Personality and Individual Differences 73 (2015) 17–23 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Social intensity syndrome: The development and validation of the social intensity syndrome scale Philip G. Zimbardo a,⇑, Anthony C. Ferreras b, Sarah R. Brunskill b a b Stanford University, 450 Serra Mall, Stanford, CA 94305, United States San Francisco State University, 1600 Holloway Ave, San Francisco, CA 94132, United States a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 26 February 2014 Received in revised form 4 September 2014 Accepted 6 September 2014 Keywords: Social intensity syndrome Military Socialization Men Gender Group dynamics Group cohesion a b s t r a c t Social intensity syndrome (SIS) is a new term coined to describe the effects military culture has on the socialization of both active soldiers and veterans. Through literature reviews, interviews, and ideas generated by {author’s name} SIS model, a questionnaire was created to measure the unexplored psychological phenomenon that is reported in the present paper. An exploratory factor analysis, internal consistency and validity tests were used to provide robust evidence for SIS as an index of a fundamental psychological construct of measuring military socialization. This scale promises to offer a glimpse into the military community to gain better insight and understanding about both positive and negative effects that military culture can have while serving, and later as a veteran. Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Much social psychological research has found that the social environment has powerful effects on individual behavior, often changing the way people normally act. The social context that persons find themselves in is a powerful influence on their behaviors, attitudes, and perceptions (Zimbardo & Ebbesen, 1970). Socialization refers to the process by which an individual is taught, through ones social environment, the proper ways to behave as a member of a community (Maccoby, 2007); when someone adopts a new culture, the process is called resocialization (Dyer, 1985). Organizational cultures are structured in a way to teach newcomers the attitudinal and behavioral norms that are appropriate and inappropriate through social pressure and local policies (O’Reilly, 1989). The military, a widely accepted organization, creates an environment to align its members tightly along its desired path. In order to be successful, the military must replace much of what their recruits have previously learned in their civilian life. The intensity of this environment is greater because every aspect of the lives of servicemen is controlled and manipulated to socialize them to adopt new specific attitudes and behaviors (Dyer, 1985) that last ⇑ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, Stanford University, 450 Serra Mall, Stanford, CA 94305, United States. Tel.: +1 (650) 723 2300. E-mail addresses: zim@stanford.edu (P.G. Zimbardo), sbrunskill@gmail.com (S.R. Brunskill). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.014 0191-8869/Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. through their service contract; consequently, these military-created attitudes tend to last well beyond their service (Zimbardo, Sword, & Sword, 2012), spilling over to their civilian lives, impacting their subsequent interactions with family (Basham, 2008) and friends (Hinojosa & Hinojosa, 2011). 2. Social intensity syndrome Social intensity syndrome (SIS), a new theoretical concept that describes the phenomenon of socialization in the military, is the descriptive term for this complex set of values, attitudes, and behaviors organized around personal attraction to, and desire to maintain association with these male-dominated social groupings. Socialization, as it occurs in the military, specifically in combat zones, is so intense that the military way of life solidifies within one’s mentality. The socialization and situational pressures that transform ordinary men into servicemen follows them beyond their service and into their civilian lives, which may cause problems for those who cannot completely readjust to civilian culture. The effect is similar to work-family life spillover and conflict, in which aspects of work permeate family boundaries creating family dissatisfaction and conflicts (Clark, 2001), creating decreased social support or negatively impacting their recovery from traumas or psychological problems (e.g., Batten et al., 2009; Keane & Barlow, 2002). The behavioral effects of SIS are theorized as several observable symptoms and can range from having little to profound 18 P.G. Zimbardo et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 73 (2015) 17–23 effects on veterans’ lives. SIS is a multidimensional construct with the following factors: (1) the need to be around particular others, especially men, (2) self-isolation from civilians, (3) poor bonding with family, and (4) participation in high-risk behaviors. 2.1. Groups Over time, veterans adapt to a level of social intensity which becomes a ‘‘set point’’. The overwhelming presence of men in the military might attract male veterans to social environments that include the pervasive presence of a group of other men over an extended time period. To match the exclusivity of military membership, the appeal to the group is probably greater the more intense the nature of the relationship, the more exclusive it is of tolerating ‘‘outsiders’’ and the more embedded each man is perceived to be within that group creating an in-group out-group mentality. They might only feel comfortable in such settings leading them to isolate themselves from being intimate with others who are not part of these groups. When they are in groups, they prefer all-male groupings over mixed gender ones. This attraction to all male groups could increase the negative behavior of self-isolating from females. 2.2. Friendships Past research has shown that being part of a military unit creates an uncommonly strong bond. Both military training and culture cultivate the concept of developing deep dependence on one’s comrades (Little, 1981). Through physical and social isolation, experiencing life threatening risks and deprivations, military units act as surrogate families by fulfilling social and emotional support, which understandably fosters strong attachments. Friendships are essential when creating unit cohesion and are linked to how well members identify with the unit, combat effectiveness (Oliver, Harman, Hoover, Hayes, & Pandhi, 1999), group performance, job satisfaction, and overall well-being (Dion, 2000). Research shows that even after discharged, veterans tend to seek out other veterans for friendships due to the assumed common understanding of central life issues and brotherhood (Hinojosa & Hinojosa, 2011). 2.3. Family Due to the intense social environment and socialization in the military, a strong connection to other members is created that cannot be replicated outside of the culture. The social intensity experienced by military unit members has been cited as a hindrance to civilian family reintegration (Karney & Crown, 2007). Studies show that post 9/11 married personnel had a harder time readjusting after returning home from deployment than those who were unmarried and 48% reported an overall negative affect on the relationship with their significant since leaving the military (Morin, 2011b). 2.4. Transitioning back to civilian life When transitioning from the military back to civilian life, the change is typically abrupt and without adequate re-entry training. Service members leave their environment to return to the civilian culture for which they have little or no training to deal with its responsibilities and military-discrepant cultural norms. Civilian society expects immediate readjustment to their former way of life, expecting them to deal with new responsibilities and people on their own. This is especially true of the younger soldiers who matured in the military and had few responsibilities previously. Consequently, as the size of the military shrinks, the link between the military and the civilian community grows more distant, which exacerbates the problems more. Research indicates that 27% of service members, and while 44% of those post 9/11, claimed that reentry to civilian life was difficult, feel that civilians do not understand the problems they face (Morin, 2011a) or find it hard to relate to civilians (Hinojosa & Hinojosa, 2011). Subsequently they seclude themselves from others in attempt to deal with their adjustment difficulties alone, in self-imposed solitary isolation (Solomon et al., 