“Not just right experiences”: perfectionism, obsessive– compulsive

Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
www.elsevier.com/locate/brat
“Not just right experiences”: perfectionism, obsessive–
compulsive features and general psychopathology
Meredith E. Coles a,∗, Randy O. Frost b, Richard G. Heimberg a, Josée Rhéaume c
a
Adult Anxiety Clinic of Temple, Department of Psychology, Temple University, Weiss Hall, 1701 North 13th Street,
Philadelphia, PA 19122-6085, USA
b
Department of Psychology, Smith College, Northampton, MA 01063, USA
c
Department of Psychiatry, Hôtel-Dieu de Lévis, Quebec, Canada
Accepted 27 February 2002
Abstract
Individuals with obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) frequently report uncomfortable sensations of
things not being just right (“not just right experiences”; NJREs) and a need to ritualize until they quiet
these sensations. However, very little work has been conducted to empirically examine the nature and
characteristics of this phenomenon. In this paper, we present two studies that systematically examine NJREs
in large undergraduate samples. In Study 1, NJREs were shown to be related to both OCD features and
maladaptive domains of perfectionism. In Study 2, NJREs were again significantly related to OCD features.
Further, NJREs were shown to have particularly strong relationships with specific OCD symptom clusters
(e.g., checking, ordering). Finally, NJREs were significantly more strongly correlated with features of OCD
than symptoms from other domains of psychopathology (e.g., social anxiety, trait anxiety, worry,
depression). General conclusions are drawn, and future directions are discussed.
 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Not just right experiences; Obsessive-compulsive features; Perfectionism
1. Introduction
Individuals with obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) frequently report uncomfortable sensations of things not being just right. Further, these individuals often report feeling driven to
perform an action until this uncomfortable sensation can be reduced, to achieve a sensation of
∗
Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: mcoles@temple.edu (M.E. Coles); rfrost@smith.edu (R.O. Frost); heimberg@temple.edu (R.G. Heimberg).
0005-7967/02/$ - see front matter  2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00044-X
682
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
things being “just right”. Indeed, Rasmussen and Eisen (1992, p. 756) state that many of their
patients with OCD describe “an inner drive that is connected with a wish to have things perfect,
absolutely certain, or completely under control”. Until this sense of perfection is achieved, patients
may be plagued with a “not just right experience” (NJRE).
Clinicians have long observed NJREs among clients with OCD. In 1903 Janet wrote of “an
inner sense of imperfection” and described the experience as follows: “they feel that actions that
they perform are incompletely achieved, or that they do not produce the sought-for satisfaction”
(in Pitman, 1987b, p. 226). More recent theorists have also discussed feelings of things not being
just right or sensations of imperfection. For example, the cybernetic model of OCD (Pitman,
1987a) is based on control systems theory, which treats behavior as a process of matching perceptual input to internal reference signals. The cybernetic model proposes that the core problem in
OCD is a mismatch between the perceptual input and internal reference signals (i.e., expectations).
Further, it proposes that repetitive and stereotyped behaviors (i.e., compulsions) result from efforts
to reduce these mismatch signals. Contemporary cognitive behavioral models (e.g., Rachman,
1997) view many manifestations of OCD as characterized by catastrophic misinterpretations of
thoughts. Just as research has shown that most people sometimes experience intrusive thoughts
(Rachman & de Silva, 1978), it is also likely that most people sometimes experience NJREs, or
mismatches between input and expectations. Further, in both cases (intrusive thoughts and
NJREs), the interpretations of these experiences may vary considerably between individuals and
dictate their emotional response. If these experiences are interpreted as catastrophic, the result
would likely be anxiety.
Despite its presence in the theoretical and clinical literature on OCD, there have been very few
attempts to directly measure the phenomenon of NJREs. One research group that has devoted
attention to this endeavor is that of Leckman and colleagues, who have conducted a number of
studies examining “just right” perceptions (and, by implication, not just right perceptions as well)
in individuals with OCD (with and without comorbid tic disorders; Cohen & Leckman, 1992;
Leckman, Walker, & Cohen, 1993; Leckman, Walker, Goodman, Pauls, & Cohen, 1994; Leckman
et al., 1995; Miguel et al., 2000). They have asked patients a series of questions concerning just
right experiences: whether they have ever been or were currently aware of a need to perform
compulsive acts until they are “just right”, whether this awareness was more mental or physical
in nature, and at what particular moment (before starting, or during, compulsions) they became
aware of the need for things to be “just right”. Leckman et al. (1994) found that a large proportion
of 177 individuals with OCD (with or without a comorbid tic disorder) reported the occurrence
of just right perceptions at some point in their illness (92%) and many reported the occurrence
of just right perceptions during the past week (73%). Further, the occurrence of just right experiences was positively related to the severity of OCD features, with those reporting the occurrence
of just right experiences in the past week having more severe symptom ratings. Individuals with
OCD with and without a comorbid tic disorder were not found to differ on the occurrence of just
right experiences. In a more recent study, 63% of 40 individuals with OCD with or without
Tourette’s Disorder endorsed repetitive behaviors preceded by feelings of things not being just
right (Miguel et al., 2000).
While the work of Leckman’s group has been groundbreaking, these studies are limited in a
number of ways. First, their measure was a single item dichotomously reporting the presence or
absence of just right perceptions. This method of measurement is unlikely to be sensitive and
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
683
may present only a crude indication of the presence or impact of NJREs. Second, these studies
were conducted solely in clinical samples, and it is important to determine whether NJREs occur
in non-clinical samples as well. Comparisons of NJREs reported by persons with OCD and by
non-anxious persons is particularly important given findings that most individuals experience
obsessions, but that clinical and non-clinical samples can be distinguished by the frequency and
other characteristics of the obsessions (e.g., duration, intensity, and consequences; Rachman &
de Silva, 1978). Third, previous studies of NJREs have not addressed the possible relationship
between NJREs and perfectionism. This is an important area for study, as some authors have
proposed that NJREs may represent a specific form of “sensation-based perfectionism” that has
a unique relationship with OCD. For example, Pitman (1987a) proposed that many perfectionistic
OCD behaviors are attempts to match sensations with expectations. To date, empirical studies of
perfectionism (mainly focusing on evaluation of performance) and OCD have failed to find specific types of perfectionism that distinguish OCD from other disorders (for a review, see Frost,
Rhéaume, & Novara, 2002). However, whether sensory perfectionism, in the form of NJREs,
represents a form of perfectionism specific to OCD has not yet been empirically examined.
