Developmental Psychology PSY-263 Rawa Haider Lecture: 2 The content on the slides is taken from the book “Developmental Psychology” by Howard Gardner. Second Edition Infancy The newborn faces two major challenges: To become acquainted with the social world of other people To gain understanding of the physical world of objects Infancy(contd.) The first of these begins when the child recognizes the existence of other people and, most particularly, the principle caretaker, usually the mother. The resulting bond of attachment and sense of trust will have important consequences for the child’s later well-being. Infancy(contd.) To understand the physical world. The child must build upon basic ways of processing information, which eventually yield complex ideas about the attributes and behavior of objects and their existence in space and over time. This ‘sensorimotor’ intelligence is a product of the interaction of the child and her environment. Upon this foundation the increasingly advanced cognitive levels of childhood and adulthood are constructed. Infancy(contd.) Infants are strangers to the world, but they are , fortunately, well equipped to master its challenges. Their genetic heritage, the nine months spent in the mother’s womb, and their capacities to perceive, to act, and to feel all combine to bring about the affective and cognitive advances that mark the first two years. By the end of the second year children have begun to know the world and to gain a sense of their own place in it. Bonds of Attachment Harry Harlow’s Monkey Experiment: John Bowlby’s studies of Human Attachment In the 1940s ‘WHO’ asked John Bowlby, A British psychiatrist , to study the effects of mother child separation in early childhood. John Bowlby’s studies of Human Attachment In 1951 Bowlby and his colleagues reported that normal infants who have been separated prematurely from their mothers are unable to relate satisfactorily to any person and are afraid to play, venture forth, explore, discover the world beyond their own skins , or even themselves. Bowlby reached this conclusion: “What is believed to be essential for mental health is that the infant and young child should experience a warm, intimate and continuous relationship with his mother (or permanent mothersubstitute) in which both find satisfaction and enjoyment” (Bowlby, 1966, p. 11). John Bowlby’s studies of Human Attachment: Insights from Ethology Bowlby became impressed by the analytic framework of ethology, at that time the new line of behavioral investigation. Unlike workers in the environmental –learning traditions, who favor controlled studies in the laboratory, ethologists study the organism in its natural habitat. Such naturalistic observation of an organism are deemed crucial to a proper understanding of its most important behaviorsaggression, sexual relations and other interaction between species members. Ethologists do not, however, simply report everything a duck, fish or monkey does. They are guided by specific hypothesis about just what factors in the environment determine a pairing, an aggressive or threatening gesture, or a location of a nest . Once their hypothesis have been tentatively confirmed in the animal’s natural habitat, ethologists may then devise miniature experiments on the scene. John Bowlby’s studies of Human Attachment: Insights from Ethology (contd.) Ethologist tries to determine which factors or releasers in the environment cause the infant to become imprinted on or permanently bonded to a mother figure. Perhaps the duckling can be imprinted on a tape recording , in which case sound is serving as a releaser. Or perhaps the duckling will become attached to a source of warmth like an electric blanket. Or perhaps the releaser is a duck like decoy or a human being who moves in a duck like manner. Ethologist Lorenz(1957) By mimicking the strutting of a bird, ethologist Lorenz(1957) managed to make himself the object of a jackdaws attachment: this jackdaw eventually regarded humans as parents and even as sexual companions. Ethologists(contd.) In addition to studying the process and effect of imprinting, ethologists also examine the time during which such early behaviors emerge. In particular they are looking for critical behaviors, fixed intervals during which pivotal events must occur, or releasers be encountered, if a certain lasting consequence is to ensue. For instance Lorenz believes that a duck must imprint on its mother (or some other objects) within the critical period of the first three days of life if a permanent attachment is to result. By the same token, Harlow has contended that monkeys must form enduring attachments during the first year of life. Ethologists(contd.) Many researchers urge caution in applying the concepts of critical periods and imprinting directly to human beings. They prefer to speak of sensitive periods, fairly circumscribed times during which important milestones are most apt to occur, or are likely to occur in their most characteristic form. The Course of