1992). After their service is over, or while they are on leave, military men might experience a sense of isolation and boredom immediately following. Civilian jobs might feel uninteresting to them and lacking in intense social interaction, which may influence veterans toward jobs such as civil protection or other dangerous, socially intense work. They may tend to develop biased memories in which they recall more positive and fewer negative aspects of their time in military, seek redeployment if still in the military or hang around settings where there are likely to be other men who also belong to such high intensity groupings (e.g., Veterans Administration hospital lobbies). In conclusion, the socialization that occurs in the military to deprogram recruits and creates military men that will fight and kill for their country, unit, and superiors highly conflicts with civilian life. Then, little or no training is provided to help them transition back into their civilian roles (e.g., child, sibling, parent, and employee). The primary purpose of the present study was to (1) present and describe a new theoretical social psychological construct, SIS, which attempts to explain the effects of military socialization, and (2) assess SIS through a new self-reported questionnaire. 3. Method 3.1. Sample A survey sample of 965 active and veteran United States military personnel participated in an anonymous online survey. 3.2. Procedure 3.2.1. Scale construction SIS is a non-standardized instrument created to assess effects military culture has on the socialization of both active soldiers and veterans. A team of clinicians and researchers authored 150 preliminary items believed to have potential for identifying and outlining military social behaviors. Questions were based on theoretical reflection, interviews with military members and their families, previous research, clinical experience and literature reviews. The criterion for item retention in the preliminary measure was based on strict alignment with the theoretical assumptions, open debate and then consensus agreement; 100 items were retained. 3.2.2. Data collection SIS asks participants to specify how much they agree with a statement by answering on a 5-point Likert scale; disagree strongly (1) to agree strongly (5). The snowball method for recruiting participants was utilized. A recruitment letter was distributed to friends, family, acquaintances, and social networking groups who were involved with military personnel. 3.3. Concurrent assessments 3.3.1. Group environment questionnaire (GEQ; adapted; Ahronson & Cameron, 2007) This 18-item scale assesses group cohesion, social aspects of ones perceptions of and attraction to the group was adapted to measure military group cohesion (original, a = .72; present sample, 19 P.G. Zimbardo et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 73 (2015) 17–23 a = .91). Higher scores signify more unity and engagement in the group. Table 1 Demographics for retained participants. % 3.3.2. Impulsive sensation seeking (ImpSS; Zuckerman, 2002) This 19-item Zuckerman–Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire subscale measures impulsivity and sensation seeking. Higher scores on this scale reflect more sensation seeking and impulsivity (original, a = .77; present sample, a = .87). 3.3.3. Marital interaction (Johnston, White, Edwards, & Booth, 1986) This four-item scale, selected based on content, measures frequency of marital interactions between participants and their partner. Higher scores reflect a higher frequency of positive interactions (present sample, a = .66). 3.3.4. Marital love and affection (Johnston et al., 1986) The marital happiness subscale measures happiness and strength of feelings in the marital relationship. The four items were chosen based on content. Higher scores reflect more happiness and stronger feelings toward the relationship (present sample, a = .90). 3.3.5. Mental health (Derogatis, Lipman, Rickels, Uhlenhuth, & Covi, 1974) The Hopkins Symptom Checklist subscales were used to assess internalizing problems: nine-item depression (original, a = .85; present sample, a = .90) and seven-item anxiety (original, a = .84; present sample, a = .90) subscales. 