Uploaded by Abir Cheaito

Unit 7 Vocab + Reading Comprehension

advertisement
1.1 Reading Comprehension
Read the following magazine article then answer the questions follow:
Time Flies or Crawls
By Vicki Oransky Wittenstein
Background
Why do some activities seem to make time race by swiftly while other actions
seem to drag? Scientists are busy exploring how the human brain influences the
awareness of the passage of time.
You are harnessed to a cable dangling 110 feet from the ground at Zero Gravity,
a thrill park outside Dallas, Texas. The ride is called SCAD, short for
Suspended Catch Air Device—a fitting name for one of the most frightening
rides anywhere. Four narrow steel beams support you and the cable. You’re
strapped in so that your back tilts to the ground and your face points to the sky.
For a few moments you hover in the air. Ready? The cable is released. You
plunge straight toward the ground, smacking into a net at a speed of 50 miles
per hour (mph). Terrifying? You bet!
What details will you remember about the fall? Will you be able to accurately
guess how long it lasted? These are some of the questions that Dr. David
Eagleman, a neuroscientist, asked 23 SCAD volunteers as part of an
experiment studying how people sense the passage of time. Dr. Eagleman is
head of the Laboratory for Perception and Action at Baylor College of Medicine
in Houston, Texas. He researches time perception in order to understand how
the brain constructs our understanding of reality.
Dr. Eagleman thinks that most of our thoughts, actions, and feelings run on
autopilot and are not within our conscious control. Instead, they are actively
constructed in the brain.
“Think of a media star, like Justin Bieber during a concert,” Eagleman says.
“He relies on an enormous team of people to do the lighting, sound, etc. Yet, all
we see is Bieber singing. We don’t always realize the size of the machinery he
depends on.”
“Sometimes the summaries we receive
do not tell the truth. . . .”
Similarly, the neurons and circuits of the brain collect lots of information but
provide us with just a summary of the important details, according to Eagleman.
People are not always aware of all that occurs around them. Sometimes the
summaries we receive do not tell the truth, or at least the complete story.
The same holds true for time perception. “Let’s suppose you’re watching me
clap my hands,” Eagleman says. “Hearing gets processed in your brain much
faster than seeing. Yet when I clap, it appears as if you hear and see at the same
time. That’s because the brain waits and collects all the information before
serving up the best story of what happened in the outside world.”
Although it may sound strange, by the time we perceive something in the world
(for example, the clap), the event has already happened. “We are all living in the
past,” Eagleman says. “Just as television networks wait a couple of seconds to
air shows, we too wait before going ‘live.’ In the case of the brain, though, the
wait is about a half a second.”
Eagleman continually thinks up creative ways to study time perception. He hit
upon the SCAD experiments when he remembered a terrifying experience of his
own. As a young boy, he fell off the roof of a construction site, badly smashing
his nose, and was raced to the emergency room. To this day, he can recount all
the details of his fall, as if in slow motion: his foot slipping on the roof, and the
brick floor looming below him, littered with nails. But what stands out in his
mind most of all is the same quality shared by many riders of the SCAD—the
feeling that the fall took forever, as in Alice in Wonderland, when Alice falls
down the rabbit hole.
“Of course I could have really hurt myself with that fall,” Eagleman says. “But
it gave me a sense from an early age that time isn’t necessarily what we think it
is, and that it’s warpable. So when I got into neuroscience, that was something
that was always on my mind.”
Why does time appear to slow down in life-threatening circumstances? Does
fear actually cause the brain to work more slowly, or is something else
happening? “I wanted to catch people in a terrifying moment and study them,”
Eagleman says. “I couldn’t put them in a car accident or something like that. So,
I thought, okay, I have to engineer an event.”
First he took his lab to an amusement park, but the rides weren’t scary enough
to experience a warping of time. “So I sent a graduate student, Chess Stetson,
on a mission to find the scariest thing possible that was still safe.” The SCAD
was the answer.
A device called a chronometer was strapped to the wrists of the SCAD
volunteers. While they were falling to the ground, they stared at the
chronometer.
“The chronometer flashes a number in such a way that the volunteers only see
what’s flashing if they are seeing the world in slow motion,” Eagleman says.
A hummingbird beats its wings multiple times per second.
Eagleman compared the flashing numbers to challenging people to see the beats
of a hummingbird’s wings. “Normally, when you look at a hummingbird’s
wings you see a blur. But now, if we put you in a super-scary situation, can you
actually see, in slow motion, the beats of the wings? Instead of wings, we
flashed numbers on the SCAD chronometer.”
The SCAD riders could not read the flashing numbers, so they did not see in
slow motion. They also overstated the duration of their falls by about
36 percent. “How can both of these things be true?” Eagleman asks. “How can
it be that you think it took a long time but you’re not seeing in slow motion?”
