Motivational states: preferences
○ Preference task
■ Woodward (1998, 1999); Song, baillargeon, and fisher
(2014)
■ 5-12 month olds
■ VoE method
■ 2 object and 1 object conditions
● 2 object condition
○ Habituation event
■ Ball on one pedestal and teddy bear on
the other pedestal. Hand comes out
from the side and reaches for the bear.
○ Test trial
■ Change position of bear and ball, old
action is reaching for bear, new action is
reaching for ball
● 1 object condition
○ Habituation even
■ Hand reaches for bear
○ Test trial
■ Hand reaches for old object (bear) or a
new object (ball)
■ Results
● At 5-12 months
○ Two object condition: infants looked reliably
longer at new object condition
○ One object condition: infants looked equally at
the 2 events
● These results have been replicated with human and
non-human agents and with 3 month olds
● Interpretation 2 object condition
○ Infants identify the arm as part of a human
agent
○ Based on unvarying choice of information,
infants attribute to the agent a preference for
the target object
○ Infants expect agent to maintain that
preference consistently.
● Interpretation 1 object condition
○ Infants have no choice information to assess
the agents disposition towards a specific object
(are they grasping it cause they like it or cause
its the only object there?)
○ Therefore infants make no prediction about
what they will reach for in the test trial
● At least by 12 months old it appears to be a
preference for a teddy bear in general and not a
specific teddy bear
● Buresh and Woodward (2007)
○ 9 and 13 month olds
○ Same agent condition and different agent condition
○ Same agent condition
■ Habituation event
● Two objects, agent keeps reaching for a certain object
over the other object
■ Test event
● Old object event
○ Agent reaches for the old object
● New object event
○ Agent reaches for the new object
○ Different agent condition
■ Habituation event same as above
■ Test phase
● New agent from before reaches for either new object
(new object event) or old object (old object event)
○ Results
■ At both ages
● Same agent condition: infants looked longer at the
new object event than the old object event
● Different agent condition: infants looked equally at
both events
■ When infants attribute a preference to an agent, they view it
as a disposition of that specific agent and not all agents.
● Emotional information, necessary for preferences?
○ Egyed, Kiraly, and Gergely (2013)
○ 18 month olds
○ Object-request method
○ Same agent condition, different agent condition
■ Same agent condition: researcher makes a pleasant face at
one object and makes a disgusted face at the other object.
Then asks the child to give the researcher one of the
objects.
■ Different agent condition: same as the one above except a
different agent than the one who made the faces asks for the
child to give them the object
○ Results
■ Same agent condition: infants are more likely to give the
agent the object she looked at with a positive face than the
object with a negative face.
■ Different agent condition: infants are equally likely to give the
agent either toy
■ Infants use emotional information to attribute preference,
also assumed this preference will be agent specific
● Epistemic states
○ Concerns what knowledge an agent has or lacks about a scene
(knowledge, ignorance)
■ Barrier: There is a barrier blocking the agents view
■ Orientation: the agent is facing in a different direction when
the event occurs
■ Absence: when an agent is absent from the scene where an
event occurs
○ Do infants keep track of what an agent knows and doesn’t?
○ In a preference task, do infants attribute a preference for A over B
only when the agent can see both A and B.
○ Luo and Baillergeon (2007) (Barrier)
■ 12 month-olds
■ VoE method
■ Preference task
■ Transparent Knowledge condition
■ Opaque ignorance condition
● Transparent condition
○ Familiarization event: two transparent screens,
researcher repeatedly reaches for one specific
object
○ Test event: they reach for old object (old object
event) or new object (new object event)
● Opaque condition
○ Familiarization event: one of the screens is
opaque and keeps one of the objects hidden.
Researcher always reached for the object with
the transparent glass
○ Test event: they reach for old object (old object
event) or new object (new object event)
■ Results
● Transparent condition: infants look reliably longer at
the new object condition over the old object condition
○ As the agent can see both objects, infants
attribute to her a preference for the target
object she chooses in each trial
● Opaque condition: they looked equally long at both
events
○ The agent can only see the target object so it is
unclear what her position is towards it
● Evidence that 12 month old infants are not egocentric:
they can distinguish between what they can see and
what others can see, later extended to 6 month olds.