3.3.6. Self-reported demographic questions Several self-reported and demographic items were included. These self-report items do not have the reliability of the other pre-established scales; nevertheless, they were anticipated to be related to SIS factors and provide valuable information for further research. 3.3.7. Violent marital conflict Straus, Hamby, Boney-McCoy, and Sugarman’s (1996) physical assault subscale was combined to form a four-item scale asking about multiple types of physical assault (present sample, a = .77). Not all original items were used from the physical assault subscale, but examples and inclusive statement about physical acts in general were given. Higher scores indicate increased levels of domestic violence. Age range (years) 18–24 25–35 36–45 46–55 56–65 66–75 76 and above Declined to answer 5.0 21.0 17.4 15.7 20.5 11.1 8.2 0.8 Ethnicity African American Asian/Pacific Islander Caucasian Hispanic/Latino Other Declined to answer 3.4 10.1 76.3 6.3 2.7 1.1 Military status Active Veteran Declined to answer 27.9 71.9 0.2 Note. Participant location information was not collected due to the sensitive subject matter. considered. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was .94 and the Baretlett’s test of sphericity was significant, v2(4950) = 37209.01, p = .000. The six factor solution, which explained 58.50% (Table 2) of the total variance in the EFA, was preferred because of its theoretical support, Kaiser’s eigenvaluesgreater-than-one test, scree plot (Fig. 1), PA, inadequate number of primary loadings and difficulty of interpreting the seventh factor and subsequent factors. Fifty-eight items were deemed viable loading above .45 and no cross loadings. A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was ran using MPlus 6.11 (Muthén & Muthén, 2007) to test orthogonal (v2(1594) = 13698.84, p = .00, CFI = .76, RMSEA = .12) and oblique rotation models (v2(1580) = 5521.51, p = .00, CFI = .92, RMSEA = .07). Since the variable responses are ordered classes, a more appropriate form of model difference test is recommended within the MPLUS technical manual, and subsequently, the MPlus diff test was implemented here (v2(14) = 1245.76, p = .00). Both the CFA and chi-square difference test indicated that oblique model was the best fit. 3.4. Statistical methods 4.2. SIS Factors Utilizing PASW 18.0 (SPSS Inc., 2009), an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) using principal-components analysis for extraction was conducted. Additionally, a Parallel Analysis (PA) was ran using FACTOR 9.2 (Lorenzo-Seva & Ferrando, 2013). Five hundred and twenty-three cases (54.2% of original) were retained in the final EFA giving a variable to subject ratio of 1:5.23, which met recommended guidelines (Kline, 1993; see Table 1 for demographic information for participants who were retained). Cases were excluded if one question of the SIS scale was left unanswered or participants did not identify themselves as male (i.e., left question blank or indicated female). 4. Results 4.1. Exploratory component factor analysis A PA indicated that 11 factors (explaining 58.55% using 95% percentile) or should be considered and when the mean was considered, 12 factors (explaining 60.15% using the mean) should be 4.2.1. Military friends This 16-item factor describes an irreplaceable bond amongst those who have also served in the United States military (eigenvalue = 9.04; 15.58% of variance explained; M = 3.32, SD = .95). Examples: ‘‘I like spending more time with my military friends than my non-military friends,’’ ‘‘I can be myself when with my military friends,’’ and ‘‘I spend time in places where other active and inactive military personnel tend to be.’’ Average factor loading was .69 (a = .95). 4.2.2. Family This 11-item factor reflects an overall negative disposition towards one’s family (eigenvalue = 6.65; 11.47% of variance explained; M = 2.23, SD = 1.03). Examples: ‘‘I feel down when with my significant other,’’ ‘‘I feel bored when with my family,’’ and ‘‘It is easier to trust my military friends than my significant other.’’ Average factor loading was .69 (a = .92). 