Relationships Between Exterior Views and Nurse Stress

advertisement
P
PA
AP
PE
ER
RS
S
HERD Volume 1, Number 2, pp 27-38 Copyright ©2008 Vendome Group, LLC
Relationships Between Exterior Views and Nurse
Stress: An Exploratory Examination
Debajyoti Pati, PhD, MASA, AIIA; Tom E. Harvey Jr., AIA, FACHA, MPH; and Paul Barach, MD, MPH
Abstract
Objective: Examine the relationships between acute stress and alertness of
nurse, and duration and content of exterior views from nurse work areas.
Background: Nursing is a stressful job, and the impacts of stress on performance are well documented. Nursing stress, however, has been typically addressed through operational interventions, although the ability of the physical
environment to modulate stress in humans is well known. This study explores
the outcomes of exposure to exterior views from nurse work areas.
Methods: A survey-based method was used to collect data on acute stress,
chronic stress, and alertness of nurses before and after 12-hour shifts. Control
measures included physical environment stressors (that is, lighting, noise,
thermal, and ergonomic), organizational stressors, workload, and personal
characteristics (that is, age, experience, and income). Data were collected
from 32 nurses on 19 different units at two hospitals (part of Children’s
Healthcare of Atlanta) in November 2006.
Results: Among the variables considered in the study view duration is the
second most influential factor affecting alertness and acute stress. The association between view duration and alertness and stress is conditional on the
exterior view content (that is, nature view, non-nature view). Of all the nurses
whose alertness level remained the same or improved, almost 60% had
exposure to exterior and nature view. In contrast, of all nurses whose alertness levels deteriorated, 67% were exposed to no view or to only non-nature
view. Similarly, of all nurses whose acute stress condition remained the same
or reduced, 64% had exposure to views (71% of that 64% were exposed to
Author Affiliations: Dr. Pati is Director of Research at HKS, Inc., in Dallas, TX. Mr. Harvey is
Senior Vice President at HKS. Dr. Barach is Chair of the Research Council at the Center for
Health Design and a visiting professor of anesthesia and emergency medicine in the Center
for Patient Safety at Utrecht University Medical Center in the Netherlands.
Corresponding Author: Debajyoti Pati, HKS, Inc., 1919 McKinney Avenue, Dallas, TX 75201
(dpati@hksinc.com).
Acknowledgments: This study would not have been feasible without the valuable
contributions of D. J. Feather, Pam Black, Taylor Sommers, and Elena Morales at Children’s
Healthcare of Atlanta. We acknowledge the assistance provided to us by Dr. Craig Zimring at
the Georgia Institute of Technology, College of Architecture. Finally, our appreciation goes to
Dr. Gary Evans at Cornell University, College of Human Ecology, for his advice on theoretical
issues and recommendations on paper-based measures for perceived stress and arousal.
a nature view). Of nurses whose acute stress levels increased, 56% had no
view or only a non-nature view.
Conclusions: Although long working hours, overtime, and sleep deprivation
are problems in healthcare operations, the physical design of units is only
now beginning to be considered seriously in evaluating patient outcomes. Access to a nature view and natural light for care-giving staff could bear direct
as well as indirect effects on patient outcomes.
Key Words: Nursing, acute stress, alertness, natural light, nature view,
patient safety, evidence-based design, healthcare architecture
Background
It could be argued that patient well-being is a function of caregiver well-being. While considerable attention in healthcare architecture and research has focused on improving patient conditions,
architectural research focus on the direct caregivers has been limited. This is despite numerous studies demonstrating that nurses
are frequently stressed and fatigued, with possible detrimental
implications on patient care. The high-stress work environment
in which nurses provide care is not new, with scientific publications as far back as the 1980s addressing the issue (Hinshaw &
Atwood, 1984). More recent studies focused on nursing empowerment, nurse burnout, and nurse satisfaction (Greco, Laschinger, &
Wong, 2006, including several of Laschinger’s previous works) underscore the prevalence and acuteness of the problem. The results
of a 2001 nationwide survey of 4,826 nurses revealed that over
70% of the respondents reported stress as one of their top three
concerns (Houle, 2001), a finding that has been replicated several
times (Tabone, 2004a).
