C H A P T E R 11 Sex Differences in the Brain: Focus on Developmental Mechanisms Margaret M. McCarthy University of Maryland School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States O U T L I N E 11.1 Introduction 129 11.2 Historical Perspective and Current Status 130 11.3 Conceptualizing Sex Differences 131 11.4 Steroid Hormones Program the Developing Brain 11.4.1 Female Development is the Default 11.4.2 Critical Periods for Sexual Differentiation of the Brain 11.5 Mechanisms of Steroid-Induced Masculinization 11.5.1 Steroids and Steroid Receptors 11.5.2 Sex Differences in the Brain are Programmed by Steroids 11.5.3 Cell Death is Developmentally Regulated by Steroids in Brain Regions Controlling Reproduction 11.5.4 Cell Birth is also Developmentally Regulated but in Brain Regions not Directly Associated with Reproduction 132 132 133 134 134 135 136 139 140 142 142 144 11.6 Elucidating Mechanisms of Normal Brain Development in Males and Females Provides Clues to Sources of Vulnerability and Resilience 145 References 145 137 11.1 INTRODUCTION Males and females have different physiologies and they behave differently, that is an absolute and raises little concern when the subjects under discussion are animals. But when it comes to translating that absolute to humans there is both emotional and scientifically rigorous debate.1,2 And that is as it should be. If health and policy decisions are to be made based on sex as a Principles of Gender-Specific Medicine. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-803506-1.00033-4 11.5.5 Nonneuronal Cells can be Key Contributors to Sexual Differentiation 11.5.6 Steroids Induce Epigenetic Changes to the DNA to Induce and Maintain Sex Differences 11.5.7 Steroids can also Act Rapidly to Induce Enduring Change in Neural Circuits 11.5.8 Steroids Indirectly Modulate Neuronal Excitability in the Developing Brain 11.5.9 Sex Chromosome Complement also Matters to Brain Development variable, the science should be substantial in amount, broad in scope, and high in caliber. When the question is sex differences in the brain, that goal cannot be achieved without a strong foundational base of research in animal models. But can rats and mice or even nonhuman primates really inform us as to the myriad of ways that boys and girls and men and women differ? Probably not. Humans are singular in their use of complex language and computational skills and our social societies 129 © 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 130 11. MECHANISMS ESTABLISHING SEX DIFFERENCES IN BRAIN and attendant cultural norms are far more variable and complicated than even the most sophisticated animal groups. Humans are also singular for possessing gender, defined as a combination of self and societal perception of an individual’s sex. We can’t ask animals if they know what sex they are and how they feel about it, so this parameter remains unique to humans. The power of gender to impact the developing brain begins at the earliest stages of life, with parents speaking to and physically interacting with newborns in a sex-biased way. The influence only grows from there as children are both actively and passively driven to conform to gender norms.3 Thus it is impossible to truly separate out the impact of environment and experience from biology in sculpting the brains of humans in ways that may differ in males and females. Given the enormity of the gap between humans and other species, some say there is little to no value in the study of animal models. But this denies the value of understanding mechanism, i.e., understanding the precise way in which molecular, biochemical, and cellular processes are impacted by the biological variable of sex. These discoveries can only be made when exploiting the experimental power of animal models, in particular the many advantages offered by rats and mice. Initially the dose of androgen was sufficiently high that the females had male-like genitalia and so their malelike behavior could be either brain- or body-derived. But subsequent experiments lowered the dose sufficiently that the females were feminized in appearance but still behaved as males, leading to the speculation that it was indeed the brain that regulated their male-like behavior. However, there was one important caveat: the females required male-like hormone levels in order to behave as males in adulthood. Moreover, they were immune to the impact of female hormones and could not be induced to exhibit female sexual receptivity. This was subsequently codified as the Organizational/Activational Hypothesis meaning that early developmental hormone effects organize the brain which then must be activated by the appropriate hormonal profile in adulthood (Fig. 11.1). Shortly after the studies of Phoenix and colleagues, a separate group of researchers at UCLA were making the parallel discovery that developing females exposed to androgens were either frankly sterile or became progressively less fertile as adulthood progressed.6 Again the initial source was not thought to be the brain but instead a direct effect of hormones on the developing ovary or possibly the pituitary. Both target organs were eventually rejected as the origins of the hormone-induced fertility and the brain identified as the culprit via its regulation 11.2 HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE AND CURRENT STATUS Attribution of the first recorded endocrinology experiment is given to Arnold Berthold in 1849 for his observations of the impact of removing the testis in roosters which not only changed their physical characteristics, but also impacted their behavior. That the gonads were the source of a masculinizing substance was known long before then, dating to at least Aristotle. Yet it wasn’t entirely clear that the hormones of the gonads were the driving force of sex differences in behavior. Instead, one of the grandfathers of the field of neuroendocrinology, Frank Beach, postulated that it was the hormonal effects on the body that mattered and then the body determined behavior. Put simply, if an individual possessed the genitals and body type of a male they would attempt to mate with females and behave aggressively toward other males. The brain was considered secondary and essentially in service to the demands of the body phenotype (see for review Ref. 4). This is not an entirely unreasonable view. It is also a testable hypothesis, a task performed in the now iconic 1959 publication of Phoenix, Goy, Gerall, and Young,5 contemporaries of Beach. These scientists treated pregnant Guinea pigs with high doses of androgens and then observed the behavior of the female offspring as adults. FIGURE 11.1 Organizational/activational hypothesis. Sex chromosome complement directs differentiation of the bipotential gonad. The Sry gene of the Y chromosome codes for a testis determining factor. In the absence of Sry the gonad will become an ovary. The fetal and neonatal testis produces high levels of androgens, which serve as precursors to estrogens and together these steroid hormones direct the parallel but distinct processes of brain masculinization and defeminization. As a result, when testicular steroids again rise in adulthood male-typic behaviors are activated. In females the developing ovary remains quiescent and the brain undergoes the default process of feminization. In adulthood a feminized brain will respond to the cyclical production of ovarian hormones with changes in sexual receptivity and fertility. PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 11.3 Conceptualizing Sex Differences of LH secretion from the pituitary.7 Specifically it is the GnRH neurons of the diencephalon which control LH release. In males there is relatively continuous pulsatile release of LH, resulting in relatively continuous steroidogenesis and sperm production by the testis. In females, however, the pattern of LH release varies across the reproductive cycle with the culmination being a massive surge of release that induces ovulation by the ovary. In females exposed to high levels of androgen developmentally, the LH surge doesn’t happen and so instead the ovary develops large preovulatory follicles that remain essentially stuck, never releasing their ova, resulting in infertility. The discoveries that adult physiology and behavior were programmed developmentally and differently in males and females were major advances in our understanding of reproduction and spawned the subdisciplines of behavioral endocrinology and neuroendocrinology. But in essence the impact stopped there, with the topic of sex differences being relegated to a boutique area focused almost entirely on reproduction. The nascent field of neuroscience, which dates back only to the early 1970s, did not embrace the notion of either the value or relevance of sex differences in the brain, further walling off the topic. Ironically, the isolation became worse with the path breaking work of the Bruce McEwen laboratory in the 1990s demonstrating a nonreproductive endpoint that was profoundly influenced by hormonal changes across the estrus cycle. They reported that the synaptic profile of hippocampal neurons, a brain region central to learning and memory, shifted up to 30% in females at different stages of reproductive readiness.8 First met with incredulity but eventually accepted in the face of overwhelming data, this finding had the perhaps unexpected effect of further marginalizing sex differences research by alarming researchers not studying reproduction. The reasoning went that including female subjects in studies of fundamental aspects of nervous system function would only introduce variability and noise, and therefore it was best to use exclusively male rats and mice, gradually leading to the elimination of female subjects from preclinical research that was not directly relevant to reproduction.9 Ironically, in 1993 an act of the US congress, the National Institutes of Health Revitalization Act, mandated equal representation of women, children, and minorities in preclinical research and was thus at the same time having the opposite effect, significantly increasing the representation of underrepresented groups in clinical trials.10 Today we are in the midst of a paradigm shift that has forced a reckoning in our views of the brain and neuroscience research. This can be attributed to a convergence of the following factors. First, what was long known to be true, that most scientists only study males, was established to be true with actual data.9,11 Second, failed 131 clinical trials based on preclinical data overwhelmingly were shut down due to adverse events in women.12 Third, the gender bias in neuropsychiatric disorders across the life span and greater vulnerability of males to neurological disorders and birth injuries demanded that attention be paid to the binary variable of sex.1 Lastly, accumulating evidence of novel mechanisms by which sex differences are established highlighted how little we know about the fundamental rules governing brain development in both males and females and illuminated the potential benefits of learning such rules for all. 11.3 CONCEPTUALIZING SEX DIFFERENCES As the conversation about sex differences in the brain has grown more sophisticated there have been increased attempts to establish exactly what is meant when an endpoint is deemed “different” in males versus females. The term sexually dimorphic is evoked frequently and inappropriately. Widespread generalizations across species and endpoints further obscure the picture. Getting the semantics right is essential as it is important to know when a sex difference should be attended to, when it should be acknowledged but not necessarily incorporated into any conclusions and when it should just be ignored. It is equally important to know when there is no sex difference in an endpoint and this takes equal rigor to that of establishing that a sex difference exists.13 A primary source of confusion as well as rancor in the discussion of sex differences is the bundling together of endpoints that should not be considered in the same framework. The major dividing line is those endpoints that are physical markers, meaning neuroanatomy and neurophysiology, and those that are the external manifestation of neural functioning, meaning behavior and physiology (Table 11.1). The two sets of endpoints are connected, and indeed the goal of studying neuroanatomy and neurophysiology is to understand the control of physiology and behavior, but the connection is often much