20 P.G. Zimbardo et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 73 (2015) 17–23 Table 2 Factor loadings for EFA. Item 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 Military friends .84 .83 .80 .77 .77 .76 .76 .74 .72 .70 .67 .65 .59 .50 .50 .45 .08 .15 .09 .27 .26 .16 .13 .18 .27 .16 .24 .25 .21 .20 .19 .22 .21 .24 .01 .06 .12 .10 .08 .05 .06 .13 .04 .02 .12 .08 .06 .16 .24 .26 .20 .29 .23 .23 .03 .07 .01 .05 Family .20 .19 .19 .23 .18 .24 .18 .20 .27 .26 .06 .12 .00 .03 .19 .21 .85 .83 .82 .79 .78 .71 .64 .63 .61 .48 .46 .10 .12 .17 .25 .21 .25 .15 .06 .10 .04 .16 .19 .25 .23 .05 .12 .06 .10 .15 .13 .20 .21 .01 .03 .00 .08 .13 .03 .06 .04 .14 Gender social preference .13 .18 .16 .15 .12 .17 .17 .22 .21 .18 .07 .19 .05 .01 .21 .25 .02 .08 .08 .12 .15 .05 .25 .20 .27 .14 .21 .79 .78 .78 .77 .76 .76 .74 .03 .01 .10 .13 .21 .17 .11 .04 .08 .19 .05 .08 .06 .06 .08 .15 .04 .06 .02 .19 .02 .01 .01 .05 Social bonding .00 .03 .03 .03 .17 .01 .03 .04 .00 .04 .20 .20 .18 .04 .15 .04 .01 .04 .04 .01 .07 .03 .05 .01 .01 .03 .02 .03 .02 .03 .02 .06 .06 .01 .81 .77 .74 .74 .69 .63 .63 .60 .59 .52 .47 .04 .00 .03 .06 .03 .00 .05 .06 .10 .03 .01 .02 .00 Nostalgia .14 .16 .12 .18 .13 .18 .18 .14 .12 .11 .18 .21 .17 .09 .15 .23 .07 .05 .08 .05 .06 .07 .09 .05 .11 .02 .21 .05 .06 .07 .06 .04 .02 .07 .05 .01 .01 .06 .01 .05 .10 .07 .04 .03 .08 .86 .84 .84 .77 .60 .59 .58 .58 .51 .02 .01 .02 .01 Drug use .02 .01 .02 .00 .03 .04 .03 .06 .05 .03 .04 .09 .06 .08 .09 .04 .04 .01 .05 .02 .01 .04 .12 .04 .12 .10 .15 .01 .00 .01 .05 .02 .01 .02 .04 .02 .01 .01 .17 .16 .13 .09 .04 .04 .06 .02 .01 .01 .01 .08 .02 .03 .03 .07 .90 .87 .84 .79 Note. Bold type indicates the factor loadings of each item. 4.2.3. Gender social preference This seven-item factor outlines a distinct male preference for social gatherings and camaraderie rather than female (eigenvalue = 5.12; 8.82% of variance explained; M = 2.69, SD = 1.10). Examples: ‘‘Women just don’t know how to have fun like guys do,’’ ‘‘I feel less comfortable around female friends than male friends,’’ and ‘‘It is not as fun if there are women in the group.’’ Average factor loading was .77 (a = .92). 4.2.4. Social bonding This 11-item factor focuses on the general need for social bonding and to be around others (eigenvalue = 5.05; 8.71% of variance explained; M = 2.68, SD = .81). Examples: ‘‘I often need to be around others,’’ ‘‘I feel an intense need to be around friends,’’ and ‘‘I would rather hangout with a group than hangout with just one friend.’’ Average factor loading was .66 (a = .87). 4.2.5. Nostalgia This nine-item factor echoes a theme of positive memories and reminiscence about one’s time in the service (eigenvalue = 4.96; 8.55% of variance explained; M = 3.63, SD = 1.01). Examples: ‘‘I often thought about seeking redeployment/reenlisting,’’ ‘‘I have more good memories with my military friends than bad,’’ and ‘‘I 21 P.G. Zimbardo et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 73 (2015) 17–23 Fig. 1. Scree plot with original 100 items. wanted to redeploy/reenlist because I missed the excitement.’’ Average factor loading was .69 (a = .89). 4.2.6. Drug use This four-item factor focused on recreational drug use (eigenvalue = 3.11; 5.36% of variance explained: M = 1.34, SD = 0.79). Examples: ‘‘I enjoy doing illegal drugs (marijuana, cocaine, crack, speed, etc.)’’ and ‘‘I often get high.’’ Average factor loading was .85 (a = .88). 4.3. Internal consistency and reliability Four measures of internal consistency were computed for each of the SIS subscales: (1) the average correlation item-total score; (2) the average inter-item correlation; (3) Cronbach’s alpha coefficient; and (4) the split-half reliability. All six subscales were found to be internally consistent (Table 3). 4.4. Validity Evidence for convergent and discriminant validity was evaluated. Since SIS is a new scale with no preexisting similar forms of measurement to-date, convergent validity was assessed by examining the association between the scores of each subscales and those of a similar measure that were completed at the same time. Validity was evaluated by examining the association between the reported scores on SIS subscales and dissimilar measures completed at the same time. Robust support for the validity of SIS comes from the general pattern of results, which is consistent with the theory and our hypotheses (Table 4). 