the clock drawing test for dementia - Vanderbilt University School of

advertisement
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF GERIATRIC PSYCHIATRY, VOL.
12: 619±627 (1997)
THE CLOCK DRAWING TEST FOR DEMENTIA OF
THE ALZHEIMER'S TYPE: A COMPARISON
OF THREE SCORING METHODS IN A
MEMORY DISORDERS CLINIC
1
HENRY BRODATY1 * AND CRESSIDA M. MOORE2
Professor of Psychogeriatrics, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
2
Research Assistant, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
ABSTRACT
Objectives. To examine the reliability and validity of the Clock Drawing Test when used as a cognitive screening
instrument for mild to moderate dementia, and to compare di€erent scoring mechanisms.
Design. Retrospective analysis of clock drawing performance using three published scoring methods (Shulman,
Sunderland and Wolf-Klein).
Setting. Hospital-based memory disorders clinic.
Participants. A sample of 28 consecutive patients attending the memory clinic for assessment who were given a
diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease (mild or moderate) and 28 age- and sex-matched control subjects comprising
17 memory clinic attenders found to be normal and 11 community volunteers.
Measurements. Sensitivity and speci®city of the three clock rating scales against memory clinic diagnoses of
dementia using DSM-III-R; their respective interrater reliabilities; and comparisons of each with measures of
cognitive impairment (the Mini-Mental State Examination and the Blessed Orientation±Information±Memory±
Concentration Test), daily performance of basic and instrumental activities (the Blessed Dementia Scale) and
depression (the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression).
Results. All methods of scoring the Clock Drawing Test correlated well with measures of cognitive impairment
(r ˆ 0:57±0:73) and daily performance (r ˆ 0:38±0:48), were independent of mild depression and demonstrated high
sensitivity, speci®city and interrater reliability. While all clock scales identi®ed mild to moderate dementia reasonably
well, the Shulman method performed best. In screening for dementia, clock drawing proved superior to the MMSE:
24/28 vs 20/28 cases identi®ed. When compared with the MMSE, clock drawing provided additional diagnostic
discrimination, identifying 7/8 AD patients with MMSE scores 524.
Conclusions. In a clinic population, clock drawing, especially if scored according to the Shulman scale and
combined with the MMSE, is an extremely ecient test screening measure for mild to moderate dementia of the
Alzheimer's type with low false negative and false positive rates. This may have implications for screening elderly
populations. # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Int. J. Geriat. Psychiatry, 12: 619±627, 1997.
No. of Figures: 1. No. of Tables: 3. No. of References: 28.
KEY WORDS ÐAlzheimer's
disease; screening; dementia; clock drawing; reliability
A useful test which is known to re¯ect frontal and
temperoparietal functioning is the easily remembered and quickly administered Clock Drawing
*Correspondence to: Professor H. Brodaty, Academic
Department of Psychogeriatrics, Prince Henry Hospital,
Anzac Parade, Little Bay, Sydney, NSW 2036, Australia.
Tel: 61 2 9382 5007. Fax: 61 2 9382 5016.
CCC 0885±6230/97/060619±09$17.50
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Test (Critchley, 1953). The individual is asked to
draw the face of a clock, mark in the hours and
then draw the hands to indicate a speci®ed time
(for example, 10 past 10). Clock drawing correlates
well with other more detailed and time-consuming
cognitive screens, and has the advantage of
being non-threatening (Shulman et al., 1986). The
e€ects of education remain controversial, with
some researchers questioning the validity of clock
Received 27 November 1995
Accepted 12 April 1996
620
H. BRODATY AND C. M. MOORE
drawing in lowly educated subjects (Ainslie and
Murden, 1993).
The utility of the clock test has been enhanced by
the recent development of a number of scales for
scoring (Table 1). Shulman et al. (1986) found that
their scale of error classi®cation for clock drawings
proved to be acceptably sensitive and speci®c to
cognitive impairment, and was later improved by
including intact clocks (ie no errors) (Shulman
et al., 1993). Sunderland et al. (1989) found nonsigni®cant di€erences between clinicians' and nonclinicians' ratings of clock drawings, and signi®cant correlations with measures of dementia
severity. Wolf-Klein et al (1989) concluded that
the test could be used as a low-cost screening tool
for cognitive impairment associated with AD.
