Quantification of the Bruckner Test for Strabismus Joseph M. Miller*^ Heidi Leising Hall,\ John E. Greivenkamp,*-\ and David L. Guyton% Purpose. To measure quantitatively the change in the coaxial fundus reflex with varying degrees of ocular misalignment. Methods. The coaxial fundus reflex was imaged with a charge coupled device camera under conditions of simulated ocular misalignment ranging from 0° to 7° offixationeccentricity. The effects of refractive error and pupil size were controlled. Average gray scale brightness values were calculated for each bright pupil image after some image processing was performed on the raw images. Results. A reliable, sharply delineated, minimum brightness at fovealfixationwas observed. Conclusions. It is estimated that this technique can be automated to detect the presence of 2° to 3° of ocular misalignment based on the difference in brightness of the bright pupil images between the two eyes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1995;36:897-905. A Amblyopia ("lazy eye") is described as decreased visual acuity in an otherwise normal, healthy eye. Causes of amblyopia include refractive error and ocular misalignment.' In patients in which the problem is found at an early age, the problem is often correctable. Thus, it is of utmost importance to detect possibly amblyogenic factors as early as possible. A screening test recommended for use by primary care providers is the red reflex, or Bruckner, test.23 In this test, a bright paraxial light source such as a direct ophthalmoscope is used to illuminate both eyes of the subject from a distance of approximately 1 m. The examiner compares the brightness of the two red fundus reflexes for evidence of asymmetry. If the subject is fixating on the light, any asymmetry of the pupil brightness indicates the presence of a potentially amblyogenic factor, be that cataract, anisometropia, or strabismus. In the case of anisometropia or strabismus, the brighter reflex is said to indicate the problematic eye. Changes in the red reflex with refractive error have been well studied and are the basis of photore- From the "Department of Ophthalmology, Optical Sciences Center, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, and lhe%Wilmer Ophlhalmological Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland. Supported by the Whitaker Foundation for Biomedical Research, Washington, DC, and by The Sensory Research Foundation, Phoenix, Arizona. Submitted for publication September 7, 1994; revised November 9, 1994; accepted November 11, 1994. Proprietary interest category: N. Reprint requests; Josejih Miller, Department of Ophthalmology, 1801 N. Campbell Ave, Tucson, AZ 85719. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, April 1995, Vol. 36, No. 5 Copyright © Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology fraction. The changes in the red reflex arising from ocular misalignment in the accommodating, focused eye, however, are less well understood. Roe and Guyton'1 described observations obtained with a beamsplitter ophthalmoscope, in which a true coaxial light source was used for fundus illumination. The change in the red reflex with ocular rotation was described but not measured. The reflex was said to dim with fixation, although not as much as with a regular ophthalmoscope. They also observed a color change with a fixation eccentricity greater than 5°. More recently, Carrera et al5 used a photographic technique6 to measure the ability of observers to detect the presence of significant asymmetry of pupillary brightness. They found high sensitivity (82%), specificity (91%), and accuracy (84%) in the screening of groups of subjects with small angle esotropia, large angle esotropia, anisometropia of 3 D, and normal subjects (26 subjects each group). This study used the simple judgment of asymmetry by a skilled or unskilled observer rather than a quantitative measure of the red reflex. We wanted to quantify the change in pupil brightness as ocular rotation occurs. The obvious application would be to automate the manual Bruckner test and compare the light reflexes for asymmetry. A more intriguing application would be the dynamic examination of the binocular reflex as the subject views in the direction of a fixation target, as proposed by Cibis et 897 898 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, April 1995, Vol. 36, No. 5 7 al. If the variation in pupil brightness reliably changes in an individual eye with ocular rotation and the minimum always occurs with fixation on the light source, it may be possible to determine the presence of bilateral central fixation by the simultaneous presence of pupillary light reflex minima. Our test was performed monocularly. Strabismus was simulated by displacing a finely detailed "accommodative" fixation target perpendicular to the common axis used for illumination and viewing of the pupil. METHODS The Bruckner Test The Bruckner, or red reflex, test examines and compares the bright pupils with a paraxial light source illuminating the eyes. If an eye is focused on the source, a minified image of the source will be located on the