● Epistemic state: orientation
○ Liszkowski (2008)
■ 12 month olds
■ Pointing tasks
■ Knowledge and ignorance conditions
■ 4 trials in which the researcher takes an object off a black
folder and places it in a drawer.
■ In one trial, one side of the folder collapses and the object
falls to the floor.
● Knowledge condition: agent sees it fall
● Ignorance condition: Agent does not see object fall.
● While experimenter looks puzzled at collapsed folder,
will the infant point to inform the experimenter of the
new location of the object
■ Results:
● Infants are reliably likely to point out in ignorance
condition over knowledge condition
● Infants are not egocentric: they understand that an
agent may be unaware of an event in a scene
because she was facing away as it happened.
● Epistemic state: Absence
○ Ting, He and Baillergeon (2021)
■ 5 months
■ VoE method
■ Preference task
■ 4 conditions
■ One wide toy, one narrow toy, both look the same at the top
but the bases are either wide or narrow.
■ Familiarization trials 1 & 2
● A hand comes out from the side of the wall and
shakes the narrow toy which does not make a sound,
it then takes the wide one and shakes it and it jingles.
■ Fam trials 3 & 4
● The objects have switched spots, wooden matt over
blue matt, hand comes out and shakes the wide toy
and it makes a sound, shakes the narrow toy and it is
silent. Researcher then shakes the wide toy.
■ Test trial (knowledge/perception condition)
● Two boxes, both boxes are equally wide, different
patterns. A hand comes out and puts narrow toy in
one box and wide toy in the other box while the
experimenter watches. Wide toy still jingles
● Infants looked longer at narrow toy event
■ Test trial (no preference condition)
● Two boxes, one narrow one wide, hand comes out
and puts narrow toy in narrow box and wide toy in
wide box. Wide toy now doesn't jingle
● Infants look at both equally long
■ Test trial (ignorance condition)
● Experimenter is not present, both wide boxes, hand
comes out and puts the two toys into the two different
boxes.
● Infants looked at both equally long
■ Test trial (inference condition)
● Experimenter is not present, one narrow and one
wide box, hand comes out and puts wide toy into wide
box and narrow toy into narrow box.
● Infants look at narrow toy event reliably longer
■ RESULTS
● Perception condition: infants looked longer at new
object over old object event. Experimenter was
present so she knows where her preferred toy was
placed.
● No preference condition: infants look equally long at
both
● Ignorance condition: infants look equally long
● Inference condition: looked reliably longer at new
object event over old object event.
● From a very young age, infants are very clever at
keeping track of what others do and do not know.
● Conclusions
○ Even young infants are not egocentric
○ They understand an agent can have a different representation of a
scene than what the infant has
○ When this occurs, they use the agents representation to determine
how they will act
● Evaluation of Agents
○ Infants attempt to infer agents motivational and epistemic states,
coupled with the rationality principle to predict what they will do next
○ Infants also evaluate an agent’s action
○ An agent who does not act in accordance with her likely mental
states, or which acts in an inefficient manner can be seen as odd or
irrational.
○ In physical reasoning there is no evaluation but there is for
psychological reasoning
● Epistemic unreliability
○ Experimenter demonstrates an agent who does not act in
accordance with their epistemic state.
■ Unreliable looker: exclaims in excitement after looking into
an empty box
■ Unreliable labeler: the agent mislabels familiar objects
■ Unreliable user: they would put sunglasses on their feet
● Once it has been established that an agent is
unreliable, infants are not surprised when they look
for the toy in the wrong location.
● Less likely to learn a new word
● Less likely to learn how a new object works
● Infants not only attempt to interpret and predict an
agents actions, they also evaluate them, and they
avoid or mistrust unreliable or irrational agents.
Counterfactual states
● False beliefs: beliefs that an individual holds about the world that is not
true. Ex: believing that your favorite shirt is in the dresser when your
roommate moved it to their bedroom
●
● Standard false-belief tasks
○ Wimmer and Perner (1983), simplified by Baron-Cohen et al.
