Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 DOI 10.1007/s00426-013-0510-z ORIGINAL ARTICLE The role of temporal delay and repeated prospective memory cue exposure on the deactivation of completed intentions Moritz Walser • Franziska Plessow Thomas Goschke • Rico Fischer • Received: 28 February 2013 / Accepted: 24 July 2013 / Published online: 7 August 2013 Ó Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013 Abstract Previous studies have shown that completed prospective memory (PM) intentions entail aftereffects in terms of ongoing-task-performance decrements in trials containing repeated PM cues which previously served as PM cues triggering the intended action. Previous research reported that PM aftereffects decrease over time, thus revealing a specific time course of PM aftereffects. In the present study, we tested two accounts for this pattern, assuming either that the decline of aftereffects is related to the temporal distance to PM task completion or may be a result of the repeated exposure of repeated PM cues in the ongoing task. In three experiments, we manipulated both the temporal distance to PM task completion and the frequency of repeated PM cues and demonstrated that aftereffects of completed intentions declined with repeated exposure of formerly relevant PM cues. In addition, effects of repeated exposure were not only limited to the repetition of specific PM-cue exemplars but also generalized to other semantically related PM cues within the PM-cue category. Together, findings show that decreased aftereffects of completed intentions are not related to the temporal duration of the subsequent test block, but crucially depend on the repeated exposure of the previously relevant PM cues. Introduction The ability to form, maintain, and retrieve an intended action in a specific situation in the future, such as mailing a letter when coming across a mailbox, is known as eventM. Walser (&) F. Plessow T. Goschke R. Fischer Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, 01062 Dresden, Germany e-mail: walser@psychologie.tu-dresden.de 123 based prospective memory (PM) and is an essential ability for every day functioning. While many prospective memory studies investigated the involvement of maintenance and monitoring processes in performing a delayed intended action (e.g., Smith, 2003), previous studies also highlighted the role of a specific PM cue (e.g., mailbox) as a trigger signal for the retrieval of the intended action (Einstein et al. 2005; McDaniel & Einstein, 2000). The attractiveness of such a view lies in the contextual trigger condition that does not necessarily require an active and demanding monitoring mechanism. The influential multiprocess framework (McDaniel & Einstein, 2000), e.g., proposes that PM retrieval is supported by specific features of the PM cue. Salient and focal PM cues support a rather spontaneous retrieval of the intended action and increase the probability for successful PM performance. Non-salient and non-focal cues, on the other hand, require rather resource-demanding monitoring processes. Such a PM cuebased focus of successful delayed intention retrieval and PM performance, however, requires a strong association between the PM cue (e.g., mailbox) and the to-be-performed action (e.g., mail a letter) that is maintained during the retention interval until the required PM-cue event occurs and the intention will have been completed successfully (Einstein et al., 2005). Such a view is also in line with findings showing that not only the content (i.e., action plan) of the intention (Goschke & Kuhl, 1993) but also PM cues are stored in an increased sub threshold activation in long-term memory compared to other memory contents (Marsh, Hicks, & Watson, 2002). A strong reliance on environmental cues as trigger for action implementation, however, raises the question of the susceptibility to reoccurring PM cues after the intended action has been completed. The successful intention deactivation (e.g., deactivating the link between intention Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 and retrieval cue) plays an important role for every day functioning because a failure to deactivate completed intentions would be dysfunctional as it might provoke erroneous retrieval of the already completed intended action (i.e., commission errors) or interfere with subsequent tasks. Not surprisingly, researchers have started to investigate aftereffects of completed intentions and the particular role of repeated occurrence of PM cues when the link between the PM cue and the intended action has become irrelevant (e.g., Pink & Dodson, 2013; Scullin, Bugg, & McDaniel, 2012; Walser, Fischer, & Goschke, 2012; Walser, Goschke, & Fischer, 2013). Studies on PM deactivation Typical studies investigating the deactivation of completed intentions use classical laboratory PM paradigms (e.g., Einstein & McDaniel, 1990) in which participants perform a choice reaction time task, such as lexical-decision or number categorization tasks (ongoing task), and an additional PM task. The PM task requires participants to suspend the ongoing task and to press a different key in response to a PM cue such as a rarely appearing prespecified word or symbol (e.g., Scullin & Bugg, 2013; Walser et al., 2012). At the end of the PM task participants are instructed that the task has been completed and is of no more relevance. Subsequently, no-more-relevant PM cues from the previous PM task are embedded in an ongoing task as PMREPEATED trials. Aftereffects of completed intentions are measured as ongoing task performance differences on PMREPEATED trials compared to control trials (i.e., oddball trials). On the basis of such paradigms, many researchers found persisting effects of completed intentions, such as increased RTs and/or (commission) errors on PMREPEATED trials suggesting that performing and completing the intended action does not lead immediately to a complete deactivation of the link between PM cue and intended action (Beck, Ruge, Walser, & Goschke, 2013; Bugg, Scullin, & McDaniel, 2013; Pink & Dodson, 2013; Scullin et al., 2012; Scullin, Bugg, McDaniel, & Einstein, 2011; Scullin & Bugg, 2013; Walser et al., 2012, 2013). The idea here is that the intention representation (i.e., PM cue, intended action and/or the link between the PM cue and the intended action) remains in a heightened state of residual activation (Walser et al., 2012) and therefore, still triggers the old PM response on PMREPEATED trials resulting in response conflict with the ongoing task, and thus in increased response times (RTs) or error rates (PM aftereffects). Of note, given that intention representations comprise several distinct components, including representations of the intended goal, the to-be-performed action, 585 and its execution conditions or trigger cues, persisting postenactment activation could in principle refer to each of these components. While it is an interesting question, which particular components of completed intentions may persist in a state of increased activation, in the present paper, we focus on a different unresolved question, namely whether aftereffects of completed intentions gradually decline with increasing temporal distance to the completion of a PM task, or whether deactivation of completed intentions depends on the repeated exposure to repeated (no longer relevant) PM cues. Because the repeated activation of a no longer relevant action is often dysfunctional, the identification of conditions and factors that modulate or minimize PM aftereffects is of major interest to researchers in the prospective memory field. For example, several factors have been recognized to result in increased aftereffects of completed intentions: Strong PM-cue salience (Scullin et al., 2012), similarity between the PM task and the subsequent task in which aftereffects are measured (Scullin et al., 2012), impaired cognitive control ability (Scullin et al., 2011, 2012) as well as personality factors such as a tendency toward state orientation as compared to action orientation (Walser et al., 2013). Furthermore, it has also been shown that the extent of aftereffects of completed intentions can be modulated by either reflecting upon no-more-relevant PM cues after PM task completion (i.e., increased aftereffects) or by performing a cognitively demanding working memory task (i.e., reduced aftereffects) (Walser et al., 2013). Time course of PM aftereffects Regarding the modulation of PM aftereffects, inconsistent results have been reported with respect to the time course of PM aftereffects. For example an intention representation may gradually decay as a function of delay after intention completion due to the mere passage of time and/or interference from other memoranda (for this discussion on short-term memory see e.g., Berman, Jonides, & Lewis, 2009; Campoy, 2012). The idea of delay-dependent intention deactivation is supported by findings of similar forgetting curves for other memory contents (for a similar decay of PM performance over time see McBride, Beckner, & Abney, 2011; for an overview of retrospective memory forgetting see Rubin & Wenzel, 1996). Indeed, a gradual decrease of PM aftereffects over time was recently reported by Walser et al. (2012). Aftereffects were assessed within blocks containing six PMREPEATED trials (compared to six oddball trials). In four experiments, RT and/or error aftereffects were increased during the early first three PMREPEATED encounters as compared to the later last three PMREPEATED encounters (for a similar finding see Beck 123 586 et al., 2013). Alternatively, and in contrast to a temporal dependency, the reduction of intention aftereffects over time, however, might also result from PM-cue effectiveness washing out with increasing number of cue encounters. Since specific PMREPEATED trials are linked during intention formation and PM performance with the associated intended action, this specific PM cue-action link could be weakened when the specific PMREPEATED cue is repeatedly bound to a new action (i.e., the ongoing task). Evidence against a temporal dependency of PM aftereffects was provided in a study by Scullin and Bugg (2013) (see also Scullin et al., 2011). Scullin and Bugg (2013) used a between-subjects design and measured commission errors on a single PMREPEATED trial to investigate the effect of delay interval length after intention completion on intention aftereffects. Importantly, the probability of commission errors did not differ between short (40 trials) versus long delay intervals (258 trials). Our goal in the present study was to investigate why results from previous studies were apparently contradictory to each other by differentiating between the two competing explanations of delay versus repeated exposure on the degradation of aftereffects of completed intentions over time. Experiment 1 We adapted the paradigm of Walser et al. (2012) (see Fig. 1). During an initial PM block, participants performed a digit parity-judgment task as ongoing task. In addition, they had to respond on PM trials, which were two different PM cue exemplars of the same category (e.g., PM A cue = square, PM B cue = rhombus, PM-cue category = quadrangles), by pressing the X key. Following an instruction that the PM task had been completed, aftereffects of completed intentions were measured in two Fig. 1 Example trials of the prospective memory (PM) block and Test blocks 1 and 2 for Experiment 1. Participants were required to perform parity judgments on all trials except for PM A and PM B trials in which they had to press the X key. In Experiment 2, PM AREPEATED trials and oddball A trials did not occur in Test blocks 1 and 2. Note, framing of trial types were not present in the experiment but serve exclusively to illustrate different trial types in this figure 123 Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 subsequent test blocks (Test block 1 and Test block 2) in which participants had to perform the ongoing task during all trials. Whereas in Test block 1 only one exemplar served as repeated PM cue (PM AREPEATED), in Test block 2 both exemplars (PM AREPEATED and PM BREPEATED) were included. To calculate comparable aftereffects triggered by PM AREPEATED and PM BREPEATED cues, the oddball trials were categorically matched. That is, from an unrelated category (e.g., punctuation marks) one arbitrary exemplar served as oddball A (e.g., two question marks) and another as oddball B (e.g., two exclamation marks), respectively. This allowed us to calculate PM aftereffects for exemplars that served in both Test blocks (A-items; i.e., PM AREPEATED vs. oddball A) and PM aftereffects for exemplars that served exclusively in Test block 2 (B-items; i.e., PM BREPEATED vs. oddball B). Comparing PM aftereffects for A-items and B-items in Test block 2 enabled us to differentiate between the two hypotheses. If the temporal delay is of crucial importance for the decline of PM aftereffects, PM aftereffects in Test block 2 should be similarly reduced for A-items and B-items, because both item sets share the same temporal distance to PM task completion. If, on the other hand, PM aftereffects decline as a function of repeated exposure, PM aftereffects should be smaller for A-items that also occurred in Test block 1 compared to B-items that were only introduced in Test block 2. Method Participants Twenty-eight students of the Technische Universität Dresden (3 male, age M = 24.46 years, SD = 6.35) attended a single experimental session lasting for about 50 min. Participants had normal or corrected-to-normal sight and received 5 € or course credit. Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 Apparatus and stimuli The digits 2–9 served as stimuli (Arial font, visual angle 2.2° with a viewing distance of approximately 60 cm) and were presented in black against a gray background on a 19-inch monitor. One of two exemplar deviant stimuli that were members of one out of ten categories (i.e., two punctuation marks, circles, quadrangles, two letters, triangle, cylinder, mathematical symbols, one single currency symbol, in parenthesis, with an arrow) could appear around or next to the ongoing-task digits as PM A trials, PM B trials, or control trials (i.e., oddball A trials, oddball B trials) (visual angle 4.5°), whereby for each participant five categories were randomly drawn to serve as PM A/PM B trials and five categories to serve as oddball A/oddball B trials. Further 15 different deviant stimuli (e.g., @-symbols, a star) that accompanied ongoing-task digits were used as further control and/or irrelevant filler trials (i.e., oddball trials). Participants responded with the ‘‘,’’ key (right index finger), the ‘‘.’’ key (right middle finger) and the ‘‘X’’ key (left index finger) on a standard German (QWERTZ) keyboard. Procedure and design The experiment started with a practice block to familiarize the participants with the ongoing task, which required them to categorize digits according to parity with the right index finger for odd digits and the right middle finger for even digits. The practice block contained 16 standard and 8 symbolic deviant trials. After the initial practice block participants completed five cycles with each a PM block, Test block 1 and Test block 2 (Fig. 1). PM block At the beginning of the PM block participants received the PM task instruction to respond to members of a deviant PM-cue category (e.g., quadrangles) with the left index finger instead of performing the ongoing task. Of each PM-cue category two members were presented each four times as PM cues (e.g., PM A cue = square, PM B cue = rhombus). Additionally on four trials an irrelevant oddball appeared. The PM block contained 48 trials. Each trial started with a fixation cross (500 ms) followed by the imperative stimulus which remained until a response was given. If an incorrect or no response was given within 3,000 ms, a high-pitch tone (750 Hz) was given through headphones as feedback for 200 ms. At the end of the PM block an instruction was shown that the PM task was completed. Test block 1 Test block 1 (144 trials) started after a short break (20 s) with an instruction to perform the parityjudgment task on all trials. Only one of the two PM cues of 587 the previous PM block served as PMREPEATED trial in Test block 1 (PM AREPEATED). From an unrelated category (e.g., punctuation marks) a single item served as oddball (oddball A, e.g., two question marks). PM AREPEATED trials and oddball A trials occurred each six times. In addition, Test block 1 contained two further unrelated oddball trial types (each 6 trials) to ensure a similar amount of deviant trials in Test blocks 1 and 2. Test block 1 served to measure aftereffects on PM AREPEATED (compared to oddball A trials). Test block 2 Test block 2 (144 trials) started after a short break (10 s). Importantly, this block contained the same PM AREPEATED trials and oddball A trials as in Test block 1. In addition, it contained six PM BREPEATED trials (e.g., rhombus) and six oddball B trials (e.g., two exclamation marks) which served to measure aftereffects of the second category member (i.e., B-items). Each digit (2–9) was presented in random order 6 times in each PM block and 18 times in each Test block 1 and Test block 2, respectively. PM trials, PMREPEATED trials and oddball trials were randomly interspersed within blocks, the only constraint being that they could not appear during the first four trials of the PM block and Test blocks, respectively. The only variation of the experimental task over the five cycles was that different deviant stimulus categories were used in each cycle. Specifically, during each cycle one out of the ten deviant stimulus categories served as PM A/PM B trials and another as oddball A/oddball B trials. Results Error trials (6.2 %) and trials with RTs 2.5 standard deviations (SDs) above or below a participant’s mean RT for a given trial type (PM block: 2.7 %; Test block 1: 2.7 %; Test block 2: 2.4 %) were excluded prior to RT analyses. PM block Participants performed equally well on PM A trials and PM B trials, as shown by similar RTs, t(27) = -0.84, p = .411, d = -0.11; and error rates, t(27) = 0.63, p = .537, d = 0.10. Overall, responses were 303 ms slower, t(27) = 13.99, p \ .001, d = 2.50; and 18.1 % more erroneous, t(27) = 7.32, p \ .001, d = 1.68, on oddball trials compared to standard trials (Fig. 2; Table 1). Test block 1 We conducted repeated-measures ANOVAs with the factor trial type (standard, oddball A, PM AREPEATED) on RTs and error rates of the ongoing task. For RT analysis, trial type 123 588 Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 Fig. 2 Results for Experiments 1 and 2. Mean response times (RT) and error rates for the PM block in PM A and PM B trials, for Test block 1 (oddball A, PM AREPEATED) and Test block 2 (oddball A, PM AREPEATED, oddball B, PM BREPEATED) as a function of trial type. Error bars represent standard errors Table 1 Mean RTs and error rates for the PM block, Test block 1 and Test block 2 by trial type in Experiments 1 and 2 (standard deviations in parentheses) Experiment 1 Experiment 2 RT (ms) Error (%) RT (ms) Error (%) Standard 583 (71) 6.2 (3.9) 593 (79) 5.5 (3.5) Oddball 886 (156) 24.3 (14.7) 921 (172) 15.3 (13.6) PM A 597 (66) 7.0 (7.9) 647 (91) 7.8 (6.6) PM B 605 (83) 6.3 (6.6) 629 (87) 9.1 (10.2) 546 (75) 5.3 (3.6) PM block Test block 1 Standard 534 (57) 5.5 (3.6) Oddball A PM AREPEATED 589 (88) 617 (92) 5.2 (4.0) 6.4 (6.2) Most important, we found aftereffects of completed intentions (M = 28 ms), as shown by slower RTs on PM AREPEATED trials than on oddball A trials, F(1, 27) = 15.41, p = .001, g2 = .36. To test for a decrease of aftereffects as a function of cue repetitions within Test block 1, we compared aftereffects between the first three encounters and last three encounters of PM AREPEATED trials (for a similar analysis see also Walser et al., 2012). We found aftereffects for early encounters (M = 60 ms, t[27] = 6.00, p \ .001, d = 0.58) but not for late encounters (M = 0 ms, t[27] = -0.03, p = .973, d = 0.00), as indicated by a Trial type 9 Block position interaction, F(1, 27) = 15.59, p = .001, g2 = .37. Test block 2 Test block 2 Standard 525 (56) 6.1 (3.9) Oddball A 547 (72) 6.7 (5.9) 548 (71) 5.9 (3.4) PM AREPEATED 549 (60) 6.1 (5.2) Oddball B 564 (81) PM BREPEATED 567 (85) 7.0 (5.9) 602 (90) 6.0 (6.2) 6.6 (5.9) 646 (120) 7.7 (6.2) reached significance, F(2, 54) = 50.87, p \ .001, g2 = .65. Repeated contrasts revealed an orientation response (55 ms) on oddball A trials in terms of slower RTs than on standard trials, F(1, 27) = 38.00, p \ .001, g2 = .56. At the same time, it should be noted that RTs on the two further oddball trial types, which served exclusively as filler trials to ensure the same ratio of standard and deviant stimuli during Test block 1 and Test block 2, did not differ from RTs on oddball A trials, Fs \ 1 (planned contrasts). 123 We compared aftereffects of completed intentions in Test block 2 by computing 2 (trial type: PMREPEATED, oddball) 9 2 (item set: A-items, B-items) repeated-measures ANOVAs on RTs and error rates. For RTs the factor item set reached significance, F(1, 27) = 7.38, p = .011, g2 = .21, indicating smaller overall RTs for A-items (M = 548 ms), that also appeared in Test block 1, compared to B-items (M = 565 ms) that were only presented in Test block 2. The factor trial type and the Trial type 9 Item set interaction did not reach significance, Fs \ 1, indicating no aftereffects and hence complete intention deactivation for both, A-items (M = 2 ms) and B-items (M = 3 ms), respectively. Further analyses To disregard explanations on the basis of the use of repeated cycles of PM and Test blocks in the present Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 design,1 in a subsequent step, we repeated the ANOVAs using only the very first cycle of Test block 1 and Test block 2. Most importantly and in line with the analysis including all cycles, we found aftereffects for A-items in Test block 1 (M = 42 ms; PM AREPEATED trials: M = 651 ms, SD = 122 ms; oddball A trials: M = 609 ms, SD = 105 ms), F(1, 27) = 7.17, p = .012, g2 = .21. In Test block 2, no PM aftereffects were found for A-items (M = 0 ms; PM AREPEATED trials: M = 553 ms, SD = 91 ms; oddball A trials: M = 553 ms, SD = 91 ms) nor for B-items (M = -6 ms; PM BREPEATED trials: M = 592 ms, SD = 130 ms; oddball B trials: M = 598 ms, SD = 159 ms), Fs \ 1. Further analyses revealed that for A-items during Test blocks 1 and 2 and for B-items during Test block 2 aftereffects did not vary over the course of the experimental session, as no Trial type 9 Repeated cycles interactions were found, Fs \ 1. An additional RT analysis on PM trials (PM A and PM B together) as a function of repeated cycle was significant, F(4, 108) = 8.65, p \ .001, g2 = .24. Repeated contrast showed that RTs were increased in the first cycle (M = 653 ms, SD = 93 ms) compared to second cycle (M = 594 ms, SD = 75 ms), F(1, 27) = 14.90, p \ .001, g2 = .36; but did not differ between subsequent cycles, Fs \ 1. In sum, these analyses revealed that repeating cycles led to faster responses on PM trials, whereas they did