These three clock drawing tests are di€erent. In
the modi®ed Shulman method (Shulman et al.,
1993), subjects are asked to add the numbers of
a clock face to a predrawn circle and to mark in
the hands at 10 after 11. Scores are as follows:
1 `a perfect clock'; 2 `mild visuospatial errors';
3 `errors in denoting the speci®ed time'; 4 `moderate visuospatial disorganisation'; 5 `severe visuospatial disorganisation'; and 6 `no reasonable
representation of a clock'.
The Sunderland method requires subjects to
draw, on a blank sheet of paper, `a clock with all
the numbers on it', and then to put the hands on
the clock to `make it read 2:45'. There are
10 scoring points. Sample anchor points include:
10 `hands are in correct position'; 7 `placement of
hands is signi®cantly o€ course'; 4 `further distortion of number sequence'; and 1 `either no attempt
or an uninterpretable attempt is made'.
In the Wolf-Klein method, subjects are merely
asked to `draw a clock' on a preprinted circle. Their
scoring system has 10 anchor points which pertain
only to the spacing of the numbers; time setting
is not assessed. Sample anchor points include: 10
`normal'; 7 `very inappropriate spacing'; 4 `counterclockwise rotation'; and 1 `irrelevant ®gures'.
Our aim was to evaluate these multiple scales
and scoring methods in a memory disorders clinic
setting in relation to (i) interrater reliability and
(ii) diagnostic accuracy with respect to the sensitivity and speci®city against categorical diagnosis,
using DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) criteria, and dimensional measures
of general cognitive function, activities of daily
living and instrumental activities of daily living.
Further aims of the study were to evaluate (iii) the
relationship between the Clock Drawing Test and
INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL.
12: 599±608 (1997)
more commonly used cognitive screens for
dementia and (iv) whether clock drawing adds
additional diagnostic information to routine
cognitive screening with the Mini-Mental State
Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975).
METHOD
Data were analysed for consecutive referrals to a
memory disorders clinic between 1990 and 1993 for
patients who were aged over 50 years, for whom
sucient data were available for analysis and who
met standard criteria for mild to moderate dementia and Alzheimer's disease (see below). Patients
with severe dementia almost invariably draw clocks
poorly or not at all. Their exclusion conservatively
biased our evaluation of the tests. Two patients
with a Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression
(HRSD) (Hamilton, 1960) score of 16 or above
were excluded from the study, as were patients with
receptive aphasias, visual impairment, vascular
dementia and other dementias. No patient had
motor impairment that prevented clock drawing.
Memory clinic patients and their informants
were assessed by a psychiatric registrar and also, if
indicated, by a psychogeriatrician, a neuropsychiatrist and/or a neurologist. Additionally, patients
were assessed by a neuropsychologist and informants by a social worker. Where indicated, an
occupational therapist assessed the patient at
home. The drawing of a clock was a standard
component of the psychiatric assessment. All
patients were given a blank sheet of paper, asked
to draw a circle, ®ll in the hours of a clock and then
mark in the hands for 10 minutes past 10. None of
the clocks produced by the subjects was incorrect
because of a faulty circle. All clocks were scored
independently by two psychologists using the three
methods. Raters for this study were blind to the
subjects' diagnoses, severity and MMSE scores.