Other empirical studies have addressed NJREs less directly than Leckman’s group. NJREs and
sensations of imperfection have frequently been discussed in empirical work on subgroups of
individuals with OCD (e.g., Calamari, Wiegartz, & Janeck, 1999) and groupings of OCD symptoms (e.g., Baer, 1994; Summerfeldt, Richter, Antony, & Swinson, 1999). For example, in
explaining why their “certainty” subgroup had more severe symptoms than the four other clusters
on various measures of checking, doubting, and slowness, Calamari et al. (1999) argued that these
findings were consistent with “this subgroup’s core concerns involving a need to be absolutely
certain to prevent a negative outcome or certainty for the sake of knowing or producing a just
right feeling unrelated to outcome” (pp. 122–123, emphasis added). Summerfeldt et al. (1999)
found that symmetry obsessions with and without magical thinking displayed substantially different magnitudes of relationship with a symmetry and exactness factor (r ⫽ 0.25 and 0.48,
respectively). Based on clinical observation, Summerfeldt et al. (1999) argued that, despite their
overt similarity, obsessions with and without magical thinking are distinct. They proposed that
this is primarily due to disparate motivations, specifically “harm avoidance in the case of symmetry obsessions with magical thinking versus a need to have things ‘just right’ in the case of
those without” (p. 308). Finally, in his identification of symptom subtypes in OCD, Baer (1994)
noted that the largest factor in his factor analysis of the Yale–Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale
(Y-BOCS) Symptom Checklist, symmetry/hoarding, was composed of symptoms reminiscent of
Janet’s description of patients who are bothered by an inner sense of imperfection.
1.1. The current studies
In summary, while NJREs have been frequently noted by OCD theorists and frequently
observed both clinically and in empirical work, there have been only limited attempts to directly
measure the phenomenon and study its role in OCD. The studies presented herein sought to further
examine the nature of NJREs and their relationships to other variables of importance in large
non-clinical samples. Study 1 examined the relationship of NJREs to features of OCD and domains
of perfectionism. Study 2 examined the relationship of NJREs to various types of OCD features
and other domains of psychopathology.
684
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
2. Study 1
The sample consisted of 119 female introductory psychology students who received course
credit for participating. Participants were freshman and sophomores at a small private liberal arts
college for women. The ethnic composition of the college was predominantly Caucasian, and the
majority of students came from middle to upper class socioeconomic backgrounds.
2.1. Measures
2.1.1. NJRE-Questionnaire (NJRE-Q)
The NJRE-Q begins by defining NJREs as “times when you have the subjective sense that
something isn’t just as it should be”. Further NJREs are defined as having their roots in the
individual, i.e., an unsettled feeling because something in the individual or in their perception
that the world around them is not right. After the definition and a series of examples,1 13 NJREs
are presented (see Fig. 1), and respondents are asked to report whether they had this experience
in the past week or ever. The 13 NJREs were developed based on clinical experience, input from
clients with OCD, suggestions from individuals with OCD via the internet, and pilot work. Efforts
were made to include NJRE situations that were not from typical domains of OCD concerns to
avoid complete overlap with OCD symptom content. After rating the occurrence of the 13 NJREs,
respondents then identified which NJRE had occurred most frequently in their life and rated it
on 22 additional dimensions. These ratings examined frequency (two items), intensity (four items),
importance (five items), and anxiety (six items). For each of these sets of items, we calculated
an average item score by summing the scores for all items and then dividing this total by the
Fig. 1. NJREs presented in the NJRE-Q used in Study 1.
1
Examples provided were as follows: feeling that you might have forgotten something, a strange physiological feeling that you
don’t recognize, the feeling that you get when you think you haven’t done something perfectly enough, feeling unsafe or vulnerable
for no recognizable reason, and feeling that objects aren’t arranged in just the right way.
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
685
number of items in the set. A single question asked respondents to indicate whether the NJRE
was (a) more like a mental awareness, (b) more like a body feeling, (c) both a mental awareness
and a body feeling, or (d) more like something else. Another item asked respondents what they
said to themselves when they experienced the feeling: (a) nothing, (b) that it was a strange experience without any importance, or (c) that it must mean there was something wrong or incomplete.
One item assessed the degree to which the respondent believed that things needed to be perfect
(from 0 ⫽ not at all to 4 ⫽ extremely strong). Another item assessed what participants did in
response to the sensation: (a) nothing, left it alone, (b) distracted myself, (c) started worrying
about it, (d) concretely did some action to correct it, or (e) did something else . And, finally,
one item asked respondents to indicate to what extent they believed that a bad mood at the time
(e.g., feeling down, anxious or irritable) had something to do with the NJRE (from 0 ⫽
not at all to 4 ⫽ totally).
2.1.2. Padua Inventory (PI)
The original PI (Sanavio, 1988) is a 60-item self-report inventory assessing four factors related
to obsessive–compulsive behavior: impaired mental control, checking, loss of control of actions,
and contamination. Each item is rated from 0 ⫽ not at all disturbing to 4 ⫽
very much disturbing. Sanavio (1988) found the PI to have good psychometric properties, including strong internal consistency (a ⫽ 0.90 for males, 0.94 for females) and good 30-day retest
reliability (r ⫽ 0.78 for males, 0.83 for females). Finally, the PI total score has been shown to
discriminate individuals with OCD from individuals with neurotic disorders (Sanavio, 1988).
Since the publication of the original version of the PI, many other versions have also been used.
For example, the 39-item PI–Washington State University Revision (Burns, Keortge, Formea, &
Sternberger, 1996), the 41-item Padua Inventory–Revised (van Oppen, Hoekstra, & Emmelkamp,
1995), and the 40-item abridged version (Rhéaume et al., 2000). The 40-item abridged version
was used in the current study.
2.1.3. Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (MPS)
The MPS (Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990) is a 35-item self-report scale developed
based on a multidimensional conception of perfectionism. The scale is composed of six subscales:
(1) concern over mistakes, (2) doubts about actions, (3) personal standards, (4) parental expectations, (5) parental criticism, and (6) organization. An overall perfectionism score can also be
calculated by summing all items except those from the organization subscale. Frost et al. (1990)
found internal consistency to be excellent (a ⫽ 0.90) for the overall perfectionism score and to
range from good to excellent for the subscales (a ⫽ 0.77 to 0.93). Further, the overall perfectionism score was significantly correlated with other measures of perfectionism.
2.1.4. Perfectionism Questionnaire (PQ)
The PQ (Rhéaume, Freeston, & Ladouceur, 1995) was developed based on a unidimensional
conception of perfectionism and is intended to distinguish between functional and dysfunctional
perfectionism. The scale is composed of 64 ratings across four different domains. The perfectionistic tendencies scale (10 items) assesses the degree to which one has high personal standards and
feels an inner drive to strive for perfectionism. The domains scale (22 items) assesses the extent
to which the respondent is perfectionistic across various domains of his or her life, such as work,
686
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
studies, and health. The demandingness scale (8 items) assesses how demanding the respondent
is towards various groups of people such as his or her children, colleagues, and strangers. Finally,
the disadvantages subscale (24 items) assesses the negative consequences of perfectionism such
as stress, discomfort, and avoidance. The PQ has been shown to have good construct validity in
a sample of 800 university students. The subscales of the PQ also have demonstrated good internal
consistency (based on three subscales, collapsing the domains and demandingness subscales above
into one domains subscale: perfectionistic tendencies a ⫽ 0.82, domains affected by perfectionistic behavior a ⫽ 0.88, and negative consequences of perfectionism a ⫽ 0.96) (Rhéaume et al.,
2000).
2.2. Results
2.2.1. Scale characteristics
To examine how well the occurrence ratings for the 13 NJREs held together, we computed
indices of the internal consistency of these scales using the Kuder–Richardson Formula #20 (KR20). These analyses revealed that the 13 items held together fairly well (past week a ⫽ 0.57,
ever a ⫽ 0.64). We also examined the cohesiveness of the items assessing the four domains of
associated features of the NJREs. These analyses revealed strong cohesiveness of the items: frequency scale a ⫽ 0.93, intensity scale a ⫽ 0.86, importance scale a ⫽ 0.76, and anxiety scale
a ⫽ 0.86.
2.2.2. Prevalence and nature of NJREs
Focusing first on NJREs experienced within the past week, 95% of the sample reported having
experienced at least one of the 13 NJREs. The percentage of participants reporting specific NJREs
within the past week ranged between 9% and 51% [mean (M) ⫽ 27%]. The NJRE most commonly reported in the past week was “When setting the alarm, I have had the feeling it wasn’t
done just right”. Turning to NJREs experienced at any time in the participants’ lives, 99% of the
sample reported having ever experienced at least one of the 13 NJREs. The percentage of participants reporting specific NJREs at any time in their lives ranged between 44% and 69% (M ⫽
59%). The NJRE most commonly reported as having ever occurred was “I have had the inexplicable feeling that something bad or out of the ordinary was about to happen”.
To better understand the characteristics of these NJREs, we then examined the mean scores
for the associated ratings (e.g., intensity, importance, etc.). All ratings were made on a scale from
0 (indicating the minimal level of that symptom) to 4 (indicating the maximum level of that
symptom). For the frequency items, 0 indicated never and 4 indicated once a day or within the
past day. The average item score for the frequency scale was 3.23 [standard deviation (SD) ⫽
0.73], suggesting that respondents experienced NJREs almost daily. The average item score for
the intensity scale (M ⫽ 1.35, SD ⫽ 0.87) revealed that participants reported experiencing
between “a little” and “moderate” intensity/distress associated with the NJRE. Regarding importance, the average item score (M ⫽ 1.31, SD ⫽ 0.77) revealed that participants attributed “a little”
to “moderate” importance to the NJRE. Finally, participants reported nearly moderate anxiety
(M ⫽ 1.86, SD ⫽ 0.92) associated with the NJRE.
Regarding the mental vs. physical nature of the sensations, 62.2% of participants reported that
their most frequent NJRE was “more like a mental awareness”, 29.4% reported the experience
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
687
as “both a mental awareness and a body feeling”, and only 7.6% reported the experience as “more
like a body feeling”. Regarding what respondents said to themselves when they experienced the
feeling, 49.1% responded “that it must mean there was something wrong or incomplete”, 27.6%
responded that they said “nothing”, and 23.3% responded that they said “that it was a strange
experience without any importance”. In response to the NJRE experience, 52.1% of participants
reported that they “concretely did some action to correct it” whereas only 16.8% reported that
they did “nothing”.2 The item assessing the degree to which the respondent believed that things
needed to be perfect revealed that participants responses averaged to a level between “1 ⫽
a little strong” and “2 ⫽ moderately strong” (M ⫽ 1.82, SD ⫽ 1.17). And, finally, the response
to an item assessing the extent to which respondents believe that a bad mood at the time had
something to do with the NJRE revealed that participants’ responses averaged to a level between
“1 ⫽ a little” and “2 ⫽ moderately” (M ⫽ 1.32, SD ⫽ 1.30).
2.2.3. Relationship of NJREs to OCD features
To examine the relationship of NJREs to OCD features, we correlated NJRE ratings with the
total and subscale scores of the PI (see Table 1). First, we correlated the total number of NJREs
in the past week (total NJREs PW) and the total number of NJREs endorsed as having ever
occurred (total NJREs ever) with the PI. The total NJREs PW was significantly correlated with
the PI total score and all subscale scores. However, the correlation of total NJREs PW with the
PI contamination subscale was much smaller in magnitude [r(119) ⫽ 0.29, P ⬍ 0.001] than the
correlation of total NJREs PW with the other subscales (r ⫽ 0.41 to 0.46) and significantly
smaller than its correlation with the PI total score [r(119) ⫽ 0.49, P ⬍ 0.001; test of dependent
correlations, z ⫽ 3.32, P ⬍ 0.001]. Interestingly, total NJREs ever showed negligible correlations
with the PI total or subscale scores (r ⫽ 0.04 to 0.20). In further contrast to the findings for the
past week, total NJREs ever was found to be significantly correlated only with the PI contamination subscale at an alpha level of 0.05 [r(119) ⫽ 0.20, P ⫽ 0.026], although the magnitude of
this correlation was small.
Given the apparent discrepancy in the magnitude of correlations for the total NJREs in the past
week as compared to ever, we decided to test whether the magnitude of these correlations differed
significantly. To this end, we conducted tests of dependent correlations comparing the correlations
of the two NJRE total scores with the PI total and each of the four PI subscales. Indeed, the PI
total score and three of the four subscales (all except contamination; z ⫽ 0.74, P ⫽ 0.23) were
significantly more strongly correlated with the total NJREs in the past week than with the total
NJREs ever.3
Next, we correlated the associated NJRE ratings, including the four average item scores, with
the PI. As can be seen in Table 1, all four average item scores were significantly correlated with
the PI total score (r ⫽ 0.34 to 0.45). Examination of the PI subscale scores revealed that all four
average item scores were significantly correlated with the impaired mental control, checking, and
2
It is also interesting to note that individuals who reported that they “concretely took some action to correct it” had significantly
higher PI total scores than those who reported that they did “nothing” [M (SD): 34.87 (20.77) and 22.65 (13.04), respectively,
t(80) ⫽ 2.47, P ⫽ 0.016]. Along similar lines, ratings of the intensity of the need to correct the NJRE were significantly correlated
with the PI total score (r ⫽ 0.26, N ⫽ 118, P ⫽ 0.005).
3
Further details of these analyses are available from the authors.
35.60 (22.00)
14.53 (9.29)
8.38 (5.89)
4.29 (4.88)
8.12 (6.76)
N ⫽ 119.
∗
P ⬍ 0.05.
∗∗
P ⬍ 0.01.
∗∗∗
P ⬍ 0.001.
10 to 50
0 to 110
0 to 40
24 to 120
(7.56)
(16.62)
(6.23)
(23.67)
30.18
66.05
16.48
68.09
160
48
32
36
40
Perfectionism Questionnaire
Perfectionistic Tendencies
Domains of Perfectionism
Demandingness
Disdadvantages
to
to
to
to
to
104.83 (21.81)
22.76 (7.96)
24.92 (5.58)
15.34 (4.95)
9.18 (4.25)
11.45 (3.95)
21.49 (5.69)
0
0
0
0
0
M (SD)
Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale
Overall Perfectionism
29 to 145
Concern over Mistakes
9 to 45
Personal Standards
7 to 35
Parental Expectations
5 to 25
Parental Criticism
4 to 20
Doubts about Actions
4 to 20
Organization
6 to 30
Padua Inventory
Total
Impaired Mental Control
Checking
Loss of Control of Actions
Contamination
Scale range
0.22∗∗
0.33∗∗∗
0.31∗∗∗
0.42∗∗∗
0.37∗∗∗
0.29∗
0.19∗
0.22∗
0.28∗∗
0.39∗∗∗
0.19∗
0.49∗∗∗
0.46∗∗∗
0.42∗∗∗
0.41∗∗∗
0.29∗∗∗
Past week
Total NJREs
0.00
0.21∗
0.12
0.10
0.09
0.09
0.08
0.09
0.09
0.09
0.07
0.09
0.04
0.07
⫺0.06
0.20∗
Ever
0.12
0.17
0.03
0.15
0.12
0.15
0.17
0.15
0.19∗
0.17
⫺0.08
0.35∗∗∗
0.30∗∗∗
0.36∗∗∗
0.23∗∗
0.20∗
0.29∗∗∗
0.25∗∗
0.39∗∗∗
0.41∗∗∗
0.40∗∗∗
0.46∗∗∗
0.36∗∗∗
0.15
0.23∗
0.36∗∗∗
0.10
0.45∗∗∗
0.45∗∗∗
0.44∗∗∗
0.39∗∗∗
0.15
Frequency Intensity
0.27∗∗
0.31∗∗∗
0.39∗∗∗
0.34∗∗∗
0.38∗∗∗
0.36∗∗∗
0.32∗∗∗
0.20∗
0.27∗∗∗
0.32∗∗∗
0.13
0.37∗∗∗
0.35∗∗∗
0.35∗∗∗
0.35∗∗∗
0.14
Importance
0.19∗
0.21∗
0.24∗
0.23∗
0.31∗∗∗
0.19∗
0.26∗
0.15
0.27∗∗
0.26∗
0.22∗
0.34∗∗∗
0.30∗∗∗
0.32∗∗∗
0.37∗∗∗
0.14
Anxiety
Table 1
Study 1: Scale ranges, means (M), standard deviations (SD), and correlations of NJRE ratings with measures of OCD features and perfectionism
688
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
689
loss of control of actions subscales (r ⫽ 0.23 to 0.45). However, only the NJRE frequency average
item score was shown to be significantly correlated with the contamination subscale of the PI [
r(119) ⫽ 0.20, P ⬍ 0.05]. Of the individual items rating associated features of the NJREs, the
item that was most strongly correlated with the PI total score was the item assessing the extent
to which the NJRE interfered with ongoing activities [r(119) ⫽ 0.44, P ⬍ 0.001].
2.2.4. Relationship of NJREs to domains of perfectionism
To examine the relationship of NJREs to domains of perfectionism, we correlated the NJRE
scores with the two measures of perfectionism (see Table 1). First, we correlated total NJREs
PW and total NJREs ever with the MPS and PQ. Total NJREs PW was significantly correlated
with all subscales and the overall perfectionism score of the MPS (r ⫽ 0.19 to 0.40). Total NJREs
PW was also significantly correlated with all four subscales of the PQ (r ⫽ 0.22 to 0.42). Of
these four subscales, total NJREs PW was most strongly correlated with the disadvantages subscale, which assesses the negative consequences of perfectionism. Once again, we found much
weaker relationships for total NJREs ever. Indeed, of all the subscales of the MPS and the PQ
along with the MPS overall perfectionism score, total NJREs ever was only significantly correlated
with the PQ domains of perfectionism subscale. Although statistically significant, this correlation
was modest [r(119) ⫽ 0.21, P ⬍ 0.03].
Next, we correlated the four NJRE average item scores with the two measures of perfectionism.
The intensity, importance, and anxiety domains for the NJREs were significantly correlated with
the MPS overall perfectionism score (see Table 1). Examination of the subscales of the MPS
revealed the strongest correlations for the intensity and importance of NJREs with the concern
over mistakes, doubts about actions, and personal standards subscales (r ⫽ 0.32 to 0.46). While
various other correlations were significant, they were all weaker in magnitude (all other correlations were ⬍0.30). The intensity and importance of the NJREs showed the strongest correlations
with the PQ subscales. Indeed, the intensity and importance ratings showed significant relationships with all four subscales of the PQ (r ⫽ 0.25 to 0.41). The anxiety ratings for the NJREs
showed significant, but weaker, correlations with the four PQ subscales (r ⫽ 0.20 to 0.24).
Finally, the frequency ratings for the NJREs were not significantly related to any PQ subscales
(r ⫽ 0.03 to 0.17).
2.3. Discussion
What conclusions can be drawn from this first study? First, unselected students experience
NJREs. Indeed, the large majority of participants (95%) endorsed having experienced at least one
of the NJREs presented within the past week, and nearly all participants (99%) reported having
experienced at least one of the NJREs at some time. Second, these results provide support for a
significant relationship between NJREs and OCD. Both the occurrence of NJREs within the past
week and the experiences associated with these NJREs (intensity, importance, and anxiety) were
related to overall levels of OCD features. Further, an examination of types of OCD features
revealed that NJREs may be more strongly related to some types of OCD symptoms than others.
For example, the NJRE ratings were more strongly related to control and checking features of
OCD than to contamination concerns. Finally, there was also a meaningful relationship between
NJREs and perfectionism. More specifically, the occurrence of NJREs within the past week and
690
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
the intensity and importance of these NJREs were meaningfully related to maladaptive domains
of perfectionism (e.g., concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, and disadvantages of
perfectionism).
One notable finding was the striking difference between results for the total NJREs experienced
in the past week versus the total NJREs ever experienced. For all correlations with measures of
OCD features and perfectionism, total NJREs PW showed a stronger correlation than total NJREs
ever. Indeed, the magnitude of correlations for total NJREs ever were generally negligible (r ⫽
0.00 to 0.21), whereas those for total NJREs PW were generally of moderate magnitude (r ⫽
0.19 to 0.49). These findings are particularly important as this is the first study to examine NJREs
in a non-clinical sample. Further, it appears that the interpretation of the intensity and importance
of these experiences, and not their frequency, is related to the presence of OCD symptoms. Indeed,
the intensity and importance ratings (not frequency) showed the highest correlations with levels
of OCD features. Consistent with cognitive–behavioral models of OCD (Rachman, 1997), our
findings suggest that it is not merely the presence or absence of NJREs that is relevant to OCD,
but instead that it is the interpretations of these experiences (e.g., interpretations of intensity and
importance). These findings are also consistent with the findings of Rachman and de Silva (1978)
that normal obsessions are common and that they resemble abnormal obsessions in both form and
content. However, normal and abnormal obsessions differ in duration, intensity, and consequences.
Our findings regarding the relationship of NJREs to OCD features are clearly consistent with
existing models and previous empirical work. It is also worthwhile to briefly consider how our
findings regarding perfectionism and NJREs fit with previous work that has examined the relationship between perfectionism and OCD. Two studies with non-clinical samples have found Concern
over Mistakes and Doubts about Actions to be positively correlated with total scores on selfreport measures of OCD features (Frost et al., 1990; Rhéaume, Freeston, Dugas, Letarte, &
Ladouceur, 1995). These findings are notably similar to the relationships found in the current
study between the subscales of the MPS and the occurrence of NJREs. Specifically, in the current
study, the total NJREs PW was most strongly correlated with the Doubts about Actions subscale
followed by the Concern over Mistakes subscale.
Finally, it is important to consider the limitations of this first study. Although the sample was
large (N ⫽ 119), the generalizablility of the findings is limited by the homogeneous nature of the
sample (unselected female undergraduates). Studies employing more heterogeneous samples (e.g.,
individuals of both genders, from more varied ethnic backgrounds, and both with and without
OCD) would serve to increase generalizability. Another limitation is that the measure of OCD
features represented only limited content domains. A more comprehensive measure of OCD features would allow for further examination of the relationship of NJREs to specific types of OCD
features. And, finally, other domains of psychopathology (e.g., depression, social anxiety) were
not examined in this study, thereby limiting conclusions regarding the specificity of the relationship between NJREs and OCD features in particular.
3. Study 2
One question not adequately addressed by the first study is whether NJREs are differentially
related to specific types of OCD features. First, based on the work of Summerfeldt et al. (1999)
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
691
and Baer (1994), we predicted that NJREs would be strongly correlated with symptoms of symmetry or ordering. Second, based on the discussion of Calamari et al. (1999), we predicted that
NJREs would be strongly correlated with checking and doubting. To test these hypotheses, Study
2 used a different measure of OCD features that provides a more comprehensive profile of OCD
features than the PI used in Study 1. Further, in addition to the benefits of the subscales of this
new measure of OCD features, use of this scale’s total score allowed us to replicate the findings
of a relationship between OCD features and NJREs found in Study 1.
Another important question not answered by Study 1 is whether NJREs represent a specific
form of perfectionism that has a unique relationship to OCD features. As discussed in the Introduction, previous studies have failed to demonstrate a unique relationship between specific forms of
perfectionism and OCD. Previous studies have shown various domains of perfectionism (e.g.,
doubts about actions, concern over mistakes) to be related to OCD, but these same domains have
also been shown to be related to many other forms of psychopathology (see Frost et al., 2002).
Therefore, the lack of specificity of this relationship has limited the utility of these findings.
However, NJREs may represent a specific form of perfectionism that characterizes features of
OCD, and Study 2 was designed to examine the specificity (discriminant validity) of the NJRE
phenomenon. We included measures of various other forms of psychopathology (e.g., trait anxiety,
worry, depressive symptoms, social anxiety) that could reasonably be related to NJREs but that
were predicted to show weaker correlations with NJREs than would OCD features.
In addition, we utilized information gathered in Study 1 to refine and streamline our measure
of NJREs. First, the questionnaire was revised to ask respondents to rate the occurrence of specific
NJREs within the past month, instead of in the past week and ever. This change was guided by
the findings of Study 1 that total NJREs PW was more strongly correlated with OCD features
and domains of perfectionism than total NJREs ever. Therefore, for the revised questionnaire, we
decided to use a discrete amount of time, but broadened the duration slightly (increased from one
week to a month) to avoid situational fluctuations that may occur in the very short period of a
week. Second, the number of associated ratings performed for the targeted NJRE was significantly
reduced. In the revised form, respondents make seven associated ratings for the most recent NJRE
(compared to 22 in Study 1). The content of these ratings was guided by the findings of Study
1 and attempts to measure the key aspects of the NJREs while avoiding redundant items.
Finally, in Study 2, data were gathered from a more diverse sample. The sample for Study 2
consisted of 242 introductory psychology students who participated in partial fulfillment of course
requirements. Their age ranged from 17 to 47 years, with a mean of 19.73 years (SD ⫽ 3.17).
Most students (226) were single, two were married, three reported living with partners, one was
divorced, one was separated, and the remainder failed to report marital status. As in Study 1, the
majority of participants were freshmen (n ⫽ 121) and sophomores (n ⫽ 67), along with some
juniors (n ⫽ 36), and 18 others. In contrast to Study 1, participants in the current study represented
both genders (161 females, 75 males, and six participants who did not report gender). Participants
were also from diverse ethnic backgrounds. Of the 212 participants reporting race, 85 were of
European descent, 59 were African–American, 26 were Asian or Asian–American, 12 were Hispanic, and 30 reported mixed heritage.
692
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
3.1. Measures
3.1.1. Revised NJRE Questionnaire (NJRE-QR)
The NJRE-QR4 is a 19-item self-report questionnaire. The first 10 items present sample NJREs
(see Fig. 2) and ask respondents to indicate whether or not they experienced each NJRE within
the past month. After rating the occurrence of each NJRE, respondents are asked to indicate which
NJRE occurred most recently and when it last occurred (from within past few hours to within
past month). Then, thinking of that particular NJRE, respondents are asked to complete seven
ratings (see Table 2).
3.1.2. Obsessive Compulsive Inventory (OCI)
The OCI (Foa, Kozak, Salkovskis, Coles, & Amir, 1998) is a 42-item self-report measure of
obsessive features. Each item is rated on 5-point (0–4) Likert-type scales for (a) the frequency
of the symptom, and (b) the severity of the associated distress. Individuals with OCD have been
shown to have significantly higher OCI distress and frequency total scores than individuals with
social phobia or post traumatic stress disorder, and controls (Foa et al., 1998). Further, OCI scores
are strongly correlated with other self-report measures of obsessive features (r ⫽ 0.65 to 0.81).
Internal consistency for the full scale is good (a ⫽ 0.86 to 0.95), and both the distress and frequency total scores have demonstrated good retest reliability in both OCD and control samples
(r ⫽ 0.84 to 0.90). Further support for the strong psychometric properties of the OCI in undergraduate samples is provided by Simonds, Thorpe, and Elliott (2000).
3.1.3. Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)
The BDI (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979) is a 21-item self-report scale, which focuses
primarily on the cognitive symptoms of depression. Each item is rated on a scale of 0 to 3 with
higher scores indicative of greater severity. The BDI exhibits good internal consistency in both
Fig. 2. NJREs presented in the NJRE-QR used in Study 2.
4
A copy of the NJRE-QR is available from the authors.
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
693
Table 2
Study 2: Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) for the associated ratings performed on the NJRE-QR and correlations
of these ratings with the OCI frequency and distress total scores
Average item
scores, M (SD)
Correlations with OCI totals
Frequency
Distress
FREQUENCY: How frequently did you experience this NJRE?
4.50 (2.14)
0.30
0.27
INTENSITY: How intense was this NJRE?
2.72 (1.52)
0.43
0.38
IMMEDIATE DISTRESS: To what degree did this NJRE bother
you or cause you distress AT THE TIME?
2.45 (1.51)
0.36
0.35
DELAYED DISTRESS: To what degree did this NJRE bother
you or cause you distress LATER THAT SAME DAY?
1.89 (1.26)
0.37
0.39
RUMINATION To what extent did you feel that you couldn’t
get this NJRE out of your mind?
1.80 (1.26)
0.41
0.44
URGE TO RESPOND: To what extent did you have an urge to
do something about this NJRE?
2.56 (1.64)
0.39
0.40
RESPONSIBILITY: To what extent did you feel that it was your 2.48 (1.63)
responsibility to do something about this NJRE?
0.35
0.36
All ratings shown ranged from 1 to 7, with 1 indicating absence of that symptom to 7 indicating extreme levels of
that symptom (e.g., 1 ⫽ not intense at all to 7 ⫽ extremely intense). For the frequency item, 1 indicated never and 7
indicated within the past day.All correlations P ⬍ 0.001.
psychiatric and non-psychiatric samples (mean a ⫽ 0 .86 and 0.81, respectively) and good concurrent validity with other measures of depression (Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988). For a detailed
discussion of the use of the BDI over the past 25 years, see Beck, Steer, et al. (1988)).
3.1.4. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory-Trait form (STAI-T)
The STAI-T (Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983) is a 20-item self-report
measure of the tendency to perceive stressful situations as dangerous or threatening and the tendency to respond to these situations with increases in state anxiety. Participants are asked to rate
each item on a 4-point Likert-type scale from 1 (“almost never”) to 4 (“almost always”) to indicate
how they generally feel. Retest reliability for the STAI-T has been shown to be high, ranging
from 0.73 to 0.86, and validity correlations with other anxiety questionnaires range from 0.73 to
0.85 (Spielberger et al., 1983).
3.1.5. Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI)
The BAI (Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, 1988)5 is a 21-item self-report measure of anxiety
symptoms developed to improve discrimination between symptoms of depression and anxiety.
5
Due to a clerical error in the preparation of packets, the BAI was only administered to a portion of the current sample (n ⫽
153). The distribution of these packets along with complete packets was random; therefore, it is believed that the subsample of those
completing the BAI is representative of the larger sample.
694
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
The BAI instructs participants to rate each item as to how much the symptom has bothered them
in the past week. Ratings are made on a scale from 0 ⫽ not at all to 3 ⫽ severely. The BAI has
been shown to have high short-term retest reliability and internal consistency (Beck, Epstein, et
al., 1988) as well as strong convergent and discriminant validity (Fydrich, Dowdall, & Chambless,
1992). In a large undergraduate sample (N ⫽ 326), Creamer, Foran, and Bell (1995) found the
BAI to have strong internal consistency (a ⫽ 0.91). Retest reliability over a period of approximately a month was shown to be modest (r ⫽ 0.62), consistent with the conceptualization of the
BAI as a state measure.
3.1.6. Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (B-FNE)
The B-FNE (Leary, 1983) is a 12-item self-report inventory designed to assess cognitions that
characterize social anxiety. The B-FNE is distinguished from the original FNE scale (Watson &
Friend, 1969) in two main ways: (1) it contains 12 items while the original scale contains 30,
and (2) item ratings for the B-FNE are made on a Likert-type scale from 1 (“not at all characteristic
of me”) to 5 (“extremely characteristic of me”), whereas item ratings for the original scale are
made using a true–false format. The B-FNE has been shown to have excellent inter-item reliability
(a ⫽ 0.92) and retest reliability (r ⫽ 0.75; Elting & Hope, 1995). The brief and full versions of
the FNE are highly correlated (r ⫽ 0.96; Leary, 1983).
3.1.7. Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ)
The PSWQ (Meyer, Miller, Metzger, & Borkovec, 1990) is a 16-item self-report inventory
designed to assess trait worry and to capture the generality, excessiveness, and uncontrollability
characteristic of pathological worry (e.g., “My worries overwhelm me”; “I worry all the time”).
Each item is rated on a 1 (“not at all typical of me”) to 5 (“very typical of me”) Likert-type
scale. In undergraduate samples, the PSWQ has repeatedly demonstrated good internal consistency
and good retest reliability over intervals as long as 8–10 weeks (Meyer et al., 1990). Individuals
with a principal diagnosis of Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD) have been shown to score
higher on the PSWQ than community controls and patients with panic disorder, panic disorder
with agoraphobia, social phobia, simple phobia, or OCD (Brown, Antony, & Barlow, 1992).
Molina and Borkovec (1994) provide a more detailed review of the psychometric properties of
the PSWQ.
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Scale characteristics
To examine the cohesiveness of the 10 items assessing the occurrence of NJREs, we computed
the internal consistency of the scale using the KR-20 Formula. This revealed that the 10 NJREs
presented had strong internal consistency (a ⫽ 0.79). Further, there were no cases in which
deletion of an item would improve the internal consistency of the scale. Therefore, we computed
a total score for the occurrence of the NJREs to use in subsequent analyses.
3.2.2. Prevalence and nature of NJREs
Eighty per cent of the sample reported having experienced at least one of the 10 NJREs within
the past month. The mean number of NJREs endorsed as having occurred within the past month
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
695
was 3.26 (SD ⫽ 2.7). The percentage of participants reporting specific NJREs within the past
month ranged from 18% to 54% (M ⫽ 33%). The most frequently endorsed NJRE was “When
talking to people, I have had the sensation that my words did not sound just right”, and the least
frequently endorsed NJRE was “When putting a bill or letter into a mailbox, I have had the
sensation that the way I placed the envelope in the mailbox and closed the door didn’t feel
just right”.
3.2.3. Ratings of characteristics of NJREs
In addition to reporting the occurrence of NJREs within the past month, participants also completed a series of ratings for the NJRE that they had experienced most recently (see Table 2). As
a whole, these ratings tended to be low. This suggests that while these college students did experience NJREs, on average, they were not associated with much distress or other aversive experiences. However, as can be seen by an examination of the standard deviations, there was variability
in the ratings, suggesting that some individuals may experience higher levels of associated symptoms.
We also examined the relationship of these ratings to OC features (see Table 2). Each item was
correlated with the OCI frequency and distress totals. Overall, these correlations were generally in
the moderate range (r ⫽ 0.27 to 0.44). Due to the small amount of variability in the magnitude
of these correlations, we did not test their statistical difference. However, it may be worth noting
that the item assessing the frequency of the NJRE consistently showed the smallest magnitude
of correlation with the OCI totals.
3.2.4. Relationship of NJREs to types of OCD features
We also sought to examine whether the occurrence of NJREs was differentially related to
various types of OC features. To examine this question we correlated the NJRE total score with
the seven subscales of the OCI (see Table 3). These correlations of NJREs to types of OCD
features were all moderate to strong in magnitude (r ⫽ 0.44 to 0.60). Based on previous work,
we had predicted that the checking, ordering, and doubting subscales would show particularly
strong correlations with NJREs. Visual examination of the correlations revealed that, of the OCI
subscales, the checking subscale consistently showed the strongest correlations with NJREs. The
Table 3
Study 2: Correlations of total NJREs with frequency and distress scores for various domains of OCD features
OCI—washing
OCI—checking
OCI—doubting
OCI—ordering
OCI—obsessing
OCI—hoarding
OCI—neutralizing
OCI, Obsessive Compulsive Inventory.
Total NJREs correlated with OCI
frequency
Total NJREs correlated with OCI
distress
0.49
0.60
0.57
0.58
0.50
0.46
0.51
0.47
0.55
0.49
0.55
0.48
0.49
0.44
696
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
ordering and doubting subscales were also consistently among the top three or four largest correlations between NJREs and subscales of the OCI.
To test the statistical significance of differences between the correlations we computed a composite correlation for each type of OCD symptom. This was calculated by combining the two
correlations for each OCI subscale (frequency and distress) via Fisher Z transformations. We then
compared the magnitude of these composite correlation coefficients for various domains of OCD
features.6 Based on our predictions we tested whether the correlation of NJREs with the checking
composite score was significantly larger than correlations of NJREs with domains of OCD features
that have not been frequently discussed in the literature on NJREs (washing, obsessing, hoarding,
neutralizing). The correlation of NJREs with the checking composite score (frequency and distress
of checking) was found to be significantly larger than the correlations of NJREs with the washing,
obsessing, hoarding, and neutralizing composite scores (all P ⬍ 0.02). The composite ordering
correlation was also found to be significantly more strongly correlated with total NJREs than was
washing, obsessing, hoarding, or neutralizing (all P ⬍ 0.05). The correlation of NJREs with the
doubting composite score was not shown to be significantly larger than the correlations of NJREs
with the composite washing, obsessing, or hoarding scores (all P ⬎ 0.10), but did show a trend
towards being larger than the correlation with neutralizing (P ⫽ 0.09).
3.2.5. Relationship of NJREs to OCD features vs. relationship to other forms of
psychopathology
To examine the specificity of NJREs to OCD, we compared the relationships of the NJRE total
scores with measures of OC features to their relationship with the measures of other domains of
psychopathology (trait anxiety, worry, social anxiety, depressive symptoms). As can be seen in
Table 4, total NJREs was strongly correlated with both total scores of the OCI. Further, total
NJREs remained significantly correlated (all P ⬍ 0.001) with both the frequency and distress of
Table 4
Study 2: Scale ranges, means (M), standard deviations (SD), and correlations with the total NJREs
OCI-F total
OCI-D total
BDI
STAI-T
BAI
Brief FNE
PSWQ
Scale range
M (SD)
n
Correlations with total
NJREs
0 to 168
0 to 168
0 to 63
20 to 80
0 to 63
12 to 60
16 to 80
34.51 (27.2)
24.43 (24.9)
6.45 (7.5)
42.07 (10.9)
10.13 (10.4)
34.25 (9.3)
47.90 (13.9)
242
242
242
241
153
239
241
0.63
0.57
0.33
0.29
0.45
0.21
0.35
OCI-F total, Obsessive Compulsive Inventory total frequency score; OCI-D total, Obsessive Compulsive Inventory
total distress score; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; STAI-T, State Trait Anxiety Inventory-Trait Form; BAI, Beck
Anxiety Inventory; Brief FNE, Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale; PSWQ, Penn State Worry Questionnaire. For
the correlations with the OCI totals all P ⬍ 0.001, all other correlations P ⬍ 0.01.
6
Further details of these analyses are available from the authors.
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
697
OCD symptoms when controlling for levels of trait anxiety (controlling for STAI-T,
r ⫽ 0.58 and 0.51, respectively; controlling for BAI, r ⫽ 0.54 and 0.43, respectively), worry
(r ⫽ 0.56 and 0.49, respectively), social anxiety (r ⫽ 0.60 and 0.54, respectively), and depressive
symptoms (r ⫽ 0.57 and 0.50, respectively). Consistent with our predictions, the correlations of
the NJRE total score with measures of other domains of psychopathology were weaker
(r ⫽ 0.21 to 0.45), although significant (all P ⬍ 0.01). Tests of dependent correlations revealed
that the NJRE total score was significantly more strongly correlated with the OCI total frequency
and the OCI total distress scores than with any of the other symptom measures (all P ⬍ 0.02).7
4. General discussion
What conclusions can be drawn from this second study? First, we again found that a substantial
percentage of the sample reported experiencing at least one NJRE within the given time frame
(in this case, one month). The percentages of the sample endorsing at least one NJRE were lower
than those found in Study 1. However, this may be a result of a more limited set of NJREs to
choose from, as Study 2 presented only 10 NJREs in each set and Study 1 presented 13. The
current study also replicated our finding of a significant relationship between NJREs and levels
of OC features, and went beyond Study 1 in showing that there is specificity in the relationship
between total NJREs and the various types of OC features. Consistent with predictions based on
previous empirical work, the OC features that tended to show the strongest correlations with
NJREs were checking, ordering, and doubting. The symptoms that tended to show weaker correlations with NJREs were washing, obsessing, hoarding, and neutralizing. Finally, the results of
Study 2 suggest a unique relationship of NJREs to features of OCD versus other types of psychopathology. NJREs were significantly more strongly correlated with levels of OC features than
with several other symptom domains. Thus, NJREs may represent a specific form of perfectionism
that has a unique relationship to OC features. This finding stands in contrast to previous studies
that failed to demonstrate a unique relationship between perfectionism and OC features.
What can we learn from the differential relationship of NJREs to specific domains of OC
features? First, the consistency of our findings with previous discussions linking NJREs to particular domains of OC features lends further credence to the viability and usefulness of measuring
NJREs in a self-report format. Our findings are clearly consistent with empirical work on subgroups of individuals with OCD (Calamari et al., 1999) and groupings of OC features (Baer,
1994; Summerfeldt et al., 1999). Our results are also consistent with the findings of Ferrari (1995)
that there was a significant positive correlation between perfectionistic cognitions and compulsive
checking, but not with compulsive washing. In addition to considering the types of OC features
that showed the strongest correlations with NJREs, it is also worthwhile to consider those that
showed smaller, but still substantial, correlations. For example, while washing was typically
ranked as one of the smaller correlations among the symptom domains, the magnitude of correlations of this subscale with NJREs was not negligible (r ⫽ 0.47 and 0.49). Further, it is possible
that some of these more modest correlations may have been diminished compared to some of the
7
Further details of these analyses are available from the authors.
698
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
others because such symptoms may occur for various reasons. For example, washing may be
motivated by an attempt to reduce the likelihood of a feared outcome (e.g., washing to remove
germs and thereby prevent illness) or motivated by an attempt to achieve a sensation of one’s
skin feeling just right (e.g., to remove a tacky or sticky feeling from the hands). This line of
thought is consistent with the work of Tallis (1996), who proposes that there are some patients
for whom washing is not to remove contamination, but rather to achieve a perfect state of cleanliness. Based on this distinction, Tallis (1996) proposes that the second type of washing may be
more closely linked to perfectionism. In summary, it is possible that further precision in distinguishing the motivations behind symptoms may reveal more precise relationships of the various
types of OC symptoms and NJREs.
Distinguishing between symptoms motivated to prevent a feared consequence versus those motivated by reducing an NJRE may also have important implications for treatment. Compulsions
are typically conceptualized as attempts to reduce anxiety by decreasing the probability of a feared
consequence. For example, checking the knob of a stove is commonly conceptualized as an
attempt to confirm that the stove is off and thereby prevent a fire. However, these same compulsions could be motivated by a desire to reduce the anxiety caused by NJREs, to decrease a sense
of uneasiness that something in the surroundings is out of place at that time. For example, the
individual who repeatedly turns the knob of the stove may not necessarily be fearful of a fire,
but may need to repeatedly turn it until the position of the knob in their hand “feels right”.
Considering the possibility of compulsions as attempts to relieve NJREs may serve to improve
therapy outcomes. In fact, Foa, Abramowitz, Franklin, and Kozak (1999) found that OCD patients
who did not articulate feared consequences showed a trend towards having higher (i.e., more
severe) post-treatment symptom scores than those who had articulated feared consequences. It is
possible that those patients who did not articulate feared consequences were distressed by NJREs.
In other words, experiencing the NJRE was their feared consequence. While typical exposures
are likely to elicit NJREs in such clients, specifically designing exposure exercises with the goal
of eliciting an NJRE may improve outcomes.
What are the next steps? First, it will be important to conduct empirical work examining the
NJRE phenomenon in clinical samples. These findings should then be compared to those from
non-clinical samples. Second, it is important to further assess whether the construct of NJREs is
separable from other constructs such as obsessional doubting, obsessions, or OCD symptoms more
broadly. It is reasonable to propose separate constructs of NJREs and doubts about actions, based
on different apparent temporal scales and differences in their physical vs. cognitive nature. However, more work is needed to clarify the boundaries between these similar constructs. Distinctions
between feelings of something not being just right and feared consequences could be incorporated
explicitly into future questionnaire items. For example, respondents could be asked whether they
perform an action (e.g., checking their alarm clock) because (a) I was afraid that if I didn’t check
it something negative or bad would happen (e.g., my alarm clock wouldn’t go off and I would
oversleep) or (b) I had an uncomfortable sensation in my body that something wasn’t just right.
Finally, it will be important to further develop our conceptual model of NJREs. As we gather
more information about NJREs, we can better articulate their relationship with obsessions and
compulsions, what triggers may produce them, and what can be done to reduce the discomfort
that they produce.
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
699
Acknowledgements
Portions of this paper were presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Advancement
of Behavior Therapy, New Orleans, November 2000. The authors would like to thank Karen
Bogert for her pilot work examining NJREs, which facilitated the development of the studies
presented herein.
References
Baer, L. (1994). Factor analysis of symptom subtypes of obsessive compulsive disorder and their relation to personality
and tic disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 55(Suppl), 18–23.
Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric
properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 893–897.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: twentyfive years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100.
Brown, T. A., Antony, M. M., & Barlow, D. H. (1992). Psychometric properties of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire
in a clinical anxiety disorders sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 33–37.
Burns, G. L., Keortge, S. G., Formea, G. M., & Sternberger, L. G. (1996). Revision of the Padua Inventory of Obsessive
Compulsive Disorder Symptoms: distinctions between worry, obsessions, and compulsions. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 34, 163–173.
Calamari, J. E., Wiegartz, P. S., & Janeck, A. S. (1999). Obsessive–compulsive disorder subgroups: a symptom clustering approach. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 113–125.
Cohen, A. J., & Leckman, J. F. (1992). Sensory phenomena associated with Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. Journal
of Clinical Psychiatry, 53, 319–323.
Creamer, M., Foran, J., & Bell, R. (1995). The Beck Anxiety Inventory in a non-clinical sample. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 33, 477–485.
Elting, D. T., & Hope, D. A. (1995). Cognitive assessment. In R. G. Heimberg, M. R. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R.
Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment (pp. 232–258). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Ferrari, J. R. (1995). Perfectionistic cognitions with nonclinical and clinical samples. Journal of Social Behavior and
Personality, 10, 143–156.
Foa, E. B., Abramowitz, J. S., Franklin, M. E., & Kozak, M. J. (1999). Feared consequences, fixity of belief, and
treatment outcome in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Behavior Therapy, 30, 717–724.
Foa, E. B., Kozak, M. J., Salkovskis, P. M., Coles, M. E., & Amir, N. (1998). The validation of a new obsessive
compulsive disorder scale: the Obsessive Compulsive Inventory (OCI). Psychological Assessment, 10, 206–214.
Frost, R. O., Marten, P. A., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy
and Research, 14, 449–468.
Frost, R. O., Rhéaume, J., & Novara, C. (2002). Perfectionism. In R. O. Frost, & G. Steketee (Eds.), Cognitive
approaches to obsessions and compulsions: Theory, assessment, and treatment. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
Fydrich, T., Dowdall, D., & Chambless, D. L. (1992). Reliability and validity of the Beck Anxiety Invenotry. Journal
of Anxiety Disorders, 6, 55–61.
Leary, M. R. (1983). A brief version of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 9, 371–375.
Leckman, J. F., Grice, D. E., Barr, L. C., de Vries, A. L. C., Martin, C., Cohen, D. J., McDougle, C. J., Goodman, W.
K., & Rasmussen, S. A. (1995). Tic-related vs. non-tic-related obsessive compulsive disorder. Anxiety, 1, 208–215.
Leckman, J. F., Walker, D. E., & Cohen, D. J. (1993). Premonitory urges in Tourette’s syndrome. American Journal
of Psychiatry, 150, 98–102.
Leckman, J. F., Walker, D. E., Goodman, W. K., Pauls, D. L., & Cohen, D. J. (1994). “Just right” perceptions associated
with compulsive behavior in Tourette’s syndrome. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 675–680.
700
M.E. Coles et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 41 (2003) 681–700
Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn State
Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 487–495.
Miguel, E. C., Rosario-Campos, M. C., Prado, H. D., Valle, R., Rauch, S. L., Coffey, B. J., Baer, L., Savage, C. R.,
O’Sullivan, R. L., Jenike, M. A., & Leckman, J. F. (2000). Sensory phenomena in obsessive–compulsive disorder
and Tourette’s Disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 61, 150–156.
Molina, S., & Borkovec, T. D. (1994). The Penn State Worry Questionnaire: psychometric properties and associated
characteristics. In G. C. L. Davey, & F. Tallis (Eds.), Worrying: Perspectives on theory, assessment and treatment
(pp. 265–283). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.
Pitman, R. K. (1987a). A cybernetic model of obsessive–compulsive psychopathology. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 28,
334–343.
Pitman, R. K. (1987b). Pierre Janet on obsessive–compulsive disorder (1903): review and commentary. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 44, 226–232.
Rachman, S. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 793–802.
Rachman, S., & de Silva, P. (1978). Abnormal and normal obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 16, 233–248.
Rasmussen, S. A., & Eisen, J. A. (1992). The epidemiology and clinical features of obsessive compulsive disorder.
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 15, 743–758.
Rhéaume, J., Freeston, M. H., Dugas, M. J., Letarte, H., & Ladouceur, R. (1995). Perfectionism, responsibility and
obsessive–compulsive symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 785–794.
Rhéaume, J., Freeston, M. H., & Ladouceur, R. (1995). Functional and dysfunctional perfectionism: construct validity
of a new instrument. Paper presented at the meeting of the World Congress of Behavioural and Cognitive Therapies,
Copenhagen, Denmark, July, 1995.
Rhéaume, J., Freeston, M. H., Ladouceur, R., Bouchard, C., Gallant, L., Talbot, F., & Vallières, A. (2000). Functional
and dysfunctional perfectionists: are they different on compulsive-like behaviors? Behaviour Research and Therapy,
38, 119–128.
Sanavio, E. (1988). Obsessions and compulsions: the Padua Inventory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 26, 169–177.
Simonds, L. M., Thorpe, S. J., & Elliott, S. A. (2000). The Obsessive Compulsive Inventory: psychometric properties
in a nonclinical student sample. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 28, 153–159.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait
Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press.
Summerfeldt, L. J., Richter, M. A., Antony, M. M., & Swinson, R. P. (1999). Symptom structure in obsessive–compulsive disorder: a confirmatory factor analytic study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 297–311.
Tallis, F. (1996). Compulsive washing in the absence of phobic and illness anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
34, 361–362.
van Oppen, P., Hoekstra, R. J., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (1995). The structure of obsessive–compulsive symptoms.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 15–23.
Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 448–457.