not affect aftereffects of completed intentions. To disregard that missing aftereffects in Test block 2 were due to a complete shielding of deviant stimulus information, we computed repeated-measures ANOVAs with the factor trial type (standard, oddball A, oddball B) on RTs and error rates, which reached significance, F(2, 54) = 12.42, p \ .001, g2 = .32. Planned contrasts revealed shorter RTs on standard trials than on oddball A trials, F(1, 27) = 12.54, p = .001, g2 = .32, indicating an orientation reaction on deviant stimuli and thus that participants could not ignore deviant stimuli completely during Test block 2. There was a tendency—albeit not significant—toward increased RTs on oddball B compared to oddball A trials, F(1, 27) = 3.25, p = .083, g2 = .10. Comparison of PM aftereffects in Test block 1 and Test block 2 (A-items) We conducted 2 (trial type: PM AREPEATED, oddball A) 9 2 (Test block: 1, 2) repeated-measures ANOVAs on 589 RTs and error rates to analyze the fade of aftereffects for A-items between Test blocks 1 and 2. Overall RTs on PM AREPEATED trials (M = 583 ms) were increased compared to oddball A trials (M = 568 ms), F(1, 27) = 12.80, p = .001, g2 = .32. Overall RTs were slower in Test block 1 (M = 603 ms) than in Test block 2 (M = 548 ms), F(1, 27) = 33.31, p \ .001, g2 = .55. Most important, the Trial type 9 Test block interaction was significant, F(1, 27) = 5.32, p = .029, g2 = .17, indicating that PM aftereffects for A-items decreased from Test block 1 to Test block 2. As hardly any commission errors (0.02 %) were made we only computed an overall error analysis. For all corresponding analyses on error rates of Test blocks 1 and 2, however, no significant effects were found, all ps [ .268. Discussion In Experiment 1, category members of PM cues that were presented as PMREPEATED cues in Test block 1 and Test block 2 (A-items) revealed PM aftereffects only in Test block 1 but not in the subsequent Test block 2. Category members of PM cues that were presented as PMREPEATED cues exclusively in Test block 2 (B-items) did also not show PM aftereffects in Test block 2. Although these results seem to suggest that sufficient temporal delay between intention completion and measurement of PM aftereffects determines the decrease of PM aftereffects, such an interpretation has to be handled with care, because two alternative interpretations need to be considered: First, whereas PM BREPEATED served as PM B cues in the PM block, oddball B cues in the Test block 2 were never presented before. Therefore, one could argue that the novelty of a first-time presentation of oddball B trials in Test block 2 resulted in a stronger orientation response (i.e., slowed RTs to oddball B trials) and thus, eliminated the PM aftereffect for B-items. To disregard this possibility, we conducted a control replication experiment in which oddball A trials and oddball B trials were presented already in the PM block. Results were virtually identical to Experiment 1. Most importantly, in Test block 2 no PM aftereffects were found for B-items.2 Second, effects of repeated exposure of PM AREPEATED trials in Test block 1 may have transferred to PM 2 1 The repeated cycles of PM and Test blocks differ to other approaches such as single-cycle paradigms (e.g., Scullin & Bugg, 2013). One could argue, e.g., that using repeated cycles, PM aftereffects may be overestimated when participants are ambiguous whether the intention has really finished and persist monitoring (Walser et al., 2012). In contrast, aftereffects may also be underestimated, because over the course of the experimental session, participants might learn to increasingly shield the ongoing task from deviant PMREPEATED and oddball stimuli during the Test blocks. Sixteen participants participated in the experiment (1 male, age M = 22.56 years, SD = 4.08). In Test block 1, RTs on PM AREPEATED trials (M = 609 ms, SD = 121 ms) were slower than on oddball A trials (M = 550 ms, SD = 89 ms), F(1, 15) = 28.65, p \ .001, g2 = .66, indicating aftereffects of completed intentions (M = 59 ms). In Test block 2, no aftereffects were found for A-items (6 ms, PM AREPEATED trials: M = 553 ms, SD = 88 ms; oddball A: M = 547 ms, SD = 85 ms) nor for B-items (10 ms, PM BREPEATED trials: M = 548 ms, SD = 79 ms; oddball B: M = 538 ms, SD = 80 ms). The factor trial type was not significant. Also, Trial type 9 Item set did not interact, both Fs \ 1. 123 590 BREPEATED trials in Test block 2. That is, because PM A cues and PM B cues were exemplar items of the same semantic category, it is conceivable that diminished PM aftereffects for B-items in Test block 2 are a consequence of generalized transfer effects from semantically related A-items. More specifically, during the repeated exposure to PM AREPEATED trials participants might have deactivated not only the specific S-R link between PM A cues of the categorical intention and the associated intended action. Instead, participants might have formed a more abstract semantic intention representation during the categorical PM task instruction (see also Walser et al., 2012). Consequently, this might have enabled a transfer of repeated exposure of PM AREPEATED trials to PM BREPEATED trials resulting even in complete intention deactivation of PM BREPEATED trials in Test block 2. Experiment 2 We conducted Experiment 2 to test for the assumption of a within-category transfer effect. To rule out this alternative explanation, we adapted Experiment 1 by omitting all A-items from Test block 1 and Test block 2. Therefore, Test block 1 did not contain any PMREPEATED trials, eliminating the possibility of repeated exposure and of within-category transfer. Only in Test block 2, B-items (PM BREPEATED and oddball B) were included. If intention deactivation in Experiment 1 would have been due to a delay effect, no aftereffects on PM BREPEATED trials should occur in Experiment 2. If intention deactivation in Experiment 1 would have been due to a transfer from PM AREPEATED trial repetitions to PM BREPEATED trials, in Experiment 2 aftereffects on PM BREPEATED trials should be observed. Method Participants Sixteen new students of the Technische Universität Dresden (2 male; age M = 20.63 years, SD = 3.01) participated in Experiment 2. Apparatus and stimuli In Experiment 2, we used the same apparatus and stimuli as in Experiment 1. Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 trials were shown. Consequently, Test block 1 contained 132 standard trials and two different specific oddball trial types, each during six trials. Test block 2 contained 132 standard trials, 6 PM BREPEATED trials and 6 oddball B trials. Results Error trials (5.8 %) and trials with RTs 2.5 SDs above or below a participant’s mean RT for a given trial type (PM block: 2.8 %; Test block 2: 2.7 %) were excluded prior to RT analyses. PM block RTs and error rates on PM A and PM B trials did not differ, t(15) = 1.45, p = .166, d = 0.20; and t(15) = -0.84, p = .411, d = -0.15, respectively. Participants responded slower and made more errors on oddball trials than on standard trials, t(15) = 11.10, p \ .001, d = 2.45, and t(27) = 3.02, p = .009, d = 0.99, respectively (Fig. 2; Table 1). Test block 2 We conducted repeated-measures ANOVAs with the factor trial type (standard, oddball B, PM BREPEATED) on RTs and error rates. The ANOVA on RTs was significant, F(2, 30) = 21.46, p \ .001, g2 = .59. RTs on oddball B trials were slower than on standard trials, denoting an orientation response (M = 54 ms), F(1, 15) = 32.80, p \ .001, g2 = .69 (repeated contrast). Most important, we found aftereffects of completed intentions (M = 44 ms) in terms of increased RTs on PM BREPEATED trials as compared to oddball B trials, F(1, 15) = 6.01, p = .026, g2 = .29. In the corresponding analysis on error data, trial type did not reach significance, F \ 1. Similar to Experiment 1, we again compared RT aftereffects between the first three encounters and the last three encounters of PM BREPEATED trials. Presumably due to the small sample size and thus lack of statistical power, the Trial type 9 Block position interaction slightly missed significance, F(1, 15) = 3.68, p = .074, g2 = .20. On a descriptive level, however, aftereffects were larger for early encounters (M = 95 ms, t[15] = 2.50, p = .025, d = 0.64) than those of late encounters (M = 28 ms, t[15] = 1.90, p = .076, d = 0.28). Further analyses Procedure and design The procedure of Experiment 2 was similar to the one of Experiment 1 except the following changes. During Test block 1 and Test block 2 no PM AREPEATED and oddball A 123 In addition, we re-analyzed aftereffects of Experiment 2 by exclusively using data from the first cycle of PM block, Test block 1 and Test block 2, to rule out the possibility that the repeated cycle of PM block and Test blocks Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 affected intention aftereffects. Importantly, even for the very first cycle, we found significant PM aftereffects in Test block 2 (M = 52 ms; PM BREPEATED trials: M = 668 ms, SD = 132 ms; oddball B trials: M = 616 ms, SD = 105 ms), F(1, 15) = 7.09, p = .013, g2 = .34. In addition, aftereffects did not vary over the course of the experimental session, as no Trial type 9 Repeated cycles interaction was found in a subsequent analysis, F \ 1. Between-experiment comparison We conducted a 2 9 2 mixed ANOVA with trial type (PMREPEATED, oddball) as within-subjects factor and experiment (Experiment 1: PM AREPEATED, oddball A in Test block 1; Experiment 2: PM BREPEATED, oddball B in Test block 2) as between-subjects factor to test whether aftereffects in Experiment 1 (M = 26 ms) and Experiment 2 (M = 44 ms) varied as a function of delay after intention completion when ruling out the influence of repeated exposure. The factor experiment, F \ 1; and the Experiment 9 Trial type interaction, F(1, 39) = 1.10, p = .299, g2 = .03, were not significant, indicating no differences in aftereffects after a short (Experiment 1) and long delay (Experiment 2). To test whether Item-B aftereffects in Test block 2 varied as a function of repeated Item-A exposure, we computed a between-experiment comparison of Item-B aftereffects in Test block 2. Importantly, the mixed ANOVA with trial type (PM BREPEATED, oddball B) as within-subjects factor and experiment (Experiment 1, Experiment 2) as between-subjects factor revealed smaller Item-B aftereffects in Experiment 1 (M = 2 ms) compared to Experiment 2 (M = 44 ms), as indicated by a Trial type 9 Experiment interaction, F(1, 42) = 5.66, p = .022, g2 = .12. Discussion Surprisingly and in contrast to Experiment 1, we observed aftereffects of completed intentions for PM BREPEATED trials during Test block 2. These aftereffects did not differ from those of PM AREPEATED trials during Test block 1 in Experiment 1 with a much shorter temporal delay to intention completion. Further, PM BREPEATED aftereffects were substantially increased compared to those of Experiment 1, when ruling out the role of delay. Consequently, the disappearance of aftereffects in Test block 2 found in Experiment 1 cannot be accounted for by a delay effect. Findings from Experiment 2 rather indicate that effects of repeated exposure (i.e., response reconfiguration) affect not only specific members of an intention but that response reconfiguration may generalize from one PM cue to other PM cues of the same semantic category (see ‘‘General 591 discussion’’ for further implications and alternative explanations of this finding). Experiment 3 Experiment 3 served to provide further and more direct evidence for the assumption that decreased aftereffects of completed intentions are specifically related to repeated exposure to PMREPEATED items and less so to temporal distance to intention completion. For this, we directly manipulated repeated exposure and temporal delay in a single experiment using only PM-cue exemplars. In particular, for this, participants had to perform the PM task in response to specific PM cues (e.g., a square) instead of different members of a PM-cue category as in Experiments 1 and 2. We used a single Test block to measure aftereffects. We tested the influence of delay versus repeated cue exposure on aftereffects using a 2 (block length: short, long) 9 2 (frequency: 4 PMREPEATED trials, 12 PMREPEATED trials) within-subjects design. If aftereffects fade as a function of delay after PM task completion, they should be smaller in the long- than the short-block condition. If in contrast aftereffects fade as a function of PMREPEATED trial repetitions, they should be smaller in the 12- than the 4-PMREPEATED trials condition. Method Participants Twenty-four new students of the Technische Universität Dresden (8 male; age M = 23.79 years, SD = 1.05) participated for 12 € or course credit in two experimental sessions lasting about 1 h each. Apparatus and stimuli In Experiment 3, we used the same apparatus as in Experiment 1. However, instead of categorical PM cues, exemplar PM cues were used. That is, 36 different deviants (e.g., square, circle, @-symbols, stars, triangles) served as PM cues, PMREPEATED cues and oddballs. Procedure and design Participants attended two experimental sessions each comprising 12 cycles with each a PM block and a single Test block. In contrast to Experiments 1 and 2 participants received at the beginning of each cycle a PM instruction to press the X key instead of performing the ongoing parityjudgment task in response to a specific PM cue (e.g., digits surrounded by a square). The PM block (48 trials) contained 4 PM trials and 4 oddball trials. We used a 2 9 2 design to manipulate block length (short: 48 trials, long: 144 trials) 123 592 and PMREPEATED trial frequency (4 trials, 12 trials) in the subsequent Test block. To separate the orientation response from intention aftereffects and thus realize a comparable baseline condition a similar number of oddball trials (i.e., 4 trials vs. 12 trials) were presented in the Test block. During each of the 12 cycles, one of the 36 deviant stimuli was assigned to serve as PM/PMREPEATED trial, one as oddball trial during the PM block and one as oddball trial during the Test block. Two experimental sessions were conducted to increase statistical power. Both sessions similarly contained each of the four Block length 9 PMREPEATED trial frequency conditions three times. The only difference between sessions was the order of conditions for each participant. Two to five days passed between the sessions. Similar to Experiments 1 and 2, each experimental session started with a practice block, in which participants were made familiar with the parity-judgment task. Results Error trials (4.9 %) and trials with RTs 2.5 SDs above or below a participant’s mean RT for a given trial type (PM block: 2.6 %; Test block: 2.7 %) were excluded prior to RT analyses. PM block Performance on PM trials was comparable to previous experiments. RTs on oddball trials were 233 ms slower Fig. 3 Mean response time (RT) and percent error as a function of trial type [prospective memory (PM), standard] in the PM block and as a function of PMREPEATED trial frequency (4 trials, 12 trials), block length (short: 48 trials, long: 144 trials) and trial type (standard, oddball, PMREAPEATED) in the Test block of Experiment 3. Error bars represent standard errors 123 Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 than on standard trials, indicating an orientation response, t(23) = 13.55, p \ .001, d = 