Information derived from clinical assessment,
laboratory investigations, electroencephalography
and neuroimaging were reviewed at a case conference (Brodaty, 1990), where a consensus diagnosis was made using DSM-III-R (American
Psychiatric Association, 1987) and NINCDS±
ADRDA (McKhann et al., 1984) criteria. Scales
relevant to this study comprised those of the
MMSE (Folstein et al., 1975), the Blessed Orientation±Information±Memory±Concentration Test
(OIMC) and Dementia Scales (DS) (Blessed et al.,
1968), as well as the HRSD. Not all the recruited
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
10-point anchored scale
(1 ˆ `either no attempt
or uninterpretable
attempt', 10 ˆ `hands
are in correct position')
Sunderland, Hill,
Mellow, Lawlor,
Gundersheimer,
Newhouse &
Grafman (1989)
10-point categorical scale
based on 10 clinically
derived categories
(1 ˆ `irrelevant ®gures',
10 ˆ `normal')
6-point hierarchical error
classi®cation scale
producing a global
rating of impairment
and including normal
clocks (1 ˆ `perfect',
6 ˆ `no reasonable
representation of a
clock')
Shulman Shedletsky
& Silver (1986);
Shulman, Gold,
Cohen & Zucchero
(1993)
Wolf-Klein,
Silverstone, Levy,
Brod and Breuer
(1989)
Scale type
Researchers
N ˆ 312 consecutive
admissions (mean age
ˆ 76:8, range 58±99).
Sample included:
130 normals, 106 AD,
26 MID, 15 AD ‡ MID,
35 other (mostly
depression)
N ˆ 150; 67 Alzheimer's
patients (mean age ˆ
67:7+9:7), 83 normal
controls (mean age ˆ
73+7:9)
N ˆ 75 (37 males, 38
females), aged 65‡
(mean age 75.5 years).
Sample comprised
approximately equal
groups of organic
mental disorder, major
a€ective disorder and
normal controls
Test sample
Di€erential diagnosis of
dementing diseases using
NINCDS±ADRDA criteria.
Categories 1±6 ˆ cognitive
impairment categories
7, 9, 10 ˆ normal:
Category 8 was excluded
as it had an indeterminate
relationship to mental
categories
AD according to DSM-III-R
and NINCDS±ADRDA
criteria. Clock score 5 6 ˆ
normal, score 56 ˆ
cognitive impairment
MMSE used as a measure of
cognitive impairment score
4 23 ˆ cognitively
impaired (N ˆ 21), score
> 23 ˆ normal (N ˆ 54).
Score on the Clock Drawing
Test of 5 3 ˆ cognitively
impaired, score of
1 or 2 ˆ normal
Criterion standard
Table 1. Summary of scoring mechanisms developed for the Clock Drawing Test
Not done
0.86
0.75
Interrater
reliability
For AD &
AD ‡ MID
subjects,
excluding
category 8,
0.87
For all subjects,
excluding
category 8, 0.68
0.78
0.86
Sensitivity
0.93
0.98
0.96
0.72
Speci®city
THE CLOCK DRAWING TEST FOR DAT
621
INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL.
12: 599±608 (1997)
622
H. BRODATY AND C. M. MOORE
normal controls received the OIMC, DS or the
HRSD (see Table 2). Data were analysed using
intraclass correlation (ICC) (Bartko and Carpenter, 1976), Pearson's product moment correlation,
the chi-square test and Student's t tests.
RESULTS
The sample
The patient group consisted of 28 subjects aged
50 or more who had received a diagnosis of
Alzheimer's disease (AD), and the controls comprised 17 memory clinic referrals with memory
complaints who were found to be normal and 11
normal older subjects with no memory recruited
for another research project within the department
(see Table 2). The two subgroups of control
subjects were found not to di€er signi®cantly on
age, years of education, scores obtained on the
MMSE, OIMC, HRSD or the DS, or in clock
drawing performance on the three scales.
Clock drawing: sex, age and education
Male and female subjects were similar as
regards diagnoses, level of cognitive impairment
or clock drawing ability. Although the AD and
normal control groups were of similar age
(t ˆ 1:64; df ˆ 54, non-signi®cant), analyses were
performed to examine the possible e€ect of
age on clock drawing performance. Age of the
whole sample (AD and normal controls) was
found to correlate signi®cantly with clock drawings scored by the Shulman (r ˆ 0:39; p < 0:01),
Sunderland (r ˆ ÿ3:35; p < 0:01) and WolfKlein (r ˆ ÿ0:29; p < 0:05) scales. When the
AD patients were examined separately, age was
found not to correlate signi®cantly with any of
the three clock scales (Shulman, r ˆ 0:35; Sunderland, r ˆ ÿ0:35; Wolf-Klein, r ˆ ÿ0:29, all nonsigni®cant). When the normal control subjects
were examined separately, a signi®cant correlation
was found only between the Shulman scale and
age (r ˆ 0:41; p < 0:05), although even this did
not remain signi®cant when adjustments for
multiple comparisons were made.