retina. This image on the retina then serves as a source for a second pass through the optical system. The retroreflection at the retina is partly diffuse8 and partly specular.9 Rays pass back through the pupil and are refracted by the lens and the cornea. Rays of light leaving the eye will retrace the paths of the light rays entering the eye (Fig. 1A). Photorefraction is the analysis of light distribution across the pupil when the image formed on the retina is perturbed by refractive error. Instead of a focused image forming on the retina, a blurred image forms. The second pass through the eye further perturbs the image (Fig. IB), but several methods1011 have been used to analyze the two-pass system. The changes in the bright pupil arising from ocular rotation are less well described in the literature. Again, a two-pass analysis is involved, but the image is assumed to be perturbed from ocular rotation rather than from simple refractive error. Four mechanisms may play a role in the change in pupillary image with ocular rotation: the specular reflection at the retina from the internal limiting membrane that changes slope with ocular rotation (Fig. 1C); changes arising from variation in retinal pigment density, with the retina displaying the characteristics of a diffuse reflector (Fig. ID); backscattering of light by the retinal nerve fiber layer proportional to the thickness of the layer (Fig. ID); and off-axis aberration12 resulting in poor image formation on the retina, with further perturbation on the second pass (Fig. ID). We wanted to evaluate the first three causes; the literature suggests that off-axis aberrations are not significant over the range of 0° to 7° used in our experiment.13 As the eye rotates and the source remains fixed, the image of the source will fall on different portions of the retina. This is what occurs in the deviated eye in ocular misalignment. Nerve fiber layer thickness Off-axis source points Image point is blurred due to aberrations FIGURE l. (A) Eye focused on point source: By diffuse reflection at the retina, the image on the retina is reimaged back to the location of the source. The rays retrace themselves through the system. (B) Eye with refractive error: The first pass through the eye produces a blurred image of the point source. The second pass blurs the image more, and the rays do not retrace themselves. (C) Specular reflection from the internal limiting membrane (ILM): Rays reflected by the sloped region of the fovea may be diverted away from the pupil on the second pass. Rays that miss the pupil on the second pass are not observed. (D) Off-axis source: Source is imaged to various regions of the retina that exhibit variation in pigment density and thickness, causing a different amount of reflection. The eye has increasing aberrations, with distance of source from axis causing blurred images on the retina. Experimental Setup A 486 personal computer that included a frame grabber board (Dipix, Montreal, Canada) was used to gather images from adult volunteers. Figure 2 shows the experimental layout. The light source was a halogen light bulb emitting white light that was fed into an endoilluminator through an optical fiber. The endoilluminator had a diameter of 50 /im and an angular subtense at the subject's eye of 0.5 arcmin, which closely approximated a point source. Based on the Gullstrand eye model for an accommodated eye,H the size of the point source on the retina was approxi- Quantification of the Bruckner Test 899 Calibration FIGURE 2. Experimental layout for imaging pupil brightness. Subjectfixateson movable target that simulates ocular misalignment relative to illumination axis. Light source, fixation target, and camera aperture are conjugate to retina. BS = beamsplitter. mately 2.26 ^m. The diffraction spot size was calculated to be approximately 4 //m in diameter. The maximum image size for HF, Hed) and H c lines (wavelengths 486 nm, 587 nm, 656 nm) was calculated to be 2.66 fim diameter, which is smaller than the diffraction spot size. The fixation target was a square 9 pixel X 9 pixel "happy face" picture on a laptop computer with a liquid crystal display. This target had an angular subtense at the eye of 0.4° horizontally and 0.47° vertically. The picture was J1+ print size and could be centered at any position in the laptop screen. The fixation distance was 33 cm. A mechanical shutter was placed between the light source and the subject. This allowed the illumination to be flashed briefly (approximately '/ 8 seconds) to avoid the pupil constriction and retinal bleaching that occur with constant illumination of the eye by a bright source. When the shutter was activated by a cable release, a light-emitting diode emitter-receiver pair detected the shutter opening and signaled the computer to initiate image capture. Two beamsplitters were used in the optical path. One beamsplitter combined light from the point source and the fixation target. The other beamsplitter reflected the combined target and light source into the subject's eye and allowed imaging of the pupil by a charge coupled device (CCD) camera. The camera, light