Though a severe problem in itself, the high stress level of direct
caregivers raises serious concerns pertaining to patient well-being.
Although it has not been empirically investigated in healthcare settings in a rigorous manner, studies from other settings suggest that
serious implications for performance could result from high levels
of stress and fatigue. Stress and fatigue could impact such criti-
HERD Vol. 1, No. 2 WINTER 2008 • HEALTH ENVIRONMENTS RESEARCH & DESIGN JOURNAL
27
PA P E R S
HERD Volume 1, Number 2, pp 27-38 Copyright ©2008 Vendome Group, LLC cal aspects of performance as reaction time or alertness, irritability, attention to detail, problem solving ability, energy level, and
decision-making ability, and thus they could contribute to errors
(Barach & Weinger, 2007; Page, 2004; Tabone, 2004b). Moreover,
reduced motivation and work performance are typical of tired
workers in workplaces, which could equally apply to stressed and
tired caregivers in healthcare settings (Tabone, 2004a). While the
Institute of Medicine report (Page, 2004) firmly established that
patient safety and well-being is heavily dependent on nurses, robust studies linking nurse stress and fatigue with work performance
and patient safety had not been widely available. Recent studies
in healthcare settings are beginning to identify the relationship
among stress, fatigue, and medication errors, especially regarding sleep deprivation and the scheduling of nursing care (Agency
for Healthcare Research and Quality, 2001). In a 2005 study that
tracked the work patterns of 393 nurses (for example, shift, overtime, hours worked) along with errors and near misses (Oklahoma
Nurses Association, 2005-6), it was found that caregiver errors
and lapses were correlated with their work patterns. It was further
identified that overtime aggravated the situation, especially when
it followed a 12-hour shift. This assumes greater seriousness because nurses working more than 12 hours regularly, and as long
as 22.5 hours at a stretch in some instances, have been reported
in the literature (Page, 2004). The Harvard Work Hours Health
and Safety Group has systematically demonstrated an association
among work hours, patient safety, and schedules among interns.
(Although these studies did not focus on nurses, they provide hard
evidence in healthcare settings.) In a randomized study involving
an intervention in interns’ work schedules that compared a traditional schedule that included extended work hours with an intervention schedule that eliminated extended work shifts and reduced
weekly work hours, Landrigan et al. (2004) demonstrated that
the traditional schedule was associated with 35.9% more serious
medical errors and 56.6% more nonintercepted serious errors. In a
similar interventional study that focused on sleep and attentional
failure and that involved 20 interns over a 3-week period, Lockley
et al. (2004) found that the rate of attentional failure for interns in
the intervention schedule was half that of those on the traditional
schedule during on-call nights.
Typically, however, the stress problem has been treated as a healthcare operational and management issue, less relevant to the physical
environment in which nurses work, where changes to the physical
environment occur as a result of operational interventions. Examples include hand-washing stations for infection control and technology integration, which demand novel design solutions. While
workflow issues do contribute to stress, the stress-modulating potential of physical design is rarely incorporated in stress research.
Instead, long work hours, frequent overtime, sleep deprivation, and
similar issues have been the focus of addressing stress and fatigue
among care-giving staff. Organizational characteristics as potential
28
WWW.HERDJOURNAL.COM ISSN: 1937-5867
stressors are also frequently addressed. These factors might include
the type of relationship with physicians, career development opportunities, recognition for work, and the competence of support
staff, among others (Aiken & Patrician, 2000).