Control subjects were signi®cantly more educated than AD subjects (t ˆ ÿ2:65; p < 0:01).
Correlations between years of education and
clock drawing performance were signi®cant for
both the Shulman (r ˆ ÿ0:35; p < 0:01) and the
Sunderland scale (r ˆ 0:32; p < 0:05), but not
the Wolf-Klein scale (r ˆ 0:20). The possible
INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL.
12: 599±608 (1997)
Table 2. Description of the sample
Alzheimer's disease
Normal controls
28
28
73.1 (8.9)
(50±89)
2
7
13
6
32.1%
69.5 (7.7)
(56±84)
3
9
12
4
25.0%
8.7 (2.6)
6±15
11.3 (4.3)
6±20
14
14
N/A
N/A
MMSE
Mean (SD)
(range)
19.5 (5.3)
(8±29)
28.7 (1.4)
(26±30)
OIMC
Mean (SD)
(range)
23.5 (8.2)
(5±36)
(N ˆ 16)
35.4 (2.7)
(28±37)
Dementia Scale
Mean (SD)
(range)
4.5 (2.9)
(0.5±12.5)
(N ˆ 11)
1.3 (1.9)
(0.0±6.5)
5.3 (4.5)
(0±14)
(N ˆ 17)
6.6 (4.9)
(0±15)
N
Age (years)
Mean (SD)
(range)
50±59
60±69
70±79
80‡
Percentage male
Years of education
Mean (SD)
(range)
Severity of dementia
(DSM-III-R categories)
Mild
Moderate
Hamilton
Mean (SD)
(range)
confound that our normal controls were more
highly educated (F ˆ 3:51; p < 0:05) was examined by analysis of covariance for each of the three
scales. The AD subjects continued to perform
signi®cantly worse than the control subjects on
each of the clock rating scales even when the e€ects
of education were partialled out.
Comparison of the three clock rating scales
The interrater reliabilities of each of the three
scales as measured by the intraclass correlation
coecient (ICC; Ritchie and Fuhrer, 1992) were all
fairly similar and relatively high (Table 3). The
second aim of the study was to compare the
relative sensitivities and speci®cities of the three
scales with respect to clinical diagnosis. The 1993
revised Shulman scale advocated that a score of
42 out of a possible 6 indicated normality, and a
score of 42 indicated cognitive impairment
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
623
THE CLOCK DRAWING TEST FOR DAT
Table 3. Correlation between clock rating scales and other memory clinic measures and assessment of interrater
reliability
Clock rating scale
Shulman et al. (1993)
Sunderland et al. (1986)
Wolf-Klein et al. (1989)
Including category 8
Excluding category 8
MMSE
OIMC
ÿ0:618
0:725
ÿ0:573
0:624
0:580
0:620
0:578
0:631
Dementia scale
(DS)
Hamilton
(HRSD)
Rater 1 vs rater 2
(ICC)
0:483
ÿ0:434
0.062
0.010
0:891
0:924
ÿ0:381
ÿ0:446
0.164
0.203
0:875
N/A
*p < 0:01; **p < 0:001; ***p < 0:0001; signi®cance calculated by F test.
(Ainslie and Murden, 1993; Shulman et al., 1986).
Our results support the use of the 2/3 cut as
representing the best trade-o€ between sensitivity
(0.86) and speci®city (0.96).
The 5/6 cut-point proposed by Sunderland et al.
for use in their clock rating scale produced a
sensitivity of 0.57 and a speci®city of 1.0. Cutpoints of 6/7 and 7/8 resulted in sensitivities of 0.61
and 0.64 and speci®cities of 0.96 and 0.93
respectively. The best result was achieved using
an 8/9 cut: 0.79 sensitivity and 0.93 speci®city.