Attachment and Separation In his book Attachment and loss(1969), Bowlby reexamined through the ethologist’s lenses many of his earlier observations of the mother-infant bond. At first the infant’s and parents behaviors are quite simple and predictable: they resemble the stereotyped sounds and movements nonhumans display during bond formation or imprinting. The human infant’s grasping, crying and sucking, the mother’s cooing and hugging – these dominate the earliest social interactions. Soon however, these behaviors are replaced by finer patterns of response. A single yelp becomes a variety of modulated cries; sucking is modified to fit specific objects, clinging is replaced by faithful following with the eyes. The Course of Attachment and Separation(contd.) In Bowlby’s view, each of the infant’s behavior is designed to result in physical (and, eventually, psychological) closeness to the mother. The baby clings to the mother; the crying child signals fright and a need for comfort; the child following the mother with limbs or eyes seeks to maintain contact with her. The ultimate origins of this attachment, buried in the evolution of the species, may forever elude scientists. Bowlby however believes it reflects the fact that, over millions of years, the infant’s best hope for survival has lain in proximity to a caring mother. The Course of Attachment and Separation(contd.) During the early weeks of life, the behaviors that maintain closeness are not directed towards a particular caretaker. But by the second half of the first year, the child clearly recognizes a primary caretaker (usually the mother), and unique styles of interaction with that person have evolved. Now one may speak of an attachment or an attachment bond: an affectional tie enduring in nature , specific in its focus – comparable in suggestive ways to the duckling’s imprinting on its mother. The Course of Attachment and Separation(contd.) When a bond specific to one person is has been formed, disruptions can be disturbing. Children a year old react with special vehemence when this bond is challenged. For example: presence of a stranger can make the baby cry or fret even during the mother’s presence. So long as the mother stays nearby, this stranger anxiety will pass. But if the mother withdraws or the stranger comes too close . Anxiety may be heightened. More profound distress,-separation anxiety– is apt to occur whenever the child is suddenly separated from the mother. If such separation persists as when a mother is hospitalised or dies, there are likely to be severe psychological costs. The Course of Attachment and Separation(contd.) Bowlby has outlined a sequence of events that typically follow long-term separation during the second year of life. At first the child protests, perhaps shouting, screaming, or running about wildly – marshaling all resources in an attempt to recapture the mother. Here is what happened when ten children were separated from their mothers. The Course of Attachment and Separation(contd.) Eight of the children were crying loudly soon after their parents’ departure. Bedtime was also an occasion for tears. The two who had not cried earlier screamed when put in a cot and could not be consoled… one little girl who arrived in the evening and was put straight to bed, insisted on keeping her coat on, clung desperately to her doll, and cried “at a frightening pitch”. Again and again, having nodded off from sheer fatigue, she awoke screaming for her mother. The Course of Attachment and Separation(contd.) During a second phase (the period of despair), such children are continually preoccupied with their mothers. Now, however, their behavior reveals increasing hopelessness. The children sit unoccupied, with mournful faces. As a defense against feelings of abandonment, the children build up extreme resentment against their mothers. For this reason such a child will fly into despairing rage if any traces of the mother appear. These children have fallen into so brittle a state that they cannot cope with reminders of earlier, more secure days. Only after some months can the children relate to others and seemingly forget their own mothers. This detachment is often welcomed as a sign that the children can finally cope realistically with the lack of a parent. The Course of Attachment and Separation(contd.) But such behavior masks damaging, long term consequences: in managing to get along without their mothers, the children may sacrifice the ability to sustain enduring relationships with anyone. Should the child’s mother return after the period of detachment , the child may turn away or show disinterest, seeming hardly to know her. If the separation has not been too long – perhaps only a few months –and if the mother can gradually reestablish the relationship, permanent damage can be averted . If however too much time has elapsed, or the separation has occurred during a particularly crucial(critical) period the most ardent efforts of the caretaking parent may prove futile. (Bowlby, 1980)