The predominant focus on operational and managerial issues constitutes a missed opportunity, because the physical environment
itself could be an additional source of stress, or it could interact
with facility operations to modulate the stress of the work environment. As elaborated below, the physical environment as a stressor
has been well documented in previous literature. Noise in the environment has been shown to be a significant environmental stressor,
with potentially unsafe consequences for patients and staff. Noise
has been shown to affect patients physiologically (blood pressure,
heart rate) as well as psychologically (sleep deprivation, pain, intensive care unit (ICU) psychosis, self-reported stress, and annoyance)
(Baker, 1984; Morrison et al., 2003; Topf & Thompson, 2001),
and it constitutes one of the most important environmental stressors (Moore et al., 1998). Noise has a substantial effect on staff,
too. The detrimental impacts of noise on communication, concentration, and cognitive performance, leading to stress and fatigue,
have been reported by the World Health Organization (Agency
for Healthcare Research and Quality, 2005). Occupational stress
originating from high noise levels (that is, telephones, alarms, and
beepers) has been shown to be positively related to nurse burnout
(Topf & Dillon, 1988). Conversely, the auditory environment also
has been shown to have a beneficial influence on patients. For instance, music in waiting rooms has been shown to reduce stress
levels in patients (Routhieaux & Tansik, 1997).
Beside the auditory environment (the most studied aspect of
healthcare physical settings) the impact of other environmental
factors is also supported in available literature. Studies in other settings suggest that inappropriate lighting and thermal environments
could induce stress in users (Boff & Lincoln, 1988a; Boff & Lincoln, 1988b). Improper ergonomics in the work environment can
also result in physical strain in healthcare settings (Bashir, 2002;
Benyon & Reilly, 2002; Davis, Badii, & Yassi, 2004; Smedley,
Egger, Cooper, & Coggon, 1995; Smedley et al., 2003; Trinkoff,
Lipscomb, Geiger-Brown, & Brady, 2002; Waters, Collins, Galinsky, & Caruso, 2006). The negative impacts of hospital wayfinding issues are widely known, and studies report their negative
consequences on stress in patients and visitors (Carpman, Grant,
& Simmons, 1984). Studies on visual environments have focused
on diverse issues, ranging from style to positive distractions. In one
study the style (traditional versus nouveau) of interiors was shown
to have an influence on mood and reported satisfaction (Leather,
Beale, Santos, Watts, & Lee, 2003). Artwork in interiors might reduce stress and improve tolerance to pain (Ulrich & Gilpin, 2003),
specifically art representing nature themes (e.g., water, flowers, a
garden). The influence of nature is not restricted to artwork alone.
Actual views of nature have a similarly favorable influence. In a
study comparing natural and urban environments, stress recovery
during exposure to nature was shown to be faster and more complete (Ulrich et al., 1991).
Typically, however, studies on the positive impact of a view of nature have focused on the patient population. For instance, Wilson
(1972) studied the effects of windowless intensive care environments by comparing 100 patients in two ICUs. Results indicated
that patients in windowless ICU settings were twice as likely to
experience delirium. In a similar study patients in windowless intensive therapy units in two U.K. hospitals were found to have a
less accurate memory of their length of stay, were less oriented to
time, and were twice as likely to have hallucinations or delusions
compared to patients in a unit with windows (Keep, James, & Inman, 1980). Focusing on a separate set of patient outcomes, Ulrich
(1984) demonstrated that patients exposed to views of trees through
their room windows (as opposed to matched patients exposed to a
brick wall) were associated with shorter post-operative stays, fewer
pain medications, more favorable comments on nurses’ notes, and
fewer minor complications. A subsequent study focusing on preferences (Verderber, 1986) found that nature views were preferred
over monotonous views and views of architectural features, such as
buildings. Indications that exterior views from healthcare settings
may have an impact on nurses are suggested from one study in
which staff working in windowless spaces or spaces far from windows reported lower levels of well-being compared to other staff
members (Verderber & Reuman, 1987).
Study Framework
Based on earlier study findings related to patients, we argue that
nature views could have similar positive effects on staff. If nature
views or representations of nature can affect such a wide assortment
of physiological and psychological parameters in patients, could
they be used to reduce the stress and fatigue problems of caregivers?
Figure 1 represents a framework articulating the relationship between views and stress. There are a number of other environmental
and organizational factors that are obvious confounding variables.