looser than we care to acknowledge. Both behavior and physiology are outputs from multifactorial inputs that must be integrated and weighted before a response is made. For instance, all of the neural circuits for mating behavior may be fully activated but if a predator is nearby the circuits controlling freezing will override those for mating. Likewise for foraging and feeding, a predator will certainly trump the urge to forage, but an attractive and available mating partner will trump the urge to forage. There is a hierarchy of control that assures survival in the short term but also motivates for successful reproduction in the long term. When we assess the neural circuits controlling these complex behaviors and find a sex difference, which is PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 132 11. MECHANISMS ESTABLISHING SEX DIFFERENCES IN BRAIN TABLE 11.1 Categories of Sex Differences Quantified in Animal Models Neuroanatomy Neural function Physiology Behavior Cell number and density (neurons, astrocytes and microglia) Firing threshold and rate Stress axis activation—HPAA Courtship, mating, and parenting Cell size and shape Neurotransmitter synthesis and release Reproductive axis activation—HPGA Foraging and feeding Synaptic patterning Neurotransmitter or modulator receptor number Metabolism Learning and memory Connectivity Signal transduction coupling Habit formation (i.e., addiction) Myelination Membrane trafficking Aggression (male:male and maternal) Neurochemical phenotype Steroid and steroid receptor levels Anxiety and emotionality Peptide synthesis and binding Audible communication (song in birds or ultrasonic vocalizations in rodents) Trophic factor synthesis and binding Chemical communication Sexual preference well established for the circuit controlling mating behavior, the relative importance of that difference only matters in the context of when the behavior is expressed, it provides a valence for external stimuli to be integrated and a calculation made as to whether the behavior is executed or not. This is perhaps best illustrated with mating behavior in which a male’s motivation to mate will override the motivation to eat relatively easily. But females must maintain a critical body mass of fat in order to reproduce and so the motivation to mate is properly tempered against that set point. A sex difference in the valences of the various nodes in both the mating and feeding circuits presumably underlies the sex difference in behavior, but precisely how this is achieved is beyond our current abilities in neuroscience to assess. We also know the neural circuits for mating are fully differentiated neonatally but the behavior will not be expressed until adulthood. Thus the neuroanatomical changes endure across the life span but the behavioral changes are both transient and context dependent (Fig. 11.2). 11.4 STEROID HORMONES PROGRAM THE DEVELOPING BRAIN As briefly reviewed above, steroids from the gonads are the determining factor for many sex differences in brain and behavior. To recap, in males the fetal testis produces high levels of androgens that will masculinize the brain so that in adulthood the animal responds to testosterone with execution of male-typic behavior and male physiology. Conversely, females are not exposed to high levels of androgens developmentally and in adulthood respond to ovarian hormones with female-typic behavior and physiology. The processes of masculinization and feminization are separate but not exclusive as evidenced by a third process of defeminization which occurs in males and removes the capacity for the male to respond to female hormones as an adult (see for review Ref. 14). 11.4.1 Female Development is the Default It is important to examine what this statement actually means. That female development is the default does not mean its not active, it is very much an active process, it just means that it is the developmental program that will proceed in the absence of an additional stimulus. In the case of the gonad, which begins as undefined, the additional stimulus is expression of the Sry gene located on the Y chromosome and which codes for a testis differentiating factor.15 This factor is now known to be a transcription factor that suppresses expression of aromatase and promotes expression of Sox-9, initiating a cascade of cell signaling pathways that execute the complex process of testis formation. If Sry is mutated or missing, the developmental progression of the undifferentiated gonad will proceed towards an ovary. And if Sry is accidentally or purposely translocated to the X or an autosome, testis differentiation will proceed. Similar to the undifferentiated gonad, the brain also proceeds by default as female, but in this case the differentiation stimulus originates in the gonads with the surge of androgen production prenatally. In this way nature assures that gonadal sex and brain sex are concordant, an essential feature for successful reproduction. Experimentally the default differentiation of the female provides the advantage that treatment with exogenous androgens can initiate the process of masculinization, thereby providing an experimental tool for interrogating PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 11.4 Steroid Hormones Program the Developing Brain 133 FIGURE 11.2 Sex differences in context. When behavioral or neuroanatomical endpoints are found to differ in males and females it is important to interpret them in the appropriate context. Neuroanatomical substrates that are organized early in development may be enduring but the behaviors they control are only expressed in response to the appropriate hormonal milieu or circumstances. Neuroanatomically differentiated substrates sometimes converge back to baseline during the juvenile hiatus and then reappear in adulthood. Alternatively, sometimes males and females have similar endpoints under basal conditions but diverge in response to nutritional or psychological challenges. Conversely, males and females may converge from different endpoints in order to compensate for unanticipated costs of the distinct reproductive costs inherent to each sex. the cellular mechanisms. On the other hand, that feminization is the default also makes it very difficult to dissect, other than as a process that is not masculinization. The study of defeminization provides some insight, but not all sexually differentiated endpoints include defeminization as a component, limiting the generalizability. 11.4.2 Critical Periods for Sexual Differentiation of the Brain Critical periods are defined as opportunistic windows during which essential developmental milestones must be met or are forever foreclosed. One of the best characterized critical periods is that for development of the visual system. Spontaneous waves of excitation from the retina to the brain prior to eye opening (in cats and rodents) initiates a critical period for synaptic innervation which is then refined by both intrinsic and extrinsic stimuli. If one eye is delayed from opening during a defined period of development, the visual field of the contralateral cortex will be invaded by the ipsilateral projections of the open eye and the first eye will be forever “blinded” even if perfectly functional. Even more dramatically, if animals, in this case kittens, are reared in an environment in which there are no vertical lines, only horizontal, they will be forever blind to horizontal lines. This critical period involves a matching of the external environment to the construction of an internal neural circuit and is based on a use-it-or-lose-it principle. In other words there is an initial overly exuberant innervation of the brain by the optic nerve and then this is pruned back and refined in response to specific visual stimuli (see for review Ref. 16). For sexual differentiation of the brain the initial trigger is the endogenous production of androgens by the male fetal testis, which begins in late gestation in rodents and at the beginning of the second trimester in humans. What initiates the sudden and dramatic upswing in fetal testis androgen production wasn’t known until a recent study which discovered a small set of kisspeptin neurons which appear only transiently and only in males to stimulate fetal GnRH neurons which in turn stimulate fetal pituitary LH release and the induction of steroidogenesis by the testis.17 Kisspeptin neurons were already known for their essential role in puberty onset, again via the regulation of GnRH neurons to establish the LH pulse generator in males, and ultimately the capacity for an LH surge in females (see for review Ref. 18). In humans fetal steroid levels are high mid-gestation but begin to drop by term in response to negative feedback from the high steroid levels associated with pregnancy. At birth this feedback inhibition is lost and the newborn hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis rebounds with prodigious gonadal steroidogenesis. Levels of androgens in infant males reach that of puberty, leading to the term minipuberty. This period of elevated steroids contributes PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 134 11. MECHANISMS ESTABLISHING SEX DIFFERENCES IN BRAIN to masculinization of the genitalia, spermagenesis, and somatic features. A contribution to masculinization of the brain is speculated but not clearly established (see for review Ref. 19). To date there is no clear role for minipuberty in feminization. In contrast to the transition to a new state of reproductive readiness that follows puberty, the induction of steroidogenesis by the fetal testis is temporary. In humans levels drop by 1–3 months and are almost absent by 6 months. In rodents levels drop within hours of birth. In both cases precisely how this occurs is unknown but is likely due to maturation of the HPG axis and establishment of central inhibition. Given the tightly constrained period of steroidogenesis, it is at first reasonable to assume that the decline in steroid levels is also the end of the critical period, but studies in rodents demonstrate this is not the case. Because females are not exposed to high levels of steroid they remain sensitive to exogenous treatment with a masculinizing dose. In rodents this sensitivity endures into the postnatal period but wanes after about the first week, with some variation depending on the endpoint. In humans it is often stated that the end of the critical period is prenatal, but as noted above there is some evidence for an impact of the minipuberty that occurs in the first few months of life. There also are no experiments administering male levels of hormone to females, for obvious reasons, and so we don’t really know when the critical period ends in humans. Also unknown has been the mechanism of how the sensitive period ends, but recent studies suggest an essential role for epigenetics (see below Section 11.5.6). 