Reprinted from II March 1977, Volume 195, pp. 1002-1004 Species Identification in the North American Cowbird: Appropriate Responses to Abnormal Song Andrew P. King and Meredith J. West Copyright© 1977 by the A merican Association fOT the Advancement oj Science • Species Identification in the North American Cowbird: Appropriate Responses to Abnormal Song Abstract. Female cowbirds raised in auditory isolation from males responded to the songs of male cowbirds with copulatory postures. The songs of males reared in isolation were more effective in eliciting the posture than the songs of normally reared males. The females did not respond to the songs of other species. These re­ sults indicate one mechanism of species identification for this parasitic species. M0- courtship and is accompanied by a bow­ lothrus ater, is reported to parasitize more than 200 species (1). Thus, unlike ing display. We collected data from a small group of males that suggested that most vertebrates, young cowbirds are male peer experience was sufficient to not con­ produce normal adult courtship song. specific parents during their earliest inter­ However, males reared in isolation from actions with adults or peers. Although both male peers and adults, developed much remains to be learned about the de­ songs that differed from the normal ver­ velopmental impact of such stimulation, sion, particularly in the extent to which there is growing evidence to demonstrate some of the notes are mod�lated (Fig. The brown-headed exposed to cowbird, stimulation by its facilitative role in avian species identi­ 2). The following experiments were de­ (2). How, then, does the cow­ signed to elaborate upon these observa­ bird, naturally deprived of such experi­ tions by testing other naive females. The ence, come to identify other cowbirds? songs of both normal and isolate males This question has puzzled students of de­ were included to help clarify the role of velopment and evolution for many years, peer experience in song development. fication and, until now, only speCUlative solu­ The subjects were six female cowbirds tions existed (3). We report a series of ex­ raised in auditory and visual isolation periments detailing a mechanism of spe­ from adult cowbirds. Their prenatal audi­ cies identification that allows us to under­ tory experience was controlled in that stand how cowbirds have evolved to eggs were obtained from a captive colo­ overcome the fact that they were not ny of cowbirds and incubated in isola­ reared by conspecifics. tion. Their postnatal social contact was Two findings led to the discovery of limited to a 1- to 5-day period during this mechanism. First, we had raised a fe­ which the individual eggs hatched into male cowbird in the laboratory in com­ barn swallow (Hirundo rustica) nests, plete auditory and visual isolation from and the young birds were fed by their other cowbirds. At 8 months of age, she hosts. After being returned to the labora­ was exposed to a recording of male cow­ tory, they were hand-reared as a group bird courtship song. She adopted a pos­ without contact with adult cowbirds. ture we term the "copulatory response": They were placed in soundproof cham­ her wings were lowered and spread bers between the ages of 35 and 60 days apart, her neck and body were arched, (4). The birds were housed in pairs to al­ and the feathers around the cloacal re­ gion were separated (Fig. 1). A series of low the opportunities for social inter­ action and auditory experience thought playbacks confirmed the specificity of to be essential for the birds to come into the response: she showed no response to breeding condition. Chambers the songs of a red-winged blackbird each housed two female cowbirds; cham­ 1 and 2 (Agelaius phoeniceus) or to the cowbird ber 3, a female cowbird and a female red­ flight call. winged blackbird; chamber 4, a female The second finding related to the de­ cowbird and a male cowbird; chamber 5, velopment of the male song. Male cow­ a male cowbird and a male cardinal bird song, which can be transcribed as (Richmendena "burble phase of their photoperiods was gradu- burble . j tsee," occurs during cardinalis). The light �'---'. -- " cowbird, was transferred to a chamber housing a female canary 3 weeks before the 1976 testing. Also, two samples of 16 - NORMAL 12 8 parts of the abnormal song were used: (i) the "burble-burble" phrase alone, (ii) the "tsee" alone, and (iii) a new sample 4 16 of abnormal song from another isolate 12 male. The abnormal song retained its superi­ or effectiveness, and abnormal, normal, and partial-abnormal songs were all more effective than the controls (Table Fig. 1. Female cowbird displaying the copula­ tory response. ABNORMAL 1). The differences among the conditions were reliable (Friedman two-way analy­ sis of variance, Xr2 = 23.3, P < .001). Both samples of abnormal song were ally extended during the spring months. equally effective, and one of the