Keys to Successful Smartphone Use in the Religious Classroom Brandon Comstock Crimson Ridge Seminary St George, Utah Abstract Smartphones in the religious classroom can be a two-edged sword. While the resources available through the Gospel Library app offer an abundance of potential for learning opportunities, the wealth of distractions associated with smartphones can cause many to question the prudence of their use in the religious classroom. This paper explores both elements of the smartphone dilemma. The first portion will review the literature on the negative impact of distractions on learning outcomes. The second portion will address the literature on the potential benefits of smartphones as a learning tool in the classroom, focusing on the research related to the utilization of the Gospel Library app. In addition to a literature review, this paper will offer several suggestions on best practices teachers can utilize to help their students righteously utilize smartphones, and meet the Objective of Seminaries and Institutes. Introduction In 2007 Steve Jobs stood before an eager audience and introduced what he described as "a revolutionary and magical product" that came to be known as the smartphone (Apple Newsroom, 2010). Twelve years later, it's hard to argue with his prediction. Still, the smartphone's ramifications have been far more nuanced than most were able to foresee after hearing Jobs' glowing introduction. From the perspective of religious educators, the smartphone presents a challenging dichotomy. On the one hand, smartphones give students access to endless amounts of material to enhance their learning experience. Still, ironically, in the same hand, it provides students access to a seemingly infinite amount of distractions that can seriously impair their learning experience. This paper will address both sides of the smartphone dilemma. First, we will review the relevant literature on the impact of smartphone distractions on learning. We will then pivot to review the literature on how teachers and students can best utilize smartphones to enhance learning and fulfill the Objective of Seminaries and Institutes. Smartphones as a Distraction In recent years, support for using smartphones as an educational tool has grown (Thomas, O'Bannon, & Bolton, 2013). Still, the reality is that most of the time that students spend on their smartphones has very little to do with education (Lin, Zhang, Jung, & Kim, 2013). One study found that 48% of all apps on students' phones were games, while just 3% were educational (Tossell, Kortum, Shephard, Rahmati, & Zhong, 2015). This discrepancy illustrates the most significant challenge to properly utilizing smartphones in the classroom; While teachers seek to use smartphones as an educational tool, most students view them as an entertainment device or a toy (Noyes & Garland, 2005). As a result, smartphones often work as a trigger to divert attention away from students' studies rather than enhance it. Several studies have sought to quantify the impact of digital distractions. End, Worhtman, Matthews, and Wetterau (2010) tested the effect of a phone ringing in class on students' ability to recall material. The researchers placed calls to two fellow researchers posing as subjects in the test group who allowed their phones to ring for exactly five seconds before silencing them. A post-movie assessment showed that students exposed to the ringing had a 25% reduction in the recall of material covered during the period that the phone was ringing compared to the control group. Froese et al. conducted a similar study but focused instead on the impact of texting. Experimenters asked subjects to watch a ten-minute PowerPoint presentation, with half of the subjects turning their devices off. The other half received periodic texts from the researchers throughout the presentation. Those in the texting group scored 27% lower on the subsequent comprehension test than those in the control group. When considering the overwhelming number of students who openly admit to engaging in these behaviors during class (Tindell, & Bohlander, 2012), educators' concerns about the impact of smartphone use in the classroom are wellfounded. Unfortunately, smartphone distractions may stretch even farther than those caused by actual use in the classroom. Several studies are now suggesting that the mere presence of a smartphone may be enough to have significant negative impacts on learning, even when students are not actively using them. For example, Bowman, Levine, Waite, and Gendron (2010) placed students into one of three groups and asked them to read a 3800-word essay. Group one received a series of instant messages before they began reading—group two received messages throughout their reading. Group three served as the control, receiving no messages. While students in all three groups scored similarly on the recall test, the time it took to complete the reading was significantly different for each group. Surprisingly, the control group was not the fastest. Students who received messages before they began reading completed the reading nearly nine minutes faster (28.63 minutes) than those in the control group (37.44 minutes). The author explained the outcome this way: The instructions given to the three groups may have had an impact. Each participant received instructions that they would receive IM's before reading, during reading, or not at all. Those who received the IMs at the beginning may have realized that they would not receive any more IMs and could attend to the passage they were reading without thinking they might be interrupted (pg. 4). In essence, the very thought that they might receive a message was enough to impact the subjects' learning experience significantly. While the idea that the thought of being distracted is enough to impact a student sounds far-fetched, additional studies seem to support the idea. One study provided subjects with a series of single-spaced digits and asked them to perform either a simple task (circling each occurrence of the number five) or a more complex task (circling each pair of consecutive digits that added up to eleven). Researchers then innocuously placed either a smartphone or a spiral notebook on the table where subjects were completing the work. While a smartphone's presence had no impact on the simpler task, those who completed the complex task in the presence of a smartphone performed much poorer than those in the notebook group (Thornton, Faires, Robbins, & Rollins, 2014). Przybylski and Weinstein used a similar design to take the experiment one step further by exploring whether