Factors Influencing Academic Motivation of Ethnic Minority Students: A Review Ulviye Isik1,2, Omaima El Tahir1, Martijn Meeter2, Martijn W. Heymans3, Elise P. Jansma3, Gerda Croiset1,2, and Rashmi A. Kusurkar1,2 Several factors were found to have either a positive or a negative influence on academic motivation, which can be classified into individual, family-related, school-related, and social factors A social neuroscienc e perspective on adolescent risk-taking Risk-taking declines between adolescence and adulthood because of changes in the brain’s cognitive control system—changes which improve individuals’ capacity for self-regulation. These changes occur across adolescence and young adulthood and are seen in structural and functional changes within the prefrontal cortex and its connections to other brain regions. Laurence Steinberg * Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122, United States Most taxpayers would be surprised—perhaps shocked—to learn that vast expenditures of public dollars are invested in health, sex, and driver education programs that either do not work, such as D.A.R.E. ( Ennett, Tobler, Ringwalt, & Flewelling, 1994 ), abstinence education ( Trenholm et al., 2007 Received 9 May 2007 Available online 28 January 2008 ), or driver training ( National Research Council, 2007 ), or are at best of unproven or unstudied effectiveness ( Steinberg, 2007 ). Alas, the search for direct hormone–behavior linkages proved more difficult and less fertile than many scientists had hoped (Buchanan, Eccles, & Becker, 1992), and there are few effects of hormones on adolescent behavior that are not conditioned on the environment in which the behavior occurs; even something as hormonally driven as libido only affects sexual behavior in the right context (Smith, Udry, & Morris, 1985). The development of risk-taking in adolescence, for example, can be approached from a psychological perspective (focusing on increases in emotional reactivity that may underlie risky decision-making), a contextual perspective (focusing on interpersonal processes that influence risky behavior), or a biological perspective (focusing on the endocrinology, neurobiology, or genetics of sensation-seeking). All of these levels of analysis are potentially informative, and most scholars of adolescent psychopathology agree that the study of psychological disorder has profited from cross-fertilization among these various approaches (Cicchetti & Dawson, 2002). Given the critical role of dopaminergic activity in affective and motivational regulation, these changes likely shape the course of socioemotional development in adolescence, because the processing of social and emotional information relies on the networks underlying coding for affective and motivational processes. Key nodes of these networks comprise the amygdala, nucleus accumbens, orbitofrontal cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, and superior temporal sulcus (Nelson, Leibenluft, McClure, & Pine, 2005). These regions have been implicated in diverse aspects of social information processing, including the recognition of socially relevant stimuli (e.g. faces, Hoffman & Haxby, 2000; biological motion, Heberlein, Adolphs, Tranel, & Damasio, 2004), social judgments (appraisal of others, Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & Gabrieli, 2002; judging attractiveness, Aharon et al., 2001; evaluating race, Phelps et al., 2000; assessing others’ intentions, Baron-Cohen, Tager-Flusberg, & Cohen, 1999; Gallagher, 2000), social reasoning (Rilling et al., 2002), and many other aspects of social information processing (for a review, see Adolphs, 2003). As a result of this remodeling, dopaminergic activity in the prefrontal cortex increases significantly in early adolescence and is higher during this period than before or after. Because dopamine plays a critical role in the brain’s reward circuitry, the increase, reduction, and redistribution of dopamine receptor concentration around puberty, especially in projections from the limbic system to the prefrontal area, may have important implications for sensation-seeking In summary, there is strong evidence that the pubertal transition is associated with a substantial increase in sensation-seeking that is likely due to changes in reward salience and reward sensitivity resulting from a biologically-driven remodeling of dopaminergic pathways in what I have called the socio-emotional brain system. This neural transformation is accompanied by a significant increase in oxytocin receptors, also within the socioemotional system, which in turn heightens adolescents’ attentiveness to, and memory for, social information. Socio emotional brain system - dopaminergic systems Conceptual Challenges and Directions for Social Neuroscience Ralph Adolphs1,* 1California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA *Correspondence: radolphs@caltech.edu DOI 10.1016/j.neuron.2010 .03.006 By now everyone is aware that the neurobiology of social behavior (henceforth, ‘‘social neuroscience’’) is a burgeoning field: there are numerous conferences, calls for funding proposals, even new journals and societies attesting to its popularity (for instance, the new society for social neuroscience, www.s4sn.org). Identifying the neural substrates of intrinsic motivation during task performance Woogul Lee1 & Johnmarshall Reeve2 When people imagine performing intrinsically motivating tasks, they show heightened anterior insular cortex (AIC) activity. Using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging, we found that the neural systemof intrinsicmotivation involves not only AIC activity, but also striatum activity and, further, AIC–striatum functional Oxytocin - creativity in education The learning and regulation of emotions features very prominently in social neuroscience (Olsson and Ochsner, 2008) Curiostiy inducing questions - things that they dont know Non curiosity inducin questions things thatthey alreayd know Interactions. Intrinsic motivation is the inherent tendency to seek out novelty and challenge, to explore and investigate, and to stretch and extend one’s capacities (Ryan & Deci, 2000, 2017). It is a naturally occurring inclination toward exploration, spontaneous interest, and environmental mastery that emerges when the individual anticipates discovering new information (exploration), learning something new (spontaneous interest), and developing and extending existing capacities (environmental mastery). These These subjective feelings (interest and enjoyment) signal experiences of intrinsic satisfaction from a job well done that then function as intrinsic rewards to encourage volitional present and future engagement in that task, activity, or environment (Deci, 1992; Krapp, 2005). Thus, intrinsic motivation is the desire to seek out novelty and explore (e.g., curiosity) and to seek out and master optimal challenge (e.g., competence) for no reason other than the resulting feelings of interest and enjoyment (Abuhamdeh, Csikszentmihalyi, & Jalal, 2015; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Loewenstein, 1994; Reeve, 1989). Similarly, Murayama, Matsumoto, Izuma, and Matsumoto (2010) examined the neural bases of intrinsic motivation when participants engaged in a Bmoderately challenging and inherently interesting^ stopwatch task. In these studies, striatum activity was repeatedly observed when participants were intrinsically motivated during the task performance phase. These results suggest the functional importance of reward processing not only in extrinsic motivation, but also in curiosity-based and competence-based intrinsic motivation. Kang and her colleagues’ (2009) study and from trivia quiz books In a parallel program of research, other researchers have sought to identify the unique neural basis of intrinsic as compared to extrinsic motivation (Lee & Reeve, 2013; Lee, Reeve, Xue, & Xiong, 2012). Specifically, these researchers examined neural activity while participants imagined an intrinsically motivating activity (e.g., writing an enjoyable paper) and compared it to neural activity while participants imagined the same activity but with extrinsic motivation (e.g., writing an extra-credit paper). The results showed that the anterior insular cortex (AIC) was uniquely activated during intrinsically rather than during extrinsically motivating situations. Considering that AIC activity is associated with the processing of bodily satisfaction (Goldstein et al., 2009; Naqvi & Bechara, 2009) and subjective feelings (Craig, 2009; Damasio, 1999), the positive subjective feelings that arise spontaneously from activity-generated feelings of satisfaction seem to be an important source of intrinsic motivation We Feel, Therefore We Learn: The Relevance of Affective and Social Neuroscience to Education Mary Helen Immordino-Yang 1 and Antonio Damasio2 In particular, the neurobiological evidence suggests that the aspects of cognition that we recruit most heavily in schools, namely learning, attention, memory, decision making, and social functioning, are both profoundly affected by and subsumed within the processes of emotion; we call these aspects emotional thought. In this article, we sketch a biological and evolutionary account of the relationship between emotion and rational thought, with the purpose of highlighting new connections between emotional, cognitive, and social functioning, and presenting a framework that we hope will inspire further work on the critical role of emotion in education. Any competent teacher recognizes that emotions and feelings affect students ’ performance and learning, as does the state of the body, such as how well students have slept and eaten or whether they are feeling sick or well. We contend, however, that the relationship between learning, emotion and body state runs much deeper than many educators realize and is interwoven with the notion of learning itself. It is not that emotions rule our cognition, nor that rational thought does not exist. Why does a high school student solve a math problem, for example? The reasons range from the intrinsic reward of having found the solution, to getting a good grade, to avoiding punishment, to helping tutor a friend, to getting into a good college, to pleasing his/her parents or the teacher. All of these reasons have a powerful emotional component and relate both to pleasurable sensations and to survival within our culture. Although the notion of surviving and fl ourishing is interpreted in a cultural and social framework at this late stage in evolution, our brains still bear evidence of their original purpose: to manage our bodies and minds in the service of living, and living happily, in the world with other people. And importantly, it underscores our fundamentally social nature, making clear that the very neurobiological systems that support our social interactions and relationships are recruited for the often covert and private decision making that underlies much of our thought. In brief, learning, in the complex sense in which it happens in schools or the real world, is not a rational or disembodied process; neither is it a lonely one. Their reasoning was fl awed because the emotions and social considerations that underlie good reasoning were compromised (Damasio, Grabowski, Frank, Galaburda, & Damasio, 1994; Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1990, 1991 ). emotions are not just messy toddlers in a china shop, running around breaking and obscuring delicate cognitive glassware. Instead, they are more like the shelves underlying the glassware; without them cognition has less support. First, because these fi ndings underscore the critical role of emotion in bringing previously acquired knowledge to inform real-world decision making in social contexts, they suggest the intriguing possibility that emotional processes are required for the skills and knowledge acquired in school to transfer to novel situations and to real life. That is, emotion may play a vital role in helping children decide when and how to apply what they have learned in school to the rest of their lives. Second, the close ties between