11.5 MECHANISMS OF STEROIDINDUCED MASCULINIZATION Identifying the mechanisms by which steroids exert an enduring effect on the developing brain requires first determining which steroid, and second what receptor the steroid is acting through. This might seem straightforward given that the testis produces testosterone and there is only one androgen receptor (AR). But studies in rodents have again demonstrated that the answer is not simple at all. 11.5.1 Steroids and Steroid Receptors Shortly after the establishment of the Organizational/ Activational hypothesis the animal model of choice switched from the Guinea pig to the laboratory rat for the simple reasons of larger litter size and shorter gestation. Fortuitously the critical period for masculinization also extends postnatally in the rat—not so true in the Guinea Pig. In the course of these early studies using postnatal androgen treatment of females, estradiol was included as a presumptive control steroid as it was considered a “female” hormone. But to the researchers’ surprise estradiol was not only capable of masculinizing the female brain; it was even more potent than testosterone. The discovery that the brain has high levels of the enzyme aromatase, which converts androgens into estrogens, was a key puzzle piece for solving the mystery and is codified as the Aromatization Hypothesis.20 But this raised another question, given the high levels of circulating estrogens in the bloodstream of the dam as well as the amniotic fluid, how do female pups avoid becoming masculinized? This mystery was solved with the discovery that alpha-fetoprotein, a steroid binding globulin with a high affinity for estradiol, is found at high levels in the fetal circulation during the critical period. Both male and female fetuses make alpha-fetoprotein and so both sequester the maternal estrogens in the bloodstream, depriving the steroid access to the brain. But the circulating testosterone in the fetal male has unfettered access to the brain, easily crossing the blood–brain barrier and then converted into estrogens once inside neurons. Thus only males experience elevated estrogens in the brain during the critical period. If alpha-fetoprotein is deleted, say by a genetic null mutation, then indeed all of the female offspring are masculinized.21 Estrogens, a class of compounds that includes estradiol, estriol, and estrone, bind to and activate multiple receptors. Two of these are canonical nuclear transcription factor receptors, ER-alpha and ER-beta, coded for by the Esr1 and Esr2 genes, respectively. Most of the signature endpoints subject to sexual differentiation (i.e., sexual and aggressive behavior, LH release) are mediated by ER-alpha but ER-beta has been specifically implicated in the defeminization process,22 although this process itself remains poorly understood. There are also rapidly mediated membrane effects of estrogens either through separate G-protein coupled receptors or the classic nuclear receptors. Even the ability to respond to estrogen with rapid effects is subject to sexual differentiation in the hippocampus.23 In primates the hormonal control of masculinization is slightly different. First, androgens rather than estrogens are the dominant masculinizing hormones.24 While there is ample aromatase in the brain of primates, including humans,25 all the evidence points to the AR and androgen levels as the principal directors of the masculinization process. This comes in part from experiments in nonhuman primates, but also from so-called naturally occurring experiments in humans. Most compelling are women with complete androgen insensitivity due to a mutated AR. These women are XY but phenotypically female with the exception of undescended testis, no uterus and a shortened vagina. They are also psychosexually differentiated as women with little to no evidence of gender identity dysphoria or homosexuality (reviewed in Ref. 26). This doesn’t mean there PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 11.5 Mechanisms of Steroid-Induced Masculinization is no role for estrogens in primate brain masculinization, and indeed there is some indirect evidence for that coming again from “natural experiments,” as in the case in men with mutated ER or aromatase genes. But these individuals are exceedingly rare in comparison to androgen-insensitive women and so the role of estrogen in masculinization of primate, including human, brain remains an open question. Because androgens are predominant in primates this causes some to question the utility of research on rodents for understanding the human condition in regards to sex differences. But steroids as a class are a different type of signaling molecule than most others in the brain. Steroids themselves do not alter neuronal activity or directly impact cell proliferation, survival, or differentiation. Instead steroids induce or repress other signaling molecules such as growth factors, receptors, neurochemicals, etc., and these are the agents that actually mediate change. So, the critical question is what are these signaling molecules and are they common to the process of masculinization across species but induced by a different steroid? i.e., estrogens in rodents and androgens in primates. Until we know those signaling molecules this question cannot be answered, but major advances on this front of late suggests that progress is beginning to be made. 135 11.5.2 Sex Differences in the Brain are Programmed by Steroids The proper formation of the brain involves a complex interplay of temporally orchestrated interdependent events. Cell proliferation, migration, differentiation, and integration are all regulated steps, as is neurochemical phenotype determination, dendritic and axonal growth and branching, and synaptogenesis. Cell death and synaptic pruning are essential for final maturation of neural circuits. Layered on these neuronal responses are the equally important and developmentally regulated astrocytes, oligodendrocytes, and the brain’s innate immune system cells, the microglia. Steroid hormones regulate every one of these processes and cell types at some point in time in some region of the developing brain, resulting in enduring sex differences in the neural landscape (Fig. 11.3). Bringing order to such a wide ranging panorama of events is found in unifying principles that act across multiple brain regions, but also by in depth analyses of tightly defined endpoints. Before discussing both some unifying principles and in depth analyses, it is worth the time to set the historical context. The earliest attempts to rigorously identify sex differences in the brain began with high resolution electron microscopy in rodents in the mid-1960s. The FIGURE 11.3 Multiple endpoints are modulated by steroids during development. The first mechanism identified as determining hormonally-mediated sexual differentiation was differential cell death in which males and females produce the same number of neurons in a particular subnucleus or brain region but more of them die in one sex versus the other. It is now known that many cellular endpoints are modulated by steroids developmentally, via unique mechanisms and in distinct brain regions at varying times but more often perinatally. Many of these changes involve epigenetic modification of the genome to both establish and maintain the sex difference. PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 136 11. MECHANISMS ESTABLISHING SEX DIFFERENCES IN BRAIN thinking at the time was that we had moved past the notion of a difference in the intelligence of men and women based on brain size or some other metric, and that if sex differences did exist they would be small and nuanced. And indeed two groups of researchers did find small and nuanced sex differences, some that only appeared in response to a lesion (reviewed in Ref. 14). Even this was big news at the time, resulting in publication in the highest caliber scientific journals. About 10 years later, a graduate student at Rockefeller University named Art Arnold contemplated the brains of canaries and zebra finches. Like most song birds, only the males of these species sing a complex and learned song. Arnold and his mentor, Fernando Nottebohm reasoned that there must be a brain region that controlled this singing ability and they predicted it would either be much smaller or not exist in female song birds. They were indeed correct and reported the first major sex difference in a vertebrate brain, so major in fact that it was visible to the naked eye once the brains were sectioned and stained for visualization. Legend has it that this report prompted another graduate student on the other side of the United States, Roger Gorski at UCLA, to step back and take a more distant view of the rodent brain, leading him to discover the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area (SDN-POA), or sexually dimorphic nucleus (SDN) for short (reviewed in Ref. 14). 11.5.3 Cell Death is Developmentally Regulated by Steroids in Brain Regions Controlling Reproduction The SDN is a small collection of nissl-dense cells and is 3–5 times larger in male compared to female rats. Because of the ease of quantification of the size of this nucleus (it’s really a subnucleus), the opportunity was presented to ask simple questions. Is it larger in males because more neurons are born there? Or because more neurons migrate to that spot? Perhaps more neurons differentiate into the phenotype in the male or they are larger or less densely packed? Or lastly, what seemed the least likely, is it because more neurons die in the female? As is so often the case, it was the least expected answer that turned out to be true. Males and females start life with the same rate of neuronal proliferation in the SDN, but in response to the elevated estradiol of the male more neurons survive. In the absence of the trophic actions of estradiol in females, most of the neurons die. The cell death occurs during the critical period and treatment of females with exogenous estradiol will save the neurons from death, but only if the treatment is during that period.27 This same principle was subsequently found to apply to a set of motor neurons in the spinal cord, a subdivision of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (pBNST) and another small subnucleus called the anteroventral periventricular region, or AVPV (reviewed in Ref. 28). Interestingly, however, the AVPV is reversed from the others in that rather than estrogens being prosurvival, there is an active initiation of cell death in the AVPV of males so that in the end the region is smaller than that of females.29 Each of these brain regions is essential to reproduction in some way. The SDN is embedded in the preoptic area (POA), which is the central node of the neural circuitry controlling male sexual behavior as well as maternal behavior. The pBNST is closely aligned with the POA and helps to integrate olfactory and social stimuli. The motor neurons of the spinal cord are in the spinal nucleus of the bulbocavernosus, or SNB, and control the penis. Lastly, the AVPV is critical to the generating of the LH surge in females that is requisite for ovulation. But a role in reproduction and a role for steroid-modulated cell death is where the commonality ends. For all of the nuclei but the SNB the principal hormone mediating cell death is estradiol, having a neuroprotective effect in the SDN and pBNST but a neurotoxic effect in the AVPV. The cellular pathways of estradiol’s effects are at times overlapping and at times unique per region and not completely understood. The BAX gene is critical to cell death in the AVPV, BNST, and SNB even though the direction of death is different in the two sexes as is the hormonal control.30 But within the AVPV there are two types of neurons that die in response to estradiol stimulation, GABA neurons and dopamine neurons, and here the cellular mechanism is different for each neuronal phenotype (Fig. 11.4). In the SNB, androgens mediate cell survival and a feedback from the muscles innervated by these motor neurons is a critical component (see for review Refs. 28,31,32). The end result of steroid hormone controlled cell death in each region is the creation of a volumetric sex difference, meaning the subnucleus is smaller in one sex versus the other due to more cells dying in that sex during the critical period. Because death is permanent, it was naturally assumed this established a permanent structural sex difference in the brain with no further modification. But this notion requires revision in light of the surprising observation that ongoing cell genesis, including neurogenesis, maintains the larger AVPV in females and SDN in males.33 This challenges both the contention that the only place in the brain adult neurogenesis occurs is in the hippocampus and subventricular zone, and the concept of rigid structural sex differences in the brain. Because the volumetric sex difference must be actively maintained it is also subject to modification if those factors maintaining it are disrupted. PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 11.5 Mechanisms of Steroid-Induced Masculinization FIGURE 11.4 One hormone, many effects. In rodents, estradiol (E2) derived from testicular androgens is the dominant steroid mediating masculinization. In one region, the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area (SDN-POA), E2 promotes cell survival but in the nearby anteroventral periventricular nucleus (AVPV), the same steroid induces cell death. At the same time, E2 stimulates neurogenesis in the male hippocampus and there is a lower rate of cell genesis in the amygdala of the same animals, resulting in females having a higher rate of cell proliferation. In each region the mechanism by which E2 exerts these divergent effects is distinct and in some cases not yet known. 