We designed the study as a preliminary examination of the relationship between view and stress/alertness, with controls for the
confounding variables shown in the framework.
Research Question
We designed the study with the objective to examine the relationship between views and stress with preliminary controls for obvious confounding variables. We studied two outcome parameters:
(1) acute stress, and (2) alertness. The study focused on the following questions:
1. What is the relationship between duration of exposure to
exterior views, and stress and alertness in nurses?
2. Is the relationship moderated by view content (nature view as
opposed to non-nature view)?
Method
Setting
Two pediatric hospitals in the Atlanta metropolitan area that afforded a range of different views were selected for the study. Both
the hospitals are under the corporate umbrella of Children’s Healthcare of Atlanta (CHOA). At one hospital the presence of a green
zone within the hospital offered a classic nature view situation in
some areas. In both hospitals, owing to the general character of
the Atlanta metropolitan landscape, views varied from the urban
(buildings, streets, and parking) to the natural (trees, greenery)
across the various units.
Dependent Variables and Measures
View Content
View Duration
PA P E R S
Stress
Alertness
Organizational
Characteristics
Work Load
Physical Environment
Characteristics
Personal Factors
Figure 1. A preliminary framework articulating the relationship
between view duration, content, stress/alertness, and other known
stressors in a nurse’s work environment.
The dependent variables in the study consisted of three measures:
(1) acute stress, (2) chronic stress, and (3) alertness. Acute stress
was defined as stress usually for a short period of time that could result from work-related pressure, minor mishaps, and other similar
factors. Chronic stress was defined as prolonged (ongoing) stress
that exists for long durations, resulting from such factors as lifestyle
and life events. Alertness was defined as a state of readiness to respond to a stimulus, or response readiness.
Acute stress and alertness were measured using the Stress/Arousal
Adjective Checklist or SACL (King, Burrows, & Stanley, 1983;
Mackay, Cox, Burrows, & Lazzerini, 1978). Adequate internal
reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.86 for acute stress and 0.74 for
arousal), construct validity of the two scales, and the independence of the two scales have been reported by King, et al. (1983).
We used the arousal scale to measure alertness, because arousal
and alertness have been used interchangeably in earlier studies
(for instance, Paus et al., 1997). Moreover, the SACL arousal
HERD Vol. 1, No. 2 WINTER 2008 • HEALTH ENVIRONMENTS RESEARCH & DESIGN JOURNAL
29
PA P E R S
Relationships between Exterior Views and Nurse Stress: An Exploratory Examination
PA P E R S
HERD Volume 1, Number 2, pp 27-38 Copyright ©2008 Vendome Group, LLC scale includes such adjectives as active, vigorous, lively, tired, sleepy,
drowsy, passive, energetic, alert, and aroused, which are the ones we
were interested in measuring, and they appeared to possess appropriate face validity. It is commonly believed that increased arousal
levels can result in performance improvements up to a point, beyond
which performance deteriorates (King, et al., 1983). The arousal
state (or for our purposes, the nurses’ alertness) was important, because the efficiency of patient care and patient safety partly depends
on the response readiness and alertness of nurses.
The SACL uses a list of 20 adjectives to assess the current stress and
arousal levels of respondents. Ten of the adjectives measure stress
and the remainder measure arousal. For each item respondents are
required to choose from four responses to indicate the extent to
which the adjectives describe their mood at that moment. The four
responses are: (1) definitely yes, (2) slightly yes, (3) not sure, and (4)
definitely no. The items on stress and arousal are positively or negatively weighted, depending on the directionality of their association
with the two parameters of interest. Total acute stress and arousal
scores are calculated by aggregating the scores associated with each
item.
Chronic stress was measured using the Perceived Stress Scale, or
PSS-10 (Cohen, 1986; Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstien, 1983).
The PSS-10 is a global measure of perceived stress, and the 10
items on the scale ask the subject to respond to questions regarding
the frequency of certain feelings and thoughts over the past month.
