ISSN 03621197, Human Physiology, 2015, Vol. 41, No. 1, pp. 11–21. © Pleiades Publishing, Inc., 2015. Original Russian Text © L.O. Tkacheva, I.A. Gorbunov, A.D. Nasledov, 2015, published in Fiziologiya Cheloveka, 2015, Vol. 41, No. 1, pp. 17–28. Reorganization of System Brain Activity while Understanding Visually Presented Texts with the Increasing Completeness of Information L. O. Tkacheva, I. A. Gorbunov, and A. D. Nasledov St. Petersburg State University, St. Petersburg, 199034 Russia; email: tkachewa.luba@gmail.com Received July 15, 2013 Abstract—The characteristic features of brain activity under variation of the information regimes associated with the stages of understanding visually presented texts in adult subjects (n = 148) have been studied. An original methodology for stepbystep presentation of texts has been developed in order to “detect” the psy chophysiological markers of different degrees of understanding and to identify the successive steps in this pro cess. The following three stages were determined by spectral analysis and calculation of the fractal dimension of EEG: “before understanding,” “understanding,” and “after understanding.” The phenomenon of V shaped changes in the values of EEG fractal dimension was discovered. The first stage is characterized by the increasing power of EEG spectrum in the high frequency range and by the enhanced fractal dimension of EEG, which probably reflects the process of idea generation and the search for solutions. At the next stage (“understanding”), the value of EEG fractal dimension decreases and the spectrum power of the lowfre quency range increases, which seems to reflect simplification of invariance to the only valid alternative. The last stage is characterized by an increasing power of EEG spectrum in the highfrequency range and the increasing value of EEG fractal dimension, which may reflect the intensification of brain activity related to solution validation. The findings are a step closer to understanding the neurophysiological mechanisms of system brain activity reorganization while making sense of visually presented texts; they outline the pathway to elimination of the contradictions existing in the literature as concerns the role of high and lowfrequency EEG components as markers of the state of understanding. Keywords: stages of making sense of the texts, psychosemantics, fractal dimension of EEG, EEG γ range, EEG θ range DOI: 10.1134/S0362119714060127 In this article, the term “understanding” means the stage of including new information into the current context. The major trends of research in the modern cognitive psychophysiology related to the process of understanding are the studies of: the cognitive con scious and unconscious [1, 2]; the recognition of weak, subthreshold stimuli [3, 4]; the characteristics of functional hemispheric asymmetry associated with creativity [5]; and the detection of network organiza tional patterns of electrical activity of the brain ensur ing the process of understanding [6, 7]. At the same time, some findings demonstrate that the process of understanding is accompanied by syn chronization of bioelectrical brain activity within the lowfrequency bands of EEG [11, 12]. It is undisputed that deep brain structures play the key role in the gen eration of slowfrequency cortical activity. To date, the role of hippocampus in consolidation of memory traces and its role as a selective input filter are unques tionable [13]. It is supposed that the lowfrequency θ oscillations serve for including new elements in the current information context and for intensifying the segregation between different memory elements [14]. Some of the studies show the role of θ activity in the processes of decoding semantic information [15, 16]. The effect of enhanced power of highfrequency rhythms was recorded in modeling the situation of cre ative task solution by many researchers under various experimental conditions [5, 8]. Description of the mechanisms of integrative brain activity that provided higher cognitive processes places emphasis on the process of neuronal synchronization in the γ band as a significant and functionally significant phe nomenon [9, 10]. Thus, in spite of extensive use of different EEG parameters as markers of the process of understanding the presented information, the role of high and low frequency bioelectrical activity of the cortex in the organization of systemic brain activity, which is per formed at different stages of information processing, is still unclear. The objective of our research was to com 11 12 TKACHEVA et al. pare the variations in EEG in different frequency bands of different cortical areas of cerebral hemi spheres when the subject understood the meaning of visually presented texts with increasing completeness of information. METHODS The basic experiment was preceded by a prepara tory step, which included expert assessment by two methods and development of a technology for step bystep presentation of the texts. Three main plotlines were obtained from 36 classical dramatic plots [17] by the first method of assessment: (1) adultery followed by revenge or aggression; (2) intellectual achievements as a result of riddle solution or cognition of something new; and (3) outrageous injustice followed by restora tion of justice. Six stimulus texts were selected as a result of the second method of assessment (two for each of the three plotlines). The factor of the influence of emotions was aligned due to variability of the plots in different emotional indicators (sign and modality). The selected texts were divided into eight fragments (presentations) in such a way that the first, second, and third presentation would contain 37, 46, and 55% of openclass words, respectively, and so on, up to the eighth presentation (100% of openclass words). Therefore, it was possible to control the gradual increase in the level of information clarity for the sub ject in relation to the type of plot. The time of presen tation for each fragment of each text was calculated in accordance with the number of words. We give frag ments 1, 4, and 7 of one of the texts as an example of stepbystep