partial The playback period began in May songs was more effective than the full (6). Female CB, despite her 1975, when the birds were 9 months old, normal song and was conducted in two phases. In the sexual experience and her lack of re­ first phase, the females were exposed to sponding in the earlier experiment, re­ recordings of normal cowbird song ob­ sponded as strongly as the sexually naive tained from a wild-caught male and the females 8 4 r • O� ____ � . r \��,� ,.. III � '�. __"L� ' � '� �� " I ' _ II R ________ _ '" , 1----112 II< -----I Fig. 2. These recordings were made on a sono­ graph (Kay) with a wide-band filter and set for flat response and linear display. The units on the ordinate are kilohertz. The most striking difference is the rapid modulation between 2 and 8 khz present in the abnormal but not the normal song. This type of modulation was not present in any of a sample of 522 song spectro­ graphs obtained from a captive colony of nor­ mally reared male cowbirds (10). (7). control songs of male red-winged black­ Another piece of evidence attests to tory and sexual experience with males birds, meadowlarks (Sturnella magna), the potency of the abnormal song as a does not diminish the effectiveness of the and Baltimore orioles (Icterus galbula). sexual releaser. Two wild female cow­ abnormal song. There birds were captured and placed in the lab­ were four playbacks spread 1976. While in In summary, these data indicate that throughout each day; the order of pre­ oratory in the winter of sentation varied each day but was the these but experience with adult cowbirds in order same for each bird. In the second phase, could not see, caged male cowbirds and for species identification to occur. Naive the procedure was identical except that male and female free-living cowbirds. Al­ females ... the playback series included the abnor­ 'tal song of an isolate male. The re­ though a full series of playbacks was not song; naive males produce an acoustical­ carried out owing to the birds' nervous­ ly atypical, but highly effective, song. presence or ab­ ness in captivity, the first time the abnor­ Isolate song, then, appears to have quali­ mal song was played to them, one of the ties of a "supernormal" releaser. Why During the first phase, four of the six females immediately adopted the copUla­ normal song appears less effective is as female cowbirds responded more often tory posture. In subsequent exposures, yet unspecified, but it may reflect the to normal song than to any of the control fact that males sing in contexts other songs (Table 1). One of these, female she continued to respond to the abnor­ mal song but never to the normal. Thus, than courting. The songs of males, there­ DB, showed a dual preference for cow­ response to abnormal song was not re­ fore, may contain more complex mes­ bird and meadowlark song. The fifth cow­ stricted to females raised in isolation. sages indicating not only the species, bird, female W, did not come into breed­ Furthermore, the response of this female sex, or breeding condition of the singer, ing condition and hence did not respond. and that of female CB suggest that audi- but agonistic or territorial information as • \......sponse measure was the sence of the copulatory posture (5). surroundings, they heard, neither male nor female cowbirds require respond adaptively to male The sixth, female CB, was housed with the male cowbird and also showed no response. She was observed, however, to copUlate frequently. The variability across the females was probably due to variations in breeding condition, which was indicated by the numbers of eggs laid. During the second phase, four females demonstrated a clear preference for the abnormal song (Table 1). For each of the four birds, the abnormal song produced the highest rates of response, the normal song the second highest and the control songs of the other species the lowest rates. /. In order to pursue this finding in great­ � '" detail, the birds were maintained in \,--tSolation for another year and retested in \ the spring of 1976. Several procedural changes were also made. Female CB, which had been reared with the male • . well. A closer assessment of the function­ another. Little is known about cowbird al properties of the songs of normally social behavior, yet data from our labora­ reared males may help to clarify these tory indicate that cowbirds have highly speculations. In addition, these data sug­ structured dominance hierarchies and gest the need to examine in more detail complex intraspecific behaviors that may the developmental role of self-stimula­ also facilitate identification or maintain tion. It may be that the self-generated social integration among acquainted cow­ sounds of the isolate males channeled birds their song development toward an em­ scribed might represent an independent phasis on the acoustic properties that system designed to ensure identification contain the sexual