Do Women and Men Respond Differently to Negative News? Stuart Soroka, McGill University Elisabeth Gidengil, McGill University Patrick Fournier, Université de Montréal Lilach Nir, Hebrew University Paper prepared for the First Annual School of Politics and Global Studies Working Group Conference: Women, Media, and Politics: A Comparative Perspective Introduction There is a persistent gender gap in news consumption and attention to hard news. Women in the United States report spending significantly less time consuming news than men do (Benesch 2012). This is the case for reading newspapers, listening to the news on the radio and getting news online. The gender gap only ceases to be significant in the case of watching news on television. Moreover, women are significantly less likely than men to follow news about politics in Washington and international affairs. These gender gaps cannot be explained by socioeconomic or other socio-demographic differences. They hold regardless of income, education, labor market status, marital status, race and age. The gender gaps are not confined to the United States. Data from the World Values Survey reveal a significant gender gap in news consumption in 45 of 63 countries, even controlling for sociodemographic characteristics (Benesch 2012). Similarly, the European Social Survey reports that women spend less time than men consuming news in all but two of the 28 countries surveyed (Benesch 2012). The pervasive negativity of news content has been suggested as one reason why women typically pay less attention than men to news about politics (Grabe and Kamhawi 2006; Kamhawi and Grabe 2008). Negative content dominates the news (for a review of the vast literature, see Soroka 2014). As Newhagen (1998) has observed, viewers of the typical television news broadcast are exposed to “more images of violence, suffering, and death in a half hour than most people would normally view in a lifetime” (p. 267). Surveys report that women are much more likely than men to cite an undue focus on war and violence as a reason for not following international news (Pew 2002) and many more women than men say that they often find the news depressing (Pew 2004). Women are also less likely than men to say that they find the news entertaining or enjoyable and more likely to agree that there is “too much violence, disaster and tragedy” in television news shows (Klein 2003). This same survey reported that women were more likely than men to report that television news makes them worry and that violent story or picture content causes them to turn the news off. Women were also more likely to say that they would be happy to see more good news. The survey evidence is certainly suggestive of a possible link between negativity in the news and the gender gap in news consumption. However, only a handful of studies have used experimental designs to test whether there is indeed a causal connection. The results have been mixed. Moreover, these experiments have relied on self-reports, raising concerns about possible social desirability bias as well as the potential unreliability of reports of internal states (Kleemans et al. 2012). In this paper, we report the results of a series of psychophysiological experiments that use physiological measurements of skin conductance and heart rate to test for gender differences in responses to negative news stories. Experiments on Gender and Negativity in the News Grabe and Kamhawi (Grabe and Kamhawi 2006; Kamhawi and Grabe 2008) conducted the first experiments on gendered responses to negativity in the news. They draw on evolutionary psychology to explain why responses to negative news are likely to be ! 1! gendered.1 Underpinning their explanation is “the media-equal-real-life idea” (Grabe 2011). The media equation holds that “There is no switch in the brain that can be thrown to distinguish the real and mediated worlds” (Reeves and Nass 1996). As a result, viewers are apt to respond automatically to threatening media messages as if they represented present and immediate threats. Indeed, according to Shoemaker (1996), the media fulfill a critical surveillance function in alerting audiences to potential threats, and human beings, in turn, are “hardwired” to prefer such negative news. According to evolutionary psychologists, human beings have been biologically programmed to respond to potential threats to their survival. These threats can induce either approach or avoidance, depending on the seriousness of the threat to survival. However, the response also depends on the gender of the person who is experiencing the threat. Research in neuroscience and psychology has found that women appear to exhibit a stronger avoidance response in the face of threat than men do. In evolutionary terms, this is attributed to the importance of the mother’s survival to the survival of her offspring. Men’s propensity to approach potential dangers, on the other hand, is linked to the need to protect their offspring. Grabe and Kamhawi suggest that these gender differences in responses to threat will also be evident when the threat is mediated rather than real. As a result, women will be more likely than men to avoid negative news. Grabe and Kamhawi use laboratory experiments to test a number of empirical implications of their argument. Participants were presented with eight stories. All eight stories featured bad news but two stories were negatively framed, two stories were positively framed and the other four were ambiguous (positively framed narration with negatively framed video and vice versa). As predicted, men reported enjoying the negatively framed stories more than women did (Kamhawi and Grabe 2008). Men also reported more appreciation of the quality of the journalism and greater identification with the people featured in the negatively framed stories than women did. These patterns were all reversed for the positively framed stories.2 The same experiment was used to test for gender differences in approach-avoidance as manifested in the allocation of processing resources and levels of arousal (Grabe and Kamhawi 2006). They predict that men would allocate more processing resources to negatively framed stories than women do. They test this by measuring participants’ recognition memory and comprehension of the stories. As predicted, men scored significantly higher on both recognition and comprehension of the negatively framed stories than women did. The pattern was reversed for positively framed stories but was only significant for comprehension. !!