the mere presence of a phone would negatively affect their subject's perception of a partners trust and empathy. Researchers asked subjects to talk to each other for ten minutes on either a casual topic (their feelings on plastic Christmas trees) or a meaningful topic (the most meaningful event that had happened in the past month). Researchers then placed either a spiral notebook or a smartphone on the table as participants talked. At the end of the period, researchers asked subjects to rate their partner on statements like: "I felt like I could really trust my conversation partner" and "my partner accurately understood my thoughts and feelings about the topic." While smartphones had no impact on casual conversations, subjects in the meaningful discussions reported significantly less empathy and trust when a smartphone was present instead of a notebook (Przybylski, Weinstein, 2013). In both of the above studies, the device used did not belong to the subjects and never rang, buzzed, or lit up during the experiment. Still, the mere presence of a smartphone was sufficient to negatively impact both complex thinking and the establishment of meaningful relationships. Most alarmingly, smartphones' impact seems to be particularly poignant on deeplevel processing and application of knowledge to new settings. As one author put it, Even if distraction does not decrease the overall level of learning, it can result in the acquisition of knowledge that can be applied less flexibly in new situations. There's a possibility that we are raising a generation that is learning more shallowly than young people in the past" (Paul, 2013: pg. 2). Given the current emphasis on obtaining deep learning in Seminaries and Institutes, these findings should be cause for concern. While such concerns are shared by church leadership, the direction for gospel teachers is both clear and comforting. In 1995, long before the smartphone existed, President Howard W. Hunter prophesied that "the role of technology in this work has been accelerated and will continue to do so. However, we stand only on the threshold of what we can do" (Hunter, 1995). Years later, Elder Richard G Scott taught that "Technology, when understood and used for righteous purposes, need not be a threat but rather an enhancement to spiritual communication" (Scott, 2013). In the most recent years, Church leadership has been even more direct in their invitations to accept technology's role. Elder Gary E. Stevenson invited gospel teachers to "teach and demonstrate the righteous use of technology to the rising generation" (Stevenson, 2017). Likewise, Brian Ashton (2018) issued an invitation from the General Sunday School Presidency for all teachers to "Embrace technology in their lessons and make every effort to help youth learn to use technology for righteous purposes." Sadly, the optimistic tone in the quotes above is often absent when parents and teachers express their feelings about smartphones in the classroom. Instead, teachers often focus on and emphasize technology's pitfalls, with benefits mentioned only as an afterthought. One study on communication between parents and teens about smartphones concluded that "while parents report that they communicate with their children about technology use, their children say that parents only tell them which behaviors to avoid" rather than which behaviors to pursue (Blackwell, Gardiner, & Shoenbeck, 2016, pg. 1390). With that in mind, it should come as no surprise that study after study has found that students struggle to view smartphones as devices they can use for serious academic study (Baron, 2017; Chen, Cheng, Chang Zheng, & Huang, 2014; Keim, 2014; Nichols, 2016; Nichols, 2018; Noyes & Garland, 2005; Rosenwald, 2014; Tossell, Kortum, Shephard, Rahmati, Zhong, 2015). Ironically, given the significant impact of viewing smartphones as a distraction, this constant labeling of smartphones as a barrier to learning may inhibit students from seeing smartphones as a serious tool, instead magnifying their distraction when they are asked to learn in a smartphone filled environment. In other words, lectures on the pitfalls of smartphones may be leading to more smartphone distractions. Smartphones as a Tool How, then, should teachers handle the challenges of smartphones? The key to the situation seems to be orchestrating a switch in both teachers' and students' minds about smartphones' purpose and role. Rather than encouraging students to view their devices as distractions, teachers must help students learn to see their devices as a tool to help them grow closer to their Heavenly Father. Only when teachers and students' mindset about the role of smartphones in the classroom has changed will the desired results manifest themselves. The remainder of this paper will outline six best practices for religious educators to help facilitate that change and help their students use their smartphones righteously in the classroom. Each of the practices will explore relevant literature before offering practical suggestions for implementation in the classroom. It is worth noting that while smartphones in the classroom have a variety of potential applications, this paper's scope will focus primarily on the effective utilization of the Gospel Library app. Future research opportunities should explore alternative methods of smartphone learning. Practice #1 - Train On the surface, the expectation that students should inherently know how to use their smartphones makes sense. In today's world, both reading and smartphone use are a part of most children's upbringing. However, on closer examination, for this argument to be valid, two assumptions must hold: 1) smartphone reading must be the same as print reading, and 2) using a device for study must require the same skills as using a device for entertainment and communication. In reality, digital study is, in many ways, an entirely different experience than studying in print. Studies have shown a difference in eye strain (Kang, Wang, & Lin, 2009), posture (Taipale, 2015), navigation methods (Sanders, 2017), emotional attachment to the content (MacWilliam, 2013), reading speed, and others; each of which plays a significant role in the readers reading experience, and comprehension of texts. Likewise, utilizing a smartphone for serious study requires very different skills than using a device for personal entertainment. As one author put it, "it cannot be assumed that knowing how to look up 'cheats' for computer games on the internet bears any relation to the skills required to assess a website's relevance for a school project" (Bennett, Maton, & Kervin, 