See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/279749704 Rayner, S. G. (2015) Cognitive Styles and Learning Styles. In, J. D. Wright, (Ed.). International Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioral Sciences (2nd edition), Vol 4, pp. 110–117.... Article · April 2016 CITATIONS READS 0 5,037 1 author: Steve Rayner Newman University 106 PUBLICATIONS 2,122 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: epistemic style differences in learners View project Ways of Thinking Survey View project All content following this page was uploaded by Steve Rayner on 08 September 2015. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. Author's personal copy Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. This article was originally published in the International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author’s benefit and for the benefit of the author’s institution, for non-commercial research and educational use including without limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific colleagues who you know, and providing a copy to your institution’s administrator. All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without limitation commercial reprints, selling or licensing copies or access, or posting on open internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier’s permissions site at: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial From Rayner, S.G., 2015. Cognitive Styles and Learning Styles. In: James D. Wright (editor-in-chief), International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, Vol 4. Oxford: Elsevier. pp. 110–117. ISBN: 9780080970868 Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. unless otherwise stated. All rights reserved. Elsevier Author's personal copy Cognitive Styles and Learning Styles Stephen G Rayner, Newman University, Birmingham, UK Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Abstract The psychological construct of style in personality, cognition, and learning is explained in this article. The development of a styles theory is interpreted as the evolution of a generic concept of individuality and its status as an individual difference in cognition and learning. The emergence of popular applications of learning styles as well as a wave of critical revisionism and new directions in researching style differences is related to this development. An application of style differences for lifelong learning in both education and workplace are then considered, including implications for further study of differential psychology, pedagogy, training, and the nature of an individual’s personal approach to learning (style). Style Differences and Individuality: Cognition, Behavior, and Affect The commonsense idea of style is one of the distinctive and personal signatures or hallmarks woven into an individual’s performance (Rayner, 2000, 2001). Less clear, however, is use of the term ‘style’ when applied in personal or differential psychology, involving aspects of affect, behavior, cognition, intelligence, personality, self-perception, and social identity (Rayner and Peterson, 2009; Peterson et al., 2009). The implications of style differences for understanding how affect, behavior, and cognition in the person combine to form individuality is reflected in a traditional contribution to psychometric assessment and factor analytic research across three key areas of human performance: first, profiling individual differences in workplace and educational settings (Messick, 1976; Jonassen and Grabowski, 1993); second, exploiting the presence and effect of style features as variables in learning and teaching (Kolb, 1976; Entwistle, 1981; Schmeck, 1988); and third, reasserting the conceptual basis of the style construct in personal and social behavior (Witkin et al., 1962; Sternberg, 1996; Rayner and Riding, 1997; Peterson et al., 2009). An aim in the research over time has arguably been to describe a holistic but valid and reliably consistent theory of style, distinct from and separate to other individual differences (constructs) such as personality, intelligence, identity, and gender (Miller, 1987; Curry, 1987; Rayner and Riding, 1997). In more recent work (Rayner and Cools, 2011; Zhang et al., 2012), an account is made reexamining the basis of styles as traits, states, identities, and intellectual processes. Renewed interest in style research has also coincided with calls for developing alternative ways of researching style combining new forms of research methodology and epistemology (Rayner and Peterson, 2009; Cools and Rayner, 2011; Rayner, 2011). The Style Construct in the Psychology of Education The exact structure and nature of the style construct remains contested (Messick, 1976, 1994, 1996; Ritter, 2007). Similarly, the effect size associated with various measurements of a style factor in educational research (variously described as cognitive, 110 thinking, learning, or intellectual styles) remains disappointingly small when applied to contexts of learning and work-related behavior (Kavale and Forness, 1987; Coffield et al., 2004; Pashler et al., 2008; Peterson et al., 2009). This is an important issue to which we will again return later in this discussion. The utility and integrity of style as a construct, however, is clearly reliant upon a well-established theory, which is both coherent and consistent in terms of conceptual rigor, distinctive validity, independence as a construct, and epistemic robustness. The following recategorization of cognitive styles begins by distinguishing research in the mid part of the twentieth century, as the production of a styles construct associated with: (1) personality-based styles reflecting type and trait as key indicators of an individual’s innate behavior across different contexts of performance; (2) more stable cognition-centered structures and/or dimensions of a person’s cognitive style associated with traitlike differences in an individual’s tendency to think, feel, or act; (3) less stable cognitive processes linked to the ways in which cognitive controls, strategies, and processes combine in an individual’s approach to demand or task in a range of contexts; and (4) preferences for learning and thinking affected by modality, cognitive processes, and learning experiences, while engaged in completing an assigned activity. The first is related to constructs associated with ‘personality-based styles,’ the second to ‘cognition-centered styles,’ the third learning strategies, learning patterns, and an orientation to learning, and the fourth, generic ‘learning-based styles.’ The fourth part of this style recategorization which involves the identification and development of ‘learning styles’ reflects a refocused interest upon the question of ‘person-environment fit,’ and so is associated with a widening range of executive functions in the psychology of the individual when engaged with a task-related performance. This includes intellectual styles related to the following: multiple intelligences or abilities, problem solving, creativity, intuition, decision-making, and a wider range of style-based learning behaviors integrating learning styles with learning strategies as part of an effort to create a framework for applying ‘holistic’ style profiles (for example, combining personal learning style, learning preferences, experiential learning, learning motivation, and forms of accelerated, extended, or enhanced learning). International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, Volume 4 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.92008-7 International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 110–117 Author's personal copy Cognitive Styles and Learning Styles Personality Styles – Types of Individuality An early influence in the development of style as type may be linked to the work of Jung’s typology of personality constructs and ‘psychoanalytic ego psychology’ (Jung, 1923). The most influential style model emerging from this paradigm was the Myers–Briggs Type Indicator (Myers, 1978; Myers-Briggs, 1980). This test is generally recognized as a measure of personal style but conversely is described as a personality test (Isaksen et al., 2003; Hough and Ogilvie, 2005). The Myers–Briggs Type Indicator is purported to have become one of the most widely used measures of psychological types in business and public sector organizations. Few studies, however, have been published examining its efficacy (Kelley, 2005). A separate development to both the theorizing and measuring aspects of personality as individual preferences was constructed by Holland (Holland, 1973, 1985; Gottfredson and Holland, 1996), focusing upon individual career choices or planning. Holland’s theory is based upon the idea of defining personality types in terms of occupational preferences: these include Realistic, Investigative, Artistic, Social, Enterprising, and Conventional (Holland, 1973). Much of this ‘type’ theory reflects a similar set of ideas about preferences and trait underpinning the application of a range of style theories including the fundamental idea of the ‘matching hypothesis,’ which supposes that a deliberate style-led matching of different personality types with task, mode of work, grouping, and context can enhance performance. According to Sternberg (1997), intellectual styles function at the interface between ability and personality. The psychological structure of style posited in this way describes a relatively stable but complex interactive system that embodies psychological controls, processes, and states but also reflects an executive function regulating personal, physiological, and sociological aspects of the individual’s mind at work. For some psychologists, it is self-evident that style and personality represent very similar explanations of individuality and simply are manifestations of one and the same construct (see Messick, 1994; Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2006; Furnham et al., 2008). It is argued here, however, that the structure and function of the style construct is closely implicated with rather than an aspect of personality. It contributes to a holistic structure (or model) shaping as well as sustaining and reflecting an individual’s psychology, including structural forms of their personality, cognition, and behavior (Riding and Rayner, 1998). Cognitive Styles – Types of Process in Human Cognition The following section provides a summary of the work related to styles defined as internal control processes and strategies in human cognition. A useful account of the historical development of this work can be found in Kozhevnikov (2007). This is further complemented by analytic reviews offered by Grigerenko and Sternberg (1995), Rayner and Riding (1997), and Zhang and Sternberg (2005). The best introduction to the original styles experiments and source for the construct can be found in Messick (1976). 111 Perceptual Differentiation Witkin and coworkers produced seminal work over a period of more than 50 years – originating in an interest with the ‘regularities’ of information processing, derived from the German gestalt school of perceptual psychology, and which led to an early development of the ‘style construct’ of ‘Field Dependence-Independence’ (FDI). The practical issues related to flying aircraft through dense cloud formation were the original spur to investigating what and how individual intradifferences were found to influence crucial variations in pilots maintaining course and control in their aircraft. Individuals, when reorienting an object relative to the vertical, were found to rely upon the surrounding ‘field’ or ‘context’ to a greater or lesser extent. This was subsequently found to correlate with competence in disembedding shapes from a perceptual field, and experimental participants were found to either rely heavily on the field for orientation or shape discrimination (field-dependent) or rely little or not at all (field-independent). The assessment tools developed as part of this research included an original rod and frame tilting chair experiment and then the ‘pencil and paper’ embedded figures test. While a great deal of critique was subsequently leveled at the FDI theory for confounding abilities and style, Witkin and Ash (1948) extrapolated a comprehensive theory of psychological differentiation to explain how it affected a wide range of human performance across a breadth of social and personal contexts. Cognitive Controls and Cognitive Processes Psychologists interested in cognitive control processes and structure produced a second alternative approach to theorizing intradifferences in personality and style. Early experimental work at the Messinger Clinic in the USA developed notions of ego adaptation to the environment in the individual (Gardner, 1953; Gardner et al., 1959). This led to further experimental research hypothesizing style as a key factor in facilitating stable responses (styles) in the individual to the situation, producing patterns of response and function in thinking and behavior (Messick, 1976). The program completed by these coworkers over more than 20 years produced a series of ‘styles’ (to function rather like a set of cogs and balances in the cognitive machinery of the mind), usually comprising a bipolar structure and implicated in a wide range of adaptive responses to differing demands in the experimental context. These included, for example, stimuli differentiation (‘levelers’ and ‘sharpeners’) identified by Klein (1958), which subsequently developed to include differences in other forms of cognitive scanning, categorization, conceptual articulation, and degree of cognitive control (Gardner et al., 1959; Messick and Fritzky, 1963). A third focus provided by Kagan and coworkers investigating types and levels of response to cognitive uncertainty typically recorded during problem-solving task completion. This latter work identified two types of typical response along a continuum of conceptual tempo, from the impulsive to reflective (Kagan, 1966). It led in turn to the claim for a unique style dimension identified using the Matching Familiar Figures Test (Kagan, 1965). International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 110–117 Author's personal copy 112 Cognitive Styles and Learning Styles Mental Imagery, Memory, and Thinking Similar research around the same time involved exploring individual differences in mental representation and visual information processing: this included looking at visual and/or verbal ways of representing information in thought, as well as visual or imaginal processes used in dealing with new information. Research led by Pask (1972, 1976) on style and information processing introduced the idea of serial or holistic processing. This research identified how the individual would split and separate (serialist type) or group and synthesize (holist type) new information when dealing with a problem-solving task (Pask and Scott, 1972). At the same time, other work investigating mental imagery revealed how some people have a predominantly verbal way of representing information in thought, while others are more ‘visual’ or ‘imaginal.’ Paivio (1971) further developed this theory with a dual coding measurement of mental imagery. Further research completed by Richardson (1977) using the Verbalizer–Visualizer Questionnaire revealed habitual methods of visual thinking. Most recent work investigating the way in which these style dimensions may function has been completed by Kozhevnikov et al. (2005), and secondly, Miller et al. (2012), providing new neurological evidence of stylerelated mental activity, elicited from the use of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and data analysis employing t-statistic mapping of brain function. Riding and Taylor (1976) initially identified, as fundamental to the style construct, a verbal-imagery dimension of cognitive style. This led to the construction of the ‘cognitive styles assessment’ (CSA), used to support further research in developing an integrated theory of cognitive style (Riding, 1991). The model of style presented subsequently by Riding and Rayner (1998) described an orthogonal arrangement of two fundamental but independent dimensions of cognitive style: the verbal-visualizer style continuum and the wholist– analytic style continuum. These dimensions are assessed by the CSA, a computerized test locating a respondent on each of the wholist–analytic and verbal-imagery dimensions of a cognitive style (Riding, 1991). Research into Executive Functioning and Differences in Thinking A widening field of research that was interested in how executive functioning and individual differences in thinking and action influence key aspects of human performance emerged in the latter part of the twentieth century. The work involved many researchers independently constructing new models and measures, thereby adding to a broad and disparate theoretical base, and fueling in turn an increasing number of loosely linked conceptions of style in thinking, learning, and performance. These various style labels reflecting aspects of cognitive functioning included a model of phenomenological style (Gregorc, 1982); the Adaptor–Innovator cognitive style of decision-making (Kirton, 1994); an intuitive–analytic cognitive style of problem solving (Hayes and Allinson, 1998); cognitive styles analysis focusing upon information processing/mental representation (see Riding, 1991; Riding and Rayner, 1998); and a cognitive style of creativity (Kaufmann, 1989). Another more elaborate model of mental self-government presented by Sternberg (1996, 1997), represented a theory of style derived from notions of a holistic regulation in the mind, integrating constructs of intelligence, abilities, and self-government. According to this theory, individual differences in styles or mental processes can be understood in terms of function, form, level, scope, and leaning, which impact upon thinking and learning. The final word on the importance of this phase of research development belongs to Kozhevnikov (2007). She explains that its significance principally lies with an attempt to both expand and clarify the mechanisms of cognitive styles in the context of an information processing approach. She argues that there is sufficient evidence to suggest a theoretical basis for the hierarchical classification of cognitive styles according to the level of information processing in which the individual is engaged (from simple perceptual decisions to complex problem solving). Learning Styles: Personal Differences in Approach and Performance The development of learning styles research was preceded by work on the assessment of the learner, learning, and teaching. This work, epitomized by the activity of the American National Association of Secondary School Principals (NASSP) task force looking at learning styles (Keefe, 1985), triggered a wave of new models of style, with a great many populist versions, and more recently accessible from the Internet. The result was a proliferation in the range of user-sourced tools of assessment, hybrid forms of style theory, and many commercial packages advocating all in one approaches to personalizing educational intervention. This development was further reinforced by popular interpretations of advances in neuroscience, producing different versions of learning styles applied to teaching and learning in the classroom (Geake, 2008). In spite of this popularizing trend, several developing theories explaining approaches to individual differences in learning reflected cautious and well-established applied research. This section includes a summary of some key theories of learning styles organized into the following four groups: models focusing on experiential learning; models based on orientation to study; models based upon instructional preferences; and models based upon learning skills associated with neuroscience. The latter research in particular reflected attempts to explore respectively and in combination interaction between inter- and intradifferences in learning and the learner when engaged in learning. Models Focusing on Experiential Learning Models derived from a theory of experiential learning, of which most influential is the work of Kolb (1976), described learning styles as the individual’s preferred method for assimilating information, in an active learning cycle. Kolb constructed a twodimensional model comprising perception (concrete/abstract thinking) and processing (active/reflective information processing), treating these as fundamental aspects of an experiential learning cycle. The assessment tool used for this learning styles model is the Learning Styles Inventory (LSI) International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 110–117 Author's personal copy Cognitive Styles and Learning Styles (Kolb, 1999). Recent research further developing this model in studies of undergraduate MBA courses in the USA is reported by Sharma and Kolb (2011), a new form of related assessment reported by Boyatzis and Mainemelis (2011) and related pedagogic development (Portugal and the UK) reported by Almeida et al. (2011). Models Based on Orientation to Study These derived from earlier theories of information processing and learning processes; the most influential has been that of Entwistle (1981), in developing the early work of Pask (1972) to produce a measure called the Approaches to Study Inventory. This paved the way for a good deal of subsequent research exploring how learners construct an approach to learning and how this interacts with motivation (Marton and Saljo, 1976; Vermunt, 1996; Entwistle and Peterson, 2004). The ‘approaches to learning’ model is widely referred to in University education (Biggs, 2001; Vermunt, 2011), and typically describes ways in which students engage with deep or surface learning, thus reflecting the adoption of one or more orientations to study (meaning, reproducing, achieving, and holistic learning). These choices of approach reflect a series of orientations that are thought to be relatively stable differences in study behavior but remain susceptible to change as patterns of learning. Models Focusing upon Instructional Preference These set out to present a comprehensive profile of learning differences in the typical response of an individual to instructional contexts. Learning style (Keefe, 1989) was typically perceived as a multimodal construct and understood to describe a range of emotional and intellectual functioning relating to the learning activity. Examples of this approach included the ‘Cognitive Profile,’ (Letteri, 1980) and the Learning Styles Profile (Keefe, 1985, 1989). It is, however, more widely associated with the program of teacher education promoted by Rita Dunn – the LSI – developed by Dunn et al. (1989). The learning style elements identified when using the Dunn and Dunn LSI are self-reported responses eliciting information for modal and learning preferences located in the individual student. These are: environmental stimulus (light, temperature); emotional stimulus (persistence, motivation); sociological stimulus (peers, adults); physical stimulus (perceptual strengths, time of day – morning vs. after-noon); and psychological stimulus (global-analytic, or impulsive reflective). A comprehensive range of learning and teaching materials has been developed by Dunn over a 30-year period to support a learning stylesbased differentiation of curricula and pedagogy across the full range of educational phases. It is summarized in Dunn and Griggs (2003), and further theorized in Dunn and Honigsfeld (2011). Models Based upon Learning Skills and Neuroscience A fourth group of learning style labels is focused upon an individual’s developing cognitive ability and repertoire of cognitive skills or capacity to learn, together with related behavioral characteristics. These were understood to comprise an individual’s learning profile. More recent versions in what has amounted to 113 a populist approach to learning styles include the application of a widely established ‘visual, auditory, and kinesthetic (VAK)’ sensory-based approach to learning and teaching, combining multiple intelligences theory (Gardner, 1983), and neurolinguistic theory (O’Connor and McDermott, 1996). Other similar ‘practical applications’ of neuroscientific research and teacher development also popular during the last decade include ‘accelerated learning’ (Rose and Nicholl, 1997), ‘cognitive enhancement’ (Adey et al., 1999), and ‘personalized learning’ utilizing ‘learning power’ (Ritchie and Deakin Crick, 2005). Revisionist Theories and Problematizing the Style Construct An original interest in researching styles from a practitioner’s perspective reflects a concern for enhancing learning and teaching that in essence seeks to help ‘teach the hard to reach and reach the hard to teach.’ For this to happen, styles theory must offer and secure clearly stated relevance, utility, and worthwhile effects for any intervention in the workplace or classroom. The nature of such take up, however, may arguably need to be much more than only the ‘Aptitude-by-Treatment Interactions’ (ATI) based experimental research advocated by Pashler et al. (2008), and identified as the evidentiary gold standard required to confirm the status of style research. Rayner (2001), moreover, has argued that the main purpose and value for importing style theory into the design of pedagogy and curricula is the proper use of profiling and assessment as a tool for formative learning. Rosenfeld and Rosenfeld (2011), reflecting a similar ‘practitioner’ perspective, also argue a need for paradigm shift from second to third person in research design, and call for research directly involving students as individuals working with their own learning styles. They very deliberately challenge the ‘ATI template’ and traditional experimental design. Such an approach reflects a concern for more applied, humanistic understanding of style differences, as well as providing evidence of impact on ‘value for instruction’ and ‘pedagogic development.’ There are a few but prominent and far reaching critiques of style published in the past 50 years reflecting a more general dissatisfaction with what is described as an unexpectedly gargantuan, complex, and disparate field of research. These criticisms include, for example, the following: no consensual or coherent field theory and consistent psychometric failings across the field in respect to measures of validity, reliability, and effect sizes produced in empirical research (Coffield et al., 2004; Pashler et al., 2008); an incestuous research tradition practiced in a field almost entirely engaged with self-affirming replication (Coffield et al., 2004); a flawed theory of the psychology of learning (Nixon et al., 2007; Geake, 2008); misplaced approaches to pedagogy, curriculum, and scholarship (Sharp et al., 2008); a shortfall in clearly stated or reliable and coherent accounts of value, relevance, and utility of learning styles in the applied context (Reynolds, 1997; Coffield et al., 2004; Peterson et al., 2009); resultant misplaced and restrictive typing, labeling, and harmful individualized practices when applying styles theory to education, instruction, pedagogy, or the curriculum (Coffield et al., 2004; Ritter, 2007; Sharp et al., 2008); reference to alternative more compelling theories and International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 110–117 Author's personal copy 114 Cognitive Styles and Learning Styles explanations of the learning process in social psychology and sociology, which in itself is claimed to offer a better prospect of return for resource in the educational setting (Coffield et al., 2004; Nixon et al., 2007; Ritter, 2007). The issues of these critiques of style research persist to date and invalidate by association a good deal of rigorous and robust work. Nonetheless, continuing criticism of styles research reflecting a mix of ideological conflicts and perspectival differences has led at times to a rejection of any need for styles research. These disputes, however, are often reflected in writers from opposing research epistemologies articulating differing conceptualizations of efficacy and equity for the educational setting. It is salutary and interesting to note Messick’s comment that such intensity of conflict was indeed part of the academic debate over 50 years ago. He commented that critique was often “. excessively polarized in either supporting or undercutting styles as meaningful constructs. This polarization seems to reflect different stances not just with reference to scientific evidence but also with respect to ideology” (Messick, 1994: 121). There has not been much change over time with the substance of style critique. Implications of Styles for Lifelong Learning The extent to which an awareness of cognitive style or learning styles and the self as a learner is currently considered and managed within the learning context raises key questions for the design of training, instruction, and pedagogy. These suggest in turn a need to consider the following key aspects of education construed as contributing to the idea of lifelong learning. Intellectual styles and