The red spot in the X-ray indicates the location of the amygdala.
It turns out that in terrifying moments a part of the brain called
the amygdala works overtime, recording all the details of the experience. “It’s a
trick of memory,” Eagleman says. “When you are in a scary situation your brain
is writing down lots of information, and as a result it seems that [the situation]
must have taken a very long time.”
So our perception of the length of time that an event lasts can be distorted.
Remember how slowly summers used to pass when you were a younger child?
Time passed more slowly then because your brain was busily taking notes on all
the new experiences you were having. When you grow older, though, life
becomes more familiar. There is less new information for the brain to process,
and the passage of time quickens.
Another example of time distortion is what Eagleman calls the “oddball effect.”
He tests this effect in his lab with a computer. A person stares at a series of
images passing across a computer screen. Usually the image is the same, say, a
brown shoe. But once in a while, a flower appears. Even though the shoe and
the flower stay on the screen for the same amount of time, to the viewer, the
flower remains longer. Why? Because the flower is novel, and when something is
new the brain pays more attention.
Then why does time pass so quickly while you are absorbed in taking a test?
Wouldn’t your brain be engaged with novel information that would slow
time down?
“It turns out,” Eagleman explains, “that in order to be aware of time, you have
to ask the question: Hey, how much time do I have left? When you’re fully
engaged during a test or an afternoon where you’re having fun with friends,
you’re not asking about how much time you have left.”
Eagleman says that people have a general sense of time passing. “Normally,
you’re always looking at your clock, or sort of aware of time,” he says. “But
when you’re really involved in something, you forget to pay attention to time.
And then when you do, suddenly you’re surprised that so much time has
passed.”
The opposite is true, though, when you’re on a long airplane flight, or waiting
for a pot of water to boil. “You check the clock over and over again, and
it seems like it’s taking such a long time. It’s so boring,” Eagleman says.
There’s a difference, though, between how time is perceived as it unfolds in the
present and how it’s perceived later, after the time has already passed.
“Time perception and memory are very intertwined,” Eagleman says. “It
depends on which direction you’re looking. When you look back on the
passage of time, it’s exactly the opposite from how you experienced it initially.
So at the end of that weekend you spent with friends, when you return to school
on Monday, it feels like you were gone for a long time. And that boring, long
airplane ride to your grandmother’s now seems short.”
Huh? But why? “That retrospect part has to do with how much fun you can call
up, how much memory you have about what happened,” Eagleman says.
“During the flight you didn’t lay down new memories because there really
wasn’t anything novel.
But the time with your friends was fun, so your brain wrote down lots of new
memories.”
“Time flies on a day packed with activity. . . .”
Time flies on a day packed with activity and when you’re learning lots of new
things. “But later, it will seem longer and you’ll remember it more,” Eagleman
says. “And when you look back at it at night, you’ll feel like it was a long, rich
day.”
So, although the brain actively constructs time, people have some control over
how fast they sense time passing. If you want those vacations and weekends to
last a long time, have lots of fun!
7.1.2 Practice
A. After reading the magazine article carefully, answer the following questions:
1- What is the meaning of the following words in the text?
a- neuroscientist: A neuroscientist is a person who studies the brain and
the nervous system.
b- neurons: A neuron is a cell in the nervous system that carries
messages between the brain and the body parts.
c- neuroscience: Neuroscience is any of the sciences that study the
nervous system.
d- retrospect: is the act of looking back on or contemplating things in
the past.
2- What is the focus of Dr. Eagleman’s research?
abcd-
depth perception
sound perception
time perception
distance perception
3- What main idea does the article convey about time perception?
abcd-
Our thoughts and feelings mildly affect our perception of how long an
event lasts.
Our thoughts and feelings have no affect on our perception of the
duration of events..
Our thoughts and feelings definitely affect our perception of how long
an event lasts.
Our thoughts affect our perception of how long an event lasts; our
feelings do not.
4- What point is made in using the example of viewers at a concert?
abcd-
Humans tend to to focus too much on their thoughts and feelings.
Humans need to pay closer attention to their surroundings.
Human brains are bombarded with too much information.
Humans have little awareness of how the brain constructs thoughts
and feelings.
5- What figurative language is used in the description of how the amygdala
works?
abcd-
a personification
simile
metaphor
onomatopoeia
6- How does memory influence the perception of time?
abcd-
The memory of either pleasant or unpleasant experiences tends to fade
quite quickly.
The memory of unpleasant experiences seems brief at first but
over time, the perception becomes longer.
The memory of pleasant experiences seems to be longer with more
details; unpleasant experiences are briefer, less-detailed memories.
The memory of unpleasant experiences is as long and detailed as
events are. However, the memory of pleasant experiences is as short
and sweet as events are.
Download