2008). Given the differences in print and digital reading, teachers should not assume that students intrinsically know how to utilize their digital scriptures. Providing students with effective training on how to utilize even basic aspects of resources like the Gospel Library app is critical to the success of smartphone usage in the classroom. Schugar, Schugar, and Penny (2011) found that while 50% of students highlighted paper texts on a daily basis, just 14.3% did the same when reading digitally. Likewise, Bold and Wagstaff (2017) found that while 74% of subjects in their study had written notes in a paper book's margins, just 35% had written notes in a digital book, despite the majority of students expressing a desire to do so. In each of the studies, the consensus amongst researchers on digital annotation was that "more would do so if they knew how or if the particular format allowed them" (Mizrachi, 2015, pg. 304). For some teachers, hesitancy to train students on the basics may come from their students' reassurance that they know how to use the resources. Unfortunately, students regularly overestimate their abilities to utilize technology (Dunning, Johnson, Ehrlinger, & Kruger, 2003). For example, in an unpublished study of 331 incoming freshman seminary students, 82% reported already having the app installed on their smartphone, and 65% reported knowing how to use the device either "very well" or "extremely well." However, when asked more specifically about their knowledge, just 41% of students knew how to log-in using their LDSAccount. Likewise, only 17% knew how to create a link, one of the app's core annotation features (Comstock, 2016). The study affirmed the idea mentioned above that students often overestimate their proficiency in smartphone use. In contrast, some students may have sufficient knowledge of how a tool works but lack the familiarity to know how to utilize it effectively. VanHorne, Russell, and Schuh (2016) conducted a study to find out how long it took students to use the various features of an etextbook. Researchers were surprised to find that even after completing training on how to utilize the features on the first day of class, the average student took 28 days to make their first highlight. None of the other features ever obtained enough use to reach a median. The researchers emphasized that adoption rates were much higher when instructors offered ongoing training. They concluded that "as part of the course, learners may need to carry out several activities in which they are provided structured guidance on effective note-taking, bookmarking, and annotating" to be able to use the tools effectively (pg. 428). While these trainings take time, the results seem to be worth the effort. Azevedo and Cromley (2004) found that students who received sufficient training on reading digital texts saw significantly higher comprehension than those who received no training. Likewise, while some studies have suggested that the benefits of asking students to annotate digitally are not as beneficial as asking them to annotate in print (Ben-Yehuda & Ehest-Alakalai 2014), when students are trained on the use of in-depth processing skills like tagging main phrases, the comprehension gap disappears (Lauterman & Ackerman, 2014) and students' motivation to read increases dramatically (Durham, & Raymond, 2016). Given the literature, it seems evident that the successful usage of smartphones in the classroom requires consistent, ongoing, specific training on how to utilize important features. Practice #2 - Model While training is critical, it represents just half of Elder Stevenson's call to "teach and demonstrate the righteous use of technology to the rising generation" (Stevenson, 2017). Perhaps more important than the impact of training students on technical skills, is the impact of modeling the use of smartphones as an effective study tool. Then-President Dieter F Uchtdorf (2018) taught that "The most effective way to preach the gospel is through example." Likewise, it may be that the most effective way to teach technology is through modeling. In the battle to change perceptions about smartphones' purpose in the classroom, modeling by respected leaders may serve as the primary tool to help students see their devices the way church leaders invite them. Since most of the time students spend on smartphones is casual, many students struggle to view their smartphones as a serious study tool (Baron, 2017; Mizrachi, 2015; Noyes & Garland, 2005). Fortunately, research has shown that effective modeling can change that. One study surveyed students at the beginning of a course and found that just 22% of students preferred to use a digital text. However, after being exposed to the instructor's modeling of the digital text on the projector throughout the semester, 50% of the students reported that they preferred it over the printed version, and 65% said that the features of the digital version enhanced their learning experience (Dobler, 2015). Training and modeling had changed the student's perspectives on the usefulness of the digital text. To accomplish such results, teachers need to understand the inherent differences in digital and print reading. Traditional reading uses a linear approach. The author controls what the reader views and guides them from start to finish down a designated path. Digital reading, however, utilizes a series of hyperlinks and digital connections to create a non-linear reading approach. In this style, the reader takes charge of which content they will view next (Mangen, Walgermo, & Brønnick 2013; Schugar, Schugar, & Penny 2011). While the opportunity to dive deeper into any given topic increases the potential for deep learning, the added complexity comes at a cost. Specifically, the energy used to organize and process the information can become a drain on students' ability to focus on a particular topic and decrease total comprehension. Given the differences in digital and paper reading, both training and modeling should focus on the functions of the Gospel Library app that help students organize and make sense of the material. Long before smartphones, Elder Boyd K Packer explained it this way: Our youth need to know how to mark the scriptures, and they need to have some kind of filing system. In addition to that, if you give each one of them a framework upon which the truths they discover at random can be organized into a personal testimony, you will have served them well (Packer, 1993) The Gospel Library app utilizes five core annotation features, highlighting, tagging, notes, notebooks, and links. While research has