1.92. Participants made 6.1 % more errors on oddball trials than on standard trials, t(23) = 7.29, p \ .001, d = 1.17 (Fig. 3; Table 2). Test block We conducted repeated-measures ANOVAs with the factors trial type (PMREPEATED, oddball), block length (short, long) and frequency (4 trials, 12 trials) on RTs and error data of the ongoing task. The ANOVA on RTs revealed a main effect of trial type, F(1, 23) = 43.58, p \ .001, g2 = .66, indicating an overall aftereffect of completed intentions (M = 27 ms). RTs were faster on short blocks (M = 581 ms) than on long blocks (M = 602 ms), F(1, 23) = 9.28, p = .006, g2 = .29. Responses were slower in the low frequency (M = 615 ms) compared to the highfrequency condition (M = 569 ms), F(1, 23) = 43.58, p \ .001, g2 = .66. Most important, aftereffects did not vary as a function of block length, as Trial type 9 Block length did not interact, F(1, 23) = 1.20, p = .285, g2 = .05. In contrast, aftereffects were substantially reduced in the 12 PMREPEATED trials condition (M = 12 ms, t[23] = 2.66, p = .014, d = 0.11) compared to the 4 PMREPEATED trials condition (M = 40 ms, t[23] = 4.89, p \ .001, d = 0.33), as indicated by a significant Trial type 9 Frequency interaction, F(1, 23) = 18.04, p \ .001, g2 = .44. There was no Trial type 9 Block length 9 Frequency interaction, F \ 1. Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 593 Table 2 Mean RTs and error rates for the PM block by trial type, and for the Test block by block length (short: 48 trials, long: 144 trials), PMREPEATED trial number and trial type in Experiment 3 (standard deviations in parentheses) RT (ms) Error (%) PM block Standard 541 (88) 4.9 (3.5) Oddball 774 (147) 11.0 (6.5) PM 617 (82) 10.3 (7.2) Standard 513 (85) 4.1 (3.4) Oddball 575 (110) 5.4 (7.0) PMREPEATED 610 (139) 4.3 (6.1) Standard 522 (88) 4.5 (4.1) Oddball 566 (109) 6.1 (6.1) 575 (113) 6.6 (8.0) 517 (86) 4.9 (4.3) Test block Short blocks 4 PMREPEATED trials 12 PMREPEATED trials PMREPEATED Long blocks 4 PMREPEATED trials Standard Oddball 614 (113) 5.6 (6.2) PMREPEATED 662 (153) 6.6 (8.0) Standard 519 (89) 4.6 (3.4) Oddball 559 (93) 5.8 (4.9) PMREPEATED 576 (112) 6.1 (6.1) 12 PMREPEATED trials Further analyses Similar to Experiments 1 and 2, we re-analyzed aftereffects of Experiment 3 only including the first cycle of PM block and Test block. Visual inspection suggests that the main findings can also be obtained in the first cycle. That is, aftereffects were not affected by block length, F \ 1. In contrast, aftereffects were at least numerically smaller in the 12 PMREPEATED trials condition (M = 27 ms, t[11] = 1.42, p = .182, d = 0.18) than in the 4 PMREPEATED trials condition (M = 88 ms, t[11] = 2.09, p = .060, d = 0.45). Due to a lack of power, however, this difference failed to reach statistical significance, F(1, 20) = 1.61, p = .219, g2 = .07.3 Subsequently, it was tested whether aftereffects in the 4 PMREPEATED trials condition and 12 PMREPEATED trials condition were comparable when the analysis was restricted to the first four PMREPEATED encounters. Importantly, the Trial type 9 Frequency interaction was no more significant, F(1, 23) = 3.12, p = .090, g2 = .12, suggesting 3 We thank Julie Bugg for suggesting this analysis. that aftereffects did not (at least statistically) differ anymore between the 12 PMREPEATED trials condition (M = 27 ms) and 4 PMREPEATED trials condition (M = 40 ms), thereby fostering the role of repeated exposure on aftereffects.4 To further test if the deactivation depends on repeated exposure to PMREPEATED trials, we re-analyzed aftereffects for early and late PMREPEATED trials (see also ‘‘Experiment 1’’ and ‘‘Experiment 2’’). Irrespective of block length, aftereffects decreased from the early six encounters (M = 21 ms, t[23] = 2.99, p = .007, d = 0.18) to the late six encounters (M = 4 ms, t[23] = 1.17, p = .254, d = 0.04) in the 12 PMREPEATED trials condition, F(1, 23) = 7.42, p = .012, g2 = .24. Similarly, for the four PMREPEATED trials condition aftereffects decreased from the early two encounters (M = 62 ms, t[23] = 5.21, p \ .001, d = 0.42) to the late two encounters (M = 21 ms, t[23] = 2.42, p = .024, d = 0.19), as indicated by a Trial type 9 Block position interaction, F(1, 23) = 12.09, p = .002, g2 = .35. An additional ANOVA on standard trials only as a function of block length and frequency revealed that RTs did not vary as a function of block length, F \ 1. RTs on standard trials were slightly increased in the high-frequency condition (M = 520 ms) compared to the lowfrequency condition (M = 515 ms), F(1, 23) = 5.76, p = .025, g2 = .20.5 We only computed an overall error analysis, because participants hardly made any commission errors (0.02 %). For all corresponding analyses on error rates of the Test block, no significant effects were found, all ps [ .092. 4 Although not significant, the at least numerically somewhat larger aftereffects in the 4 compared to the 12 PMREPEATED trials condition may be due to the task structure, as we cannot control for a potential imbalance of encountered oddball trials prior to the first 4 PMREPEATED trials in both conditions. It is conceivable, e.g., that in the 12 PMREPEATED trials condition participants have an increased experience of task-irrelevant deviants prior to the first 4 PMREPEATED trials, which should decrease aftereffects and RTs in trials including deviants in general (oddball trials and PMREPEATED trials alike). In fact, this is supported by a main effect of frequency, with faster responses to deviant trials in the high-frequency (12 repeat) condition compared to the low-frequency (4 repeat) condition. 5 This might have been caused by the increased number of postdeviant standard trials (i.e., 24) following the 24 deviant stimuli (12 PMREPEATED, 12 oddballs) in the high-frequency condition compared to the 8 post-deviant standard trials following the 8 deviant stimuli (4 PMREPEATED, 4 oddballs). Switching attention back from deviant stimuli to subsequent standard trials might have caused reorientation costs (see also Meier & Rey-Mermet, 2012). Re-analyzing standard trial RTs while excluding post-deviant standard trials strongly diminished the RT difference between low- and high-frequent conditions to a non-significant level, F(1, 23) = 2.71, p = .113, g2 = .10. 123 594 Discussion In Experiment 3, we measured aftereffects of completed intentions in a single Test block after intention completion, varying orthogonally the block length and the frequency of PMREPEATED trials. Aftereffects did not vary as a function of block length, arguing against the hypothesis that aftereffects might decrease as a function of delay after intention completion. In contrast, aftereffects were reduced in conditions with 12 PMREPEATED trials compared to conditions with 4 PMREPEATED trials supporting the assumption that aftereffects might decrease as a function of repeated exposure of the no-more-relevant PM cue during the ongoing task. General discussion Our aim in the present study was to shed light on the controversy of whether aftereffects of completed intentions fade as a function of delay after intention completion or as a function of repeated exposure of PMREPEATED trials. In Experiment 1, aftereffects for both PM-cue category members vanished in Test block 2 independently of whether they were repeatedly exposed (i.e., A-items) or never shown (i.e., B-items) during Test block 1. At first sight, this finding was consistent with the assumption that aftereffects fade as a function of delay after intention completion. However, in Experiment 2, in which we prevented repeated exposure by omitting all PMREPEATED trials from Test block 1, aftereffects re-occurred during Test block 2. This finding indicates