these patients ’ decision making, emotion, and social functioning may provide a new take on the relationship between biology and culture. Specifi cally, it may be via an emotional route that the social infl uences of culture come to shape learning, thought, and behavior. Emotion, then, is a basic form of decision making, a repertoire of know-how and actions that allows people to respond appropriately in different situations. The more advanced cognition becomes, the more high-level reasoning supports the customization of these responses, both in thought and in action. With evolution and development, the specifi cations of conditions to which people respond, and the modes of response at their disposal, become increasingly nuanced. The more people develop and educate themselves, the more they refi ne their behavioral and cognitive options. Emotions entail the perception of an emotionally competent trigger, a situation either real or imagined that has the power to induce an emotion, as well as a chain of physiological events that will enable changes in both the body and mind ( Damasio, 1994 ). Emotional thought encompasses processes of learning, memory, and decision making, in both social and nonsocial contexts. It is within the domain of emotional thought that creativity plays out, through increasingly nuanced recognition of complex dilemmas and situations and through the invention of correspondingly fl exible and innovative responses. Both the recognition and response aspects of creativity can be informed by rational thought and high reason. That is, the brain areas associated with interoception (the sensing of body states) are particularly active as people feel emotions such as happiness, fear, anger, or sadness ( Damasio et al., 2000 ). After all, we humans cannot divorce ourselves from our biology, nor can we ignore the high-level sociocultural and cognitive forces that make us special within the animal kingdom. When we educators fail to appreciate the importance of students ’ emotions, we fail to appreciate a critical force in students ’ learning. One could argue, in fact, that we fail to appreciate the very reason that students learn at all. A SOCIOCULTURAL APPROACH TO MOTIVATION: A LONG TIME COMING BUT HERE AT LAST Richard Walker, Kimberley PressickKilborn, Erica Sainsbury and Judith MacCallum Central to this change has been the recognition that learning and thinking need to be thought of as social in nature rather than individual in nature. Sivan’s (1986) seminal article provided the first sociocultural analysis of motivation based on Vygotskian ideas. This article presented motivation as a socially constructed norm which, like other classroom norms, formed the basis on which teacher and student expectations and judgements are made. These expectations include how motivation is displayed in the classroom in the form of motivated behaviour demonstrating interest and willingness to engage in learning activities their motivational relevance. The interest in a sociocultural perspective on motivation sparked by Hickey’s (1997) article, however, for the most part led to a consideration of Sivan, E. (1986).Motivation in social constructivist theory. Educational Psychologist, 21, 209–233. the role of context in motivation, rather than a concern with the social nature and origins of motivation. This has been especially true of researchers (e.g. Perry et al., 2006) who have attempted the important task of understanding motivation in authentic classroom contexts. However, as Walker, Pressick-Kilborn, Arnold and Sainsbury (2004) have pointed out, the recognition of the contextual nature of motivation is not new. social influence theorists consider that social and contextual factors have an influence on individual motivational processes and functioning. Russian psychologist Vygotsky, have changed the way the educators and psychologists think about learning and thinking. Central to this change has been the recognition that learning and thinking need to be thought of as social in nature rather than individual in nature Learning Behavior and Motivation of At-Risk College Students: The Case of a SelfRegulatory Learning Class Jerry Chih-Yuan Sun, Youn Joo Oh, Helena Seli, and Matthew Jung Self -efficacy Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine the motivational characteristics and learning behaviors of atrisk freshmen at a four-year university as well as to identify the class-level components of an effective self-regulatory learning course designed for this population in a university setting. The researchers proposed a series of hypotheses about the relationships among (a) self-efficacy, (b) learning and motivation indicators, and (c) academic outcomes for this population in general. How do the self-efficacy and the learning and study strategies of at-risk college students influence their academic achievement? What is the relationship between the self-efficacy and the learning and study strategies as predictors, and academic achievement as an outcome, of at-risk college students? Is there an increase in students’ self-efficacy as a result of their participation in a self-regulatory learning class? Which particular learning and study strategies best predict the academic achievement of at-risk students? (Thompson & Geren, 2002). Gray (2013) indicated that universities define the students who are not able to achieve success in school due to factors such as socioeconomic status, family status, and academic failure as at-risk students. In Bandura (1997) defined self-efficacy as an individual’s judgment of his or her capability to execute and perform tasks successfully in a specific domain. Academic self-efficacy is the general conceptualization of self-efficacy in an educational setting that is not limited to a particular academic subject (Majer, 2009). Huang (2014) believed that the most likely psychological problems that freshmen might encounter occur when they are forced to undertake compulsory courses and when acquiring poor test scores caused by lack of basic Knowledge Conversely, college students who have a high level of academic self-efficacy are academically successful because they implement effective learning strategies (Caprara et al., 2008; Pajares & Valiante, 2002). The Learning and Study Strategies Inventory (LASSI; Weinstein, Palmer, & Schulte, 1987) was administered to all student groups to determine their scores on different motivational subscales. The results of the study showed that at-risk college students scored lower on the self-reported use of learning variables (i.e., attention, concentration, and motivation) compared with students who were not at risk. Weinstein et al.’s (1987) study supported the hypothesis that learning strategies are correlated with academic achievement. Thus, the researchers proposed that at-risk students be identified by their incoming GPAs as well as their LASSI scale scores. They found that self-efficacy was the best predictor of GPA. The MSLQ was developed by the National Center for Research on Improving Postsecondary Teaching and Learning at the University of Michigan in 1986 (Pintrich et al., 1991), including six subscales: Intrinsic Goal Orientation, Extrinsic Goal Orientation, Task Value, Control Beliefs, Self-Efficacy for Learning and Performance, and Test Anxiety. The subscale self-efficacy for learning and performance in this instrument was used to measure college students’ levels of self-efficacy for learning. The internal consistency coefficient (Cronbach’s α) in the current study was .89 for the Self-Efficacy for Learning and Performance, which met the standard of .70 (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). Investigating Motivation in Context: Developing Sociocultural Perspectives Richard A. Walker1, Kimberley PressickKilborn2, Lynette S. Arnold1, and Erica J. Sainsbury1 1University of Sydney, Australia, 2University of Technology, Sydney, Australia Using multiple methodologies that allow focus at the levels of both classroom and individual, these studies employ the notion of transformative internalization and subsequent externalization to explain the social origins of individual motivational Processes employed a range of qualitative methods including observation of lessons, the videotaping of lesson segments, interviews with students, and written student reflections Both studies contribute to the development of sociocultural perspectives on motivation through empirical research guided by such theoretical notions as intersubjectivity, canalization, and coregulation. The notion of the person-in-context has been evident (Pintrich&Maehr, 2002) inmotivational research since at least the time ofKurt Lewin, although not always evident in the methods used by motivational researchers (Pintrich&Maehr, 2002; Turner&Meyer, 2000). As both Goodenow (1992) and Hickey (1997) note, in sociocultural theories deriving from Vygotsky, human activities, events, and actions cannot be separated from the context in which they occur so that context becomes an important issue in sociocultural research. Most notably, the situated approach to learning and cognition also has origins in ecological psychology, which provides a specific theoretical orientation to the understanding of context and the individual’s interaction with their environment. Situated motivation Also important is the “attempt (PressickKilborn&Walker, 2002). Nolen, 2003; Renninger, 2000; Pressick-Kilborn &Walker, 2002 Hickey, 1997) and researchers (e.g., PressickKilborn & Walker, 2002)) Pintrich & Maehr, 2002; Volet & Järvelä, 2001 Pressick-Kilborn, K., & Walker, R. (2002). The social construction of interest in a learning community. In D. McInerney & S. Van Etten (Eds.), Research on sociocultural influences on motivation and learning (pp. to capture the larger meaning of the setting” (Turner & Meyer, 2000, p. 82); context in this research is not reducible to the interrelationships amongst the variables studied. 153–182). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. While the emphasis on context in motivational research has its origins, to some extent, in sociocultural theories deriving from the work ofVygotsky and followers, sociocultural approaches to motivation require more than a concern with context. Hickey, D.T. (1997). Motivation and contemporary socioconstructivist instructional perspectives. Educational Psychologist, 32, 175–193. Central to sociocultural theories is the understanding that learning and the higher order cognitive processes are fundamentally social in nature (e.g., Rogoff, 1998), As it was deemed more useful to consider the social nature of motivation in two distinct motivational domains, that of interest in learning and student academic regulatory processes, Firstly, a sociocultural approach must account for the emergence of motivation from social and cultural Practices. Following Vygotsky, Valsiner suggests that social practices are transformatively internalized by the individual then subsequently externalized in the same or other social contexts. Volet, S., & Järvelä, S. (Eds.). (2001). Motivation in learning contexts: Theoretical advances and methodological implications. Oxford: Elsevier. Secondly, sociocultural approaches to motivation require the reconceptualization of motivational variables since the original conceptualizations were developed from the understanding of motivation as an individual Phenomenon. for instance, the separation of individual and situational interest is problematic (PressickKilborn&Walker, 2002) as it artificially separates the individual and the context In sociocultural theory, individuals are considered to be both embedded in and constituted by social and contextual processes Sociocultural investigations of motivation are best undertaken in authentic contexts utilizing qualitative or mixed method research approaches (Pressick-Kilborn, Sainsbury, & Walker, in press). Academic achievement motivation should, therefore, be investigated in authentic classroom environments, which can be considered, from the perspective of sociocultural theory, as communities (Walker, 2003; Pressick-Kilborn & Walker, 2002) engaged