EJSP RESEARCH ARTICLE What do I gain from joining crowds? Does self-expansion help to explain the relationship between identity fusion, group efficacy and collective action? Tomasz Besta , Michał Jaśkiewicz, Natasza Kosakowska-Berezecka, Rafał Lawendowski & Anna Maria Zawadzka University of Gdansk, Gdańsk, Poland Correspondence Tomasz Besta, Ph.D., Institute of Psychology, University of Gdansk, Bażyńskiego 4, 80-952 Gdańsk. E-mail: t.besta@ug.edu.pl Received: 8 June 2016 Accepted: 7 August 2017 https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2332 Keywords: group processes, social identity, self, identity fusion, self-expansion Abstract Four studies were carried out to examine how identity fusion, self- and group efficacy, and collective action are related and what role self-expansion plays in these relationships. In the pilot study, participants recalled their experience of participating in mass gatherings. The three other studies were conducted during mass gatherings organized for collective purposes: a music concert (Study 1), a bicycle activist event (Study 2), and Equality Days (Study 3). The results showed (a) a significant positive relationship between personal and group identity fusion, self-expansion, and self-efficacy (Study 1); (b) a significant mediating effect of self-expansion on the relationship between personal and group identity fusion and group efficacy (Studies 1 and 2); and (c) a significant mediating effect of self- expansion and group efficacy on the relationship between identity fusion and collective action tendency (Studies 2 and 3). Social identity is a dynamic construct and can be the both the cause of collective action and also its consequence (Doosje, Spears, & Ellemers, 2002; Reicher, 1996). At the same time, maintaining and strengthening one’s social identities can be achieved by a shared tendency for progroup behaviors (Drury & Reicher, 2005; Ellemers, 1993) or lower willingness to act on behalf of other groups (Kosakowska-Berezecka et al., 2016). According to the social identity approach (Reicher, 2001), identity is a multifaceted social construct enacted by experiencing others and interacting with them. One source of strong identification with the group is the experience of being with a crowd of like-minded people. Crowds constitute an immense source of the experience of social relations. Being part of a mass gathering allows individuals to shift from personal to social identity, from personal opinions to more empowering group-based judgments, values, and beliefs that the crowds feel more free and open to express themselves out loud, even if their actions or opinions are against the external general public’s doing or thinking. Reicher and Haslam, following the elaborated social identity model (Reicher, 1996), emphasized that in crowd gatherings members become more empowered to express their values and beliefs. Communal gatherings, thus, can serve as (a) a real experience that strengthens existing social identities and (b) a source of expansion of our sense of the self, as the experience of being in the crowd may in some cases alter an individual’s self-descriptions and lead O152 people to develop more collective sense of their new experience. Thus, being with the crowd can lead to the formation of new representational systems of one’s social reality and increase the relevance of one’s group identity (Reicher & Haslam, 2013). Although group identification is related to committing oneself to act on behalf of that group (for an overview, see Thomas, McGarty, & Mavor, 2009), other forms of group adherence have been shown to better explain a person’s relatedness to his or her group and willingness to engage in progroup behaviors. For example, Neville and Reicher (2011) showed that the feeling of shared identity with other members of the group is a better predictor of the feeling of relatedness than group identification based on self-categorization. A special case of experiencing shared identity with other members of the group is identity fusion (Swann, Gómez, Seyle, Morales, & Huici, 2009). This state resembles the visceral feeling of being part of the group, perceiving group values and norms as one’s own, and experiencing a strong emotional bond with other group members. Research shows that comparing a group’s identification identity fusion constitutes a better predictor of strong connection to the group expressed by individuals’ willingness to defend in-group members (for an overview, see Swann & Buhrmester, 2015). Although the results of most studies show that identity fusion usually occurs among group members who have very close personal relationships, people may also become fused with more general, collective groups European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. T. Besta et al. (e.g., country or political party; cf. Swann et al., 2014). When a person fuses with a group, his or her personal self is not deactivated temporarily; instead, her or his actions reflect “both their personal and social identities, working together by virtue of the porous borders that define them” (Swann, Jetten, Gómez, Whitehouse, & Bastian, 2012, p. 443). Identity fusion is often considered to be distinctly different from identification with a group, especially when identification is understood in terms of self-categorization as a group member. For example, in the classical concept of fluidity of self-construal, once group identity is salient, personal identity is not and vice versa (Onorato & Turner, 2004). However, the identity fusion construct describes such strong group adherence when group and personal identities are not interchangeable but strengthen each other (Gómez et al., 2011). Additionally, the difference between identity fusion and group identification is also visible when we take the methods they are measured with. Gómez et al. (2011) showed that the verbal fusion scale is visibly distinct from group identification scales. Keeping these differences in mind, more recent conceptualizations of group identification go beyond defining identification as merely a categorization process. For example, Leach et al.’s (2008) view of identification includes self-categorization as a group member as only one aspect of this multifaceted construct. Given the high correlations between identity fusion and measures of group identifications obtained in previous research (i.e., fusion correlated the strongest with Leach and colleagues scale; r = .83) and low average scores for fusion (see e.g., Besta, Mattingly, & Błażek, 2016; Besta, Szulc, & Jaśkiewicz, 2015), one may argue that fusion could be perceived as a rare and extreme case of group identification that is based on solidarity and feeling of oneness with other group members, with personal identity merged with group identity (and not alternatively salient). The aspect of solidarity with other group members stands in line with the concept that people fused with a group are especially likely to value the reciprocal aspect of group membership. When an individual is devoted to protect the group and make the group stronger, he or she expects other group members to be devoted to protect him or her. Gómez et al. (2011) showed that this feeling of mutual support links identity fusion to belief in one’s invulnerability (i.e., fused individuals agree more with statements such as, “In the face of danger, I am convinced that my group and I will survive” and “My group will be able to cope with any sort of threat”). Previous studies also showed that the visceral feeling of fusion with a group could be positively related to (a) the stronger feeling of group agency (e.g., Gómez et al., 2011) and positively related to (b) an individual’s agentic self-description and goal adherence (Besta et al., 2016). Based on those studies, we examine whether identity fusion is related to willingness to engage in collective action, similarly to group identification, as shown in the literature cited (e.g., Thomas et al., What do I gain from joining crowds? 2009). Thus, in this research, we were focused on identity fusion as the proposed antecedent of feeling of group efficacy and collective action tendencies. Pathways to Collective Action Despite some differences, most social psychological models of collective action imply that experiencing undeserved or illegitimate group-based negative treatment, along with the resulting feelings of injustice and group-based anger, as well as social identity and group-based efficacy, are key triggers for collective action (Becker & Tausch, 2015; Thomas et al., 2009; Van Zomeren, Spears, & Leach, 2008). One distinct pathway within the model of collective action shows that belief that the group has the potential to solve issues collectively increases the likelihood that group members take action on behalf of the group and for their sake (Van Zomeren, Spears, Fischer, & Leach, 2004). Meta-analytic evidence suggests that group identity and group efficacy are positively correlated, and that they are important predictors of collective action, thus leading to the following, potentially valid, assumption of their causal relationship: group identification ➔group efficacy ➔collective action (Van Zomeren, Postmes, & Spears, 2008). In a dual pathway model of collective action, Van Zomeren, Leach, and Spears (2012) argued that this path is one of two important causal links leading to collective action (the other one constitutes the path of emotion-based coping with a group situation and is less relevant to our reasoning as we do not concentrate on group-based emotions in this research). However, there is some evidence that group identity salience, in certain conditions, has actually no effect on an individual’s perception of group efficacy (Van Zomeren et al., 2008). An additional experimental study showed that the feeling of group efficacy leads to stronger identification with the group, because belief in the group’s efficacy triggers individuals’ identity to act on behalf of the group (Van Zomeren, Leach, & Spears, 2010). Thomas et al. (2009) proposed a theoretical model that is in line with these experimental studies. In the encapsulated model of social identity in collective action (EMSICA), they proposed that social identification acts as a link between group-based emotions and group efficacy beliefs on one side and collective action intentions on the other side. It leads to a conclusion that heightened group efficacy would