See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/230036055 The global trend in plant twining direction Article · May 2007 DOI: 10.1111/j.1466-8238.2007.00326.x CITATIONS READS 51 828 3 authors: Will Edwards Angela Moles James Cook University UNSW Sydney 94 PUBLICATIONS 2,112 CITATIONS 139 PUBLICATIONS 17,151 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Peter J. Franks The University of Sydney 73 PUBLICATIONS 5,888 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: World Herbivory Project View project Latitudinal gradients in colouration View project All content following this page was uploaded by Peter J. Franks on 08 June 2018. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. SEE PROFILE Global Ecology and Biogeography, (Global Ecol. Biogeogr.) (2007) 16, 795–800 Blackwell Publishing Ltd RESEARCH PAPER The global trend in plant twining direction Will Edwards1*, Angela T. Moles2,3 and Peter Franks1 1 School of Tropical Biology, James Cook University, Cairns, QLD 4878, Australia, 2 Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, NSW 2109, Australia, 3School of Biological Sciences, Victoria University of Wellington, PO Box 600, Wellington, New Zealand ABSTRACT Aim To examine, at a global scale, patterns in the direction in which climbing plants twine. We tested three hypotheses: (1) that twining direction is determined randomly; (2) that twining direction is determined by apices following the apparent movement of the sun across the sky; and (3) that twining direction is determined by the Coriolis effect. Location Seventeen sites spanning nine countries, both hemispheres and 65° of latitude. Methods Twining direction was recorded for the first c. 100 stems encountered along transects through natural vegetation at each site. Results Ninety-two per cent of the 1485 twining stems we recorded grew in righthanded helices, i.e. they twined in an anticlockwise direction. This is significantly (P < 0.001) different from random. The proportion of stems twining right-handedly (anticlockwise) was independent of both latitude (P = 0.33) and hemisphere (P = 0.63). These data are inconsistent with the idea that twining direction is determined by either the relative passage of the sun through the celestial sphere or by the Coriolis effect. Thus, we reject all three of our hypotheses. *Correspondence: Will Edwards, School of Tropical Biology, James Cook University, Cairns, QLD 4878, Australia. E-mail: will.edwards@jcu.edu.au Main conclusions The predominance of right-handed helical growth in climbing plants cannot be explained by hypotheses attempting to link plant growth behaviour and global location. One alternative hypothesis for our findings is that the widespread phenomenon of anticlockwise twining arises as a function of microtubule orientation operating at a subcellular level. Keywords Chirality, climbing plants, Coriolis, lianes, microtubules, twining, vines. Global patterns in biological systems give insight into the basic forces that shape the biosphere. In the last few decades there has been considerable effort directed at documenting broad-scale biogeographical patterns, and developing ecological theories that may explain them. Examples of broad-scale patterns for which much information has been gathered include latitudinal gradients in species diversity (MacArthur, 1972), geographical range sizes (i.e. Rapoport’s rule; Stevens, 1989) and body masses of animal species (i.e. Bergmann’s rule; Mayr, 1963). Here we examine the broad-scale pattern of twining direction in climbing plants. Anecdotal reports suggest a general tendency for twining plants to ascend their hosts in right-handed helices, corresponding to anti-clockwise motion in the direction of growth (Gardner, 1970; Hashimoto, 2002). Unlike other broad-scale biological phenomena, however, the pattern in plant twining direction has (apparently) attracted little attention amongst scientists. Consequently, there is very little published empirical evidence to support this claim, and no data on the global extent of any such bias in naturally occurring plant communities. Here, we present quantitative evidence of the broad-scale pattern of twining direction in plants growing under natural conditions from a wide range of locations and habitats from around the world, and explore three potential underlying determinants of twining direction in climbing plants. Whether or not the reported predominance of right-handed helices represents a