(1985)
○ Ages 3 and older
○ Story with props
○ Standard transfer-of-location tasks
○ Classic Salle-Anne task
■ Sally has a basket, Anne has a box. Sally has a marble and
puts it into her basket. Then sally goes out for a walk. Anne
takes the marble out of the basket and puts it into the box.
Sally comes back from the walk and wants to play with her
marble. Where will she look for the marble?
■ RESULTS
● At age 4 children can succeed in this task (Point to
basket)
● They understand that Sally will falsely believe that her
marble is still in her basket
● At age 3, children fail this task (point to box)
○ Similar transition aout 3-4 years of age has been obtained with a
wide range of FB tasks, including
■ Presence of objects
■ Number of objects
■ Identity of objects
■ Properties of objects
■ Contents of an object
● Two broad views
○ Fundamental change view
■ First proposed by Josef Perner
■ Children under 4 cannot reason about counterfactual states
■ They cannot understand beliefs
● Belief: Representation not a copy of reality so it can
be inaccurate or false
■ FB understanding is achieved through significant conceptual
change at about age 4. It constitutes a major milestone in
the development of human psychological reasoning (or
Theory of Mind)
○ Processing load view
■ First proposed by Alan Leslie
■ Children under age 4 can reason about counterfactual states
as well as motivational and epistemic states (they
understand pretend play which is counterfactual)
■ They fail false belief tasks because the processing load is
too high for these tasks.
○ How can we test between these 2 views?
■ Reduce the processing load in FB task
■ Still fail = Fundamental change view
■ Succeed = Processing load view.
■ Spontaneous-response tasks, we do not ask questions, we
measure their spontaneous response to a scene.
■ VoE FB task
● Do infants expect an agent to search for a toy where
she believes it's hidden even if it's false?
● Onishi & Baillergeon (2005)
● Transfer of location task
● 15 month-olds
● False-belief or true-belief conditions
○ Familiarization trial 1: researcher holds a toy
and hides it in a green box. There is a yellow
and green box.
○ Familiarization trials 2-3: same as trial 1, trying
to habituate
○ True-Belief green condition
■ Yellow box moves close to green box
and back, toy did not move boxes
○ True-belief yellow condition
■ Researcher sees as toy moves from
green box into yellow box
○ False-belief green condition
■ Researcher is not looking and toy
moves into yellow box
○ False-belief yellow condition
■ Researcher is looking as toy moves
from green to yellow box. When she is
not looking, it moves back into green
box.
○ Infants would look longer when researcher
reaches into a box that isn't aligning with her
belief of where the toy is. These results have
been extended to 6 month olds
■ Anticipatory looking FB tasks
● Southgate, Senju, & Csibra (2007)
● 25-month old toddlers
● Removal task: the object is removed from the scene,
instead of being moved to a different location
● Two FB conditions, videotaped events
○ Familiarization trial 1
■ Bear puts toy in left box
■ Doors above boxes light up
■ Agent reaches through left door to grab
toy.
○ Familiarization trial 2
■ Same as trial 1 but with right box.
○ False belief A
■ Bear puts toy in left box
■ Phone rings and researcher turns
around
■ Bear takes toy out and moves it to right
box
■ Bear takes toy away
■ Which door does child anticipate agent
will open?
○ False belief B
■ Bear puts toy in left box
■ Bear moves toy to right box
■ Bear opens left box
■ Researcher turns around as phone rings
■ Bear takes toy away
● RESULTs
○ Toddlers correctly anticipated where the agents
FB would lead her to search for the toy
○ Support for processing load view
○ Children and adults with high functioning
autism have specific difficulties with FB tasks
○ They don't show anticipations in anticipatory
looking tasks.
● Neural belief-processing FB task
○ Do infants, like adults, show more activation in the
Temporal-parietal junction when an agent has a true vs false belief.
○ Hyde et al. (2018)
■ 7 month olds
■ Transfer of location
■ Used fNIRS (functional near-infrared spectroscopy) to
measure activation in TPJ
Early sociomoral reasoning
Distinguishing positive and negative actions
● Do infants evaluate actions as good or bad?