manifest itself in stronger identification with a group, and as a result, in stronger willingness to act on behalf of a group. Experimental studies focused on the relationship between group identification, group efficacy, and collective action have not often been conducted among individuals who actually experienced a collective identity by interacting with others. The individuals were merely primed for their temporary salience by being reminded of the unjust treatment of their group of reference (e.g., being a student whose university plans to significantly raise annual tuition fees; Van Zomeren et al., 2010). However, when group members interact European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. O153 T. Besta et al. What do I gain from joining crowds? with a crowd, we can expect stronger links between group identification and collective action intention. When people manifest their emergent social identities by interacting with other group members, their commitment to take collective action and beliefs in the efficacy could be boosted, as the process of actualizing one’s social identity takes place (Drury & Reicher, 2000). For example, in research on small group discussions, Thomas et al. (2016) showed that small group interaction heightened identification with a newly formed group, as well as the tendency to act collectively. Thomas and colleagues, based on the encapsulated model of social identity, showed that causal pathway group efficacy ➔ group identification ➔collective action fit their data. An alternative pathway based on the social identity model of collective action (SIMCA; Van Zomeren et al., 2012), namely, group identification ➔ group efficacy ➔collective action, also seemed to fit data obtained by Thomas et al. (2016). However, in this case the path from group efficacy to collective action was statistically insignificant. Researchers concluded that the SIMCA model might be thus more relevant when describing established social identities and their consequences (e.g., gender identity or membership in protest movements), whereas the EMSICA model might be more useful when explaining the group identifications of the participants of the newly formed groups (Thomas et al., 2016). Collective action researchers (Becker & Tausch, 2015; Thomas et al., 2016; Van Zomeren, 2015) and those focused on crowd behavior (Drury & Reicher, 2000; Reicher, 1996) highlighted the importance of the ability to explain and describe the processes related to the transformation of group identities and the formation of new identities by interacting with other group members. This is especially vivid when interacting with others during gatherings of people united by causes and common goals. We could assume, in line with the reasoning presented by Reicher and Haslam (2013), that those interactions taking place during mass gatherings are the possible source of the sense of the self’s expansion. Self-expansion relates to the formation of new representations of social reality, new perspectives, and new ways of understanding a person’s experiences. In the studies presented in this article, we explored whether including a measure of nonrelational self-expansion helps explain the willingness to engage in collective action presented by participants in mass gatherings. We collected data in the social context of positive group gatherings, allowing for individual expansion of the self and thus increasing one’s feeling of self-efficacy. The present research in natural settings is intended to extend the results of previous studies on crowd behavior and collective action. Because to date researchers have not identified key antecedents of group efficacy experimentally (Van Zomeren et al., 2010), we present four studies that address this gap. We argue that the lack of potential antecedents (such as self-expansion as a result of being engaged in crowd gathering) in previous studies might help to explain O154 the relationship between group adherence, group efficacy, and collective action intention. We argue that being part of a crowd gives an individual the opportunity to express his or her identity and enact it behaviorally. As a result, the self expands by interacting with other group members. Thus, this experience of being with the crowd and the resulting expansion of the self could be antecedents of self-efficacy, acting as a contextual trigger mediating the relationship between salient social identity and group efficacy and collective action tendency. Self-Expansion, Self- and Group Efficacy, and Collective Actions Mummendey, Kessler, Klink, and Mielke (1999) proposed that group efficacy is a better predictor of collective action than group identification. Group efficacy gives people an experience of collective power, which enables the group to change its destiny (Drury & Reicher, 2005; Reicher, 2001). Thus, the stronger the feeling of group efficacy, the more likely people are to engage in collective action (e.g., Hornsey et al., 2006; Mummendey et al., 1999). However, people who feel a strong connection to a group do not necessarily have to experience the feeling of stronger group efficacy as shown in previous studies (Van Zomeren et al., 2008). Group identification must be accompanied by the belief that the group can take collective actions that allow the members to realize collective goals effectively. According to the self-expansion model, the yearning for self-growth and self-expansion is an important human motivation (Aron, Lewandowski, Mashek, & Aron, 2013; Wright, Aron, & Tropp, 2002). This yearning can be realized by engaging in exciting and challenging activities that help to develop new perspectives on the social world and to advance one’s knowledge, as well as by including significant others in one’s self-concept and by internalization of new identities (Aron et al., 2004). As a result, individuals feel more positively about themselves and have more identity buffers and resources to rely on, thus enhancing their feeling of self-efficacy and agency (Aron et al., 2013; Mattingly & Lewandowski, 2014). In this view, realization of the self-expansion motive is considered an antecedent of self-efficacy belief. Researchers have shown that an expanded self-concept is associated with an increased sense of self-efficacy (Mattingly & Lewandowski, 2013). Scholars also showed that selfexpansion is linked to a greater likelihood of accomplishing goals (Xu, Floyd, Westmaas, & Aron, 2010) and heightened approach motivation (Mattingly, Mcintyre, & Lewandowski, 2012). Most studies on self-expansion took place in laboratory settings or focused on close relationships with significant others. There is also the under-researched topic of a direct relationship between self-expansion and willingness to act on behalf of the group. Thus, we go beyond the self-expansion–self-efficacy relationship and explore whether the feeling of self-expansion European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. T. Besta et al. experienced during participation in mass gatherings is linked to willingness to engage in collective action. Finally, we extend the existing models of antecedents of collective actions by proposing that the feeling of self-expansion as a result of participating in a crowd of likeminded people could be considered the mediator of the relationship between group identity, and both group efficacy belief and collective action tendencies. Self-Expansion and Crowd Processes Self-expansion motive can be satisfied on the intrapersonal level (by engaging in novel and challenging activities) and on the interpersonal and intergroup levels. Here, self-expansion motive might be satisfied by including other people and entire groups in the self (Wright et al., 2002). Thus, in this article we examine whether including the experience of mass gathering events in the self and the stronger feeling of fusion with participants of such mass gatherings are related to greater self-expansion, which, in turn, is linked to the group efficacy belief and the collective action intention. This model extends previous studies on crowds’ behavior by adding a direct examination of self-expansion. Previous conceptualizations of triggers of collective action related to mass gathering attendance mainly focused on variables such as empowerment, collective resilience, or collective self-objectification. Thus, the inclusion of self-expansion extends research on crowd behaviors that were mainly analyzed in the context of protests demonstrating oppositional views or demands targeted at a dominant out-group. Drury and Reicher (2009) argued that participation in a crowd (e.g., during such protests) strengthens identification with the group and triggers collective empowerment, but they also emphasized the role of inclusive self-categorization that emerges and makes way for feelings of unity and expected support. Actions that materialize participants’ social identity potentially lead to collective self-objectification where participants’ identity becomes oppositional to the dominant out-group (e.g., police or government; Drury, Cocking, Beale, Hanson, & Rapley, 2005; Stekelenburg & Klandermans, 2013). This sense of unity and expectation of support were also described by Drury, Cocking, and Reicher (2009) among emergency or disaster group victims who due to a common traumatic experience constitute a new sociality. However, the authors emphasized the need for a more thorough explanation of togetherness and crowd resilience leading to cooperation within a group of participants who are unknown to each other yet create social bonds and feel empowered and ready for collective action. All of these constructs are important antecedents of progroup behaviors but describe the emergence of a shared feeling of unity with others and a shared reality constructed as a result of being part of the crowd, whereas self-expansion is intended to describe changes in the self-construct. In the present studies, we explore whether the intention to participate in future events What do I gain from joining crowds? on behalf of one’s group and group efficacy belief are stronger when being a part of mass gatherings results in self-expansion. We argue that the role of individual self-expansion is an important area to examine, and is parallel to these mentioned above. That is, crowd events could facilitate the emergence of collective empowerment, resilience, and solidarity, thus leading to progroup behaviors. In