in the enculturation of academic practices naturalistic classroom environments This study explored the social nature of the emergence, development, and maintenance of interest with an emphasis on the dynamic transactions between the classroom environment and individual students, in particular the affordances and constraints on interest development in the classroom (Pressick-Kilborn&Walker, 2002). participant observation, videotaping of lessons, semi-structured interviews, student interest trajectories, written student reflections Qualitative analysis involved examination of teacherstudent and student-student interactions and the construction of discourse maps (Hogan, Nastasi,&Pressley, 2000) Transformative internalization should not be understood as indicating that motivational values and standards are internalized “whole cloth” (Hickey, 2003 p. 410); values and standards associated with motivated action are selectively and constructively transformed (Lawrence & Valsiner, 1993) in the internalization process and are subsequently externalized in differing contexts Both also make use of multiple methods (e.g., self-reports, interviews, observations) to provide a rich and detailed account of student motivation (e.g., Järvelä, Salonen, & Lepola, 2002; Pressick-Kilborn & Walker, 2002; Arnold, 2004). Secondly, sociocultural motivation researchers need to investigate their reconceptualized sociocultural motivation conceptions and mechanisms in a range of naturalistic classrooms using qualitative and mixed method research designs. The interest in a sociocultural perspective on motivation sparked by Hickey’s (1997) article, however, for the most part led to a consideration of the role of context in motivation, rather than a concern with the social nature and origins of motivation. To differentiate these theorists and researchers from Vygotskian sociocultural researchers, Walker et al. (2004), have referred to them as social influence theorists, applying a distinction Rogoff (1998) made in the domain of learning and thinking to the motivational domain; social influence theorists consider that social and contextual factors have an influence on individual motivational processes and functioning. In making the distinction between sociocultural and social influence approaches, however, it needs to be recognised that there are some social influence motivational researchers who refer to their work as ‘sociocultural’. Pressick-Kilborn and Walker (2002) reported an investigation into the emergence and development of interest in a primary school classroom. In the study, interest was conceptualised as emerging from collaborative activities in a fifth grade science community of learners. This conceptualisation emphasised the dynamic and interdependent relationship between the children and their classroom activities for understanding the development of interest, and suggested that traditional distinctions between situational and personal interest were problematic. Studying interest in a real and complex classroom setting presented challenges to distinguishing situational and individual interest along traditional lines. While in some activities, students participated in ways that indicated an ongoing and more personally meaningful form of interest in learning, the situational aspect of interest was always apparent. Similarly, situational features of tasks, such as their hands-on or collaborative nature, contributed to personal experiences of interest in learning in the context of a real classroom. The analysis of the development of interest in the study drew on the integration of several established sociocultural notions: the idea of a community of learners, the ZPD and its extensions (Valsiner, 1997a), and Valsiner’s (1997a) notions of canalisation and self-canalisation. Canalisation refers to the way that cultural practices channel the activities of individuals while self-canalisation refers to way individuals are able to channel and direct their own activities. The canalisation of opportunities in the social world thus creates the context in which values and goals are internalised and from which interest may subsequently emerge. It is important to recognise, however, that individuals may resist these canalisation processes. Akane Zusho & Revathy Kumar (2018) Introduction to the Special Issue: Critical Reflections and Future Directions in the Study of Race, Ethnicity, and Motivation, Educational Psychologist, 53:2, 6163, DOI: 10.1080/00461520.20 18.1432362 Of importance to this special issue are those that stress the increasing racial and ethnic diversity in our schools. motivation—Martin L. Maehr Despite these trends, the number of studies focused primarily on issues of race and ethnicity in the field of educational psychology is still quite limited. Maehr, 1974; Maehr & Nicolls, 1980) DeCuirGunby and Schutz (2014) estimated that only about 1% of articles published in top-tiered educational psychology journals during the period 2001–2011 explicitly focused on topics such as race, ethnicity, stereotype threat, and prejudice. Maehr, M. L. (1974). Culture and achievement motivation. American Psychologist, 29, 887–896. doi:10.1037/h0037 521 Maehr, M. L., & Nicolls, J. G. (1980). Culture and achievement motivation: A second look. In N. Warren (Ed.), Studies in As Graham (this issue) notes in her article, we need more “rigorous programs of research with theory-driven and testable hypothesis about how ethnic diversity leads to better outcomes rather than if it does” (p. 65). Specifically, they frame their analysis according to the four motivational principles ofmeaningfulness, competence, autonomy, and relatedness, and they discuss areas of convergence and divergence across these two related yet distinct Literatures. methodology remains an obstacle to the cultural research on motivation. Likewise, they encourage use of implicit association tests for examining unconscious motivations that inform behavior. crosscultural psychology (vol. 3, pp. 221–267). New York, NY: Academic Press. The belief that motivation is a dynamic process of personal investment informed by context and sociocultural values was a defining aspect of his theoretical writings and empirical work. Weaving Cultural Relevance and Achievement Motivation Into Inclusive Classroom Cultures Ultimately our goal is to emphasize the importance of interdisciplinary research and to demonstrate how the principles of culturally responsive education are instantiated in motivationally supportive classrooms where teachers are more culturally sensitive and create an environment where cultural differences are appreciated and valued. Motivational scholars define achievement motivation as that which influences the initiation, direction, magnitude, perseverance, continuation, and quality of goal-directed academic behavior (Ames, 1992; Maehr & Zusho, 2009; Weiner, 1974). assume that motivation can be assessed through students’ reports of their beliefs and perceptions, as well as through their behaviors, including choice of activities, level and quality of task engagement, persistence, and performance (Maehr & Zusho, 2009). A significant goal of the research on achievement motivation is to build empowering and inclusive learning communities that promote the learning of all students. Comer & Emmons, 2006 Juvonen, Yueyan, & Espinoza, 2010; Murdock, 1999); However, recent reviews show that scholars are becoming increasingly attentive to how culture and ethnicity impact motivational processes, but there remains less focus on issues related to race such as oppression and prejudice (DeCuir-Gunby & Schutz, 2014; R. King & McInerney, 2014; Kumar & Maehr, 2010; Zusho & Clayton, 2011) how the principles of motivation can be instantiated in classrooms where teachers are more culturally sensitive and create an environment where cultural differences are appreciated and valued. To increase the practical relevance of our analysis and promote further integration of motivational frameworks with one another, we consider the motivational processes of underrepresented, racially, and ethnically diverse students across the dominant social-cognitive theories of motivation (e.g., expectancy-value theory, achievement goal theory, selfdetermination theory, self-efficacy theory, and interest theory). By bridging these literatures, it is our intent to demonstrate that culture is not just “out there” in the macrosystem but an integral part of the microsystems of all youth. The difficulty in defining culture can be attributed to the diversity of perspectives on the topic, further complicated by the introduction of the term into the vernacular (see Condon & LaBrack, 2015; Zusho & Clayton, 2011). Is culture internal, external, or both? Is it rooted in time and/or place, or does it change? Is it something that one can own? Is it something that is observable, and if so, what are indicators of it? We define culture in this article as the framework for human life that consists of people collectively using all the resources in their environment to achieve; is a part of all human groups; is learned, shared, and regulated by political, legal, and social systems; is socially transmitted; represents both external (observable behaviors) and internal (inferred traits) aspects of an individual; and is an abstraction of people’s knowledge and beliefs about themselves, other people, and the world. (Zusho & Clayton, 2011, p. 240) Social-cognitive studies of achievement motivation are commonly described as taking a person-in-context perspective (see Maehr & Zusho, 2009; Zusho & Clayton, 2011). Therefore, contemporary motivational theories all recognize how motivational processes are shaped by contextual Factors. Bronfenbrennian (1977) perspective, however, motivation researchers tend to define context in terms of microlevel factors such as the nature of instruction, tasks, and activities that take place in a classroom (Anderman & Gray, 2017; Linnenbrink-Garcia & Patall, 2016; Urdan & Turner, 2005), as well as the interpersonal context of the classroom (see Wentzel, 2017; Wentzel & Muenks, 2016) Thus, the research on motivation primarily defines culture in terms of microlevel contextual factors related to the classroom or school, or in terms of macrolevel factors such as nation of origin, or even individual-level factors such as ethnicity ( learning is culturally Grounded. Meaningfulness is an important principle of motivation. This is reflected in the theoretical assumption—across multiple theories of motivation—that perceptions of competence are not always enough to spur action; individuals must also want to complete the task. For example, expectancy-value theory suggests that the quality of motivated behavior is higher when students find the task and/or subject domain important, interesting, and useful, and when opportunity costs are minimized (Wigfield & Eccles, 2000). Achievement goal theory, specifically the work on classroom goal structures, suggests that promoting relevance is one of the keys to promoting a mastery-oriented focus in the classroom (Maehr & Zusho, 2009). Finally, self-determination theory argues that intrinsic motivation flourishes in contexts where the psychological needs of autonomy, competence, and relatedness are met (R. M. Ryan & Deci, 2017). Therefore, the norms, values, and belief systems emphasized in the various subject curricula, and the social thoughts and interpretations of reality reflected therein, exclusively legitimize European American cultural knowledge and delegitimize the cultural knowledge of minority, disenfranchised groups (Castenell & Pinar, 1993; S. Nieto, 2000). Consequently, schooling may not be a meaningful experience for many students of color by (a) acknowledging the legitimacy of the cultural heritages of different ethnic groups; (b) incorporating culturally relevant curricular content; (c) relating academic abstractions with students’ everyday sociocultural realities; (d) recognizing that students’ dispositions, attitudes, and approaches are informed by their culture; and (e) adopting instructional strategies aligned with the discourse styles and behavioral