4.4.1. Military friends We hypothesized that high scorers on this subscale would report higher levels of group cohesion (measured by GEQ), depression, anxiety, ImpSS and reported greater number of military friends, while reporting low marital interactions and smaller number of non-military friends. These predications were validated and significant correlations were found: GEQ, r(492) = .52, p < .001; depression, r(473) = .42, p < .001; anxiety, r(473) = .40, p < .001; ImpSS, r(473) = .12, p < .01, and number of military friends, r(518) = .22, p < .001. Negative correlations were found between marital interactions, r(473) = .12, p < .05 and number of non-military friends, r(518) = .27, p < .001. 4.4.2. Family We hypothesized that high scores on this subscale would produce high reports of GEQ, depression, anxiety, violent marital conflict and ImpSS, while being associated with lower reports of marital interactions, marital love and affection. These predications were validated and significant correlations were found: GEQ, r(492) = .18, p < .001; depression, r(473) = .56, p < .001; anxiety, r(473) = .50, p < .001; violent marital conflict, r(373) = .23, p < .001, and ImpSS, r(486) = .21, p < .001. Negative correlations were found amongst: marital interactions, r(380) = .41, p < .001; marital love and affection, r(373) = .51, p < .001, and number of non-military friends, r(518) = .29, p < .001. Additionally, the number of children, r(518) = .10, p < .05, was found to have a positive effect on the participants’ report on family quality. 4.4.3. Gender social preference We hypothesized that high scores on this subscale would be have a positive association with GEQ, depression, anxiety, violent Table 3 Internal consistency. Index Military friends Family Gender social preference Social bonding Nostalgia Drug use Average Average correlation with total score Average inter-item correlation Alpha coefficient Split-half reliability correlation .71 .53 .95 .85 .67 .50 .92 .70 .76 .63 .92 .76 .57 .38 .87 .64 .64 .47 .89 .65 .73 .67 .88 .81 .68 .53 .90 .74 22 P.G. Zimbardo et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 73 (2015) 17–23 Table 4 Validity. Military friends Family Gender social preference Social bonding Nostalgia Drug use GEQ Depression Anxiety Marital interactions Marital love & affection Violent marital conflict ImpSS Number of children Number of siblings Number of military friends Number of non-military friends Alcohol abuse Drug abuse * ** *** Military friends Family Gender social preference Social bonding Nostalgia Drug use 1.00 .56*** .50*** .14*** .47*** .08 .52*** .42*** .40*** .08 .11* .08 .12** .05 .02 .22*** .27*** .12** .05 1.00 .50*** .07 .31*** .15*** .18*** .56*** .50*** .41*** .51*** .23*** .21*** .10*** .05 .00 .29*** .15** .07 1.00 .01 .21*** .08 .26*** .40*** .41*** .21*** .10* .15** .04 .00 .06 .09* .21*** .06 .07 1.00 .06 .03 .16*** .08 .06 .17** .09 .06 .17*** .06 .10*** .23*** .17*** .01 .11* 1.00 .05 .47*** .30*** .23*** .11* .18** .12* .25*** .00 .07 .07 .17*** .11* .08 1.00 .02 .20*** .16** .05 .03 .21*** .18*** .06 .01 .08 .01 .29*** .41*** p 6 .05. p 6 .01. p 6 .001. marital conflict and reported number of military friends. Negative associations, would include marital interactions, marital love and affection, and number of non-military friends. These predications were validated and significant correlations were found: GEQ, r(492) = .26, p < .001; depression, r(473) = .40, p < .001; anxiety, r(473) = .41, p < .001; violent marital conflict, r(373) = .15, p < .01, and number of military friends, r(518) = .09, p < .05. Negative correlations were found between: marital interactions, r(380) = .21, p < .001; marital love and affection, r(373) = .10, p < .05, and number of non-military friends, r(518) = .21, p < .001. 4.4.4. Social bonding We hypothesized that high scores on this subscale would be have a positive association with GEQ, marital interactions, ImpSS and number of military and non-military friends. These predications were validated and significant correlations were found: GEQ, r(492) = .16, p < .001; marital interactions, r(380) = .17, p < .01; ImpSS, r(486) = .17, p < .001; number of military, r(518) = .23, p < .001, and non-military friends, r(518) = .17, p < .001. Additionally, number of siblings, r(518) = .10, p < .05, was found to have a positive effect on the participants’ social bonding. 