addition, changes in selfperception, linked to expansion of the self-construct, could reveal how individual- and group-level psychological variables are interconnected. Overview of the Present Studies The overall goal of the present studies is to integrate crowd research and collective action research, and by including the self-expansion motive to describe important factors in developing collective action tendencies among crowd members. More specifically, our goal is to explore whether those participants in mass events who felt they developed new perspectives and increased their knowledge by interacting with others in large groups (that is, declared high self-expansion) would be most willing to believe in group efficacy and to engage in future collective actions. In four studies (for datasets see Supporting Information provided online), we explored the relationship among identity fusion, self-expansion, self- and group efficacy, and collective action tendencies. A pilot study was conducted using the retrospective method among undergraduate students. Studies 1 to 3 were conducted among participants at the sites of various mass events. The first two studies (the pilot study and Study 1) were designed to examine the link between self-expansion and a feeling of self- and group efficacy. Based on previous research, we assumed in hypothesis 1 (H1) that self-expansion would be linked to a higher level of self-efficacy. As self-efficacy is related to general personal agency, we also measured an individual’s agency, although we consider agency and self-efficacy two distinct constructs. In the pilot study conducted among Polish students, we checked whether as a result of recalling the experience of being part of a mass gathering students felt self-expansion and that this related to their declared self-efficacy. As the relationship between feelings of self-expansion and self-efficacy was especially vivid, when identity fusion was the strongest, in Study 1, we explored the relationship among identity fusion, self-expansion, self-efficacy, and one’s personal agency among music festival attendees. In Study 1, we added a variable important to our reasoning, namely, group efficacy, and proposed hypothesis 2 (H2) that self-expansion is a mediator of the relationship between identity fusion and group efficacy. In Study 2 and Study 3, we conducted a survey among activists participating in gatherings for a collective cause, for bicyclists (Study 2) and lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender (LGBT) individuals’ rights (Study 3). The aim of Studies 2 and 3 was to test our assumption that a feeling of self-expansion caused by participating in a mass gathering could be an important European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. O155 T. Besta et al. What do I gain from joining crowds? mediator of the relationship between strong personal and group identity fusion ➔ self-efficacy ➔ collective action. We assumed in hypothesis 3 that the selfexpansion resulting from being part of the crowd mediates the relationship between identity fusion with other crowd attendees, group efficacy, and collective action tendencies (H3; Studies 2 and 3). Thus, we expected that the feeling of self-expansion would partially explain the relationship among identity fusion, group efficacy, and collective action. Pilot Study greater awareness of things?”). Participants indicated their answers on a 7-point scale (1 = not very much to 7 = very much). The scale turned out to be a reliable measure (Cronbach’s α = .91). Self-efficacy. We used the 10-item Polish version of the general self-efficacy scale (GES; Luszczynska, Gutiérrez-Doña, & Schwarzer, 2005) for assessing beliefs about one’s self-efficacy (e.g., “I can always manage to solve difficult problems if I try hard enough”). Participants indicated their answers on a 4-point scale (1 = not at all true to 4 = exactly true). The scale was reliable (Cronbach’s α = .84). Method Participants. Undergraduate students in various study majors (social sciences, humanities, biology, and chemistry) participated voluntarily in this study. One hundred ten individuals completed all questionnaires (90 were women, three were missing data on gender; Mage = 23.34, SD = 6.21). Procedure and materials. For all four studies, Institutional Review Board approval was obtained from the Institute of Psychology, University of Gdansk ethical committee. The questionnaire, with a fixed order of scales, was administered in small groups (15–25 participants per group). After a short introduction that presented the study as research on the relationship between personality traits and the perception of social groups, participants were asked to recall a big mass gathering they had attended recently, state the name of the event, when it took place, and the estimated number of total attendees. Then they answered questions using the following scales: identity fusion with other participants, self-expansion, agency and communion, and self-efficacy. Identity fusion. To assess the strength of a visceral feeling of oneness with a group, we used the Polish version (Besta, Gómez, & Vázquez, 2014) of the seven-item identity fusion scale (IFS; Gómez et al., 2011). We measured how close participants felt to other attendees and how strongly they included attendees in the self (e.g., “I felt one with other attendees of this event” and “I have a deep emotional bond with other participants”). Participants indicated their answers on a 6-point scale (0 = strongly disagree to 6 = strongly agree). The scale showed great reliability (Cronbach’s α = .91). Self-expansion. To tap the feeling of nonrelational self-expansion resulting from being part of a past mass gathering, we used the measure of the individual selfexpansion questionnaire (SEQ; Mattingly & Lewandowski, 2013). The five-item scale was backtranslated into Polish for this study. Participants were asked to recall the event they had written about and described how this event had influenced them (e.g., because of attendance, “Do you feel an increase in your ability to accomplish new things?” and “Do you feel a O156 Agentic self-perception. To use a measure of selfdescription of traits related to agency, we included agency-communion items based on traits used by Laurin, Kay, and Shepherd (2011; five items for agency, e.g., I am... “self-confident,” “competent”; five items for communion, such as “warm” and “caring”). Participants indicated their answers on a 7-point scale (0 = strongly disagree to 6 = strongly agree). Both scales showed acceptable reliability (α = .69 for agency and .70 for communion). In this study, we focused on the agency scale as it is related to our reasoning. Results and Discussion Preliminary analyses. For the events the participants recalled, 50% wrote down a music festival or concert, 14.5% recalled a sports event (e.g., football/soccer game), 13.6% recalled a personal event (e.g., a big wedding), and the other participants described other mass gatherings (e.g., pilgrimage, demonstration). Relationship among self-expansion, self-efficacy, and personal agency on different levels of identity fusion. To test hypothesis 1 on the link between self-expansion and self-efficacy and agency, we conducted correlation analyses. They showed that recalling a feeling of self-expansion as a result of participation in mass gatherings was related to self-efficacy (r = .23, p = .02) and personal agency (r = .17, p = .07) but not to communion (r = .07, p = .48). As the pilot study was correlational and we asked to assess the participants’ self-expansion caused by attendance at mass gatherings, we could not compare the relations to nonattendees. Moreover, with the retrospective methodology we could assess the vividness of recalling the event only indirectly. We divided the participants into three groups based on their identity fusion scores (low, with 1 SD from mean, average, and highly fused, with +1 SD from the mean) and ran separate correlational analyses. The difference between the strength of the relationship between self-expansion and self-efficacy among high-fused (r = .32) and low-fused (r = .22) individuals was statistically significant (Z = 2.26, p = .02). The same applied to the self-expansion–agency relation, with Pearson r higher among highly fused European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. T. Besta et al. What do I gain from joining crowds? (r = .38) than low-fused individuals (r = .17; Z = 2.32, p = .02). Results of the pilot study confirmed our assumption of the relationship between feelings of self-expansion and self-efficacy among the Polish student sample. This relationship was especially vivid when individuals recalled mass events when personal and group identity overlapped the most. Interestingly, for weakly fused individuals, the relationship between self-expansion and efficacy and agency was negative. This effect could be related to the fact that perception of belonging to the larger group (i.e., inclusion of others in the self that results in identity fusion) facilitates the relationship between the feeling of self-expansion and self-efficacy. That is, if, in the context of the group relation, self-expansion is strong among people who did not adhere to the group, it is not necessarily linked to the increased efficacy beliefs. An important limitation of the pilot study was also that the participants may have experienced the reported events (the music festival, spot event, family gatherings) in different ways; thus, this could have influenced their responses. Based on the results of the pilot study and to overcome its limitations, we decided to conduct studies among crowd participants. In Study 1, we explored whether the survey conducted during a day-long music festival among attendees would confirm this relationship. We also extended the pilot study by adding a measure of group efficacy to assess whether—as we proposed in hypothesis 2—the feeling of self-expansion is a mediator of the identity fusion–group efficacy relationship. Study 1 and group identity in the context of other festival participants (e.g., “I have a deep emotional bond with other participants”). The reliability was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = .91). Self-expansion. We used the same scale, the SEQ, as in the pilot study to assess what people gained from attending the festival (e.g., because of attendance, “Do you feel a greater