general phenomenon or is a result of each plant’s response to its environment will have important implications © 2007 The Authors Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd DOI: 10.1111/j.1466-8238.2007.00326.x www.blackwellpublishing.com/geb INTRODUCTION 795 W. Edwards et al. for our understanding of the nature of twining and its underlying basis. For example, if twining direction is associated with the environment in which a plant exists, then bias in twining direction must arise as a biological response to the physical conditions associated with its location. If, on the other hand, directional bias in twining is a global phenomenon, then alternative explanations for twining direction (independent of individual plant responses) will be needed. Our approach was, first, to determine on a global scale whether the incidence of left- versus right-handed twining direction in the stems of climbing plants differs significantly from random (hypothesis one), and secondly, if the incidence was found to be non-random (biased towards one direction), to test, by inference, two further hypotheses that could link twining bias with geographical location. The factors that control twining or determine twining direction are largely unknown. Randomness might be expected if climbing direction is set as a function of both circumnutation and a mechanical (thigmotropic) response initiated when a twining plant first contacts its supporting host (hypothesis one) (Darwin, 1876; Darwin & Darwin, 1880). Because the location of twining plants with respect to their potential hosts is determined by non-directional seed dispersal and deposition patterns, twining plants are equally likely to first encounter a host stem on their left as on their right. Thus, under this hypothesis, the incidence of left- and right-handed twining direction should be more or less equal. Alternatively, finding a broad-scale bias in twining direction would indicate that twining direction is under the influence of one or more controlling stimuli that operate at such a scale. Under this scenario, we were to test the influence of two such stimuli. The first is the potential influence of the position of the sun and its passage across the sky (a heliotropism). If plant apices track the sun’s passage, handedness may be related to the global position of the site in which a plant occurs (hypothesis two). For a twining plant growing in the Southern Hemisphere, the passage of the sun across the sky will be to its north for most of the year (for plants below the tropics this will be true for all times of the year). The reverse situation occurs for the Northern Hemisphere. If the apices of climbing plants track the apparent east–west movement of the sun across the sky while they cross the sunny faces of trees (the south side in the Northern Hemisphere and the north side in the Southern Hemisphere), climbing plants would twine in right-handed helices in the Southern Hemisphere and left-handed helices in the Northern Hemisphere. This hypothesis further predicts that the incidence of twining in a particular direction should be an increasing function of latitude, since the sun can appear to the north or the south of the plant at low latitudes (depending on the time of the year) but its northerly or southerly orientation is fixed at latitudes outside of the tropics. The second broad-scale influence we were to test for in the event of unequal incidence of left- and right-handed twining was the Coriolis effect. The Coriolis effect is a phenomenon related to the decrease in velocity experienced at the Earth’s surface as a function of increasing latitude that appears to deflect moving objects to the right in the Northern Hemisphere and to the left in 796 the Southern Hemisphere (Gill, 1982; Persson, 1998). We would test the hypothesis that the direction of circumnutation in apices (the rhythmic, elliptical movement in leader stems) of twining plants is influenced by the Coriolis effect (hypothesis three). This hypothesis predicts a simple switch from right-handed twining in the Northern Hemisphere to left-handed twining in the Southern Hemisphere. MATERIALS AND METHODS Surveys of twining direction were carried out on transects in natural vegetation at 17 sites from nine countries: Argentina, Australia, China, Mexico, New Zealand, Panama, Peru, the Republic of Congo and Zambia, across five continents (Figure 1 & Table 1). Eleven sites were in the Southern Hemisphere and