● Sociomoral reasoning - using moral principles (right, wrong, fairness) to
understand and predict social interactions.
● According to the nativist view, infant’s sociomoral reasoning is guided by a
few principles about how individuals should interact with one another.
● At Least 3 sets of sociomoral principles guide early expectations
○ Principles that apply to all individuals (fairness, harm avoidance)
○ Principles that depend on prior interactions of individuals involve
(reciprocity)
○ Principles that apply when individuals are identified as members of
the same group (ingroup love)
● The value of a social action is determined by
○ Its valence (positive, negative, neutral)
○ Its magnitude (how much)
■ Actions with a positive valence are actions that are helpful to
others
■ Actions with a negative valence are actions that are harmful
to others.
● Hamlin, Wynn, and Bloom (2007)
○ 10 month olds
○ Habituation phase: help and hinder events shown in alteration
○ Test phase
■ VoE task
■ Choice task
○ Both measures focus on evaluation of characters
○ Help event: circle cannot climb hill on its own so triangle helps push
him up.
○ Hinder event: square pushes circle down the hill as it tries to climb
○ Test Phase 1: VoE method
■ Approach helper event
■ Approach hinderer event
■ Infants looked reliably longer at approach hinderer over
approach helper
○ Test Phase 2 : choice task
■ Infants are presented with both helper and hinderer and
encouraged to touch one.
■ They prefer the helper over the hinderer
○ REsults
■ Infants distinguish between positive and negative actions
towards others
■ Infants prefer
● Helper over hinderer
● Helper over a neutral character
● Neutral character over a hinderer
■ Same preferences for other positive and negative actions
■ Same preferences with younger infants
■ Same preferences even when helping action was not
successful.
○ More results
■ Intentions count! Same results at 8 months old whether the
helper or hinderer succeed or fail at their task (hamlin,
2013).
Engaging in Prosocial Behavior
● Infants engage in positive actions toward others
● Warneken and tomasello (2006) would infants help an experimenter in
need of assistance
○ 18 month olds
○ First, warm-up phase with experimenter and child
○ 10 different test scenarios
○ Each child receives 5 experimental scenarios (E needs help) and 5
control scenarios (E does not need help), randomly determined
○ Scenarios include: out of reach, obstacles, achieving, wrong result,
or using wrong means
○ Results
■ Reliable difference between experimental and control in 6/10
scenarios; out of reach scenarios seem easiest
■ Most infants helped in at least on situation
■ In most cases, infants helped spontaneously, before E
looked at or spoke to them
■ Even 12 month olds help with out-of-reach scenarios
● Infants can distinguish between positive and negative actions directed
towards them
● Dunfield & Kuhlmeier (2010): would infants give a toy to an experimenter
that was clumsy but well-intentioned or an experimenter that teased them
○ 21 month olds
○ 2 women E1 and E2, seated at a table slanted towards them
○ Phase 1: E1 and E2 took turns offering toys, 4 each.
■ Clumsy E1: tried unsuccessfully to give child the toy, toy
rolled back down the slant
■ Teasing E2: teased, removed toy when an infant reached
○ Phase 2: a third experimenter brings out a new toy, falls in between
E1 and E2, and both reach out for it.
○ RESULTS
■ Of the infants who were willing to give new toy away,
majority gave it to clumsy E1 over teasing E2.
■ Infants were equally likely to give the new toy to clumsy E1
or a successful E2
Expectations about other’s behavior
● The principle of fairness
○ Individuals should be fair when diving
■ Resources
■ Rewards for effort or merit
■ Punishment
● Sloane , Baillergeon, & Promack (2012)
○ 19 month olds
○ Experimental condition: agent says I have cookies, two penguins
say yay
■ Equal event: gives each one cookie
■ Unequal event: gives two cookies to only one penguin and
none to the other.
○ Inanimate control condition: penguins make no noise or movements
■ Still has equal and unequal event
○ Cover control condition: penguins are agents, but have boxes in
front of them
■ Still has equal or unequal events. Cookies were always
present under the box, not distributed by experimenter.
○ Results
■ Infants looked longer at the unequal event in experimental
condition, but looked equally at both for the other two
conditions
■ This result was also extended to 9 and 4 month olds.