expectations within students’ home and community (Griner & Stuart, 2013; Yamauchi & Tharp, 1995). Similarly, Boykin and his colleagues (Boykin, 2014; Hurley, Allen, & Boykin, 2009) found that when teachers infused the curriculum and instruction with culturally valued themes such as communalism, elementary and middle school low-income African American students achieved at higher levels in several subjects including language arts, mathematics, and social science. As Eccles (2009) stated, “In the past, I conceptualized attainment value in terms of the needs and personal values that an activity/behavior or task fulfills. Today I am conceptualizing it more in terms of personal and collective identities” (p. 83). However, empirical testing does not do sufficient justice to the expectancy-value theoretical model, because many studies continue to operationalize culture as a categorical variable, seeing it, for example, in terms of membership to a cultural group (e.g., Eccles, Wong, & Peck, 2006). At the very least, we should develop mixed-method approaches that include both in-depth qualitative approaches enabling us to understand the cultural basis of students’ subjective values and advanced quantitative approaches to test the theoretical Model. Consider, by way of example, the curricular areas of math and science that are sometimes described as neutral and value free—a position strongly contested by many math educators (Gutstein, 2016; Martin, 2009; Tate, 2005). A widely accepted assumption is the racial hierarchical totem pole regarding math ability, which places Asian and White students at the top and assigns African American, Native American, and Latinx students to the lower rungs (McGrady & Reynolds, 2013; McKown & Weinstein, 2008). The cultural cost of such a racial hierarchical ordering is the distancing of mathematical identity from minority students’ cultural identity, thereby undermining the meaningfulness of mathematics in these students’ lives (Martin, 2009). Based on a series of ethnographic and participant classroom observation studies, Martin (2009) argued that mathematics teachers need to develop awareness that teaching practices and school policies informed by stereotypic beliefs associating students’ cultural background and their mathematical abilities often have irrevocable consequences for students’ mathematical learning and literacy. Social realities, cultural processes, and cultural knowledge are important considerations for making learning meaningful, capturing students’ interest in tasks, or sustaining their interest in the subject matter. In future research, interdisciplinary, mixed-methods analysis may be essential to capturing cultural learning processes. Motivation research assumes that individuals are motivated toward competence, that is, to effective interaction with their environment (Elliot, Dweck & Yaeger, 2017; Usher, 2016; White, 1959). The overall empirical literature on expectancy-related constructs generally finds that students who believe themselves to be more competent academically are also more likely to put forth greater effort, persist in the face of failure, use deeper-processing cognitive and metacognitive strategies, report positive rather than negative emotions, and endorse mastery goals (Klassen & Usher, 2010; Usher & Pajares, 2008; B. J. Zimmerman & Cleary, 2006 CRRE scholars argue that for students of color to feel academically efficacious and experience academic success, they need to perceive academic success as inherent to their cultural identity. Using classroom observations and group interviews, Brown and Crippen (2017) demonstrated that students’ interest in science and motivation to learn increased when middle school science teachers applied the principles of CRP to build bridges between students’ home and school, promote thoughtful interactions in class, and give voice to students’ thoughts and concerns Another area of focus relates to the inherent difficulty of interweaving cultural identity and academic competence when stereotypes associated with the cultural group suggest that the two are antithetical. For example, Ladson-Billings (2009) noted that African American students need to develop a “relevant black personality” that allows them to “choose academic excellence yet still identify with African American culture” (p. 20), suggesting a dissonance between academic competence and cultural identity These findings suggest that societal stereotypes regarding the academic abilities of a cultural group can jeopardize students’ feelings of cultural, and consequently academic, competence. On this issue, CRRE scholars are unequivocal in their assertion that achieving cultural competency precedes minority students’ academic competence—actual or perceived (Ladson-Billings, 2014; Milner, 2011). They also agree that it is an essential aspect of teachers’ efficacy for teaching culturally diverse students A fundamental assumption of all social-cognitive theories of motivation is that individuals have an inherent need for independence, personal agency, responsibility, and control. This notion is perhaps best illustrated in self-determination theory, which considers autonomy—the extent to which individuals perceive that they can accept, endorse, and regulate their own goals or actions—as a basic human need and a fundamental motive necessary for intrinsic motivation (R. M. Ryan & Deci, 2017) For example, a growing body of work examines how teachers support students’ autonomy (Stefanou, Perencevich, DiCintio, & Turner, 2004). Researchers commonly describe teachers who are rated high in autonomy support as teachers who not only provide choice but also listen to students and allow them to manipulate instructional materials and ideas. Accordingly, in a culturally responsive and relevant teaching framework, autonomy is understood both as a product and a process. As a product, students who achieve a sense of autonomy feel empowered. As a process, autonomy is a quality that individuals strive to cultivate by thinking critically about what they learn. It is a process because minority students intentionally assert their autonomy when they encounter racism and discrimination The theoretical and empirical literature in motivation and CRRE disciplines reflect a fundamental philosophical difference. The former views autonomy as an inherent psychological need that, if satisfied, supports the growth and wellbeing of the individual; the latter describes autonomy as a quality to be fostered and developed to benefit both the individual and society. The empirical work by motivation researchers therefore focuses on the consequences of satisfying this inherent need for autonomy, recognizing that when individuals feel compelled to behave in ways that disregard personal interests and values they experience a loss of autonomy (Koestner & Losier, 1996) Many motivation frameworks assume that individuals have an inherent desire to relate to others. This may be best illustrated in self-determination theory, particularly relationships motivation subtheory, which reasons that humans have a need to form and maintain close and secure relationships with others (R. M. Ryan & Deci, 2017) Such feelings of safety can be heightened when students feel that their teachers care for and respect them (Wentzel, 2010) and when students are encouraged to work productively and collaboratively in groups (Roseth, Johnson, & Johnson, 2008). Interest theory, too, suggests that situational interest can often be triggered by a caring teacher or by group work (Renninger & Hidi, 2011). However, if schools are to be culturally responsive and relevant to diverse school populations, peer-related issues are important. Such issues include students’ self-segregating into groups defined largely by race and ethnicity (Kim, Park, & Koo, 2015; Kumar, Seay, & Karabenick, 2011; Villapando, 2003) and their marginalizing out-group peers due to ethnocentrism, racial harassment (Crozier & Davies, 2008; Kumar et al., 2015), or differences in academic values and achievement (O’Connor, Fernandez, & Girard, 2007). Results indicated that when teachers felt responsible for engaging in culturally relevant teaching practices, students from all cultural groups perceived the learning context as more mastery focused (i.e., valuing improvement, emphasizing effort and understanding, and recognizing mistakes as essential to the learning process). This study also highlights that teachers can be culturally responsive even as they are teaching a heterogeneous student body, because culturally responsive teaching is not teaching to specific groups of students. Rather, as stated earlier, culturally responsive teachers create an inclusive classroom environment, fostering positive cross-cultural relationships among students. Further, culturally competent teachers perceive the diversity manifested in the student body as strength to be used in linking curriculum to instruction and the teaching–learning process. have won a world championship and Set in Singapore, the study documents the lived experiences of four individuals from the normal technical Ismail and Tan (2006) now I can retire’: Exploring normal technical students’ ways of unpacking academic expectations in Singapore Pauline S.K. Ho * The course who have succeeded academically. It will be argued that these individuals appear to be active and capable of drawing on valued knowledge and resources to participate in the institutionalised academic community. The participants’ experiences also indicate that theremay be a mismatch between the participants’ interpretations of what was required of them and the institutionalised academic expectations. Their collective narratives challenge the exclusion of educationally disadvantaged students and confront traditional narratives of these youth as high potential dropouts. However, it remains the case that a percentage of students are not performing up to their academic potential, exacerbating global disparities in achievement between rich and poor countries (UNESCO, 2009). Even within wealthy and developed countries, inequalities exist between regions, communities, schools and classrooms: illustrates the challenges that lower track, normal technical students face as they negotiate interlocking personal, cultural, societal and political pressures in unpacking academic expectations and requirements throughout their educational Journeys. there seems to be a lack of awareness of the relationship between educationally disadvantaged students’ understandings of specific academic demands, expectations and requirements. Little is known about individual educationally disadvantaged students’ educational experiences and their specific strategies to gain access Ismail, M., Tan, A.L., 2006. Voices from the Normal Technical World: An Ethnographic Study of Low-track Students. Centre for Research in Pedagogy and Practice National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore. Cole and Knowles (2001) Cole, A.L., Knowles, J.G., 2001. Lives in Context: The Art of Life History Research. Alta Mira Press, Walnut Creek, CA. to membership in academic discourse (Teese and Polesel, 2003). However, in practice, pedagogical and instructional practices were found to be largely teacher-centred and didactic. Ismail and Tan (2006) found that teachers in normal technical classrooms tended to rely on ‘‘prescriptive’’pedagogy,where learning activitieswere consistently teacher-directed, highly structured and provided little room for active construction of knowledge and concepts. According to Kellaghan (2001), educational disadvantage is defined in two ways: (1) discontinuities between the competencies and dispositions which children bring to school and the competencies and dispositions valued in schools and (2) factors, conceptualised in terms of economic, cultural, social resources, which influence development of the competencies and dispositions. Scholars who advocate looking at educational risk through cultural deficit lens tend to associate ‘‘disadvantage’’ with low SES and membership in particular racial or ethnic minority groups, linking poor educational outcomes to