source, and target appeared coaxial to the subject without any one component occluding either of the other two. A 6.6 mm X 8.8 mm format CCD array was used to image the pupil of the subjects. An 8-mm focal length iens focused the pupil of the subject onto the CCD array. An adjustable chin rest was used to achieve alignment with the subject's pupil. After the system was assembled, test images were obtained from several subjects. Settings of electronic gain and offset were established that allowed the bright pupil to be imaged at camera apertures ranging from f/1.4 to f/2.8 without saturation of the electronics, while preserving the noise floor from clipping. At this point, electronic gain and offset were fixed. At these reference settings, 10 images were obtained without any ambient illumination. These "dark" images were then analyzed pixel by pixel, and the average and standard deviation were computed for each pixel. In the region of the CCD used for imaging, there was systematic variation in the dark offset values, but the standard deviation indicated all pixels were active. The average dark value for each pixel could then be subtracted from the subject data to reduce systematic error introduced by the nonuniform response of the CCD. An artificial bright pupil was then constructed in which a 9.525-mm diameter aperture, masked by frosted tape, was retroilluminated uniformly by a yellow light-emitting diode. Images were obtained at various retroillumination levels until a bright pupil could be imaged at camera aperture f/1.4 without clipping. The light-emitting diode power level was then fixed, and images were obtained at camera apertures f/2 and f/4. Linear regression was performed, and a linear system response was verified by the linear increase in brightness with increase in aperture. The absolute value of the light incident on the subject's eye, after transit through two beamsplitters, was measured with a light meter. It was found that 1.3 lux was incident at 33 cm from the source. An absolute measure of a typical value for light reflected back to the CCD array was found by using a model eye with a mirror at the location of the retina. By careful adjustment of the mirror, approximately 90% of the light entering the pupil could be made to pass out of the pupil to the CCD camera. Neutral density filters were added to the path until the light hitting the CCD array had a value similar to that obtained for a volunteer (maximum pixel value, 200). The result indicated that a typical light return from the human eye was approximately 0.02%. A scale factor was required to calculate the actual size of features in the captured images based on their sizes in pixels. The scale factor was determined by imaging a circle with a diameter of 9.525 mm, which was found to contain 371 pixels. Procedure The 18 subjects were young adults 20 to 40 years of age, each able to read Jl + (3-point) print at a distance 900 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, April 1995, Vol. 36, No. 5 TABLE l. Summary of Subject Data Subject Number Age (years) 1 28 28 25 30 23 39 25 28 28 26 29 23 28 27 25 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 31 24 Gender Ethnic Group Eye Color Refractive Correction White White White White White White White White White Asian White White Wliite White White White White Asian Hazel Hazel Brown Brown Blue Blue Blue-green Brown Brown Brown Brown Brown Blue Blue-green Brown Brown Brown Brown Contacts Contacts Contacts None None None None Contacts Glasses Glasses Contacts Contacts Contacts None Glasses Contacts None Glasses F M M M F M M M M M M F M M M M M M of 33 cm, either without correction or with contact lenses (Table 1). They were healthy volunteers from the Optical Sciences Center. Informed consent was obtained from each subject, and the study was approved by the Human Subjects Committee. The tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki were followed in this research, and institutional human experimentation committee approval was granted. The mean age was 27.1 years. Of the subjects, 89% were white and 11% were Asian. The distribution of eye color among subjects was: 27.8% blue eyes, 61.6% brown eyes, and 11.1% hazel eyes. Five subjects had 20/20 uncorrected vision. Four subjects had myopia but were able to fixate on the target without glasses. Nine subjects wore contact lenses to enable fixation during the experiment. The subject was instructed to set her or his head in the chin rest and to adjust the chin rest to be able to see all parts of a rectangle that bordered the area within which the target was to be placed throughout the experiment. Final head position was verified by setting the fixation target at (0,0), triggering the shutter, and asking the subject if the light appeared coincident with the target. The target was positioned to simulate central fixation and then moved radially outward. Images of the pupil were taken with the subject gazing 0° (foveal fixation), 0.25°, 0.5°, 1°, 2°, 3°, 4°, and 5° in eight different radial directions evenly spaced every 45°. In six of the radial directions, images also were gathered