11.5.4 Cell Birth is also Developmentally Regulated but in Brain Regions not Directly Associated with Reproduction For many years the control of cell death by steroids stood as the only clear mechanism by which a brain region was differentiated. This notion too, however, has required revising in light of observations of sex differences in cell genesis in the hippocampus and amygdala during the critical period (Fig. 11.4). Interestingly, both of these brain regions are more involved in cognitive and emotional responses than pure reproductive functions. In the neonatal rat hippocampus males have twice the rate of cell genesis as females as determined by injection of the cell birth marker BrdU, a thymidine analog that is incorporated into the DNA during cell division. When the fate of these cells is determined, the majority of the newly born cells that survive in males become neurons, up to 80%, whereas less than half do so in females.34 This is a rather remarkable sex difference and raises two interesting questions: (1) how is this sex difference 137 regulated and (2) what is the functional significance? The answer to the first question begins with exploration of the hormonal basis of the sex difference, an obvious place to start. Administering estradiol or testosterone to females increases their rate of cell genesis to that of males, and blocking estradiol synthesis or antagonizing the estrogen receptor reduces cell genesis in males to even below that of females.34 Thus it would seem this is another straightforward example of a hormonallymediated sex difference during the critical period. But there is one critical caveat, when the amount of estradiol in the hippocampus is measured in males and females at the time of the sex difference in cell genesis, there is no sex difference.35 And not only is there no sex difference, the amount of estradiol in the hippocampus is extremely low relative to other brain regions, including even the cortex. The same is true for testosterone and dihydrotestosterone.35 There also are no sex differences in the amount of estrogen or AR in the developing hippocampus. Thus the developing hippocampus appears to be exquisitely sensitive to steroids, yet steroids do not appear to be the driver of the sex difference. If steroid levels are not the primary source of the sex difference in cell genesis, there must be additional factors that either differentially regulate the sensitivity to the small amount of steroid that is present in male and female hippocampus, or a nonhormonal source that originates with the sex chromosomes. Candidates include genes on the X chromosome that escape inactivation and that could suppress neurogenesis in females, or genes on the Y chromosome that promote neurogenesis in males. Neither have been identified to-date. Alternatively, microRNAs that originate on the X chromosome could broadly modulate autosomal gene expression to modulate neurogenesis.36 Currently it is not known precisely how the sex difference in hippocampal neurogenesis during the critical period is regulated, and the potential for a combination of hormonal and genetic regulation, including epigenetic, is also possible. Understanding at the cellular and molecular level how this fundamental process is being regulated so differently in males and females has broad ranging implications for normal neural development and potentially for neuropsychiatric disorders in which the dysregulation of the hippocampus is strongly implicated. In the adult the hippocampus is key to two broad functional categories: learning and memory, and stress and anxiety. Anatomically the hippocampus is also broadly divided into ventral and dorsal subdivisions which are preferentially involved in learning versus stress responding.37 Both of these functional responses differ broadly in males and females and the sex difference in neurogenesis exists in both the dorsal and ventral regions. In rodent animal models the hippocampus is strongly PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 138 11. MECHANISMS ESTABLISHING SEX DIFFERENCES IN BRAIN associated with spatial learning as assessed by the Morris water maze or other food reward-based mazes. What was originally considered a superior performance by male animals is now understood to be an impact of context that differs for the two sexes. Specifically, if the conditions under which the animal is tested are stressful, females perform poorly, but if there is ample familiarization with the task before hand, there is either no sex difference or in some cases females perform better than males.38,39 Additionally, males and females attend to different contextual cues when navigating the maze.40 Males rely on geographic cues such as directionality (i.e., north vs south, right vs left), whereas females use local cues (swim in the direction of the object on the wall), an effect that has been generalized to humans.41 By manipulating the presence of geographic versus local cues, the relative performance of males and females changes. Why this is the case remains a mystery, but origins in the sex difference in neurogenesis during the critical period is one possibility. The hippocampus is also central to the negative feedback arm of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. Glucocorticoid receptors are heavily expressed by hippocampal neurons which transmit an inhibitory message to the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) to shut down the activation of the HPA axis following a stressful event.42 Many factors can modulate or impair the negative feedback loop, including sex and reproductive status. Just as with spatial learning, stress responding is highly contextualized and sex differences in the activation of the HPA axis depend on past experience and the type of stressor. Stage of life also seems to impact the response of each sex, with males being more susceptible to prenatal stress and females more susceptible to adolescent stressors (see for review Ref. 43). Again, why these differences exist is not known but it is an active area of investigation by laboratories around the world. Little consideration has been given to the idea that neonatal neurogenesis may be a contributing variable to sex differences in stress responding. The complex and context dependent nature of learning and stress response, and how they impact each other, makes understanding the role of developmental sex differences in those systems even more challenging. In brain regions controlling reproduction it is clear that developmental events are setting the stage for adult functions. But HPA axis modulation and hippocampaldependent spatial learning begin well before reproductive capacity is reached. Indeed about two weeks of age in the rodent is when both these functions come online.44 Cells that are born in the neonatal hippocampus require about two weeks to differentiate into neurons and begin to integrate into the neural network. Thus in males, in which substantially more neurons are born in the first few days, there would be many more immature FIGURE 11.5 Juvenile social play behavior is sexually differentiated. In all species which exhibit play behavior, characterized in rodents by chases, pins, pounces, and boxing, males engage with a higher frequency and intensity than females. The sex difference is robust and stable over many days. The medial amygdala is a key brain region controlling this sex difference in play which is organized by gonadal steroids perinatally but is expressed at a time in life when there are no circulating steroids, therefore the behavior is not activated as is the case of sexual, maternal, and aggressive behaviors. neurons integrating into the hippocampal network at two weeks of age compared to females in which the overall maturation would be greater. As a result, the way new information is processed and encoded into the developing brain may be quite different in males and females and have enduring consequences for adult functioning. The amygdala is a collection of nuclei with associated functions relevant to social behavior, emotionality, and fear-based learning.45–47 One particular social behavior that is controlled by the amygdala is displayed only during a restricted life period, juvenile play.48 In all species that play, which is the majority in mammals, males are observed to play with a higher physical intensity and frequency, leading to the term rough-and-tumble play.49 This robust and pan-species sex difference is particularly of interest because it occurs at a time in life when there are no circulating sex steroids (Fig. 11.5). However the propensity to play is programmed by steroids during the same critical period as all of the other organized sex differences identified to-date.50,51 The amygdala is often referred to as a sexually dimorphic brain region with reports of a larger size in males for specific subnuclei and the dendritic profile of neurons therein.52–55 There is also a sex difference in cell genesis in the neonatal amygdala only in this case more new cells are born in the female, and many of them will differentiate into astrocytes as well as neurons.56 Indirect evidence is consistent with this sex difference being hormonally determined, meaning higher androgens PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 11.5 Mechanisms of Steroid-Induced Masculinization in males suppresses cell proliferation in the amygdala. Intriguingly, the cell signaling pathway involves endocannabinoids,56 what some call the brain’s own marijuana.57 Endocannabinoids are membrane-derived locally acting modulators best known for their ability to inhibit GABA or glutamate release from presynaptic terminals.58 The two dominant endocannabinods are anandamide and 2-aceylglycerol, or 2AG and their concentration in the brain is tightly regulated by associated degradative enzymes. There are two receptors for endocannabinoids, CB1 and CB2, both G-protein coupled and widely distributed throughout the brain.59 There are no sex differences reported for CB1 or CB2 levels but developing males have a higher endocannabinoid tone in the amygdala during the first few days of life because of lower levels of the associated degradative enzymes.56 Administration of exogenous endocannabinoid agonists to females raises the tone and, surprisingly, reduces cell genesis to the level of males. How this occurs at the cellular level is not known but there is a correlation with the later appearance of juvenile play. Thus females in which neonatal cell genesis is suppressed by endocannabinoid treatment play like males as juveniles, meaning they exhibit more frequent physical encounters, such as pinning, boxing, pouncing, and chasing. At this time there is only a correlation between the sex difference in cell genesis and the sex difference in play, but both are modulated by endocannabinoids in a consistent direction.56 11.5.5 Nonneuronal Cells can be Key Contributors to Sexual Differentiation The POA is a small region just rostral to the hypothalamus and above the optic chiasm (hence its name as it is “pre” to the optic nerve). As already discussed, the SDN resides here and is one of the largest mammalian neuroanatomical sex differences (Fig. 11.6). In rats there is also a robust sex difference in the morphology of neurons in this region in the density of synapses on the dendrites, specifically spine synapses which are known to be excitatory glutamate synapses. On a given length of dendrite males have 2–3 times more spine synapses as females.60 This brain region is essential for expression of male sexual behavior in adulthood and the higher density of synapses endures across the life span. On the individual level, the density of spines is correlated with the intensity of male sexual behavior, further corroborating the importance of this developmentally organized and then hormonally activated synaptic profile to behavior.61 The sex difference in synaptic profile of the POA falls into the category of a classic hormonally mediated sexually differentiated endpoint in that treating newborn females with a masculinizing dose of estradiol during the critical period fully recapitulates the masculinization process. But if estradiol treatment occurs outside 139 FIGURE 11.6 The innate immune system mediates sexual differentiation. The preoptic area (POA) is the most important brain region for appropriate expression of adult male sexual behavior. The SDN is found here and on neurons found outside the SDN but in the POA the density of excitatory synapses is 2–3 times greater in males than females. The sex difference in synapses is determined by the inflammatory signaling molecule prostaglandin E2 (PGE2), which is found at higher levels in the male POA compared to the female. Microglia are the brains innate immune cells and derived from modified macrophages. Microglia display a range of morphologies and their shape is representative of their activational state. Ameboid-like microglia are more activated and both respond to and produce PGE2. This morphology of microglia is also more prevalent in the POA of newborn males and is the source of the higher PGE2 in their brains. Source: Modified from McCarthy MM, Pickett LA, VanRyzin JW, Kight KE. Surprising origins of sex differences in the brain. Horm Behav. 