problems in their homes, families and cultures A significant point has been raised by Moll et al. (1992) and his colleagues who contend that the factors for educationally disadvantaged students’ successful academic outcomes are often mediated by socially valued resources that can potentially assist them to perform important tasks in life. Termed as ‘‘funds of knowledge’’, these resources are ‘‘historically accumulated and culturally developed bodies of knowledge and skills essential for household or individual functioning’’ (Moll et al., 1992, p. 133). The availability of home- and community-based funds of knowledge challenged the perception that educationally disadvantaged students are unlikely to share similar educational values to those of schooling institutions (e.g. Gregory and Williams, 2000; Knight et al., 2004; Sanchez, 2007). This paper is an attempt to highlight social exchange theory by exploring how normal technical students in Singapore understand and make sense of what was expected from them in terms of institutionalised academic expectations, and in doing so, take control of their educational lives. A life narrative approach not only provides the means to understand how the educational context influences and contributes to the normal technical students’ educational transformation, it also offers the ‘capacity’ to present a realistic viewpoint that can challenge institutionalised norms and conventions in the broader context (Cole and Knowles, 2001). The interviews lasted between 1.5 and 2.5 h (averaging about 2 h). Cole and Knowles (2001) argue that lives should be studied through participants’ own voices and accounts and understood through all dimensions of one’s social realm – political, sociological, personal and cultural. Thus, the interview allowed opportunities for the participants to reflect on their challenges of navigating the normal technical course, and in particular, on the ways they have attempted to interpret institutionalised academic expectations. The interview also elicited deeper information on the experience of being a normal technical student and what achievement meant to them, why they were doing it, and to what effect and where it was fitted with a macro and broader set of views that they were committed to, for example, beliefs, values and perspectives of family, community, education or institutional Structures. across multiple contexts of their family, school and Community. Carol recognised the mismatch between her personal learning expectations and the institutionalised ways of learning, noting how she was from ‘‘a different path . . . it is not right to compare me with the other students.’ Carol’s narratives provided understandings of how schools and pedagogic patterns are based on a one size fits all model that is oriented towards the valued knowledge of the dominant society (Freebody et al., 1995; Ga´ndara and Contreras, 2009). Within these pedagogies, families use valuable funds of knowledge (Moll et al., 1992) – ‘‘historically accumulated and culturally developed bodies of knowledge and skills essential for household or individual functioning’’ (Moll et al.,1992, p. 133) – to support and shapeyouth’s identities in the academic-social network. Specifically, Carol talked about how her mother’s counter-narratives about societal roles and expectations illustrated to her about how she could actively shape her own educational decision making processes. Families support educationally disadvantaged students to negotiate the daily realities of the working-class in urban neighbourhoods throughtaking themtowork, prayingwiththem, or working to help pay tuition fees (Lo´ pez, 2001). It is an individual’s familiarity within the mainstream culture of the family context, or habitus (Bourdieu, 1994), they inhabit that forms their value and belief systems which in turn affects their behaviour, conduct and actions. This is an important insight for the Singapore’s secondary mainstream schooling context, given that students are grouped homogeneously according to their academic abilities into different classrooms within the same school. It can be seen that Carol’s positioning as a normal technical student came with the notion that normal technical students are ‘‘no good’’ in the perception of the school and public. These perspectives construct discriminatory notions of normal technical students as ‘different’, ‘deficient’, with confused academic identities and responsible for their failure to achieve greater academic goals. It is hoped that the findings of this study will provide theoretical grounds for further research in understanding the role of peer students in influencing the academic aspirations of low-performing students The findings have implications for policies and practices and future research that can be constructed in three ways: (1) a shift from educationally disadvantaged students as passive learners to activators of knowledge, (2) a tripartite model in urban schools, and (3) a focus on learning pedagogies that necessitate a rich connection between learning and understanding. Applications of Social Capital in Educational Literature: A Critical Synthesis Sandra L. Dika and Kusum Singh Virginia Tech The focus of the review is on educational literature that studies social capital and educational outcomes social capital is positively linked to educational and psychosocial outcomes. Finally, they discuss gaps in the conceptualization, measurement, and analysis of social capital in educational literature This critical synthesis explores the usage of social capital as an explanatory variable in educational research, drawing on theoretical literature in sociology and economics, and empirical literature in education and family/child studies Next, the review assesses whether there is theoretical and empirical support for generalized claims that social capital is (Dyk & Wilson, 1999). Dyk, P. H., & Wilson, S. M. (1999). Familybased social capital considerations as predictors of attainments among Appalachian youth. Sociological Inquiry, 69(3), positively linked to educational achievement (grades, test scores), educational attainment (graduation, college enrollment), and psychosocial factors that affect educational development (engagement, motivation, self-concept). In the fourth and final section of the article, methodological issues, including operationalization and measurement, in the reviewed body of literature are discussed and critiqued 477–503. The work of researchers such as Lareau (1989), Lareau and Horvat (1999), and Bourdieu’s conceptualization is grounded in theories of social reproduction Stanton-Salazar and symbolic power. As a result, social capital has been elaborated in two (1997, 2001) principal ways: in terms of norms and in terms of access to institutional resources. holds promise for This differentiation is apparent in theoretical interpretations and resulting empirical revealing how Work. social capital is accessed and He defined social capital as the utilized by youth aggregate of actual or potential resources linked to possession of a durable network and of essentially institutionalized relationships of mutual acquaintance and recognition. families in This group membership provides members with the backing of the collectively educational owned capital. Relations may exist as material or symbolic exchanges. Social institutions. capital is made up of social obligations or connections and it is convertible, in certain Lareau, A. (2001). conditions, into economic capital. Linking Bourdieu’s concept of capital Bourdieu (1986) proposes that the volume of social capital possessed by a person to the broader field: depends on size of the network of connections that he or she can mobilize and The case on the volume of capital—economic, cultural, and symbolic—possessed by each of family-school person to whom he or she is connected. Thus, Bourdieu’s social capital is relationships. In B. decomposable J. Biddle (Ed.), into two elements: first, the social relationship that allows the individual Social class, to claim resources possessed by the collectivity, and, second, the quantity and poverty, and quality education: of those resources (Portes, 1998). Ultimately, Bourdieu sees social capital as the Policy and practice investment of the dominant class to maintain and reproduce group solidarity and (pp. 77–100). New preserve the group’s dominant position (Lin, 1999a). York: Routledge Falmer. cultural capital, habitus, and field Cultural capital can exist in three states: embodied (dispositions of mind and body), objectified (cultural goods), and institutionalized (educational qualifications). Certain forms of cultural capital are valued more than others, and each person brings a different set of dispositions (habitus) to the field of interaction. Social space is a field of forces and struggles between agents with different means and ends (Bourdieu, 1998). The field is characterized by the “rules of the game,” which are neither explicit nor codified. Because the field is dynamic, valued forms of social and cultural capital are also dynamic and arbitrary Coleman proposes that social capital is intangible and has three forms: (a) level of trust, as evidenced by obligations and expectations, (b) information channels, and (c) norms and sanctions that promote the common good over self-interest. Particularly, he emphasizes intergenerational closure—parents know the parents of their children’s friends—as a social structure that facilitates the emergence of effective norms. Bourdieu sees social capital as a tool of reproduction for the dominant class, whereas Coleman sees social capital as (positive) social control, where trust, information channels, and norms are characteristics of the community. Thus, Coleman’s work supports the idea that it is the family’s responsibility to adopt certain norms to advance children’s life chances, whereas Bourdieu’s work emphasizes structural constraints and unequal access to institutional resources based on class, gender, and race (Lareau, 2001) Bourdieu’s theories of cultural reproduction and of cultural and social capital were developed as alternative explanations of unequal academic Lareau, A., & Horvat, E. M. (1999). Moments of social inclusion and exclusion: Race, class, and cultural capital in familyschool relationships. Sociology of Education, 72, 37–53. achievement to skill deficit and human capital theories literacy and how pedagogic discourse is an instrument of class reproduction—specifically, “how . . . power and control translate into principles of communication . . . [that] differentially regulate forms of consciousness with respect to their reproduction” (Bernstein, 2000, p. 4). Surprisingly, Bernstein’s theory is not referenced in most current educational literature on cultural and social capital of linguistic and cultural minorities. Lareau (2001) is one of the only authors to explicitly incorporate Bourdieu’s idea of field into educational research on social and cultural capital. She suggests that the institutional standards or “rules of the game” to which Bourdieu referred in his notion of “field” have been generally overlooked by researchers. The relationship between social capital and educational achievement is examined in fourteen of the studies reviewed in this article. All nine studies that specifically link achievement test scores with social capital use the NELS database. Four achievement tests were completed by NELS respondents: math, science, reading, and history. Overall, relationships are significant in the expected direction. Achievement on these tests is negatively associated with family size, moving, and nontraditional family structure (Sun, 1998, 1999). While most of the research indicates that social capital is indeed positively associated with educational achievement, the studies by McNeal (1999) and StantonSalazar and Dornbusch (1995) raise questions about the direction and nature of the relationship between these variables Five studies find positive relationships between educational aspirations and social capital, including parental expectations (Muller & Ellison, 2001), parent-teen