awareness of things?”). The reliability was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = .90). Agentic self-perception. To measure selfdescription of agentic and communal traits, we included a scale based on adjectives used by Laurin et al. (2011; six items for agency, e.g., I am... “self-confident,” and six items for communion, e.g., “caring”). Both scales showed acceptable reliability (α = .72 for agency and .79 for communion). Self-efficacy. We included a three-item scale (the GES) to assess general beliefs about one’s self-efficacy (“If I do my best, I can solve most problems I have to deal with”; “When unexpected problems arise, I can usually find several solutions”; “I am confident that I could deal efficiently with life’s challenges”). The reliability was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = .89). Group efficacy. We assessed group efficacy with one item: “I think that together with other fans of the music we can together work toward our common goals” (based on an item used by Van Zomeren et al., 2004). Subjects responded on a 6-point scale (1 = not at all to 7 = very much). Method Results and Discussion Participants. Attendees at three music festivals that took place in Poland participated voluntarily in Study 1. As all measures were in Polish, only Poles were included in the analyses. The festivals varied from medium (around 1000 participants) to large (around 30 000 participants). A total of 781 individuals participated in the study (469 women, 24 were missing data on gender; Mage = 27.88, SD = 8.39). Procedure and materials. While waiting for the concerts to start or listening to the live music, festival attendees were approached by a research assistant and asked to voluntarily participate in the study. Participants filled out a questionnaire that among other measures not relevant to this study (i.e., on music preferences) included the identity fusion scale with the target group festival participants, the agency and communion scale, and the self-expansion scale and items measuring selfefficacy and group efficacy to tap general self-efficacy beliefs (see Table 1 for details). Identity fusion. This time, we used the seven-item verbal IFS to assess overlap between personal identity Preliminary analyses. We conducted factor analyses to explore whether self-expansion and identity fusion are related to two independent constructs. After we inserted items from the self-expansion scale and the identity fusion measure, the analyses with direct oblimin rotation showed a two-factorial solution that explained 68% of the variance. Items from the selfexpansion scale were part of one factor (with factor loadings from .80 to .89), and items from the identity fusion scale were part of the second factor (with factor loadings from .70 to .90). Similarly, as self-expansion and measures of efficacy beliefs could be seen as tapping into similar constructs, we conducted factor analyses for items from the self-expansion scale, self-efficacy, and one item measure of group efficacy. Analyses with direct oblimin and a fixed number of three factors revealed that the items from the three scales were divided into three factors (factor one included items from the self-expansion scale, with loadings from .74 to .92; factor two with one item for group efficacy that loaded with .86; and factor three with items from the self-efficacy scale that loaded from .90 to .93). European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. O157 T. Besta et al. What do I gain from joining crowds? Table 1 Descriptive statistics for measures used in Study 1, n = 781; Study 2, n = 103; and Study 3, n = 69 Variable Self-expansion Self-efficacy Group-efficacy Agency Communion Study 2 Identity fusion Self-expansion Group-efficacy Collective action Study 3 Identity fusion Self-expansion Group-efficacy Collective action Scale items (range) Reliability 5 (1–7) 3 (1–7) 1 (1–7) 6 (1–7) 6 (1–7) 7 (0–6) 5 (1–7) 1 (1–7) 3 (1–7) 7 (0–6) 5 (1–7) 1 (1–7) 3 (1–7) .90 .89 .72 .79 .92 .92 .91 .86 .90 .97 Relationship between self-expansion, personal agency, and self-efficacy. To test hypothesis 1, we conducted zero-order correlation analyses. They confirmed the predictions and showed that all variables used in Study 1 were correlated. As in the pilot study, a feeling of self-expansion was positively related to selfefficacy and personal agency (see Table 2). Identity fusion, group efficacy, and self-expansion as a mediator. To test hypothesis 2 concerning the role of self-expansion in the identity fusion–group efficacy relationship, we conducted a simple mediation analysis using the PROCESS bootstrapping macro (Hayes, 2013). The fusion of personal and group identity was indirectly related to group efficacy beliefs through the effect on the feeling of self-expansion. Stronger fusion with fellow festival participants and inclusion of this group in the self were related to the feeling of self-expansion resulting in new knowledge, and the feeling of having a larger perspective on reality (a = .55). Participants who felt greater self-expansion expressed stronger beliefs that together with other festival attendees they could attain common goals (b = .40). A bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for the indirect effect (ab = .218) based on 5000 bootstrap samples was entirely above zero (0.17 to 0.28). In addition to an indirect effect, there was a significant direct effect of fusion on group efficacy, showing overlap between personal and group identity related to beliefs about the greater group’s ability to obtain common goals (c’ = 0.386, p < .001) (see Figure 1). Minimum Maximum 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.33 1.67 .00 1.00 1.00 1.00 .00 1.00 1.00 1.00 7.00 7.00 7.00 7.00 7.00 6.00 7.00 7.00 7.00 5.17 7.00 7.00 7.00 Mean 4.10 5.05 3.65 5.34 4.79 2.97 3.78 5.18 4.53 2.97 4.45 5.50 5.46 SD 1.36 1.37 1.68 0.81 0.81 1.45 1.39 1.48 1.60 1.21 1.40 1.60 1.85 Study 1 showed that the suggested feeling of selfexpansion resulting from participation in mass gatherings could be useful in explaining the relationship between group identification and group efficacy. However, this study was conducted during music festivals, events that are not directly linked to collective action. Thus, Studies 2 and 3 were conducted during events related to protests and collective actions. In these studies, we examined the proposed mediational model with identity fusion as a predictor, self-expansion and group efficacy as mediators, and collective action tendency as the dependent variable. Study 2 Method Participants. Study 2 was conducted among people participating in Great Critical Mass Tricity 2015, a gathering of bicycle activists and cycling enthusiasts. The demonstration took place in the metropolitan area of Gdansk–Sopot–Gdynia (Tricity) in northern Poland. One hundred and three participants voluntarily filled out questionnaires (45 women, four were missing data on gender; Mage = 33.76, SD = 11.48). Procedure and materials. Just after the Great Critical Mass event ended research assistants asked event attendees to voluntarily participate in the study. Those who agreed filled out the questionnaires. This time, we controlled for the scale order. The questionnaires included the identity fusion scale, the selfexpansion scale, the group efficacy scale, and Table 2 Study 1. Zero-order correlations with Person’s r Self-expansion Identity fusion Self-expansion Self-efficacy Group-efficacy Agency .48*** Self-efficacy .13*** .17*** Group-efficacy .43*** .45*** .25*** Agency .17*** .25*** .32*** .26*** Communion .20*** .21*** .37*** .29*** .66*** ***p < .001. O158 European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. T. Besta et al. What do I gain from joining crowds? l l l l l Fig. 1: Study 1. Model of identity fusion with participants as predictor of group efficacy, with mediator self-expansion; n = 781 collective action tendencies, as well as measures not related to this article (e.g., questions about commuting routines and motives for using a bike; see Table 1 for details). Identity fusion. We used the seven-item IFS to assess overlap between personal identity and group identity in the context of other people participating in the demonstration (e.g., “I have a deep emotional bond with other participants”). The reliability was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = .92). Self-expansion. We used the same SEQ scale as in previous studies to assess what people gained from attending this demonstration (e.g., because of attendance, “Do you feel a greater awareness of things?”). The reliability was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = .92). Group efficacy. We assessed group efficacy with one item, “I think that together with other participants we can together work to change cyclists’ situation” (based on an item used by Van Zomeren et al., 2004). Collective action tendencies. To assess collective action tendencies, we used three items based on scale used by Van Zomeren et al. (2004; e.g., “I am willing to take part in protests and demonstrations to improve the policy on cycling and bicycle use”). Participants indicated their answers on a 7-point scale (1 = not at all to 7 = very much). Results Preliminary analyses. Preliminary correlation analyses showed that all variables used in Study 2 were correlated, with the strongest link between identity fusion and self-expansion and between group efficacy and collective action (see Table 3). Moreover, we analyzed the identity fusion of the participants for each festival. Post-hoc Bonferroni tests revealed that identity fusion was strongest among the participants of the biggest festival (M = 2.90) compared to the medium size festival (M = 2.65), and the smallest festival (M = 2.57). Identity fusion did not differ statistically significantly among participants of the medium and smallest festivals. As in Study 1, we also conducted factor analyses to explore whether self-expansion and identity fusion are related to two independent constructs. After we inserted items from the self-expansion scale and the identity fusion measure, the analyses with direct oblimin rotation showed a two-factorial solution that explained 73% of the variance. Items from the self-expansion scale were part of one factor (with factor loadings from .82 to .90), and items from the identity fusion scale were part of the second factor (with factor loadings from .62 to .84). Similarly, we