shown that these features significantly impact comprehension and student enjoyment of reading (Durham, & Raymond, 2016), research also shows that unless students are provided with modeling from either their peers or their instructor, to use as a scaffolding for their own annotations, the benefits of use were negligible (Johnson, Archibald, & Tenenbaum, 2010). In practice, modeling smartphone usage in the classroom is quite simple. The more students become familiar and comfortable utilizing their devices, the more capable they will be using them for deep learning (Chen, Cheng, Chang, Zheng, & Huang, 2014). Instructors can mirror their device using a projector, then briefly explain their annotation decisions. The following are some suggested phrases for teachers to utilize while modeling the Gospel Library app for their students: "I chose to highlight this in blue because. . ." "You can see that I wrote that principle in the margins." "I wanted to be able to find that quote later, so I tagged it as. . . " "I'd like you to take a minute and write in your journal about what you've felt today; here's what I wrote in mine." "When I read this, it reminded me of my favorite talk from last General Conference, so I linked it to this verse." In addition to the annotation features, instructors may wish to model practical usage of the search and share functions in the Gospel Library app. Before moving on, it is worth mentioning one warning about modeling. While there will certainly be times when teachers need to show students how to navigate through the content, the purpose of modeling is to give students an effective framework, not to help them replicate what the teacher has done. Dowd and Green (2016) were emphatic about this point: Do not let students attempt to follow you click by click. This does not work. Keeping a group of twenty in the same place when going through multiple clicks is impossible. . . Ask students to put their devices down and watch you go through all the steps. You will find at least half of the students can then proceed independently, and you can empower the students to ask each other for help (pg. 53). Practice #3 - Simplify Much of the difficulty for students trying to learn both the Gospel, and the Gospel Library app can be explained through cognitive load theory (Sweller, 1994). The concept of cognitive load is simple. Each person has a fixed amount of capacity for processing information at one time. Each task they complete utilizes some degree of that capacity. When a person is less familiar with an activity, it typically requires more cognitive energy. However, with practice, an activity can become nearly automatic and require very little cognitive capacity (Antonenko, Niederhauser, 2010; Mangen, Walgermo, Bronick, 2013; Niederhasuer, Reynolds, Salmen, & Skolmoski, 2000). For digital reading, this concept is critical. Since most students' reading experience comes from printed books, and most students' smartphone experience comes from leisure activities, when students first begin studying on a smartphone, the cognitive load is significant (Nichols, 2016). This considerable burden is likely why multiple surveys have found that readers still prefer to do serious reading on paper (Baron, 2017; Dobler, 2015; Kurata, Ishita, Miyata, & Minami, 2017; Noyes & Garland, 2005; Two Sides, 2015; Scholastic, 2015). It is also likely why college students' preferences for print or digital material shift dramatically towards digital reading as they obtain more years of experience with the format (Nichols, 2018). When students are new to digital reading, the additional cognitive load associated with learning to use the format makes it harder to study effectively, but once they have obtained sufficient experience, that demand is negligible. For gospel teachers, the implication is simple. The more complicated the use of Gospel Library is for a student, the less cognitive space the student will have to focus on the lesson's content. For this reason, it is wise for religious educators to do all that they can to make the use of the Gospel Library app as intuitive and simple as possible. Within the Gospel Library app, two main areas present the greatest simplification possibilities: navigation and annotation. Ironically, the abundance of material available on the Gospel Library app is both the main reason it is such a useful tool, and the main reason its navigation can be taxing. For example, if a student were reading from the scriptures and wanted to get access to the previously mentioned Elder Stevenson quote, the student would have to use the following steps: 1. Select the screen manager, 2. select the add screen button, 3. select General Conference, 4. select the October 2017 session, 5. scroll to the Saturday afternoon session, 6. select "Spiritual Eclipses," 7. and scroll to the thirty-second paragraph. Since each task exerts additional demands on the students' cognitive load, the seven-step process of reaching the quote is likely to result in the student losing engagement with the lesson. Instead, a teacher seeking to reduce that load may invite students to select the search bar, then type in "teach and demonstrate" in quotations in the search field. If students type in the phrase in quotations, their search will yield just one general conference talk. When the student selects the talk, they are taken directly to the appropriate paragraph without scrolling, significantly reducing their cognitive load demand. Even better, a teacher may use the share function in the Gospel Library app to generate a paragraph specific link. Once the teacher has generated a link in the Gospel Library app, they can use a free QR code generator to create a paragraph specific link that will take a student directly to the quote in their own Gospel Library app. An example of such a QR code is provided to the right. To use the code, simply open your smartphone's camera (or QR code reader) and point it at the code. A button will appear asking you to open the Gospel Library app, which will open directly to the Elder Stevenson quote we have been discussing. Using approaches such as these, teachers can dramatically decrease students' cognitive load demands and increase their ability to use technology to learn the gospel effectively. In addition to simplifying navigation, teachers can take several small steps to simplify students’ experience with annotation. The following phrases are examples of ways to reduce the demand on students' cognitive load by reminding them how to annotate digitally. "We're going to do a journal now; if you'd like to do it digitally, go ahead