message in cowbird during the most important context, the song. Males reared with other males, breeding season, and to enhance repro­ however, whether age mates or adults, ductive opportunities by inducing sex­ may receive acoustically more varied ually appropriate behavior in the female. stimulation, may learn to modify their Thus, song components as a result of auditory multiple means to identify one another, and behavioral feedback from their com­ the presence of a mechanism geared di­ panions (thereby leading to a dilution of rectly for reproduction may represent a the purely sexual message), or both. In critical adaptation for a parasitic species. any case, these data implicate self-stimu­ ANDREW P. KING (9). The mechanism we have de­ although cowbirds lation as possibly a very important form Department of Psychology, of species-typical experience for cow­ Cornell University, birds as has been hypothesized Ithaca, New York (8). Final­ 5. may have 14850 MEREDITH J. WEST* Iy, although it is tempting to label the cowbird's response to isolate song as id­ Department of Psychology, iosyncratic or unrepresentative of other University of North Carolina, songbirds, this cannot be done, as little Chapel Hill 6. 7. 27514 late songs. Such information would be useful in species that, like the cowbird, have courtship songs. Songs of a primari­ ly territorial nature may be more difficult to assess. Although these results provide one an­ swer to the question of the mechanism by which cowbirds identify one another, they do not explain how young or juve­ nile cowbirds first come to recognize one References and Notes I. H. Friedmann, U.S. Natl. Mus. Bull. 233, 6, 8. 9. (1963). 2. G. Gottlieb, Development 0/ Species Identifica­ tion: An Inquiry into the Prenatal Determinants o/Perception (Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1971). 3. D. Lehrman, in Ethology and Psychiatry, N. F. White, Ed. (Univ. of Toronto Press, Toronto, 1974) pp. 187-197; E. Mayr, Am. Sci. 62, 650 (1974). 4. Each chamber consisted of two concentric boxes constructed of elywood and sheetrock. Wood and acoustic ule baffles between the boxes were designed to be most effective be­ tween 2 and 16 khz. Suppression was greater tested with t-tests. Reliable differences were obtained for the following comparisons: abnor­ mal one or two versus normal song (abnormal 11.04, P < .001; abnormal two: one: t t = 9.95; P < .(01); abnormal versus control songs (abnormal one: t = 8.85, P < .001; abnor­ mal two: t = 19.43, P < .(01); normal versus control song (t = 6.09, P < .(01); the "burble" phrase versus the "tsee" phrase (t = 3.17, P < .05); and either partial song versus the nor­ mal "burble" (t = 4.22, P < .(01). There was no reliable difference between the two samples of abnormal song or between the "tsee" phrase and normal so . ng. Female CB's lack of response the previous year probably resulted from her being housed with a male cowbird, who repeated his sorog to her many times each day, thus raising her threshold for song responsiveness. Furthermore, her com­ panion's song was undoubtedly of superior acoustic quality, a factor that might also have diminished her interest in the recorded songs. G. Gottlieb, Psychol. Bull. 79, 362 (1973). A. King, paper presented at the meeting of the Animal Behavior Society, Wilmington, N.t., 22 to 27 May 1975. D. Eastzer, thesis, Cornell University (1975). We thank G. Wilcox who designed the isolatior chambers, R. Chu and D. Eastzer who helped i the field by collecting eggs and nestlings, R. E�� Johnston for valuable suggestions during the initial stages of this - project, and P. Cabe, W. Dilger, D. Miller, P. Ornstein, and H. Rhein­ gold for helpful comments on the manuscript. We are especially grateful to G. Gottlieb for both technical and conceptual assistance. Request for reprints should be sent to M,J. W. = comparative information exists regard­ ing the responses of other species to iso­ than 39 db at 1000 hertz, and it increased with higher frequencies to greater than 50 db between 8 and 16 khz. The interior box was a I. l-m cube, fabric-lined to reduce sound reflection, lighted by two 40-watt Vita Lite tubes and continuously ventilated. White noise was broadcast in the room housing the chambers. The songs were recorded with a dynamic mien" / phone (Uher 5(7) and tape recorders (Uher-4000). The recordings were played through a driver (JBL 2420) and hom (JBL 2340). Play­ back levels were determined with a sound pres­ sure meter (General Radio 1933). The same sound pressure levels (SPL's) \\ere used for playbacks of the normal and the abnormal songs. At 0.35 m from the speaker along the axis, the SPL was 86 ± 1.5 db slow reading and 104 + 1.5 db impulse. Control songs of the other species were adjusted to the slow reading. The A-weighted SPL inside the chamber was 50 db slow reading. Differences between categories of song were 10. 11. 15 September 1976; revised 15 November 1976 "