Psychology UG and PGT Conversion Assessment Coversheet This coversheet should be completed and attached to all assignments submitted via Blackboard, unless otherwise specified. Ensure your student number (not IT Username) is included in the header above , as well as below. Marking criteria are available on the relevant Blackboard site and should be followed for each assignment. Ensure that you understand and work with academic integrity by reviewing the University’s Academic Integrity Toolkit : http://libguides.reading.ac.uk/academicintegrity APA style of referencing should be used for all assignments , please ensure that you cite appropriately to prevent accidental plagiarism. Please see your year handbook if you are unsure about plagiarism and/or APA style . Student Number (also add to header): Module Code: PY3FOR Marker (if known): Dr Kai Li Chung Assignment title: Critical Review Word count (excluding coversheet & reference list): 2008 I have read the marking criteria & guidance: Yes _____________________________________________________________________ How have you addressed previous feedback? What would you particularly welcome advice on? In the justice system, investigations of a crime include processes of obtaining evidence to understand and reveal the incident. One of the evidence that are commonly sought after and deemed valuable is the testimonies of eyewitnesses. This form of evidence is deemed valuable because of its presumed credibility, therefore it can be presented and used regardless of its validity. However, certain groups of people may be deemed unfit to be eyewitnesses or they are commonly perceived by the court for providing unreliable testimonies. One such group is comprised of children. The criticism of children being unreliable eyewitnesses stems from speculations about children being more susceptible to suggestibility, their underdeveloped memory as well as unrefined perceptual abilities. However, these claims and research evidence have to be critically discussed as certain situations may only have children being present around or during the incident, such as domestic abuse cases. During interviews with a child eyewitness, the interaction between interviewer and the eyewitness requires caution during extraction of information. This is because during an interview, there are possible ways that the interaction may contaminate the testimony given which may not be what the eyewitness intend to report. One way that can do so is by presenting suggestive questions or information that can influence, in this case, a child’s recall and reporting of events (Ceci & Bruck, 1993). In the past, suggestibility was deemed a significant factor that are relatively more likely to affect children, in turn forming false memories and recalls. A few reasons explaining this claim is their susceptibility to social pressure and certain issues with event recall, which opens up the platform for implanting false memory. Social pressure, such as compliance, have been shown to motivate the false recall of false experiences in children. Children has been explained to be more likely to be compliant to suggestive questions posed by interviewers or parents, as compared to other adults (Ceci & Freidman, 2000). An experiment by Bjorklund and colleagues (2000) looked into the elicit of social demand characteristics when interviewed twice by the same interviewer, which thereby intensify the inaccuracy of free recall, particularly when the participants were asked misleading questions. This study compared 5- and 7-years olds with adults and found results that opposes to the notion that social demand characteristics, assumingly elicited when the same person interviewed children, would drive them to repeat their errors done in the previous interview. Although the results was generated due to the lesser number of interviews as compared to usual interviews in forensic cases, but it nonetheless showed support to children being more accurate in free recall and recognition performance when reinstatements are made. The results from this study can also suggest that misleading questions may only produce erroneous responses in that instance, but may not be powerful enough to change a child’s event memory. Contrary to this claim, several studies have found otherwise that misinformation can cause significant memory distortions. A study found that with a brief exposure of misinformation, false memory that was generated was correlated with false memories tested a year and a half later (Zhu et al., 2012). The strengths of the traces for true and false memories were both similar. Furthermore, false memories were also explained to be driven by implanted memory traces, not compliance in children (Otgaar, Verschuere, Meijer, & Oorsouw, 2012). A study specifically emphasised that false memory can be implanted when narrative containing false, or even fictitious events, were given (Otgaar et al., 2012). Interestingly, two-thirds among their 4-years olds sample who developed a false memory