■ Human infants possess an expectation of fairness.
● Buyukozer-Dawkins, Sloane, & Baillergeon (2019)
○ 9 and 4 month olds
● Sloane et al. (2012)
○ 21 month olds
○ VoE
○ Both agents work OR one agent works
○ Explicit, implicit, and control conditions
○ One work event
■ Two agents at each end of a display, third agent says “Wow
look at all these toys, if you put the toys away you can have
a sticker. Only one agent works to put toys away but both
receive a sticker.
○ Both work event
■ Same as above but both work to put toys away
○ Explicit condition: The researcher told them that they would give
them a reward.
○ Implicit condition: the distributor asked the agents to pick up their
toys, but did not tell agents she had a reward.
○ Control condition: the containers are opaque, so the distributor
could not see how much work each agent accomplished.
○ Results
■ Explicit condition: infants that saw the one work event looked
longer than infants that saw both works events
● Infants had an expectation that workers should be
rewarded and slackers should not be rewarded.
■ Implicit condition: infants that saw the one works event
looked longer than infants who saw two works event
● The expectation is still there even without an explicit
declaration
■ Control condition: infants looked equally at both
● Infants were responding to the work that was done
from the perspective of the third agent.
○ First-party distribution tasks
■ Resource distribution tasks with older kids (3-5) years old
● Children are asked to divide a reward between
themselves and an experimenter or another child.
● Result: children take the bigger reward for themselves
● Children’s own self-interest gets in the way here.
Expectations about group membership
● Ingroup support principle
○ Individuals in a social group should act in ways that support the
group and its members.
○ Has 2 main components
■ Ingroup care: one should act prosocially toward ingroup
member, limit negativity
■ Ingroup loyalty: when dealing with ingroup and outgroup
individuals, we should prefer ingroup and favor ingroup when
resources are limited.
■ Not about outgroup hate
● Jin & Baillergeon (2017)
○ 17 month old
○ VoE tasks
○ Ingroup and outgroup conditions
○ Group membership is marked by novel labels (following
minimal-group paradigm)
○ Ingroup condition labeling trials
■ 2 agents are a Bem and one is a Tig
■ Test trial, bem reaches for an object but cannot reach.
● Ignore event: the other bem does not help give the
object
● Help event: the other bem helps give other bem the
object
○ Outgroup condition labeling trials
■ Test trial, now with one bem and one tig rather than 2 bems
○ Ingroup condition
■ Infants view helping the ingroup as obligatory, so they detect
a violation if the ingroup member does not help
○ Outgroup condition
■ Infants view helping the outgroup as optional, so they looked
equally at help or ignore
● Language (accent and dialect) is a good indicator of ingroup membership
● Kinzler, Dupoux, & Spelke (2007)
○ 10 month olds (some american some french)
○ Speech phase: infants watch 2 movies in alteration, one with
woman speaking infant directed english and the other with infant
directed french
○ Toy offering phase: both women now side by side in the movie,
silently offer identical toys which appear in real life before the infant.
○ RESULTS
■ In each country, infants were more likely to reach for the toy
offered by a woman who spoke their native language.
● Bian & Baillargeon (prep.)
○ 12 month olds
○ VoE method
○ Using outfits to mark groups
○ Prefer-same event
○ Prefer-different event
○ Infants look longer at prefer-different event.
● Bian, Sloane, & Baillargeon (2018)
○ 19 month olds
○ VoE tasks
○ Group membership is marked by monkey puppets and giraffe
puppets
○ 3 item condition (all puppets can get a cookie)
■ Results: infants expected fairness, they looked longer when
it was not distributed fairly
○ 2 item condition (only enough for the distributors group)
■ Results: Infants looked longest at equal event and favors
outgroup event. They expect the distributor to favor their
ingroup.
Numerical thinking
● What numerical systems are we born with?
○ Approximate number system
○ Object tracking system
● Approximate number system “number sense”
○ Allows for rouch approximation or estimate without counting
○ Limited precision (ratio signature) better for smaller numbers worse
for larger numbers.
Download