Uploaded by natsalgo

Perception Assignment

advertisement
Temporal Dynamics of Proprioceptive Hand Model Recalibration
1
On the Temporal Dynamics of Proprioceptive Hand Model Recalibration and the Interaction
with Pre-existing Hand Representations
460375766
The University of Sydney
Temporal Dynamics of Proprioceptive Hand Model Recalibration
2
Abstract
Performing specific actions with precision requires an exact knowledge of the bodies current
shape and location. To do this, the brain creates as ongoing mental representation of the body
using two mains sources of information, vision and proprioception. If these sources of
information come into conflict and a body part is not seen to be where it is felt the brain must
create a new representation of this body part, and the new representation is predominately
based off vision. This paper aimed to determine the temporal dynamics of this process as well
as investigate how this new representation interacts with the prior, long-term representation
of the body that the brain has. To do so, participants wore a VR headset through which their
hand appeared to be roughly 10cm offset from its actual position. We then measured whether
participants altered their hand movements to be more in line with this virtual hand and over
what time period this change took place. We found that over the first five trials participants
adjusted their hand representation to be significantly more similar to the virtual hand,
however over the complete twenty trials this effect disappeared. This suggests that while the
brain is capable of creating a new representation of the hand very rapidly it is in conflict with
the long-term hand representation. This results in neither body representation being
successfully maintained.
Temporal Dynamics of Proprioceptive Hand Model Recalibration
3
Performing precise action requires a specific and accurate representation of the body’s
shape and location (Henriques & Cressman, 2012). Often however, the representation needed
to accurately perceive the body changes, such as in situations of a lost limb or when using a
tool, causing the brain to recalibrate its representation of the body. Research has since
focused on in what specific situations recalibration takes place, the mechanisms behind it and
what conclusions can be drawn from this process surrounding brain plasticity. When trying
to calibrate where a body part is, the brain mainly relies on visual and proprioceptive
processes (Tuthill & Azim, 2018). These two processes show neural convergence in the
brain, such that the same neurons in the premotor cortex respond to both visual and
proprioceptive cues to determine where a body part is (Graziano, 1999). When these two
processes are in conflict (i.e. a body part is seen to be in a different position to what is felt)
the visual process has been shown to dominate the proprioceptive process (Martel, Cardinali,
Roy & Farné, 2016). This results in the brain representing the body part where it is seen, not
where it is felt through proprioception (Cressman & Henriques, 2010). This has been
demonstrated in embodiment illusions such as the rubber hand illusion in which participants
report taking ownership of and having feeling in a false arm while their real arm is hidden
from sight (Botvinik & Cohen, 1998). Since then many studies have examined this
phenomenon, generally following a similar method. Usually, the participant’s real hand is
obscured, and they are led to believe that a fake hand is their real hand, called the adaptation
period. Experiments then measure how strong the representation of the fake hand is, and in
what contexts this recalibration can occur (Zbib, Henriques & Cressman, 2016; Keys, Rich &
Zopf, 2018).
A relatively unresearched aspect of embodiment extension is the temporal dynamics
concerning how the brain forms a new representation of an altered limb. A time-resolved
model would be an invaluable tool in the continuing research of body perception and
Temporal Dynamics of Proprioceptive Hand Model Recalibration
4
peripersonal space (Martel et al.,2016). Specifically, this model would be influential in
determining the brain’s plasticity toward new body representations as well as helping to
determine how new body representations interact with the long-term representations. The
small literature there is on this topic however is conflicting. Zbib et al. (2016) found that
proprioceptive recalibration was a slow process, taking over 70 trials for participants to
demonstrate a sensory change. This experiment however was examining proprioceptive
adaptation specifically to a change in hand orientation, rather than a shift in an entire body
part. While never having specifically developed a model of the temporal dynamics for
complete hand movements, previous research suggests it to be a rapid process
(Ganesh, Yoshioka , Osu, R & Ikegami, 2014). Ganesh et al. (2014) found that when using
tools, a new representation of the arm with the tool is developed in only several trials, and
that this process was qualitatively different to the representations made after repeated tool use
over many trials. Rather than incorporate the tool into the body representation, the arm
representation was shortened so that the tool ended where the arm previously did. After
examining the rate at which adaptation occurs, it can then be used to examine the interactions
between new representations of the body and pre-existing representations. Kitago, Ryan,
Mazzoni, Krarkauer and Haith (2013) examined this, specifically looking at how participants
returned to their normal body representation after adaptation to a new representation. They
suggested that after the adaptation process, rather than simply forgetting the learned
representation the original body representation actively replaces it with the long-term
representation as the brain is aware this is again the most accurate representation of the body.