presentation of texts (see Appendix). The psychophysiological experiment was carried out in 148 subjects, 18 to 30 years old, 105 women and 43 men. All subjects were acquainted with experimen tal conditions and gave their informed written consent in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki. EEG was recorded using a Telepat 104P electroencephalo graph (bandwidth, 0.5–70 Hz) with a 250Hz sam pling rate for each channel. Nineteen monopolar leads were arranged symmetrically according to the Interna tional 10–20 System (Fp1, Fp2, Fp3, Fp4, F7, F8, С3, С4, Fz, Cz, Pz, Т3, Т4, Т5, Т6, Р3, Р4, О1, andО2). The aver aged potential of two ear clip electrodes was used as reference. EEG was recorded continuously, both in the background state (quiet wakefulness with the eyes closed) and when the subjects made tests on under standing and categorization of the texts (relating them to one of the plots). In the beginning of observation, EEG was recorded with the eyes closed and then with the eyes opened (1 min each). Then, a subject received the instructions (on a monitor screen and through speakers) explaining task sequence and ordering to understand the plot type as soon as possible. Then, six different texts were sequentially presented on screen display, each of them being divided into eight presentations with the increasing degree of information completeness (see the Appendix). The subject indicated his/her choice of the plot by pressing the button. It was not necessary to remember the types of plots; this infor mation was given in the lower part of the monitor screen at each presentation of the text. In conclusion, EEG was recorded again with the eyes closed and opened (1 min each). The correlation between different rhythmic com ponents of EEG was determined by calculating spec tral powers (Fourier transform) in WinEEG. The entire 15s EEG segment was analyzed, or a smaller segment if the subject pressed the button earlier, but no shorter than 4 s (the minimal window for implementa tion of the analysis). Spectral analysis was performed in separate 4s windows at a 2s pace (2s overlapping from 0 to 4, from 2 to 6, from 4 to 8, etc.). This was done to enhance the epoch analysis in the analyzed data segment in order to validate the calculations excluding artifacts. The mean spectral power values were calculated for each 4s fragment (for each sub ject). As a result of this transformation, EEG data are presented as total powers for the main spectral bands: Δ (1.5 to 3 Hz), θ (4 to 7.5 Hz), α (8.5 to 14 Hz), β1 (15 to 20 Hz), β2 (21 to 30 Hz), and γ (31 to 45 Hz). Then, the values of EEG fractal dimension (FD) were calculated for each subject, for each of the 19 leads, and for each state (“before understanding,” “understanding,” “after understanding”) via the loga rithms of power spectrum and frequency [18]. Factor analysis by the method of principal components (PC) was used to reduce the dimensions of data [19]. The method of PC revealed the most informative parame ters characterizing organization of the work of differ ent brain regions in the three states of understanding. For comparability of PC values in different states, the analysis was performed with the data where the repeated measurements of the leads (19 × 3 = 57 vari ables altogether) for the sample of N = 148 were pre sented as groups of observations. Thus, the analysis of PC was performed for 19 variables and 148 × 3 = 444 observations. The software for EEG data visualization (as corre lation pleiads and visualization of local distribution) was developed at the Laboratory of Psychophysiology, Chair of Medical Psychology, St. Petersburg State University. The influence of the factor of state (3 lev els: “before understanding,” “understanding,” “after understanding”) on the power values of Δ, θ, α, β1, β2, and γ activities of EEG for 19 leads was assessed by ANOVA with repeated measurements. The scheme in Figure 1 shows the algorithm that we used to compare the secondary EEG parameters recorded in the three pairs of states (“before under standing,” “understanding,” “after understanding”). The greatest changes in the process of understand ing were revealed by pairwise comparison of EEG data from the three stages according to the scheme pre sented in Figure 1. HUMAN PHYSIOLOGY Vol. 41 No. 1 2015 REORGANIZATION OF SYSTEM BRAIN ACTIVITY RESULTS In Figures 2–4, the upwardpointing triangle denotes statistically significant differences corre sponding to power increase in the first state (i.e., “before understanding”) relative to the second state (i.e., “understanding”). The increase in EEG power in the second state relative to the first one is denoted with a downwardpointing triangle. The triangle sizes and brightness (three grades) reflect the degrees of signifi cance of the detected difference. Statistically significant increase in the powers of Δ and θ activities was revealed at the instant of under standing the meaning of the texts relative to other two states (“before understanding” and “after understand ing”). Variations were recorded in the frontal areas, especially in the right hemisphere. The results of differences in the Δ, θ, and α bands are presented in Fig. 2. The maximum power in both Δ and θ EEG bands was recorded in the state of “understanding,” in rela tion both to the state “before understanding” and to the state “after understanding.” Significant changes in the spectrum power in the α band are observed in the occipital and frontal regions, also mainly in the right hemisphere. In the state of “understanding,” the sys tem brain activity is organized in such a way so that EEG power increases in the slowwave region of the spectrum: the Δ and θ bands. The tendency towards gradual increase in EEG power in highfrequency bands was observed in pro portion to the degree of understanding of the material and the amount of perceived information (see Fig. 3). Considerable increases in EEG power in the β1 and β2 bands were recorded in the state “understanding” relative to the state “before understanding” and in the state “after understanding” relative to the state “understanding.” These changes were observed not only in the