2015;76:3–10. the critical window, there is no change in the synaptic profile of the POA nor will the females behave as males in adulthood. None of this is particularly surprising but where the surprises come is in the mechanism of hormone action. Given the endpoint in question is the density of synapses a likely candidate for the target of hormone action would be a neurotransmitter or its associated receptors. But instead, the critical signaling molecule is prostaglandin E2 (PGE2), a membrane-derived signaling molecule that binds to G-protein coupled receptors broadly distributed throughout the brain. This is remarkably similar to the endocannabinoid system and they are indeed actually synthesized from the same lipid precursor.62 New born male rat pups have higher levels of PGE2, and higher levels of the synthesizing enzymes COX-1 and COX-2.63 If males are treated with the COX inhibitor, indomethacin, shortly after birth, they display little to no male sexual behavior as adults. Conversely, if females are treated with a masculinizing dose of estradiol both the level of PGE2 and the COX enzymes rise to the level PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 140 11. MECHANISMS ESTABLISHING SEX DIFFERENCES IN BRAIN of males and, more importantly, injecting PGE2 directly in the POA of newborn female rats is equally effective at masculinizing this brain region as peripheral estradiol injection.63 In fact a single injection of PGE2 is sufficient to permanently alter the synaptic profile and behavior in females.64 This raises the question of the source of the PGE2 and whether the single injection is actually initiating further prostaglandin production. The brain possesses an innate immune system consisting of modified macrophages called microglia, an unfortunate name given they have no real relation to glia and they aren’t all that micro. Microglia originate in the embryonic yolk sac and migrate into the CNS prior to the closing of the blood–brain barrier.65 They tile throughout the brain and lay down stem cells from which they continuously replenish.66 Until recently the primary function attributed to microglia was the removal of cellular debris following an injury that involved necrotic cell death.67,68 The primary signaling molecule for phagocytosis is ATP but microglia also respond to and make prostaglandins.69 Recently a more nuanced view of microglia has emerged, particularly regarding their role in development as sculptures of neural networks by regulating the pruning of synapses and the size of proliferative neuronal precursor pools.70–72 The morphology of microglia is an indicator of their activational state, which changes in response to local conditions. Ramified microglia have long branching processes with which they check on neighboring neurons. These are considered nonactivated surveying microglia.73,74 On the opposite end of the spectrum are ameboid shaped microglia which have a blebby appearance representative of their activated state following an insult (Fig. 11.6). Visualization of microglia is achieved by immunohistochemical detection of the cell specific protein Iba1 and the activational state categorized by the relative shape of the cell. This was done for the microglia of the developing POA in male and female rat pups and a striking sex difference discovered. In males the microglia were in an activated state while in the females they were overwhelmingly in the ramified surveying state. If females are masculinized by estradiol injection, so are their microglia. And, if females are treated with a single injection of PGE2 their microglia become activated and generate still more PGE2, thereby initiating the masculinization synaptogenicprocess.75,76 Thus microglia are intimate partners with neurons for building the neural circuit driving male sexual behavior. Connecting PGE2 to increased dendritic spine density requires first identifying the GPRC by which the PGE2 signal is transduced. There are four principle receptors for PGE2, EP1–4, but they are a promiscuous and facile group with much cross-talk.77 A combination of pharmacological and gene expression manipulation approaches converged on EP2 and EP4 as the key receptors,61 both of which are adenylate cyclase linked and lead to activation of protein kinase A.78 Specifically a form of PKA that is localized to the head and neck of dendritic spines where it regulates the phosphorylation of select subunits of the AMPA form of the glutamate receptor.64 Phosphorylation increases the trafficking of the AMPA receptor to the membranes of both neurons and astrocytes,79 maximizing responsiveness to glutamate release, which can be induced by PGE2.80 Thus the following sequence of events appears to occur in the POA of neonatal males. First, estradiol stimulates production of PGE2 by upregulating COX-1 and COX-2. This PGE2 then activates microglia which make still more PGE2, the combination of which leads to activation of PKA, phosphorylation of AMPA receptors which move to the membrane, and induction of glutamate release by neighboring astrocytes. The released glutamate activates the membrane clustered AMPA receptors leading to the formation and stabilization of dendritic spine synapses (reviewed in Ref. 36). So three cell types, one of which is nonneuronal, the immune microglia, are required for the masculinization of the developing brain. 11.5.6 Steroids Induce Epigenetic Changes to the DNA to Induce and Maintain Sex Differences Epigenetics literally means above the genome and refers to modifications to the DNA and associated histones that will change gene expression but not be heritable in the classic sense. Through this mechanism early life experiences, including in the womb, can imprint on the genome characteristics that will be beneficial to the predicted future environment.81 The two canonical forms of epigenetic change are direct modification of the DNA via methylation of cytosines proximal to guanines on the DNA strand, and decoration of the histones surrounding DNA with methyl, acetyl, or other modifying groups. In either case the access of transcription factors to gene promoters and enhancer sites is either facilitated or sterically hindered. The two forms of modification interact in complex and still poorly understood ways (reviewed in Refs. 82,83). The original invocation of epigenetics was as a means to explain how every cell in the body could contain the entire genome yet differentiate into and maintain a differentiated phenotype as a hepatocyte versus an islet cell and so on. Because the maintenance of cell fate is vitally important the epigenetic suppression of gene expression is strong and enduring. Consistent with this need, the covalent bond between the methyl group and 5’ carbon on cytosine is irreversible. This is proving not to be the case in the brain, however, where epigenetic regulation is far more dynamic than expected,84 including an PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 11.5 Mechanisms of Steroid-Induced Masculinization FIGURE 11.7 Epigenetic modifications mediate sexual differentiation. There are two canonical mechanisms for epigenetic modification of gene expression. Chromatin remodeling involves addition and subtraction of acetyl, methyl, and other groups to the histone tails associated with nucleosomes. These modifications are achieved by specific enzymes. Sex differences in the amount of changes to the histones suggest that enzymatic activity differs in some regions of male and female brains during development. Pharmacology inhibition of the enzymes confirms a functional role for these modifications by disrupting the normal sexual differentiation process. Epigenetic changes to the DNA involve the addition of a methyl group to cytosines proximal to guanines. This too is achieved enzymatically and once the methyl group is added it recruits methyl binding proteins (MBDs) which further suppress gene expression by sterically hindering the binding of transcription factors. Methylation patterns of various genes differ in specific brain regions of males and females. DNA methylation also recruits enzymes that modulate histones (HDACs) and is one way in which the two forms of epigenetic modifications interact. Source: Modified from Nugent BM, McCarthy MM. Epigenetic underpinnings of developmental sex differences in the brain. Neuroendocrinology 2011;93(3):150–158. enzymatic pathway for demethylation that was originally discovered as a component of DNA repair.85 The acetylation and methylation of histones are also actively regulated following novel experiences.86,87 Steroid receptors by their very nature as transcription factors interact directly with the DNA and some associated cofactors exert histone modifying activity,88 making them an obvious candidate as epigenetic modifiers. Both DNA methylation and histone modifications have been identified as contributors to the sexual differentiation process (Fig. 11.7). Sex differences in the amount of specific histone marks are found in specific brain regions,89,90 and inhibition of the enzymes that place acetyl groups on the lysine residues of histone tails during the critical period impairs masculinization of sexual behavior.91 The naturally occurring progression of cell death that leads to a sex difference in the size of the BNST is also modulated by histone acetylation, with the larger size in males 141 being prevented if histone deacetylating enzymes are blocked, which increases acetylation and suppression of gene expression during the critical period.92 This implies that in the normal course of events estrogen receptor and its associated cofactors directly deactylate genes critical for neuronal survival, thereby increasing expression of those genes. Direct changes to the DNA via methylation are also implicated in sexual differentiation of brain and behavior. Both isoforms of the estrogen receptor as well as the progesterone receptor show dynamic and hormonally modulated patterns of DNA methylation in their promoter regions.93 Dynamic in that sex differences and hormonal effects seen in newborns are no longer apparent but replaced by additional changes in juveniles. A still different pattern emerges in adults. Moreover, these changes are region-specific. In general, however, these changes do not seem directly related to expression levels of the receptors and may instead reflect past expression. Similarly, an in depth analysis of the methylation of the genome following neonatal hormone treatment found very few changes on the short term but dramatic changes in the adult,94 suggesting an epigenetic “echo.” The most causal connection between DNA methylation and the sexual differentiation process is found in the study of neonatal rats treated with inhibitors of the methyl transferase enzymes directly into the brain and the impact on adult neuranatomy and behavior assessed.2 Under normal conditions, females were found to have higher levels of overall DNA methylation in the POA and when this was reduced by pharmacological inhibition of the key enzymes, not only was the sex difference in DNA methylation eliminated, these females had a male-like synaptic pattern in the POA and exhibited male sexual behavior as adults. Analyses of gene expression revealed that many genes are epigenetically repressed in females during development and that a function of higher steroid levels in the neonatal male is to inhibit methylation of DNA and emancipate those genes to achieve masculinization of brain and behavior (Fig. 11.8). Moreover, the higher DNA methylation in females also determines the closing of the critical period as pharmacological inhibition of the key enzymes outside this time also induced masculinization, whereas administration of steroid did not. These observations demand a rethinking of the female developmental program as the default as there is clearly an active repression of the male developmental program. The process of masculinization also requires rethinking as one of the primary impacts of steroids is not to directly induce gene transcription but to instead broadly modify the genome via DNA methylation. An important consequence of this strategy is that DNA methylation can be reversed, either chemically or perhaps by physiology and experience. PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 142 11. MECHANISMS ESTABLISHING SEX DIFFERENCES IN BRAIN FIGURE 11.8 Sex differences in DNA methylation differentiate the POA and adult male sexual behavior. The enzymes that confer methyl groups to cytosine residues are more active in the newborn female POA and are suppressed by the higher estradiol found in the males. As a result there is more overall DNA methylation in females, leading to suppression of genes required for masculinization. In males, the DNA is demethylated following inhibition of the methylation enzymes (DNMT), allowing key masculinization genes to be expressed, leading to masculinization of the brain and adult male sexual behavior. 