at 6° and 7°. Figure 3 illustrates the positions at which images were gathered. The initial images gathered were 640 X 480 pixels. Image processing software, written in the C language, was used to detect the bright pupil section in the image, and a 50 X 50 pixel area that included the bright pupil was extracted from each image. The gray-scale histogram of the image was computed (Fig. 4A). Figure 4 shows the intermediate results of the processing performed on the raw data gathered. First, the mean dark image was subtracted from the raw image to reduce interpixel variation as described in the calibration section. Figure 4B shows a typical flat-field-corrected image and corresponding histogram. The histograms show that by subtracting the dark image from the raw data, as described in the calibration, the noise 45° 315° 225°' H N135 FIGURE 3. Direction of gaze for data points indicated by intersections of circles and lines. Degrees of fixation eccentricity are indicated and are the same along each of the eight radial directions. B C D E FIGURE 4. (A) A sample 50 X 60 pixel image of a bright pupil and a corresponding grayscale histogram. (B) The image after subtraction of the mean dark image (flat fielding) and corresponding gray-scale histogram. (C) The binary representation of pixels determined to be in the pupil. (D) Binary result of a dilation operator to fill in holes and smooth the edges of the pupil. (E) Final gray-scale representation of the pupil area. the data is reduced from about 14 parts in 256 MS to about 12 parts in 256 RMS. The initial binary eshold was then determined by finding the gray ale threshold value corresponding to the brightest % of the image pixels. A value of 5% was chosen cause it represented a maximally dilated pupil size 10 mm diameter. Figure 4C demonstrates the biry output after thresholding. Next, the pupillary reon was located using a connected components algohm and processed with a dilation operator to fill in ged or missing areas. The results are shown in Fige 4D. The final result was a gray-scale image of the pil shown in Figure 4E, Figure 5 demonstrates a l set of data for a single subject. The change in ghtness with fixation eccentricity is readily appart. Figure 3 shows the direction of gaze for these ages. The pixels in the pupil were counted, and, from s, the area of the pupil in mm2 was determined. If wer than 20 pixels were detected by the thresholding ogram, the image was not used. Of the 1334 total ages captured, 4% were discarded for this reason. These discarded images represented data at var values of fixation eccentricity. It was assumed that the pupil size remained same for a single subject throughout that subj data. This assumption was made because the rounding light level and accommodative effort, w would influence pupil size, were controlled. The e ronmental light level was held constant through the experiment, and the point light source was flas briefly (% second) with the image gathered (wi 33 msec) before the pupil had a chance to const Accommodative demand remained fixed through the experiment. Foreshortening of the area of pupil as the eye rotated, with a maximum angl rotation of 7°, was not corrected because it resu in less than 1 % variation. Although the pupillary size was assumed to rem constant throughout the experiment, the obser size of the bright image varied from image to im for an arbitrary threshold. The maximum obser bright pupil for each subject was found, and all o images were normalized to this size by including b Degrees of Fixation Eccentricity 0 0.25 0.5 1.0 2.0 3.0 4.0 5.0 6.0 7.0 0 •2 45 S 8 S U FIGURE 5. Complete data set gathered for subject 10 before image processing. Degrees of fixation eccentricity are indicated for the columns. Degrees from horizontal (clockwise) are indicated for the rows and correspond to Figure 3. und pixel values with the bright area. The padding he pupil size with background values ranged from pixels to 0 pixels, with the maximum pupil size he subjects varying from 310 pixels (8.7 mm diam) to 110 pixels (5.18 mm diameter). The detected of the pupil ranged from 42 pixels to 310 pixels. average brightness per pixel for each image was n calculated. Any images that contained more than saturated pixels (gray-scale value 255) in the ht pupil were discarded. SULTS minimum brightness for all subjects was observed in 0.5° of central fixation. The maximum brightoften appeared at approximately 5° eccentricity. normalize the data within each subject's set, the mest gray scale average value was assigned a value , the brightest 1, and the remaining data were ed accordingly. The data for all subjects are summarized in Figure he normalized data were averaged over all radial ctions for each amount of fixation eccentricity. 