conducted factor analyses for items from the self-expansion scale, and one item that measured group efficacy. Analyses with direct oblimin and a fixed number of two factors revealed that the items from the two scales were divided into two factors that explain 80% of the variance (factor one includes items from the self-expansion scale, with loadings from .77 to .91; factor two had one item for group efficacy that loads with .98). Table 3 Study 2. Zero-order correlations with Person’s r Identity fusion Self-expansion Group-efficacy Self-expansion Group-efficacy Collective action .61*** .22* .36*** .25* .36*** .59*** *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. O159 T. Besta et al. What do I gain from joining crowds? Self-expansion as a mediator of fusion– collective action tendencies. To test the mediation models, we conducted a series of mediational analyses. First, we analyzed the model proposed by Van Zomeren et al. (2010). Based on the PROCESS bootstrapping macro (Hayes, 2013), we included group efficacy as a predictor, the collective action tendency as the proposed mediator, and identity fusion with other participants as the dependent variable. The mediation analyses were not statistically significant. A bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for the indirect effect (ab = .10) based on 5000 bootstrap samples included zero ( 0.04 to 0.26). We tested whether the relationship between group and personal identity fusion (the predictor) and collective action tendencies (the dependent variable) was mediated by group efficacy as the second model. The indirect effect was not statistically significant, with a confidence interval (ab = .14) that included zero ( 0.004 to 0.29). Finally, we tested hypothesis 3 stating that the feeling of self-expansion mediates the fusion–collective action tendencies relationship. To explore whether the indirect effect of fusion on collective action tendencies through the feeling of self-expansion and group efficacy is statistically significant, we conducted a third mediation analysis using the PROCESS bootstrapping macro. We added an additional variable to the second model (that is, feeling of self-expansion as the first mediator, leaving group efficacy as the second mediator; see Figure 2). Stronger fusion with the gathering’s participants was related to a feeling of self-expansion (a1 = .58). Participants who felt greater self-expansion as a result of their Critical Mass attendance expressed stronger beliefs that together with other bicycle activists they could be efficient in pursuing common goals (d21 = .38). This group efficacy belief, however, was positively related to collective action tendencies (b2 = .57). A bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for the indirect effect (a1d21b2 = .13) based on 5000 bootstrap samples was entirely above zero (0.05 to 0.26). Fusion was not independently related to collective action tendencies in addition to the indirect effect through the feeling of self-expansion and group efficacy (c’ = 0.04, p = .70). Results of Study 2 confirmed that inclusion of the feeling of self-expansion caused by the gathering’s attendance helped in explaining the relationship between strong group and personal identity fusion and tendencies to act on behalf of the group. We specifically showed that in the natural condition of participation in a mass gathering, fusion with other participants could be a link to action tendencies because of the feeling of self-extension related to the crowd experience. In Study 3, we aimed to replicate the indirect effect obtained in Study 2. This time, we conducted research among LGBT rights activists. The study took place during the Equality Days events that attracted supporters of equality rights for nonheterosexual individuals. Study 3 Method Participants. In Study 3, attendees who supported equal rights for LGBT individuals, sympathizers, and organizers of Equality Days in Poland (69 persons, 44 women, three did not state their gender; Mage = 29.76, SD = 9.50) with a minimum age of 18 and a maximum age of 66. l ll l l l l Fig. 2: Study 2. Model of identity fusion with participants as predictor of collective action tendencies, with mediator self-expansion and groupefficacy; n = 103 O160 European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. T. Besta et al. What do I gain from joining crowds? Procedure and materials. Activists and people involved in Equality Days in Gdansk who participated in the gatherings related to this event (e.g., LGBT equal rights conferences and discussion groups; each event attracted around 50–80 people) were asked to voluntarily participate in a study and fill out the questionnaire. Research assistants approached participants during or at the end of each event. We controlled for the scale order. See Table 1 for detailed information about the scales used. Identity fusion. As in previous studies, we used the IFS to assess the personal and group identity overlap in the context of the Equality Days participants (e.g., “I have a deep emotional bond with other participants”). The reliability was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = .86). Self-expansion. We used the same SEQ scale as in previous studies to assess what people gained from attending Equality Days events (e.g., because of attendance, “Do you feel a greater awareness of things?”). The reliability was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = .90). Group efficacy. We assessed group efficacy with one item, “I think that together with others we can together work to change sexual minorities’ situation” (based on an item used by Van Zomeren et al., 2004). Collective action tendencies. To assess collective action tendencies, we used three items based on a scale used by Van Zomeren et al. (2004; e.g., “I am willing to take part in protests and demonstrations for the rights of sexual minorities”). The reliability was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = .97). Results Preliminary analyses. Preliminary correlation analyses showed that most of the variables used in Study 3 were correlated, although this time identity fusion was not related to collective action tendencies and group efficacy (see Table 4). Despite this lack of a relationship, we conducted indirect effects analysis in line with reasoning presented by Preacher and Hayes (2008) that indirect effects could be analyzed even when the relationship between two variables is not statistically significant. As in previous studies, we also conducted factor analyses to explore whether self-expansion and identity Table 4 Study 3. Zero-order correlations with Person’s r Identity fusion Self-expansion Group-efficacy *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. Self-expansion Group-efficacy Collective action .28* .18 .38*** .10 .50*** .80*** fusion are related to the two independent constructs. After we inserted items from the self-expansion scale and the identity fusion measure, analyses with direct oblimin rotation showed a two-factorial solution that explained 65% of the variance. Items from the selfexpansion scale were part of one factor (with factor loadings from .71 to .91), and items from the identity fusion scale were part of the second factor (with factor loadings from .68 to .81). Similarly, analyses with direct oblimin rotation revealed that items from the selfexpansion and group efficacy scales are divided into two factors that explain 78% of the variance (factor one includes items from the self-expansion scale, with loadings from .80 to .89; factor two had one item for group efficacy that loaded with .81). Self-expansion as a mediator of fusion– collective action tendencies. As in previous studies to test mediation models, we used the PROCESS bootstrapping macro (Hayes, 2013). We explored the alternative to the model we proposed with group efficacy as the predictor, collective action tendency as the proposed mediator, and identity fusion with other participants as the dependent variable. As in Study 2, the mediation analyses were not statistically significant. A bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for the indirect effect (ab = .07) based on 5000 bootstrap samples included zero ( 0.34 to 0.14). We tested whether the relationship between group and personal identity fusion (predictor) and collective action tendencies (dependent variable) is mediated by group efficacy as the second model. The indirect effect was not statistically significant, with a confidence interval for the indirect effect (ab = .22) that included zero ( 0.05 to 0.50). To explore the role of feeling of self-expansion, we conducted a third mediation analysis, with identity fusion as a predictor, self-expansion as the first mediator, group efficacy as the second mediator, and collective action tendencies as the dependent variable. This time, as with Study 2, the indirect effect of fusion on collective action tendencies through a feeling of selfexpansion and group efficacy turned out to be statistically significant (see Figure 3). Including the group of equal rights activists in the self-concept was related to a feeling of self-expansion (a1 = .32). Participants who felt greater self-expansion as a result of event attendance expressed stronger beliefs that together with other activists they could be efficient in pursuing common goals (d21 = .41). Finally, this group efficacy belief was positively related to collective action tendencies (b2 = .83). A bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for the indirect effect (a1d21b2 = .11) based on 5000 bootstrap samples was entirely above zero (0.02 to 0.32). Fusion was not independently related to collective action tendencies in addition to the indirect effect through the feeling of self-expansion and group efficacy (c’ = .016, p = .17). That confirmed hypothesis 3. Moreover, the indirect effect of fusion on collective action tendencies through a feeling of self-expansion European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. O161 T. Besta et al. What do I gain from joining crowds? l ll l l l l Fig. 3: Study 3. Model of identity fusion with participants as predictor of collective action tendencies, with mediator self-expansion and groupefficacy; n = 69 only was statistically significant (a1b1 = .11; interval 0.01 to 0.31). General Discussion What do we gain from joining crowds? Participation in crowd gatherings generates the specific social context that