The scale measures the perceived level of stressfulness. Adequate
internal reliability (alpha coefficient = 0.78) and construct validity
of the scale have been reported by Cohen et al. (1983), and Cohen
and Williamson (1988). Each item on the scale offers five responses
with corresponding numerical ratings: never (0), almost never (1),
sometimes (2), fairly often (3), and very often (4). The aggregated
chronic stress score is calculated by adding the ratings associated
with all 10 items for each respondent. A minor modification was
made to the scale by reducing the time period to the past 12 hours
as opposed to the past one month. Chronic stress was not expected
to change over the period of one shift as a result of the stressors one
encounters in the nursing environment. The purpose of including
the chronic stress scale was to confirm that the chronic stress level
indeed did not change over one shift.
Independent Variables and Measures
The study involved two independent variables: (1) the duration
of exposure to exterior view, and (2) view content (nature versus
non-nature view).
View duration was defined as the percentage of a work shift that
subjects were exposed to exterior views as they performed their
work. The measure employed was perceptual, and the data reported the perceived percentage of time that subjects were exposed to
exterior views. The exact question posed to respondents was: In
30
WWW.HERDJOURNAL.COM ISSN: 1937-5867
your estimation what is the percentage of the 12-hour shift during
which you have a view of the outside?
View content was defined as the type of exterior environment that
was predominant in the exterior view frames. Like view duration,
this was also a perceptual measure (as perceived by the respondents). Two types of environments were included: (1) nature, including trees, plants, bushes, lawns, sky, and water features; and
(2) non-nature, including buildings, streets, terraces, and surface
parking. The exact question posed to respondents was: Of the duration when you had an exterior view during your work today, what
percentage of the view was: (a) A view of nature, and (b) Non-nature
(other exterior) views? Both were reported in percentages.
The independent variables were measured through self-reporting by
the subjects. The data were collected by means of an investigatordesigned questionnaire that included the two independent variables,
and a subset of the variables that were used as control measures. Figure 2 presents sample photographs that represent predominantly
nature and non-nature views through patient rooms.
Figure 2. Photographs through patient room windows showing a
representative sample of predominantly nature views (top) and nonnature views (bottom) at the study sites.
Control Variables and Measures
Environmental factors
Data on a number of confounding variables were collected to
serve as control measures in the data analysis. Lighting, acoustics,
and thermal and ergonomic conditions are known to be potential environmental stressors. Accordingly, these four factors were
included in the investigator-designed questionnaire. Respondents
were asked to specify on a five-point Likert-type scale the extent to
which the four environmental conditions were stressful during that
shift (from “not at all” to “very stressful” or “bothersome”).
PA P E R S
Workload factor
Personal factors
The second factor that was considered as a potential stressor was
workload. Data were collected on the number of hours respondents
had worked since their last day off, the number of patients assigned
to them on a particular shift, and the percentage of required tasks
conducted by an aide (if any).
Finally, data on a number of personal and demographic variables
were collected. The variables included: (1) age, (2) sex, (3) education level, (4) provider role (e.g., RN, LPN), (5) position in the
clinical unit, (6) total years worked as a nurse, (7) total years worked
as a nurse in the hospital, and (8) hourly pay range. Table 1 lists the
variables included in the study and the corresponding measures.
Organizational stress
We used the Revised Nursing Work Index (NWI-R) (Aiken &
Patrician, 2000) as a surrogate measure for organizational stress.
The NWI-R was developed to capture or measure organizational
characteristics, not as an organizational stress measure. However,
the measure has been shown to be highly correlated with organizational stress outcomes, such as satisfaction, turnover, and
retention (Aiken & Patrician, 2000). The NWI-R is a 57-item
scale with four response options for each item with associated
numerical ratings: strongly agree (1), somewhat agree (2), somewhat disagree (3), and strongly disagree (4). The NWI-R score
for respondents is calculated by aggregating the individual scores
of the 57 items. We considered higher aggregated scores (higher
aggregated disagreement with the existence of positive attributes
in the job environment) as a stressor, representing a higher degree
of stress.