95% confidence intervals were found, and Figure ows these results. For the pooled observations, it was determined 0 1 Z 3 4 S 6 7 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Fixation Eccentricity (Degrees) FIGURE 6. Summary of data by subject. Data have been a aged for a given fixation eccentricity over the eight rad directions. Data have been normalized for each subject the range of 0 to 1. The standard deviation is shown. SO 100 ISO 250 200 Thickness of Retinal Layer (microns) 220 T =• 200 • • 3 180I 160 •• 80Fixation Eccentricity (Degrees) GURE 7. Summary of normalized results averaged for all bjects. The 95% confidence intervals for the data are own. hat the brightness from 2° to 7° could be distinuished from the brightness from 0° to 0.5°. Figure gives a graphic representation of the variation in rightness observed with varying degrees of ocular misalignment. The brightness values correspond to hose illustrated in Figure 7 along the left-hand side f the graph. We observed that the shape of our graph of pupil rightness versus distance from the center of the fovea esembles the variation in thickness of the retina in hat region. A plot of observed pupil brightness versus etinal thickness15 is shown in Figure 9. The plot has n r2 value of 0.96, ISCUSSION nterpretation of these data is somewhat speculative. n the present experiment, neither defocus nor varia- GURE 8. Gray-scale representation of the average brightess observed versus fixation eccentricity. i f 60" 40- • It I 140- 20 • 0 0.2 0.4 % 120' | 100" 80 40 0.6 0.2 0.4 Distance Iran Fixation (mm) FIGURE 9. (A) Observed brightness versus thickness of r nerve fiber layer.15 (B) Comparison of observed brigh and retinal thickness versus distance from fixation. tion in pupil size can be responsible for the obse variation of pupillary brightness. Only the effect duced by ocular rotation are observed. Does the reflex brighten with eccentric fixation because o axis aberrations, decreased absorption by pigm away from the fovea, more specular reflection the internal limiting membrane returning thro the pupil, or simply increased backscattering of from the thicker retinal layers as the image m away from the fovea centralis? Off-axis aberrations are unlikely to be the c given that the fovea itself is off-axis, and the minim is observed with foveal fixation. Off-axis aberra may play a role with larger angles,13 but over our r of data the results are remarkably symmetric a the fovea. Increased pigment density may be responsibl the observed diminution at fixation. The incre pigment density would have to increase as the fov is approached. This increase in pigment would c less light to be reflected, and the reflex would ass the color of the pigment. We used white light and not analyze the returned spectra so we cannot exc this mechanism, but the sharpness of the minim observed is not in keeping with clinical observa of macular pigment density. Another hypothesis is that the internal lim membrane of the retina acts as a source of signif specular reflection.4'9 The foveola subtends Along the edges of the foveal pit, the steep angle result in significant light being reflected away from pupil and may account for the decrease in pup brightness observed within 1° of fixation. This hy esis would also account for the decrease in obse 904 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, April 1995, Vol. 36, No. 5 brightness from reflection farther out in the periphery of the retina. On the other hand, such a mechanism would predict maximum brightness from reflection from the very center of the foveal pit where the incident ray is normal to the surface. Such a "hot spot" at the center of fixation was never seen. Our subjects were older (mean age, 27.1 years) than the young children routinely screened using the Bruckner test. It is possible that the internal limiting membrane reflectivity decreases significantly with increasing age, perhaps blunting the effect observed. However, none of our subjects was likely to have a posterior vitreous detachment on the basis of age. If the retinal layers act as scattering medium, more light would be backscattered in the thicker regions than in the thinner regions. In thinner regions, where pigment density is also greatest, more light would reach the pigment epithelium and be absorbed. In thicker regions of the retina, more backscatter would occur, resulting in more light returning through the pupil. Such backscattering would correlate with our observations as shown in Figure 9, with clinical observations of the nerve fiber layer. Thick nerve fiber layers partially obscure the outlines of underlying vessels because of the light scattering that occurs.16 We suspect that all the above factors play a role over the range of angles observed. Artal and Navarro17 examined the two-pass point spread function simultaneously at fixation and at 1° of eccentricity. Within this region, the specular component dominated the changes observed. As the eccentricity increases, the slope of the internal limiting membrane decreases, the incident ray is perpendicular to the retina surface, gross color changes become more clinically apparent, and brightness increases. Regardless of the mechanism, the steepness of the response that is observed allows predictions to be made about binocular changes in the red reflex with strabismus. We have begun work to model the expected results of using this technique in a binocular setting. The model assumes one eye fixating on a target while the other eye is deviated a constant amount. The fixating eye is modeled to have fixation instability less than 1°. Figure 10A shows the expected correlation of right and left eye pupil brightness with no strabismus present and with symmetric responses between the eyes. Brightness values are assigned based on the results from Figure 7. The brightness of the "fixating" eye versus the fellow eye is plotted for 200 simulations with no strabismus. The model thus predicts that for binocular, foveal fixation, the data points form a line with a slope of 1. Figure 10B predicts how pupil brightness changes in the presence of 2 prism diopters (1.15°) of strabismus with the same fixation instability. 