and select the icon that looks like a piece of paper with the corner folded up." "If you want to tag that so that you can come back to it later, just select the button that looks like the Best Buy logo and type in whatever word you think best describes the verse." "If you want to link that verse to a conference talk, you can just push the button that looks like a chain and link them together." It is important to note that even when students have received training on how to annotate and claim to know how to complete these tasks, the cognitive load demands will still be significant if the student is required to recall these instructions each time they annotate. For this reason, it may be wise for teachers to briefly explain how to annotate each time they invite students to do so. When teachers adopt this practice, it allows students to preserve as much cognitive space as possible for Gospel-centered thinking. Practice #4 – Direct To this point, the majority of suggestions for maximizing technology have dealt with how to utilize smartphones' features. In contrast, offering direction focuses primarily on when to use the various features. In many cases, despite being able to explain the technical aspects of how to use features in the Gospel Library app, a student may need to be prompted as to when a certain tool would be particularly effective. While knowing when to annotate may appear to be a natural extension of knowing how to annotate, digital reading's unique non-linear approach makes the application far more challenging than it appears. In contrast to the traditional linear approach where a reader spends a long amount of time on one article or book, digital reading uses an entirely different approach, encouraging readers to browse and scan a variety of texts by jumping from link to link (Keim, 2014). In theory, this approach has several benefits. Antonenko and Niederhauser (2010) explained that: Reading hypertext is a task of exploration. . . The unique characteristics of hypertext allow hypertext authors to create connections to other related topics that are not easily accomplished in traditional print text presentations. Hyperlinks form a more intricate web of connected information nodes than is permitted by the straightforward flow of a print text" (pg. 140). Unfortunately, these outcomes are often difficult to realize. Instead, readers often compensate for the overwhelmingly large amount of content available by bouncing from text to text, without taking the time to process and analyze each piece of information (Carr, 2008; Jabr, 2013; Nichols, 2018). As this new style of surface-level reading becomes more and more typical, readers need specific directions on how to reengage their minds in deeper level processing while reading. One scholar concluded her study on the importance of assigning students to annotate their texts by explaining that: When readers slow down, they become more active. Readers allow themselves to become more aware of their thinking process when they are active, and they consider and work to make sense of ideas that they may not have been aware of even existed when they read quickly. Many students found themselves reading to make meaning rather than reading to complete the assignment (Porter-Odonnell, 2004: pg. 87). Her students seemed to support her theory. One student explained that "it is so much harder to fake read if you have to annotate like we have to do now. So now I actually read, because it's too hard to fake annotate" (pg. 87). Another student explained the impact of annotating by saying, "I have learned to not read very fast so I can comprehend the reading better. I used to read fast, and it made me not understand what exactly I was reading. I discovered that even if annotating is an annoyance, it can really help me in the end" (pg. 87). Other studies lend themselves to the same conclusion. Lauterman and Ackerman (2014) found that while digital reading typically resulted in lower comprehension than print reading, when students completed a meaningful task while reading (identifying four keywords), there was no significant difference between print and paper readers. More importantly, RockinsonSzapkiw, Holder, and Dunn (2011) found that when students received a specific assignment to annotate digital texts as they read, they performed better than students who completed the same assignment on paper and felt a deeper connection with the text. Durham and Raymond's (2016) work also supports the importance of specific directions on annotation. Their study gave specific directions to young students on how to annotate texts to identify main ideas. Results showed that 75% felt it helped them remember more of the text, 65% claimed it helped them enjoy reading more, and 50% said it helped motivate them to read and understand more. Given these findings, regardless of the medium students are using to access the scriptures, instructors would be wise to give specific directions to students on how to slow down and process the information in the text more carefully. The following is a list of phrases that teachers might utilize to help direct students as they use their smartphones to complete reading assignments in the classroom. "As you read through these verses, will you please highlight what you feel is the most important word in each of the verses?" "As you read this passage, I need you to identify and tag the basic doctrine that you think is best taught." "When you finish the reading, will you create a note next to your favorite verse that summarizes what you feel is the main principle of the passage?" (after reading a quote) "As you read through these verses, will you find the verse that best relates to the quote we just read and create a link to it?" The phrases above, of course, are only examples. Regardless of what is said, the key is that students receive specific directions on utilizing their devices effectively as often as possible. Ideally, teachers should not give a reading assignment without providing specific directions on how to understand the text more deeply. Practice #5 - Honor Agency Perhaps the most undervalued factor in utilizing smartphones is the role of the student's agency. In 2013 Elder Paul V. Johnson cautioned against being overly prescriptive about one format or another, teaching: What if some teacher has left the impression that one cannot truly study and use the scriptures if they are in an electronic form, and then when students become missionaries and begin using a tablet for their scripture study and teaching? We wouldn't want them to think they could only experience a lower level of scripture