that aftereffects did not cease as a function of temporal distance to intention completion. Instead, in Experiment 1 response reconfiguration processes due to repeated exposure of A-items during Test block 1 transferred from one PM-cue category member to another. We found conforming evidence for this assumption in Experiment 3, in which we orthogonally tested delay and repeated exposure in a single experiment. Importantly, delay in terms of block length did not affect the size of aftereffects. Instead, aftereffects of completed intentions were strongly affected by repeated exposure as indicated by reduced aftereffects in high-frequency conditions in which PMREPEATED trials were shown 12 times as compared to lowfrequency conditions in which PMREPEATED trials were shown only four times. Interpretations of the repeated exposure effect Our findings enable explaining contradictory results of previous studies. First and consistent with studies, in which a between-subjects manipulation of delay interval length between PM task completion and measurement of 123 Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 aftereffects was used (Scullin et al., 2011; Scullin & Bugg, 2013), we did not find evidence for a delay effect, neither in a between-experiment comparison between Experiments 1 and 2, nor by manipulating block length in Experiment 3. Secondly, we found decreasing aftereffects that were associated with the repeated exposure of PMREPEATED trials in both, Experiment 1 (A-items), and in Experiment 3 with smaller aftereffects in the high frequency compared to the low-frequency condition, irrespective of the temporal distance to intention completion. We assume that the first PMREPEATED encounters triggered retrieval of the associated intended action, resulting in a response conflict and thus increased ongoing task RTs and/or commission errors. Over the course of PMREPEATED trial repetitions the link between the PM cue and the nomore-relevant PM task (e.g., pressing the X key) was destabilized resulting in decreasing reactivation of the old PM response during PMREPEATED encounters. It is even conceivable that during this response reconfiguration, participants formed a new link between PMREPETEAD trials and performing an ongoing task response. This interpretation of a response reconfiguration effect is consistent with studies showing that new stimulus–response links can be acquired within only a few stimulus repetitions (De Baene, Kühn, & Brass, 2012; Ruge & Wolfensteller, 2010). Interpretations of the transfer effect of repeated exposure The finding of a transfer effect of repeated exposure from PM AREPEATED trials to PM BREPEATED trials is very informative. First, it is in line with the findings from a previous experiment (Walser et al., 2012, Experiment 4), in which participants received a categorical PM instruction and performed the PM task exclusively on one of two PMcue category members. Interestingly, aftereffects of completed intentions were also found for another PM-cue category member that never served as PM cue during the PM block. Findings from Walser et al. (2012) and the transfer effect observed in the present study indicate that PM intentions might not only be stored as specific links between the PM cue and its associated intended action. Rather, PM intentions might be stored on a more abstract, semantic level in episodic memory (Goschke & Kuhl, 1993) and/or specific links might generalize and transfer to other related items. The assumption of an abstract semantic intention representation is, however, not mandatory for explaining transfer effects in Experiment 1. That is, it is also conceivable that participants deactivated their left-hand responses during Test block 1, thereby leading not only to no more aftereffects on PM AREPEATED trials, but also on Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 PM BREPEATED trials in Test block 2.6 At the same time it should be noted, though, that other studies reported aftereffects of completed intentions when a single hand was used to complete both, the ongoing task and the PM response, respectively (Scullin et al., 2012; Scullin & Bugg, 2013). Still, this alternative explanation highlights that it remains a theoretically extremely important question what exactly is deactivated (e.g., the abstract semantic intention representation; a specific stimulus–response link between PM cue and motor action on a procedural level) after intention completion, how intentions are represented in memory, and which aspects of intentions are responsible for aftereffects of completed intentions. In addition, one might assume that the transfer effect of repeated exposure might alternatively be explainable by retrieval-induced forgetting,7 suggesting that the retrieval of a practice-item impairs the activation of related items (Anderson, Bjork, & Bjork, 1994). Although we cannot entirely exclude this possibility, we render this explanation as rather unlikely. That is, in previous work applying virtually the same experimental design, PMREPEATED trials, which were members of a PM-cue category and never presented during the PM block and thus ‘‘unpracticed’’, yielded reliable aftereffects (Walser et al., 2012, Experiment 4). Furthermore, given the possibility of a transfer of the repeated exposure effect, it might play an important role in interpreting findings of a seemingly delay effect in previous studies on aftereffects of completed intentions. For instance, Förster, Liberman, and Higgins (2005) measured aftereffects of a PM cue (e.g., the symbol of glasses) using semantically related words (e.g., professor, read, sun). In a first test block after intention completion, aftereffects were found in terms of increased lexical-decision RTs on words related to glasses as compared to control words (and interpreted as an inhibition effect). In a subsequent second test block, lexical-decision RTs were similar on intentionrelated words and control words. The authors interpreted their finding as intention aftereffects disappearing as a function of delay after intention completion. Against the backdrop that repeated exposure might transfer to semantically related aspects of a completed intention, these results have to be interpreted with caution and a transfer effect of repeated exposure has to be taken into consideration as an alternative explanation. More specifically, the construct glasses and its semantically related items might have been bound to the ongoing lexical-decision task with repeated exposure, leading to a disappearance of aftereffects. In the present study, we did not aim to directly test this assumption. Nevertheless, our finding of transfer 6 7 We thank Michael Ziessler for raising this point. We thank Julie Bugg for this suggestion. 