anterior cerebral regions but also in the central and occipital brain regions, being particularly pronounced in the right hemisphere. The most gener alized changes in bioelectrical activity during the com parison of functional states of the brain before and after understanding were revealed in the γ band (see Fig. 3c). The results suggest that the changes in the system brain activity at an instant of understanding manifest themselves in local variations of EEG power within a range of 0.5–13 Hz mainly in the frontal regions, while the characteristics of brain activity in the process of understanding the texts manifest themselves mainly in generalized intensification of the power of highfre quency EEG spectrum. One can safely assume that the structure of interre gional relations undergoes qualitative changes in the process of understanding, which is accompanied by reduced redundancy in the system of spatiotemporal correlations between distantly disconnected cerebral structures, i.e., reduced chaoticity in organization of HUMAN PHYSIOLOGY Vol. 41 No. 1 2015 Before understanding 1 13 After understanding Understanding 2 3 Fig. 1. The sequence of comparisons of the pairs of states: 1, comparison of the states “before understanding” and “understanding”; 2, comparison of the states “before understanding” and “after understanding”; 3, comparison of the states “understanding” and “after understanding.” brain activity. It is well known that electrical brain activity recorded from head surface as EEG is an oscil lation process characterized by enhanced variability and nonstationarity and has the properties of chaotic and fractal dynamics [19]. EEG power spectra do not always adequately represent the changes in the func tional state of the brain, whereas the changes in multi fractal parameters are more informative [20]. An ade quate mathematical apparatus for quantitative assess ment of the lowering level of entropy is an increasingly widely used method for calculating the fractal dimen sion (FD) of EEG signal [21–23], which we have attempted to use in order to answer the following ques tion: Which of the three states of understanding (“before understanding,” “understanding,” or “after understanding”) is characterized by the highest degree of coordination of intercentral relations reflecting the higher level of organization of spatiotemporal correla tions and quantitatively represented by the lower val ues of EEG FD? The EEG FD value was calculated on the basis of 19 leads and three states (see Fig. 4). The comparison of characteristic features of bio electrical brain activity on the basis of EEG FD anal ysis has shown that the minimum level of chaos, i.e., the maximum level of certainty, is observed in the state of “understanding” (see Figs. 4a, 4b), while the states “before understanding” and “after understanding” are characterized by the higher level of uncertainty in organization of interregional relations. At the same time, it should be noted that the state “before under standing,” compared to the state “understanding,” is characterized by the high values of EEG FD mainly in the anterior central regions, especially in the right hemisphere. Organizational entropy of brain activity in the state “after understanding” is characterized by the generalized variation of EEG FD values, which encompasses nearly all areas except for posterior regions. It is interesting that the states “before under standing” and “after understanding” have the com mon properties with respect to the parameters of changes in the EEG FD values; therefore, no signifi cant differences have been revealed in any lead (see Fig. 4b). This means that these states have the higher level of uncertainty compared to the state “under standing.” 14 TKACHEVA et al. (a) 1 > 2 p < 0.001 2 > 1 p < 0.001 1 > 2 p < 0.01 2 > 1 p < 0.01 1 > 2 p < 0.05 2 > 1 p < 0.05 BeforeUnderstanding BeforeAfter UnderstandingAfter (b) 1 > 2 p < 0.001 2 > 1 p < 0.001 1 > 2 p < 0.01 2 > 1 p < 0.01 1 > 2 p < 0.05 2 > 1 p < 0.05 BeforeUnderstanding BeforeAfter UnderstandingAfter (c) 1 > 2 p < 0.001 2 > 1 p < 0.001 1 > 2 p < 0.01 2 > 1 p < 0.01 1 > 2 p < 0.05 2 > 1 p < 0.05 BeforeUnderstanding BeforeAfter UnderstandingAfter Fig. 2. Representation of the process of understanding in the variations of EEG power spectrum in lowfrequency bands: (a) Δ band; (b) θ band; and (c) α band. See the text for explanations. Two factors accounting for 74.47% of the total vari ance were obtained by the method of PC. The first fac tor, F1 (38.30% of total variance), represents the FD value for the following leads: O1, O2, P3, P4, T6, C4. The second factor, F2 (36.16% of the total variance), rep resents this value for the leads Fp1, Fp2, F3, Fz, F4, F7, and F8. Further, the values of the factors were calcu lated (by the regression technique) for 444 observa tions. The resultant two new variables were the calcu lated estimates of the factors designated as F1 and F2, respectively. Then, the calculated factors were recon structed as duplicate measurements for the three states and represented as 2 × 3 = 6 variables for N = 148 observations. These data were subjected to vari ance analysis with duplicate measurements: 3level factor “State” (“before understanding,” “understand ing,” “after understanding”) and dependent variables F1 and F2 (the calculated factors). The statistically significant main effect of the “State” factor (“before understanding,” “understanding,” “after understand ing”) (F (2, 296) = 10.96; p < 0.001; magnitude of the effect = 0.069; the observed power = 0.991) and statis tically significant orthogonal contrast (F (1, 148) = 13.05; p < 0.001; magnitude of the effect = 0.081; the observed power = 0.948) were revealed for the second factor (F2). Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for the paired differences (shifts) of values: the differences between the duplicate measurements of F2 in two states compared. The values are rounded to the third decimal place. Thus, the most informative changes in the system brain activity by the EEG FD parameters have been recorded in the frontocentral brain areas. Figure 5 shows the mean values of the factor F2. The mean values of F2 representing the total FD of EEG are plotted along the Y axis, and the three types HUMAN PHYSIOLOGY Vol. 41 No. 1 2015 REORGANIZATION OF SYSTEM BRAIN ACTIVITY 15 (a) 1 > 2 p < 0.001 2 > 1 p < 0.001 1 > 2 p < 0.01 2 > 1 p < 0.01 1 > 2 p < 0.05 2 > 1 p < 0.05 BeforeUnderstanding BeforeAfter UnderstandingAfter (b) 1 > 2 p < 0.001 2 > 1 p < 0.001 1 > 2 p < 0.01 2 > 1 p < 0.01 1 > 2 p < 0.05 2 > 1 p < 0.05 BeforeUnderstanding BeforeAfter UnderstandingAfter (c) 1 > 2 p < 0.001 2 > 1 p < 0.001 1 > 2 p < 0.01 2 > 1 p < 0.01 1 > 2 p < 0.05 2 > 1 p < 0.05 BeforeUnderstanding BeforeAfter UnderstandingAfter Fig. 3. Representation of the process of understanding in the variations of EEG power spectrum: in the (a) А band; (b) β2 band; and (c) γ band. See the text for explanations. of states while recognizing the type of plot (“before understanding,” “understanding,” “after understand ing”) are plotted along the X axis. The changes in the F2 value reflecting the EEG FD shift are statistically significant and take the form of a Vshaped curve. Hence, this value is relatively high at the stage “before understanding,” decreases abruptly at the stage “understanding,” and increases again at the stage “after understanding.” The background EEG was recorded in the beginning, the middle, and at the end of the session; however, the primary mea surements did not show any substantial change in the background. Significance of the revealed Vshaped change in the EEG FD value was verified by multivariate ANOVA with repeated measurements for all of the seven leads as dependent variables (Fp1, Fp2, F3, Fz, HUMAN PHYSIOLOGY Vol. 41 No. 1 2015 F4, F7, F8). Figure 6 shows the mean value of EEG FD for six leads before, during and after under standing. The value of the “State” effect for all leads (Pillai’s trace test) was 7.6%. The effect is statistically signifi cant for 6 out of 7 leads; the effect magnitudes for these six leads were 2.5% to 8.9% and, taking into account quadraticity of the effect, based on the criteria of intragroup quadratic contrasts, the effect magni tudes for separate leads were 4.7% to 12.6%. Thus, the analysis of characteristics of variation of EEG FD val ues in the six most informative EEG leads confirms the appropriateness of the revealed Vshaped distribu tion of the level of entropy characterizing brain activ ity in three states of understanding. 16 TKACHEVA et al. (a) (b) (c) BeforeUnderstanding BeforeAfter UnderstandingAfter 1 > 2 p < 0.001 2 > 1 p < 0.001 1 > 2 p < 0.01 2 > 1 p < 0.01 1 > 2 p < 0.05 2 > 1 p < 0.05 Fig. 4. The changes in EEG fractal dimension values by 19 leads in the three pairs of states. DISCUSSION The analysis of spectral power characteristics makes it possible to ascertain the specific activity of frontal brain regions at all stages of understanding. To date, it is indisputable that the prefrontal cortex plays a special role in formulating goals and objectives, developing plans and cognitive strategies, and assess ing actions as success or failure [24]. The data on the key role of the prefrontal cortex in recognition of frag mented images were obtained from the analysis of regional eventrelated potentials [25], while the model of realtime stimulus comparison showed the most marked involvement of the frontal cortex in working memory at the stage of comparing the current infor mation with the trace of the preceding stimulus [26]. At the initial stages of “information pumping,” the amount of perceivable information was not related to the level of certainty, since the level of clarity had remained obscure up to the third presentation of the text with the increase of information flow. Then, a cer tain critical point was reached at a certain instance of time, most often in the interval of the third to sixth presentation of the text, which was accompanied by the appearance of θ activity. It is noteworthy that the burst of electrical activity in the θrange (4 to 7.5 Hz) was recorded precisely at the instant of under standing and encompassed mainly the frontal and central leads of both hemispheres and the frontotem poral region of the right hemisphere (Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, Fz, F8). It was probably connected with the activity of limbic brain structures responsible for maintenance of voluntary attention and with the processes of informa tion comparison and consolidation in working mem ory [14]. The study of episodic memory has shown that brain activity in the theta range (5 to 9 Hz) may serve as a neuronal marker of selective search and retrieval of information [27]. It can be assumed that any problemsolving pro cess in the situation of uncertainty is related to the alternating predominance of high and lowfrequency activities representing the phases of increasing amount and crystallization of the sense of information. The analysis of connection between physiology and poten tial functional roles of the rhythms reveals the possibil ity of sequential transition of γ slots into θ cycles, and such variations and switching over of the rhythms are supposed to maintain different components of cogni tive act [28]. The alternation of synchronization and desynchronization phases in the θ and γ bands was 1 revealed in the study of formation of traces of episodic memory in patients during restorative therapy after neurosurgical intervention [29]. Interesting results were obtained from the study of interaction between phasic relationships during visual target detection, when obtaining a result is related to the appearance of a cross connection between low and highfrequency activities [30]; the development of cognitive strategy in the situation of uncertainty, when first the context of situation and only then the percept was processed, also showed the alternation of slow and highfrequency activities [31]. The following characteristics of the topography of functional connections by the θ rhythm were revealed: localization of interaction foci of in the frontal polar areas and integration of these frontal areas with the anteriorassociative and temporal Descriptive statistics for paired differences (shifts) in the values of factor