remained confident about experiencing the narrated false event after debriefing. The evidence from their study suggests that faulty memory traces can be suggested to younger children and produce false memories that are long-lasting. One of the possible reasons that can explain the proneness of younger children to be convinced of experiencing false events is their poor working memory (Peters, Jelicic, Verbeek, & Merckelbach, 2007; Jaschinski & Wentura, 2002). Another important aspect to note is that the implantation paradigm adopted by that study instructed their participants to give some thought to the false narratives as well as restraining them from any external interventions like asking their parents to verify the narratives during the gap period between the two interviews. The use of this paradigm may then be criticised to exclude ecological factors that can be investigated to examine their influences on implanted false memories. Besides from methods or information presented in interviews which seemingly can suggest children into false reports or memories, things in society should also be considered as factors for suggestibility to children. One major influence from everyday life is mass media that may also suggest information that can distort one’s memory about an event. Otgaar and colleagues (2009) discovered that after children participants were presented implausible events in the form of a false newspaper article, the suggestive material was later followed by participants’ reports of witnessing the false event, noting that the interviewer also included their parents into the false narratives. It was also found that this effect declines in age. This study shows the significant strength of prevalent information such as the media in suggesting false memories. Furthermore, the authors also mentioned that the authority and closeness of the characters (ie. parents) in the narratives may have acted as a drive for younger children to believe and report false events. This kind of narratives can be interpreted as children being more likely to believe in false statements when conveyed by parents. However, this evaluating point by the authors have been disproved in the context of forensic cases. In cases where parents are accused of child abuse, Gardner (1994) proposed that custodial parents have the ability to instil false events into children in order to form testimonies against the abusive parent. However, strong evidence have shown that children may not be that easily influenced to lie about events involving aggression (Clemente & Padilla-Racero, 2015; Clemente, 2013) and there is currently no scientific research that supports the PAS theory (Blizard & Shaw, 2019). Related to relational factors, 11- and 12-years old children were demonstrated to be relatively susceptible to peer input of misinformation as compared to adolescents (Calado, Otgaar & Muris, 2018). As mentioned before, poor working memory may be the underlying reason for children to be more susceptible to memory distortions (Peters et al., 2007). It was explained that working memory processing is positively correlated with episodic encoding and subsequent retrieval. Reduced working memory processing would also lead to increased frequencies of misattributing certain aspects within retrieving memory, especially when said working memory is overloaded during encoding. Source monitoring is a reflective mechanism that screens for the correct memory information upon retrieval, and its errors are antecedents for false memory (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Linsay, 1993; Lindsay & Johnson, 2000). The lacking in working memory performance that is attributed to lead to a higher proneness to false memories is based on the source monitoring framework (SMF; Johnson et al., 1993; Mitchell & Johnson, 2000). This framework proposed that source monitoring highly rely on perceptual abilities, as the information generated from the sensory modalities will be used as sources for memory retrieval (Lindsay & Johnson, 2000). The functional link between working memory and source monitoring are also supported by neuroimaging experiments, where there were great activation in the lateral prefrontal cortex during a task that requires the maintenance of working memory during source judgements (Mitchell, Johnson, Raye, & Greene, 2004). These evidence can be used to further discuss why young children’s are prone to memory falsifications. Based on neurological research on the lateral prefrontal cortex, children between 4 to 7 years of age undergo a gradual maturation of the said brain area as well as the corresponding working memory (Tsujimoto & Sawaguchi, 2004, as cited in Uytun, 2018) and would only reach full maturation by the age of 11 and 12 (Fuster, 2008, as cited in Uytun, 2018). Nevertheless, inconsistent findings were presented regarding children's memory recall. A study comparing free recall of eyewitness memory and autobiographical memory across three age groups (i.e. children, adolescents