discussion about school (Muller & Ellison; Pribesh & Downey, 1999; Smith-Maddox, 1999), parent-school involvement (Pribesh & Downey; Smith-Maddox), Although most of the relationships are significant in the expected directions, the current body of research does not provide sufficient theoretical or empirical support for hypotheses about the positive relationship between social capital and education-related factors. This is due primarily to weaknesses in and misapplications of Coleman’s concept, which have been named throughout the article. Nearly all of these studies focus on the conceptualization of social capital as norms rather than access to institutional resources. Methodological gaps in the conceptualization and measurement of social capital, including the reliance on cross-sectional data, hamper the utility of the concept as an explanatory variable in education. These gaps are discussed in detail in the next section. Conventional statistical measures of supportive ties (e.g., number of parents, parent-child discussion) are poor and unreliable indicators of social capital, and they give little information about relationship dynamics or the quality of the resources accessed (Stanton-Salazar, 2001). In most of these studies, prior measures of academic resources, academic performance, and social capital are not taken into consideration. Longitudinal studies are necessary to an understanding of the direction of the relationship between educational outcomes and social resources. Sage handbook of applied research methods - bickman and rog Ethnography is about telling a credible, rigourous and authentic stoy. Ethnogrpahy gives voice to people in thei own local context, typically relying on verbatim quotations and a thick description of evemts. The storu is told through the eyes of local people as they pursue their daily lives in their own communities. The ethnographer adopts a cultural lens to interpret observed behaviour, ensuing that the behaviours are placed in a culturally relevant and meaningful context. Goodwin, pope, mort, smith , 2003 ethics and ethnography: an experimental account. Ethnography is thus both a research method and a product, typically a written text. Controlled, biases can focus and limit the research effort. Uncontrolled they can undermine the qulaity of ethnogrpahic research. Culture is the sum of a social group’s observable patterns of behaviour, customs and way of life (Harris, 1968, p.16; murphy & margolis, 1995; ross, 1980) Emic perspective - the insider’s or native’s perspective of reality - is at the heart of most ethnographic research. The insider’s eprception of reality is instrumental to understanding and accurately describing situations and behaviours. Native perceptions may not conform to an obejctive reality but they help the fieldworker understand why members of the social group do what they do. Ethnography typically takes a phenomenologically oriented research approach. An etic perspective is an external social scientific perspectve on reality. Most ethnographers start collecting data from the emic perspective and then try to make sense of what they have collected in terms of both the native’s view and their own scientific analysis. Structure refers to the social structure or configuration of the group such as the kinship or political structure. Ethnogrpahes use the concepts of structure and function to guide their inquiry. Methods of ethnogrpahy - fieldwork Working with people fr long periods o time in their natural setting. The ethnographer conducts research in the native enviornment to see people and ther behaviour given all the real world incentives and constrinats. This naturalist approach avoids the artificial response typicels of controlleld and lab conditions. Qualittative health research Most ethnographers mix and mingle with everyone they can at first. As the study preogresses the ocus narrows to specific portions of the population under study. Participant observation combines participation in the lives of the people under study with maintenance of a professional distance that allows adequate observation and recording of data. Ethnographic techniques but not ethnography Interviews - informal, semistructured interviews. Informal interviews with a specific but implicit research agenda - use informal approaches to discover the categories of the meaning of culture. The key actor is another term for informant Multiple key actors Analysis of ethnographies Process information in a meaningful and useful manner Triangulation is at the heart of ethnographic validity, testing one source of inofmration against one another to strip away alternative explanaltions and prove a hypothesis. Compares life information sources to test the quality of information and the person sharing it. To understnad more completel the part an actor plays in the social drama Patterns are ethnographic realibility . ethnographers see patterns of through and action repat in various situations and among various players. Looking for patterns in a form of analysis. Ethnographers crystallizetheir thoughts at various stages thoughtout the ethno endeavour. The crystallization might bring about a mundane conclusion, a novel insight, or an earth shattering epiphany. It is the result of a convergence of similarities that spontaneously strike the ethnographer as a relevant or important to the study. One of the most exciting moments in ethnogaphic research is when they discover something counter intuitive - conception that defies common sense Ethnographers do not work in a vaccum, they work with people. They often pry into peoples innermost secrets, sacred rites, achievements and failtures. This code specifies that the ethnographer does no harm to the people or community under study Do not trample on the feealing of the ket actors or praticipants. Respect for the social integrity. Non - invasive ethnography is not only good ethics but also good science Permission Honestty Trust Reciprocity - use a great deal of people’s time, they owe something in return. Sympathetic ear - teach math and science. Or english. Guilty knowledge - confidential information of illegal activities Dirty hands - where ethnograophers cannot emerge innocent of worng doing Ethical decision making will be handled situarionally bacuse o the complecity of them problem (goodwin, et al 2003; goode, 1999) this fact of ethnographic life sharpens the senses and ultimately refines and enhances the quality of the endeavour. Linking bourdieu’s concept of capital to How the social class background of cheildren influences theri life chances 2 ethnic minority students - one boy the broader field - the case of family school relationships and one girl Experiences in normal academic/technical streams annette lareau Comparing that with Bourdieu highlights the ay that practics are infused (unequally) with social legitimaion so that not all cultural practices are vewed as having equal value. Researchers have looked too much at individuals only and not at the standrads of the ht einstitutions through which individuals seek to succeed. Double vision offered by bourdei’us work on biography and social structure through the study of the fields and practices of individuals - is often absent in empirical work. Importance of using boirdeiu’s concept of fields and social capital. Empirical study of family-school relationships 2 white boys attending different schools Worksing class family x dominant institution Middle class amily x dominant institution Bourdieu suggests that one cannot restrcit ones attnetion to the culturl practices per se. In his mind, inqeuality in schooling cannot be reduced to the practices paretns engage in within their homes (bourdeiu & passeron, 1977). Instead he points to the standards of instirutions and their relations to individual actions. 2 ethnic minorit stuents - middle class family Experiences in express streams The school and at a smaller subset the stream as the field; the habitus which are the dispositions yhat the students bring into the classroom and school - the interaction of these - how it infleunces their life chances or educational outcomes Impact of social class on educational experience Bourdieu reflects on the existence of dominant standards or rules of the game in the field. The individual brings a different set of dispositions (habitus) to the fields of interactions (different rules of the game) and therby helpds to create different practices in a particular moment. Patterns of regularities (Lareau, 2001, p. 84) Habitus is about social training - haitus is a socialised subejectivitity (Bourdieu & Wacquant, 1992, p. 18) Habitus is a system of lasting and transposable dispositions which integrating past experiences, functions at every moment as a matrix of perceptions, appreciations, and actions and makes possibel the acheivement of infinitely diversified tasks (Bourdieu & wacquant, 1992, p. 18) When the habitus of the person meshes with the habitus of the broader cultrue, it is often invisible. On the new critical East Asian educational studies Article in Curriculum Inquiry · February 2018 it is notable that the work published here is not by White Western scholars working in specialised “area studies” (cf. Woo & King, 2013) but by people with familial, educational, linguistic and generational roots in these places. Taken together, these two generations of critical work mark a shift in the locus of formation and exchange of knowledge As Takayama notes, PISA reading tests are run through item response validation, a technical process that ensures that any items that generate differentiated results by, for example, cultural group membership or gender are deleted from item banks. Hence, conventional item analysis technically precludes cultural bias. But in so doing, it militates against engaging with culturally variable practices – in this case, longstanding close reading practices from Japanese schooling. So the frustration of the Japanese curriculum experts involved in PISA consultations that Takayama reports is unsurprising. Leonel Lim & Michael Tan (2018) Culture, pedagogy and equity in a meritocratic education system: Teachers’ work and the politics of culture in Singapore, Curriculum Inquiry, 48:2, 184-202, DOI: 10.1080/03626784.20 18.1435974 In Singapore, however, the state’s official discourse of meritocracy has for long remained silent on the role of culture in students’ learning and how it relates to the systemic underachievement of various social groups. Instead, the state has consistently appealed to meritocracy’s principle of non-discrimination (especially in terms of ethnic differences) as being fundamental to the establishment of a level-playing field in the education system. Drawing upon qualitative data comprising lesson observations of and interviews with a group of teachers who teach in low-progress classrooms, the paper documents the creative approaches taken by these teachers as they engage their students in ways attuned to the latter’s family backgrounds, home conditions, and personal aspirations. The findings suggest that in actively if sometimes unconsciously foregrounding such a cultural dimension in their teaching, the five teachers studied are resisting, even challenging meritocracy’s principle of non-discrimination. Prominently articulated in the US by Ladson-Billings (1994, 1995a, 2009) in Gloria LadsonBillings’ (1994, 2009) her study of the pedagogic practices of exceptional teachers of African-American students, one such approach – culturally relevant pedagogy – identifies students’ unique cultural backgrounds as strengths and these are nurtured to promote academic achievement (Brown-Jeffy & Cooper, 2011; Ladson-Billings, 1995a; Morrison, Robbins, & Rose, 2008). Nevertheless, in this self-professed meritocracy, official state discourses have for long remained silent on the role of culture in students’ learning and how it relates to the systemic underachievement of various social groups (see, e.g., Kang, 2005, p. 15). One of the central insights arising out of the literature on students’ cultural backgrounds is that teachers need to understand that in academically low-progress