six sites were in the Northern Hemisphere. The sites spanned approximately 65° of latitude [41°04′ S (Kaitoke, New Zealand) to 24°47′ N (Linares, Mexico)]. The direction of twining was recorded for the first c. 100 stems that were climbing upwards and that made at least two loops around another stem we encountered along transects through natural vegetation. The number of stems recorded at each site is reported in Table 1. We scored stems on the basis of whether their growth represented left- or right-handed directions by following the major growth axis from the ground upwards on host plants. Right-handed growth is recognizable as an increase in height from left to right around trees (viewed from any angle) (Hashimoto 2002) (Fig. 1, legend). All surveys except those from Cairns and Cape Tribulation (Australia) were carried out by A. T. Moles. We first tested whether there was a significant bias in the frequency of twining direction within sites via a simple chi-square goodness-of-fit test using expected frequencies based on equal proportions of left- and right-handed twining. We then examined the relationship between the percentage of plants at each site twining right-handedly and latitude via linear regression. We tested for differences in the proportion of right-handed helices between hemispheres via a t-test. Because percentage data do not meet the assumptions of standard statistical tests, all data on twining direction were logit-transformed (Dytham, 1999) before analysis. We had to subtract 0.5% from each of the three sites in which 100% of the sampled plants twined anticlockwise, in order to perform the logit transformation. RESULTS We found overwhelming bias in the direction of twining. In total, 1372 (92.4%) of all 1485 plants we encountered produced righthanded helices and were climbing up their hosts in an anticlockwise direction (Table 1 & Fig. 1). This is significantly more than expected if twining was equally likely to occur in both directions (χ2 = 553.3, d.f. = 16, P < 0.0001). The mean per site percentage of plants twining in a right-handed direction was 92.5% (range 67–100%). There was no evidence for a relationship between the percentage of right-handed stems at each site and either latitude (F1,16 = 1.02, P = 0.33) or hemisphere (t = 0.444, d.f. = 7.16, P = 0.63) © 2007 The Authors Global Ecology and Biogeography, 16, 795–800, Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd The global trend in plant twining direction Figure 1 Global distribution of the 17 study sites, and the percentage of plants producing either left- (open segment) or right-handed (filled segment) helices at each site. Right-handed growth is recognizable as an increase in height from left to right (see legend). Site details, country, sample sizes, vegetation types and results are provided in Table 1. Table 1 Sample size, location, vegetation description and the results of surveys of plant twining direction along transects in 17 sites spanning nine countries and approximately 65° latitude. Site location Number of stems right-handed Number of stems left-handed Percentage of stems right-handed Latitude Longitude Vegetation Bariloche, Argentina BCI, Panama Cairns, Australia Cape Tribulation, Australia Chajul, Mexico Chamela, Mexico Jujuy, Argentina 81 89 97 121 17 92 98 0 11 5 13 3 0 2 100.00 89.00 95.10 90.30 85.00 100.00 98.00 41°01′ S 09°09′ N 16°55′ S 16°05′ S 16°06′ N 19°30′ N 23°45′ S 71°49′ W 79°51′ W 145°46′ E 145°47′ E 90°59′ W 105°03′ W 64°51′ W Kabo, Republic of Congo Kaitoke, New Zealand Kuringai NP, Australia Lamington NP, Australia Linares, Mexico Los Amigos, Peru Royal NP, Australia Serenje, Zambia Tucuman, Argentina 96 99 67 58 30 68 93 83 87 4 1 33 4 0 11 7 2 13 96.00 99.00 67.00 93.55 100.00 86.08 93.00 97.65 87.00 02°08′ N 41°04′ S 33°34′ S 28°13′ S 24°47′ N 12°33′ S 34°09′ S 13°15′ S 26°46′ S 16°11′ E 175°10′ E 151°18′ E 153°08′ E 99°31′ W 70°06′ W 151°02′ E 30°03′ E 65°20′ W Xishuangabanna, China 96 4 96.00 21°57′ N 101°12′ E Nothofagus forest Tropical rain forest Tropical rain forest Tropical rain forest Tropical rain forest Tropical dry forest Yungas (seasonally dry subtropical woodland) Tropical rain forest Temperate rain forest Woodland (Eucalypt) Subtropical rain forest Thornscrub Tropical rain forest Temperate rain forest Mateshi (vine thicket) Yungas (seasonally dry subtropical woodland) Rain forest © 2007 The Authors Global Ecology and Biogeography, 16, 795–800, Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd 797 W. Edwards et al. Figure 2 Scatterplot of the percentage of plants producing right-handed helices (logit scale) versus latitude for each of the 17 study sites. Also shown are boxplots for the distribution of the percentage of right-handed helices in each hemisphere. There was no effect of latitude or hemisphere on twining direction. Site details, country, sample sizes and vegetation types are provided in Table 1. (Fig. 2). That is, twining plants growing at low latitudes were just as likely to produce right-handed helices as those growing at higher latitudes, and right-handed twining direction was equally prevalent in the Northern and Southern Hemispheres. We therefore reject the hypotheses that twining direction is a product of either the Coriolis effect or the apparent movement of the sun across the sky. DISCUSSION Our results confirm previously unverified claims that the production of right-handed helices is a general phenomenon among twining plants. The vast majority (92%) of stems of twining plants found in a wide range of vegetation types and geographical locations across the globe grew by twining in a right-handed direction. The results show that at a global scale the distribution of left- and right-handed helical growth in twining plants is nonrandom, and indeed is heavily biased to right-handed forms. Thus, we reject the first hypothesis, i.e. that twining direction is determined by a thigmotropic response in association with circumnutation and with the position of first contact between twining stem and support structure (Darwin & Darwin, 1880). This is consistent with a recent study using a non-twining gravitropic mutant cultivar of Japanese morning glory (Pharbitis nil or Ipomoea nil), Shidareasagao (weeping) (Kitazawa et al., 2005), which suggests that although circumnutation may play a role in winding, thigmotropism appears to have little influence (Kitazawa et al., 2005). Numerical dominance of anticlockwise twining occurred independent of hemisphere and latitude. Thus, we also reject the hypotheses that twining direction is determined by the apparent movement of the sun across the sky (hypothesis two) or by the Coriolis effect (hypothesis three). 798 The tendency towards right-handedness in twining direction cannot be the result of selective forces operating on mechanical strength either. The two dominant forces providing mechanical strength in twining plants are axial tensions and normal loads (Silk & Holbrook, 2005). Axial tensions operate tangentially to the helix created by the plant’s stem, while normal loads act in the horizontal plane, towards the centre of the supporting pole (Silk & Holbrook, 2005), and neither force is determined by twining direction. At present we know of no alternative adaptive hypothesis capable of explaining the bias towards right-handedness independent of latitude and hemisphere. In this context, the over-representation of right-handed twining that we observed may have no functional basis. What, then, may explain this pattern? In many animal taxa, directional asymmetries have been linked with microtubule orientation and operation. For example, early cloning of genes associated with asymmetry in mice coded for tubulin-based cilia, including two microtubule-based motors (Nonaka et al., 1998; Okada et al., 1999; Baum, 2006). These cilia occur in ordered rows and rotate in a clockwise direction that generates a leftward flow of extracellular fluid that gives rise to asymmetry (Horikawa et al., 2006). Similarly, microtubule alignment has been proposed as the basis for > 90% right-handed helices in the shells of snails under natural conditions (Schilthuizen & Davidson, 2005), and > 99% dextral (right-handed) internal organ orientation in nematodes (as well as many other metazoans with external bilateral symmetry) (Wood, 1998). Evidence from the study of plant root growth suggests that microtubules could also provide a possible explanation for directional asymmetries in twining plants. In plant cells, cortical microtubules are thought to guide the deposition of microfibrils: strands of structural cellulose that constitute a major mechanical element of the plant cell wall (Hussey, 2002). Cellulose microfibrils determine the final shape of plant cells by channelling non-directional turgor pressure during cell expansion. (We note, however, that while microtubules are most often parallel to microfibrils and help to align them, microfibril