metacognition – involving research looking at how intellectual styles (cognition and learning) interact with an awareness of self as a learner and thinker. An obvious implication of this approach to valuing style as a concept lies with its effect upon influencing motivation, behavior, and attribution governing responses to personal achievement. The recent work of Sadler-Smith (2009, 2012) provides an interesting start to a conceptualization linking intuition styles, motivation, and experience with duplex theory and intellectual styles with metacognition. l Style and self-regulation in learning – involving work linking theories of self-regulation with awareness and perceptions of self as a learner. A great deal of integrating work completed by researchers in the area of self-attribution and/or regulation (Zimmerman, 2008) offer a useful model as well as related work for further developing pedagogic approaches to working with student motivation and approaches to learning (see Rayner, 2011; Vermunt, 2011). l Assessment-led learning and curriculum differentiation – meeting the need to further demonstrate relevance and positive effects of style awareness for instructional design in work around the original interest in the matching hypothesis (aligning or contrasting style with task or social context to enhance flexibility in the learner and learning outcome). Indeed many writers seem to regard this aspect of ‘style’ as the only serious implication of style in education (for example, Pashler et al., 2008, devote most of their criticism for the meshing hypothesis). Recent work in several areas of style research has reflected confirmation of flexible approaches to l learning styles in application (Dunn and Honigsfeld, 2011; Sharma and Kolb, 2011). Similarly, work looking at curriculum differentiation has produced new integrative approaches drawing upon both a cognition and learning centered theory of styles (Rayner, 2000; Evans and Waring, 2011; Zhang et al., 2012). l Personalizing education and learning technologies – a great deal of research is concerned with developing an understanding and thereby facilitating the design of personalized learning. Mourlas et al. (2009) provide an overview of this area exploring ‘pedagogic’ aspects of the interaction between the individual and web-based or digital infrastructure and/or architecture, with a view to enhancing ways in which an alignment between cognitive style and demands may enhance learning. This not only embraces remote or mobiledigital learning, but learning and teaching applications of social media, intelligent design of technologies, user interface with digital technology and more widely personalizing computer-mediated learning and teaching. l Organizational learning, training & business management – the intuitive appeal of style differences in both cognition and learning continues to generate research and development in the workplace. The key themes emerging in this area are a consideration of the role of style differences in organizational behavior and human resource development including: selection and person-environment fit, vocational choice and career success; diversity, group processes, and conflict management; intuition and emotion in the workplace; training and development; personal styles’ profiling; and career management in global organizations (Armstrong and Cools, 2009; Armstrong et al., 2012; Cools, 2012). l Pedagogic differences and professional development – personal and social differences identified in style research impact directly upon the training and the development of professional expertise in professional practitioners (e.g., teachers, trainers, and educators). Much of the research into this area reflects adherence to one or more models of learning styles (for example, Dunn and Griggs, 2003; Sharma and Kolb, 2011). An interesting development in further developing a holistic theory of style, however, is reflected in the earlier work of Sadler-Smith (1996), Peterson et al. (2010) as well as Evans and Waring (2008, 2011), focusing upon teacher training and the effects of raising self-awareness of cognitive style in the teacher’s developing pedagogic practice. The significance of ‘style’ as reflected in this range of research for the life cycle is clearly about much more than ‘style’ simply used as a term that has linguistic or conceptual versatility in describing personal performance. While this common usage of the term clearly denotes a popular as well as intuitive appeal and so clearly reflects a universal value, the import of this discussion is that style as an individual difference marks something much more specific in a range of learning associated with the idea of the person engaged in lifelong learning. Conclusion: A Matter of Style Why do style differences matter? Knowing more about how another or I as an individual(s) tend to think, feel and act in International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 110–117 Author's personal copy Cognitive Styles and Learning Styles a learning context provides rich possibilities for new ways of setting up, facilitating and leading to any learning activity. Profiling differences and potentials in human performance affords greater awareness for self-directed learning and participation or efficacy in a project group. It is equally relevant to informing differentiated design in programs of study, pedagogic practices, and formative curricula. A stylebased contribution to education is also one way in which consideration may be given to further customizing digital technology in better serving personal and social learning across contexts and community. These implications for further study in differential or personal psychology (individual differences), pedagogy, training and the better management of an individual’s personal approach to learning (style) should finally require a closer focus upon types of epistemology and methodology in research design. Rayner (2011) and Rayner et al. (2012), for instance, have argued the need to deliberately attempt paradigm shift to supplement the traditional styles research agenda, which has been largely located in a positivistic paradigm, and more usually employed as an experimental or survey based design associated with the science of cognitive psychology. Whilst this experimental work remains central to style research (Kozhevnikov, 2007), there is a conjoint need for more pluralistic approaches in the design and use of psychological assessment or theorizing of style in cognition, learning, and management. Sternberg et al. (2012: 416) on the other hand, in a retrospective comment on the recent past in style research makes an argument for much greater emphasis in renewing a factor analytic mode in style research. He acknowledges an urgent need for ‘sorting out the theories of style,’ and developing ‘better forms of style assessment,’ but finally stresses the need to reinforce the psychological status of style research utilizing ‘random-assignment studies with control groups.’ He comments that The research that has been conducted has been of variable quality and styles would get more research if they were more rigorously researched. What is very apparent in this view is a need, as suggested earlier, for new ways of constructing the methodology of assessment or testing to better treat complex models of style differences, as have, for example, more recently been occurring in the field of personality (see Hopwood and Donnellan, 2010). Finally, and as described by Rayner (2013), concern for validity, efficacy, and utility as key markers in a theoretical revisionism within future research and development for the field of ‘style differences’ rests upon attempting a paradigmatic step change and establishing new ways of modeling and measuring the style construct. Such an approach requires a reexamining of epistemic issues underpinning both the style construct and, as crucially, careful thought about research methodology and project design. It also more fundamentally means responding with greater clarity and certainty to a question about the epistemological status of a ‘style’ construct asked by Schmidt (2012). Schmidt points out that the style construct to date is on the one hand conceptualized as a typology in the sense of Hempel’s ‘aspects of scientific explanation’ and more recently tested by procedures such as Latent Class or Latent Profile Analysis (Hempel, 1962, 1965), and, on the other hand, the more traditional approach to style is one in which a classical 115 psychometric method, using exploratory or confirmatory factor analysis for testing reliability and validity, is applied and then classical multivariate models used to test the specific contribution of style. Schmidt questions, therefore, if style research might usefully benefit from adopting an approach combining both perspectives and thereby following a three-step methodology in mixture modeling (see, Jung and Wickrama, 2007; Vermunt, 2010), and which is implemented in programs like Latent Gold or MPLUS (Asparouhov and Bengt, 2013). This pertinent suggestion offered by Schmidt merits serious consideration as part of what Rayner (2013) argues is a need for problematizing the style construct as a basis for moving forward in the field, thereby leaving a fourth period of research and development (see, Kozhevnikov, 2007), to enter a fifth wave of style research (Rayner, 2013). In many ways this arguably reflects a need to move ‘back to the future’ and robustly reconsider the question of construct validity at the heart of Messick’s work on psychological testing (Schiffman and Messick, 1963; Messick, 1988), and more widely any factorial interaction between personality, style, and abilities (Messick, 1994, 1996). In conclusion, using style as a psychological construct can aid organizing and developing teams of practitioners in a wide range of organizational activity. But we do need to know much more and as such clearly reestablish or confirm the validity of key style differences, as well as related models or measures, and further affirm a ‘grand theory’ (general understanding) of the meaning or implications of style. The key theme for utilizing the style construct is captured perfectly in the popular song, It is not what you do, it is the way that you do it. The point is not that content is unimportant, but that being aware and knowing more about ‘how I tend to do it’ can immeasurably help in ‘my doing it better.’ A greater understanding and improvement of human performance, as psychological process, social behavior, and educational achievement, is the touchstone for future work in applied research aimed at developing style theory, research, and practice. See also: Academic Self-Concept and Achievement; Computer-Assisted Instruction; Education for the Gifted and Talented; Instructional Design; Instructional Psychology; Learning Theories and Educational Paradigms; Learning To learn; Personality Development and Temperament; SelfConcepts: Educational Aspects; Self-Efficacy: Education Aspects; Self-Regulated Learning: Theories, Measures, and Outcomes; Tangible User Interfaces in Learning and Education. Bibliography Adey, P., Fairbrother, R., Wiliam, D., 1999. A Review of Research on Learning Strategies and Learning Styles. King’s College, London. Almeida, P., Pedrosa de Jesus, H., Watts, C., 2011. Kolb’s learning styles and approaches to learning through the use of students’ critical questions. In: Rayner, S., Cools, E. (Eds.), Style Differences in Cognition, Learning and Management. Routledge, New York, pp. 115–128. Armstrong, S.J., Cools, E., 2009. Cognitive styles and their relevance for business and management: a review of development over the past two decades. In: International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 110–117 Author's personal copy 116 Cognitive Styles and Learning Styles Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J. (Eds.), Perspectives on the Nature of Intellectual Styles. Springer, Heidelberg, pp. 253–290. Armstrong, S.J., van der Heijden, B.I.J.M., Sadler-Smith, E., 2012. Intellectual styles, management of careers, and improved work performance. In: Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J., Rayner, S.G. (Eds.), Handbook of Intellectual Styles. Springer Pubs, NY, pp. 273–294. Asparouhov, T., Bengt, M., 2013. Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: 3-step approaches. Using Mplus. Mplus Web Notes 15 (7), 1–48. Biggs, J., 2001. Enhancing learning: a matter of style or approach? In: Sternberg, R.J., Zhang, L.F. (Eds.), Perspectives on Thinking, Learning and Cognitive Styles. Springer Pubs, NY, pp. 73–102. Boyatzis, R.E., Mainemelis, C., 2011. Assessing the distribution of learning and adaptive styles in an MBA program. In: Rayner, S., Cools, E. (Eds.), Style Differences in Cognition, Learning and Management. Routledge, New York, pp. 99–114. Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Furnham, A., 2006. Intellectual competence and the intelligent personality: a third way in differential psychology. Review of General Psychology 10 (3), 251–267. Coffield, F.C., Moseley, D.V.M., Hall, E., Ecclestone, K., 2004. Learning Styles and Pedagogy in Post-16 Learning: Findings of a Systematic and Critical Review of Learning Styles Models. Learning and Skills Research Centre, London. Cools, E., Rayner, S., 2011. Researching style: more of the same or moving forward. In: Rayner, S., Cools, E. (Eds.), Style Differences in Cognition, Learning and Management. Routledge, New York, pp. 295–306. Cools, E., 2012. Understanding styles in organizational behaviors: a summary of insights and implications. In: Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J., Rayner, S.G. (Eds.), Handbook of Intellectual Styles. Springer Pubs, NY, pp. 329–352. Curry, L., 1987. Integrating Concepts of Cognitive or Learning Style: A Review with Attention to Psychometric Standards. Canadian College of Health Service Executives, Ottawa, ON, Canada. Dunn, R., Griggs, S., 2003. Synthesis of the Dunn and Dunn Learning Styles Model research: Who, What, When, Where and So What – The Dunn and Dunn Learning Styles Model and Its Theoretical Cornerstone. St John’s University, New York. Dunn, R., Honigsfeld, A., 2011. Learning-styles-based differentiating instruction for at-risk students: rationale, research documentation, assessment tools, and alternative approaches. In: Rayner, S., Cools, E. (Eds.), Style Differences in Cognition, Learning and Management. Routledge, New York, pp. 263–277. Dunn, R., Dunn, K., Price, G.E., 1989. Learning Styles Inventory. Price Systems, Lawrence, KS, USA. Entwistle, N., Peterson, E.R., 2004. Learning styles and approaches to studying. In: Spielberger, C. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Applied Psychology. Academic Press, New York, pp. 537–542. Entwistle, N.J., 1981. Styles of Teaching and Learning: An Integrated Outline of Educational Psychology for Students, Teachers and Lecturers. Wiley, Chichester, NY. Evans, C., Waring, M., 2008. Trainee teachers’ cognitive styles and notions of differentiation. Education and Training 50, 140–154. Evans, C., Waring, M., 2011. Enhancing feedback practice: a personal learning styles pedagogy approach. In: Rayner & E Cools, S. (Ed.), Style Differences in Cognition, Learning, and Management: Theory, Research and Practice. Routledge, New York, pp. 188–203. Furnham, A., Christopher, A., Garwood, J., Martin, N.G., 2008. Ability, demography, learning style, and personality trait correlates of student preference for assessment method. Educational Psychology 28 (1), 15–27. Gardner, R.W., Holzman, P.S., Klein, G.S., Linton, H.B., Spence, D.P., 1959. Cognitive control: a study of individual consistencies in cognitive behaviour. Psychological Issues 1 (4). Gardner, R.W., 1953. Cognitive styles in categorizing behaviour. Journal of Personality 22, 214–333. Gardner, H., 1983. Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. Basic Books, New York. Geake, J., 2008. Neuromythologies in education. Educational Research 50 (2), 123–133. Gottfredson, G.D., Holland, J.L., 1996. Dictionary of Holland Occupational Codes, third ed. Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL. Gregorc, A.R., 1982. Style Delineator. Gabriel Systems, Maynard, MA. Grigerenko, E.L., Sternberg, R.J., 1995. Thinking styles. In: Saklofske, D.H., Zeidner, M. (Eds.), International Handbook of Personality and Intelligence. Plenum Press, NY, USA, pp. 205–229. Hayes, J., Allinson, C.W., 1998. Cognitive style and the theory and practice of individual and collective learning in organizations. Human Relations 51, 847–871. Hempel, C., 1962. Explanation in science and history. In: Colodny, R.C. (Ed.), Frontiers of Science and Philosophy. The University of Pittsburgh Press, Pittsburgh, pp. 9–19. Hempel, C., 1965. Aspects of Scientific Explanation. The Free Press, New York. Holland, J.L., 1973. Making Vocational Choices: A Theory of Careers. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ. Holland, J.L., 1985. Manual for the Vocational Preference Inventory. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc, Odessa FL. Hopwood, C.J., Donnellan, M.B., 2010. How should the internal structure of personality inventories be evaluated? Personality and Social Psychology Review 14 (3), 332–346. Hough, J.R., Ogilvie, D.T., 2005. An empirical test of cognitive style and strategic decision outcomes. Journal of Management Studies 42 (2), 417–448. Isaksen, S.G., Lauer, K.J., Wilson, G.V., 2003. An examination of the relationship between personality type and cognitive style. Creativity Research Journal 15 (4), 343–354. Jonassen, D.H., Grabowski, B.L., 1993. Handbook of Individual Difference, Learning and Instruction. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ. Jung, T., Wickrama, K.A.S., 2007. An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 2 (1), 302–317. Jung, C., 1923. Psychological Types. Harcourt Brace, NY. Kagan, J., 1965. Individual difference in the resolution of response uncertainty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2, 154–160. Kagan, J., 1966. Reflection-impulsivity: the generality and dynamics of conceptual tempo. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 71, 17–24. Kaufmann, G., 1989. The Assimilator-explorer Inventory. University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway. Kavale, K.A., Forness, S.R., 1987. Substance over style: assessing the efficacy of modality testing and teaching. Exceptional Children 54 (3), 228–239. Keefe, J.W., 1985. Assessment of learning style variables: the NASSP task force model. Theory into Practice 24, 138–144. Keefe, J.W., 1989. Profiling and Utilising Learning Style. National Association of Secondary School Principals, Reston, VA. Kelley, K.N., 2005. Using the Myers-Briggs type indicator. PsycCRITIQUES 50 (33). http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/05159912. Kirton, J.W. (Ed.), 1994. Adaptors and Innovators, second ed. Routledge, London. Klein, G.S., 1958. Cognitive control and motivation. In: Lindzey, G. (Ed.), Assessment of Human Motives. Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York, pp. 87–118. Kolb, D.A., 1976. Learning Style Inventory: Technical Manual. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, USA. Kolb, D.A., 1999. The Kolb Learning Style Inventory Version 3. Hay Group, Boston, MA. Kozhevnikov, M., Kosslyn, S.M., Shephard, J., 2005. Spatial versus object visualizers: a new characterization of visual cognitive style. Memory & Cognition 33, 710–726. Kozhevnikov, M., 2007. Cognitive styles in the context of modern psychology: toward an integrated framework of cognitive style. Psychological Bulletin 133, 464–481. Letteri, C.A., 1980. Cognitive profile – basic determinants of academic achievement. Journal of Educational Research 73, 195–199. Marton, F., Saljo, R., 1976. On qualitative differences in learning outcome and process. British Journal of Educational Psychology 46, 4–11. Messick, S., Fritzky, F.J., 1963. Dimension of analytic attitude in cognition and personality. Journal of Personality 31, 346–370. Messick, S., 1976. Individuality in Learning: Implications of Cognitive Styles and Creativity for Human Development. Jossey Bass, San Francisco, CA. Messick, S., 1988. The once and future issues of validity: assessing the meaning and consequences of measurement. In: Wainer, H., Braun, I.H. (Eds.), Test Validity. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 33–45. Messick, S., 1994. The matter of style: manifestations of personality in cognition, learning and teaching. Educational Psychologist 29, 121–136. Messick, S., 1996. Bridging cognition and personality in education: the role of style in performance and development. European Journal of Personality 10, 353–376. Miller, M.B., Donovan, C.L., Bennett, C.M., Aminoff, E.M., Mayer, R.A., 2012. Individual differences in cognitive style and strategy predict similarities in the patterns of brain activity between individuals. NeuroImage 59, 83–93. Miller, A., 1987. Cognitive styles: an integrated model. Educational Psychology 7 (4), 251–268. Mourlas, C., Tsianos, N., Germanakos, P. (Eds.), 2009. Cognitive and Emotional Processes in Web-Based Education: Integrating Human Factors and Personalization. IGI Global Pub, Hershey, PA. Myers, I., 1978. Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 110–117 Author's personal copy Cognitive Styles and Learning Styles Myers-Briggs, I., 1980. Gifts Differing: Understanding Personality Type. DaviesBlack Publishing, NY. Nixon, L., Gregson, M., Spedding, T., 2007. Pedagogy and the intuitive appeal of learning styles in post compulsory education in England. Journal of Vocational Education and Training 59 (1), 39–50. O’Connor, J., McDermott, I., 1996. Principles of NLP. Thorsons, London. Paivio, A., 1971. Styles and strategies of learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology 46, 128–148. Pashler, H., McDaniel, M., Rowher, D., Bjork, R., 2008. Learning styles: concepts and evidence. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 9, 105–119. Pask, G., Scott, B.C.E., 1972. Learning strategies and individual competence. International Journal of Man-machine Studies 4, 217–253. Pask, G., 1972. A fresh look at cognition and the individual. International Journal of Man-machine Studies 4, 211–216. Pask, G., 1976. Styles and strategies of learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology 46, 128–148. Peterson, E.R., Rayner, S.G., Armstrong, S.J., 2009. Researching the psychology of cognitive style and learning style: is there really a future? Learning and Individual Differences 19, 518–523. Peterson, E.R., Brown, G.T.L., Irving, S.E., 2010. Secondary school students conceptions of learning and their relationship to achievement. Learning and Individual Differences 20 (3), 167–176. Rayner, S., Cools, E. (Eds.), 2011. Style Differences in Cognition, Learning and Management. Routledge, New York. Rayner, S., Peterson, E., 2009. Re-affirming style as an individual difference – global paradigm or knowledge diaspora? In: Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J. (Eds.), Perspectives on the Nature of Intellectual Styles. Springer Pubs, New York, pp. 107–134. Rayner, S., Riding, R., 1997. Towards a categorisation of cognitive styles and learning styles. Educational Psychology 17, 5–28. Rayner, S., Roodenburg, J., Roodenburg, E., 2012. Understanding an integrated theory of intellectual styles: models, measures, and meaning. In: Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J., Rayner, S.G. (Eds.), Handbook of Intellectual Styles. Springer Pubs, NY, pp. 47–66. Rayner, S., 2000. Reconstructing style differences in thinking and learning: profiling learning performance. In: Riding, R., Rayner, S. (Eds.), International Perspectives on Individual Differences, Cognitive Styles, vol. 1. Ablex, Stanford, CT, pp. 115–177. Rayner, S.G., 2001. Cognitive styles and learning styles. In: Smelser, N.J., Baltes, P.B. (Eds.), International Encyclopaedia of the Social and Behavioural Sciences. Elsevier Press, Oxford, pp. 2171–2175. Rayner, S., 2011. Researching style: epistemology, paradigm shifts and research interest groups. Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2), 255–262. Rayner, S.G., 2013. Problematizing style differences theory and professional learning in educational psychology. The Australian Educational and Developmental Psychologist 30 (1), 13–35. Reynolds, M., 1997. Learning styles: a critique. Management Learning 28, 115–133. Richardson, A., 1977. Verbalizer–visualizer: a cognitive style dimension. Journal of Mental Imagery 1, 109–126. Riding, R.J., Rayner, S.G., 1998. Cognitive Styles and Learning Strategies. David Fulton Pub, London. Riding, R.J., Taylor, E.M., 1976. Imagery performance and prose comprehension in 7-year-old children. Educational Studies 2, 21–27. Riding, R.J., 1991. Cognitive Styles Analysis. Learning and Training Technology, Birmingham. Ritchie, R., Deakin Crick, R., 2005. Distributing Leadership for Personalizing Learning. Network Educational Press, Stafford. Ritter, L., 2007. Unfulfilled promises: how inventories, instruments and institutions subvert discourses of diversity and promote commonality. Teaching in Higher Education 12 (5&6), 569–579. Rose, C., Nicholl, M.J., 1997. Accelerated Learning for the 21st Century. Dell Publishing, NY. Rosenfeld, M., Rosenfeld, S., 2011. Illustrating a complementary paradigm for styles research: from a third-person to a second-person perspective. In: Rayner, S., Cools, E. (Eds.), Style Differences in Cognition, Learning and Management. Routledge, New York, pp. 143–159. Sadler-Smith, E., 1996. Learning styles: a holistic approach. Journal of European Industrial Training 20 (7), 29–36. Sadler-Smith, E., 2009. A duplex model of cognitive style. In: Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J. (Eds.), Perspectives on the Nature of Intellectual Styles. Springer, Heidelberg, pp. 3–28. Sadler-Smith, E., 2012. Metacognition and styles. In: Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J., Rayner, S.G. (Eds.), Handbook of Intellectual Styles. Springer Pubs, NY, pp. 153–172. Schiffman, H., Messick, S., 1963. Scaling and measurement theory. Review of Educational Research 33 (5), 533–542. Schmeck, R.J., 1988. Learning Strategies and Learning Styles (Perspectives on Individual Differences). Springer, New York. Schmidt, P., 2012. The epistemological status of the style construct? Private Correspondence Received: 12.12.2013. Sharma, G., Kolb, D., 2011. The learning flexibility index: assessing contextual flexibility in learning style. In: Rayner, S., Cools, E. (Eds.), Style Differences in Cognition, Learning and Management. Routledge, New York, pp. 60–77. Sharp, J.G., Bowker, R., Byrne, J., 2008. VAK or VAK-uous? towards the trivialisation of learning and the death of scholarship. Research Papers in Education 23 (3), 293–314. Sternberg, R.J., Zhang, L.F., Rayner, S.G., 2012. Epilogue. In: Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J., Rayner, S.G. (Eds.), Handbook of Intellectual Styles. Springer Pubs, NY, pp. 415–418. Sternberg, R.J., 1996. Styles of thinking. In: Baltes, P.B., Staudinger, U.M. (Eds.), Interactive Minds. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 347–365. Sternberg, R.J., 1997. Thinking Styles. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Tiedemann, J., 1989. Measures of cognitive styles: a critical review. Educational Psychology 24, 261–275. Vermunt, J.D., 1996. Metacognitive, cognitive and affective aspects of learning styles and strategies: a phenomenographic analysis. Higher Education 31, 25–50. Vermunt, J.K., 2010. Latent class modeling with covariates: two improved threestep approaches. Political Analysis 18, 450–469. Vermunt, J.D., 2011. Patterns in student learning and teacher learning: similarities and differences. In: Rayner, S., Cools, E. (Eds.), Style Differences in Cognition, Learning and Management. Routledge, New York, pp. 173–187. Witkin, H.A., Ash, S.E., 1948. Studies in space orientation: IV further experiments on perception of the upright with displaced visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology 43, 58–67. Witkin, H.A., Goodenough, D.R., 1981. Cognitive Style: Essence and Origins. International Universities Press, New York. Witkin, H.A., Dyk, R.B., Faterson, H.F., Goodenough, D.R., Karp, S.A., 1962. Psychological Differentiation. John Wiley & Sons, New York. Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J., 2005. A threefold model of intellectual styles. Educational Psychology Review 17, 1–53. Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J., Rayner, S.G. (Eds.), 2012. Handbook of Intellectual Styles. Springer Pubs, NY. Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J., Rayner, S.G., 2012. Intellectual styles: challenges, milestones, and agenda. In: Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J., Rayner, S.G. (Eds.), Handbook of Intellectual Styles. Springer Pubs, NY, pp. 1–20. Zimmerman, B.J., 2008. Investigating self-regulation and motivation: historical background, methodological developments, and future prospects. American Educational Research Journal 45 (1), 166–183. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition, 2015, 110–117 View publication stats 117