study because they are using the electronic version. There may also be teachers, in an attempt to utilize the technology they find advantageous, that send the message to students or colleagues that unless they are studying the scriptures in a digital format, they are not doing it correctly. As far as the effect of the word on lives is concerned, one source for the scriptures is not inherently better than another (Johnson, 2013). Not surprisingly, several academic studies support Johnson's perspective. Stone and BakerEveleth (2013) found that social pressure from peers and instructors was a significant factor in whether students choose to utilize an e-textbook or a printed version. More importantly, research shows that when a student is required to utilize a format that is not in line with their preferences, its usefulness is greatly diminished. For example, Lauterman and Ackerman (2014) found that students overcame the traditional setbacks of digital reading with training and practice but were quick to note that this was only the case when students reported a preference for the digital format. The authors explained, "Using their non-preferred medium seems to prevent participants from recruiting the mental effort required to achieve cognitive and metacognitive processes as effective as those demonstrated by on-screen learners preferring screen, and on paper, learners preferring paper" (pg. 481). Similarly, several studies have shown that the utilization of smartphones in the classroom can increase student engagement and desire for learning (Nichols, 2018; Odom, 2012, Rockinsaw-Szapkiw, Holder, & Dunn, 2011). Other studies have found that readers who have had a particularly meaningful experience with paper books in the past may struggle to have the same connection with a digital version (Read, Robertson, & McQuilken, 2011; Waheed, Kaur, & Sanni, 2015). Conversely, research shows that students with visual impairments and certain reading disabilities can read quicker and with higher comprehension when reading from a handheld device than from print (Schneps, Shomason, Chen, Sonnert, & Pomplun, 2013). All of this research leads to a simple conclusion; some students will achieve better outcomes utilizing smartphones as their reading device, while others will achieve better results using a printed version. For this reason, teachers should direct students to utilize whatever format gives them the best chance to succeed, rather than insisting that all students use a particular format. To help students properly utilize their agency, teachers may consider taking time to ask students some of the following questions: "What do you think is the most important factor for you to consider when deciding which format to use this year?" "Which of these formats do you think would give you the best chance to grow closer to your Heavenly Father?" "How might it benefit you to utilize a different format than you are currently using to study the scriptures?" Once a teacher has assisted a student in deciding which format will work best for them, it may be appropriate to express the expectation that each student utilize the format that will best help them meet the Objective of seminary, whether it be print, digital, or some combination of the two. Practice #6 - Discipline appropriately Unfortunately, the drawback of allowing students to utilize their agency is that sometimes they will misuse it. For better or worse, however, this trend is not unique to Seminaries and Institutes. One study placed observers in the back of a college classroom to see how much time students spent on task when asked to work on an assignment during class. The researcher found that behavior started declining steeply after only two minutes. By fifteen minutes, students had spent just 65% of their time on task (Paul, 2013). The same article reported that in one study, "spies" were used to see how regularly students were off-task and found that 58% of students were off task more than half the time (Paul, 2013). Why are so many students spending so much time off-task? The reality may be that many teens are full-on addicted to their devices. A 2016 Common Sense Media report found that 59% of parents felt that their teens were addicted to their phones. Still, more alarmingly, 50% of teens acknowledged that they were addicted. While the previously discussed steps of training, modeling, simplifying, directing, and activating agency can go a long way towards helping teenagers shift their perception about phone use, with 50% of students addicted to devices, it should come as no surprise when distractions arise (Felt, & Robb, 2016). There are, however, environmental circumstances that either enhance or diminish the likelihood that students will be distracted in class. For example, Tindell and Bohlander (2012) found that the top reasons students texted in the class were that 1. 2. 3. 4. There was no clear policy, and the instructor didn't seem to mind. The instructor stays in front of the class without circulating. The instructor often has his back to the class, The instructor doesn't require student participation. While not mentioning smartphones specifically, the Gospel Teaching and Learning Handbook offers remedies to each of these problems in the how-to "correct disorderly or inappropriate behavior" section. The handbook suggests that teachers "Make eye contact. . . Stop talking. . . Move closer. . . [and] Direct a question" (Gospel Teaching and Learning Handbook, 2.2.3). Likewise, Bernstein (2019) gave a perspective that is challenging for many teachers to accept: The blame is usually placed on the shoulders of the listener, distracted in the smartphone era by multiple screens and multitasking. . . But now communication experts say we need to focus on what the talker is doing wrong, too. Often, they say, talkers engage in a monologue rather than a dialogue. They drone on and ignore the listeners' cues that he or she is disengaged. They sometimes accuse the listener of spacing out, causing hurt feelings, or starting an argument. In essence, if many of a teacher's students are distracted by their smartphones, the best way to resolve the issue may have more to do with the teacher making changes than the student. Regardless of why, when distractions arise and discipline is necessary, applying a few fundamental principles will lead to a better outcome. The scriptures teach us that "if thy brother shall trespass against thee, go and tell him his fault between thee and him alone" (Matthew 18:15; D&C 42:88). Whenever possible, teachers should follow this counsel in correcting students. Private corrections