classrooms, students frequently bring into the classroom cultural capital that is different from mainstream norms and worldviews. Yet difference is not a code word for deficience (Howard, 2003; see also Heng, 2016). In recent decades, one of the most influential pieces of research in the field is Gloria Ladson-Billings’ (1994, 2009) three-year ethnographic study of eight “exceptional” teachers of African-American students. Ladson-Billings found that while on the surface the teachers subscribed to a xxxvariety of instructional strategies and classroom routines from structured/unstructured to didactic/dialectic, all of them sought to harness the cultural and social capital embedded in students’ home backgrounds and to use these as resources to promote a more meaningful and engaged teaching and learning environment. Thus, for example, in utilizing students’ culture as a vehicle for learning, one of the teachers developed students’ appreciation of poetry through their own love of rap music Singapore, where the twin national beliefs of meritocracy and non-discrimination have often been simultaneously invoked to both emphasize the “fairness” of the socio-political system and “explain” (away) the subordinate role of the large population of non-Chinese races who lag behind in areas of life such as politics, income, language dominance and education (Barr & Low, 2005; Moore, 2000; Rahim, 1998). In such a context, as Barr (2006) warns, the state’s “charade of meritocracy” can in fact evacuate a concerted focus at a politics of difference, giving the veneer of equality while at the same time justifying the belief that various social groups “have not been able to make it in a meritocratic society because they have not worked hard enough and thus have only themselves to blame” (Rahim, 1998, p. 58). Data from the 2010 population census indicate that the Chinese are significantly overrepresented in local universities, forming 86% of the total enrolment, while constituting 74% of the overall population. Malays (5.5%) and Indians (6.8%) are correspondingly underrepresented in tertiary institutions (Singapore Department of Statistics, 2011). From 1966 to 2016, of the 251 winners of Singapore’s most prestigious scholarship, the President’s Scholarship, only 18 (7.2%) were non-Chinese (see also Barr & Skrbis, 2008). While official data is not available, our observations as well as those of other researchers who have studied these classrooms show that students from the minority Malay and Indian ethnic groups are over-represented in both Normal tracks (especially in the Normal Technical track), as are students from working-class families (Anderson, 2015; Kang, 2005; Albright, Heng & Harris, 2006; Wang, Tan, Li, Tan, & Lim, 2016) What Counts as Evidence and Equity? Allan Luke Queensland University of Technology Judith Green University of How we name and describe, document and understand educational equality and inequality, inclusion and exclusion, centrality and marginality, then, is the issue facing educational systems in economically hard time Adjacent volumes of Review of Research in Education have focused on reviews of critical research and development work on Bryk & Gomez, 2007 California, Santa Barbara Gregory J. Kelly Pennsylvania State University T race, difference, and diversity (Parker, 2007), and on “at risk” student and youth communities (Gadsden, Artiles, & Davis, 2009). Social capital in variablesTo measure social capital, respondents were asked the followingquestion: ‘Can you please tell me if you know anyone in Singaporewho works in the following kinds of jobs? You do not need to knowthese people really well, but should know them by name, by sight,and well enough to talk to.’ Singapore: Gender differences, ethnic hierarchies,and their intersectionVincent Chuaa,∗, Mathew Mathewsb, Yi Cheng LohcaNational Social Class and the Hidden Curriculum The laudable goal of many of these policies has been closure of what is now termed the equity gap—the differential between general population norms and the performance of identified equity groups: for example, African Americans, Latinos, migrants, second language speakers, and students from low socioeconomic backgrounds (see Lucas & Beresford, Chapter 2; Jordan, Chapter 5). of Work JEAN ANYON Theory and educational research : toward critical social explanation / Jean Anyon with Michael J. Dumas ... [et al.]. Bridiging the theory and data - analytics of exogeny. That is we assume one cannot understand or explain X by merely describing X. one must exogenousy look at nn x - particularyl the context and social forces in which the object of study is emdedded. Class size, curriculum, or student demographics, teacher experience, pedagogy or skill, leadership, budgers are not all that makes a school what it is. And describing them does not constitute a satisfying explantation of what occurs there. Without theory, our data on school experience or social phenomena do not go very far, and do not tll us very much that is not already obvious. Our data do not leave the groundd on which they were found, our explanations do not soar, and they may fail to inspire. Stephen J ball (2006,p.64). Too often in educational studies, theory becomes no ore than a mantric reaffirmation of belief rather than a tool for exploration and for thinking otherwise. For Writght Mills, the point was to reveal that people’s personal problems are in fact also matters of social structure, and that neither raw empiricism nor abstract theorizing can connect the two (mills, 1959, 2000) As noguera and wind point out, high school is the last opportuniy for young peopl to acquire the knowledge and skills they need to acheive in life goals (2006). It is the final destination for all students who make it to the ninth grade and it is the place where future trajectories to ivy league colleges, to state and community colleges, to dead end job or to prison are determined. Annette lareau finds that bourdieu’s concept of cultural capital has the potential to show how individual biography intersects with social structure, a potental that theoretical and empirical work must take advantage of. (1989, 179) The concept of cultural capital is useful for researching families relationships with schools becasue educators tend to perceive the cultural capital of those who control the economic, social and political resoices as the natural and preoper sort, thus they favour students and familieis who posesse the cultural forms of the dominat groups (harker, 1984) EDUCATION POLICY | REVIEW ARTICLE A systematic review of factors linked to poor academic performance of disadvantaged students in science and maths in schools Pallavi Amitava Banerjee1* The protocol from preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (PRISMA) was followed. Results suggest major factors linking deprivation to underachievement can be thematically categorised into a lack of positive environment and support. Several factors can however potentially limit a child’s academic achievement. Contextual indicators continue to be the determining parameters for educational attainment, learning trajectories and careers. Adversities in education are faced by ethnic minorities, refugee/ asylum seekers, immigrants, young people who have spent time in care and poorer pupils (OECD, 2016; Strand, 2014). Eligibility for free school meals (FSM)/reduced price lunches, family income below a certain threshold, residence in a potentially deprived area or low progression neighbourhoods identified by indices of multiple deprivation are all indicators of a lower socioeconomic status (SES) and efforts for widening participation agenda are targeted by government in the United Kingdom and elsewhere. Disadvantaged pupils do not perform as well academically as their elite peers (Reardon, 2011; Steele, 2010). Evidence comes from a study exploring the effect of poverty on achievement of urban African- American students successfully completing high school. Welch (2014) shows cumulative 10th-grade GPA was strongly negatively correlated with student poverty level. This situation certainly needs remediation as (a) it is always desirable to have a range of people from different sections of the society studying and working in different fields for instance, having a diverse intake for STEM courses will lead to a more innovative and responsive STEM workforce (RAE, 2012), Westerlund, Gustafsson, Theorell, Janlert, and Hammarstrom (2013) provides evidence that parental involvement is a more definite predictor as opposed to parent’s social class or availability of practical academic support (N = 365 women, 352 men). Children whose mothers have less than a high school education have lower cognitive skill scores at three years of age (Ayoub et al., 2009; Hanson et al., 2011). Similarly, teen mothers and mothers who are illiterate or unemployed are more likely to raise academically underachieving children as compared to those who have a primary or tertiary level of education despite belonging to the same SES (N = 3001, national datasets USA). Using PISA data from more than 40 participating countries, Nonoyama (2005) in a cross-national study tried to understand the effect of family background on student achievement. The study concluded family SES and background effects had a larger bearing on student achievement than SES alone or school effects across countries. Just as parental support and involvement sometimes the conditions under which children thrive affects their educational outcomes. An analysis of 10th-grade sample of US Educational Longitudinal Study shows student perceptions of teachers and teacher’s attitudes can predict academic performance and discipline. Using SES as a measure of disadvantage the study provides evidence that students’ relationship with teachers, perception of teacher sensitivity and the reasons for attendance are the strongest predictors of scholastic achievements. Students in the lowest SES quartile very often did not attend school because of their teacher’s expectation of success and for the fear of humiliation in class (Whitehead, 2007). Strambler and Weinstein (2010) using a sample of 111 students from California showed student’s higher perception of negative teacher feedback predicts more devaluing of academics and greater perceived teacher care at classroom level predicts less devaluing. A lower regard for academics consistently predicts poorer maths test scores. Positive teacher expectations, support and motivation have progressive effects on students regardless of their risk status and particularly for lower income students (Gregory & Huang, 2013; Sorhagen, 2013). Survey data analysis suggested perceived discrimination is a risk factor for psychosocial and academic outcomes. Similarly, “public regard”—the extent to which individuals felt that others view their racial/ethnic group positively or negatively was significantly associated with school importance. The perception that one’s group is valued in society predicts a stronger belief that school is important. Organisational citizenship behaviours and measures of student achievement in Biology also showed a significant positive correlation (Tindle, 2013). Kevin Michael Foster (2004) Coming to terms: a discussion of John Ogbu’s culturalā ecological theory of minority academic achievement, Intercultural Education, 15:4, 369384, DOI: 10.1080/14675980420 00313403 To link to this article: https://doi.org/ cultural-ecological (CE) theory of minority student responses to schooling. I also offer critical commentary and point out ways in which CE theory can be sharpened to facilitate increasingly nuanced and accurate analyses Ogbu described cultural ecology as ‘the study of institutionalized patterns of behavior interdependent with features of the environment’ (Ogbu, 1990a, p. 122). posits that there are two sets of factors influencing minority school performance: how society at large and the school treats minorities (the system) and how minority groups respond to those treatments and to schooling (community forces). The theory further posits that differences in school performance between immigrant and