Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-0663/97/J3.00 Journal of Educational Psychology 1997, Vol. 89, No. 4,682-685 Mechanisms That Facilitate the Effectiveness of Elaboration Strategies Teena Willoughby Eileen Wood Brock University Wilfrid Laurier University Serge Desmarais Suzanne Sims University of Guelph St. Mary's University Michelle Kalra University of Guelph The authors examined the role of distinctiveness in the differential memory performance of visual and verbal elaboration strategies. Twenty-eight undergraduates learned information about familiar and unfamiliar animals while using either the visual (imagery) or verbal (answering why questions) elaboration strategies. There was no difference between the 2 strategies in level of organization for the familiar animal material, but imagery students organized unfamiliar animal information into intact sets more than verbal elaboration students. Imagery easily permits the generation of relations and distinctions for unfamiliar material through the creation of unique mental images. In contrast, learners using verbal elaboration may be more likely to access general rather than specific information when answering why questions for unfamiliar information. In an effort to enhance independent learning in the classroom, many researchers and educators have advocated instructing students to use a variety of powerful elaboration strategies. New information can be made more memorable if learners create meaningful elaborations (e.g., Stein, Morris, & Bransford, 1978). The source of elaborations can be both visual (e.g., imagery) or verbal (e.g., answering questions). Apart from determining that a strategy works, researchers also have been increasingly motivated to explain how strategies work to better predict for whom and for which situations strategies will be most effective. The purpose of this paper was to investigate the mechanisms that facilitate the effectiveness of elaboration strategies by examining the role of distinctiveness in the differential memory performance of visual and verbal elaboration strategies. When using visual elaboration strategies (e.g., imagery), adult students are often asked to form a mental picture of a word or situation (Paivio, 1971, 1991). Marschark, Richman, Yuille, and Hunt (1987) suggest that the typical Teena Willoughby, Department of Child Studies, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada; Eileen Wood, Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada; Serge Desmarais and Michelle Kalra, Department of Psychology, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada; Suzanne Sims, Department of Psychology, St. Mary's University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada. This article was supported by grants from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. This study was presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, California, April 1995. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Teena Willoughby, Department of Child Studies, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada L2S 3A1. Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to twiUoug@spartan.ac.brocku.ca. 682 potency of the imagery strategy can be explained through two processes, the building of relations and distinctions. When learning prose material, relations can be understood as the creation of interactive images or links between pieces of information and distinctions as the creation of a unique representation for one group of items relative to other groups. For example, if students were instructed to create mental images of a series of animal facts, they would make interactive images or relations among the facts for each animal (i.e., students would imagine the animal exhibiting the stated trait or behavior). Then students would make a unique and distinctive image for each animal apart from the other animals. Therefore, relations would be constructed within one animal, whereas distinctions would be made between different animals. At test, when students are asked to identify the facts that correspond to each animal, they can draw on their construction of relations to allow them to access the appropriate image while having a distinctive image for each animal would help them retrieve the correct animal name (see Einstein & Hunt, 1980; Hunt & Einstein, 1981). Both adult and child populations can be instructed to create mental images to facilitate acquisition of prose materials (Pressley, Symons, McDaniel, Snyder, & Tumure, 1988; Woloshyn, Willoughby, Wood, & Pressley, 1990; Wood, Pressley, & Winne, 1990). Imagery strategies may be particularly advantageous because they allow learners to generate concrete images even from unfamiliar material. For example, when asked to learn facts about an unfamiliar animal, learners can create a mnemonic for the animal name, which would allow them to create a concrete image (e.g., coat for coat; see Willoughby, Wood, & Khan, 1994). Verbal elaboration strategies, in contrast, have also facilitated 683 ELABORATION STRATEGIES memory performance but the mechanisms that foster learning may differ. Elaborative interrogation is a verbal questioning strategy that enhances learning by encouraging students to connect novel information to their own richly developed knowledge base (Martin & Pressley, 1991). The strategy is potent for both children and adults across a variety of learning tasks (see Pressley et al., 1992, for a review). Specifically, students using elaborative interrogation answer a why question for each fact (e.g., "Why would that fact be true?"). When students are asked to generate answers to why questions, they are encouraged to draw on their existing knowledge (cf. Sadoski, Paivio, & Goetz, 