offer a far better setting for students to feel the sincere love and concern of a teacher than correction that occurs in front of a group of the student's peers during class. Once a teacher is in the appropriate setting, teachers must remember that powerful corrections only come: …by persuasion, by long-suffering, by gentleness and meekness, and by love unfeigned; By kindness, and pure knowledge, which shall greatly enlarge the soul without hypocrisy, and without guile— Reproving betimes with sharpness, when moved upon by the Holy Ghost; and then showing forth afterwards an increase of love toward him whom thou hast reproved, lest he esteem thee to be his enemy (D&C 121:41-43) Understanding this principle, a teacher will likely want to avoid phrases like "I need you to put your phone away" or "You can't use your phone like that in this classroom." Instead, teachers should utilize phrases that allow a student to decide what to do themselves, like: "I've noticed you've been spending more time than usual on your phone lately, is there something that's changed recently?" "It seems like you've been a little distracted with your phone lately. Do you feel like digital scriptures are still the best option to help you get closer to the Savior in class?" "It seems like you've been less focused in class lately, is there something I can do to help you have a better experience?" "It doesn't seem like using your smartphone for scriptures is working. Do you think it might help you focus more if you choose to use paper scriptures instead?" It is important to remember that the principles of discipline only work when aligned with the principles of agency. While a teacher insisting that a student use their phone a certain way may temporarily solve the problem, a teacher cannot reasonably police every student's smartphone usage during every class. Instead, as Elder David Evans of the Church's missionary department taught, "We know that even with filters and every other safeguard that we can provide that the only really effective filter for lifelong technology use is the individual heart and mind of the individual young person" (Newsroom, July 2014). By helping our students learn to make righteous decisions with smartphones, we can prepare them for success, not just in gospel classrooms but in their daily lives. Conclusion While each of these practices helps youth learn to utilize technologies in righteous ways, teachers should remember that even if they are applied correctly, there will still be struggles when implementing smartphones in a seminary classroom. Speaking to that concern at a mission presidents seminar, Elder David A Bednar taught, "We should not allow a fear of mistakes to hold us back from receiving the great blessings these tools can provide" (Olson, 2018; Weaver, 2016). Mistakes have, and will continue to be made as we strive to meet church leaders' call to utilize smartphones in the way the Lord desires. However, we can move forward with confidence as we implement the proven practices of: 1. Proper training. 2. Effective modeling. 3. Simplification. 4. Directing students on when and how to use devices. 5. Honoring the agency of students. 6. And administering appropriate discipline. In time, our efforts will contribute to changing paradigms about smartphones in the classroom and accelerating the Lord's work in the last days. References Antonenko, P. D., & Niederhauser, D. S. (2010). The influence of leads on cognitive load and learning in a hypertext environment. Computers in Human Behavior, 26(2), 140-150. Apple Newsroom, (2010). Apple Launches iPad, retrieved from: https://www.apple.com/newsroom/2010/01/27Apple-Launches-iPad/ Azevedo, R., & Cromley, J. G. (2004). Does training on self-regulated learning facilitate students' learning with hypermedia?. Journal of educational psychology, 96(3), 523. Baron, N. S. (2017). Reading in a digital age. Phi Delta Kappan, 99(2), 15-20. Bennett, S., Maton, K., & Kervin, L. (2008). The 'digital natives' debate: A critical review of the evidence. British Journal of Educational Technology, 39(5), 775-786. Ben-Yehudah, G., & Eshet-Alkalai, Y. (2014, February). The influence of text annotation tools on print and digital reading comprehension. In Proceedings of the 9th Chais Conference for Innovation in Learning Technologies (p. 28-35). Bernstein, E. (2019, Feb 26). Life & arts -- bonds: On relationships: No-one listening? maybe it's you. --- listeners often get blamed for being distracted, but talkers should take responsibility, too. Wall Street Journal Blackwell, L., Gardiner, E., & Schoenebeck, S. (2016, February). Managing expectations: Technology tensions among parents and teens. In Proceedings of the 19th ACM Conference on Computer-Supported Cooperative Work & Social Computing (pp. 13901401). ACM. Bold, M. R., & Wagstaff, K. L. (2017). Marginalia in the digital age: Are digital reading devices meeting the needs of today's readers? Library & Information Science Research, 39(1), 16-22. Bowman, L. L., Levine, L. E., Waite, B. M., & Gendron, M. (2010). Can students really multitask? An experimental study of instant messaging while reading. Computers & Education, 54(4), 927-931. Carr, N. (2008). Is Google making us stupid?. Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education, 107(2), 89-94. Chen, G., Cheng, W., Chang, T. W., Zheng, X., & Huang, R. (2014). A comparison of reading comprehension across paper, computer screens, and tablets: Does tablet familiarity matter?. Journal of Computers in Education, 1(2-3), 213-225. Comstock, B. J. (2017). The Impact of Modeling on Students Perceptions of Electronics in the Classroom. Unpublished. Dobler, E. (2015). e‐Textbooks. Journal of Adolescent & adult literacy, 58(6), 482-491. Dowd, H., & Green, P. (2016). Classroom management in the digital age: Effective practices for technology-rich learning spaces. Irvine, CA: EdTechTeam Press. Dunning, D., Johnson, K., Ehrlinger, J., & Kruger, J. (2003). Why people fail to recognize their own incompetence. Current directions in psychological science, 12(3), 83-87. Durham, P., & Raymond, R. D. (2016). Building Cognitive Reading Fluency through" Tagging" for Metacognition. Texas Journal of Literacy Education, 4(1), 46-56. End, C. M., Worthman, S., Mathews, M. B., & Wetterau, K. (2010). Costly cell phones: The impact of cell phone rings on academic performance. Teaching of Psychology, 37(1), 5557. Felt, L., & Robb, M. (2016). Technology addiction: Concern, controversy, and finding balance. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media. Froese, A. D., Carpenter, C. N., Inman, D. A., Schooley, J. R., Barnes, R. B., Brecht, P. W., & Chacon, J. D. (2012). Effects of classroom cell phone use on expected and actual learning. College Student Journal, 46(2), 323-333. Jabr, F. (2013). Why the brain prefers paper. Scientific American, 309(5), 48-53. Johnson, P. V. (2013, Aug). What More Might the Lord Be Willing to Give Us?, Seminaries and Institutes of Religion Summer Broadcast. Johnson, T. E., Archibald, T. N., & Tenenbaum, G. (2010). Individual and team annotation effects on students' reading comprehension, critical thinking, and meta-cognitive skills. Computers in human behavior, 26(6), 1496-1507. Kang, Y. Y., Wang, M. J. J., & Lin, R. (2009). Usability evaluation of e-books. Displays, 30(2), 49-52. Keim, B. (2014). Why the smart reading device of the future may be… paper. Wired (May 2014). Kurata, K., Ishita, E., Miyata, Y., & Minami, Y. (2017). Print or digital? Reading behavior and preferences in Japan. Journal of the Association for Information Science and Technology, 68(4), 884-894. Lauterman, T., & Ackerman, R. (2014). Overcoming screen inferiority in learning and calibration. Computers in Human Behavior, 35, 455-463. Lin, W. Y., Zhang, X., Jung, J. Y., & Kim, Y. C. (2013). From the wired to wireless generation? Investigating teens' Internet use through the mobile phone. Telecommunications Policy, 37(8), 651-661. MacWilliam, A. (2013). The engaged reader. Publishing Research Quarterly, 29(1), 1-11. Mangen, A., Walgermo, B. R., & Brønnick, K. (2013). Reading linear texts on paper versus computer screen: Effects on reading comprehension. International Journal of Educational Research, 58, 61-68. Mizrachi, D. (2015). Undergraduates' academic reading format preferences and behaviors. The Journal of Academic Librarianship, 41(3), 301-311. Newsroom (2014, July). Church Expands Use of Digital Devices for Missionary Work. Retrieved from: https://newsroom.churchofjesuschrist.org/article/church-expands-use-ofdigital-devices-for-missionary-work Nichols, M. (2016). Reading and studying on the screen: An overview of literature towards good learning design practice. Journal of Open, Flexible, and Distance Learning, 20(1), 33-43 Nichols, M. (2018). Addendum: Reading and studying on the screen. Journal of Open, Flexible, and Distance Learning, 22(2), 49-60. Niederhauser, D. S., Reynolds, R. E., Salmen, D. J., & Skolmoski, P. (2000). The influence of cognitive load on learning from hypertext. Journal of educational computing research, 23(3), 237-255. Noyes, J., & Garland, K. (2005). Students' attitudes toward books and computers. Computers in Human Behavior, 21(2), 233-241. Odom, J. D. (2012). A study of the impact of mobile phones as learning tools for youth in southern baptist churches (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global. (1286700120). Retrieved from http://libproxy.boisestate.edu/login?url=http://search.proquest.com.libproxy.boisestate.ed u/docview/1286700120?accountid=9649 Olson, A. C. (2018, August). Worshiping in a Digital Age, Ensign. Paul, A. M. (2013). You'll never learn. Students can't resist multitasking, and it's impairing their memory. Packer, B. K. (1993, August). The Great Plan of Happiness, Address given to religious educators at a symposium on the Doctrine and Covenants and Church History, Brigham Young University, 10 August 1993. Porter-O'Donnell, C. (2004). Beyond the yellow highlighter: Teaching annotation skills to improve reading comprehension. English Journal, 82-89. Przybylski, A. K., & Weinstein, N. (2013). Can you connect with me now? How the presence of mobile communication technology influences face-to-face conversation quality. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 30(3), 237-246. Read, W., Robertson, N., & McQuilken, L. (2011). A novel romance: The Technology Acceptance Model with emotional attachment. Australasian Marketing Journal (AMJ), 19(4), 223-229. Rockinson-Szapkiw, A. J., Courduff, J., Carter, K., & Bennett, D. (2013). Electronic versus traditional print textbooks: A comparison study on the influence of university students' learning. Computers & Education, 63, 259-266. Rosenwald, M. S. (2014). Serious reading takes a hit from online scanning and skimming, researchers say. The Washington Post, 6. Sanders, L. (2017). Digital Minds. Science News, 191(6), 18. Scholastic, (2015), Kids & Family Reading Report Schneps, M. H., Thomson, J. M., Chen, C., Sonnert, G., & Pomplun, M. (2013). E-readers are more effective than paper for some with dyslexia. PloS one, 8(9), e75634. Schugar, J. T., Schugar, H., & Penny, C. (2011). A Nook or a Book? Comparing College Students' Reading Comprehension Levels, Critical Reading, and Study Skills. International Journal of Technology in Teaching & Learning, 7(2). Stevenson, G. E. (2017, November). Spiritual Eclipse. Ensign. Stone, R. W., & Baker-Eveleth, L. J. (2013). Students' intentions to purchase electronic textbooks. Journal of Computing in Higher Education, 25(1), 27-47. Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learning and instruction, 4(4), 295-312. Taipale, S. (2015). Bodily dimensions of reading and writing practices on paper and digitally. Telematics and Informatics, 32(4), 766-775. Thomas, K. M., O'Bannon, B. W., & Bolton, N. (2013). Cell phones in the classroom: Teachers' perspectives of inclusion, benefits, and barriers. Computers in the Schools, 30(4), 295308. Thornton, B., Faires, A., Robbins, M., & Rollins, E. (2014). The mere presence of a cell phone may be distracting. Social Psychology. Tindell, D. R., & Bohlander, R. W. (2012). The use and abuse of cell phones and text messaging in the classroom: A survey of college students. College Teaching, 60(1), 1-9. Tossell, C. C., Kortum, P., Shepard, C., Rahmati, A., & Zhong, L. (2015). You can lead a horse to water, but you cannot make him learn: Smartphone use in higher education. British Journal of Educational Technology, 46(4), 713-724. Two sides, (2015), Reading from Paper or Reading from Screens. What do Consumers Prefer? Van Horne, S., Russell, J. E., & Schuh, K. L. (2016). The adoption of mark-up tools in an interactive e-textbook reader. Educational Technology Research and Development, 64(3), 407-433 Waheed, M., Kaur, K., Ain, N., & Sanni, S. A. (2015). Emotional attachment and multidimensional self-efficacy: extension of innovation diffusion theory in the context of eBook reader. Behaviour & Information Technology, 34(12), 1147-1159. Weaver, S. J. (2016, July). Elder Bednar Tells 2016 Mission Presidents Not to Fear Technology. Church News. Retrieved from: https://www.churchofjesuschrist.org/church/news/elderbednar-tells-2016-mission-presidents-not-to-fear-technology?lang=eng