F2 from one state of understanding to another (N = 148) Comparison of paired states Before understanding – Understanding Understanding – After understanding Before understanding – After understanding Mean shift 0.146 –0.048 0.098 Standard deviation of the shift N 0.422 0.355 0.404 148 148 148 95% confidence interval of the mean difference (shift) lower limit upper limit 0.078 –0.105 0.033 0.215 0.008 0.163 HUMAN PHYSIOLOGY Vol. 41 No. 1 2015 REORGANIZATION OF SYSTEM BRAIN ACTIVITY frFp1 frFp2 frF3 frFz frF4 frF8 0.10 1.91 0 –0.0167 –0.05 –0.0651 –0.10 In the process Before After understanding of understanding understanding Fig. 5. The Vshaped curve of the change in the total EEG fractal dimension (FD) value at three stages of under standing. There is a characteristic drop in the EEG FD values at the stage of “understanding.” regions of the left hemisphere, as well as with the pari etal and occipital regions of both hemispheres [32]. Investigation of the dynamics of interhemispheric interaction in the processes of selective coding of the memory revealed a similar neurophysiological mecha nism of alternating synchronization of bioelectrical brain activity at the level of θ and γ bands of EEG [33]. The recording of local field potentials showed that lowfrequency oscillations contributed to the appear ance of interzonal crosscorrelations and modulated the highfrequency γactivity within certain regions [34]. It is known that the oscillatory fluctuations of local field potentials in the θ and γ bands are involved not only in different parts of working memory but also in assessment of the novelty and sequence of events, induction of plasticity in the course of coding, as well as information consolidation and retrieval from mem ory [35–36]. The process of understanding seems to be crystallization of information at the level of semantic relations. Within the scope of psychosemantics it is assumed that more information can be perceived with more semantic relations actualized at a given moment [37]. Significant changes in the α band power were observed in the occipital and frontal lobes, especially in the right hemisphere (Fp1, Fp2, C4, T4, Pz, T6, O1, O2); in the state “before understanding,” the α rhythm power was higher in the temporal and central areas of the right hemisphere and lower in the frontal areas. It is in agreement with the study of the role of rhythmic synchronization in the α band for enhancing the accu racy and efficiency of communication between differ ent brain regions when providing active attention [38]. In the process of understanding, the EEG α band power considerably decreased in the occipital and Vol. 41 1.89 1.87 1.85 1.83 1.81 State HUMAN PHYSIOLOGY Mean EEG FD values by 6 leads Mean factor values F2 0.0818 0.05 17 No. 1 2015 Before understanding Understanding After understanding Fig. 6. Variations of the mean EEG fractal dimension (FD) values by 6 leads at the three stages of understanding. posttemporal areas. It seems that α rhythm may be considered in the context of its role in information processing, as well as in connection with the inhibitory function related to attention: suppression and choice [39]. In addition, the physiological role of α activity is known to provide pulse inhibition of reducing expen ditures on information processing for a potential area, accompanied by reduction of α activity and synchro nization in the γ band [40]. The study of verbal creative activity has shown that complication of the task may result in strengthening of the activation component representing more intensive use of brain resources and, accordingly, inversion of the dependence of cre ativity level on the level of cortical inactivation accom panied by EEG power attenuation in the α band, espe cially in the anterior and parietal cortex [41]. Regarding the variations of three states in the β1 band, we have observed the most marked changes in the state “after understanding.” This is most likely to be related to the situation of solution validation by a subject. It is interesting that the posttemporal lead Т6 is activated in all three states in the β1 band (p < 0.01), while the Pz lead is employed in the state “before understanding” (p < 0.01). It is known that the dorsal parietal cortex is involved mainly in the processing of spatial properties of external signals and is part of the visual spatial attention system [42]. Probably, the acti vation patterns that we have obtained are related to the specific features of experimental situation: the conflict of alternative solutions in a subject, which is in agreement with the results of choice situation modeling [43]. The data on the β2 band are comparable with the results of analysis of EEG coherence in the β band during verbal–mnestic activity. The maximum coher 18 TKACHEVA et al. ence values have been found for distant connections of posttemporal and parietal regions of the left hemi sphere with frontal areas of both hemispheres and with central areas of the right hemisphere [44]. We have observed the β2 band power increase in the process of understanding in the occipital, right posterior and anterior temporal leads, as well as in the left frontal lead (р < 0.01). This is in agreement with the results of the study of creative thinking, when fundamental dif ferences in spectral power were obtained in the high frequency band in the left parietal, right occipital, left and right temporal cortex [45]. In the state “after per ception” there occurs a significant power increase in the β2 range, mainly in the frontal areas; these changes are spread over the frontal, central, and even parietal leads but with the lower significance (p < 0.05). The dynamics of variations in the γ band activity in the three states demonstrates a gradual increase in γ activity in accordance with the amount of perceived and comprehended information. The most significant changes are recorded in the frontal, parietal and occipital areas, particularly in the right hemisphere (Fp1, Fp2, F7, F8, P4, T4, T6, O1, O2; p < 0.001). The state “after understanding,” compared to the state “understanding,” is characterized by an increase in γ rhythm power, especially