This study has two main aims. Firstly, we aim to clearly demonstrate how quickly the
brain is able to recalibrate proprioceptive models of the hand. While the process has been
shown to take a relatively long time (over 70 trials) to recalibrate hand orientation, the speed
to taken to recalibrate the absolute position of the hand in space has not been researched
Temporal Dynamics of Proprioceptive Hand Model Recalibration
5
(Martel et al., 2016; Zbib et al., 2016). Secondly, the study aims to examine the way in which
new proprioceptive models interact with a pre-existing, long term model of the body. Kitago
et al. (2013) demonstrated that when de-adapting from a recalibration (reverting back to your
normal body representation) it is an active process that involves replacement of the learned
model with the original view of the body, rather than merely the passive extinction of the
recalibration once adaptation is no longer occurring. This study looks to expand on that
research and examine the causal processes for this active replacement. We are looking to
determine whether the new hand representation competes with, integrates with or acts
independently of the long-term body representation. To do so, participants will wear a VR
headset and be asked to track a marker while they cannot see their real or virtual hand over
several consecutive trials. Between each trial a period of recalibration will occur, in which
the participant can view their virtual hand only, however this hand is offset from their real
hand by roughly 10cm. We can then measure whether participants adapt to the virtual
position of the hand while doing the tracking task or maintain the real position of their hand
and measure the time this process takes.
Firstly, we predict that recalibration of the model of the hand will occur such that
throughout the 20 trials the distance between the virtual hand and the marker (Position
Discrepancy) will decrease. We also predict this process will occur rapidly, such that a
change can be detected within five trials. Despite previous research suggesting hand
orientation adaptation takes much longer than this, the overwhelming majority of research
suggests recalibration of hand position is a very rapid process (Ganesh et al., 2014; Martel et
al., 2016). Following from this, we secondly predict that the virtual hand will become closer
to the tracker than the real hand throughout the experiment. Finally, we predict that tracking
in the second half of trials will be significantly more similar to the real hand than the virtual
hand compared to the first half of trials. This stems from the fact that when adaptation is no
Temporal Dynamics of Proprioceptive Hand Model Recalibration
6
longer taking place, the active process of reverting to the original body representation begins
immediately. As we predict recalibration to occur so rapidly, it follows that de-adaptation
will similarly occur very rapidly, potentially over the course of a single trial.
Discussion
Position discrepancy between the virtual hand and the marker did not decrease
throughout the trials. This suggests that participants did not recalibrate their hand
representation to be more similar to the virtual hand. Secondly however, it was found that
over the first five trials alone there was a significant decrease in position discrepancy
between the virtual hand and the marker. This suggests that in the early parts of the
experiment recalibration of the hand representation did take place, but over the course of the
complete 20 trials it was not sustained. Foremost, this shows that recalibration can occur very
rapidly, in this case within five trials, corresponding to only 30 seconds of time in which the
virtual hand was present for recalibration to take place (Ruttle, Hart & Henriques, 2018). This
shows that the brains representation of the body is extremely plastic and can change very
rapidly in order to accommodate changes in the environment (Ganesh et al., 2014). This also
suggests that the representations for hand orientation and hand location are distinct, as
orientation recalibration takes many more trials than hand location recalibration (Zbib et al.,
2016).
Furthermore, these results that over the course of the 20 trials the brain did recalibrate
its body representation initially but shifted away from this in subsequent trials. When a new
body representation is learned it is in conflict with the long-term representation of the body
that the brain has, and it actively tries to replace the new representation with the long-term
representation when active adaptation is not occurring i.e. when the virtual hand is not
present (Kitago et al., 2013). In this experiment, as each adaptation period was much shorter
Temporal Dynamics of Proprioceptive Hand Model Recalibration
7
than each trial (6 seconds vs 20 seconds), most time was spent trying to revert the hand
model to the original representation. This resulted in the brain not being able to use a
representation of the virtual hand, despite adaptation periods between each trial (Huberdeau,
Krakauer & Haith, 2015). This suggests that the new models the brain builds of the body
compete with the long-term representation of the body (Martel et al., 2016). Furthermore, it
shows that to successfully change the representation of the body even only in the short term,
recalibration must occur for longer time periods than six seconds per trial.
Secondly, we found that over the course of the experiment participant’s virtual hand
became comparatively closer to the target than participant’s real hand. However, as discussed
above, the virtual hand did not get closer to the target in terms of absolute distance. This
means that participant’s real hand moved further away from the target in successive trials. So,
despite the virtual hand not moving closer to the target this suggests that participant’s
representation of their hand did change. We suggest this may be due to the conflict between
the adapted recalibration of the hand and the prior hand representation, resulting in failure to
produce an accurately represent either representation (Warren & Cleaves, 1971; Fink et al.,
1999).