11.5.7 Steroids can also Act Rapidly to Induce Enduring Change in Neural Circuits Given the enduring nature of sexually differentiated traits and the canonical slow action of steroids it was naturally assumed that all hormonal effects in the developing brain required gene transcription and protein translation. But this assumption was overthrown by the observation that estradiol activates a signal transduction pathway in the hypothalamus within minutes. PI3 kinase in presynaptic terminals phosphorylates its dominant substrate, Akt, 30 min after estradiol administration. The activation of PI3 kinase requires the classic estrogen receptor be present in the presynaptic neuron, but presumably action is restricted to the membrane. Via a mechanism not yet known, activated PI3 kinase induces glutamate release which binds to postsynaptic glutamate receptors, activating MAP kinase and induction of dendritic branching.95 As a result the dendritic tree of neurons in the male hypothalamus are larger and more complex than that of females. Functionally, this neuroanatomical network change appears to underlie the process of defeminization in which the male brain loses the capacity to express female sexual behavior.96 Why the default feminization pathway needs to be removed in males remains a mystery but it clearly involves a separate mechanism from that of masculinization. 11.5.8 Steroids Indirectly Modulate Neuronal Excitability in the Developing Brain Neurotransmission as a target of steroid hormone modulation during development seemed obvious but largely failed to deliver as a mechanism for establishing sex differences. But steroids do modulate excitability during the critical period, and interestingly this is achieved in a manner similar to that noted above, through the modulation of kinases. The amino acid GABA is the dominant inhibitory neurotransmitter of the mature brain, so vitally important that even modest antagonism of its receptors can lead to seizures and ultimately death. But surprisingly, early in development GABA is the dominant source of excitation and is vitally important to the maturation of healthy neuronal networks.97 This chameleon change is achieved by reversing the concentration gradient of a single molecule, chloride.98 GABA-A receptors are chloride permeable ion channels which when open allow chloride to flux down its concentration gradient. In PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 11.5 Mechanisms of Steroid-Induced Masculinization 143 FIGURE 11.9 Steroids modulate excitation in developing neurons via depolarizing GABA. (A) GABA is an excitatory amino acid neurotransmitter. (B) The GABA-A receptor is a chloride permeable ionophore that allows chloride to flow into or out of the cell depending on driving force (a combination of concentration gradient and electric charge). (C) In immature neurons the intracellular chloride concentration is high because it is actively pumped into the cell by NKCC1. The activity of the pump is regulated by phosphorylation. Estradiol increases the activity of the pump by increasing the production of specific kinases (SPAK and OSR1) which phosphorylate the pump. (D) As a result, GABA is more excitatory in males than females and leads to a larger and longer lasting influx of calcium into the cell via L-type voltage-gated calcium channels. Over the course of development GABA gradually reverts to its mature inhibitory action but this progression takes longer in males than females. (E) The calcium influx induced by excitatory GABA activates many cellular processes, including the phosphorylation of the transcription factor CREB (pCREB). When the amount of cells expressing pCREB is quantified following administration of a GABA-A agonist, there is more in several regions of the male brain compared to the female. mature neurons chloride is maintained at low intracellular levels by an electroneutral pump called KCC2 for its ability to pump both potassium and chloride. When the channel opens chloride flows into the cell and hyperpolarizes the membrane, pushing it further away from the threshold for an action potential. In immature neurons, however, the opposite occurs. Intracellular chloride is maintained at a high concentration by a different pump called NKCC1 because it pumps sodium, potassium and chloride.99 In this case when the GABA receptor channel opens chloride rushes out of the cell, depolarizing the membrane which is also studded with voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCC). The L-type VGCC is particularly sensitive to low voltage changes and opens in response to the GABA-induced depolarization, allowing calcium into the cell and activation of a myriad of signal transduction and cellular processes that promote growth and maturation.100 Ultimately the neuron matures and the chloride potential is reversed to maintain hyperpolarization and inhibition.101 Steroids potently enhance the excitatory actions of GABA but not by a direct effect on neurotransmitter synthesis or receptor number or even predominantly by directly affecting the chloride pumps, although there are some sex differences in expression.102 Instead, estradiol upregulates two highly specific kinases, SPAK and OSR1, both of which phosphorylate NKCC1 to increase its activity.103 As a result, higher intracellular chloride is achieved and when the GABA-A receptor opens the magnitude of the transmembrane chloride concentration gradient is higher, leading to a stronger depolarization and an increased frequency and duration of L-type VGCC opening.104 Ultimately this means more calcium enters the cell and all the attendant processes that go with that are more intense (Fig. 11.9).105 Quantification of calcium influx in cultured hippocampal neurons finds PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 144 11. MECHANISMS ESTABLISHING SEX DIFFERENCES IN BRAIN more neurons that respond to GABA as depolarizing if they are pretreated with estradiol, and the peak of the calcium influx is greater and for a longer duration.106 In primates (Papio anubis), in utero estradiol is confirmed to enhance the excitatory actions of GABA,107 while in the rat model continuous treatment with estradiol maintains the depolarizing action of GABA, artificially extending the developmental period.108 This could have important consequences if neonates are exposed to estrogenic compounds in the environment. 11.5.9 Sex Chromosome Complement also Matters to Brain Development The emphasis on hormones as the sole source of sex differences in the brain held sway for over 30 years. Chromosome complement was considered only important as the determinant of gonadal differentiation and all else flowed from there. But it is undeniable that every cell in the brain has a sex, meaning it is either XX or XY, and that means there are some genes that are potentially expressed differently in males and females.109 In females one X chromosome is subject to inactivation in order to provide dosage compensation compared to males with a single X. Therefore only the Y would be at issue and there are overwhelmingly few Y chromosome specific genes and they are largely concerned with spermatogenesis. Thus it was easy to dismiss chromosome complement as a source of sex differences in the brain. But there were persistent instances in which hormones could not quite explain everything regarding sexual differentiation of brain and behavior, especially in birds (which have WW and WZ for male and female sex chromosomes) and this prompted Art Arnold, who made the first discovery of a major neuroanatomical sex difference in the brain in birds, to turn his attention to the role of genetic sex.110 The challenge was how to separate the impact of chromosome complement from gonadal phenotype and this is where the modifiability of mouse genetics provided a powerful tool. The Sry gene on the Y chromosome is essential for testis determination. By deleting the gene from the Y chromosome and reinserting it on an autosome, scientists were able to generate a mouse that had an XX chromosome compliment but developed testes. Likewise, the same manipulation led to the creation of XY mice that developed ovaries. By comparing XX and XY animals that both have ovaries or both have testis the impact of chromosome complement could be assessed independently of gonadal phenotype (Fig. 11.10). This is referred to as the 4-core genotype model.111 Early studies focused predominantly on reproductive endpoints such as mating and parenting behavior and the associated neural underpinnings but found little to no effect of chromosome complement. This was reassuring in that it confirmed 50 years of research about the importance of FIGURE 11.10 The 4-core genotype allows for separation of hormonal versus sex chromosome effects. Every cell in the brain of mammals is either XX or XY. In order to isolate the impact of that genetic difference from gonadal differentiation, a mouse line was generated in which the testis determining gene Sry was transferred to an autosome. This allows for the generation of XX animals with testis and XY animals with ovaries. By comparing XX to XX-sry the effects of hormones can be assessed independent of the sex chromosomes whereas comparison of XX-sry to XY or XX to XY-no Sry allows for assessing the impact of chromosome complement in animals with the same gonadal phenotype. Many effects of sex chromosome complement have been detected using this approach. hormones to sexual differentiation of brain and behavior. However, when the analyses extended outside the realm of reproduction, effects of chromosome complement were found on aggression, habit formation, feeding behavior, and so on.112 This is not to say that hormones have no effect on those endpoints and how they are organized developmentally, but that there is a contribution to the variability in responses between males and females that comes from the sex chromosomes. Once a role for sex chromosomes in sex differences in behavior was confirmed the race was on to identify the key genes. But this has turned out to be harder than expected. There are several ways in which chromosome compliment could impact the developing brain differently in males and females. First would be a gene on the Y chromosome outside the pseudoautosomal region which would only be expressed in males brains, but as noted above, other than Sry most of these genes are related to spermatogenesis. Second would be a gene on the X chromosome that escapes inactivation and therefore predicted to be at twice the level in females as males. The number of genes that escape inactivation has recently been found to be much more than originally thought: up to 15% in humans,113 and so this is a possibility. Moreover, the X chromosome is particularly enriched in genes associated with neural development and cognitive functioning. To-date no mammalian genes on the X chromosome that contribute to sex differences in brain development have been identified. The third possibility is that the presence of an inactivated X chromosome is not neutral. X inactivation is an energetically PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE REFERENCES expensive epigenetic process that involves heavy methylation of the DNA and associated changes to the chromatin in order to suppress expression.114 There is speculation that this process sequesters many of the key enzymes and other factors needed for epigenetic modifications of autosomes, thereby exerting wide ranging effects throughout the genome. This effect is referred to as a heterochromatin sink (reviewed in Ref. 115). Consistent with this interpretation is the increasingly deleterious consequence of each additional X chromosome in cases of sex chromosome aneuploidy such as XXY versus XXXY. However, multiple Y chromosomes also has deleterious consequences for brain development.116,117 Sex chromosome aneuploidies always have associated gonadal dysfunction, making it difficult to cleanly separate hormonal and genetic effects. Ongoing studies involving animal models in which these variables can be separated,118 as done for the 4-core genotype model, will advance our knowledge on this front. 