Assuming that higher scores on the Wolf-Klein
scale represent better performance, the recommended cut-point of 6/7 achieved a sensitivity of
0.36 and a speci®city of 1.0. The best result was
achieved with an 8/9 cut: 0.79 sensitivity and 0.89
speci®city. When we repeated the analyses of the
Wolf-Klein scale, excluding category 8 (`mild
spacing abnormalities'), as recommended by
Wolf-Klein et al. (1989), the results were inferior.
As there were many category 8 scorers in our
sample and exclusion did not improve performance, we used the full Wolf-Klein scale with an 8/9
cut for further analyses.
Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve
analysis (Murphy et al., 1987; Ritchie and Fuhrer,
1992) demonstrating that the Shulman scale had
the highest discriminatory power, followed by the
Sunderland scale and lastly the Wolf-Klein scale
(Fig. 1).
Fig. 1. Receiver operating characteristic curves for the Shulman, Sunderland and Wolf-Klein scales
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL.
12: 599±608 (1997)
624
H. BRODATY AND C. M. MOORE
Clock drawing as a measure of cognitive
impairment
The third issue addressed was the relationship
between the various clock rating scales and other
measures of cognitive impairment. Table 3 shows
the rank order correlations between each of the
three clock rating scales and other memory clinic
assessment measures. Both the MMSE and the
OIMC were found to be highly signi®cantly and
the DS signi®cantly related to all of the three clock
rating scales with minimal di€erences between
them. Not surprisingly, since patients with signi®cant depression were excluded from the study, the
HRSD did not correlate signi®cantly with any of
the three clock rating scales.
Clock drawing: additional diagnostic information?
Twenty of the 28 patients with a diagnosis of
dementia obtained MMSE scores 524. In contrast, the Clock Drawing Test alone identi®ed 24 of
the 28 dementia patients. Of the eight AD patients
with MMSE scores 524, the numbers correctly
identi®ed by the di€erent scales were: seven on the
Shulman scale (2/3 cut); three or ®ve on the
Sunderland scale (5/6 or 8/9 cuts respectively); and
one or seven on the Wolf-Klein scale (6/7 or 8/9
cuts). Of the 20 AD patients with MMSE scores
524, the Shulman 2/3 cut failed to identify three
(false negative rate …FNR† ˆ 15%), the Sunderland 8/9 cut missed three (FNR ˆ 15%) and the
Wolf-Klein 8/9 cut missed ®ve (FNR ˆ 25%).
Clock drawing as a screening instrument
At the optimal cut-points identi®ed in the
current article, there are two choices for determining the e€ectiveness of clock drawing in combination with the MMSE as a cognitive screen in a
memory clinic population. First, persons can be
regarded as cognitively impaired (and at a risk of
dementia) if they fail both the MMSE and the
Clock Drawing Test. This resulted in sensitivities
and speci®cities of 0.40 and 1.0 for the Shulman,
0.50 and 1.0 for the Sunderland and 0.36 and 1.0
for the Wolf-Klein scale. Secondly, they can be
identi®ed by failure on either the MMSE or the
Clock Drawing Test. Sensitivities and speci®cities
for this procedure were 0.96 and 0.96 for the
Shulman, 0.86 and 0.96 for the Sunderland and
0.86 and 1.0 for the Wolf-Klein scale.
INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL.
12: 599±608 (1997)
Benign senescent forgetfulness
As an aside, we were curious whether the controversial diagnosis of benign senescent forgetfulness (BSF; Kral, 1962) could be distinguished from
early dementia by clock drawing. Seven memory
clinic patients were diagnosed at index assessment
and con®rmed at follow-up 1±2 years later to have
stable non-progressive memory impairment without development of other cognitive de®cits. These
patients were compared with only the mild AD
patients (N ˆ 14) using all three clock scales. The
BSF patients did not di€er signi®cantly from
the AD group in terms of their education, age,
gender distribution or Hamilton score. Using the
suggested 2/3 cut-point, the Shulman scale correctly identi®ed ®ve out of seven of the BSF
subjects as normal and 12 out of 14 of the mild AD
as impaired, yielding a sensitivity of 0.71 and a
speci®city of 0.86. The 8/9 cut on the Sunderland
resulted in both a sensitivity and speci®city of 0.86.