nonimmigrant minorities are partly due to differences in their community forces. (Ogbu, 1999, p. 156) Voluntary minorities—those who immigrate to a host country ‘more-or-less by choice’—were said to have an ‘instrumental’ approach to their host society and its institutions, while involuntary minorities Ogbu’s CE theory includes four important layers: (1) the general idea that students’ academic success is impacted by community forces and system forces, and that not enough attention has been paid to the ways in which community Hermans, 2004; Kalekin-Fishman, 2004). forces contribute to involuntary minority student failure; (2) the distinction of voluntary, involuntary and autonomous minorities; (3) the recognition of universal, primary and secondary discontinuities between students and the schools they attend; (4) the idea that involuntary minorities have developed survival strategies—some of which facilitate academic success and others of which hinder it—including clientship/ Uncle Tomming, collective struggle, hustling, emulation of whites and camouflage. In addition to such system factors (also referred to as system forces), Ogbu believed that the culturally determined responses of minorities to their circumstances were also critical. He called these either community factors or community forces (Ogbu,1992a, p. 287; 2003, pp. vii–viii). Ogbu felt that the impact of community forces upon minority responses to schooling was consistently understudied Nor did he assume that discrimination did not have direct negative effects upon minority academic outcomes. He did, however, believe that, even within the context of systematic discrimination, there was room for minority agency. he saw a need for systematic study of community forces to complement analyses of system forces. It was his strong contention that it is not possible adequately to address issues of minority academic achievement without distinguishing between kinds of minorities. According to Ogbu, each kind of minority has different sets of experiences that inform their relationships to schools and schooling and, as a result, have developed different approaches to schooling. Articles where Ogbu’s explanation of this typology appear include Ogbu (1983a, 1985, 1990b, c, 1991, 1992a–c). Perhaps the first thing to note about Ogbu’s typology is its apparent usefulness on a broad conceptual level. Several scholars, who have at different times agreed or disagreed with aspects of his analysis, have nonetheless found that Ogbu’s categories hold in a number of different national contexts, and often provide useful units for analysis. In other settings, however, scholars have noted circumstances where the CE typology of minority students is complicated by local circumstances and by the varied circumstances and understandings under which minorities choose to immigrate. Primary discontinuities are the differences between the cultural norms and language of students and the culture and language norms of the schools they attend. In other words, those African-Americans who experience academic success would be those who find a way to distance themselves from African-American culture. This explanation allowed for the possibility of African-. American student success without undermining Ogbu’s longstanding and steadfast belief in the incompatibility between African-American culture and academic success. It shows the complexity and difficulty of the circumstances marginalized groups often face, as well as the complexity of their responses to such conditions. In many respects, this could be considered the essence of the CE work that Ogbu advocates—a comprehensive look at how environment influences patterns of behavior. (2004, p. 7). To Fischer Culture is not a variable; culture is relational, it is elsewhere, it is in passage, it is where meaning is woven and renewed, often through gaps and silences, and forces beyond the conscious control of individuals, and yet the space where individual and institutional social responsibility and ethical struggle take place. (2004, p. 7) Moreover, while culture is ‘configured historically’, this is to say that culture is forged in context and that it is an ongoing and interactive process. Culture (or the culture of an ideal type group such as the voluntary minority) is not a singular entity that, once formed, simply always is, as is. Rather, to understand culture and cultural process, we must consider what Weber would refer to as the complex interaction of innumerable different historical factors (1992 [1930], p. 49). The Theory and Practice of Culturally Relevant Education: A Synthesis of Research Across Content Areas Brittany Aronson Miami University Judson Laughter University of Tennessee, Knoxville Gay (2013) believed that there were four actions essential to implementing culturally responsive teaching. The first required replacing deficit perspectives of students and communities. In the second, teachers must also understand the resistance to culturally responsive teaching from critics so they are more confident and competent in implementation; Gay suggested methods, such as having teachers conduct their own analysis of textbooks, to investigate how different knowledge forms affect teaching and learning. Third, teachers need to understand how and why culture and difference are essential ideologies for culturally responsive teaching given they are essential to humanity. Finally, teachers must make pedagogical connections within the context in which they are teaching. Ladson-Billings (1994) defined culturally relevant pedagogy as one “that empowers students intellectually, socially, emotionally, and politically using cultural referents to impart knowledge, skills, and attitudes” (pp. 16–17). Culturally relevant pedagogues think in terms of long-term academic achievement and not merely end-of-year tests. Culturally relevant pedagogues focus on cultural competence, which “refers to helping students to recognize and honor their own cultural beliefs and practices while acquiring access to the wider culture, where they are likely to have a chance of improving their socioeconomic status and making informed decisions about the lives they wish to lead” (Ladson-Billings, 2006, p. 36). Culturally relevant pedagogues understand that students must learn to navigate between home and school, and teachers must find ways to equip students with the knowledge needed to succeed in a school system that oppresses them (Delpit, 2006; Ladson-Billings, 2006; Urrieta, 2005). Culturally relevant educators use constructivist methods to develop bridges connecting students’ cultural references to academic skills and concepts. Culturally relevant educators build on the knowledges and cultural assets students bring with them into the classroom; the culturally relevant classroom is inclusive of all students. •• Culturally relevant educators engage students in critical reflection about their own lives and societies. In the classroom, culturally relevant educators use inclusive curricula and activities to support analysis of all the cultures Represented Voluntary and Involuntary Minorities: A Cultural-Ecological Theory of SchoolPerformance with Some Implications for Education Author(s): John U. Ogbu and Herbert D. Simons Addressing reciprocity between families and schools: Why these bridges are instrumental for students’ academic success Yune Tran George Fox University, USA One instrumental step in promoting overall children’s academic success across the trajectory of early childhood, elementary, middle, and secondary grades is purposefully establishing positive linkages for families and schools through a shared partnership. By facilitating an ongoing collaborative approach to sustain family engagement practices both in and out of the classroom, schools can help to build parents’ capacity to effectively support their children’s academic development key factors in promoting student outcomes are successful home and school partnerships. Extensive studies have also documented the vital roles that families play in fostering learning experiences for their children and their relationships to academic success. Therefore, the relationship between family involvement and student achievement must first be examined to understand the greater benefits in building positive partnerships between home and school. Epstein (2002) provided a model of parental involvement discussed within six types of activities that build relationships between the family, school, and community. These six types include: parenting, communicating, volunteering, learning at home, decision-making, and collaborating with the community and serve as the core for helping teachers and school professionals develop ways to involve families. higher levels of family engagement correlated to cognitive advantages and higher scores on math and language-arts content areas with reduced behavior problems and more positive interactions with their peers (Domina, 2005; Rimm-Kaufmann, Pianta, Cox, & Bradley, 2003). Izzo, Wiessberg, Kasprow, and Fendrich (1999) assessed how certain behaviors of family involvement enhance children’s academic achievement and social development. Such behaviors include the frequency of educator contact with parents; the quality of those interactions; family participation in school activities; and family engagement in home activities or other extracurricular activities. Marchant, Paulson, and Rothlisberg’s (2001) examination of the correlation of family (parenting style and parent involvement) and school contexts on students’ motivation suggested a positive relationship existed between parental values to student motivation. The study concluded that parental values on achievement were important to students’ motivations, confidence, and competence. Parent involvement that emphasized achievement rather than a general parenting style was strongly related to student motivation. Students who perceived that their parents value the importance of effort and academic success generally were achieving academically and placed a high priority on grades as compared to students who imagined that their parents did not value education. The study indicated the stark contrast between the ethnic distributions of public school teachers and administrators as compared to the diverse racial groups represented by students and their families. Most teachers come from predominantly European-American middle-class backgrounds and enter the field without much exposure to social and ethnic diversity or the skills to cope with these realities. Moreover, teachers and administrators’ attitudes about parent or family engagement are seen through lens of EuropeanAmerican middle-class values, experiences, and assumptions Moreover, most teachers do not have the same points of views or cultural frames of reference as their students (Heath, 1983; Sleeter & Grant, 1987, 2005) resulting in educators who adopt similar ideologies or are unprepared to empathize with students whose family backgrounds differ from their own. Social and cultural barriers such as these, along with incompatible expectations and parents’ negative experiences in schools have hindered home and school partnerships for ethnically diverse families. However, these obstacles are diminished through conscientious teacher practices that recognize families as crucial members in the educational process of their children. One effective change is moving from a subtractive model to an assets-based approach highlighting the diverse experiences that culturally and linguistically students bring to the classroom because they are navigating their way between two different worlds as they transition from home to school (Colombo, 2006). The uniqueness of families allows for the promotion of strengths based on family-centered practices since beliefs and behaviors of school achievement vary across cultural and ethnic groups (Muscott, 2002). Assignments should provide opportunities for children to reinforce conceptual understandings from grade-level content with realistic goals that tap into their diverse educational experiences and contexts of what, when, and how students learn.