Finally, it was found that in the second half of each trial participant’s real hand was
significantly closer to the target than the virtual hand compared to the first half of each trial
(Note, no statistical test was run due to a lack of power but this trend can clearly be seen in
the last results figure). This suggests that in this second half of each 20 second trial period
participants hand representation had already begun to regress back toward their original,
long-term hand representation (Huberdeau et al., 2015). In the adaptation period, participants
relied on visual information to determine the location of the hand whereas while completing
the trials they relied on proprioceptive information. Visual information has been shown to
dominate proprioception if the two are directly in conflict (Scheidt, Conditt, Secco & Mussa-
Temporal Dynamics of Proprioceptive Hand Model Recalibration
8
Ivaldi, 2005; Cressman & Henriques, 2010). However, this shows that if there is no current
visual information being perceived, despite prior adaptation using vision the brain will update
its body representation based on proprioception even if this conflicts with previous visual
information. Secondly, this provides more evidence for the plasticity of the brain when
building body representations, as it is able to both adapt and de-adapt from to changing
representations very rapidly, even within the space of a twenty second trial (Ganesh et al.,
2014). Lastly, this results also provides further evidence that, as discussed above, the new
learned representation of the virtual hand competes with the long-term body representation,
as participant’s reverted toward the long-term representation within a single trial (Kitago et
al., 2013).
A limitation of this paper was that recalibration was only tested over a short, 20
second period while the long-term effects of whether recalibration could be sustained was not
tested. We suggest future research to determine the time period for adaptation needed to
create longer lasting, sustained effects. A second limitation was that the virtual hand was not
present while participants completed the tracking task, only during adaptation. This makes
the findings less externally valid, as in real-world situations a change to the body would be
present both when trying to recalibrate this change and when performing actions based on a
recalibrated representation.
Future research should examine what time period of adaptation with the offset virtual
hand present is needed for robust, short-term recalibration to take effect. As a new
representation is in conflict with the long-term body representation, future research should
also examine whether the ratio between adaptation time and tracking time without adaptation
effects the ability to recalibrate to a new representation of the hand. Lastly, we suggest
researching whether other changes to body representation such as tool use occur with similar
temporal dynamics to that found in this paper.
Temporal Dynamics of Proprioceptive Hand Model Recalibration
Overall, this paper provides strong evidence that the brain rapidly forms new
representations of the body when visual changes are detected. However, this changed
representation is in conflict with the prior, long-term representation of the body resulting in
neither representation being successfully modelled in the short term.
9
Temporal Dynamics of Proprioceptive Hand Model Recalibration
10
References
Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature,
391(6669), 756–756. https://doi.org/10.1038/35784
Cressman, E. K., & Henriques, D. Y. (2010). Reach adaptation and proprioceptive
recalibration following exposure to misaligned sensory input. Journal of
neurophysiology, 103(4), 1888-1895.
Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Halligan, P. W., Frith, C. D., Driver, J., Frackowiak, R. S., &
Dolan, R. J. (1999). The neural consequences of conflict between intention and the
senses. Brain, 122(3), 497-512.
Ganesh, G., Yoshioka, T., Osu, R., & Ikegami, T. (2014). Immediate tool incorporation
processes determine human motor planning with tools. Nature
Communications, 5, 4524.
Graziano, M. S. (1999). Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in
the neuronal representation of limb position. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 96(18), 10418-10421.
Henriques, D. Y., & Cressman, E. K. (2012). Visuomotor adaptation and proprioceptive
recalibration. Journal of motor behavior, 44(6), 435-444.
Huberdeau, D. M., Krakauer, J. W., & Haith, A. M. (2015). Dual-process decomposition in
human sensorimotor adaptation. Current opinion in neurobiology, 33, 71-77.
Temporal Dynamics of Proprioceptive Hand Model Recalibration
11
Keys, R. T., Rich, A. N., & Zopf, R. (2018). Multisensory temporal processing in own-body
contexts: plausibility of hand ownership does not improve visuo-tactile asynchrony
detection. Experimental brain research, 236(5), 1431-1443.
Martel, M., Cardinali, L., Roy, A. C., & FarneĢ€, A. (2016). Tool-use: An open window into
body representation and its plasticity. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 33(1–2), 82– 101.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2016.1167678
Ruttle, J. E., Marius‘t Hart, B., & Henriques, D. Y. (2018). The fast contribution of visualproprioceptive discrepancy to reach aftereffects and proprioceptive recalibration. PloS
one, 13(7), e0200621.
Scheidt, R. A., Conditt, M. A., Secco, E. L., & Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A. (2005). Interaction of
visual and proprioceptive feedback during adaptation of human reaching
movements. Journal of neurophysiology, 93(6), 3200-3213.
Tuthill, J. C., & Azim, E. (2018). Proprioception. Current Biology, 28(5), R194-R203.
Warren, D. H., & Cleaves, W. T. (1971). Visual-proprioceptive interaction under large
amounts of conflict. Jounral of Experimental Psychology, 90(2), 206-214.
Download