and adults) typically found that amount of information reported increases with age (Jack, Leov, & Zajac, 2014). Interestingly, accuracy in both types of memory conditions remained high across the age groups, suggesting children in being similarly reliable in recalling either types of memory as compared with other age groups. However, empirical studies that support an otherwise idea that young children were able to recall distant memories of specific events that they experienced when they were as young as 3-years old, even after years of delay (Bauer et al., 2007; Jack, Simcock & Hayne, 2011). The results from these studies challenge the facilitation of a matured working memory to prevent false memories. From another perspective, these results may also be due to that younger children may not have a lot of similar sources that can cause erroneous recollection of information instead of some sort of compensatory mechanisms that replace working memory abilities. Based on the SMF, memories are sourced collectively from perceptual, semantic and affective information that are usually invoked in a subconscious manner (Lindsay & Johnson, 2000). Fundamentally, in order to gain these experiential information to then be used as building blocks for subsequent memory retrieval, one’s perceptual abilities can be a determinant for the quality and accuracy of memory recall. This ability is an important aspect as it can determine the reliability and validity of an eyewitness’ testimony. In forensic investigations, not only are interviews used to gain testimonies about the incident, but there are also procedures of identifying a suspect. One of the common procedure is known as a line-up, where a witness are shown a row of target people which may or may not contain the suspect. Therefore, in order to identify a suspect accurately, it is crucial to assess eyewitness’ performance in face perception. In neurological studies, one main area that governs face perception as well as long-term memory is the fusiform face area (FFA; Grill-Spector, Knouf & Kanwisher, 2004). And indeed, it was found that the FFA was larger in adults as compared to children (Golarai et al., 2007). Besides, the significantly correlated function of face recognition accuracy also produced similar increasing age differences, where children are less accurate. It was also demonstrated that children were not well-versed in domain-specific and selective activation in FFA, inferring that their reduced accuracy may also be attributed to the inability in selectively retrieving and recognising specific facial features (Kadosh & Johnson, 2007). Golarai and colleagues (2007) also examined the differences in object recognition. They instead found that there were equal activation volumes in the lateral occipital complex across age, and the corresponding task of object recognition showed the same results. This observation can act as supportive evidence for healthy children’s visual encoding of an environment, which would not jeopardise subsequent memory of the incident. However, it was argued that when someone who is perceived as being a threat, preschool-age children exhibited enhancement in face recognition (Kinzler & Shutts, 2008). Thus, this finding further suggests that face perception of younger children can be relied on when the suspect is observed and identified as threatening. In conclusion, being an eyewitness is faced with various external influences and individual factors that may affect one’s own reported testimony and even memory. In particular, when the eyewitness is a child, some influences may produce even stronger effects. One major cause of forming false testimonies or even implanting false memories in children is suggestibility. This has been shown that it not only can be done deliberately via questioning methods during interviews, but any narrative information during the interview can also produce similar misinformation effects on children. Besides that, external influences like peer pressure and prevalent information that is out of the control of both the justice system and the eyewitnesses themselves can also cause false memory recalls. After reviewing the developmental factors of children, it seems that they can attend line-up procedures to identify the suspect, given that the suspect has been observed to act out harmful behaviours. (2008 words) References Benedan, L., Powell, M. B., Zajac, R., Lum, J. A. G., & Snow, P. (2018). Suggestibility in neglected children: The influence of intelligence, language, and social skills. Child Abuse & Neglect, 79, 51–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.01.005 Bjorklund, D. F., Cassel, W. S., Bjorklund, B. R., Brown, R. D., Park, C. L., Ernst, K. I. M., & Owen, F. A. (2000). Social Demand Characteristics in Children’s and Adults’ Eyewitness Memory and Suggestibility: The Effect of Different Interviewers on Free Recall and Recognition. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 433, 421–433. Blizard, R. A., & Shaw, M. (2019). Lost-in-the-mall : False memory or false defense ? Journal of Child Custody, 16(1), 20–41. https://doi.org/10.1080/15379418.2019.1590285 Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2012). Reliability of Children’s Testimony in the Era of Developmental Reversals. Development, 32(3), 224–267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2012.06.008.Reliability Brown, D. A., & Lamb, M. E. (2015). Can Children Be Useful Witnesses ? It Depends How They Are Questioned. Child Development Perspectives, 9(4), 250–255. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12142 Calado, B., Otgaar, H., & Muris, P. (2019). Are children better witnesses than adolescents ? Developmental trends in different false memory paradigms. Journal of Child Custody, 15(4), 330–348. https://doi.org/10.1080/15379418.2019.1568948 Clemente, M., & Padilla-racero, D. (2015). Are children susceptible to manipulation ? The best interest of children and their testimony. Children and Youth Services Review, 51, 101–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2015.02.003 Fuster, J. M. (2000). Executive frontal functions. Experimental Brain Research, 133(1), 66– 70. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210000401 Gardner, R. A. (1998). The parental alienation syndrome. 2nd. Cresskill, New Jersey: Creative Therapeutics, 1-5. Golarai, G., Ghahremani, D. G., Reiss, A., Eberhardt, J. L., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Grill-spector, K. (2007). Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with categoryspecific recognition memory. Nature Neuroscience, 10(4), 512–522. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1865 Hayne, H., Gross, J., Mcnamee, S., Fitzgibbon, O., & Tustin, K. (2014). Cognitive Development Episodic memory and episodic foresight in 3- and. Cognitive Development, 26(4), 343–355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.09.006 Hritz, A. C., Royer, C. E., Helm, R. K., Burd, K. A., Ojeda, K., & Ceci, S. J. (2015). Children’s suggestibility research: Things to know before interviewing a child. Anuario de Psicología Jurídica, 25(1), 3–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apj.2014.09.002 Jack, F., Leov, J., & Zajac, R. (2014). Age-related Differences in the Free-recall Accounts of Child, Adolescent, and Adult Witnesses. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 38(September 2013), 30–38. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2951 Age-related Jaschinski, U. T. A., & Wentura, D. (2002). Misleading Postevent Information and Working Memory Capacity: An Individual Differences Approach to Eyewitness Memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 231, 223–231. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.783 Kinzler, K. D., & Shutts, K. (2008). Memory for ‘“ mean ”’ over ‘“ nice ”’: The influence of threat on children’s face memory. Cognition, 107, 775–783. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.005 Kinzler, K. D., & Shutts, K. (2008). Memory for ‘“ mean ”’ over ‘“ nice ”’: The influence of threat on children’s face memory. Cognition, 107, 775–783. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.005 Lindsay, D. S., & Johnson, M. K. (2000). False memories and the source monitoring framework Reply to Reyna and Lloyd ( 1997 ). Learning and Individual Differences, 12, 145–161. Malloy, L. C., Johnson, J. L., & Goodman, G. S. (2013). Children’s Memory and Event Reports: The Current State of Knowledge and Best Practice. Journal of Forensic Social Work, 3, 106–132. https://doi.org/10.1080/1936928X.2013.763672 Otgaar, H., Candel, I., Merckelbach, H., & Wade, K. A. (2009). Abducted by a UFO: Prevalence Information Affects Young Children’s False Memories for an Implausible Event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 125, 115–125. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1445 Otgaar, H., Verschuere, B., Meijer, E. H., & Oorsouw, K. Van. (2012). The origin of children’s implanted false memories: Memory traces or compliance ? Acta Psychologica, 139, 397–403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.01.002 Peters, M. J. V, Verbeek, H., & Merckelbach, H. (2007). Poor Working Memory Predicts False Memories. European Journal of Cogntive Psychology, 19(2), 213–232. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440600760396 Tsujimoto, S., & Sawaguchi, T. (2004). Properties of delay-period neuronal activity in the primate prefrontal cortex during memory- and sensory-guided saccade tasks. European Journal of Neuroscience, 19(2), 447–457. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0953816X.2003.03130.x Uytun, M. C. (2018). Development Period of Prefrontal Cortex (p. 20). https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.78697 Zhu, B. I., Chen, C., Loftus, E. F., He, Q., Chen, C., Lei, X., … Dong, Q. I. (2012). Brief Exposure to Misinformation Can Lead to Long-Term False Memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 301–307. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1825 Brief