4.4.5. Nostalgia We hypothesized that high scores on this subscale would be have a positive association with GEQ, depression and anxiety, and ImpSS. Negative associations will include marital interactions, marital love and affection and number of non-military friends. These predications were validated and significant correlations were found: GEQ, r(492) = .47, p < .001; depression, r(473) = .30, p < .001; anxiety, r(473) = .23, p < .001, and ImpSS, r(486) = .25, p < .001. Negative correlations were found between: marital interactions, r(380) = .11, p < .05; marital love and affection, r(373) = .18, p < .01, and number of non-military friends, r(518) = .17, p < .001. 4.4.6. Drug use We hypothesized that high scores on this subscale would be have a positive association with depression, anxiety, violent marital conflict, ImpSS, and also self-reports of having problems drinking or using drugs. These predications were validated and significant correlations were found amongst: depression, r(473) = .20, p < .001; anxiety, r(473) = .16, p < .01; violent marital conflict, r(373) = .21, p < .001; ImpSS, r(486) = .18, p < .001; alcohol, r(468) = .29, p < .001, and drug abuse, r(468) = .41, p < .001. 5. Discussion This was the first systematic research to focus on veterans’ difficulties in functioning in civilian relationships based upon the effects of the intense organizational socialization and social context of the military. The primary purpose of the present paper was to introduce and describe a new theoretical social psychological construct, SIS, and revealed through an EFA that translated the complex effect of military socialization on veterans. The six components extracted supported the predicted multidimensional construct that participants would need to be around others, especially men, self-isolation from civilians, poor bond with family, and participate in high-risk behaviors. Overall, the pattern of data that emerged from our analysis provides evidence for the value of the SIS as an index of the fundamental psychological construct of measuring military socialization. Demonstrated through a multitude of diverse relationships with established personality and behavioral measures, it is revealed that SIS is both a reliable and valid measure. This scale and the associated theoretical concept promises to offer a glimpse into the military community to gain a better understanding about what effects the military culture can have while serving and later while transition back into civilian life as a veteran. Research on military socialization and its effects on veterans are lacking and should be addressed to assist servicemen better transition into civilian life (e.g., inactive vs. active, combat vs. noncombat, and the role of personality in one’s transition). Other intense group environments (e.g., police, firemen, paramedics) should be examined in light of this research. We predict that while they will not report as high of levels, first responders will endorse similar qualities due to the nature of their jobs. Generalization of SIS may be limited by culture and gender. Since only United States males participated, other country’s military personnel may report different experiences. Lastly, there were several reasons that influenced our decision to exclude female military personnel: (a) males make up the significant majority of the United States military; (b) focusing only males provides extra control in the data collection P.G. Zimbardo et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 73 (2015) 17–23 and analysis; (c) since female military personnel represent the minority in the military, data collected from this population may represent a separate but related phenomenon and confound the study, and (d) after standardizing this version on males, we hope to extend the research to females in future research. Acknowledgments We thank Savpreet Bhandal, Troy Bruggs, Ryan Daley, Wenson Fung, Julie Heffernan, Nicole Koenigsmann, Parker Longwell, Sarah Mast, Mikela Moore, Richard Sword, Rosemary Sword and all of our other contacts we met along the way for their help in the data collection and analysis for this paper. References Ahronson, A., & Cameron, J. E. (2007). The nature and consequences of group cohesion in a military sample. Military Psychology, 19(1), 9–25. Basham, K. (2008). Homecoming as safe haven or the new front: Attachment and detachment in military couples. Clinical Social Work Journal, 36(1), 83–96. Batten, S. V., Drapalski, A. L., Decker, M. L., DeViva, J. C., Morris, L. J., Mann, M. A., et al. (2009). Veteran interest in family involvement in PTSD treatment. Psychological Services, 6(3), 184–189. Clark, S. C. (2001). Work cultures and work/family balance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58(3), 348–365. Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., Rickels, K., Uhlenhuth, E. H., & Covi, L. (1974). The hopkins symptom checklist (HSCL): A self-report symptom inventory. Behavioral Science, 19(1), 1–15. Dion, K. L. (2000). Group cohesion: From ‘‘field of forces’’ to multidimensional construct. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 4(1), 7–26. Dyer, G. (1985). War: Past, present, and future. In S. J. Ferguson (Ed.), Mapping the social landscape: Readings in sociology (3rd ed., pp. 153–164). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Hinojosa, R., & Hinojosa, M. S. (2011). Using military friendships to optimize postdeployment reintegration for male Operation Iraqi Freedom/Operation Enduring Freedom veterans. Journal of Rehabilitation Research & Development, 48(10), 1145–1158. Johnston, D. R., White, L. K., Edwards, J. N., & Booth, A. (1986). Dimensions of marital quality: Toward methodological and conceptual refinement. Journal of Family Issues, 7(1), 31–49. 23 Karney, B. R., & Crown, J. S. (2007). Families under stress: An assessment of data, theory, and research on marriage and divorce in the military. Arlington, VA: RAND Corporation (Rand National Defense Research Institute, Monograph No. MG599-OSD). Keane, T. M., & Barlow, D. H. (2002). Posttraumatic stress disorder. In D. H. Barlow (Ed.), Anxiety and its disorders (pp. 418–453). New York, NY: Guilford Press (2nd ed.). Kline, P. (1993). An easy guide to factor analysis. London, England: Routledge. Little, R. W. (1981). Friendships in the military community. Research in the Interweave of Social Roles, 2(1), 211–235. Lorenzo-Seva, U. & Ferrando, P. J. (2013). FACTOR (Version 9.2) [Computer software]. Tarragona, Spain: Rovira i Virgili University. Maccoby, E. E. (2007). Historical overview of socialization research and theory. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 13–41). New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Morin, R. (2011a). The difficult transition from military to civilian life. Retrieved from Pew Research Center website: <http://www.pewsocialtrends.org/files/2011/12/ The-Difficult-Transition-from-Military-to-Civilian-Life.pdf>. Morin, R. (2011b). The military-civilian gap: Fewer family connections. Retrieved from Pew Research Center website: <http://www.pewsocialtrends.org/2011/11/23/ the-military-civilian-gap-fewer-family-connections/>. Muthén, L. K. & Muthén, B. O. (2007). MPlus (Version 6.11) [Computer software]. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. O’Reilly, C. (1989). Corporations, culture, and commitment: Motivation and social control in organizations. California Management Review, 31(4), 9–25. Oliver, L. W., Harman, J., Hoover, E., Hayes, S. M., & Pandhi, N. A. (1999). A quantitative integration of the military cohesion literature. Military Psychology, 11(1), 57–83. Solomon, Z., Waysman, M., Levy, G., Fried, B., Mikulincer, M., Benbenishty, R., et al. (1992). From front line to home front: A study of secondary traumatization. Family Process, 31(3), 289–302. SPSS Inc. (2009). PASW Statistics (Version 18.0) [Computer software]. Chicago, IL. Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney-McCoy, S. B., & Sugarman, D. B. (1996). The revised conflict tactics scales (CTS2): Development and preliminary psychometric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17(3), 283–316. Zimbardo, P. G., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1970). Influencing attitudes and changing behavior: A basic introduction to relevant methodology, theory, and applications. Boston, MA: Addison-Wesley. Zimbardo, P. G., Sword, R. M., & Sword, R. K. (2012). The time cure: Overcoming PTSD with the new psychology of time perspective therapy. San Francisco, CA: JosseyBass. Zuckerman, M. (2002). Zuckerman–Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ): An alternative five-factorial model. In B. De Raad & M. Perugini (Eds.), Big five assessment (pp. 377–397). Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe & Huber.