595 effects clearly calls for future research on the implications for previous findings and on its generalizability. For instance, it remains an empirical question whether observed transfer effects from encoding at a category level (Experiment 2) to related PM exemplar cues are restricted to the specific encoding at the category level or whether a similar transfer would also be observable from encoding at an exemplar level to other related PM exemplar cues. The role of delay and interference on intention deactivation The present and previous experiments on deactivation of completed intentions did not find unambiguous proof for delay-dependent deactivation processes (Beck et al., 2013; Förster et al., 2005; Scullin et al., 2011; Scullin & Bugg, 2013; Walser et al., 2012). Despite the clear evidence for repeated exposure on intention deactivation, we do not deny that decay might play a role for deactivation of completed intentions. Given the relatively short time intervals investigated so far, future research might investigate the role of delay by systematically varying the timeinterval length between intention completion and measurement of intention aftereffects. At the same time, determining the specific time course of PM aftereffect decline is not trivial, as other mechanisms have to be considered as well. For example, it is conceivable that some aspects of the intention representation (i.e., the readiness of the PM cue, intended action, and/ or PM cue–intended action link) might lose their strength as a mere function of delay after intention completion. Furthermore, interference from other memoranda might play a crucial role for intention deactivation to work. In line with the latter idea, it might be crucial for intention deactivation what a person does after intention completion. Consistent with this idea Walser et al. (2013) recently showed smaller intention aftereffects when participants were required to perform a resource demanding working memory task between intention completion and measurement of aftereffects. In addition, aftereffects could even be increased when participants reflected upon the no-morerelevant PM cues. A similar assumption has been discussed for forgetting in other memory fields such as short-term memory (e.g., Berman et al., 2009; Campoy, 2012; McKeown & Mercer, 2012). Consequently, future research might more systematically investigate the role of interference and delay on intention deactivation. Conclusions The present study integrates heterogeneous findings from previous studies that were apparently opposing each other. 123 596 That is, decreasing aftereffects in studies using multiple PMREPEATED trials (Beck et al., 2013; Walser et al., 2012, 2013) might be explained with repeated exposure and are thus consistent with studies using a between-subject comparison and/or a single PMREPEATED trial that did not find differences in aftereffects (Scullin et al., 2011; Scullin & Bugg, 2013). Further, the present findings indicate that repeated exposure might also transfer between semantically related (but not identical) PMREPEATED trials (Förster et al., 2005). The present study suggest that previous findings of decreasing aftereffects in studies using multiple PMREPEATED trials cannot be explained by a delay-dependent decay of intention aftereffects. Acknowledgments We are grateful to Julia Kleindienst, Laura Pepernick and Sarah Richter for assistance in data collection. We thank Julie Bugg and Michael Ziessler for thoughtful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. This research was partly supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG) (SFB 940/1-2013). References Anderson, M. C., Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(5), 1063–1087. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1063 Beck, S. M., Ruge, H., Walser, M., & Goschke, T. (2013). The functional neuroanatomy of spontaneous retrieval and strategic monitoring of delayed intentions (in revision). Berman, M. G., Jonides, J., & Lewis, R. L. (2009). In search of decay in verbal short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(2), 317–333. doi:10. 1037/a0014873. Bugg, J. M., Scullin, M. K., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013). Strengthening encoding via implementation intention formation increases prospective memory commission errors. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(3), 522–527. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0378-3 Campoy, G. (2012). Evidence for decay in verbal short-term memory: A commentary on Berman, Jonides, and Lewis (2009). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(4), 1129–1136. doi:10.1037/a0026934. De Baene, W., Kühn, S., & Brass, M. (2012). Challenging a decade of brain research on task switching: Brain activation in the taskswitching paradigm reflects adaptation rather than reconfiguration of task sets. Human Brain Mapping, 33(3), 639–651. doi:10. 1002/hbm.21234. Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(4), 717–726. doi:10. 1037/0278-7393.16.4.717 Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Thomas, R., Mayfield, S., Shank, H., Morrisette, N., et al. (2005). Multiple processes in prospective memory retrieval: Factors determining monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(3), 327–342. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.134.3.327. Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). Accessibility from active and fulfilled goals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41(3), 220–239. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.06.009. 123 Psychological Research (2014) 78:584–596 Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (1993). Representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(5), 1211–1226. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.19.5.1211. Marsh, R. L., Hicks, J. L., & Watson, V. (2002). The dynamics of intention retrieval and coordination of action in event-based prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(4), 652–659. doi:10. 1037/0278-7393.28.4.652. McBride, D. M., Beckner, J. K., & Abney, D. H. (2011). Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance. Memory & Cognition, 39(7), 1222–1231. doi:10.3758/s13421-011-0105-0. McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14(7), S127–S144. doi:10.1002/acp.775. McKeown, D., & Mercer, T. (2012). Short-term forgetting without interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(4), 1057–1068. doi:10.1037/ a0027749. Meier, B., & Rey-Mermet, A. (2012). Beyond monitoring: Aftereffects of responding to prospective memory targets. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(4), 1644–1653. doi:10.1016/j.concog. 2012.09.003. Pink, J. E., & Dodson, C. S. (2013). Negative prospective memory: Remembering not to perform an action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(1), 184–190. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0337-4. Rubin, D. C., & Wenzel, A. E. (1996). One hundred years of forgetting: A quantitative description of retention. Psychological Review, 103(4), 734–760. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.4.734. Ruge, H., & Wolfensteller, U. (2010). Rapid formation of pragmatic rule representations in the human brain during instruction-based learning. Cerebral Cortex, 20(7), 1656–1667. doi:10.1093/ cercor/bhp228. Scullin, M. K., & Bugg, J. M. (2013). Failing to forget: Prospective memory commission errors can result from spontaneous retrieval and impaired executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(3), 965–971. doi:10.1037/a0029198. Scullin, M. K., Bugg, J. M., & McDaniel, M. A. (2012). Whoops, I did it again: Commission errors in prospective memory. Psychology and Aging, 27(1), 46–53. doi:10.1037/a0026112. Scullin, M. K., Bugg, J. M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2011). Prospective memory and aging: Preserved spontaneous retrieval, but impaired deactivation, in older adults. Memory & Cognition, 39, 1232–1240. doi:10.3758/s13421-011-0106-z. Smith, R. E. (2003). The cost of remembering to remember in eventbased prospective memory: Investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(3), 347–361. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.29.3.347. Walser, M., Fischer, R., & Goschke, T. (2012). The failure of deactivating intentions: Aftereffects of completed intentions in the repeated prospective memory cue paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(4), 1030–1044. doi:10.1037/a0027000. Walser, M., Goschke, T., & Fischer, R. (2013). The difficulty of letting go: Moderators of the deactivation of completed intentions. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-013-0509-5