11.6 ELUCIDATING MECHANISMS OF NORMAL BRAIN DEVELOPMENT IN MALES AND FEMALES PROVIDES CLUES TO SOURCES OF VULNERABILITY AND RESILIENCE Early life is not kind to males. Intrauterine mortality is so much greater than for females that a profoundly higher rate of conception of male fetuses converges to parity by birth.119 When male fetuses do survive they are more likely to have prenatal strokes, be born prematurely, suffer a birth injury, and fare far worse than females if one occurs. Neuropsychiatric disorders with origins in development exhibit a profound gender bias in prevalence and/or presentation. Boys are diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), attention deficit and hyperactivity disorders, and early onset schizophrenia at markedly higher rates than girls. Neurological disorders such as dyslexia, stuttering, and Tourette’s syndrome are 2–3 times more frequent in boys (reviewed in Refs. 81,120–125). Yet the biological basis for the fragility of the developing male is entirely unknown. It is tempting to speculate that the higher testosterone levels experienced by the developing and adult male are the source of the vulnerability, and indeed there is evidence in experimental models that androgens can increase neurotoxicity following an insult, but the same androgens can also be neuroprotective.126 The toxic versus protective effects are obviously mediated by different mechanisms but this highlights the importance of looking beyond the steroid itself to understand the actions it is exerting. Only by identifying the mechanism can we hope to understand how steroids might be contributing to sex differences in either vulnerability or resilience. 145 The potential of this approach is just beginning to be realized. The four- to five fold higher rates of diagnosis of ASDs in boys has proven maddeningly difficult to unravel. A recent, extensive transcriptomic analysis of postmortem tissue from autistic and nonaffected individuals was used to distinguish between two nonexclusive hypotheses: (1) risk genes for autism are expressed at higher levels in males, versus (2) genes involved in normal male brain development are more highly expressed in males with autism.127 The authors concluded the latter, that rather than autism risk genes being higher in males it is the genes normally involved in male brain development that were being overexpressed. More importantly, many of these genes were disproportionately associated with neuroinflammation. The authors were comfortable reaching that conclusion because of extensive corroboration from the basic science research on neuroinflammatory signaling molecules such as prostaglandins, and the key role played by microglia and astrocytes in the masculinization process in rodents, as reviewed above. Hopefully this is the first of many examples in which basic science in animal models will inform us as to the origins and potential treatments of human disorders that vary profoundly in frequency, severity, and presentation in boys versus girls and men versus women. References 1. McCarthy MM. Multifaceted origins of sex differences in the brain. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016;371(1688):20150106. 2. Nugent BM, Wright CL, Shetty AC, et al. Brain feminization requires active repression of masculinization via DNA methylation. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(5):690–697. 3. Joel D, Fausto-Sterling A. Beyond sex differences: new approaches for thinking about variation in brain structure and function. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016;371(1688):20150451. 4. Nelson RJ. An Introduction to Behavioral Endocrinology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates Inc; 1995. 5. Phoenix CH, Goy RW, Gerall AA, Young WC. Organizing action of prenatally administered testosterone proprionate on the tissues mediating mating behavior in the female guinea pig. Endocrinology. 1959;65:369–382. 6. Barraclough CA. Production of anovulatory, sterile rats by single injections of testosterone propionate. Endocrinology. 1961;68:62–67. 7. Barraclough CA, Gorski RA. Evidence that the hypothalamus is responsible for androgen-induced sterility in the female rat. Endocrinology. 1961;68:68–79. 8. Woolley CS, McEwen BS. Estradiol mediates fluctuation in hippocampal synapse density during the estrous cycle in the adult rat. J Neurosci. 1992;12(7):2549–2554. 9. Beery AK, Zucker I. Sex bias in neuroscience and biomedical research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;35:565–572. 10. McCarthy MM. Incorporating Sex as a Variable in Preclinical Neuropsychiatric Research. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(5):1016–1020. 11. Zucker I, Beery AK. Males still dominate animal studies. Nature. 2010;465(7299):690. 12. Klein SL, Schiebinger L, Stefanick ML, et al. Opinion: sex inclusion in basic research drives discovery. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. 2015;112(17):5257–5258. PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 146 11. MECHANISMS ESTABLISHING SEX DIFFERENCES IN BRAIN 13. McCarthy MM, Arnold AP, Ball GF, Blaustein JD, De Vries GJ. Sex differences in the brain: The not so inconvenient truth. J Neurosci. 2012;32(7):2241–2247. 14. McCarthy M, De Vries G, Forger N. Sexual differentiation of the brain: mode, mechanisms and meaning. In: Pfaff D, Arnold AP, Etgen AM, Fahrbach SE, Rubin RT, eds. Hormones, Brain and Behavior.. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2009:1707–1744. 15. Goodfellow PN, Lovell-Badge R. SRY and sex determination in mammals. Ann Rev Genet. 1993;27:71–92. 16. Sengpiel F, Kind PC. The role of activity in development of the visual system. Curr Biol. 2002;12(23):R818–R826. 17. Clarkson J, Herbison AE. Hypothalamic control of the male neonatal testosterone surge. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016;371(1688):20150115. 18. Kauffman AS. Coming of age in the kisspeptin era: sex differences, development, and puberty. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2010;324(1–2):51–63. 19. Kuiri-Hanninen T, Sankilampi U, Dunkel L. Activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis in infancy: minipuberty. Horm Res Paediatr. 2014;82(2):73–80. 20. Naftolin F. Brain aromatization of androgens. J Reprod Med. 1994;39(4):257–261. 21. Bakker J, De Mees C, Douhard Q, et al. Alpha-fetoprotein protects the developing female mouse brain from masculinization and defeminization by estrogens. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(2):220–226. 22. Kudwa AE, Michopoulos V, Gatewood JD, Rissman EF. Roles of estrogen receptors alpha and beta in differentiation of mouse sexual behavior. Neuroscience. 2006;138(3):921–928. 23. Meitzen J, Grove DD, Mermelstein PG. The organizational and aromatization hypotheses apply to rapid, nonclassical hormone action: neonatal masculinization eliminates rapid estradiol action in female hippocampal neurons. Endocrinology. 2012;153(10):4616–4621. 24. Wallen K, Baum MJ. Masculinization and defeminization in altricial and precocial mammals: Comparative aspects of steroid hormone action. In: Pfaff D, ed. Hormones Brain and Behavior. London: Academic Press; 2002:385–424. 25. Biegon A, Kim SW, Alexoff DL, et al. Unique distribution of aromatase in the human brain: in vivo studies with PET and [N-methyl-11C]vorozole. Synapse. 2010;64(11):801–807. 26. Meyer-Bahlburg HF. Sex steroids and variants of gender identity. Endocrinol Metab Clin North Am. 2013;42(3):435–452. 27. Davis EC, Popper P, Gorski RA. The role of apoptosis in sexual differentiation of the rat sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area. Brain Res. 1996;734:10–18. 28. Forger NG. Control of cell number in the sexually dimorphic brain and spinal cord. J Neuroendocrinol. 2009;21(4):393–399. 29. Waters EM, Simerly RB. Estrogen induces caspase-dependent cell death during hypothalamic development. J Neurosci. 2009;29(31):9714–9718. 30. Forger NG, Rosen GJ, Waters EM, Jacob D, Simerly RB, De Vries GJ. Deletion of Bax eliminates sex differences in the mouse forebrain. PNAS. 2004;101:13666–13671. 31. Sengelaub DR, Forger NG. The spinal nucleus of the bulbocavernosus: firsts in androgen-dependent neural sex differences. Horm Behav. 2008;53(5):596–612. 32. Forger NG. Cell death and sexual differentiation of the nervous system. Neuroscience. 2006;138(3):929–938. 33. Ahmed EI, Zehr JL, Schulz KM, Lorenz BH, Doncarlos LL, Sisk CL. Pubertal hormones modulate the addition of new cells to sexually dimorphic brain regions. Nat Neurosci. 2008. 34. Bowers JM, Waddell J, McCarthy MM. A developmental sex difference in hippocampal neurogenesis is mediated by endogenous oestradiol. Biol Sex Differ. 2010;1(1):8. 35. Konkle AT, McCarthy MM. Developmental time course of estradiol, testosterone, and dihydrotestosterone levels in 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. discrete regions of male and female rat brain. Endocrinology. 2011;152(1):223–235. McCarthy MM, Pickett LA, VanRyzin JW, Kight KE. Surprising origins of sex differences in the brain. Horm Behav. 2015;76:3–10. Wang ME, Fraize NP, Yin L, et al. Differential roles of the dorsal and ventral hippocampus in predator odor contextual fear conditioning. Hippocampus. 2013;23(6):451–466. Perrot-Sinal TS. Sex differences in performance in the Morris water maze and the effects of initial nonstationary hidden platform training. Behav Neurosci. 1996;110:1309–1320. Beiko J, Lander R, Hampson E, Boon F, Cain DP. Contribution of sex differences in the acute stress response to sex differnces in water maze performance. Behav Brain Res. 2004;151:239–253. Andreano JM, Cahill L. Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learn Mem. 2009;16(4):248–266. Driscoll I, Hamilton DA, Yeo RA, Brooks WM, Sutherland RJ. Virtual navigation in humans: the impact of age, sex, and hormones on place learning. Horm Behav. 2005;47(3):326–335. Jankord R, Herman JP. Limbic regulation of hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical function during acute and chronic stress. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1148:64–73. Bale TL, Epperson CN. Sex differences and stress across the lifespan. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(10):1413–1420. Foster JA, Burman MA. Evidence for hippocampus-dependent contextual learning at postnatal day 17 in the rat. Learn Mem. 2010;17(5):259–266. Pessoa L, Adolphs R. Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a 'low road' to 'many roads' of evaluating biological significance. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11(11):773–783. Roozendaal B, Koolhaas JM, Bohus B. The role of the central amygdala in stress and adaption. Acta Physiol Scand Suppl. 1997;640:51–54. Pitkanen A, Savander V, LeDoux JE. Organization of intraamygdaloid circuitries in the rat: an emerging framework for understanding functions of the amygdala. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20:517–523. Argue KJ, McCarthy MM. Characterization of juvenile play in rats: importance of sex of self and sex of partner. Biol Sex Differ. 2015;6:16. Auger AP, Olesen KM. Brain sex differences and the organisation of juvenile social play behaviour. J Neuroendocrinol. 2009;21(6):519–525. Meaney MJ, McEwen BS. Testosterone implants into the amygdala during the neonatal period masculinize the social play of juvenile female rats. Brain Res. 1986;398(2):324–328. Meaney MJ, Stewart J, Poulin P, McEwen BS. Sexual differentiation of social play in rat pups in mediated by the neonatal androgen-receptor system. Neuroendocrinology. 1983;37:85–90. Cooke BM, Stokas MR, Woolley CS. Morphological sex differences and laterality in the prepubertal medial amygdala. J Comp Neurol. 2007;501(6):904–915. Cooke BM, Woolley CS. Sexually dimorphic synaptic organization of the medial amygdala. J Neurosci. 2005;25(46):10759–10767. Cooke BM, Tabibnia G, Breedlove SM. A brain sexual dimorphism controlled by adult circulating androgens. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. 