alignment also occurs independent of microtubules in some cases; see Baskin, 2001; Somerville, 2006). In roots, arrays of cortical microtubules can align transversely, helically or longitudinally to the primary growth axis. Liang et al. (1996) demonstrated that the orientation of microtubule arrays was consistent at specific locations in the roots of maize and arabidopsis. In both species, rapidly elongating cells displayed transverse arrays, ‘followed by helical arrays with consistent right-handed organization, followed in maize (and probably also in arabidopsis) by longitudinal and finally left-handed helical arrays’ (Liang et al., 1996, p. 13). When microtubules (and thus microfibrils) are transverse (as in normal elongating cells), in order for the cell to expand cross-linking glycans between microfibrils are cut. This allows the cellulose microfibrils to move apart under pressure, and cells to expand perpendicular to microtubule orientation and in line with the principal apical/basal growth axis (Lloyd & Chan, 2002). Two recent studies have shown that the early development of helical microtubule orientation can cause root growth to be ‘twisted’ in the opposite direction © 2007 The Authors Global Ecology and Biogeography, 16, 795–800, Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd The global trend in plant twining direction of rotation to the microtubule array (Furitani et al., 2000, Thitamadee et al., 2002). Whether or not mechanisms associated with the twisting of plant roots can be extended to explanations for twining direction in stems is uncertain, and should only be considered as a hypothesis at present. First, while some authors have argued that regular oscillation and a high degree of curvature in arabidopsis roots (a phenomenon knows as root waving) reflects circumnutation (Simmons et al., 1995; Rutherford & Masson, 1996; Migliaccio & Piconese, 2001), Thompson & Holbrook (2004) have demonstrated that much of this behaviour should be expected given the simple mechanical relationship between the size of roots and root tips, and the impedance of root growth due to the substrate. Second, we know of no previous study that has examined cortical microtubule or microfibril arrays in the shoots of twining plants. Patterns of left–right handedness in plant organs appear to be much less constant than our results for twining direction. For example, Spiranthes australis produces a helical arrangement of flowers on inflorescences. In self-pollination experiments, both left- and right-handed inflorescences are produced with equal frequency (Callos & Medford, 1994), and the spiral phyllotaxis in leaf arrangement of Nicotiniana tabacum also occurs equally in both left- and right-handed forms (Hashimoto, 2002). In the coconut palm (Cocos nucifera), spiral phyllotaxy producing left- and right-handed forms is similarly not an inherited trait (Toar et al., 1979, Minorsky & Bronstein, 2006). However, in C. nucifera, left- and right-handed forms are non-randomly distributed at a global scale: right-handed forms predominate in the Northern Hemisphere and left-handed forms in the Southern Hemisphere (Davis & Davis, 1987). Recent evidence suggests that asymmetry in phyllotaxy in coconut palms results from geomagnetic variability between locations rather than a simple Northern Hemisphere–Southern Hemisphere effect (Minorsky & Bronstien, 2006). While this appears to be true in coconut palms, the pattern in twining we report shows over-representation of right-handed helices in both hemispheres. Thus, it is unlikely that asymmetries in coconut palm phyllotaxy and plant twining have similar causes. The mechanism provided by the microtubule orientation hypothesis explains our data better than any alternative at present. If the microtubule basis for asymmetric growth in roots of maize and arabidopsis is also shown to be the cause of helical growth of shoots in twining species, one question still remains: why is microtubule orientation biased? Within arabidopsis, mutations can lead to both left-handed (Thitamadee et al., 2002) and right-handed (Furitani et al., 2000) forms. Twining plants are a polyphyletic grouping, the twining habit having evolved independently many times (Schnitzer & Bongers, 2002). Across all twining plants, there must have existed numerous opportunities for mutations resulting in patterns of microtubule orientation producing either left- or right-handed pathways. Indeed, there have been claims that