triggers self-expansion and therefore leads to acting on behalf of the group. Instead of losing one’s identity, an individual actualizes his or her social identity through interactions with other participants and experiencing the crowd. The results are in line with current models of crowd behavior that are critical of irrationalist approaches. That is, thanks to research on emerging group norms and self-objectification, we explained behaviors during mass gatherings without faulty assumptions about people being irrational and prone to violence for no apparent reason (Drury et al., 2005; Drury & Reicher, 2009). In the present studies, perceived self-expansion was linked to people’s action intentions, and those who felt an expansion of their selves in the context of a mass gathering were more responsive to the group’s goals and endorse stronger group efficacy beliefs. Consistent with the selfexpansion model, individuals with an expanded selfconcept also reported greater group efficacy. We believe the studies presented in this article extend previous research on crowd behavior and collective action. In the pilot study, among participants who recalled a big mass gathering, a feeling of self-expansion was related to self-efficacy and personal agency. These relations were especially significant among participants highly fused with a group. In Study 1, we continued the present research within natural settings, and we included as participants attendees at three music festivals. Analysis revealed that self-expansion was a mediator of the relationship between identity fusion and group efficacy. Finally, the results for Studies 2 and 3 conducted during the Great Critical Mass and O162 Equality Days confirmed our assumption that selfexpansion mediates the relationship between identityrelated variables and collective action tendency. These studies enrich the self-based perspective on motivation to participate in collective actions. Similarly to previous studies (Drury & Reicher, 2000, 2009; van Zomeren et al., 2008), we analyzed the changes in the self-concept to explain why group members could be more willing to engage in future collective actions. In the case of the present studies, the feeling of selfexpansion could be a plausible answer. However, as Van Zomeren (2015) noted, a self-based perspective has limitations because it describes instead of explaining such a change in self-concept and does not answer the question of what leads nonactivists to see themselves as activists. We hope that the current studies shed more light on the group efficacy ➔ group identification ➔collective action pathway to overcome these limitations. By demonstrating the role of self-expansion in four studies, we proposed an explanation for how the experience of being part of a mass gathering may promote participation in future collective actions. Our findings also confirmed the assumption that group efficacy is a potential predictor of collective actions (Mummenday et al., 1999). Self-expansion as a result of interaction with others and experience of being with a crowd gives people a sense of collective empowerment, based on which people perceive themselves as able to pursue collective goals. Those goals can be adopted as personally important. Thus, a group who wants to engage its participants in collective actions might start by fostering opportunities for expansion of the members’ self-concept. The psychological meaning of crowd gatherings is to give participants the opportunity to interact and therefore to actualize their social identity that serves to mobilize people for social change. Our results provide a new potential insight into the group identity–collective action relationship based on identity fusion and self-expansion theories. In these European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. T. Besta et al. studies, the self-expansion construct originally developed in a nonrelational context by Mattingly and Lewandowski (2013) was adopted in the social context of a mass gathering. We assumed that the crowd experience might be understood as something novel, exciting, and interesting. Although we treated self-expansion as resulting from an inclusion of group in the self, the higher agency of individuals fused with groups (Besta et al., 2015) indicated a more complex picture that goes beyond typical markers of strong identification as unity, oneness, solidarity, and self-sacrifice (Swann et al., 2014). Identity fusion as a visceral feeling of being part of the group could be an important factor in understanding crowd processes, adding another dimension to the research on cognitive categorization. The novel and interesting experience of participation in Equality Days or the Great Critical Mass may lead people who fused with a group to see their self-concept in more diverse, extended ways (Mattingly & Lewandowski, 2013). In these studies, we found self-expansion was an important contextual trigger variable that mediates the relationship between fusion with other participants and collective action tendency. The context of crowd gatherings probably stimulates participants’ transitions to the emergent social identity. Although we concentrated on identity fusion and not group identification, the present results are in accordance with the analysis of Reicher and colleagues (Drury & Reicher, 2005; Reicher, 2004) that focused on the ways social identities are mobilized to give a feeling of power to individuals who would otherwise be relatively powerless. In a similar vein, van Zomeren et al. (2008) proposed a social identity model of collective action in which social identity understood in more dynamic terms predicts collective action directly, as well as indirectly through group efficacy. The mechanism that shapes the tendency to collective action probably varies depending on the level of identity fusion. Those with a strong overlap between personal and group identity would probably be more willing to see the enactment of group identity among other group members as a positive experience related to the feeling of selfexpansion. This could strengthen their tendency to act collectively in the future. It also helps to fulfill the psychological need for agency: Through experiencing the crowd and expressing their group identity, individuals achieve a sense of individual and collective agency. In the present research, we concentrated on identity fusion as a construct related to strong group adherence. As we noted in the introduction, the status of this construct is still debatable. Swann and others (e.g., Swann & Buhrmester, 2015) proposed that fusion is distinct from group identification and better predicts radical behaviors than various measures of group identification. Nevertheless, the high correlation between measures of fusion and group identification (Besta et al., 2016) could also imply that identity fusion describes a specific form of extremely strong identification, based on relational ties, group solidarity, and mutual reciprocity. The relatively low scores for identity What do I gain from joining crowds? fusion even when measured during collective events (i.e., during mass gatherings in the present studies) support the notion that fusion is rare among in-group members. Possibly, fusion is a state when a social or group identity is no longer activated contextually, but instead influences one’s behavior regardless of whether the intergroup context is salient or not. In this view, the relationship between identify fusion and extreme sacrifices on behalf of the group could be easily understandable as high identity fusion would be declared by people whose personal self-concept is merged with the group schema and norms. Thus, these individuals often enact their group identity even when they are in an individualized situation. Future studies could further explore the relationship between the concept of identity fusion and group identification. For example, Swann et al. (2012) highlights that fusion is relatively stable, and once fused people will tend to remain fused. On the other hand, highly identified individuals tend to be more guided and influenced by external contextual conditions. Thus, future longitudinal studies could explore whether indeed identity fusion is more stable than identification with group, and whether effect of various contextual factors (i.e. salience of intergroup relationship) is more visible within identification measures than in fusion scale. Moreover, as fusion is defined as visceral feeling of oneness with a group, it could be helpful to analyze the relationship between fusion and identification with affect and visceral reaction to in-group threat. This would help to verify whether fusion indeed differs from identification, that is, whether fusion leads to stronger emotional and physiological reactions related to group membership. Future studies on differentiation between these two concepts should also include various measures of identification, especially these that are not limited to selfcategorization. The results also shed a slightly different light on the distinction between symbolic social identity motives and instrumental motives for collective action (Blackwood & Louis, 2012), where the former are related to expressed group emotions and group identity and the latter to collective efficacy, problem-solving, or personal analyses of costs and benefits. The mediation model presented here indicated the interdependence of those motives instead of differentiation. Positive emotions related to the feeling of self-expansion and experiencing the expanded self-concept as more capable (Mattingly & Lewandowski, 2013) may lead to collective efficacy and to collective actions. Similar to other sociopsychological processes that are context dependent, the problem of long-term changes in collective action tendency seems noteworthy. We speculate that if the expansion of the self-concept is caused by engaging in self-expanding activities and interacting with a group, every social movement interested in pursuing long-term goals must provide participants with the opportunity to actualize their group identity and to experience the positive outcomes of self-expansion. For example, Nasie, Bar-Tal, and European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. O163 T. Besta et al. What do I gain from joining crowds? Shnaidman (2014), in analyzing the activism of an Israeli radical peace organization, showed that