Using the 8/9 cut on the Wolf-Klein, the sensitivity
was 0.57 and the speci®city 0.79.
DISCUSSION
While various clock tasks and scoring methods
have demonstrated validity as cognitive screens,
none has hitherto been identi®ed as superior
(Varner et al., 1995). This is the ®rst article to
compare three of the most commonly used and
cited scoring systems, the Shulman, Sunderland
and Wolf-Klein scales.
Whichever scoring system was used, the Clock
Drawing Test was found to be e€ective in identifying mild to moderate dementia in a memory
disorders clinic population and to correlate highly
signi®cantly with two commonly used measures
of cognitive ability and a measure combining
basic and instrumental activities of daily living
(the Blessed Dementia Scale). Additionally, clock
drawing performance was found to be independent
of depressive symptoms as measured by the
Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression. This
replicated the ®ndings of Shulman et al. (1986),
who found depression, as measured by the
Geriatric Depression Rating Scale, to be independent of clock drawing performance. Further,
di€erences in education between our two groups
did not appear to a€ect the Clock Drawing Test's
discriminatory ability.
All three scales had similarly high interrater
reliabilities. However, in this clinic population
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
THE CLOCK DRAWING TEST FOR DAT
there were di€erences in their sensitivities and
speci®cities. The Shulman scale performed best in
terms of (i) replication of the authors' suggested
cut-points, (ii) higher sensitivity and speci®city
values, and (iii) ROC analysis. To obtain acceptable sensitivities and speci®cities with the Sunderland and Wolf-Klein scales, the published cutpoints had to be signi®cantly altered, probably
re¯ecting sampling di€erences. Cut-points are
likely to di€er when instruments are evaluated in
populations with lower prevalence rates of dementia. Also, our study placed more demandings on
the tests in that patients with severe dementia, who
are reasonably easy to identify, were excluded.
Our study supported the argument by Tuokko
et al. (1992) that spacing errors may be a particularly sensitive discriminator between normal and
abnormal cognitive performance. Given the very
high frequency of category 8 scores using the WolfKlein method in those with mild to moderate AD,
the relationship between cognitive impairment and
mild spacing errors may not be as indeterminate as
Wolf-Klein et al. suggested and argues against
their recommendation to eliminate category 8
(`almost normal except for spacing') from scoring.
Whether category 8 is included or not, the ROC
curve demonstrates that the Wolf-Klein scale is not
as impressive as the other two scales examined in
discriminating AD from normality. This may in
part result from its omission of potentially vital
information by not examining time-setting ability,
as the hands are not scored. Time-setting appears
to enhance test sensitivity.
The limitations of the MMSE, the most commonly used screening instrument for dementia, are
well known (Tombaugh and McIntyre, 1992). In
particular, its ceiling characteristics mean that
early dementia can be easily missed. Our results,
albeit in a clinic population, indicate that 87.5% of
those patients with a clinical diagnosis of AD, who
performed within the normal range on the MMSE,
would be correctly identi®ed by the Shulman scale
as being cognitively impaired. This is superior to
the Sunderland scale (62.5%), and vastly superior
to the Wolf-Klein scale (14.3%). Of those subjects
detected by the MMSE and clinically diagnosed as
having dementia, but performing normally on the
Clock Drawing Test, the Shulman and Sunderland
scales had equal and signi®cantly lower false
negative rates than the Wolf-Klein scale.
When used in combination with the MMSE,
the Clock Drawing Test provided additional
diagnostic information. This might be expected,
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
625
as frontal and temporoparietal functions (both
measured by clock drawing) are not well assessed
by the MMSE, despite these functions being
commonly a€ected in AD. In the current study,
not only did the addition of clock drawing to the
MMSE result in a higher true positive rate for
dementia, using DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric
Association, 1987) criteria and consensus diagnosis
as the gold standard, but the addition of the
MMSE to clock drawing also reduced false
negative rates. This suggests that the combination
of MMSE and the Clock Drawing Test may
provide a more powerful screen for cognitive
impairment. The performance of such a combination in an elderly general population sample would
need to be tested, as lower prevalence rates of
dementia signi®cantly alter the properties of any
screening instrument (Weinstein and Fineberg,
1980; Ritchie and Fuhrer, 1992).