300-,- 200-CD LLJ 100-. 0 A o 100 200 300 Right Eye 250 •Hi 1 .il 1 ' 200 <» 1 5 0 ai % 100 ^ i . ] !••' _J 50 50 B 100 150 200 Right Eye 250 FIGURE 10. Computer simulation results of the brightness detected for each eye. One eyefixateson a target with some gaze instability, and the second eye has some misalignment relative to the first eye of (A) 0 prism diopters and (B) 2 prism diopters. Comparison of Figures 10A and 10B reveals that if many repeated measures of simultaneous pupil brightness can be obtained, it may be possible to detect bifoveal fixation on a statistical basis. The method would require recognizing the line of unity present in Figure 10A under bifoveal fixation that is transformed into a locus of points that largely excludes the line of unity in Figure 10B in the presence of strabismus. It is likely that 2 prism diopters or more of strabismus could be detected, but the determination of the exact amount would be difficult, and differentiation be- Quantification of the Bruckner Test tween esotropia and exotropia would probably not be possible without additional data. A significant limiting factor in the present data is CCD camera noise. Our system was limited by how bright a point source could be achieved. To image the pupil, camera gain was maximal, resulting in interpixel background noise of approximately 16 parts in 256 RMS. Addition of noise of this magnitude renders the distinction between Figure 10A and Figure 10B patterns more difficult. This problem may be addressed by increasing the point source illumination and reducing camera noise. In summary, we have documented the monocular change in pupil image brightness, from fundus reflection of coaxial illumination, with fixation eccentricity of up to 7°. This phenomenon, although widely observed as part of the Bruckner or red reflex test, has not been systematically used in a screening instrument for strabismus. Our results suggest that asymmetry of the red reflex may reliably be detected with ocular misalignment as small as 2°. Key Words amblyopia, image analysis, strabismus, retinal reflectivity, optics References 1. von Noorden GK. Binocular Vision and Ocular Motility, Theory and Management of Strabismus. 4th ed. St Louis: CV Mosby; 1990. 2. Bruckner R. Exakte Strabimusdia mostik bei V2—3 jahrigen Kindern mac einem einfachen Verfahren dem 'Durchleuchtungstest' Ophthalmologica. 1962; 144:184-198. In German. 3. Bruckner R. Praktische Ubungen mit dem Durchleuchtungstest zur Friihdiagnose des Strabismus. Ophthalvwlogica. 1965; 149:497-503. In German. 905 % 4. Roe LD, Guyton DL. The light that leaks: Bruckner and the red reflex. Surv Ophthalmol. 1984;28:665-670. 5. Carrera A, Maornil MA, Zamora MI, et al. Detecting amblyogenic diseases with the photographic Bruckner test. Stralrismus. 1993; 1:3-9. 6. Day S, Norcia T. Photographic detection of amblyogenic factors. Ophthalmology. 1986;93:25-28. 7. Cibis GW, Luke T. Characterizing the dynamic relationship of the Hirschberg ratio, Briieckner reflex and pupil width. ARVO Abstracts. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sri. 1993; 34:861. 8. Campbell FW, Gubisch RW. Optical Quality of the human eye. /Physiol. 1966; 116:558-578. 9. Gorrand JM. Separation of the reflection by the inner limiting membrane. Ophlhabnol Physiol Opt. 1986; 6:187-196. 10. Walsh G, Charman WM, Howland HC. Objective technique for the determination of monochromatic aberrations of the human eye. J Opt Soc Am [A]. 1984; 1:987-992. 11. Kaakinen KA, Kaseva HO, Teir HH. Two-flash photrefraction in screening of amblyogenic refractive errors. Ophthalmology. 1987;94:1036-1042. 12. Jennings JAM, Charman WN. Off-axis image quality in the human eye. Vision Res. 1980;21:445-455. 13. Navarro R, Artal P. Modulation transfer of the human eye as a function of retinal eccentricity. / Opt Soc Am [A]. 1993; 10:201-212. 14. Le Grand Y, El Hage SG. Physiological Optics. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1980:65-66. 15. Spalton DJ, Marshal J. In: Spalton DJ, Hitchings RA, Hunter PA, eds. Atlas of Clinical Ophthalmology. Philadelphia: JB Lippincott; 1984; 13:7. 16. Quigley HA, Miller MR, George T. Clinical evaluation of nerve fiber layer atrophy as an indicator of glaucomatous optic nerve damage. Arch Ophthalmol. 1980;98:1564-1571. 17. Altai P, Navarro R. Simultaneous measurement of twopoint-spread functions at different locations across the human fovea. Appl Opt. 1992; 31:3646-3656.