participating in political actions constitutes the socialization framework for adolescents, giving them the possibility of reinforcement of their values and providing them with new skills. In terms of the present results, it means that these socialization frameworks included the possibility of self-expansion. Important limitations of the present studies should be highlighted as well. The present research line is correlational, and experimental validation of the proposed paths should be encouraged. We also measured group efficacy with one item scale. Although we based our methods on methods used in previous studies that employed similar measures (Van Zomeren et al., 2004), it could be seen as an important limitation of these presented results, and more elaborate measures should be used in future research. Moreover, it could be important to include established measures of group identification to compare the strength of the relationship between the identity variables (i.e., fusion and group identification) and collective action tendencies. We assume that measuring self-expansion allows us to concentrate on perceived identity gains (i.e., that result from participating in new activities or developing new perspectives by interacting with various people). However, the limitations of this position should be pointed out. We did not control for previous identification with the cause or group. As research on opinion-based group membership has suggested (McGarty, Bliuc, Thomas, & Bongiorno, 2009), individuals might be motivated to attend gatherings that involve a cause already important to them and one with which they strongly identify before they engage in collective action. Thus, instead of forming new identities or expanding the self-construct during gatherings, people might strengthen one that already is established. This perspective is important and could suggest that the self-expansion we measured in studies is not necessarily related to forming a new identity as a crowd participant. Broader Implications and Future Research To expand the generalizability of the results, the research should be extended further, and the relationship between social identity, group efficacy, and collective actions and the role of self-expansion in the relationship should be examined, in a different cultural context, for example, a more collectivistic or individualistic context than the Polish cultural context. This research was a questionnaire-based study, and thus, uture studies in experimental settings would be important follow-ups. In addition, applying the mechanisms presented in the current studies to analyze the relationships between activists’ identity and engagement with prosocial actions would be interesting. This prediction also extends to possible behavioral measures of O164 progroup activities. For example, a self-expanding event could lead to stronger engagement in other collective actions and events in the future. It could be also important to examine whether similar mechanisms of self-expansion by experiencing crowds could be identified in different activist actions, as well as whether some of the activists’ involvements are more related to selfexpansion than others. This study did not answer the question of which relational model (Fiske, 1992) may emerge from such interaction with other participants during crowd gatherings. As Van Zomeren (2015) argued, changes in the self-concept result from changes in relational models with out-groups and in-groups. The present studies concentrated on in-group involvement and the consequences for the self-concept, not on the perception of a relationship with others. Adopting the relational perspective of collective action and integrating it with mechanisms of self-expansion may enrich future studies. Outside the activist domain, it could be interesting to relate the proposed model to the context of collective behavior and to different kinds of psychological experience associated with participation in gatherings, such as communities formed by music festival audiences. Increasingly, people use music as a means of formulating and expressing their individual identities and present themselves to others (Hargreaves, Miell, & MacDonald, 2002), whereas the social functions of music include establishing social identity (North, Hargreaves, & O’Neill, 2000) and social atmosphere (Laukka, 2007) and facilitating social bonding (Huron, 2001). Packer and Ballantyne’s (2011) literature review showed that participation in music festivals offers opportunities for the participants to engage in identity work—to define, develop, or reflect on their understanding of themselves and to cultivate new expressions of self-identity and social identity. Being part of a festival audience involves breaking away from reality and engaging in intense, concentrated interactions, which, in turn, facilitate building relationships between the participants (Ballantyne, Ballantyne, & Packer, 2014). Therefore, it seems justified to assume that greater openness to new kinds of experiences, exploration of new relationships, new ways of perceiving others, and understanding oneself give rise to self-acceptance, self-realization, and personal growth (see Ryan & Deci, 2000). Participants in this specific type of community (a music festival audience) become more positive about themselves, other group members, and—in some cases—life in general (Packer & Ballantyne, 2011). These considerations are further supported by empirical evidence of multiple positive psychological outcomes of participation in live music events (Lamont, 2012). However, thus far, few attempts have been made to develop a theoretical model that integrates and explains the causal relationship between these mechanisms, which, instead, have been studied separately (see Groarke & Hogan, 2016). European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. T. Besta et al. What do I gain from joining crowds? Conclusions The shared social identity of those who engage in crowd gatherings could lead to a feeling of positivity about the experience with other members of the group (Neville & Reicher, 2011). The strength of group identity is also related to group efficacy and to collective action tendencies (Van Zomeren et al., 2008). However, the idea of the direct or indirect influence of shared identities and group identification on progroup actions becomes unsubstantiated when group membership is made salient in lab experiments or among student samples. The study results revealed a psychological pathway through which mass gatherings of similarly minded people, the majority of whom are strangers nevertheless, could create a feeling of communal efficacy that leads to stronger willingness to engage in actions on behalf of the group. The important factor, based on the present results, is the perception that interactions with members of the group that form a given gathering develop, enrich, and extend one’s self-concept. Specifically, for individuals fused with other participants, the feeling of selfexpansion by experiencing the crowd fosters the perception of group efficacy, which, in turn, increases people’s willingness to declare personal support for future progroup actions. From this point of view, caring for communal interest and group goals on the part of persons who included the group in the self and feel self-developed thanks to being in the crowd is not without self-interest. They gain something from participation. These identity gains, broader perspectives, and acquired new capabilities lead these individuals to believe that together with others they can achieve common goals and that some struggles to obtain those goals are worth a try. Acknowledgements We would like to thank Marta Drzewiczewska, Piotr Egler, Anna Gadomska, and Aleksandra Pytlos for their assistance in gathering the data, as well as Dr. Anna Strzałkowska for her help in organizing and conducting Study 3. The presented research and preparation of this article were supported by a grant Tomasz Besta received from the Narodowe Centrum Nauki in Poland, #2014/14/E/HS6/00587. Supporting information Additional Supporting Information may be found online in the supporting information tab for this article. References Aron, A., Lewandowski, G. W. Jr., Mashek, D., & Aron, E. N. (2013). The self-expansion model of motivation and cognition in close relationships. In J. A. Simpson, & L. Campbell (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of close relationships (pp. 90–115). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Ballantyne, J., Ballantyne, R., & Packer, J. (2014). Designing and managing music festival experiences to enhance attendees’ psychological and social benefits. Musicae Scientiae, 18(1), 65–83. https://doi.org/10.1177/1029 864913511845 Becker, J. C., & Tausch, N. (2015). A dynamic model of engagement in normative and non-normative collective action: Psychological antecedents, consequences, and barriers. European Review of Social Psychology, 26(1), 43–92. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2015.1094265 Besta, T., Gómez, Á., & Vázquez, A. (2014). Readiness to deny group’s wrongdoing and willingness to fight for its members: The role of the Poles’ identity fusion with the country and religious group. Current Issues of Personality Psychology, 2(1), 49–55. https://doi.org/10.5114/cipp.2 014.43101 Besta, T., Mattingly, B., & Błażek, M. (2016). When membership gives strength to act: Inclusion of the group into the self and feeling of personal agency. The Journal of Social Psychology, 156(1), 56–73. https://doi.org/10.1080/002245 45.2015.1053838 Besta, T., Szulc, M., & Jaśkiewicz, M. (2015). Political extremism, group membership, and personality traits: Who accepts violence? Revista de Psicología Social: International Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 563–585. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/02134748.2015.1065085 Blackwood, L. M., & Louis, W. R. (2012). If it matters for the group then it matters to me: Collective action outcomes for seasoned activists. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(1), 72–92. Doosje, B., Spears, R., & Ellemers, N. (2002). Social identity as both cause and effect: The development of group identification in response to anticipated and actual changes in the inter-group status hierarchy. British Journal of Social Psychology, 41(1), 57–76. Drury, J., Cocking, C., Beale, J., Hanson, C., & Rapley, F. (2005). The phenomenology of empowerment in collective action. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44(3), 309–328. Drury, J., Cocking, C., & Reicher, S. (2009). The nature of collective resilience: Survivor reactions to the 2005 London bombings. International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters, 27(1), 66–95. Drury, J., & Reicher, S. (2000). Collective action and psychological change: The emergence of new social identities. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39(4), 579–604. Drury, J., & Reicher, S. (2005). Explaining enduring empowerment: A comparative study of collective action and psychological outcomes. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35(1), 35–58. Drury, J., & Reicher, S. D. (2009). Collective psychological empowerment as a model of social change: Researching crowds and power. Journal of Social Issues, 65(4), 707–725. Ellemers, N. (1993). The influence of socio-structural variables identity management strategies. In W. Stroebe, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (pp. 27–50). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99(4), 689–723. Gómez, A., Brooks, M. L., Buhrmester, M. D., Vázquez, A., Jetten, J., & Swann, W. B. Jr. (2011). On the nature of identity fusion: insights into the construct and a new European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. O165 T. Besta et al. What do I gain from joining crowds? measure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 918–933. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022642 Groarke, J. M., & Hogan, M. J. (2016). Enhancing wellbeing: An emerging model of the adaptive functions of music listening. Psychology of Music, 44(4), 769–791. https://doi. org/10.1177/0305735615591844 Hargreaves, D., Miell, D., & MacDonald, R. (2002). What are musical identities and why are they important? In R. MacDonald, & D. Hargreaves (Eds.), Musical identities (pp. 1–20). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: The Guilford Press. Hornsey, M., Blackwood, L., Louis, W., Fielding, K., Favor, K., Morton, T., et al. (2006). Why do people engage in collective action? Revisiting the role of perceived effectiveness. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36(7), 1701–1722. Huron, D. (2001). Is music an evolutionary adaptation? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930(1), 43–61. Kosakowska-Berezecka, N., Besta, T., Adamska, K., Jaśkiewicz, M., Jurek, P., & Vandello, J. A. (2016). If my masculinity is threatened I won’t support gender equality? The role of agentic self-stereotyping in restoration of manhood and perception of gender relations. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 17(3), 274–284. https://doi.org/10.1037/ men0000016 Lamont, A. (2012). Emotion, engagement and meaning in strong experiences of music performance. Psychology of Music, 40(5), 574–594. https://doi.org/10.1177/030 5735612448510 Laukka, P. (2007). Uses of music and psychological wellbeing among the elderly. Journal of Happiness Studies, 8(2), 215–241. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-006-9024-3 Laurin, K., Kay, A. C., & Shepherd, S. (2011). Self-stereotyping as a route to system justification. Social Cognition, 29(2), 360–375. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2011.29.3.360 Luszczynska, A., Gutiérrez-Doña, B., & Schwarzer, R. (2005). General self efficacy in various domains of human functioning: Evidence from five countries. International Journal of Psychology, 40(2), 80–89. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/00207590444000041 Mattingly, B. A., & Lewandowski, G. W. Jr. (2013). An expanded Self is a more capable Self: the association between Self-concept size and Self-efficacy. Self and Identity, 12(6), 621–634. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2012.718863 Mattingly, B. A., Mcintyre, K. P., & Lewandowski, G. W. Jr. (2012). Approach motivation and the expansion of self in close relationships. Personal Relationships, 19(1), 113–127. McGarty, C., Bliuc, A. M., Thomas, E. F., & Bongiorno, R. (2009). Collective action as the material expression of opinion-based group membership. Journal of Social Issues, 65(4), 839–839. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.15404560.2009.01627.x Mummendey, A., Kessler, T., Klink, A., & Mielke, R. (1999). Strategies to cope with negative social identity: Predictions by social identity theory and relative deprivation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(2), 229–245. Nasie, M., Bar-Tal, D., & Shnaidman, O. (2014). Activists in Israeli radical peace organizations: Their personal stories about joining and taking part in these organizations. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 20(3), 313–329. https://doi.org/10.1037/pac0000040 O166 Neville, F., & Reicher, S. (2011). The experience of collective participation: Shared identity, relatedness and emotionality. Contemporary Social Science, 6(3), 377–396. https://doi. org/10.1080/21582041.2012.627277 North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., & O’Neill, S. A. (2000). The importance of music to adolescents. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70(2), 255–272. https://doi.org/ 10.1348/000709900158083 Onorato, R. S., & Turner, J. C. (2004). Fluidity in the selfconcept: the shift from personal to social identity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34(3), 257–278. Packer, J., & Ballantyne, J. (2011). The impact of music festival attendance on young people’s psychological and social well-being. Psychology of Music, 39(2), 164–181. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735610372611 Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879–879. Reicher, S. D. (1996). `The battle of Westminster’: Developing the social identity model of crowd behaviour in order to explain the initiation and development of collective conflict. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26(1), 115–134. Reicher, S. D. (2004). The context of social identity: Domination, resistance, and change. Political Psychology, 25(6), 921–945. Reicher, S., & Haslam, S. A. (2013). Towards a ‘science of movement’: Identity, authority and influence in the production of social stability and social change. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 1(1), 112–131. https://doi. org/10.5964/jspp.v1i1.266 Ryan, R., & Deci, M. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78. https:// doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68 Stekelenburg, J. V., & Klandermans, B. (2013). The social psychology of protest. Current Sociology Review, 61(5–6), 886–905. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011392113479314 Swann, W. B. Jr., & Buhrmester, M. D. (2015). Identity fusion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(1), 52–57. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414551363 Swann, W. B., Buhrmester, M. D., Gómez, M., Jettem, J., Bastian, B., Vázquez, A., … Zhang, A. (2014). What makes a group worth dying for? Identity fusion fosters perception of familial ties, promoting self-sacrifice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(6), 912–926. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/a0036089 Swann, W. B. Jr., Gómez, Á., Seyle, D. C., Morales, J. F., & Huici, C. (2009). Identity fusion: The interplay of personal and social identities in extreme group behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 995–1011. https:// doi.org/10.1037/a0013668 Swann, W. B. Jr., Jetten, J., Gómez, Á., Whitehouse, H., & Bastian, B. (2012). When group membership gets personal: A theory of identity fusion. Psychological Review, 119(3), 441–456. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028589 Thomas, E. F., McGarty, C., & Mavor, K. I. (2009). Aligning identities, emotions, and beliefs to create commitment to sustainable social and political action. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(3), 194–218. Van Zomeren, M. (2015). Collective action as relational interaction: A new relational hypothesis on how non-activists European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. T. Besta et al. become activists. New Ideas in Psychology, 39, 1–11. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2015.04.001 Van Zomeren, M., Leach, C. W., & Spears, R. (2010). Does group efficacy increase group identification? Resolving their paradoxical relationship. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 1055–1060. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jesp.2010.05.006 Van Zomeren, M., Leach, C. W., & Spears, R. (2012). Protesters as “passionate economists”: A dynamic dual pathway model of approach coping with collective disadvantage. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(2), 180–199. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311430835 Van Zomeren, M., Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (2008). Toward an integrative social identity model of collective action: A quantitative research synthesis of three socio-psychological perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 504–535. Van Zomeren, M., Spears, R., Fischer, A. H., & Leach, C. W. (2004). Put your money where your mouth is! Explaining What do I gain from joining crowds? collective action tendencies through group-based anger and group efficacy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(5), 649–664. Van Zomeren, M., Spears, R., & Leach, C. W. (2008). Exploring psychological mechanisms of collective action: Does relevance of group identity influence how people cope with collective disadvantage. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47(2), 353–372. https://doi.org/10.1348/ 014466607X231091 Wright, S. C., Aron, A., & Tropp, L. R. (2002). Including others (and groups) in the self: self-expansion and intergroup relations. In J. P. Forgas, & K. D. Williams (Eds.), The social self: Cognitive, interpersonal and intergroup perspectives (pp. 343–368). New York: Psychology Press. Xu, X., Floyd, A. H. L., Westmaas, J. L., & Aron, A. (2010). Self-expansion and smoking abstinence. Addictive Behaviors, 35(4), 295–301. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh. 2009.10.019 European Journal of Social Psychology 48 (2018) O152–O167 Copyright ª 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. O167 Copyright of European Journal of Social Psychology is the property of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.