Practically, the Shulman scale has, in addition to
the points discussed above, the additional advantage of being only six points (as opposed to 10 with
the Sunderland and Wolf-Klein scales), making it
simpler to score. The slightly more complicated
Sunderland scale has been criticized for assuming
that `the representation of the hands is ®rst and
solely a€ected (score 6 to 10), and additional errors
in the representation of numbers and the clock
face occur later (score 1 to 5), (Rouleau et al.,
1992). A potential advantage of the Sunderland
scale is in relation to BSF patients, where its ability
to discriminate between the mild AD and BSF
patients was marginally superior to that of the
Shulman scale.
We suggest that the limitations to our studyÐ
sample size, lack of poorly educated subjects, lack
of rural subjects and use of a clinic populationÐ
are balanced by our rigorous subject selection
criteria, exclusion of severe cases of dementia and
those with aphasia, careful diagnostic assessment,
use of categorical as well as multiple dimensional
outcome criteria, choice of clinically relevant
normal controls and follow-up of borderline cases.
Longitudinal study of a population at high risk of
developing dementia using a battery of screening
instruments, including serial MMSEs and Clock
Drawing Tests, would be helpful in con®rming the
utility of these tests and their cut-points. Di€erences in optimal cut-points on clock scores from
published reports may re¯ect sampling di€erences or the more rigorous selection of comparative groups here. In a clinical setting where detailed
neuropsychological testing will be performed to
INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL.
12: 599±608 (1997)
626
H. BRODATY AND C. M. MOORE
investigate suspected cognitive de®cits, it may be
preferable to use a lower threshold since clock
drawing may be more useful as a cognitive screen
with higher sensitivity, at the expense of speci®city.
Despite only testing seven cases of BSF, the ability
of the Clock Drawing Test to discriminate between
BSF and AD was impressive.
CONCLUSION
The Clock Drawing Test proved useful in testing
older populations attending a memory disorders
clinic for assessment of dementia. It requires
minimal equipment, is portable, is quick and can
be administered to the hearing impaired (with
written instructions if necessary), the very ill and
the poorly educated (Death et al., 1993). We have
found the Shulman method of scoring easy to
follow and reliable. Clock drawing might prove
especially helpful for screening for signi®cant
cognitive impairment in general practice and
hospital settings (Huntzinger et al., 1992), especially in combination with the MMSE.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Thanks to Natalina Nheu and Georgina Luscombe
who assisted with scoring and the sta€ of the
memory disorders clinic and Noelene Rose for their
help with assessing patients; Kaarin Anstey, Brian
Draper, Kerrie Eyers, Felicia Huppert, Dusan
Hadzi-Pavlovic, Alexandra Walker and the
assessors for helpful comments on earlier drafts.
REFERENCES
Ainslie, N. and Murden, N. (1993) E€ect of education on
the Clock-drawing dementia screen in non-demented
elderly persons. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 41, 249±252.
Allen, N. and Burns, A. (1995) The treatment of
Alzheimer's disease. J. Psychopharmacol. 9(1), 43±46.
American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Third Edition)
Revised. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
Bartko, J. J. and Carpenter, W. T. (1976) On the
methods and theory of reliability. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis.
163(5), 307±317.
Blessed, G., Tomlinson, B. and Roth, M. (1968) The
association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of
elderly subjects. Brit. J. Psychiat. 114, 797±811.
INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL.
12: 599±608 (1997)
Brodaty, H. (1990) Low diagnostic yield in a Memory
Disorders Clinic. Int. Psychogeriat. 2, 149±159.
Brodaty, H., Howarth, G., Mant, A. and Kurrle, S. E.
(1994) General practitioners and dementia: A national
survey of Australian general practitioners. Med. J.
Aust. 160, 10±14.
Critchley, M. (1953, reprinted 1966) The Parietal Lobes.
Hafner, New York.