in the right hemisphere. The enhancement of highfrequency rhythmic pat terns was also observed when modeling the situation of conscious and unconscious word processing [46]. A relationship between enhanced γ rhythm power and activation of the attention system was revealed [47]. Comparative study of changes in the spectral activity of EEG during the solution of verbal creative and non creative tasks showed significant enhancement of rhythm powers in the β2 and γ bands during the solu tion of creative tasks [48]. Summarizing the abovedescribed changes in orga nization of system brain activity demonstrated by the analysis of EEG parameters, we should emphasize a distinct tendency of significant enhancement of the power of slow rhythms in the Δ and θ bands at the instance of understanding the sense of the text relative to other two states. These changes mostly affect frontal areas, especially those of the right hemisphere. In highfrequency EEG bands (12–45 Hz), intensifica tion of the tendency towards an increase in rhythm frequency is accompanied by enhancement of EEG power in a subject making sense of the text. This pro cess encompasses both occipital and anterior brain regions. The results of EEG FD calculation for 19 leads and three states presented in Figures 5 and 6 are of partic ular interest: the maximum values were shown for “before understanding”; the values drastically decreased for “understanding”; and the values increased for “after understanding.” This dynamics probably reflects the stages of choice and decision making, as well as solution validation. The first state (“before understanding”) is characterized by intense searching for the solution and high information com plexity in brain activity; that is why the values of EEG fractal dimension are so high at this stage. It is known that the higher FD values correspond to the higher complexity of the system; hence, these values can be considered as a measure of stochasticity of the studied process [49, 50]. The state “understanding” is related to decision making and simplification of system com plexity to a single choice; therefore, at this stage we observe the lowest values of EEG FD. It has been shown that the values of EEG dimension recorded in healthy subjects in the state of rest are relatively low [51]. It is interesting that the state of rest and the state of “understanding” are characterized by comparative simplicity of brain activity. The state “after under standing” is characterized by higher values and enhanced information complexity of brain activity. This fact deserves particular attention. Apparently, when the text plot is understood by the subject, usually upon the fourth to sixth presentation (see the Appen dix), in the future, it should cause no increase in the EEG FD values. However, we can see a persistent trend of a significant increase in this value in all sub jects. It is most likely to be related to the specificity of experimental conditions: presentations of the text continue to the end (eight presentations) irrespective of the time of understanding; hence, the subject is in the situation of validating the correctness of his/her choice and begins to pay attention to additional infor mation that was excluded at the instance of under standing. Thus, in the scope of description of putative neuro physiological mechanisms of the process of under standing verbal information, we may assume the exist ence of different regimes in the integrative brain activ ity, which is in agreement with the modern concepts of the leveled spatial organization of intersystem interac tion [52] and network interactions underlying cogni tive processes [53]. The first regime is intended for sit uations of intense search for and generation of ideas and choosing the only correct solution among several alternatives followed by the validation of correctness of this solution; it manifests itself in the states “before understanding” and “after understanding.” This regime is accompanied by intensification of EEG power in the γ and β frequency bands, as well as in the frontal cortex of both hemispheres, especially the right hemisphere, and is accompanied by the increase in EEG FD values. The second information regime of brain activity is switched on in the state of “under standing”; it is characterized by enhanced EEG power in the lowfrequency θ band and, in addition, a decrease in EEG FD value. This regime is activated by the situation of “simplification” of a set of variants to the single alternative. Probably, the biological sense of this information regime consists in consolidation of information in the context of semantic relations. Thus, the process of understanding, as we see it, occurs at the lower disagreement between predictions HUMAN PHYSIOLOGY Vol. 41 No. 1 2015 REORGANIZATION OF SYSTEM BRAIN ACTIVITY and real events, which is reflected in the energetically more optimal state of nerve cells involved in this func tional system. CONCLUSIONS In all likelihood, the more complex is the problem, the larger group of nerve cells is needed for its solution and the most complicated is the observed dynamics of interaction between the elements. However, in our opinion, the disagreement between the conceptual schema existing in the brain (which describes the observed events) and the sensory flow coming in to the analyzers makes the main contribution to the dynam ics of interaction between nerve cells. If the scheme correctly describes the reality, the bioelectric dis charges of brain cells are synchronized, which pro vides more energyefficient existence of cells and reduces the complexity of their interaction. In EEG dynamics, this process is represented by the degree of complexity of a signal, which can be measured by calculating its EGG FD. In the terms conventional for EEG analysis, the increase and 1 decrease in complexity correspond to desynchroniza tion and synchronization, respectively. Probably, the process of complication of EEG dynamics, which reflects the contradiction between the conceptual schema and the real situation and is accompanied by enhanced dimensions of the functional system and employment of new cells for the latter, is not local; it affects a pretty large group of leads and entire func tional units of the brain. In our opinion, the decrease in EEG FD value reflects the decrease in disagreement 2 between conceptual schemas and real situation, which is represented at the physiological level by the modifi cation of synaptic connections in a given functional system, which corresponds to the conceptual schema optimal for the given situation. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This study was supported by the Russian Founda tion for the Humanities, project no. 130600637. APPENDIX A Sample of Methodology for StepbyStep Presentation of Text Text: “The Eagle and the Fox” (plot: injustice; experts' opinions coincided by 100%). 1. *** to settle close to each other, *** because of ***. *** high *** under shrubs below. *** into ***. *** came back, understood what ***, *** misera ble—not *** because *** left for her *** *** week could ***? *** pay for *** friendship *** brought *** to *** it to the *** the wind *** blew *** bright *** burnt *** not *** fox ***. 4. The eagle and the fox decided *** to settle close to each other, *** friendship *** because of ***. The eagle *** *** high tree, *** under shrubs below. *** HUMAN PHYSIOLOGY Vol. 41 No. 1 2015 19 the fox *** the prey, *** got hungry, *** into ***, *** with ***. *** came back, understood what ***, *** miserable—not only because *** cubs *** but rather *** not ***: *** could not ***. *** was left for her *** the offender: *** *** the week one ***? *** soon the eagle *** pay for *** friendship. *** offered *** field; *** to *** and *** burning entrails. *** soon as *** it to the nesting place, *** strong wind blew, *** bright *** burnt *** they *** not ***; and *** the fox *** them *** in the face of the eagle. 7. The eagle and the fox decided to be friends and *** settle close to each other, so that the friendship would be *** because of ***. The eagle nested on a tall tree, *** the fox gave birth to her cubs under shrubs below. But once the fox *** the prey, and the eagle got hungry, flew down into ***, seized *** cubs and gorged them with his eaglets. *** came back, under stood what had happened, *** was miserable—not only because her cubs were dead but rather because *** she could not revenge: she could not catch *** the bird. Everything that was left for her was *** the offender: what else the *** week one can do? But soon the eagle had to pay for *** friendship. Somebody offered a goat *** in the field; the eagle flew down to the sacrificial altar and brought the burning entrails from there. And as soon as he *** it to the nesting place, *** strong wind blew, *** thin old twigs *** with a bright flame. The burnt eaglets fell onto the ground—they *** not fly yet; and then the fox *** them all in the face of the eagle. REFERENCES 1. Allakhverdov, V.M., Agafonov, A.Yu., Vishnyakova, E.A., et al., Kognitivnaya logika soznatel’nogo i bessozna tel’nogo (Cognitive Logic of Conscious and Uncon scious), St. Petersburg: S.Peterb. Gos. Univ., 2006. 2. Dehaene, S. and Changeux, J.P., Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing, Neu ron, 2011, vol. 70, p. 200. 3. Marcel, A.J., Conscious and unconscious perception: experiments on visual masking and word recognition, Cogn. Psychol., 1983, vol. 15, p. 197. 4. Gaillard, R., Dehaene, S., Adam, C., et al., Converging intracranial markers of conscious access, PLoS Biol., 2009, vol. 7(3), p. 0472. 5. Razumnikova, O.M., Characteristics of information selection in the process of creative thinking, Zh. Vyssh. Shkoly Ekon., 2009, vol. 6, no. 3, p. 134. 6. Lou, H.C., Gross, J., BiermannRuben, K., et al., Coherence in consciousness: paralimbic gamma syn chrony of selfreference links conscious experiences, Hum. Brain Mapp., 2010, vol. 31, p. 185. 7. Lou, H.C., Joensson, M., and Kringelbach, M.L., Yoga lessons for consciousness research: a paralimbic net work balancing brain resource allocation, Front. Psy chol., 2011, vol. 2, article 366, p. 1. 8. Shemyakina, N.V. and Dan’ko, S.G., Changes in the power and coherence of the β2 EEG band in subjects performing creative tasks using emotionally significant 20 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. TKACHEVA et al. and emotionally neutral words, Hum. Physiol., 2007, vol. 33, no. 1, p. 20. Palva, S. and Palva, J.M., Discovering oscillatory inter action networks with M/EEG: challenges and break throughs, Trends Cogn. Sci., 2012, vol. 16, no. 4, p. 219. Power, J.D., Cohen, A.L., Nelson, S.M., et al., Func tional network organization of the human brain, Neu ron, 2011, vol. 72, p. 665. Bruno, M.A., Gosseries, O., Ledoux, D., et al., Assess ment of consciousness with electrophysiological and neurological imaging techniques, Curr. Opin. Crit. Care, 2011, vol. 17 (2), p. 146. Gray, J.A., The contents of consciousness: a neuropsy chological conjecture, Behav. Brain Sci., 1995, vol. 18, no. 4, p. 659. Rutishauser, U., Schuman, E.M., and Mamelak, A.N., Activity of human hippocampal and amygdal neurons during retrieval of declarative memories, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 2008, vol. 105, no. 1, p. 329. Freunberger, R., WerkleBergner, M., Griesmayr, B., et al., Brain oscillatory correlates of working memory constraints, Brain Res., 2011, vol. 1375, p. 93. Ghitza, O., The thetaSyllable: A unit of speech infor mation defined by cortical function, Front. Psychol., 2013, vol. 4, article 138, p. 1. Ghitza, O., On the role of thetadriven syllabic parsing in decoding speech: Intelligibility of speech with a manipulated modulation spectrum, Front. Psychol., 2012, vol. 3, article 238, p. 1. Lunacharskii, A.V., Iskusstvo i revolyutsiya. Sbornik statei (Art and Revolution: Collected Works), Moscow: Novaya Moskva, 1924. Vasserman, E.L., Kartashev, N.K., and Polonnikov, R.I., Fraktal’naya dinamika elektricheskoi aktivnosti mozga (Fractal Dynamics of Electric Brain Activity), St. Petersburg: Nauka, 2004. Nasledov, A.D., Matematicheskie metody psikholog icheskogo issledovaniya. Analiz i interpretatsiya dannykh (Mathematical Methods of Psychological Investiga tion: Data Analysis and Interpretation), St. Petersburg: Rech’, 2007. Nurujjaman, M., Narayanan, M., and Iyengar, S., Comparative study of nonlinear properties of EEG sig nals of normal persons and epileptic patients, Nonlin. Biomed. Phys., 2009, vol. 3, p. 6. Dik, O.E., Svyatogor, I.A., Ishinova, V.A., and Nozdrachev, A.D., Fractal characteristics of the func tional state of the brain in patients with anxiuos phobic disorders, Hum. Physiol., 2012, vol. 28, no. 3, p. 249. Cusenza, M., Fractal analysis of the EEG and clinical applications, Agostino ACCARDO Univ. Studi Trieste Anno Accad., 2012, p. 115. Bojic, T., Vuckovic, A., and Kalauzi, A., Modeling EEG fractal dimension changes in wake and drowsy states in humans—A preliminary study, J. Theor. Biol., 2010, no. 262, p. 214. Barbey, A.K., Colom, R., Solomon, J., et al., An inte grative architecture for general intelligence and execu tive function revealed by lesion mapping, Brain, 2012, vol. 135, p. 1154. 