1999;96(13):7538–7540. Morris JA, Jordan CL, Breedlove SM. Sexual dimorphism in neuronal number of the posterodorsal medial amygdala is independent of circulating androgens and regional volume in adult rats. J Comp Neurol. 2008;506(5):851–859. Krebs-Kraft DL, Hill MN, Hillard CJ, McCarthy MM. Sex difference in cell proliferation in developing rat amygdala mediated by endocannabinoids has implications for social behavior. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. 2010;107(47):20535–20540. Nicoll RA, Alger BE. The brain's own marijuana. Sci Am. 2004;291(6):68–75. PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE REFERENCES 58. Alger BE. Retrograde signaling in the regulation of synaptic transmission: focus on endocannabinoids. Prog Neurobiol. 2002;68:247–286. 59. Alger BE. Endocannabinoids at the synapse a decade after the dies mirabilis (29 March 2001): what we still do not know. J Physiol. 2012;590(Pt 10):2203–2212. 60. Amateau SK, McCarthy MM. A novel mechanism of dendritic spine plasticity involving estradiol induction of prostglandin-E2. J Neurosci. 2002;22:8586–8596. 61. Wright CL, Burks SR, McCarthy MM. Identification of prostaglandin E2 receptors mediating perinatal masculinization of adult sex behavior and neuroanatomical correlates. Dev Neurobiol. 2008:68. 62. Nomura DK, Morrison BE, Blankman JL, et al. Endocannabinoid hydrolysis generates brain prostaglandins that promote neuroinflammation. Science. 2011;334(6057):809–813. 63. Amateau SK, McCarthy MM. Induction of PGE(2) by estradiol mediates developmental masculinization of sex behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(6):643–650. 64. Wright CL, McCarthy MM. Prostaglandin E2-induced masculinization of brain and behavior requires protein kinase A, AMPA/kainate, and metabotropic glutamate receptor signaling. J Neurosci. 2009;29(42):13274–13282. 65. Ginhoux F, Lim S, Hoeffel G, Low D, Huber T. Origin and differentiation of microglia. Front Cell Neurosci. 2013;7:45. 66. Elmore MR, Najafi AR, Koike MA, et al. Colony-stimulating factor 1 receptor signaling is necessary for microglia viability, unmasking a microglia progenitor cell in the adult brain. Neuron. 2014;82(2):380–397. 67. Neumann H, Kotter MR, Franklin RJ. Debris clearance by microglia: an essential link between degeneration and regeneration. Brain. 2009;132(Pt 2):288–295. 68. Streit WJ. Microglial response to brain injury: a brief synopsis. Toxicol Pathol. 2000;28(1):28–30. 69. Repovic P, Benveniste EN. Prostaglandin E2 is a novel inducer of oncostatin-M expression in macrophages and microglia. J Neurosci. 2002;22(13):5334–5343. 70. Cunningham CL, Martinez-Cerdeno V, Noctor SC. Microglia regulate the number of neural precursor cells in the developing cerebral cortex. J Neurosci. 2013;33(10):4216–4233. 71. Schafer DP, Lehrman EK, Kautzman AG, et al. Microglia sculpt postnatal neural circuits in an activity and complement-dependent manner. Neuron. 2012;74(4):691–705. 72. Sierra A, Encinas JM, Deudero JJ, et al. Microglia shape adult hippocampal neurogenesis through apoptosis-coupled phagocytosis. Cell Stem Cell. 2010;7(4):483–495. 73. Boche D, Perry VH, Nicoll JA. Review: activation patterns of microglia and their identification in the human brain. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol. 2013;39(1):3–18. 74. Nimmerjahn A, Kirchhoff F, Helmchen F. Resting microglial cells are highly dynamic surveillants of brain parenchyma in vivo. Science. 2005;308(5726):1314–1318. 75. Lenz KM, McCarthy MM. A starring role for microglia in brain sex differences. Neuroscientist. 2014. 76. Lenz KM, Nugent BM, Haliyur R, McCarthy MM. Microglia are essential to masculinization of brain and behavior. J Neurosci. 2013;33(7):2761–2772. 77. Sugimoto Y, Narumiya S. Prostaglandin E receptors. J Biol Chem. 2007;282(16):11613–11617. 78. Regan JW. EP2 and EP4 prostanoid receptor signaling. Life Sci. 2003;74:143–153. 79. Lenz KM, Wright CL, Martin RC, McCarthy MM. Prostaglandin E regulates AMPA receptor phosphorylation and promotes membrane insertion in preoptic area neurons and glia during sexual differentiation. PloS One. 2011;6(4):e18500. 147 80. Bezzi P, Carmignoto G, Pasti L, et al. Prostaglandins stimulate calcium-dependent glutamate release in astrocytes. Nature. 1998;391(6664):281–285. 81. Bale TL, Baram TZ, Brown AS, et al. Early life programming and neurodevelopmental disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68(4): 314–319. 82. McCarthy MM, Rissman EF. Epigenetics of reproduction. In: Zeleznik AJ, Tony MP, eds. Knobil & Neill's Physiology of Reproduction. 4th ed. London: Academic Press; 2014:2439–2501. 83. Nugent BM, McCarthy MM. Epigenetic underpinnings of developmental sex differences in the brain. Neuroendocrinology. 2011;93(3):150–158. 84. Sweatt JD. The emerging field of neuroepigenetics. Neuron. 2013;80(3):624–632. 85. Tan L, Shi YG. Tet family proteins and 5-hydroxymethylcytosine in development and disease. Development. 2012;139(11):1895–1902. 86. Gagnidze K, Weil ZM, Pfaff DW. Histone modifications proposed to regulate sexual differentiation of brain and behavior. BioEssays. 2010;32(11):932–939. 87. Bredy TW, Wu H, Crego C, Zellhoefer J, Sun YE, Barad M. Histone modifications around individual BDNF gene promoters in prefrontal cortex are associated with extinction of conditioned fear. Learn Mem. 2007;14(4):268–276. 88. Smith CL, O'Malley BW. Coregulator function: a key to understanding tissue specificity of selective receptor modulators. Endocr Rev. 2004;25(1):45–71. 89. Tsai HW, Grant PA, Rissman EF. Sex differences in histone modifications in the neonatal mouse brain. Epigenetics. 2009;4(1): 47–53. 90. Shen EY, Ahern TH, Cheung I, et al. Epigenetics and sex differences in the brain: A genome-wide comparison of histone-3 lysine-4 trimethylation (H3K4me3) in male and female mice. Exp Neurol. 2014. 91. Matsuda KI, Mori H, Nugent BM, Pfaff DW, McCarthy MM, Kawata M. Histone deacetylation during brain development is essential for permanent masculinization of sexual behavior. Endocrinology. 2011;152(7):2760–2767. 92. Murray EK, Hien A, de Vries GJ, Forger NG. Epigenetic control of sexual differentiation of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Endocrinology. 2009;150(9):4241–4247. 93. Schwarz JM, Nugent BM, McCarthy MM. Developmental and hormone-induced epigenetic changes to estrogen and progesterone receptor genes in brain are dynamic across the life span. Endocrinology. 2010;151(10):4871–4881. 94. Ghahramani NM, Ngun TC, Chen PY, et al. The effects of perinatal testosterone exposure on the DNA methylome of the mouse brain are late-emerging. Biol Sex Differ. 2014;5:8. 95. Schwarz JM, Liang S-L, Thompson SM, McCarthy MM. Estradiol induces hypothalamic dendritic spines by enhancing glutamate release: a mechanism for organizational sex differences. Neuron. 2008;58:584–598. 96. Schwarz JM, McCarthy MM. The role of neonatal NMDA receptor activation in defeminization and masculinization of sex behavior in the rat. Horm Behav. 2008;54:662–668. 97. Ben-Ari Y. Developing networks play a similar melody. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24(6):353–360. 98. Payne JA, Rivera C, Voipio J, Kaila K. Cation-chloride cotransporters in neuronal communication, development and trauma. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26(4):199–206. 99. Delpire E. Cation-Chloride Cotransporters in Neuronal Communication. News Physiol Sci. 2000;15:309–312. 100. Perrot-Sinal TS, Auger AP, McCarthy MM. Excitatory GABAinduced pCREB in developing brain is mediated by L-type Ca+ 2 channels and dependent on age, sex and brain region. Neuroscience. 2003;116:995–1003. PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE 148 11. MECHANISMS ESTABLISHING SEX DIFFERENCES IN BRAIN 101. Pfeffer CK, Stein V, Keating DJ, et al. NKCC1-dependent GABAergic excitation drives synaptic network maturation during early hippocampal development. J Neurosci. 2009;29(11): 3419–3430. 102. Perrot-Sinal TS, Sinal CJ, Reader JC, Speert DB, McCarthy MM. Sex difference in the chloride cotrasnporters NKCC1 and KCC2, in the developing hypothalamus. J Neuroendo. 2007;19:1–7. 103. Nugent BM, Valenzuela CV, Simons TJ. McCarthy MM. Kinases SPAK and OSR1 are upregulated by estradiol and activate NKCC1 in the developing hypothalamus. J Neurosci. 2012;32(2):593–598. 104. Nunez JL, McCarthy MM. Resting intracellular calcium concentration, depolarizing GABA and possible role of local estradiol synthesis in the developing male and female hippocampus. Neuroscience. 2008. 105. Auger AP, Perrot-Sinal TS, McCarthy MM. Excitatory versus inhibitory GABA as a divergence point in steroid-mediated sexual differentiation of the brain. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:8059–8064. 106. Perrot-Sinal TS, Davis AM, Gregerson KA, Kao JPY, McCarthy MM. Estradiol enhances excitatory gamma-aminobutyric acidmediated calcium signaling in neonatal hypothalamic neurons. Endocrinology. 2001;143:2238–2243. 107. Nunez JL, Aberdeen GW, Albrecht ED, McCarthy MM. Impact of estradiol on GABA- and glutamate-mediated calcium responses of fetal baboon (papio anubis) hippocampal and cortical neurons. Endocrinology. 2008. 108. Nunez JL, Bambrick LL, Krueger BK, McCarthy MM. Prolongation and enhancement of gamma-aminobutyric acid receptor mediated excitation by chronic treatment with estradiol in developing rat hippocampal neurons. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;21(12):3251–3261. 109. Arnold AP, Burgoyne PS. Are XX and XY brain cells intrinsically different? Trends Endocrinol Metab. 2004;15(1):6–11. 110. Arnold AP, Xu J, Grisham W, Chen X, Kim YH, Itoh Y. Minireview: sex chromosomes and brain sexual differentiation. Endocrinology. 2004;145(3):1057–1062. 111. De Vries GJ, Rissman EF, Simerly RB, et al. A model system for study of sex chromosome effects on sexually dimorphic neural and behavioral traits. J Neurosci. 2002;22(20):9005–9014. 112. Arnold AP. The end of gonad-centric sex determination in mammals. Trends Genet. 2012;28(2):55–61. 113. Berletch JB, Yang F, Disteche CM. Escape from X inactivation in mice and humans. Genome Biol. 2010;11(6):213. 114. Chang SC, Tucker T, Thorogood NP, Brown CJ. Mechanisms of X-chromosome inactivation. Front Biosci. 2006;11:852–866. 115. Arnold AP, Reue K, Eghbali M, et al. The importance of having two X chromosomes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016;371(1688):20150113. 116. Reardon PK, Clasen L, Giedd JN, et al. An Allometric Analysis of Sex and Sex Chromosome Dosage Effects on Subcortical Anatomy in Humans. J Neurosci. 2016;36(8):2438–2448. 117. Ellegood J, Anagnostou E, Babineau BA, et al. Clustering autism: using neuroanatomical differences in 26 mouse models to gain insight into the heterogeneity. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20(1):118–125. 118. Ngun TC, Ghahramani NM, Creek MM, et al. Feminized behavior and brain gene expression in a novel mouse model of Klinefelter Syndrome. Arch Sex Behav. 2014;43(6):1043–1057. 119. Naeye RL, Burt LS, Wright DL, Blanc WA, Tatter D. Neonatal mortality, the male disadvantage. Pediatrics. 1971;48:902–906. 120. Werling DM, Geschwind DH. Understanding sex bias in autism spectrum disorder. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(13):4868–4869. 121. Goldstein JM, Holsen L, Handa R, Tobet S. Fetal hormonal programming of sex differences in depression: linking women's mental health with sex differences in the brain across the lifespan. Front Neurosci. 2014;8:247. 122. Abel KM, Drake R, Goldstein JM. Sex differences in schizophrenia. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2010;22(5):417–428. 123. Bao AM, Swaab DF. Sex differences in the brain, behavior, and neuropsychiatric disorders. Neuroscientist. 2010;16(5):550–565. 124. Hill CA, Fitch RH. Sex differences in mechanisms and outcome of neonatal hypoxia-ischemia in rodent models: implications for sex-specific neuroprotection in clinical neonatal practice. Neurol Res Int. 2012;2012:867531. 125. Lauterbach MD, Raz S, Sander CJ. Neonatal hypoxia risk in preterm birth infants: the influence of sex and severity of respiratory distress on cognitive recovery. Neuropsychology. 2001;15(3): 411–420. 126. Zup SL, Madden AM. Gonadal hormone modulation of intracellular calcium as a mechanism of neuroprotection. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2016;42:40–52. 127. Werling DM, Parikshak NN, Geschwind DH. Gene expression in human brain implicates sexually dimorphic pathways in autism spectrum disorders. Nat Commun. 2016;7:10717. PRINCIPLES OF GENDER-SPECIFIC MEDICINE