1991). By relating what they already know to the new information, links are created with existing information and memory for the material is therefore facilitated (Anderson, 1990; Bower, Black, & Turner, 1979; Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Thorndyke & Yekovich, 1980). Willoughby, Waller, Wood, and MacKinnon (1993) demonstrated that the amount of prior knowledge affects learning gains with elaborative interrogation (see also Kuhara-Kojima & Hatano, 1991, and Woloshyn, Pressley, & Schneider, 1992, for a related discussion). When learning animal facts, memory for familiar animals was more easily integrated with existing knowledge and produced greater memory performance relative to rare or unfamiliar animals. One disadvantage for elaborative interrogation relative to imagery may be that when presented with facts about unfamiliar objects, learners using elaborative interrogation may be able to create relations but fail to process distinctions between the objects. For example, general information rather than specific information may be accessed when answering why questions about unfamiliar animals. This could prevent the processing of distinctive information between the animals. In addition, students may access information about an inappropriate animal when searching their prior knowledge for information that may relate to unfamiliar animals. For example, students may make the association of fish to the unfamiliar animal American Pika because it sounds like pike, but they have to revise their conception when they encounter facts that are not consistent with fish (for example, the American Pika lives around rock piles). This would result in a lack of distinctiveness for that animal at retrieval. For many studies comparing elaborative interrogation to imagery, there are no significant differences between the two strategies in facilitation of memory performance (Pressley et al., 1988; Woloshyn et al., 1990). However, differences are apparent when object familiarity has been manipulated. The two strategies are equally potent when novel facts about familiar concepts are encountered, but there is an advantage for imagery when facts about unfamiliar concepts are used (Willoughby, Wood, & Khan, 1994). Willoughby et al. explained this advantage by suggesting that when students use imagery, they are not required to make logical connections to their knowledge base. Creating an image of an animal (any image of it, whether consistent with reality or not) provides the learner with a unique or distinctive peg for each animal (Paivio, 1971). Memory performance in existing studies comparing imagery and elaborative interrogation was measured through matching tasks where students were asked to match animals with facts associated with them. The memory advantage for imagery on such tasks presumably reflects access to highly developed connections between the animals and their related facts. In this study, we examined students' organization of the material at retrieval rather than through the typical matching test. There would be support for the hypothesis if imagery students organized the facts into accurate, intact stories (i.e., no errors in grouping appropriate animal facts) more often than elaborative interrogation students. Because any peg will do in the case of the imagery strategy, it was expected that imagery would be a better strategy than elaborative interrogation for unfamiliar animals, although both strategies should be effective with familiar animals. Method Participants and Design Twenty-eight students (22 men and 6 women) enrolled in a first-year psychology course participated in this experiment. Students ranged in age from 19 to 33 years (M — 22 years, SD - 3 years, 8 months). Students were drawn from one university in a midsize Canadian city. Students were randomly assigned to either an elaborative interrogation or imagery strategy condition. Proportionately equal numbers of men and women were assigned to each condition. Materials The stimulus materials consisted of 10 sets of animal facts. Each set was composed of six statements describing the type and special attributes of one of 10 animals (5 familiar and 5 unfamiliar animals). Each of the six statements described one characteristic of an animal (e.g., physical living environment, diet, source of predation). Five of the animals were judged to be familiar to students (i.e.. Swift Fox, House Mouse, Little Brown Bat, Townsend Mole, and Western Spotted Skunk) and five were judged to be unfamiliar (i.e., pronghorn, coati, chickaree, American Pika, Collared Peccary). To ensure that the familiar and unfamiliar animals selected for this study would indeed correspond to undergraduate students' knowledge, Willoughby et al. (1993) pretested a comparable student population for familiarity with the animals and facts (see the Appendix for an example of a familiar and unfamiliar animal set). The 10 animals were chosen because the students indicated unanimously that they were either familiar with the animals (selected for familiar animals) or unfamiliar with the animals (selected for unfamiliar animals). Each sentence was typed on one white 12-cm X 19-cm card. Three cards served as examples that were used when instructions were provided to the students. The 60 remaining sentences were the study materials. Procedure The experimental phase included instructions, practice, presentation of the study materials, and a memory task. Instructions and practice. Participants were told that they would be shown individual sentences stating true facts about animals and that they would be asked to remember that information. Students in the elaborative interrogation condition were instructed to read each sentence and answer out loud the why 684 WTLLOUGHBY, WOOD, DESMARAIS, SIMS, AND KALRA question written below each statement. They were told that their answer should clearly state why the fact was true of the specific animal being discussed and not another similar animal. Students in the imagery condition were instructed to read each sentence and make an interactive picture in their mind linking the animal to each fact, so that the image clearly depicted the animal being discussed and not another similar animal. Students were not required to make a collective image for the six facts about each animal. Three example sentences were used to ensure that students could effectively execute their assigned strategy. Feedback was provided about their performance. For example, students in the elaborative interrogation condition were prompted to elaborate their answer further or generate another elaboration if their answer did not explain the relation between the animal and its behavior. Students in the imagery condition were required to describe their image out loud during practice. This manipulation check was done to ensure that all students were able to generate an image even for the more difficult items. For the practice items, additional prompting was given if students did not describe a full image. All students were capable of creating images for the practice items. Study phase. Students were reminded of the instructions and the 60 facts were presented with no feedback provided. Each sentence was presented for 15 s, with a 15-s interval separating the presentation of each of the 10 animal sets. Sentences were presented in a constant order across students. A paced presentation was used to ensure that all students had access to the material for the same amount of time across all strategy conditions. For the elaborative interrogation students, the experimental session was audiotaped to ensure that students complied with instructions. To avoid interference with the creation of visual images, we did not require students in the imagery condition to verbalize their images during the study phase and no recording was made of the experimental session for the imagery students. However, after the session was completed (i.e., after the memory task), we asked students in the imagery condition to describe their images that they had created for 4 randomly picked animal facts (this manipulation check was also audiotaped). Memory test. After all 60 sentences were presented, the participants first were asked to organize the animals facts into intact stories (animal names were absent). Each fact was typed on a separate sheet of paper and students were instructed to sort the 60 randomly presented facts into organized units. Students knew, however, that the stories were to be organized according to animal type. Later, these students were asked to indicate either the name of the animal they thought corresponded to each set of facts or any descriptors that they might have associated with the facts. Results The transcription of the audiotapes confirmed that all students complied with instructions and used their assigned strategy. That is, during study, all elaborative interrogation students answered why each fact was true, whereas after the memory test, each imagery participant was able to verbalize the images they had created for the four designated facts (see procedure). The primary analyses examined students' organization of the facts into intact animal stories. The percentage of animal units that were correctly organized into intact stories was calculated for both familiar and unfamiliar animals. Because of some distributional characteristics of the data, we analyzed the percentage scores for both the familiar and unfamiliar animals using the nonparametric Kruskal-Wallis test (Siegel & Castellan, 1988). Strategy Table 1 Mean Percentage of Familiar and Unfamiliar Animals Organized Into Intact Animal Sets as a Function of Strategy Condition in Experiment 1 Unfamiliar animals Familiar animals M Strategy condition M SD SD Elaborative interro27.0 14.7 gation 32.0 8.0 Imagery 35.9 23.3 24.6 33.8 condition was the between-subjects variable. (See Table 1 for means.) Consistent with our expectations, there were no differences in organization for facts about familiar animals, X2(l, N = 28) = .01, p > .05, but imagery students more often organized unfamiliar animal facts into accurate, intact stories compared with students in the elaborative interrogation condition, x 2 d, N = 28) = 4.84,p < .05. After students organized the facts at retrieval, they were instructed to indicate the name of the animal they thought corresponded to each set of facts that they constructed. This task replicated previous studies where students matched the facts with the appropriate animal name. Memory performance was determined by adding the number of correct matches (animal name with fact). Using the Kruskal-Wallis test again (Siegel & Castellan, 1988), imagery and elaborative interrogation conditions did not differ in performance for familiar animals, x2(l> N = 28) = .86, p > .05, but imagery outperformed elaborative interrogation with unfamiliar animals, x 2 (l, N = 28) = 6.48, p < .05. (See Table 2 for relevant means.) Discussion Imagery most likely outperforms elaborative interrogation for unfamiliar facts because it encourages the construction of the basic association between the fact and its referent (relations) as well as more attention to distinctions as a result of creating unique images for each animal. Because elaborative interrogation, in contrast, forces students to go beyond the given information, they have difficulty using the strategy successfully when only general background knowledge is available. Both relations and distinctions are important mechanisms with respect to the efficient use of verbal and visual elaboration strategies. Whereas imagery easily permits the Table 2 Mean Matching Scores for Familiar and Unfamiliar Animals as a Function of Strategy Condition in Experiment I Familiar animals Unfamiliar animals Strategy condition M SD M SD Elaborative interrrogation Imagery 19.33 16.62 7.78 7.92 3.80 10.08 5.09 6.14 Note, Maximum score is 30 for familiar and unfamiliar animals. 685 ELABORATION STRATEGIES generation of relations and distinctions through the creation of unique mental images, verbal elaboration is more tied to prior knowledge. Elaborative interrogation requires access to existing knowledge and making associations between the learners' prior knowledge and the new material. When learners have access to a rich knowledge base, the strategy promotes the creation of unique or specific associations and memory is facilitated. With a more limited knowledge base, however, students using the elaborative interrogation strategy may be more likely to access general rather than specific information leading to a lack of distinctiveness for the material. Imagery, therefore, may be the strategy of choice when learning information accessing an unfamiliar domain. By enhancing our understanding of when and how strategies work, we can better assure that strategy instruction is designed to maximize learning. References Anderson, J. R. (1990). Cognitive psychology and its implications (3rd ed.). New York: Freeman. Bower, G. H., Black, J. B., & Turner, T. J. (1979). Scripts in memory for text. Cognitive Psychology, 11,177-220. Einstein, G. O., & Hunt, R. R. (1980). Levels of processing and organization: Additive effects of individual item and relational processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning andMemory, 6,588-598. Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill. Hunt, R. R., & Einstein, G. 0 . (1981). Relational and item-specific information in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20,497-514. Kuhara-Kojima, K., & Hatano, G. (1991). Contribution of content knowledge and learning ability to the learning of facts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 253-263. Marschark, M , Richman, C. L-, Yuille, J. C , & Hunt, R. R. (1987). The role of imagery in memory: On shared and distinctive information. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 28-41. Martin, V. L., & Pressley, M. (1991). Elaborative interrogation effects depend on the nature of the question. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 113-119. Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45, 255-287. Pressley, M., Symons, S., McDaniel, M. A., Snyder, B. L., & Tumure, J. E. (1988). Elaborative interrogation facilitates acquisition of confusing facts. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 80,268-278. Pressley, M., Wood, EM Woloshyn, V. E., Martin, V., King, A., & Menke, D. (1992). Encouraging mindful use of prior knowledge: Attempting to construct explanatory answers facilitates learning. Educational Psychologist, 27,91-109. Sadoski, M., Paivio, A., & Goetz, E. T. (1991). Acritique of schema theory in reading and a dual coding alternative. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 463-484. Siegel, S., & Castellan, N. (1988). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill. Stein, B. S., Morris, C. D., & Bransford, J. D. (1978). Constraints on effective elaboration. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17,701-714. Thomdyke, P. W., & Yekovich, F. R. (1980). Acritique of schema-based theories of human story memory. Poetics, 9,23-49. Willoughby, T., Waller, T. G., Wood, E., & MacKinnon, G. E. (1993). The effect of prior knowledge on an immediate and delayed associative learning task following elaborative interrogation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 18, 36-46. Willoughby, T., Wood, E., & Khan, M. (1994). Isolating variables that impact or detract from the effectiveness of elaboration strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 279-289. Woloshyn, V., Pressley, M., & Schneider, W. (1992). Elaborative interrogation and prior knowledge effects on learning of facts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84,115-124. Woloshyn, V. E., Willoughby, T., Wood, E., & Pressley, M. (1990). Elaborative interrogation facilitates adult learning of factual paragraphs. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 513-524. Wood, E., Pressley, M., & Winne, P. (1990). Elaborative interrogation effects on children's learning of factual content Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 741-748. Appendix Example of a Familiar and Unfamiliar Animal Set Western Spotted Skunk (Familiar Animal) Collared Peccary (Unfamiliar Animal) 1. The Western Spotted Skunk's hole is usually found on a sandy piece of farmland near crops. 2. The Western Spotted Skunk mostly eats corn. 3. The Western Spotted Skunk's biggest danger is the Great Horned Owl. 4. The Western Spotted Skunk lives in a hole in the ground. 5. The Western Spotted Skunk often lives alone but sometimes stays together in families. 6. The Western Spotted Skunk is able to live close to people but never be seen. 1. The Collared Peccary lives in southwestern United States. 2. The Collared Peccary eats roots and cacti. 3. The Collared Peccary's biggest dangers are the jaguar and mountain lion. 4. The Collared Peccary often rests in bushes or under large boulders. 5. The Collared Peccary has no obvious leaders among its males and females. 6. The Collared Peccary's stomach has two sections. Received April 2,1996 Revision received February 27,1997 Accepted February 28,1997 •