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!! 1 Note, though, that Grabe and Kamhawi (Grabe and Kamhawi 2006; Kamhawi and Grabe 2008) do not attribute the gender differences entirely to biological hardwiring. They recognize that socialization may play a role in reinforcing biological predispositions. 2 As predicted, women reported greater enjoyment of the ambiguously framed stories than men did as well as higher levels of identification with the main characters. They were also generally more appreciative of the professionalism, objectivity and so on of these stories. ! 2! Grabe and Kamhawi also predict that men will be more aroused by negative stories than women are. As predicted, men rated the negatively framed stories significantly more arousing than the positively framed stories. Women, on the other hand, reported higher arousal for the positively framed stories than they did for the negatively framed ones. However, contrary to predictions, there were no significant gender differences in reported arousal for the negatively framed stories. Indeed, if anything, women reported higher arousal than men. A recent Dutch experiment also reports null findings. Kleemans and her colleagues (2012) find no evidence of gender differences in preferences for negative news content. Participants were shown four different news stories. Two stories were negative and two were neutral.3 After watching each story, participants were asked how likely they would be to change channels if they had the opportunity. Once they had watched all four stories, they were shown the introductory frames of pairs of the stories and asked which of each pair they would choose to watch again. Neither the “staying tuned” measure nor the “choosing” measure revealed significant differences between women and men in their preferences for negative content. The authors suggest that the null findings may be explained by the European practice of including eyewitnesses and victims in news reports. They suggest that this practice serves to “feminize” the news by emphasizing the human interest aspect and encouraging emotional involvement in the news story through a less detached form of reporting (see van Zoonen 1998). The effect, they argue, may be to inhibit women’s avoidance response to negative content. The findings in the existing literature are thus somewhat mixed on whether there are gender differences in reactions to negative news content. These mixed findings are partly a function of the use of rather different dependent variables: self-reported arousal, “staying tuned,” comprehension and memory, and so on. In this study, we focus on variables that more directly capture the variables of interest—arousal and attention. We do so using physiological measurements of skin conductance and heart rate. Methodology Our results are based on an experiment on the impact of negative versus positive television content on a range of physiological indicators (Soroka, Nir and Fournier 2013).4 There are a number of advantages to using psycho-physiological measurements to examine viewers’ responses to television news. They may be particularly useful when it comes to getting information about emotional responses (see discussion in Ravaja 2004.) People may not be consciously aware of their own responses, making their self-reports unreliable. Social !!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!! 3 The form of the stories was also varied. For each type of content, one story was produced in a tabloid style while the other had a standard production style. 4 Note that the primary objective was not to examine gender differences in reactivity to negative content, but all the experiments included both males and females. Our description of the experiment draws on Soroka and McAdams (forthcoming). ! 3! desirability bias may also inhibit accurate reporting of their internal states. Viewers, for example, may be reluctant to admit to being aroused by watching portrayals of violent behavior.5 As a result, the conclusions drawn from self-reports of arousal or attention may be at odds with those based on physiological responses. Indeed, faced with such findings, Balzer and Jacobs (2011) suggest that comparing self reported states with physiological responses can provide insight into the role of socialization. Moreover, even when physiological responses and self-reports prove to be equally strong predictors of behavior, each type of measure can provide independent information. Note that the gap between physiological responses and self-reports may be different for women than for men. For instance, a tendency to under-report arousal (or over-report disgust) after viewing violent images may be more prevalent for women, who are likely to have been socialized to be less aggressive than men. We draw here in particular on Smith et al. (2011), whose examination of disgust sensitivity and left-right political orientations finds a gender difference in self-reports but not in physiological measures (see similar findings in Rohrmann et al. 2008; Schienle et al. 2005.) The authors note that, “One possible explanation of these results is that females claim to be more disgust sensitive because they feel societal pressure to project sensitivity just as males report being less disgust sensitive because they feel societal pressure to project toughness” (p. 5). The end result is that malefemale comparisons using self-reports may well reveal interesting consequences of socialization and gender norms, but might also misstate actual gender differences in reactions to news content. There are other reasons that physiological measurements and self-reports may produce different results. While self-reports can only be elicited after exposure to a stimulus, for instance, physiological responses are measured continuously during actual exposure to the stimulus. This yields a large number of real-time data points for analysis without interfering with message processing (Ravaja 2004). But it also means that physiological measures are capturing reactions at a different time than self-reports. In short, there are good reasons to expect differences between our findings and previous work based on self-reports. To the extent that arguments for media effects invoke biological predispositions, or “hard wiring”, however, it seems appropriate to test them by measuring their physiological implications. Variations in skin conductance, or electro-dermal activity, can be used as a measure of arousal (Dawson, Schell and Filion 2007). Ravaja (2004) characterizes electro-dermal activity as “an excellent operational definition of arousal” (p. 212) and its usefulness has been demonstrated in a number of studies of media effects (Simons et al. 1999; Lang et al. 1999, 2000; Bolls et al. 2001). Heart rate is used as a measure of attentiveness (Lang 1990, 1995; Mulder and Mulder 1981; see also review in Ravaja 2004). !!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!! 5 A common way of measuring arousal is the Self Assessment Mannikin pictorial scale (see, for example, Grabe and Kamhawi 2006). The scale shows illustrations of a manikin displaying various levels of excitement ranging from bored-looking to a heart racing with excitement. ! 4! Experimental Design Participants were recruited at McGill University. There were 38 females and 24 males. Participants were told that they were taking part in an experiment about the news and that their physiological responses would be monitored as they viewed a news program. They watched the news program on their own, on a large computer monitor in a quiet room, wearing noise-canceling headphones. Biosensors were placed on one hand, on their face, and around their torso. These sensors captured their heart rate, skin conductance, respiration amplitude, and activation of the corrugator and zygomaticus facial muscles. A ProComp Infiniti encoder from Thought Technology Ltd. was used to capture physiological responses. The experiments were run using custom-designed software developed for Soroka and McAdams (forthcoming). Table 1. Experimental Stimuli Title Description Tone Domestic Stories Coder Negativity Participant Negativity (-2 to +2) (1 to 7) Canada Magnotta Serial killer is caught and returned to Canada negative 0.96 6.02 Lottery Group of workers win lottery positive -1.06 1.88 Peru Small town of Chimbote burns down negative 1.07 5.05 May Day May Day protests following economic downturn negative 0.58 4.42 Niger Food Shortages in Niger negative 1.26 5.61 UN Sri Lanka UN investigations in war crimes in Sri Lanka negative 1.31 5.68 Gorillas Gorillas released into wild positive -0.98 1.81 Folding Car New electric, folding car intended to reduce congestion positive -0.49 1.48 Young Director 11-yr old makes stop-motion films positive -1.23 1.25 positive -0.68 1.59 International Stories Cured Liver Disease Young child recovers from liver disease The experiment lasted roughly 25 minutes, during which participants viewed seven news stories on a variety of topics, including both political and general news. The stories were a carefully selected (non-random) sample of real news stories from BBC World News. The use of BBC News is based on the long-term objective of fielding the same experiment in multiple countries.6 Two of the seven stories were domestic. One domestic news story was !!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!! 6 Replicating the experiment in multiple countries requires the use of a news source that is familiar around the world. This long-term objective also informs our decision to include domestic stories, alongside international ones, since viewers in different countries may exhibit more or less interest in local versus international news. ! 5! negative and the other was positive. The remaining five stories were all international stories drawn from a sample of four positive and four negative stories. The stories ranged in tone from very positive to very negative. Tone was measured in two ways: a second-by-second coding conducted by three expert coders using a scale ranging from -2 (very positive) to +2 (very negative) and participants’ coding of the stories they viewed on a scale ranging from 1 (very positive) to 7 (very negative). The stories are listed in Table 1, along with short descriptions and the average ratings of tone provided by the expert coders and the study participants. Note that none of the stories were rated as being mildly negative or very negative. This is important given Grabe and Kamhawi’s (2006) argument that moderately negative news stories are the most likely to elicit gendered responses. All participants viewed the two domestic stories plus five stories that were randomly selected from the pool of eight international stories. The stories were presented in random order, and separated by 40 seconds of grey screen. Note that the random selection of international stories means that respondents saw varying numbers of positive and negative stories – from two negative and five positive to five negative and two positive. After viewing the stories, the participants completed a short survey. Measuring Physiological Responses We focus on a combination of skin conductance and heart rate. Variations in skin conductance (SC) are used as an indicator of physiological arousal (Simons et al. 1999; Lang et al. 1999; Bolls et al. 2001; see review in Ravaja 2004). SC reflects the level of moisture exuded by the ecrine sweat glands. When we sweat the skin conducts electricity better. Changes in the level of sweat are related to activity in the sympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system. It is important to note that arousal does not indicate anything about the direction of the response; it simply indicates the degree of activation elicited by a stimulus (Larsen and Diener 1992; Russell 1980). In other words, arousal is not the same as valence. However, it serves our purpose by enabling us to measure whether positive or negative stories elicit stronger reactions and whether these reactions are gendered. Heart rate serves as a measure of attention. Attention is defined as “the allocation of limited mental resources to a specific stimulus” (Ravaja 2004, 197). A decreasing heart rate is often viewed as an indication of increasing attentiveness (Lang 1990; Mulder and Mulder 1981), and that is how we use it below. That said, we note that heart rate likely reflects a combination of attentiveness and arousal, where the former leads to deceleration while the latter leads to acceleration. Our interpretation of heart rate (in line with Lang and others) hinges on the assumption that whatever acceleration comes from arousal will be overwhelmed by the deceleration that comes with attentiveness (see Potter and Bolls 2012 for a useful discussion). We thus expect participants’ heart rate to be lower when watching negative stories, indicating greater attentiveness. Heart rate was measured using a blood volume pulse sensor that detects variations in the volume of blood in the distal phalanx of the middle finger as reflected in the amount of light transmitted through the finger tissue. ! 6! Analyzing the Data For skin conductance (SC) analyses, data are originally sampled 256 times per second, but downsampled for analysis by taking averages over 125-ms intervals. The SC signal is smoothed slightly for analysis, using Lowess smoothing with a bandwidth of .02. Skin conductance measures can tend to decrease over the experiment (a consequence of measurement issues with the electrodes). One option is to detrend the SC measure (see, for example, Soroka and McAdams 2012); here, we simply include the impact of time in our model. Heart rate is also downsampled to 125-ms intervals and Lowess-smoothed. Past work using very similar data has included respondent dummy variables in the analysis. These within-respondent ANOVAs have the advantage of filtering out the differences in skin temperatures across respondents. Of course, the respondent dummies also capture many other respondent characteristics, both demographic and attitudinal. Here, we care about these respondent-level characteristics, in particular, gender. Rather than include respondent dummies in our models, then, we use normalized measures of skin conductance and heart rate. Normalized measures take, for each participant, (1) the raw measure, (2) subtract that participant’s mean over the course of the experiment, and (3) divide the results by the standard deviation over the course of the experiment. Results are in standard units, then, and account not just for the fact that different respondents will have different levels of skin conductance and different heart rates, but also different variances. Results We take two approaches to analyzing our results. First, we use relatively simple analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) of SC, averaged over entire stories. Each respondent-story combination is a case, and we are interested in the possibility that negative stories lead to different levels of activation and attentiveness on the part of women and men than positive stories. Second, we analyse our data in 5-second intervals. There is, as we shall see, real variation in the tone of stories. We can (and do) capitalize on this variation by analyzing the data in smaller intervals. Story-Level Analysis Our story-level analysis is relatively simple. We first model average SC as a function of the following variables: (1) an ordinal variable representing the order of presentation of the stories, to capture the possibility that respondents’ reactions change based on the number of stories they have seen thus far (1 to 7); (2) a local dummy (local=1), to capture the possibility that domestic stories elicit stronger reactions than international stories; (3) an interval-level measure of negativity, based on the average of by-second tone of stories (see the penultimate column of Table 1); (4) a dummy variable (female=1) for participant sex; (5) and a series of interactions allowing for variations in the impact of negativity across variables 1 through 4. ! 7! The results are shown in Table 2 and depicted graphically in Figure 1. The left panel in the table presents the ANCOVA results, while the right panel shows the corresponding OLS regression coefficients. Table 2A. Story-level analyses: Skin Conductance Model Negative tone Order Order*Negative Female Female*Negative Local Local*Negative ANCOVA Partial SS 41.35 6.16 30.85 5.40 .02 .02 2.03 3.73 df 7 F 11.51*** 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 12.01*** 60.09*** 10.51** .05 .03 3.96* 7.27** Residual 211.52 412 Total 252.88 419 N=420 * p < .05, ** p< .01, *** p < .001 OLS Regression Raw Coefficient Negative tone Order Order*Negative Female Female*Negative Local Local*Negative .793 (.188) .196 (.027) -.116 (.035) -.002 (.103) -.026 (.143) .363 (.110) -.417 (.154) Constant R-squared -1.008 (.145) .163 As found in past research on responses to negative visual stimuli (see, for example, Soroka and McAdams forthcoming; Soroka, Nir and Fournier 2013; Soroka 2014), negative stories lead to higher average skin conductance levels (see Figure 1). This is the case for women and men alike. The results are clear: women and men are equally aroused by negative stories. We find no difference in activation between the sexes. Table 2B. Story-level analyses: Heart rate Model Negative tone Order Order*Negative Female Female*Negative Local Local*Negative ANCOVA Partial SS 8.13 .21 7.44 .29 .03 .08 .03 .18 df 7 F 12.19*** 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2.23 78.12*** 3.08 .29 .86 .35 1.89 Residual 39.16 411 Total 47.29 418 N=420 * p < .05, ** p< .01, *** p < .001 ! 8! OLS Regression Raw Coefficient Negative tone Order Order*Negative Female Female*Negative Local Local*Negative -.032 (.081) -.081 (.012) .027 (.015) .012 (.045) -.057 (.062) .026 (.047) -.092 (.067) Constant R-squared .258 (.063) .172 The story for heart rate is a little different. As Table 2B shows, the interaction between gender and negativity hints at a possible gender difference, though the coefficient is not statistically significant. Figure 1 illustrates the gender difference that the estimates suggest: women are somewhat more attentive to negative news stories than men. Indeed, men’s heart rates appear to speed up, indicating that they become less attentive, whereas women experience a small drop in heart rate, indicating greater attentiveness as they watch negative stories. Figure 1. Story-level analyses: The impact of tone on skin conductance and heart rate That said, none of the interactions with gender in Tables 2A and 2B is statistically significant. There is thus little evidence in these story-level data of gender differences in reactions to positive and negative news. However, story-level data offer only a very broad (noisy) view of physiological reactivity. And there are hints of a gender difference in attentiveness. We therefore pursue this possibility further in more finely grained, higherfrequency data below. 5-second Analysis Story-level analyses have the advantage of being relatively simple to estimate, but they mask the fact there is a good deal of variation in tone within stories. Figure 2 shows 5-second means of by-second tone, coded by and averaged across three expert coders. Vertical dashed ! 9! lines show 95-percent confidence intervals, based on the three sets of codes. There are larger confidence intervals as stories change tone — largely a consequence of coders selecting slightly different seconds in which to change their codes. Even so, there are clear trends within the articles. Positive stories tend to be positive most of the time; negative stories similarly tend to be negative most of the time. But there are times when the tone is clearly neutral in each. Figure 2. Tone by Story We can capitalize on this variation by analysing our data in 5-second intervals. (5-second intervals are relatively common in the existing literature, since they tend to capture physiological changes that may not be immediate.) Table 3 shows a re-estimation of our models using these 5-second data. The structure of 5-second data requires a slightly different specification. We move to a more dynamic measure of negativity —- here, changes in the average by-second negativity in the current and preceding 5-second periods. The negativity variable thus captures changes in a ten-second rolling average. We also add a variable capturing within-story time (in 5-second intervals). ! 10! The 5-second data produce rather more robust results. The most pertinent effects are illustrated in Figure 3. The full ANCOVA and OLS results are presented in Table 3. The heart rate results confirm what was hinted at in the story-level analyses. Table 3A. 5-second interval-level analyses: Skin conductance Model Negative tone Order Order*Negative Time (5 Sec) Time*Negative Female Female*Negative Local Local*Negative ANCOVA Partial SS 1318.15 46.74 750.27 84.43 232.63 14.29 3.50 .34 20.39 70.90 df 9 F 197.62*** 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 63.06*** 1013.16*** 113.92*** 313.89*** 19.28*** 4.73* .45 27.51*** 95.67*** Residual 8721.42 11768 Total 10039.57 11777 N=420 * p < .05, ** p< .01, *** p < .001 OLS Regression Raw Coefficient Negative tone Order Order*Negative Time (5 Sec) Time*Negative Female Female*Negative Local Local*Negative .254 (.023) .131 (.004) -.039 (.004) -.014 (.001) .004 (.001) -.035 (.016) -.010 (.014) .104 (.020) -.166 (.017) Constant R-squared -.343 (.026) .131 Table 3B. 5-second interval-level analyses: Heart rate ANCOVA Partial SS df F Model 230.49 9 59.01 Negative tone Order Order*Negative Time (5 Sec) Time*Negative Female Female*Negative Local Local*Negative 3.19 215.74 9.92 6.64 1.44 .87 2.08 3.55 .98 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 7.35** 497.08*** 22.86*** 15.30*** 3.32 .35 4.80* 8.17** 2.26 Residual 5058.50 11655 Total 5288.99 11664 N=420 * p < .05, ** p< .01, *** p < .001 ! 11! OLS Regression Raw Coefficient Negative tone Order Order*Negative Time (5 Sec) Time*Negative Female Female*Negative Local Local*Negative -.022 (.018) -.071 (.003) .013 (.003) -.002 (.001) -.001 (.001) -.078 (.013) -.025 (.011) -.044 (.015) -.020 (.013) Constant R-squared .292 (.020) .043 Negativity is positively related to increased skin conductance and, once again, this is the case regardless of sex (see Table 3A). Moreover, the relationship holds equally strongly for women and men. In other words, women are just as aroused as men by watching negative news stories. On the other hand, women clearly pay more attention to negative news stories than men do. Their heart rates decreased significantly as they viewed negative news stories. There is no evidence of a similar drop for men. On the contrary, if anything, men’s heart rates speed up as they watch the negative news stories, indicating a lower level of attentiveness. In short, the key gender difference in reactions to negative news relates to women’s apparently heightened attentiveness to negative content. There is no evidence of any gender difference in arousal. Figure 3. 5-second analyses: The impact of tone on skin conductance and heart rate Conclusions It is not clear why gender differences appear for attentiveness but not for arousal. The finding that women react more strongly to negative information does fit with a (small) literature on gender and negativity biases, however. Work in psychology suggests that women have a stronger avoidance response to negative stimuli than do men (see, for example, Canli et al. 2002). Research on impression formation also finds evidence of a ! 12! stronger negativity bias for women (Ito and Cacioppo 2005; though see Huma 2010 for a related discussion). This is mirrored in work in political behavior as well – drawing on models of impression formation, Soroka (2014) finds that women exhibit a stronger negativity bias in their assessments of US presidents. Work in economics also finds gender differences in risk aversion: in a meta-analysis of experimental studies, Croson and Gneezy (2009) find evidence that women are more risk averse than men are. However, research on impression formation and loss aversion cannot distinguish between attentiveness and arousal. The particular difference between the two that we find here is neither contrary to, nor supported by, this previous work. But there are already strong hints of a larger negativity bias for women than for men, and this finds some support above. There may be neuro-physiological bases for the gender difference in attentiveness to negative news that we have observed. A study using functional magnetic resonance imaging reports women activated the anterior cingulate, a region known to integrate attention and emotion, when presented with affectively negative visual stimuli (Wrase et al. 2003). There was no evidence of a comparable activation for men. Another study based on the use of steady-state probe topography (SSPT) finds that the processing of unpleasant images (relative to neutral images) is associated with latency reductions within the right frontal and temporal regions in women but not in men (Kemp et al. 2004). Latency refers to the time taken for an electrical impulse to travel from a visual stimulus to the site where the recording is taking place and is typically taken as an indicator of the speed of processing neural information. Kemp and his colleagues suggest that the frontal latency reductions observed in women may reflect “an inability to successfully suppress activation associated with the presentation of unpleasant stimuli” (p. 641). Note that our finding that women are more attentive to negative news stories than men is at odds with Grabe and Kamhawi’s (2006) predictions, and does not square with their finding that men received higher scores for comprehension and memory recognition of negatively framed stories than women did. This may be a function of differences in the design of our respective experiments. First, Grabe and Kamhawi only examined whether men are more aroused by and attentive to moderately negative news stories than women are. They assume that gendered responses to negative news are most likely to appear when the story is not too negative but nonetheless unambiguously negative: stories that are very negative are unlikely to elicit different responses from women and men because they create a ceiling effect with women and men alike manifesting an avoidance response whereas stories that are only mildly negative are unlikely to induce much of a response in either gender. This implies a non-linear relationship between negativity and arousal. Our preliminary tests find no evidence of a non-linear relationship, however, and so we focus on a simple linear effect. Second, while all of the stories used in Grabe and Kamhawi’s experiments featured bad news, some were framed positively or ambiguously while others were framed negatively. Our study, on the other, hand, includes stories featuring good news as well as bad. Third, and perhaps most importantly, the difference in findings may also reflect differences in the way that we measure attentiveness. As discussed above, we capture attentiveness in real time by measuring how participants’ heart rates vary as they watch positive and negative news stories, whereas Grabe and Kamhawi focus on recognition memory and ! 13! comprehension. Variations in heart rate simply reflect the allocation of information processing resources; they do not tell us anything about the application of those resources to encoding (recognition memory) and retrieving (comprehension) information. One possibility is that negative stories tend to elicit different emotions from women and men (see below) and these emotions influence how story content is encoded and retrieved. At this stage, however, this is purely speculative. Understanding why gender appears to condition these measures in different—indeed, apparently opposite—ways may require experiments that incorporate all three types of measurement. Our finding that women are more attentive than men to negative news stories also raises interesting questions about how attentiveness translates into actual behavior. As we saw above, there is abundant evidence that women consume less news than men do, and it is plausible that the dominance of negative news helps to explain this gender difference. Our experiment puts women in a situation where they get to watch negative news stories, whether they enjoy them or not. In this situation, they appear to be more attentive to the stories than men are. Outside the lab7, where they have the choice to watch or not, they may simply choose not to watch the news. We can only speculate about the reasons why women’s immediate psycho-physiological reactions and their long-term behavioral responses to negative news are different. One possibility is that, because they are greatly affected by (but dislike) negative information, and because they know that political news is predominantly negative, women avoid political news. Hyper-reaction to negativity thus produces a withdrawal. Alternatively, women’s behavior may reflect their socialization. In avoiding negative news when they have the option, women may be acting in accord with deeply socialized gender role expectations. As noted above, using physiological responses in tandem with self-reports can provide new insights into the interplay between biological predispositions and learned ones. At the same time, though, we need to recognize an important limitation of relying on measurements of psycho-physiological responses. They do not permit us to identify the emotion that has triggered an increase in skin conductance or a slowing down of the heart rate (see Dawson, Schell and Filion 2007). Physiological responses to negative news could reflect fear, anger, anxiety or disgust. Differentiating these emotional responses requires experiments that systematically manipulate the news coverage to elicit specific emotions. Women and men appear to be equally aroused by watching negative news stories, whether arousal is measured by self-reports as in Grabe and Kamhawi’s (2006) experiment or by changes in skin conductance levels as in our experiment. It is quite possible, though, that viewing negative news content is exciting for men but induces anxiety in women. In other words, the emotions are different even though the level of arousal is similar. This is another possible explanation for why their behavioral responses differ. This study represents the first that we are aware of that uses physiological measures to examine gendered responses to television news content. There is clearly a great deal more work that needs to be done if we are to come up with a compelling explanation of why !!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!! 7 Our participants were free, of course, to withdraw from the experiment at any time without prejudice. ! 14! women typically consume less news than men, however. Uncovering answers is important given the enduring gender gap in political knowledge. Women typically know less about politics than men do, even taking account of gender differences in the propensity to guess and other measurement artifacts (see, for example, Delli Carpini and Keeter 2000; Dolan 2011; Dow 2009; Frazer and Macdonald 2003; Gidengil et al. 2004; Mondak and Anderson 2004). Being uninformed or, worse, misinformed about politics impairs women’s ability to voice their needs and wants effectively. One reason women know less about politics than men do is that they consume less news. Finding ways of countering the gender gap in news consumption could therefore help to close the knowledge gap but that can only happen if we understand why women consume less news.! ! 15! References Balzer, Amanda, and Carly M. Jacobs. 2011. Gender and Physiological Effects in Connecting Disgust to Political Preferences.” Social Science Quarterly 92(5): 1297-1313 Benesch, Christine. 2012. “An Empirical Analysis of the Gender Gap in News Consumption.” Journal of Media Economics 25(3): 147-67. Bolls, Paul D., Annie Lang and Robert F. Potter. 1999. “The Effects of Message Valence and Listener Arousal on Attention, Memory, and Facial Muscular Responses to Radio Advertisements.” Communication Research 28(5): 627-51. Canli, T., Desmond, J. E., Zhao, Z., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. 2002. “Sex differences in the neural basis of emotional memories.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 99(16): 10789-10795. Croson, Rachel, and Uri Gneezy. 2009. “Gender Differences in Preferences.” Journal of Economic Literature 47(2): 1-27. Dawson, Michael, Anne Schell, and Disan Filion. 2007. “The Electrodermal System.” In Handbook of Psychophysiology, 3rd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press. Delli Carpini, Michael X., and Scott Keeter. 2000. Gender and Political Knowledge. In Gender and American Politics: Women, Men, and the Political Process, ed. Sue TollesonRinehart and Jyl J. Josephson. New York: M. E. Sharpe, pp. 21-45. Daignault, Penelope, Stuart Soroka and Thierry Giasson. 2013. “The Perception of Political Advertising During An Election Campaign: A Preliminary Study of Cognitive and Emotional Effects.” Canadian Journal of Communication 28: 167-186. Dolan, Kathleen. 2011. Do Women and Men Know Different Things? Measuring Gender Differences in Political Knowledge. The Journal of Politics 73(1): 97-107. Dow, Jay K. 2009. Gender Differences in Political Knowledge: Distinguishing Characteristics-Based and Returns-Based Differences. Political Behavior 31: 117-36. Frazer, Elizabeth, and Kenneth Macdonald. 2003. Sex Differences in Political Knowledge in Britain. Political Studies 51: 67-83. Gidengil, Elisabeth, André Blais, Neil Nevitte, and Richard Nadeau. 2004. Citizens. Vancouver: UBC Press. Grabe. Maria Elizabeth. 2011. “News as Reality-inducing, Survival-relevant, and Genderspecific Stimuli.” In S. Craig Roberts, ed., Applied Evolutionary Psychology New York: Oxford University Press. ! 16! Grabe, Maria Elizabeth, and Rasha Kamhawi. 2006. “Hard Wired for Negative News? Gender Differences in Processing Broadcast News.” Communication Research 33: 346-369 Huma, Bogdana. 2010. “Gender Differences in Impression Formation.” Journal of Comparative Research in Anthropology and Sociology 1(1): 57-72. Ito, Tiffany A. and John T. Cacioppo. 2005. “Variations on a human universal: Individual differences in positivity offset and negativity bias.” Cognition and Emotion 19(1): 1-26. Kamhawi, Rasha, and Maria Elizabeth Grabe. 2008. Engaging the Female Audience: An Evolutionary Psychology Perspective on Gendered Responses to News Valence Frames. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 52(1): 33-51 Kemp, A.H., R.B. Silberstein, S.M. Armstrong and P.J. Nathan. 2004. “Gender Differences in the Cortical Electrophysiological Processing of Visual Emotional Stimuli.” NeuroImage 16: 632-46 Kleemans, Mariska, Paul G. J. Hendriks Vettehen, Johannes W. J. Beentjes and Rob Eisinga. 2012. “The Influence of Age and Gender on Preferences for Negative Content and Tabloid Packaging in Television News Stories.” Communication Research 39(5): 679-97. Klein, Roger. 2003. “Audience Reactions to Local TV News.” American Behavioral Scientist 46(21): 1661-72. Knobloch-Westerwick, Silvia, and Scott Alter. 2007. “The Gender News Use Divide: Americans’ Sex-Typed Selective Exposure to Online News Topics.” Journal of Communication 57: 739-58. Lang, Annie. 1990. “Involuntary Attention and Physiological Arousal Evoked by Structural Features and Emotional Content in TV Commercials.” Communication Research 17(3): 27599 Lang, Annie. 1995. “What Can the Heart Tell Us about Thinking?” In Annie Lang, ed., Measuring Psychological Responses to Media, pp, 99-112. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Lang, Annie, Paul Bolls, Robert F. Potter and Karlynn Kawahara. 1999. “The Effects of Production Pacing and Arousing Content on the Information Processing of Television Messages.” Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 43(4): 451-75. Lang, Annie, Shuhua Zhou, Nancy Schwartz, Paul D. Bolls and Robert F. Potter. 2000. “The Effects of Edits on Arousal, Attention, and Memory for Television Messages: When an Edit Is an Edit Can an Edit Be Too Much?” Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 44(1): 94-109 ! 17! Larsen, Randy J., and Edward Diener. 1992. “Promises and Problems with the Circumplex Model of Emotion.” In Emotion: Review of Personality and Social Psychology, no. 13, ed., Margaret S. Clark, pp. 25-59. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Mondak, Jeffrey J., and Mary R. Anderson. 2004. The Knowledge Gap: A Reexamination of Gender-Based Differences in Political Knowledge. Journal of Politics 66: 492-512. Mulder, G., and L.J.M. Mulder. 1981. “Information Processing and Cardiovascular Control.” Psychophysiology 18(4): 392-402. Newhagen, John E. 1998. “TV News Images that Induce Anger, Fear, and Disgust.” Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 42(2): 265-276. The Pew Research Center for the People and the Press. 2002. Public’s News Habits Little Changed by September 11. http://www.people-press.org/files/legacy-pdf/156.pdf. Retrieved March 24, 2014. The Pew Research Center for the People and the Press. 2004. News Audiences Increasingly Politicized. http://www.people-press.org/files/legacy-pdf/215.pdf. Retrieved March 24, 2014. Potter, R.F. and P.D. Bolls. 2012. Psychophysiological Measurement and Meaning: Cognitive and Emotional Processing of Media. New York: Routledge. Ravaja, Niklas. 2004. “Contributions of Psychophysiology to Media Research: Review and Recommendations.” Media Psychology 6(2): 193-235. Reeves, Byron, and Clifford Nass. 1996. The Media Equation: How People Treat Computers, Television, and New Media Like Real People and Places. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Rohrmann S, Hopp H, Quirin M. 2008. “Gender differences in psychophysiological responses to disgust.” Journal of Psychophysiology 22: 56–75. Russell, James A. 1980. “A Circumplex Model of Affect.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39(6): 1161-78. Schienle A, Schafer A, Stark R, Walter B, Vaitl D. 2005. “Gender differences in the processing of disgust and fear-inducing pictures: An fMRI study.” Neuroreport 16: 277–280. Simons, Robert F., Benjamin H. Detenber, Thomas M. Roedema and Jason E. Reiss. 1999. “Emotion Processing in Three Systems: The Medium and the Message.” Psychophysiology 36: 619-27 Smith, Kevin B., Douglas Oxley, Matthew V. Hibbing, John R. Alford and John R. Hibbing. 2011. “Disgust Sensitivity and Neurophysiology of Left-Right Political Orientations.” Plos One 6(10) e25552: 1-9. ! 18! Soroka, Stuart. 2014. Negativity in Democratic Politics: Causes and Consequences. Cambridge Studies in Public Opinion and Political Psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press. Soroka, Stuart, Lilach Nir and Patrick Fournier. 2013. “Negativity Biases in Reactions to Network News: A Cross-National, Psychophysiological Pilot Study.” Paper presented at the Toronto Political Behaviour Workshop, November, Toronto ON. Soroka, Stuart and Stephen McAdams. Forthcoming. “News, Politics and Negativity.” Political Communication. Van Zoonen, Liesbet. 2002. “One of the Girls? The Incipient Feminization of News.” In News, Gender and Power, eds., Cynthia Carter, Gill Branston and Stuart Allan. London: Routledge. Wrase, Jana, Sabine Klein, Sabine M. Gruesser, Derik Hermann, Herta Flor, Karl Mann, Dieter F. Braus and Andreas Heinz. 2003. “Gender Differences in the Processing of Standardized Emotional Visual Stimuli in Humans: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study.” Neuroscience Letters 348(1): 41-5. ! 19!