25. Farber, D.A. and Petrenko, N.E., Recognition of frag mented images and mechanisms of memory, Hum. Physiol., 2008, vol. 34, no. 1, p. 1. 26. Cornette, L., Dupont, P., Salmon, E., and Orban, G.A., The neural substrate of orientation working memory, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 2001, vol. 13, no. 6, p. 813. 27. Staudigl, T., Hanslmayr, S., and Buml, K.H.T., Theta oscillations reflect the dynamics of interference in epi sodic memory retrieval, Neuroscience, 2010, vol. 30 (34), p. 11356. 28. Kopell, N., Kramer, M.A., Malerba, P., and Whitting ton, M.A., Are different rhythms good for different functions?, Front. Hum. Neurosci., 2010, vol. 4, article 187, p. 1. 29. Burke, J.F., Zaghloul, K.A., and Jacobs, J., Synchro nous and asynchronous theta and gamma activity dur ing episodic memory formation, J. Neurosci., 2013, vol. 33 (1), p. 292. 30. Fiebelkorn, I.C., Snyder, A.C., Mercier, M.R., et al., Cortical crossfrequency coupling predicts perceptual outcomes, NeuroImage, 2013, vol. 69, p. 126. 31. Kveraga, K., Ghumanc, A.S., Kassam, K.S., et al., Early onset of neural synchronization in the contextual associations network, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 2011, vol. 108, no. 8, p. 3389. 32. Koshelkov, D.A. and Machinskaya, R.I., Functional coupling of cortical areas during problemsolving task: analysis of θ rhythm coherence, Hum. Physiol., 2010, vol. 36, no. 6, p. 665. 33. Osipova, D., Takashima, A., Oostenveld, R., et al., Theta and gamma oscillations predict encoding and retrieval of declarative memory, J. Neurosci., 2006, no. 12, vol. 26 (28), p. 7523. 34. Wang, L., Saalmann, Y.B., Pinsk, M.A., et al., Electro physiological lowfrequency coherence and crossfre quency coupling contribute to bold connectivity, Neu ron, 2012, vol. 76, p. 1010. 35. Duzel, E., Penny, W.D., and Burgess, N., Brain oscilla tions and memory, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 2010, vol. 20, p. 143. 36. Axmacher, N., Cohen, M.X., Fell, J., et al., Intracra nial EEG correlates of expectancy and memory forma tion in the human hippocampus and nucleus accum bens, Neuron, 2010, vol. 65, p. 541. 37. Petrenko, V.F., Psikhosemantika soznaniya (Psychose mantics of Conscience), Moscow: Mos. Gos. Univ., 1988. 38. Doesburg, S.M., Green, J.J., McDonald, J.J., and Ward, L.M., From local inhibition to longrange inte gration: A functional dissociation of alphaband syn chronization across cortical scales in visuospatial atten tion, Brain Res., 2009, vol. 1303, p. 97. 39. Klimesch, W., Alphaband oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information, Trends Cogn. Sci., 2012, vol. 16, no. 12, p. 606. 40. Jensen, O. and Mazaheri, A., Shaping functional archi tecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibi tion, Front. Hum. Neurosci., 2010, vol. 4, article 186, p. 1. 41. Razumnikova, O.M., Volf, N.V., and Tarasova, I.V., Strategy and results: sex differences in electrographic HUMAN PHYSIOLOGY Vol. 41 No. 1 2015 REORGANIZATION OF SYSTEM BRAIN ACTIVITY 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. correlates of verbal and figural creativity, Hum. Physiol., 2009, vol. 35, no. 3, p. 285. Ungerleider, L.G. and Haxby, J.V., “What” and “where” in the human brain, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 1994, vol. 4, no. 2, p. 157. Jensen, O. and Bonnefond, M., Prefrontal alpha and betaband oscillations are involved in rule selection, Trend Cogn. Sci., 2013, vol. 17, no. 1, p. 10. Tsitseroshin, M.N. and Shepoval’nikov, A.N., Stanov leniye integrativnoi funktsii mozga (Formation of Inte grative Brain Function), St. Petersburg, 2009. Shemyakina, N., Nagornova, Zh.V., and Ponomarev, V.A., Independent component analysis for creative versus noncreative task performance, in Advances in Biomed ical Research (Proc. Int. Conf. Medical Physiology), University of Cambridge, 2010, p. 222. Gaillard, R., Dehaene, S., Adam, C., et al., Converging intracranial markers of conscious access, PLoS Biol., 2009, vol. 7(3), p. 0472. Chalk, M., Herrero, J.L., Gieselmann, M.A., et al., Attention reduces stimulusdriven gamma frequency oscillations and spike field coherence in V1, Neuron, 2010, vol. 66, p. 114. SPELL: 1. desynchronization, 2. schemas HUMAN PHYSIOLOGY Vol. 41 No. 1 2015 21 48. Danko, S.G., Shemyakina, N.V., Nagornova, Zh.V., and Starchenko, M.G., Comparison of the effects of the subjective complexity and verbal creativity on EEG spectral power parameters, Hum. Physiol., 2009, vol. 35, no. 3, p. 381. 49. Mekler, A.A. and Bolotova, E.V., Agerelated changes in nonlinear dynamic characteristics of EEG, in Psil hologiya XXI veka (Psychology of the 21st Century) (Proc. Int. Conf.), Chesnokov, V.B., Ed., St. Peters burg: S.Peterb. Gos. Univ., 2005, p. 124. 50. Lutzenberger, W., Birbaumer, N., Elbert, T., et al., Dimensional analysis of the human EEG and intelli gence, Neurosci. Lett., 1992, vol. 143, p. 10. 51. Shen, Y., Olbrich, E., Achermann, P., and Meier, P.F., Dimensional complexity and spectral properties of the human sleep EEG, Clin. Neurophysiol., 2003, vol. 114, p. 199. 52. Siegel, M., Donner, T.H., and Engel, A.K., Spectral fingerprints of largescale neuronal interactions, Neu roscience, 2012, vol. 13, p. 121. Translated by E. Makeeva