Death, J., Douglas, A. and Kenny, R. (1993) Comparison of clock drawing with Mini-Mental State Examination as a screening test in elderly acute hospital
admissions. Postgrad. Med. J. 69, 696±700.
Folstein, M., Folstein, S. and McHugh, P. (1975) MiniMental State. A practical method for grading the
cognitive status of patients for the clinician.
J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189±198.
Hamilton, M. (1960) A rating scale for depression.
J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat. 23, 56±62.
Huntzinger, J. A., Rosse, R. B., Schwartz, B. L., Ross,
R. B. and Deutch, S. (1992) Clock drawing in the
screening assessment of cognitive impairment in an
ambulatory care setting: A preliminary report. Gen.
Hosp. Psychiat. 14, 142±144.
Jorm, A. and Henderson, A. (1993) The Problem of
Dementia in Australia, 3rd edn. Aged and Community
Care Division, Department of Health, Housing
and Community Services, Australian Government
Publishing Service, Canberra.
Kral, V. A. (1962) Senescent forgetfulness: Benign and
malignant. Can. Med. Assoc. J. 86, 257±260.
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman,
R., Price, D. and Stadlan, E. M. (1984) Clinical
diagnosis of Alzheimer's Disease: Report of the
NINCDS±ADRDA work group under the auspices
of Department of Health and Human Services Task
Force on Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 34, 939±944.
Murden, R. A., McRae, T. D. and Kaner, S. (1991)
Mini-Mental State Exam scores vary with education in
blacks and whites. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 39, 149±155.
Murphy, J. M., Berwick, D. M., Weinstein, M. C.,
Borus, J. F., Budman, S. H. and Klerman, G. L.
(1987) Performance of screening and diagnostic tests:
Application of receiver operating characteristic analysis. Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 44, 550±555.
Ritchie, K. and Fuhrer, R. (1992) A comparative study
of the performance of screening tests for senile
dementia using receiver operating characteristics
analysis. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 45, 627±637.
Rosenman, S. (1991) The validity of the diagnosis of
mild dementia. Psychol. Med. 21(4), 923±934.
Rouleau, I., Salmon, D. P., Butters, N. et al. (1992)
Quantitative and qualitative analyses of clock drawings in Alzheimer's and Huntingdon's disease. Brain
Cogn. 18, 70±87.
Shulman, K., Gold, D., Cohen, C. and Zucchero, C.
(1993) Clock-drawing and dementia in the community: A longitudinal study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiat.
8, 487±496.
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
THE CLOCK DRAWING TEST FOR DAT
Shulman, K., Shedletsky, R. and Silver, I. (1986)
The challenge of time: Clock-drawing and cognitive
function in the elderly. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiat. 1,
135±140.
Sunderland, T., Hill, J., Mellow, A., Lawlor, B. A.,
Gundersheimer, J., Newhouse, P. A. and Grafman, J.
H. (1989) Clock drawing in Alzheimer's disease: A
novel measure of dementia severity. J. Am. Geriatr.
Soc. 37, 725±729.
Tombaugh, T. N. and McIntyre, N. J. (1992) The MiniMental State Examination: A comprehensive review.
J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 40, 922±935.
Tuokko, H., Hadjistavropoulos, J., Miller, A. and
Beattie, B. (1992) The Clock Test: A sensitive measure
# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
627
to di€erentiate normal elderly from those with
Alzheimer's disease. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 40(6), 579±
584.
Varner, R. V., Chen, Y. W., Gruber, N. P. and Lesser, J.
(1995) Clock drawing versus the Pfei€er Short
Portable Mental Status Questionnaire (SPMSQ) in a
geriatric psychiatry clinic. Presented at the American
Association for Geriatric Psychiatry, Eighth Annual
Meeting, Cancun, Mexico, February 17±20.
Weinstein, M. D. and Fineberg, H. V. (1980) Clinical
Decisions Analysis. W. B. Saunders, Philadelphia.
Wolf-Klein, G., Silverstone, F., Levy, A. and Brod, M.
(1989) Screening for Alzheimer's disease by Clock
Drawing. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 37, 730±734.
INT. J. GERIAT. PSYCHIATRY, VOL.
12: 599±608 (1997)
Download