twining handedness can be constant within a species, but differ between species (Gardner, 1970; Hashimoto, 2002; Hussey, 2002; Thitamadee et al., 2002). However, these claims are mostly anecdotal and have not been accompanied by quantitative data. Species identities were not recorded in our surveys. This is an important next step. We acknowledge that there is some potential for random taxonomic bias or bias in twining stem size associated with species that may have occurred in individual sites, and that these effects could give rise to the over-representation of one helical form or another. However, we consider it very unlikely that either effect repeatedly operated to produce a consistently high proportion of right-handed forms across independent locations. We have begun to investigate the occurrence of left- and right-handed helices in a phylogenetic framework. Resolving the question of what controls microtubule orientation could provide deeper insights into the nature of directional bias in twining plants. One possible explanation might be that microtubule directional bias reflects the chiral purity that exists in the terrestrial biosphere (19 of the 20 biologically important amino acids are left-handed; the other is achiral) (Goldanski & Kuzman, 1991). Left-handed amino acids give rise to predominantly right-handed helices in proteins (Novotny & Kleywegt, 2005). Tests of correspondence between the chirality of proteins, microtubule arrays and twining direction will be required to test this hypothesis. If true, the global pattern of dominance of righthanded twining forms may thus reflect the chance events that determined chiral purity. In summary, we have shown a global tendency for climbing plants to twine in an anticlockwise direction. This tendency cannot be explained by the side of the climbing stem that first encounters a host, by apices following the apparent movement of the sun across the sky, by the Coriolis effect or by differences in mechanical strength. As noted by Hussey (2002), in reference to von Marilaun (1902), different directions of twining may be determined by subcellular forces. An investigation of cell wall structure in left- and right-handed stems will be required to test the idea that this global pattern in twining direction might be due to the interplay between the chirality of biological molecules and the orientation of microtubule arrays. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank Nina Babiuk for first posing this question to us, Tang Yong, Sarah Boulter and Benjamín Magaña for their help with data collection, and Linda Beaumont for help in producing Fig. 1. An Australian Research Council grant supported A. T. Moles, and funded her international data collection. Tobias Baskin and one anonymous referee provided invaluable advice and suggestions, especially in relation to microtubule operation and orientation. REFERENCES Baskin, T. (2001) On the alignment of cellulose microfibrils by cortical microtubules: a review and a model. Protoplasma, 215, 150–171. Baum, B. (2006) Left-right asymmetry: actin-myosin through the looking glass. Current Biology, 16, R502–R504. Callos, J.D. & Medford, J.I. (1994) Organ positions and pattern formation in the shoot apex. Plant Journal, 6, 1–7. © 2007 The Authors Global Ecology and Biogeography, 16, 795–800, Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd 799 W. Edwards et al. Darwin, C. (1876) The movement and habits of climbing plants. Appleton, New York. Darwin, C. & Darwin, F. (1880) The power of movement in plants. John Murray, London. Davis, T.A. & Davis, B. (1987) Association of coconut foliar spirality with latitude. Mathematical Modelling, 8, 730–733. Dytham, C. (1999) Choosing and using statistics: a biologist’s guide. Blackwell Science, Oxford. Furitani, I., Watanabe, Y., Prieto, R., Masukawa, M., Suzuki, K., Naoi, K., Thitamadee, S., Shikanai, T. & Hashimoto, T. (2000) The SPIRAL genes are required for directional control of cell elongation in Arabidopsis thaliana. Development, 127, 4443– 4453. Gardner, M. (1970) The ambidextrous universe. Penguin, Victoria, Australia. Gill, A.E. (1982) Atmosphere–ocean dynamics. Academic Press, New York. Goldanskii, V. & Kuzman, V.V. (1991) Chirality and the cold origin of life. Nature, 352, 114. Hashimoto, T. (2002) Molecular genetic analysis of left-right handedness in plants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Biological Sciences, 357, 799 – 808. Horikawa, N., Tanaka, Y., Okada, Y. & Takeda, S. (2006) Nodal flow and the generation of left-right asymmetry. Cell, 125, 33 –45. Hussey, P.J. (2002) Microtubules do the twist. Nature, 417, 128 –129. Kitazawa, D., Hatakeda, Y., Kamada, M., Fujii, N., Miyazawa, Y., Hoshino, A., Iida, S., Fukaki, H., Terao Morita, M., Tasaka, M., Suge, H. & Takahashi, H. (2005) Shoot circumnutation and winding movements require gravisensing cells. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102, 18742 –18747. Liang, B.M., Dennings, A.M., Sharp, R.E. & Baskin, T.I. (1996) Consistent handedness of microtubule helical arrays in maize and Arabidopsis primary roots. Protoplasma, 190, 8–15. Lloyd, C. & Chan, J. (2002) Helical microtubule arrays and spiral growth. The Plant Cell, 14, 2319–2324. MacArthur, R.D. (1972) Geographical ecology: patterns in the distribution of species. Harper & Row, New York. von Marilaun, K.A. (1902) The natural history of plants, their forms, growth, reproduction and distribution (transl. by F.W. Oliver with the assistance of Lady Busk and M.F. Macdonald). Blackie, London. Mayr, E. (1963) Animal species and evolution. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. Migliaccio, G.D. & Piconese, S. (2001) Spiralizations and tropisms in Arabidopsis. Trends in Plant Science, 6, 561–565. Minorsky, P.V. & Bronstein, N.B. (2006) Natural experiments indicate that geomagnetic variations cause spatial and temporal variations in coconut palm asymmetry. Plant Physiology, 142, 40 –44. Nonaka, S., Tanaka, Y., Okada, Y., Takeda, S., Harada, A. & Kanai, Y. (1998) Randomization of left-right asymmetry due to loss of nodal cilia generating leftward flow of extraembryonic fluid in mice lacking KIF3B motor protein. Cell, 95, 829 – 837. 800 View publication stats Novotny, M. & Kleywegt, G.J. (2005) A survey of left-handed helices in protein structures. Journal of Molecular Biology, 347, 231–241. Okada, Y., Nonaka, S., Tanaka, Y., Saijoh, Y., Hamada, H. & Hirokawa, N. (1999) Abnormal nodal flow precedes situs inversus in iv and inv mice. Molecular Cell, 4, 459–468. Persson, A. (1998) How do we understand the Coriolis force? Bulletin of the American Meteorological Society, 79, 1373 –1385. Rutherford, R. & Masson, P.H. (1996) Arabidopsis thaliana sku mutant seedlings show exaggerated surface-dependent alteration in root growth vector. Plant Physiology, 111, 987–998. Schnitzer, S.A. & Bongers, F. (2002) The ecology of lianas and their role in forests. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 17, 223–230. Schilthuizen, M. & Davidson, A. (2005) The convoluted evolution of snail chirality. Naturwissenschaften, 92, 504–515. Silk, W.K. & Holbrook, N.M. (2005) The importance of frictional interactions in maintaining the stability of the twining habit. American Journal of Botany, 92, 1820–1826. Simmons, C., Soll, D. & Migliaccio, F. (1995) Circumnutation and gravitropism cause root waving in Arabidopsis thaliana. Journal of Experimental Botany, 46, 143–150. Somerville, C. (2006) Cellulose synthesis in higher plants. Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology, 22, 53–78. Stevens, G.C. (1989) The latitudinal gradients in geographical range: how so many species co-exist in the tropics. The American Naturalist, 133, 240–256. Thitamadee, S., Tuchihara, K. & Hashimoto, T. (2002) Microtubule basis for left-handed helical growth in Arabidopsis. Nature, 417, 193–196. Thompson, M.V. & Holbrook, N.M. (2004) Root-gel interactions and the root waving behavior of Arabidopsis. Plant Physiology, 135, 1822–1837. Toar, R.E., Rompas, T.M. & Sudasrip, H. (1979) The noninheritance of the direction of foliar spiral in coconut. Experientia, 35, 1585–1587. Wood, W.B. (1998) Handedness asymmetry in nematodes. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology, 9, 56–60. BIOSKETCHES Will Edwards is a senior lecturer in tropical ecology. His interests are rainforest diversity, seed dispersal and decisions involved in optimal foraging. Angela Moles’ interests are in the ecology and evolution of seed size, and in global patterns in plant traits. Data for the present paper were gathered while Angela was establishing study sites for a worldwide study of herbivory. Peter Franks is a senior lecturer in plant physiology, with research interests in xylem function and the regulation of plant gas exchange. Editor: Martin Sykes © 2007 The Authors Global Ecology and Biogeography, 16, 795–800, Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd