DRUG NFO clearinghouse www.druginfo.adf.org.au PreventionResearchQuarterly March 2010 ISSN 1832-6013 Drugs and driving Prevention Research Quarterly ISSN 1832-6013 © DrugInfo Clearinghouse 2010 This publication is copyright, but its contents may be freely photocopied or transmitted, provided the authors are appropriately acknowledged. Copies of this publication must not be sold. The Issues Paper and the Reading and Resource List are part of the Druglnfo Clearinghouse’s quarterly publications on drug prevention. Other publications and resources include the newsletter DrugInfo and a range of fact sheets tailored for specific audiences, such as professionals and others working in the drug prevention and related sectors, teachers, students, parents and others with an interest in drug prevention. The quarterly publications usually provide a range of perspectives on current research and best practice relating to a central theme in drug prevention. All these publications are available for download. The Druglnfo Clearinghouse provides a first port of call for workers, professionals and others seeking information on drugs and drug prevention. You can sign up for free membership at the DrugInfo website, or by visiting, telephoning or writing to: Druglnfo Clearinghouse Australian Drug Foundation 409 King Street, West Melbourne, Victoria 3003 Australia Tel: 1300 85 85 84 (Victoria only) Email: druginfo@adf.org.au Web: www.druginfo.adf.org.au Any enquiries or comments on this publication should be directed to the Program Manager (Information Services), at the above address. The research in this publication represents work done on behalf of the DrugInfo Clearinghouse by Professor Con Stough and Rebecca King. The work of the authors was supported by a Reference Group that included key stakeholders: Richard Colbran, National Innovation & Sector Support Manager, Australian Drug Foundation Mark Durran, Manager, Information Services, Australian Drug Foundation Stephen McConchie, Senior Policy Officer, Mental Health and Drugs Division, Department of Health Chantelle Miller, Senior Policy Officer, Drugs Policy and Services Branch, Department of Human Services Robyn Ramsden, Sector Development Manager, Information Services, Australian Drug Foundation. DrugInfo Clearinghouse is an initiative of the Australian Drug Foundation and the Victorian Government. Contents Drugs and driving Issues Paper no. 12 Reading and Resource List no. 30 1 24 Issues Paper DRUG No. 12 • March 2010 NFO clearinghouse www.druginfo.adf.org.au The role of alcohol and other drugs in road deaths and serious injuries Professor Con Stough and Rebecca King, Drugs and Driving Research Unit, Brain Sciences Institute, Swinburne University, Melbourne, Victoria The total number of people killed on Australian roads in 2006 was 1601, with a further 31 204 people suffering serious injuries.1,2 Many factors are believed to contribute to traffic accidents, including drug and alcohol use, fatigue and speed. This paper examines the impact of alcohol and other drug use, including various pharmaceuticals, on road deaths and injuries. It also outlines a number of strategies that are currently being implemented to minimise and reduce the number of drivers who consume drugs. Prevention strategies include current education activities in schools, advertising campaigns and information provided by health professionals. Detecting drug driving, and treatment and education following the loss of licence due to drug driving, are also discussed. All of these approaches work together to reduce the number of individuals who drive under the influence of drugs. In addition to a review of relevant literature, the views of key informants who have professional experience in a range of fields (alcohol and other drug service delivery, road safety and research) were sought to inform this paper. Introduction Alcohol has long been recognised as a major contributor to fatal road accidents. In 2008 28 per cent of all drivers killed on Victorian roads had a blood alcohol concentration (BAC) of 0.05g/100mL (0.05 per cent) or higher.3 Alcohol significantly increases a driver’s risk of a serious road crash,4 with drivers over 0.05 per cent BAC being more than twice as likely to be responsible for the accident than a driver who is drug- and alcohol-free.5 Figure 1 shows the number of drivers and motorcyclists killed between 1987 and 2008 with BAC levels over 0.05 per cent. Over one in 10 (13.4 per cent) Australians aged 14 years and older report illicit drug use within the past 12 months.6 The most commonly used illicit drug in Australia is cannabis, followed by ecstasy (methylenedioxymethamphetamine, MDMA) and methamphetamine. Although the use of cannabis has declined since 1998, it has been used recently by close to one in 10 Australians (9.1 per cent) and is still a serious road safety concern. The use of synthetic drugs such as ecstasy, amphetamine and methamphetamine is steadily increasing around Australia with over one in 10 (12.0 per cent) 20–29 year olds reporting use within the past 12 months. These types of synthetic drugs are gaining popularity and are used predominantly by young people and truck drivers.7 Due to the increasing number of Australians using illicit drugs there is growing concern over their involvement in traffic accidents and fatalities. Road statistics indicate that drug-related road accidents and deaths have increased dramatically during the past 15 years. From 1990 to 1993, 22 per cent of all Victorian drivers killed tested positive for the presence of drugs other than alcohol (9.6 per cent involved cannabis, 3.9 per cent amphetamines and other stimulants, 4.5 per cent benzodiazepines and 3.3 per cent opioids). This percentage has increased to 33 per cent in 2004 (12.6 per cent involved cannabis, 4.9 per cent amphetamines and other stimulants, 4.4 per cent benzodiazepines and 7.1 per cent opioids).3,8–10 These statistics are similar to those reported in other countries. For instance, in Washington, data have revealed that the incidence of impairing drugs found in fatally injured drivers has increased significantly Issues Paper | No. 12 | March 2010 1 140 120 100 80 60 40 20 0 1987 1988 1989 1990 1991 1992 1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 0.05 to 0.10 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 0.101 to 0.15 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 Over 0.15 Figure 1. Number of drivers and motorcyclists killed with a blood alcohol concentration of 0.05g/100mL or over (TAC, 2009) during the past decade; from 25 per cent in 1991 to 35 per cent in 2001.11 In particular, fatally injured drivers who tested positive for methamphetamine increased a staggering 200 per cent. Schwilke suggests that this is reflective of the general growth in popularity of this class of illicit substance.11 The number of fatally injured drivers in the United Kingdom who test positive to illicit drugs has also risen from 3 per cent in the mid-1980s to 18 per cent in 1999.12 France also saw a significant increase in the presence of impairing drugs in fatally injured drivers between 2001 and 2004, with cannabis increasing from 16.9 per cent to 28.9 per cent, cocaine from 0.2 per cent to 3.0 per cent and amphetamines from 1.4 per cent to 3.1 per cent.13 These statistics demonstrate the significant impact that drug driving has on road safety. Analysis of drivers injured in road traffic accidents also reveals the significant impact of drugs on road safety. A study that screened blood samples taken from 436 injured drivers attending the Alfred Emergency and Trauma Centre in Prahran, Victoria, reported that cannabis metabolites were found in 46.7 per cent of injured drivers, with 7.6 per cent testing positive for delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), the active form of cannabis. The next highest drug group detected was benzodiazepines in 15.6 per cent of cases, 2 Drugs and driving followed by opiates in 11 per cent, amphetamines in 4.1 per cent, methadone in 3 per cent and cocaine in 1.4 per cent. Polydrug use was also reported in 9.4 per cent of cases with the most common combination being benzodiazepines and opiates, followed by benzodiazepines and cannabis.14 Fatigue alone is also a serious road safety concern. VicRoads estimate that around 20 per cent of fatal road accidents involve driver fatigue. Many studies have shown that moderate sleep deprivation of 17 to 19 hours produces driving impairments that are equivalent to or worse than those seen at 0.05 per cent BAC,15–18 while impairments observed at 20 to 24 hours of sleep deprivation have been shown to be equivalent to 0.1 per cent BAC.15,16 When fatigue is combined with substance use such as alcohol or ecstasy, driving impairments are even greater than with either alone.19–21 Assigning responsibility for accidents Some studies have moved beyond stating the mere prevalence of drugs in samples taken from fatally or seriously injured drivers by using methods to assign culpability or responsibility for each accident. This allows us to better understand whether one type of drug is more impairing than another. Fatal accidents In the first of a series of studies, Drummer collected data for 1045 drivers killed.8 Culpability or responsibility was determined according to the mitigating factors (independent of drug analysis), and drivers were classified as “culpable”, “contributory”, or “not culpable”. The mitigating factors used in the analyses were the condition of the road and vehicle, driving conditions, type of accident, witness observations, road law obedience, difficulty of the task involved and the level of fatigue.22 The proportion of drivers classified as “culpable” (odds ratio, OR) was calculated for each drug type condition. The large majority (73 per cent) of drivers in the sample as a whole were classified as culpable, while only 18 per cent were not culpable. The highest culpability ratio for any drug alone was found for alcohol. Culpability ratios were considerably higher for all drugs in combination with alcohol, except opiates. More recently, a large study across Victoria, New South Wales and South Australia revealed that drivers who tested positive to psychoactive drugs were more likely to be responsible for a fatal accident.5 The highest culpability ratio for a single drug was again alcohol. Drivers with a BAC <0.05 per cent were 1.2 times more likely to be responsible for the accident, while drivers with a BAC over 0.20 per cent were found to be 24 times more likely to be responsible. Drivers intoxicated with cannabis were also found to be at an increased risk of causing the fatal accident, with THC concentrations over 5ng/ mL resulting in a culpability ratio of 6.6 (this is a similar ratio to drivers with a BAC of >0.15 per cent). Drivers also frequently combined alcohol and cannabis, further increasing the risk of a fatal accident. Stimulants significantly increased a driver’s risk of a serious road crash, particularly in truck drivers (culpability ratio of 8.8), while benzodiazepines and opiates were found to have a weak relationship with crash risk. Non-fatal accidents Responsibility analysis has also been used to determine crash risk for injured drivers.4 Drivers testing positive to alcohol only, benzodiazepines only, and the combination of alcohol and cannabis and alcohol and benzodiazepines, were significantly more likely to be regarded as responsible for the accident in comparison with those found to be drug- and alcohol-free. However, unlike results reported by Drummer,5 drivers who tested positive to cannabis in this study were not found to have an increased crash risk. One explanation for the difference in findings is that lower concentration levels of THC were reported in Longo’s study compared to Drummer’s. Preliminary data from an ongoing Victoria Police and Swinburne University study conducted in Melbourne examining responsibility in injured drivers by Ogden et al. have revealed that alcohol again had the highest culpability ratio for a single drug; at less than 0.05 per cent BAC drivers were 4.7 times more likely to be responsible for the accident than drug- and alcoholfree drivers.23 This increased to an OR of >6 for those drivers who were only a little bit over (0.05–0.08 per cent BAC), while drivers with a BAC of over 0.20 per cent were found to be 20 times more likely to be responsible. The most common illicit drug detected was cannabis with 18 per cent of drivers testing positive. Drivers detected with any level of THC had only a slightly larger crash risk than drug-free drivers (OR 1.5); however, when THC concentrations were 5ng/mL or more the risk increased dramatically with an OR >6. Amphetamine-type stimulants were detected in 13 per cent of drivers and 95 per cent of these drivers were determined as being responsible for the accident (Figure 2). Of interest in this study are the high levels of benzodiazepines detected, with 21 per cent of drivers testing positive and 3 per cent with levels in the toxic range. Most types of benzodiazepines were associated with moderate increases in crash risk and were found to be dose-dependant. Alprazolam in particular stands out with 100 per cent of drivers detected with this benzodiazepine being responsible for the accident in which they were injured and half of the drivers had toxic levels—clear evidence of misuse. The data suggest that at therapeutic levels benzodiazepines cause only a slight increase in crash risk, whereas abuse or misuse is associated with much higher crash risks. Polydrug use was also frequently detected with over 20 per cent of drivers injured in traffic accidents in this sample having more than one drug in their system. The combinations most often detected were alcohol and cannabis (OR 5.4), alcohol and benzodiazepines (OR 13.8) and cannabis and benzodiazepines (OR 6.9). While responsibility analyses provide important information about the role of drugs in crashes, they are not conclusive evidence of a causal relationship because they only include crash-involved drivers. So, Issues Paper | No. 12 | March 2010 3 a 8.0 7.0 6.0 5.0 4.0 3.0 2.0 1.0 0 Any THC THC >5 Any alcohol + THC b 10 5 0 Methamphetamine Amphetamine c MDMA Methylenedioxyamphetamine (MDA) 20 16 12 8 4 0 Figure 2. Drugs and responsibility analysis from the study by Ogden et al.23 while the analyses indicate that drugs are associated with higher crash risk, experimental research is also needed to examine the link between the presence of these drugs in the blood and subsequent impairment. In the next section we describe research that has examined the effect of various drugs on driving, and include some direct experimental studies in which various drugs have been administered to drivers or participants tested on driving simulators. Drugs and driving Alcohol Alcohol is the most studied drug in relation to driving impairment. A comprehensive review of 109 articles about the effects of alcohol on driving-related skills showed that alcohol, even in low doses, impairs 4 Drugs and driving driving performance.24 Divided attention tasks were shown to be extremely sensitive to alcohol consumption with impairments being reported at BACs as low as 0.001 per cent. Impairments in vigilance tasks measuring alertness and concentration were consistently observed at BACs of 0.03 per cent and the majority of studies reported impairments in cognitive tasks, psychomotor skills, perception and tracking tasks between BACs of 0.04 per cent and 0.06 per cent. Driving simulator studies demonstrate that alcohol can be detrimental to many driving skills. Alcohol has been shown to impair brake reaction time,25,26 divided attention skills27 and speed control.28,29 Tracking, the ability to maintain lane position, has consistently been shown to be impaired by alcohol.30–32 Arnedt et al. reported that after alcohol consumption, participants’ speed and lane positions became more variable and they drove off the simulated road more frequently.17 Further reductions in driving performance were observed when alcohol was combined with prolonged wakefulness. Strayer found that participants under the influence of alcohol drove more aggressively by following closer to the car in front of them which necessitated braking with greater force.33 Hazard perception has also been reported to be compromised by alcohol. West et al. reported that a moderate dose of alcohol increased mean reaction time to hazards.34 Consistent with these results, Deery & Love found that subjects with a BAC of 0.05 per cent took significantly longer to detect hazards than when sober and responded to those hazards in a more abrupt manner.35 The road toll, injury statistics, responsibility studies as well as hundreds of experimental studies, leave little doubt as to the impairing effects of alcohol, even in small doses and especially in a complex task such as driving. Cannabis Within a few minutes of inhaling cannabis smoke, users often report feeling, along with intoxication, rapid heartbeat, loss of coordination, poor sense of balance, slower reaction time, distorted perception, poor concentration and forgetfulness. Following this acute phase is the residual or rebound phase where intoxication subsides rapidly and is characterised by fatigue, excessive sleepiness and inattention. Table 1. The effects of various licit and illicit drugs on driving-related skills Drug Effects on driving-related skills Alcohol Impaired psychomotor skills, reaction time, perception, ability to keep the vehicle within traffic lanes, ability to focus on more than one task, vigilance, brake reaction time, speed control, increased aggression, decreased hazard perception. Cannabis Impaired psychomotor skills, reaction time, ability to keep the vehicle within traffic lanes, visual functions, attention, ability to maintain speed, rebound fatigue. Amphetamines Impaired ability to keep the vehicle within traffic lanes, erratic driving, speed, increased risk-taking, tunnel vision, rebound fatigue. Ecstasy (MDMA) Impaired ability to keep the vehicle within traffic lanes, visual functions, increased risk-taking, speed control. Ketamine Impaired reaction time, balance and coordination, visual functions. Research examining driving performance and cannabis indicates a dose-dependant relationship, with impairments demonstrated for every performance area related to safe driving: reaction time; tracking; psychomotor skills; visual functions; and attention.36 Simulated and on-road driving studies report impaired perceptual processes, such as: monitoring the speedometer and maintaining speed; response to stimuli, such as stopping and starting; and subsidiary tasks.37–39 Tracking ability is the most consistently reported driving skill to be impaired after cannabis consumption and is highlighted by an increase in the number of cones hit on a driving course, an increase in sideways movements of the vehicle and an increase in the percentage of time spent out of a lane.40–42 In line with these findings, a study funded by the National Drug Law Enforcement Research Fund (NDLERF) at the Drugs and Driving Research Unit (DDRU) at Swinburne University in Melbourne found that the consumption of low-dose and highdose cannabis was associated with an increase in vehicle lane-weaving (straddling solid and barrier lines).43 Cannabis impairment is most prominent in the first 2 hours after smoking; however, driving performance has been shown to be compromised up to 5 hours after cannabis consumption.36 Drivers can be impaired first by cannabis intoxication, and later in the residual phase due to fatigue and inattention. Despite common stereotypes regarding the low risk of having an accident under the influence of cannabis, there is no doubt that cannabis poses a significant crash risk for drivers. Amphetamines Amphetamines can be taken orally, snorted or injected, with users experiencing increased energy levels and mental alertness. The effects intensify with increased doses, and users may become overexcited, talkative and experience a false sense of self-confidence or superiority. Physical effects of amphetamines can include increased/distorted sensations, hyperactivity, dry mouth, increased heart rate, blurred vision, impaired speech, dizziness, uncontrollable movements or shaking. In addition to the physical effects, users often report feeling restless, anxious and moody. There has been little research examining the effects of amphetamines on driving performance. Laboratory studies show that at low doses amphetamines improve some skills related to driving.37,44 However, due to ethical constraints it is difficult to assess larger recreational doses in the laboratory. Crash statistics indicate that larger doses can lead to driving impairments resulting in death and injury, particularly for truck drivers.5,23 Logan evaluated actual driving behaviour of drivers arrested or killed in traffic accident who tested positive for methamphetamines.45 He reported that drivers often drifted out of the lane of travel, drove erratically, had a tendency to weave, sped, drifted off the road and were involved in high-speed collisions, suggesting increased risk-taking. The only simulated driving study to date showed that dexamphetamine had a deleterious effect on driving performance. Drivers under the influence of dexamphetamine were more likely to fail to stop at a red traffic light and signal incorrectly.46 A more recent study funded Issues Paper | No. 12 | March 2010 5 by the Australian Research Council (ARC) showed methamphetamine impaired some aspects of driving performance despite improvements in simple reaction time.47 Similar to cannabis, amphetamine users can experience rebound fatigue due to prolonged wakefulness. More research is needed to evaluate the effects of methamphetamine on driving; in particular it would be useful to determine the extent of rebound fatigue at various doses that approximate community use. However, the rebound effect is likely to cause driving impairment, therefore individuals should not drive at any stage after consuming amphetamines and particularly not drive during the rebound phase. As some amphetamine users also show fatigue (due to staying awake and attending rave and dance parties) the rebound effect is further compounded by fatigue. This latter issue is also observed in MDMA users. Ecstasy An ecstasy (MDMA) pill takes effect after 30–45 minutes, starting with little rushes of exhilaration, and with intoxication lasting up to 5 hours. Along with the positive feelings for which the drug is taken, users may experience disorientation, dry mouth, blurred vision and involuntary muscular activity. Further research has examined the effects of MDMA on driving performance. MDMA has been demonstrated to improve some aspects of driving such as speed of manual movement,48 tracking49 and sustained attention50 and impair other driving skills such as the ability to perceive and predict motion,48 visual scanning51 and accuracy of speed adaptation.52 A study conducted by De Waard reported that MDMA-influenced drivers were prepared to accept higher levels of risk.53 These types of impairment could have serious consequences as drivers manoeuvre in traffic. In contrast to Ramaeker and Kuypers’ finding that tracking performance was improved after MDMA,49 Kuypers et al. found impairments in tracking ability.21 The difference between these studies is that Kuypers et al. administered night-time doses of MDMA in order to more realistically demonstrate what an MDMA user would experience after taking the drug during the evening and going to a party. The impairments reported were additive to sleep loss. A recent placebo-controlled study at the DDRU in 6 Drugs and driving which MDMA was administered to 60 participants, showed performance was significantly worse under the influence of MDMA.47 Drivers were more likely to: signal incorrectly; undertake dangerous actions like skidding; brake inappropriately; follow cars at an unsafe distance; and speed. While MDMA may improve some basic driving skills, it also impairs skills that are essential for driving safely in traffic, especially when combined with sleep loss. As the results of these large federal governmentfunded studies (e.g. ARC funded study) become available it is imperative that the results are appropriately disseminated to government agencies and to the wider public. Information that contradicts stereotypes about the effects of common illicit drugs (e.g. “that cannabis makes me drive more cautiously”) is essential in changing driving behaviours in the wider community and therefore decreasing accidents and deaths on our roads. The results of these experimental studies provide specific information about which driving behaviours are impaired and at what time after ingestion. Ketamine Ketamine is primarily used as an anaesthetic agent and analgesic in medicine and veterinary practice.54 The hypnotic and hallucinogenic effects of this drug are gaining popularity among those who frequent dance parties as well as within the medical profession.55–56 Psychological effects include novel body sensations, dissociation, confusion57 and feelings of invulnerability.54 Physical effects include tachycardia, tachypnoea, blurred vision, uncoordination and catatonia.57 Some users describe a spiritual near-death experience known as the K-hole.54 There have been no direct studies examining the effects of ketamine upon driving. Instead, studies have focused on subjective and objective data and correlate this to whether this may or may not impair driving. A study by Guillermo et al. revealed that reaction time is adversely affected by even subacute doses of ketamine.58 A study undertaken on dance party volunteers who tested positive to ketamine revealed that 90 per cent of subjects with ketamine above the cut-off concentrations of saliva 300 ng/mL were deemed drug-impaired as determined by roadside impairment tests.59 Further research on ketamine and driving is required in a controlled clinical setting. Opioids Opioid drugs include opium, heroin, morphine, codeine, pethidine, oxycodone, buprenorphine and methadone. Prescription opioids are commonly used in the community and prescribed for their analgesic and antitussive properties. Following an initial intense surge of euphoria, heroin users alternate between a wakeful and drowsy state known as “the nod”. Other common effects include lethargy, disconnectedness, self-absorption, mental clouding, delirium, the inability to concentrate and diminished reflexes. Peak effects last for 1–2 hours and the overall effects wear off in 3–5 hours, depending on dose. Due to ethical constraints there are very few experimental studies assessing heroin effects. An early study conducted in the 1960s found that heroin produced subjective feelings of sedation and slowed reaction.60 Methadone and buprenorphine have been found to be an effective long-term maintenance therapy for those dependent on heroin.61 Due to this prevalence, studies have been undertaken to review whether these substances adversely affect driving ability. While some studies reveal impairment,62–64 other studies found that performance impairment does not differ significantly in comparison to drug-free subjects.65–68 Results appear to be dose-related with effects differing between opioid-naïve and opioid-dependent subjects. O’Neill et al.,66 Hanks et al.,67 and Brooke et al.68 administered 10–15mg of morphine, either as an acute dose or every 4 hours, and found no significant impairment in driving tasks. In fact morphine was found to improve reaction time. Nevertheless, while Zacny et al. found that hand–eye coordination was not affected by 2.5, 5 or 10mg/70kg doses of intravenous morphine, other psychomotor skills were impaired relative to the dose administered.62 Bruera et al. examined patients taking morphine-based drugs for pain relief.69 Those patients who received an increased dose within the last 3 days experienced significant cognitive impairment, with no difference noted in those patients who were stabilised on a set dose. Chesher et al. found similar results when comparing driving ability between patients stabilised on methadone and those beginning or receiving an increased dose of methadone.70 A key finding appears to be that patients on a maintenance dose have no psychomotor impairment in relation to driving ability. This is likely to be due to an increased opioid tolerance.71 Therefore, patients are often advised to stop driving for up to 4 weeks until their opioid regimen is stabilised.72,73 Benzodiazepines Benzodiazepines are the most common psychotropic drugs prescribed in Australia and are frequently used for the treatment of anxiety and panic disorders. They are also misused by some to self-medicate for the adverse effects of other drug use. For example, they may be used to alleviate withdrawal or “crash” symptoms that arise from heavy amphetamine use. Although drugs classed in this category have varying effects on individuals, they are all known to have some degree of sedative, hypnotic and anxiolytic action. There is a lack of conclusive research on the effects of benzodiazepines on driving skills, in particular in patient populations for which the drug is intended. To date, research on the effects of benzodiazepines on psychomotor performance have provided inconsistent results. The reports suggest that benzodiazepines can either impair74–75 or have no effect on performance tasks that assess drivingrelated skills.76 Improvement in performance has even been reported,77 which suggests that the variation in findings may be, in part, due to the variation in an individual’s response to the drug itself, withdrawal effects and whether the drug is misused or taken to improve functioning in various patient conditions. Driving simulator studies on opioids and benzodiazepines often conflict with reported crash risk statistics, because there have been few studies that examine administration of these substances in a controlled manner, taking into account the disorder for which they may be prescribed, tolerance and dose. Carefully controlled studies using driving simulators should be a focus of future research in order to properly inform the general public of the dangers of driving under the influence of these substances. Other prescription medications There are many prescription drugs; however only a few classes and a limited range of actual drugs within those classes have been studied experimentally. The prescription drugs that are reviewed below (and above e.g. benzodiazepines) are often reported in Issues Paper | No. 12 | March 2010 7 Table 2. The effects of antidepressants on driving-related skills Antidepressant Effects on driving-related skills Tricyclic antidepressants (TCA) Some studies have found that TCAs have a greater relative risk for motor vehicle accidents,140,141 while others have found no increased risk.142 This variance in findings could be due to the differing ages of participants as well as the variance in treatment periods.81 TCAs have antihistaminic and anticholinergic effects which may cause sedation.81 Most individuals adapt to these side effects after the initial treatment period, particularly if they are not elderly, have no evidence of hepatic dysfunction and are maintained on a stable dose.81 Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) These cause less impairment in psychomotor and cognition functioning than TCAs.79,140,143 Noradrenergic-specific serotonergics (NASSA) The NASSA mirtazapine has a sedative effect due to its antihistamine effects. This is particularly apparent when taken as a morning dose.79,144 However, those who take mirtazapine as a nocturnal dose show improvement in their performance-based tasks as opposed to untreated depressives.80 studies showing an impairment in driving behaviours, cognitive and motor skills or have been shown to increase crash risk in epidemiological studies. Antidepressants Depression can cause symptoms of poor concentration, attention disturbances, and deficits in memory and executive function that may impair driving ability.78 The objective of pharmacological treatment for depression is to allow the patient to participate fully in the activities of daily living, and this includes driving.79 Nevertheless, antidepressants may impair driving due to their effects on sedation, agitation and insomnia.79 Studies have revealed that different classes of antidepressants have differing effects on driving ability. A study by Brunnauer et al., examined the effects of tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs), selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), noradrenergic specific serotonergic antidepressants (NASSAs) and selective noradrenalin reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs) on driving ability and noted differences in the measures of reactivity, selective attention and stress tolerance.79 In another study Brunnauer et al. found that only 10 per cent of participants on TCAs passed the tests without impairments compared to 20 per cent on venlafaxine (SNRI), 28 per cent on SSRIs and 50 per cent on NASSAs.80 Table 2 outlines the effects of different classes of antidepressants on driving. 8 Drugs and driving As there appears to be a potential risk, counselling regarding driving safety needs to be tailored to the individual, taking into account such factors as age, type and dose of antidepressant, phase of treatment, co-administered psychotropic drugs and individual insight into psychomotor and cognitive impairment.79,81 Again the role of the clinical illness of depression or anxiety may cause significant impairment in driving. When most studies have been conducted in healthy controls it is difficult to ascertain the relative impairment of depression versus antidepressant therapy. A depressed unmedicated patient may be more at risk of accident than a medicated depressed patient, even though the medication may itself impair driving. Additionally, because different classes of antidepressants work differently on the brain, it is difficult at this stage of research to be conclusive about the impairing effects of antidepressants in general. Antihypertensives All antihypertensives can impact on driving due to their vasomotor effects. In the initial stage of treatment, hypotension and dizziness may occur. Table 3 outlines the effects of some antihypertensive medications on driving ability. Patients need to be informed regarding the risks of taking these medications and driving, particularly in the treatment initiation stage.82 However, there are few studies in this area. Table 3. The effects of antihypertensive medication on driving-related skills Antihypertensive medication Effects on driving-related skills Centrally acting antihypertensives (e.g. clonidine, methyldopa and reserpine) May have a sedative action which calls for particular attention.82 Atenolol (beta blocker) A comparison study between methyldopa and reserpine and the beta blocker atenolol revealed that while driving performance deteriorated in those who were on the centrally acting antihypertensives, kinetic visual acuity (KVA) improved significantly for those on atenolol.145 Atenolol does cause dizziness; however, this is short in duration, moderate to weak in action and resolves with a lower dose.146 Angiotensin-converting enzyme (ACE) inhibitors (e.g. ramipril) Have side effects of dizziness, drowsiness, vertigo, hypotension, hyperkalemia and syncope which may impair driving.146–147 Calcium channel blockers Have been associated with a decreased risk of motor vehicle accidents. This could be due to their effects on stabilising abnormal heart rhythms and relieving angina.88 Oral hypoglycaemics Hypoglycaemia among those with insulin or noninsulin dependent diabetes mellitus (IDDM and NIDDM) may result in cognitive–motor slowing and loss of consciousness. This could negatively impact driving performance.83 This risk is more associated with mismanagement of dosage regimens, reduced food intake or strenuous exercise than with the medication itself.82 A study by Hemmelgarn et al. explored whether antidiabetic medication increased the chances of a motor vehicle crash.83 They concluded that the use of insulin alone or a combination of sulfonylurea and metformin increased the risk of a crash by 30–40 per cent, particularly for those on a higher dose. This risk is higher for those who take a combination of insulin and oral hypoglycaemics. On the contrary, no increased risk was found for those who are on oral monotherapy. Hemmelgarn et al. are unclear whether this small increased risk is medication-induced or related to latter-stage diabetic complications such as retinopathy and neuropathy, given that treatment regimens correlate with disease progression.83 However, a driving simulator study on a group of 25 patients with IDDM found that while driving performance was not impaired with mild hypoglycaemia (3.6 mmol/L), moderate hypoglycaemia (2.6 mmol/L) reduced driving performance with more swerving, spinning, crossing the midline and compensatory slow driving.84 Blood glucose levels are advised to be tested before driving, especially in those who have asymptomatic hypoglycaemia.83,85 Antihistamines Antihistamines are H1-receptor antagonists that are used widely by those suffering seasonal or chronic allergic rhinitis.86 Because of their widespread use, many studies have been undertaken to assess whether they impact driving (Table 4).75,87 Unfortunately there is not enough research examining whether prescribed medicines when appropriately used impair driving behaviour. In some cases, prescribed medicines may improve driving behaviours in patients. It is not in the scope of this report to review every prescription medication. However, there is much research that is required before an adequate level of knowledge is gained on precisely which prescribed medicines impair driving. This lack of research is a particular problem in educating the community and medical staff. A sensible message for many patients in the absence of clearly articulated findings from research is to continue to take medications but in many cases to stop driving. Issues Paper | No. 12 | March 2010 9 Table 4. The effects of antihistamines on driving-related skills Antihistamines Effects on driving-related skills First-generation antihistamines (e.g. brompheniramine, dimenhydrinate and diphenhydramine) Have lipophilic properties which enable cross-over through the blood–brain barrier.86 The depressive action on the central nervous system (CNS) can cause drowsiness, dizziness, reduced coordination and increased reaction time.106 The sedative effects persist into the next day.86 Anticholinergic effects of dry mouth and blurry vision are also evident.106 Second-generation antihistamines (e.g. loratadine) May also impair driving through psychomotor retardation and drowsiness.82,148 However, the effects are generally milder than those of first-generation antihistamines and depend on gender, dose and time of intake.82,148 Tolerance tends to develop after 4–5 days.148 O’Hanlon et al. found that while impairment is sometimes evident at higher than recommended doses, impairment is small to almost undetectable at recommended doses.149 One exception to this is cetirizine.150 Third-generation antihistamines (e.g. fexofenadine and levocetirizine) Fexofenadine and levocetirizine have been shown to produce no driving impairment.148 Weiler et al. compared fexofenadine (60 mg), diphenhydramine (50 mg), alcohol (0.1 per cent blood alcohol concentration) and placebo on driving performance.87 They found little difference in driving performance between those participants on fexofenadine or placebo. However, those participants in the diphenhydramine group performed worse on the driving tasks than those in the alcohol group. Polydrug use Polydrug combinations are often detected in drivers involved in accidents.88–89 Results of studies indicate, on the whole, that impairment in driving performance increases when drivers combine alcohol with other drugs or use multiple drugs simultaneously. Ecstasy (MDMA) is frequently used in combination with other drugs, mainly with alcohol, cannabis and/ or amphetamines.90 An Australian survey revealed that 40 per cent of MDMA users also consumed alcohol.91 Studies have investigated the effects of this combination on driving performance in controlled experimental settings.21,49,92–94 Overall, while the central nervous system (CNS) stimulating effect of MDMA reduces the subjective feelings of alcohol’s sedative effect, it does not reduce alcohol-induced psychomotor impairment.49,92,94 This difference between subjective and objective sedation may result in risk-taking among drivers.92 Cannabis and alcohol combined have a cumulative effect.95 A study by Robbe found that when low to moderate levels of cannabis were taken with sufficient 10 Drugs and driving alcohol to achieve a BAC of 0.04 per cent, the participants exhibited marked driving impairment.96 As a BAC of 0.04 per cent is below the defined level of intoxication, breathalyser tests alone would not identify this cumulative effect.42 Benzodiazepines and alcohol are both CNS depressants.97 An additive or synergistic sedative effect is therefore likely to occur.98 However, studies investigating these combined effects are limited and conflicting. English et al. and Taberner et al. suggest in their research of temazepam and alcohol that there is no evidence of potentiation or prolongation of combined effects as each substance displays different patterns of CNS depressant activity.99,100 In contrast, Van Steveninck et al. and Weathermon and Crabb measured performance with several cognitive and psychomotor tests and concluded that a combination of these two substances does impair performance.98,101 Neutal suggests that those who have commenced initial treatment with benzodiazepines should abstain from alcohol and driving.102 More research appears to be needed in regard to the concomitant use of these substances. In their simulated driving study, Lenne et al. found no difference between clients on a stabilised dose of methadone or buprenorphine treatment when compared with a non-drug control group.103 Both groups had a BAC of 0.05 per cent. However, when a therapeutic dose of diazepam was added to the methadone/alcohol combination a significant impairment was noted.104 Prescription medications can also impair driving when used in conjunction with alcohol. The antihistamines cetirizine and loratadine have been shown to have additive effects.105 Further research is needed to assess concomitant use of antihistamines and other prescribed medications.106 Prevention and early intervention We now turn our attention to strategies to minimise and reduce the number of drivers who consume drugs. We start with current strategies in schools and move on to discuss advertising campaigns and information provided by other agencies such as pharmacists. Next we discuss approaches to detect drug-driving and conclude by discussing issues relating to treatment and education following the loss of licence due to drug-driving. All of these approaches work together to reduce the number of individuals who drive under the influence of drugs. Schools In Victoria, young drivers, aged 18–25 years, make up almost one-third of the road toll, and yet this age group only holds 13 per cent of Victorian licences.3 The Department of Education and Early Childhood Development in collaboration with key road safety agencies (Transport Accident Commission (TAC), VicRoads, Victoria Police, Department of Infrastructure, Metlink and RACV) have developed a traffic education strategy for Victoria. To assist young people to develop the skills required for safe travel, the strategy recommends that Traffic Safety Education (TSE) is provided to children in Preparatory to Year 2, to children transitioning from primary to secondary school and to all Year 10 students. The TSE resource for secondary schools (“Traffic Safety Essentials—for young road users, not crash test dummies”) is based on research undertaken by Barry Elliott and was developed to assist schools implement a TSE program for Year 10 students.107 The program includes core activities in speed, alcohol and other drugs, fatigue, and peer group pressure. It is recommended that a minimum of 12 hours of class time should be spent on these activities. These activities are designed to assist students in improving their decision-making skills, developing an understanding of consequences and improving safety for themselves and their peers. The “Alcohol and other drugs” unit in the TSE program covers a range of topics from effects of alcohol, cannabis, methamphetamine and MDMA on driving, to initiatives implemented in Victoria such as roadside saliva testing for illicit drugs and alcohol interlocks. At the end of the unit students will understand that: ◗◗ it is illegal and unsafe when alcohol and other drugs are combined with driving, riding and walking ◗◗ alcohol and other drugs can impact decisionmaking related to safety, impair driving abilities, and put affected pedestrians at great risk of injury or death. The resource is designed for teachers and is free to those who attend a professional development session. In 2009, 86 per cent of secondary schools obtained copies. Initial responses by teachers and practitioners are that the program targets the correct audience (pre-licence students). A baseline evaluation is currently being finalised and further assessment of use in schools will be carried out in 2010. Along with TSE, it is also recommended that schools include drug education as part of their curriculum. The Department of Education and Early Childhood Development trains teachers and provides support to schools in developing their drug education plans. They provide schools with resources and information and recommend that government schools deliver 10 hours of drug education each year, at each year level.108 Students who complete the recommended TSE as well as drug education, should be well informed of the effects of different drugs and the possible consequences of combining them with driving. Issues Paper | No. 12 | March 2010 11 Advertising campaigns Mass media campaigns are used around the world to highlight the road safety message and are successful at conveying information and changing attitudes.109 An evaluation of 265 campaigns estimated that on average a road safety mass media campaign will result in an 8.5 per cent reduction in crashes during the campaign and a 14.8 per cent reduction after the campaign. However, the campaigns delivered in Australia are more comprehensive and long-term than those delivered in most European countries and the United States, which may result in larger reductions in crashes.110 The TAC has used mass media advertising campaigns since 1989 to promote road safety and reduce the road toll. The first campaign focused on drink driving and incorporated mass media and increased police enforcement with the introduction of “booze buses”. An evaluation conducted by Monash University’s Accident Research Centre (MUARC) demonstrated substantial reductions in road trauma in Victoria due to the combination of TAC advertising, increased random breath-testing using “booze buses” and the speed camera program.111–112 A further MUARC study evaluating the impact of major factors that influence road trauma found that the drink-driving campaign (advertising and enforcement) resulted in a 9–10 per cent reduction in serious road casualties.113 While it is difficult to disentangle mass media effects from supporting activities, the success of the intensive drink-driving campaign is highlighted by the reduction in drivers killed with illegal BACs falling from 114 in 1989 to 59 in 2007. The TAC’s drug-driving campaign began in 2004 and was undertaken due to the increase in positive drug samples from fatally injured drivers. Designed to coincide with the introduction of roadside drug saliva testing, TAC began mass advertising which focused on the risk of detection. The TAC’s advertising sign-posted the changes to legislation, like the introduction of drug testing, and demonstrated how the public will get caught and the penalties associated with drug-driving. This phase of the campaign was followed by advertising focusing on education, such as the effect of cannabis on driving and decisionmaking. Both types of advertising are needed for an effective campaign along with use of multiple media, carefully targeted messages and repetition.109 Advertising in conjunction with police enforcement 12 Drugs and driving builds the public perception of detection and the risks of drinking and drug-taking much earlier than would be achieved by enforcement alone.109 The biggest deterrent is a highly visible advertising campaign combined with stepped-up enforcement.110–111 At this stage it is too early to know the benefits of the drug-driving advertising campaign. More educationbased advertising is needed to inform the public of the risks associated with driving after consuming different drugs and increased random drug testing is needed to increase deterrence. The drug testing regime needs to be broadened to regional areas as drivers in these areas know they will not get tested, so there is no deterrence value in advertising risk of detection. However, it is early days in the campaign and based on the success of the drink-driving campaign, in time it is probable that a decrease in drug-driving injuries and fatalities will be observed. Pharmacists Pharmacists are required to counsel consumers about possible effects of medicines on driving performance when dispensing prescriptions. This is achieved through warning stickers on medications combined with verbal information. Consumer Medicine Information sheets are also provided that detail all of the medication’s side effects, including effects on driving. So while the information is provided to the individual, it is up to the person to determine if they adhere to that advice, and also to determine if they are affected and should be driving. Pharmaceuticals are perceived to have less of an impairing effect on driving than alcohol and illicit drugs, with one in four Australian drivers admitting to ignoring medical advice not to drive after taking certain prescription medicines.114 There is a need to improve driver awareness, knowledge of the effects of medications on driving performance and also compliance with medication warnings.115 Queensland Transport undertook a drugs and driving public education campaign detailing the effect of over-the-counter and prescription medications as well as illicit drugs on driving ability. The campaign involved over 1000 pharmacies that were supplied with fact cards, display stands and posters displaying the slogan “Don’t discover the side effects by accident. Ask the pharmacist if you’ll be right to drive.” Before the campaign commenced 400 Queensland motorists were surveyed about attitudes to road safety; over 30 per cent said they thought medicines would not affect their driving. This decreased to 13 per cent a year after the campaign began.116 Educating pharmacists on the effects of prescribed medicines on driving is a particularly efficient way to educate the general community and to therefore reduce the number of accidents caused by prescription medicines. There may also be natural medicines that impair driving behaviour, although this has been significantly less researched than prescribed medicines. Pharmacists would also be a logical and efficient resource to use to educate the public. Work with the National Institute of Complementary Medicine could also help initiate research in this area. Role of venues Venues that serve alcohol and ones that are frequented by drug users such as nightclubs and dance parties also have a role in drink- and drugdriving prevention. By limiting levels of intoxication attained by customers, providing relevant information on harms of drug and alcohol use and suggesting alternate means of transport, venues can minimise drug- and alcohol-related harm. “Responsible Service of Alcohol” (RSA) programs were developed to prevent drink-driving by limiting intoxication levels in licenced premises. In Victoria it is compulsory to complete an RSA course if you work at a venue which has on its licence a condition that requires staff to have an RSA certificate, or when applying for a new liquor licence. Otherwise, it is recommended but not compulsory. The half-day course covers topics including problems associated with excessive alcohol consumption, alcohol, and the law, facts about alcohol and handling difficult customers. The aim is to reduce intoxication and associated alcohol-related harms by providing liquor service staff with the knowledge and awareness necessary to responsibly serve alcohol in licensed premises. Recommended RSA practices include providing food, slowing service to intoxicated customers and taking steps to prevent intoxicated customers from driving. Early studies showed that RSA programs reduced levels of customer intoxication117 with one study finding BACs attained by customers served by trained staff were close to half (0.059 per cent) compared with untrained staff (0.103 per cent).118 However, later larger studies failed to show the same success,119–120 which may have been due to a failure of venue management to successfully implement and encourage RSA principles.121 A study that evaluated a law enforcement approach to RSA found that alcohol-related road crashes were reduced following the introduction of the enforcement program.122 It has been suggested that to be effective, RSA programs and principles need to be wholly supported and encouraged by venue management. If there is also consistent law enforcement, in which premises are fined for liquor licence breaches, a more significant impact on alcohol-related harms including traffic crashes will result.121–123 Venues can also provide education on drug and alcohol use to reduce drug driving. One method to do this is peer education. Peer education models aim to provide an informative and credible health promotion message to target populations. In Victoria, a peer education program called DanceWize (formerly RaveSafe) run by VIVAIDS, attend a number of large dance parties to provide education on safe drug use. At these events DanceWize staff can provide information and referral advice on a wide range of health and related issues, including safe sex/sexual health, drug-driving and how to get home safely from an event, penalties for drug offences and where to get legal advice, drug treatments and services, etc. While few studies have successfully evaluated peer education models, anecdotal evidence from youth and community organisations have attested to peer education being an effective way to educate young people and illicit drug users.124–125 Studies have shown that informal advice stemming from a peer’s experience is seen as more authentic than advice or information coming from an official source.126–127 In line with these findings, a survey conducted by DanceWize revealed that the program is well received by its peers as a site of “real” drug information and concluded that in situ peer education appears to be an effective and credible method of harm minimisation.128 As nightclubs also attract people who use drugs, a similar peer education program in these types of venues would be beneficial to raise awareness of harms associated with drug use, combining alcohol and drugs and drug-driving. Detection of impaired driving Although education is an important contributor to the reduction in the number of drivers consuming drugs, for some individuals there also needs to be a Issues Paper | No. 12 | March 2010 13 perception that there are valid methods of detecting drug-drivers by police. Without the risk of being identified, fined or prosecuted, education is not sufficient to deter some individuals from consuming drugs and then driving. Sobriety testing In response to the rising number of drug-related motor vehicle accidents and fatalities, performance tests have been incorporated into many police programs to detect drivers impaired by drugs other than alcohol. Although the tests vary from country to country, they are all based on the same principle: that certain physiological and/or psychomotor behaviours will be affected by drug consumption, and that these effects can be compared to the normative data for unimpaired (drug-free) individuals. The tests are designed to assess psychomotor performance, cognitive functioning and divided attention. As well as maintaining coordination and balance, the individual is required to remember instructions and simultaneously perform more than one task. Impairment on performance tests is therefore directly related to impaired driving ability, as the driving task frequently requires the individual to divide attention between two or more tasks. The most common sobriety tests used are the Drug Evaluation and Classification Program (DECP) developed in the United States of America, the Field Impairment Tests (FIT) adapted from the DECP for use in the United Kingdom, and the Performance Impairment Tests (PIT), used in Australia. Victorian sobriety testing Currently the Victorian police use the Standard Impairment Assessment (SIA) to identify the presence of impairment in drivers believed to be under the influence of drugs other than alcohol. If a police officer observes a serious driving impairment they can require the driver to undertake the SIA. The SIA is a structured and systematic assessment that is carried out by trained police officers. The assessment consists of four components: interview and observation (a standardised series of questions that examine the circumstances that led to the detainment of the suspect, as well as recent history of illness, injury, medical treatment and drug use); physical impairment test; information review process; and opinion on the presence of impairment. The physical impairment test consists of the horizontal gaze nystagmus, 14 Drugs and driving the walk and turn test, and the one leg stand test. These tests allow impairment to be identified at a level that is equivalent to the impairment at a BAC of 0.05 per cent or above.129 When impairment is identified by the officer, a blood or urine sample is taken to confirm the decision. During the first 12 months of the drug detection program in Victoria, police identified a total of 227 individuals suspected of driving while impaired by a drug.129 Of these 227 suspects, 181 were charged with offences. Convictions were obtained in 27 cases with 133 remaining in the court system; thus far no cases have been dismissed. The results suggest that the Victorian SIA procedure is effective in identifying and removing drug-impaired drivers from the road. Sobriety testing in other states Other Australian states employ different methods of drug detection. New South Wales (NSW) police use a standard set of questions regarding alcohol or drug consumption and a number of behavioural and physical signs (breathing, facial colour, speech patterns, mental state, balance and the state of the eyes and pupils). Perl and Moynihan suggest that despite little formal training, NSW police have been successful in detecting drug-positive drivers, with approximately 90 per cent of suspected drivers found to be drug positive.130 In Tasmania, if a driver is suspected of driving under the influence of a drug, the police will request the driver to take a sobriety test. Tests include picking up coins from the floor, walking heel to toe, handwriting and standing with eyes closed and touching the nose with an index finger. Western Australia implemented a program using the Standardised Field Sobriety Test (SFST) to identify drug impairment (similar in operation to the Victorian model) in 2007. Queensland uses a similar model to NSW which uses a standard question and observation guide. At the present time, the Northern Territory and the Australian Capital Territory have no specific provisions for drug impairment testing and prosecutions are rare. Clearly an agreed-upon set of procedures adopted across all states and territories would have the most impact in deterring individuals from driving under the influence of drugs. This lack of uniformity notwithstanding, performance tests are an effective procedure to detect certain drugs that cause significant impairment in driving. They probably do not detect more subtle driving impairment. In an attempt to detect a larger number of drug-affected drivers than impairment tests and to therefore provide a greater deterrent, saliva tests were first introduced in Victoria. Saliva testing In December 2003, the Victorian Government approved the trial of random roadside saliva testing to identify the presence of drugs in drivers, with positive drug saliva tests resulting in prosecution and fines. The cost of illicit drug-related road crashes and deaths in Australia has been estimated at $531.6 million annually.131 The cost of administering saliva-based tests to a driver and the completion of all evidentiary testing and administrative steps required to confirm the presence of drugs is calculated at approximately $833 per detection.132 Considering that random drug testing is likely to be administered to a large number of drivers, the cost of introducing random saliva drug testing is high. However, the cost of this procedure is likely to significantly reduce with increasing volume and technological advances, and its use is likely to decrease a component of motor vehicle accidents that were predominantly invisible to alcohol testing. During the past decade there has been a significant reduction in accidents and deaths due to alcohol in Australia, although this is not true for drugs other than alcohol. Therefore, roadside drug detection is an important next step in reducing accidents and deaths on our roads. In a recent publication Boorman and Owens outlined the general results of the first two years of the legislative framework (December 2004–December 2006).133 Of the 25 317 drivers (17 965 car drivers and 7352 truck drivers) who were screened for drugs on the roadside, 520 were charged with drug-driving, of which 506 were convicted. The majority of the roadside tests involved assessment of methamphetamine, ecstasy and cannabis. Given that these drugs (particularly cannabis) are widely taken in our community and have been shown to impair driving, this initiative is significant in removing potential future accidents from our roads. Of particular note is the finding that “…the detection rate of illicit drug-offending drivers in comparison to alcohol-offending drivers was double”.133 This is of particular interest given that if the presence of alcohol is identified in drivers then drug testing may not then proceed. The significance of this is that it appears that alcohol testing is still more of a deterrent than drug testing. Increasing the number of saliva tests therefore may significantly increase the number of drivers who are intercepted with drugs in their system and could be considered an important next step in the public’s awareness of drug detection techniques in Victoria. According to Martin Boorman from Victoria Police, in 2008, 25 000 saliva tests were performed, which will progressively increase and double in three years (compared to 1.4 million random blood tests). Also of interest are the demographics of drivers detected with drugs in their saliva. For car drivers, those who were found to have detectable drugs in their saliva were more likely to be under the age of 26 years, employed, with a regular licence but with less than five years experience. Conversely, truck drivers detected were somewhat older (aged between 31 and 45 years), employed and with greater driving experience. Clearly the uses of illicit drugs in these two cohorts are due to different causes. Educational campaigns aimed at these demographics is a logical next step in reducing the prevalence of drugged drivers in our community. Additionally, the inclusion of other drugs in saliva analysis is also likely to identify currently undetected drug-affected drivers, and therefore prevent accidents due to drugs in general. Benzodiazepines and opiates are obvious next choices, based on previous collision research, their effects on the CNS and their use in our community. Clearly, the introduction of roadside saliva testing has been a significant, successful and cost-effective new approach in the detection of individuals driving under a narrow range of illicit drugs, which represents a large percentage of commonly abused drugs in our community. Treatment and education following loss of licence In addition to education and the threat of detection, treatment and education are also needed after the loss of licence or successful prosecution for drug-driving. When a driver has her/his licence suspended or cancelled for drink- or drug-driving the offender is required to undertake a licence restoration process. This is a complicated, lengthy and costly process which varies from person to person depending on the type of offence, whether the licence was disqualified or suspended and the driver’s age. The restoration process may include payment of fines, participation in a driver education program, undertaking clinical Issues Paper | No. 12 | March 2010 15 assessments, court hearings and for drink-drivers the installation of alcohol interlock devices. Driver education is an essential part of the rehabilitation/licence restoration system and when combined with fines and disqualification periods, has been shown to be an effective deterrent for drink-drivers.134 To support the drug-driving licence restoration process and prevent people drug driving, Moreland Hall developed the drug-drive education program. Moreland Hall is the primary drug-drive educator in Victoria, running six education sessions per year. The program covers topics including legislation and the restoration process, substance effects on driving ability (cannabis, stimulants, ecstasy, opiates, polysubstance use, and prescription drugs), risks and consequences of drug driving, and strategies for safer driving. The program aims to educate participants on the risks associated with drug driving and promotes an attitude shift. However, the drink- and drug-driver education programs have been criticised for having a “one size fits all” approach, with substance-dependent offenders, first-time and low-level offenders all completing the same education program. The current program has limited capacity to change drink- and drug-driving, especially for people with dependence issues.135 A more effective system may be a streamed approach that would cater for the different offender groups and ensure that education and rehabilitation can be tailored to each type of offender. Best practice for the treatment and rehabilitation of drink-drive offenders is to combine education with case management, psychotherapy and follow-up monitoring and aftercare. The system has also been criticised as it does not take into account access and equity issues. The program is a “user pays” system and as such it is unlikely that some groups (unemployed, disadvantaged, serious and recidivist offenders) will comply and will simply slip through the cracks. Another issue with the current licence restoration process is that assessment, rehabilitation or treatment is not undertaken at the time of the offence but at the time of re-licensing. Alcohol interlocks Alcohol interlocks were developed to reduce impaired driving and rehabilitate repeat drink-driving offenders. The devices are fitted to drink-driving offender’s vehicles and require the driver to blow 16 Drugs and driving a zero BAC reading before the vehicle will start. At random intervals when driving, the driver must also blow into the alcohol interlock to ensure the reading is still zero. The first alcohol interlock program was introduced in California over 20 years ago and today most US states and many European Union nations have interlock-enabling legislation.136 Alcohol interlocks are now being used in Victoria for serious and repeat drink-driving offenders. Legislation was passed in May 2002 and the first alcohol interlock device was installed in May 2003. The Victorian alcohol interlock program begins when a drink-drive offender has been disqualified from driving and the disqualification period is about to end. A magistrate can make it a condition of a new licence that the offender’s vehicle is fitted with an alcohol interlock device. Once the minimum interlock period has expired the driver can make application to the court to remove the interlock condition. The court and police will get a report including a printout of the functioning of the interlock, usage of the vehicle and any failed attempts to start the car. The cost of the program is paid by the offender with the average cost of installation, servicing, maintenance and having the interlock removed costing approximately $160 per month. Approximately 18 000 interlocks are currently installed in Victoria. Interlocks have been demonstrated to be effective while the interlock is installed; however, they are less effective at changing long-term behaviour.137 Research shows that punishment such as alcohol interlock programs will do little in the long-term to change the behaviour of people with alcohol dependence. To enhance the possibility of long-term behaviour change, punishment needs to be combined with treatment, rehabilitation and intensive case management.135,136,138 The Victorian system has been criticised as the education and interlock programs are only required at the re-licensing stage rather than the penalty stage.135 As the gap between re-licensing and the offence can be a number of years, it has been suggested that treatment and education is required at the penalty stage to address dependence issues. Best practice is to shorten the disqualification period if the offender installs an interlock; the benefit of this system is that the offender receives earlier rehabilitation as well as preventing drink-driving.136 Current problems, issues and suggestions for the future Many of the experts interviewed believed that there is a significant lack of understanding of the effect of various drugs on driving, both acutely and chronically in our community. Specifically there is a lack of education and knowledge about how specific drugs impair driving in the general community. More detailed analysis and education based on this analysis may be important in changing community behaviour. This could also lead to advertising campaigns that are more compelling and convincing. There are still many misconceptions about the effect of various drugs on driving ability. For instance there is a belief that cannabis leads to safer driving by many regular users. There is also a belief that amphetamines improve processing speed which leads drivers to take greater risks than normal. There is also a common belief that opiates are safe if they are prescribed. These assumptions can be easily challenged and defeated in properly developed media campaigns. There is a lack of information and research about the effects of many prescription drugs on driving. There is also a lack of knowledge about how low levels of alcohol (i.e. less than 0.05 per cent BAC) combined with various drugs (licit and illicit) may impair driving. Again this information needs to be made public if public behaviour is to be changed. Although saliva testing for drugs has been successful in detecting drug-affected drivers, there is still only a limited number of drugs tested and at levels for some drugs that may only measure recent use. We know that many drugs, even at low levels or many hours after use, can still impair driving. Developing a wider battery of drugs tested may deter many drivers from driving. Of course not all drugs impair driving and the detection of low concentrations of certain drugs may merely be detecting those drugs and not driving impairment. This problem may be solved by the development of computerised roadside assessments for cognitive and motor functioning. This may be particularly important in detecting drivers who are showing fatigue (who have not consumed drugs) and drivers who are rebounding from previous drug taking, such as the case of amphetamine users (i.e. who have no levels of amphetamines in their system but who show great impairment in driving). There is a lack of understanding of whether some drugs improve driving skills in certain types of patients who use prescribed drugs. For instance, does a patient with an anxiety disorder drive better after taking a prescribed medication than without medication to treat his/her disorder? Understanding whether patients dependent on alcohol or other drugs should be allowed licences and how potential future accidents involving these drivers can be avoided is important. Is it possible to suspend licences in patients who are dependent on alcohol or other drugs before they have had an accident? On what basis can a person prove that they are no longer dependent on alcohol or a drug that impairs driving? In this respect hair analysis could be investigated as a tool in identifying potential drivers who are still dependent on drugs. This could then be used to prevent currently dependent drivers from driving. Perhaps one of the biggest problems is in the area of truck driving. Truck accidents have a disproportionate contribution to the road toll in Australia relative to motor cars. Saliva testing has identified the use of amphetamines in truck drivers who use these substances to drive further and for longer durations. Unless there is regular drug detection for drivers, then this issue will remain a problem as there is an obvious economic advantage for truck drivers (and their employers) to find ways in which they can drive a truck greater distances. One expert suggested that changing legislation that limited the number of hours a truck driver could continuously work within a 24-hour period is essential. New interventions in the area of detecting fatigue in truck drivers are starting to gain momentum. This technology measures basic visual processes that underpin fatigue such as eye movements and saccades. Although likely to be an important next step in reducing fatigue-related accidents with trucks, their adoption may lead to an increase in the amount of drug-taking that alleviates fatigue, such as amphetamines and newer drugs such as modafinil. Both these drugs may cause their own impairments to driving and only one is currently detected by saliva tests. Clearly a unified national approach is essential in reducing truckrelated accidents in Australia, as many trucks cover vast distances. In Victoria the legislation relating to the number of continuous hours of truck driving is currently being reviewed. Issues Paper | No. 12 | March 2010 17 Case study Prevent Alcohol and Risk-Related Trauma in Youth (PARTY) www.partymelbourne.net.au Contact person: Jen Thompson, Coordinator, PARTY program (Prevent Alcohol and Risk-Related Trauma in Youth), National Trauma Research Institute Why the program is run PARTY is aimed at providing teenagers with an experience of trauma that will enable them to recognise potential injury-producing situations, make prevention-oriented choices, and adopt behaviours that minimise unnecessary risk. Program description One of the National Trauma Research Institute’s (NTRI) leading educational initiatives is the PARTY Program. An acronym for Prevent Alcohol and Risk-Related Trauma in Youth, PARTY is an inhospital injury awareness and prevention program originally established in Ontario, Canada in 1986. It was developed by a team of emergency nurses and doctors who were unhappy with the numbers of young people presenting to the department as a result of risk-related activities. The program is designed to engage and confront young people. Holding the program within a hospital environment enhances the experience and leaves a significant and lasting impression of the consequences of Encouraging trucking organisations to carry out their own drug testing at the beginning and end of truck journeys, which can be independently audited, will also reduce the incidence of truck drivers taking drugs. A current study funded by the parliamentary road action committee and conducted by Edward Ogden (assessing levels of drugs in non-fatal but serious car accidents in Victoria) is likely to provide important results regarding the presence of a wide range of drugs, licit and illicit, that cause accidents in Melbourne. The analysis of these results should be widely disseminated to the community, Victoria Police and the government agencies described in this document. 18 Drugs and driving trauma and risk-taking behaviour. Students are addressed by emergency services (Victoria Police, Ambulance Victoria), various health professionals (doctors, nurses, therapists), and patients and their families—people who have experienced trauma and survived, often with significant disabilities. There is a mixture of Power-Point presentations, hands-on activities, hospital tours and patient encounters, to enable students from all learning persuasions to “get the message”. Initial funding was from Tattersall’s George Adams Foundation. The current major sponsor is AAMI Skilled Drivers. A significant amount of “in-kind” support for the program is given by staff and volunteers from The Alfred Hospital that allows the cost of the program to be reduced. The program is targeted at young persons aged 15–19 years. It currently runs fortnightly throughout school terms. At this point only school students attend the program from both independent and public educational models. What’s good about this program? The PARTY program appears to be a well thought out intervention and has been shown to effectively reduce the incidence of traumatic injuries among its participants in Canada.139 A proper analysis of its efficacy for an Australian audience should be conducted. If it is shown to be effective the program could be applied more widely and with more “at risk” young groups. Acknowledgments The authors wish to acknowledge the contributions of the following experts in the production of this report: Karen Marsh, Department of Education and Early Childhood Development, Drug Education, Victorian Government Conrad C Remenyi, Senior Project Officer, Targeted Programs Branch, Student Learning Programs Division, Department of Education and Early Childhood Development, Victorian Government David Healy, General Manager, Road Safety, Transport Accident Commission, Victoria Alan Freedman and Bev Baxter, Pharmacy Guild of Australia—Victorian Branch Martin C Boorman, APM, Inspector, Traffic Drug and Alcohol Section, Technical Unit, Victoria Police John Quiroga, Senior Education and Training Officer, Moreland Hall Professor Phil Swann, VicRoads, Victorian Government Daniel Holness, DanceWize Coordinator, VIVAIDS Dr Edward Ogden, Victoria Police, Victorian Government Jen Thompson, Coordinator, PARTY Program (Prevent Alcohol and Risk-Related Trauma in Youth), National Trauma Research Institute References 1. Australian Transport Safety Bureau 2007 “Road deaths Australia 2006 statistical summary. ATSB research and analysis report”, Canberra: Road Safety, Australian Government. 2. Berry JG & Harrison JE 2008 “Serious injury due to land transport accidents Australia, 2005–06, Australian Institute of Health and Welfare”, Injury research and statistics series number 42, cat. no. INJCAT 113, Adelaide: AIHW. 3. Traffic Accident Commission (TAC), Victoria, Australia 2009 www.tacsafety.com.au/jsp/homepage/home.jsp 4. Longo MC, Hunter CE, Lokan RJ, White JM & White MA 2000 “The prevalence of alcohol, cannabinoids, benzodiazepines and stimulants amongst injured drivers and their role in driver culpability. Part II: The relationship between drug prevalence and drug concentration, and driver culpability”, Accident Analysis and Prevention, 32, pp. 623–32. 5. Drummer OH, Gerostamoulos J, Batziris H, Chu M, Caplehorn J, Robertson MD & Swann P 2004 “The involvement of drugs in drivers of motor vehicles killed in Australian road traffic crashes”, Accident Analysis and Prevention, 36:2, pp. 239–48. 6. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare 2008 “2007 National Drug Strategy Household Survey: first results”, Drug Statistics Series number 20, cat. no. PHE 98, Canberra: AIHW. 7. Drugs and Crime Prevention Committee 2004 “Inquiry into Amphetamine and ‘Party Drug’ Use in Victoria – Final Report”, Melbourne: DCPC, Parliament of Victoria. 8. Drummer OH 1994 “Drugs in drivers killed in Victorian road traffic accidents. The use of responsibility analysis to investigate the contribution of drugs to fatal accidents. Report No. 0394”, Melbourne: Victorian Institute of Forensic Pathology and Monash University Department of Forensic Medicine. 9. Drummer OH 1998 “Drugs in drivers killed in Victorian road traffic accidents. Report No. 0298”, Melbourne: Victorian Institute of Forensic Medicine. 10. Drummer OH, Gerostamoulos J, Batziris H, Chu M, Caplehorn JRM, Robertson MD & Swann P 2003 “The incidence of drugs in drivers killed in Australian road traffic crashes”, Forensic Science International, 134:2–3, pp. 154–62. 11. Schwilke EW, Sampaio dos Santos MI & Logan BK 2006 “Changing patterns of drug and alcohol use in fatally injured drivers in Washington state”, Journal of Forensic Science, 51, pp. 1191–8. 12. Jackson PG, Tunbridge RJ & Rowe DJ 2000 “Drug recognition and field impairment testing: Evaluation of trials” in Alcohol, drugs and traffic safety. Proceedings of the 15th International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety, May 21–26, 2000, Stockholm, Sweden. 13. Mura P, Chatelain C, Dumestre V, Gaulier JM, Ghysel MH, Lacriox C, Kergueris MF, Lhermitte M, Moulsma M, Pepin G, Vincent F & Kintz P 2006 “Use of drugs of abuse in less than 30 year old drivers killed in a road crash in France: A spectacular increase for cannabis, cocaine and amphetamines”, Forensic Science International, 160, pp. 168–72. 14. Ch’ng CW, Fitzgerald M, Gerostamoulos J, Cameron B, Bui D, Drummer OH, Potter J & Odell M 2007 “Drug use in motor vehicle drivers presenting to an Australian, adult major trauma centre”, Emergency Medicine Australasia, 19, pp. 359–65. 15. Dawson A & Reid K 1997 “Fatigue, alcohol and performance impairment”, Nature, 388, pp. 235. 16. Williamson AM & Feyer A-M 2000 “Moderate sleep deprivation produces comprehensive cognitive and motor performance impairments equivalent to legally prescribed levels of alcohol intoxication”, Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 57, pp. 649–55. 17. Arnedt JT, Wilde GJS, Munt PW & MacLean AW 2001 “How do prolonged wakefulness and alcohol compare in the decrements they produce on a simulated driving task?” Accident Analysis and Prevention, 33, pp. 337–44. 18. Falleti MG, Maruff P, Collie A, Darby DG & McStephen M, 2003 “Qualitative similarities in cognitive impairment associated withy 24 h of sustained wakefulness and a blood alcohol concentration of 0.05%”, Journal of Sleep Research, 12, pp. 265–74. 19. Vakulin A, Baulk SD, Catcheside PG, Anderson R, van den Heuvel CJ, Banks S, Banks S & McEvoy RD 2007 “Effects of moderate sleep deprivation and low-dose alcohol on driving simulator performance and perception in young men”, Sleep, 30:10, pp. 1327–33. 20. Arnedt JT, Wilde GJS, Munt PW & Maclean AW 2000 “Simulated driving performance following prolonged wakefulness and alcohol consumption: separate and combined contributions to impairment”, Journal of Sleep Research, 9, pp. 233–41. 21. Kuypers K, Samyn N & Ramaekers J 2006 “MDMA and alcohol effects, combined and alone, on objective and subjective measures of actual driving performance and psychomotor”, Psychopharmacology, 187, pp. 467–75. 22. Robertson MD & Drummer OH 1994 “Responsibility analysis: methodology to study the effects of drugs in driving”, Accident Analysis and Prevention, 26:2 pp. 243–7. 23. Ogden E, Frederiksen T, Stough C & King R 2009 “Responsibility analysis: Screening of drugs in blood samples from injured drivers in Victoria”, Poster presented at the Australasian Professional Society on Alcohol and other Drugs, Darwin, Australia, 1–4 November. 24. Moskowitz H & Fiorentino D 2000 “A review of the literature on the effects of low doses of alcohol on driving related skills”, Washington: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. 25. Liguori A, D’Agostino Jr RB, Dworkin SI, Edwards D & Robinson JH, 1999 “Alcohol effects on mood, equilibrium, and simulated driving”, Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 23, pp. 815–21. 26. Rimm DC, Sininger RA, Faherty JD, Whitley MD & Perl MB 1982 “A balanced placebo investigation of the effects of alcohol vs. alcohol expectancy on simulated driving behavior”, Addictive Behaviors, 7, pp. 27–32. 27. Leung S & Starmer G 2005 “Gap acceptance and risk taking by young and mature drivers, both sober and alcohol intoxicated, in a simulated driving task”, Accident Analysis and Prevention, 37, pp. 1056–65 28. Iudice A, Bonanni E, Gelli A, Frittelli C, Iudice G, Cignoni F, Ghicopulos I, Murri L 2005 “Effects of prolonged wakefulness combined with alcohol and hands-free cell phone divided attention tasks on simulated driving”, Human Psychopharmacology 20, pp. 125–32. 29. Ranney TA & Gawron VJ 1986 “The effects of pavement edgelines on performance in a driving simulator under sober and alcohol-dosed conditions”, Human Factors, 28:5, pp. 511–25. Issues Paper | No. 12 | March 2010 19 30. Roehrs T, Beare D, Zorick F & Roth T 1994 “Sleepiness and ethanol effects on simulated driving”, Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 18, pp. 154–8. 31. Louwerens JW, Gloerich ABM, de Vries G, Brookhuis KA & O’Hanlon JF 1987 “The relationship between drivers’ blood alcohol concentration (BAC) and actual driving performance during high speed travel” in PC Noordzij & R Roszbach (eds), Alcohol, Drugs, and Traffic Safety-T86, Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers, pp. 183–7. 32. Vermeeren A & O’Hanlon JF 1998 “Fexofenadine’s effects, alone and with alcohol, on actual driving and psychomotor performance”, Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology, 101, pp. 306–11. 33. Strayer DL, Drews FA & Crouch DJ 2006 “A comparison of the cell phone driver and the drunk driver”, Human Factors, 48:2, pp. 381–91. 34. West R, Wilding J, French D, Kemp R & Irving A 1993 “Effects of low and moderate doses of alcohol on driving hazard perception latency and driving speed”, Addiction, 88:4, pp. 527–32. 35. Deery HA & Love AW 1996 “The effect of a moderate dose of alcohol on the traffic hazard perception profile of young drinkdrivers”, Addiction, 91:6, pp. 815–27. 36. Berghaus G, Scheer N & Schmidt P 1995 “Effects of cannabis on psychomotor skills and driving performance—a metaanalysis of experimental studies” in CN Kloeden & AJ Mclean (eds) Proceedings—13th International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety, NHMRC Road Accident Research Unit, The University of Adelaide, pp. 403–9. 37. Kelly E, Darke S, Ross J & Kelly E 2004 “A review of drug use and driving: Epidemiology, impairment, risk factors and risk perceptions”, Drug and Alcohol Review, 23:3, pp. 319–44. 38. Ramaekers JG, Berghaus G, van Laar M & Drummer OH 2004 “Dose related risk of motor vehicle crashes after cannabis use”, Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 73:2, pp. 109–19. 39. Smiley A 1998 “Marijuana: On road and driving simulator studies”, prepared for World Health Organisation, Geneva, Switzerland. 40. Smiley A 1986 “Marijuana: On-road and driving simulator studies”, Alcohol, Drugs and Driving 2:3–4, pp. 121–34. 41. Sexton BF, Turnbridge RJ, Brooke-Carter N, Jackson PG, Wright K, Stark MM & Englehart K 2000 “The influence of cannabis on driving: Technical report 477”, Transport Research Foundation Limited (TRL), United Kingdom. 42. Ramaekers JG, Robbe HW & O’Hanlon JF 2000 “Marijuana, alcohol and actual driving performance”, Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 15:7, pp. 551–8. 43. Papafotiou K, Carter JD & Stough C 2005 “The relationship between performance on the standardised field sobriety tests, driving performance and the level of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in blood”, Forensic Science International, 155:2–3, pp. 172–8. 44. Silber BY, Croft RJ, Papafotiou K & Stough C 2006 “The acute effects of d-amphetamine and methamphetamine on attention and psychomotor performance”, Psychopharmacology, 187:2, pp. 154–169. 45. Logan BK 1996 “Methamphetamine and driving impairment”, Journal of Forensic Sciences, 41:3, pp. 457–64. 46. Silber BY, Papafotiou K, Croft RJ, Ogden E, Swann P & Stough C 2005 “The effects of dexamphetamine on simulated driving performance”, Psychopharmacology, 179:3, pp. 536–43. 20 Drugs and driving 47. Stough O & Papafotiou K, Ogden E 2009 “Roadside saliva based testing for amphetamine-type stimulants in drivers”, Australian Research Council Discovery Grant. 48. Lamers CT, Ramaekers JG, Muntjewerff ND, Sikkema KL, Samyn N, Read NL, Brookhuis KA & Reidel WJ 2003 “Dissociable effects of a single dose of ecstasy (MDMA) on psychomotor skills and attentional performance”, Journal of Psychopharmacology, 17:4, pp. 379–87. 49. Ramaekers J & Kuypers K 2006 “Acute effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymetamphetamine (MDMA) on behavioral measures of impulsivity: alone and in combination with alcohol”, Neuropsychopharmacology, 31, pp. 1048–55. 50. Kuypers KPC, Wingen M, Samyn N, Limbert N & Ramaekers JG 2007 “Acute effects of nocturnal doses of MDMA on measures of impulsivity and psychomotor performance throughout the night”, Psychopharmacology, 192:1, pp.111–9. 51. Parrott AC & Lasky J 1998 “Ecstasy (MDMA) effects upon mood and cognition: before, during and after a Saturday night dance”, Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 139:3, pp. 261–8. 52. Ramaekers JG, Kuypers KPK, Samyn N 2006 “Stimulant effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) 75 mg and methylphenidate 20 mg on actual driving during intoxication and withdrawal”, Addiction, 101, pp. 1614–21. 53. De Waard D, Brookhuis KA & Pernot LMC 2000 “A driving simulator study on the effects of MDMA (Ecstasy) on driving performance and traffic safety”, Proceedings of the International Council on Alcohol Drugs and Traffic Safety (ICADTS), Stockhom, Sweden, May 2000. 54. Mozayani A 2002 “Ketamine-effects on human performance and behaviour”, Forensic Science Review, 14:1/2, pp. 124–31. 55. Curran H & Morgan C 2000 “Cognitive, dissociative and psychogenic effects of ketamine in recreational users on the night of drug use and 3 days later”, Addiction, 95:4, pp. 575–90. 56. Moore N & Bostwick J 1999 “Ketamine dependence in anesthesia providers”, Psychosomatics, 40:4, pp. 356–9. 57. Dillon P, Copeland J & Jansen K 2002 “Patterns of use and harms associated with non medical ketamine use”, Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 69:1, pp. 23–38. 58. Guillermo Y, Micallef J, Possamai C, Blin O & Hasbroucq T 2001 “N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors and information processing: human choice reaction time under a subanaesthetic dose of ketamine”, Neuroscience Letters, 303, pp. 29–32. 59. Cheng W, Ng K, Chan K, Mok V & Cheung B 2006 “Roadside detection of impairment under the influence of ketamine— evaluation of ketamine impairment symptoms with reference to its concentration in oral fluid and urine”, Forensic Science International, 170:1, pp. 51–8. 60. Martin WR & Fraser HF 1961 “A study of physiological and subjective effects of heroin and morphine administered intravenously in post-addicts”, The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 133, pp. 388–399. 61. Ogden E & Moskowitz H 2004 “Effects of alcohol and other drugs on driver performance”, Traffic Control Prevention, 5, pp. 185–98. 62. Zacny J, Lichtor J, Fleming D, Coalson D & Thompson W 1994 “A dose-response analysis of the subjective, psychomotor and physiological effects of intravenous morphine in healthy volunteers”, Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapy, 268, pp. 1–9. 63. Specka M, Finkbeier T, Lodemann E, Leifert K, Kluwig J & Gastpar M 2000 “Cognitive-motor performance of methadonemaintained patients”, European Addiction Research, 6, pp. 8–19. 64. Darke S, Sims J, Macdonald S & Wickes W 2000 “Cognitive impairment among methadone patients”, Addiction, 95:5, pp. 687–95. 65. Schindler S, Ortner R, Peternell A, Eder H, Opgenoorth E & Fischer G 2004 “Maintenance therapy with synthetic opioids and driving aptitude”, European Addiction Research, 10, pp. 80–7. 66. O’Neill W, Hanks G, Simpson P, Fallon M, Jenkins E & Wesnes K 2000 “The cognitive and psychomotor effects of morphine in healthy subjects: a randomized controlled trial of repeated (four) oral doses of dextropropoxyphene, morphine, lorazepam and placebo”, Pain, 85, pp. 209–15. 67. Hanks G, O’Neill W, Simpson P & Wesnes K 1995 “The cognitive and psychomotor effects of opioid analgesics. II. A randomised controlled trial of single doses of morphine, lorazepam and placebo in healthy subjects”, European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 48, pp. 455–60. 68. Brooke C, Ebnhage A, Fransson B, Haggi F, Jonzon B, Kraft I & Wesnes K 1998 “The effects of intravenous morphine on cognitive function in healthy volunteers”, Journal of Psychopharmacology, 12, pp. A45. 69. Bruera E, MacMillan K, Hanson J & MacDonald R 1989 “The cognitive effects of the administration of narcotic analgesics in patients with cancer pain”, Pain, 39:1, pp. 13–16. 70. Chesher G, Lemon J, Gomel M & Murphy G 1989 The effects of methadone, as used in a methadone maintenance program, on driving related skills. Sydney: National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre. 71. Fishbain D, Cutler R & Rosomoff H 2003 “Are opioid-dependent/ tolerant patients impaired in driving-related skills? A structured evidence-based review”, Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 25:6, pp. 559–77. 72. Smith A 1996 “Patients taking stable doses of morphine may drive” BMJ, 312:7022, pp. 56–7. 73. Drug and Alcohol Services, Government of South Australia 2006 Benzodiazepines, opioids and driving prescriber resource kit, Adelaide: Drug and Alcohol Services, Government of South Australia. 74. Walburga M, Laar VAN, Volkerts ER & Van Willigenburg AP 1992 “Therapeutic effects and effects on actual driving performance of chronically administered buspirone and diazepam in anxious outpatients”, Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 12, pp. 86–95. 75. O’Hanlon J & Ramaekers J 1995 “Antihistamine effects on actual driving performance in a standard test: a summary of Dutch experience”, Allergy 50, pp. 234–42. 76. Hindmarch I 1995 “The psychopharmacology of clobazam”, Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical & Experimental, 10, pp. 15–25. 77. Wetherell A 1979 “Individual and group effects of 10 mg diazepam on drivers’ ability, confidence and willingness to act in a gap-judging task”, Psychopharmacology, 63:3, pp. 259–67. 78. Austin M, Mitchell P & Goodwin G 2001 “Cognitive deficits in depression: possible implications for functional neuropathy”, British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, pp. 200–206. 79. Brunnauer A, Laux G, Geiger E, Soyka M & Moller H 2006 “Antidepressants and driving ability: results from a clinical study”, Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 67:11, pp. 1176–81. 80. Brunnauer A, Laux G, David I, Fric M, Hermisson I & Moller H 2008 “The impact of reboxitine and mirtazapine on driving simulator performance and psychomotor function in depressed patients”, Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69:12, pp. 1800–06. 81. Randy A, Sansone M, Lori A 2009 “Driving on antidepressants. Cruising for a crash?” Psychiatry, 6:9, pp. 13–16. 82. Castot A, Delorme B & Tricotel A 2009 “Medicinal products and driving”, Paris: AFSSAPS, pp. 1–26. 83. Hemmelgarn B, Levesque L & Suissa S 2006 “Anti-diabetic drug use and the risk of motor vehicle crash in the elderly”, Canadian Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 13:1, pp. 112–20. 84. Cox D, Gonder-Frederick L, Kovatchev B, Julian D & Clarke W 2000 “Progressive hypoglycaemia’s impact on driving simulation performance”, Diabetes Care, 23, pp. 163–70. 85. Stork A, Haeften T & Veneman T 2006 “Diabetes and driving, desired research methods and their pitfalls, current knowledge, and future research”, Diabetes Care, 29:8, pp. 1942–9. 86. Kay G 2000 “The effects of antihistamines on cognition and performance”, Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology, 105:6, pp. 622–7 87. Weiler J, Bloomfield J, Woodworth G, Grant A, Layton T, Brown T, McKenzie D, Baker T & Watson G 2000 “Effects of fexofenadine, diphenhydramine, and alcohol on driving performance. A randomized, placebo-controlled trial in the Iowa driving simulator”, Annals of Internal Medicine, 132:5, pp. 354–63. 88. Mason A & McBay A 1984 “Ethanol, marijuana, and other drug use in 600 drivers killed in single-vehicle crashes in north Carolina, 1978-1981”, Journal of Forensic Science, 29, pp. 987–1026. 89. Tunbridge R, Rowe D, Keigan M & Jackson P 2000 “The incidence of drugs in road accident fatalities in Great Britain”, paper presented at the International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety, Stockholm. 90. Barrett S, Gross S, Garand I & Pihl R 2005 “Patterns of simultaneous polysubstance use in Canadian rave attendees”, Substance Use Misuse, 40:9–10, pp. 1525–37. 91. Topp L, Hando J, Dillon P, Roche A & Solowij N 1999 “Ecstasy use in Australia: patterns of use and associated harm”, Drug and Alcohol Dependency, 55, pp. 105–15. 92. Hernandez-Lopez C, Farre M, Roset P, Menoyo E, Pizarro N, Ortuno J, Torrens M, Cami J & de la Torre R 2002 “3-4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy) and alcohol interactions in humans: psychomotor performance, subjective effects and pharmacokinetics”, Journal of Pharmacological Experimental Therapy, 300, pp. 236–44. 93. Brookhuis K, de Waard D & Samyn N 2004 “Effects of MDMA (ecstasy) and multiple drugs use on (simulated) driving performance and traffic safety”, Psychopharmacology, 173, pp. 440–45. 94. Dumont G, Wezenberg E, Valkenberg M, Jong C, Buitelaar J, Gerven J & Verkes R 2008 “Acute neuropsychological effects of MDMA and ethanol (co-) administration in healthy volunteers”, Psychopharmacology, 197, pp. 465–74. 95. Stein A, Allen R, Cook M & Karl R 1983 “A simulator study of the combined effects of alcohol and marijuana on driving behavior. Phase II. Report DOT HS-5-01257“, Washington, DC: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. 96. Robbe H 1998 “Marijuana’s impairing effects on driving are moderate when taken alone but severe when combined with alcohol“, Human Psychopharmacology, Clinical and Experimental, 13, pp. S70–S78. 97. Riedel W, Vermeeren A, Van Boxtel M, Vuurman E, Verhey F, Jolles J & Ramaekers J 1998 “Mechanisms of drug-induced driving impairment: a dimensional approach”, Human Psychopharmacology, Clinical and Experimental, 13, pp. S49–S63. 98. Van Steveninck A, Gieschke R, Schoemaker R, Pieters M, Kroon J, Breimer D & Cohen A 1993 “Pharmacodynamic interactions of diazepam and intravenous alcohol at pseudo steady state”, Psychopharmacology, 110, pp. 471–8. Issues Paper | No. 12 | March 2010 21 99. English L, Pycock C, Roberts C, Shorsbree E & Taberner P 1982 “The interactions between ethanol and nitrazepam and temazepam on human psychomotor performance”, British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 14, pp. 620P–621P. 100. Taberner P, Roberts C, Shrosbree E, Pycock C & English L 1983 “An investigation into the interaction between ethanol at low doses and the benzodiazepines nitrazepam and temazepam on psychomotor performance in normal subjects”, Psychopharmacology, 81:4, pp. 321–6. 101. Weathermon R & Crabb D 1999 “Alcohol and medication interactions”, Alcohol Health & Research World 23:1, pp. 40–55. 102. Neutal I 1998 “Benzodiazepine-related traffic accidents in young and elderly drivers”, Human Pyschopharmacology, Clinical and Experimental, 13, pp. S115–S123. 103. Lenne M, Dietze P, Rumbold G, Redman J & Triggs T 2002 “The effects of the opioid pharmacotherapies methadone, LAAM and buprenorphine, alone and in combination with alcohol, on simulated driving”, Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 21:3, pp. 271–8. 104. Chesher G 1989 “Understanding the opioid analgesics and their effects on skills performance”, Alcohol Drugs Driving, 5, pp. 111–38. 105. Ramaekers J, Uiterwijk M & O’Hanlon J 1992 “Effects of loratadine and cetirizine on actual driving and psychometric test performance and EEG during driving”, European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 42, pp. 363–9. 106. Jauregui I, Mullol J, Barta J, Cuvillo A, Davila I, Montoro J, Sastre J & Valero A 2006 “H1 antihistamines: psychomotor performance and driving”, Journal of investigational allergology and clinical immunology, 16:1, pp. 37–44. 107. Elliot B 2004 “Strategic Review of Best Practice: Key issues in the delivery of TSE in Victoria. Report prepared for TAC and Victoria’s arrive alive! Partnership Agencies”, Melbourne: Transport Accident Commission. 108. McBride N, Farringdon F, Midford R, Meuleners L & Philip M 2004 “Harm minimisation in school drug education: Final results of the School Health and Alcohol Harm Reduction Project (SHAHRP)”, Addiction, 99:3, pp. 278–91. 109. Rodriguez L & Anderson-Wilk M 2002 Communicating highway safety: what works, Centre for Transportation Research and Education, Iowa State University, CTRE Project 01-85. 110. Delhomme P, Truls V, Thierry M, Gordon H, Goldenbeld C, Stina J, Nicola C & Vlasta R 1999 “Deliverable 4. Evaluated road safety media campaigns: An overview of 265 evaluated campaigns and some meta-analysis on accidents. Gadget project. Contract No. RO-97-SC.2235”. INRETS. 111. Cameron MH, Haworth N, Oxley J, Newstead S & Le T 1993 Evaluation of Transport Accident Commission road safety television advertising, Report No. 52, Melbourne: Monash University Accident Research Centre. 112. Cameron M & Newstead S 2000 ‘Response by Monash University Accident Research Centre to “Re-investigation of the effectiveness of the Victorian Transport Accident Commission’s road safety advertising campaigns” (White, Walker, Glonek and Burns, 2000) (Report No. 177).’ Melbourne: Monash University Accident Research Centre. 113. Newstead S, Cameron M & Narayan S 1998 Further modeling of some major factors influencing road trauma trends in Victoria: 1990–1996. Report 129, Melbourne: Monash University Accident Research Centre. 114. Mallick J, Johnston J, Goren N & Kennedy V 2007 Drugs and driving in Australia, a survey of community attitudes, experience and understanding, Melbourne: Australian Drug Foundation, pp. 1–110. 22 Drugs and driving 115. International Council on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety 2001 “The ICADTS working group on prescribing and dispensing guidelines for medicinal drugs affecting driving performance”, www.icadts.org/reporter. 116. Queensland Transport 2003 “Drugs and driving public education campaign”, www.transport.qld.gov.au 117. Saltz R 1987 “The role of bars and restaurants in preventing alcohol impaired driving; an evaluation of server intervention”, Evaluation and Health Professions, 10, pp. 5–27. 118. Russ NW & Geller ES 1987 “Training bar personnel to prevent drunken driving: A field evaluation”, American Journal of Public Health, 77, pp. 952–4. 119. McKnight AJ 1988 “Development and field test of a responsible alcohol service program volume I: research findings. Report no. DOT HS 807 221”, Washington, DC: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, Department of Transportation. 120. Lang E, Stockwell T, Rydon P & Beel A 1998 “Can training bar staff in responsible serving practices reduce alcohol-related harm?” Drug and Alcohol Review, 17:1, pp. 39–50. 121. Stockwell T 2001 “Responsible alcohol service: Lessons from evaluations of server training and policing initiatives”, Drug Alcohol Review, 20, pp. 257–65. 122. McKnight AJ & Streff FM 1994 “The effect of enforcement upon service of alcohol to intoxicated patrons of bars and restaurants”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, 26:1, pp. 79–88. 123. Babor T, Caetano R, Casswell S, Edwards G, Giesbrecht N, Graham K, Grube J, Gruenewald P, Hill L, Holder H, Homel R, Österberg E, Rehm J, Room R & Rossow I 2003 Alcohol: No ordinary commodity. Research and public policy, Oxford: Oxford University Press. 124. Health Advisory Service 1996 Children and young people: substance misuse services, London: HMSO. 125. Standing Conference on Drug Abuse (SCODA) 1997 Drug-related early intervention: developing services for young people and families, London: SCODA. 126. Turner G & Shepherd J 1999 “A method in search of a theory: peer education and health promotion”, Health Education Research, 14:2, pp. 235–47. 127. Orme J & Starkey F 1999 “Peer drug education: the way forward?” Health Education, 1, pp. 8–16. 128. VIVAIDS 2005 “RaveSafe survey”, Melbourne: Centre For Youth Drug Studies. 129. Boorman M 2004 “Detection of drug impaired drivers— Standard Field Sobriety Tests”, The 17th Meeting of the International Council on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety [ICADTS] Glasgow, Scotland, United Kingdom, 8–13 August 2004. 130. Perl J & Moynahan A 1999 “Special aspects of drugs and driving”, Paper presented to the Australasian Conference on Drugs Strategy, Adelaide, 27–29 April. 131. Collins DJ & Lapsley HM 1996 The social costs of drug abuse in Australia in 1988 and 1992, National Drug Strategy monograph series no. 30, Canberra: Commonwealth Department of Human Services and Health. 132. Hoskins B 2005 “Roadside detection of drug impaired driving” in Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Clinical Forensic Medicine of the World Police Medical Officers, May 13, 2005. 133. Boorman M & Owens K 2009 “The Victorian legislative framework for the random testing drivers at the roadside for the presence of illicit drugs: An evaluation of the characteristics of drivers detected from 2004 to 2006”, Traffic Injury Prevention, 10, pp. 16–22. 134. Freeman J & Liossis P 2002 “Drink driving rehabilitation programs and alcohol ignition interlocks: Is there a need for more research?” Road and Transport Research, 4, pp. 3–13. 135. Sheehan MC, Watson BC, Schonfeld CC, Wallace AM & Partridge BJ 2005 Drink driver rehabilitation and education in Victoria, Melbourne: Royal Automobile Club of Victoria (RACV). 136. Beirness DJ & Marques P 2004 “Alcohol ignition interlock programs”, Traffic Injury Prevention, 5, pp. 299–308. 137. O’Hare M 2005 “Alcohol Interlocks as a management option for recidivist drink-drivers”, Melbourne: Monash University Accident Research Centre. www.austroads.com.au 138. Schonfeld CC & Sheehan MC 2004 “Critical overview of alcohol ignition interlock programs in Australia” in 17th International Conference on Alcohol Drugs and Traffic Safety, T2004, August 9, 2004, Glasgow. 139. Banfield J 2009 “Effectiveness of the P.A.R.T.Y (Prevent Alcohol and Risk-Related Trauma in Youth) Program in preventing trauma injuries: a ten year analysis”, The combined Australasian Trauma Society and Trauma Association of Canada Annual Scientific Meeting, Auckland, New Zealand, 5–7 March 2009. 140. Ray W, Fought R & Decker M 1992 “Psychoactive drugs and the risk of injurious motor vehicle crashes in elderly drivers”, American Journal of Epidemiology, 136, pp. 873–83. 141. Leveille S, Buchner D & Koepsell T 1994 “Psychoactive medications and injurious motor vehicle crashes in elderly drivers”, American Journal of Epidemiology, 5, pp. 591–8. 142. Barbone F, McMahon A & Davey P 1998 “Use of benzodiazepines increased road traffic accidents whereas use of tricyclic antidepressants and SSRIs did not”, Lancet, 24, pp. 1331–6. 143. Edwards J 1995 “Drug choice in depression: selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors or tricyclic antidepressants?” CNS Drugs, 4, pp. 141–59. 144. Ridout F, Meadows R & Johnsen S 2003 “A placebo controlled investigation into the effects of paroxetine and mirtazapine on measures related to car driving performance”, Human Psychopharmacology, 18, pp. 261–9. 145. Betts T 1981 “Effects of beta blockade on driving”, Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 11: 2, pp. 540–5. 146. Almova I & Elgarov I 1999 “Atenolol and ramipril in arterially hypertensioned motor vehicle drivers: efficacy and safety”, Journal of Traffic Medicine, supplement 22:1. 147. McGwin G. Sims R. Pulley L & Roseman J 2000 “Relations among chronic medical conditions, medications, and automobile crashes in elderly: a population-based case-control study”, American Journal of Epidemiology, 152:5, pp. 424–31. 148. Verster J & Edmund R 2004 “Antihistamines and driving ability: evidence from on-the-road driving studies during normal traffic”, Annals of Allergy, Asthma and Immunology, 92:3, pp. 294–30. 149. O’Hanlon J, Vermeeren A, Uiterwijk AA, van Veggel LM & Swijgman HF 1995 “Anxiolytics’ effects on the actual driving performance of patients and healthy volunteers in a standardized test. An integration of three studies”, Neuropsychobiology, 31:2, p. 81. 150. Hennessy S & Strom, B 2000 “Non sedating antihistamines should be preferred over sedating histamines in patients who drive“, Annals of Internal Medicine 132:5, pp. 405–07. Issues Paper | No. 12 | March 2010 23 Reading and Resource List DRUG No. 30 • March 2010 NFO clearinghouse www.druginfo.adf.org.au Drugs and driving This list is intended as a guide and a starting point for the researcher. It does not aim to be comprehensive of the subject. For further information please search the library online public access catalogue, or contact DrugInfo for assistance. The list is sorted chronologically and by author within each time period. All of the following resources are available in the DrugInfo Clearinghouse Library. Alcohol and driving—effects Allen AJ, Meda SA, Skudlarski P, Calhoun VD, Astur R, Ruopp KC & Pearlson GD 2009 “Effects of alcohol on performance on a distraction task during simulated driving”, Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 33:4, pp. 617–25. This study examines the role of the serotonin (5-HT) system in alcohol-related aggression. Specifically, it experimentally examined the effects of 5-HT augmentation on alcohol-related aggression in men. The results indicated that alcohol intoxication increased aggression, particularly under low provocation. Brown TG, Oimet MC, Nadeau L, Gianoulakis C, Lepage M, Tremblay J & Dongier M 2009 “From the brain to bad behaviour and back again: neurocognitive and psychobiological mechanisms of driving while impaired by alcohol”, Drug and Alcohol Review 28:4, pp. 406–18. Driving while impaired by alcohol (DWI) is responsible for substantial mortality and injury. This narrative review summarises the evidence for the contribution of neurocognitive and psychobiological mechanisms to DWI behaviour and recidivism. Marczinski CA & Fillmore MT 2009 “Acute alcohol tolerance on subjective intoxification and simulated driving performance in binge drinkers”, Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 23:2, pp. 238–47. check link High rates of binge drinking and alcohol-related problems, including drinking and driving, occur among college students, however underlying reasons for the heightened impaired driving rates in this demographic group are not known. This study investigates the effects of acute alcohol tolerance on subjective intoxification, simulated driving performance and willingness to drive in binge drinkers. Miller MA, Weafer J & Fillmore MT 2009 “Gender differences in alcohol impairment of simulated driving performance and driving-related skills”, Alcohol and Alcoholism 44:6, pp. 586–93. 24 Drugs and driving Although laboratory studies indicate that the intensity of impairment from alcohol is generally dependent on blood alcohol concentration, some reviews of the literature suggest that gender is also a contributory factor. This study tests this hypothesis by measuring alcohol-induced impairment in men and women in functions relating to driving performance. Gustin Jl & Simons JS 2008 “Perceptions of level of intoxication and risk related to drinking and driving”, Addictive Behaviors 33:4, pp. 605–15. This study investigated variables of perceived risk associated with one’s decision to drink and drive, as well as with the occurrence and success of intervention efforts by others in preventing individuals from drinking and driving. Alcohol and driving—prevalence Trimboli L & Smith N 2009 Drink-driving and recidivism in NSW, Sydney: NSW Bureau of Crime Statistics and Research. This bulletin provides information on re-offending among drink-drivers in NSW. Overall, 15.5 per cent of drink-drivers returned to court for another drink-driving offence within five years, and 14.3 per cent returned to court within five years charged with another offence, such as a registration, a roadworthiness or a driving licence offence. www.lawlink.nsw.gov.au/lawlink/bocsar/ll_bocsar.nsf/ vwFiles/cjb135.pdf/$file/cjb135.pdf Flowers NT, Naimi TS, Brewer RD, Elder RW, Shults RA & Jiles R 2008 “Patterns of alcohol consumption and alcohol-impaired driving in the United States”, Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 32:4, pp. 639–51. This article investigates prevalence and patterns of alcohol consumption and alcohol-impaired driving in the United States. A strong association was found between alcoholimpaired driving and binge drinking. Senserrick T, Hoareau E, Lough B, Diamantopoulou K & Fotheringham M 2004 Involvement of 21–26 year olds in drink-driving behaviour, Melbourne: Monash University. This project examined the involvement of 21–26 yearold Victorian drivers in drink-driving behaviour following concerns of their over-representation in alcohol-related, fatal and serious casualty crashes. A perceived need/ desire to get home was the most commonly reported reason for drink-driving. DrugInfo Clearinghouse no. AN66 SEN www.monash.edu.au/muarc/reports/muarc211.html Alcohol and driving—interventions Mills KL, Hodge W, Johansson K & Conigrave K 2008 “An outcome evaluation of the New South Wales Sober Driver Programme: a remedial programme for recidivist drink drivers”, Drug and Alcohol Review 27:1, pp. 65–74. This study evaluates the effectiveness of a remedial program for recidivist drink drivers, the New South Wales Sober Driver Programme (SDP). The program combines educational components and elements of group cognitive behavioural therapy in relation to drinkdriving behaviour. It is delivered in conjunction with punitive sanctions. The aims of this study were to assess New Zealanders’ knowledge, attitudes and behaviours around driving under the influence of psychoactive substances, including illicit drugs, prescription medicines, and alcohol, for both users and non-users, and drug drivers and non-drug drivers. www.idpc.net/sites/default/files/library/Drug%20 Driving%20in%20New%20Zealand,%20full%20 research%20report,%20August%202009.pdf Matthews A, Bruno R, Johnston J, Black E, Degenhardt L & Dunn M 2009 “Factors associated with driving under the influence of alcohol and drugs among an Australian sample of regular ecstasy users”, Drug and Alcohol Dependence 100:1–2, pp. 24–31. This study investigates factors associated with driving under the influence of alcohol and other drugs (ecstasy, cannabis and methamphetamine) among a group of regular ecstasy users. Participants were those who participated in the Australian Ecstasy and related Drug Reporting System (EDRS) in 2007 and had recently driven a motor vehicle. McIntosh J, O’Brien T & McKeganey N 2008 “Drug driving and the management of risk: the perspectives and practices of a sample of problem drug users”, International Journal of Drug Policy 19:3, pp. 248–54. Bjerre B & Kostela JS 2007 “Positive health-care effects of an alcohol ignition interlock programme among driving while impaired (DWI) offenders”, Addiction 102:11, pp.1771–81. This paper reports on a qualitative study of the attitudes and risk management strategies of a sample of problem drug users in relation to driving while under the influence of drugs. This study compares the costs of hospital care and sick leave/disability pensions between two groups of driving while impaired offenders. One group was in an alcohol ignition interlock program and the other had revoked licences. Walsh JM, Verstraete AG, Huestis MA & Morland J 2008 “Guidelines for research on drugged driving”, Addiction 103:8, pp. 1258–68. Delaney A, Diamantopoulou K & Cameron M 2006 Strategic principles of drink-driving enforcement, Melbourne: Monash University Accident Research Centre. This study reviews existing drink-driving enforcement research conducted by the Monash University Accident Research Centre to develop operational principles of drink-driving enforcement and emphasise where the best results are to be achieved. www.monash.edu/muarc/reports/muarc249.html Drug driving Hammond K 2009 Drug driving in New Zealand: a survey of community attitudes, experience and understanding, Wellington: New Zealand Drug Foundation (NZDF). This article explores issues surrounding the need to standardise the variables being used to measure the public health impact of drug-use on driving and overall traffic safety. Australian Institute of Criminology 2008 Drug driving in Australia, Canberra: Australian Institute of Criminology. In 2007 it was estimated that 12 per cent of Australians aged 14 years and over had driven a motor vehicle while under the influence of alcohol, and 3 per cent had driven while under the influence of illegal drugs in the previous 12 months. DrugInfo Clearinghouse no. vf AIC 08 www.aic.gov.au/documents/7/A/B/%7B7AB1481A4036-4811-A17E-5FCD841C8167%7Dcfi173.pdf Reading and Resource List | No. 30 | March 2010 25 Caldicott D, Pfeiffer J, Edwards N & Pearce A 2007 The impact of drugs on road crashes, assaults and other trauma: a prospective trauma toxicology study, Canberra: National Drug Law Enforcement Research Fund This study examines the experimental and epidemiologic evidence linking benzodiazepine use to driving impairment. DrugInfo Clearinghouse no. research NDLERF 20 www.ndlerf.gov.au/pub/Monograph_20.pdf Cannabis and driving Ch’ng CW, Fitzgerald M, Gerostamoulos J, Cameron P, Bui D, Drummer OH, Potter J & Odell M 2007 “Drug use in motor vehicle drivers presenting to an Australian, adult major trauma centre”, Emergency Medicine Australasia, 13 May, pp. 1–7. This article reports the drug use in injured Victorian drivers involved in motor vehicle collisions and subsequently transported to a major adult trauma centre in Victoria. DrugInfo Clearinghouse no. vf CH’NG 07 Mallick J, Johnston J, Goren N & Kennedy V 2007 Drugs and driving in Australia: a survey of community attitudes, experience and understanding, Melbourne: Australian Drug Foundation. This research focuses on the prevalence of drug driving in Australia, the driving impairment associated with drug use, the attitudes and perceptions of drivers towards drugs and driving and the road safety countermeasures adopted to address drugs and driving. DrugInfo Clearinghouse no. AN65 MAL www.druginfo.adf.org.au/downloads/External_Reports/ Drugs_and_Driving_in_Australia_fullreport.pdf Amphetamines and driving Jones AW, Holmgren A & Kugelberg FC 2008 “Driving under the influence of central stimulant amines: age and gender differences in concentration of amphetamine, methamphetamine, and ecstasy in blood”, Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs 69:2, pp. 202–8. A zero-tolerance law for driving under the influence of drugs was introduced in Sweden in 1999. This article reports the age and gender of offenders along with the concentrations of amphetamine, methamphetamine, and ecstasy in blood samples analysed since the institution of the new legislation. Benzodiazepines and driving Rapoport MJ, Lanctot KL, Streiner DL, Bedard M, Vingilis E, Murray B, Schaffer A, Shulman KI & Herrmann N 2009 “Benzodiazepine use and driving: a meta-analysis”, Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 70:5, pp. 663–73. 26 Drugs and driving DrugInfo Clearinghouse no. vf RAPOPORT 09 Sewell RA, Poling J & Sofuoglu M 2009 “The effect of cannabis compared with alcohol on driving”, American Journal on Addictions 18:3, pp. 185–93. Cannabis and alcohol acutely impair several drivingrelated skills in a dose-related fashion, but the effects of cannabis vary more between individuals than they do with alcohol because of tolerance, differences in smoking technique, and different absorptions of tetrahydrocannabinol, the active ingredient in cannabis. Jones AW, Holmgren A & Kugelberg FC 2008 “Driving under the influence of cannabis: a 10year study of age and gender differences in the concentrations of tetrahydrocannabinol in blood”, Addiction 103:3, pp. 452–61. This paper compares age, gender and the concentrations of THC in blood of individuals apprehended for driving under the influence of drugs in Sweden, where a zerotolerance law operates. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ & Boden JM 2008 “Is driving under the influence of cannabis becoming a greater risk to driver safety than drink driving? Findings from a longitudinal study”, Accident Analysis and Prevention 40, pp. 1345–50. This study examined the associations between driving under the influence of (a) cannabis and (b) alcohol, and motor vehicle collisions during, in a longitudinal study of a New Zealand birth cohort. The results suggest that, for some populations, the risks of driving under the influence of cannabis may now be greater than the risks of driving under the influence of alcohol. Jones C, Donnelly N, Wendy S & Weatherburn D 2005 Driving under the influence of cannabis: the problem and potential countermeasures, Sydney: BOCSAR This bulletin assessed (a) whether recent drug drivers were more likely to self-report accidents than nonintoxicated drivers; (b) the likely deterrent effect of roadside drug testing (RDT), increasing the severity of sanctions for drug driving and providing factual information about accident risk associated with drugdriving; and (c) what factors were predictive of driving under the influence of cannabis. www.lawlink.nsw.gov.au/lawlink/bocsar/ll_bocsar.nsf/ vwFiles/CJB87.pdf/$file/CJB87.pdf Gamma hydroxybutyrate and driving In this study, focus group discussion was used to elicit descriptive experiences of driving under the influence of gamma hydroxybutyrate (GHB). This study used a pre- to post-design to evaluate the influence on drinking-and-driving fatal crashes of six laws directed at young people aged 20 years and younger and four laws targeting all drivers. Data on the laws were drawn from the Alcohol Policy Information System data set (1998 to 2005), the Digests of State Alcohol Highway Safety Related Legislation (1983 to 2006), and the Westlaw database. Opioids and driving Detection approaches Bernard JP, Morland J, Krogh M & Khiabani HZ 2009 “Methadone and impairment in apprehended drivers”, Addiction 104:3, pp. 457–64. Brady JE, Baker SP, DiMaggio C, McCarthy ML, Rebok GW & Li G 2009 “Effectiveness of mandatory alcohol testing programs in reducing alcohol involvement in fatal motor carrier crashes”, American Journal of Epidemiology 170:6, pp. 775–82. Barker JC & Karsoho H 2008 “Hazardous use of gamma hydroxybutyrate: driving under the influence”, Substance Use & Misuse 43, pp. 1507–20. This study investigates apprehended drivers who had methadone in their blood at the time of apprehension and the relationship between blood methadone concentration and impairment as measured by the clinical test of impairment. Baewert A, Gombas W, Schinler S-D, PeternellMoelzer A, Eder H, Jagsch R & Fischer G 2007 “Influence of peak and trough levels of opioid maintenance therapy on driving aptitude”, European Addiction Research 13:3, pp. 127–35 The Addiction Clinic at Medical University Vienna conducted a prospective, open-label trial where 40 opioid-dependent patients maintained either on buprenorphine or methadone were assessed regarding their traffic-relevant performance. Prevention approaches Asbridge M, Mann RE, Smart RG, Stoduto G, Beirness D, Lamble R & Vingilis E 2009 “The effects of Ontario’s administrative driver’s licence suspension law on total driver fatalities: a multiple time series analysis”, Drugs, Education, Prevention and Policy 16:2, pp. 140–51. On 29 November 1996, Ontario introduced an Administrative Driver’s Licence Suspension (ADLS) law, which required that anyone charged with driving with a blood alcohol concentration (BAC) over the legal limit or failing to provide a breath sample would have their licence suspended for a period of 90 days at the time the charge was laid. This study evaluates the effects of Ontario’s ADLS law on total driver fatalities over a 25-month period after the law was introduced, and compares Ontario’s experience with that of two comparison provinces (Manitoba and New Brunswick) that did not introduce ADLS at that time. Using data from the United States Fatality Analysis Reporting System during 1982–2006, the authors assessed the effectiveness of mandatory alcohol testing programs in reducing alcohol involvement in fatal motor carrier crashes. DrugInfo Clearinghouse no. vf BRADY 09 Global Road Safety Partnership 2007 Drinking and driving: a road safety manual for decision-makers and practitioners, Geneva: Global Road Safety Partnership. The purpose of this manual is to inform readers of practical ways to develop programs to reduce drinking and driving. DrugInfo Clearinghouse no. AN66 GLO www.grsproadsafety.org/themes/default/pdfs/ Drinking%20Driving%20Manual.pdf Stough C, Boorman M, Ogden E & Papafotiou K 2006 An evaluation of the Standardised Field Sobriety Tests for the detection of impairment associated with cannabis with and without alcohol, Canberra: AGPS. This research aimed to investigate the effects of cannabis and alcohol on driving performance, and on Standardised Field Sobriety Test (SFST) performance. DrugInfo Clearinghouse no. MO62 STO www.ndlerf.gov.au/pub/Monograph_17.pdf Audiovisual resources Moreland Hall 2007 Driving straight. Drugs and driving educational DVD, Melbourne: Moreland Hall Fell JC, Fisher DA, Voas RB, Blackman K & Tippetts AS 2009 “The impact of underage drinking laws on alcohol-related fatal crashes of young drivers”, Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 33:7, pp. 1208–19. This DVD is designed to support drug education programs by providing information about drugs and driving in the State of Victoria. The DVD contains acted scenarios based on actual case studies and interviews with a number of key experts. DrugInfo Clearinghouse no. adf av AN65 MOR Reading and Resource List | No. 30 | March 2010 27 All material listed is available from the Australian Drug Foundation library. Membership to the library is open to professionals in Victoria who work in the areas of health, welfare and education. Members are able to borrow from the collection as well as access other services provided by the library. Membership is free to these groups. For more information about membership or how to access material: Tel: 1300 85 85 84 (Monday to Friday, 9am to 5pm) Fax: (03) 9328 3008 Email:library@adf.org.au Or visit our website at: www.druginfo.adf.org.au/library www.druginfo.adf.org.au DrugInfo Clearinghouse 409 King Street West Melbourne VIC 3003 Email: druginfo@adf.org.au