Table 1. Study Variables and Corresponding Measures
Variables
Measures
Dependent Variables
Acute Stress
Stress/Arousal Adjective
Checklist: SACL
Chronic Stress
Perceived Stress Scale:
PSS-10
Arousal State
Stress/Arousal Adjective
Checklist: SACL
Independent Variables
Duration of exterior view
Questionnaire
View content
Questionnaire
Control Variables
Stress from lighting environment
Questionnaire
Stress from auditory environment
Questionnaire
Stress from thermal environment
Questionnaire
Stress from ergonomic factors
Questionnaire
Workload
Questionnaire
Work experience
Questionnaire
Personal/demographic data
Questionnaire
Stress from organizational characteristics
Revised Nursing Work
Index: NWI-R
The questionnaire was piloted for reliability using a combination
of field testing and cognitive pretesting methods prescribed by
Krosnick (1999). Two sets of survey instruments were created for
the study. The first set was created for data to be collected at the
beginning of a shift (before-shift survey), and the second set was
created for data to be collected at the end of a shift (after-shift
survey). The before-shift survey set included (1) the PSS-10 and
(2) the SACL instrument. The after-shift survey included: (1) the
PSS-10, (2) the SACL, (3) an investigator-designed questionnaire,
and (4) the NWI-R survey. The study protocol was approved by
the institutional review board of CHOA.
Study Sample
We did not restrict the study to any particular unit. Rather, all
nurses on all units at the two hospitals were invited to participate
in the study. Only registered nurses were invited to participate.
Nurse aides were excluded from the study. Before data collection,
descriptions of the study and data collection dates were sent by
email to all nurses in the two hospitals. In addition, posters announcing the study were placed at key locations in the two hospitals. Data were collected over a period of one 12-hour shift in each
of the two hospitals during the month of November 2006. The
shifts started at 7:00 a.m. and concluded at 7:00 p.m.
Results
All respondents were female. There was considerable variability in
age and experience. The mean age was 43 years with a range of
24 to 56 years. The mean total work experience as a nurse was 17
years with a range of 1.3 to 35 years. Years worked as a nurse in the
hospital had a mean of 11 years, with the range varying between
fresh recruits to those having served for 34 years. A total of 55
nurses at the two hospitals volunteered to participate in the study.
Volunteering nurses were instructed to complete the before-shift
survey set at the beginning of their shift and the after-shift survey
set at the end of their shift. A total of 32 nurses from 19 different
units in the two hospitals returned their completed surveys.
Similar variability was evident in view duration and content. The
view duration ranged from zero to 80% of the 12-hour shift, with
43.75% of nurses reporting zero hours of exposure to an external view. Nurses with zero hours’ exposure to the exterior reported
working in the pediatric intensive care unit, the emergency department, the rehabilitation unit, the neonatal intensive care unit, the
cardiac catheterization lab, and the recovery room. Another 37.5%
HERD Vol. 1, No. 2 WINTER 2008 • HEALTH ENVIRONMENTS RESEARCH & DESIGN JOURNAL
31
PA P E R S
Relationships between Exterior Views and Nurse Stress: An Exploratory Examination
PA P E R S
HERD Volume 1, Number 2, pp 27-38 Copyright ©2008 Vendome Group, LLC reported view durations of 10% or less. Of the nurses exposed to
some view during the shift, 38% reported exposure to predominantly non-nature views, and 28.6% reported predominantly nature views.
Data Analysis
Data analysis was conducted using SPSS version 15 for Windows
(SPSS 15.0 for Windows, SPSS, Inc., 2006). Three main categories of data analyses were conducted: (1) paired sample comparison,
(2) multivariate ordinary least square (OLS) regression and a joint
partial F-test, and (3) multivariate regression with interaction terms.
The first set of analyses was conducted to assess the construct validity of the instruments used for the study. We expected to observe
the following from the paired sample comparisons: (1) Between
the beginning and the end of the shift, chronic stress levels would
not change. This was not dependent on day-to-day events at one’s
workplace; (2) Between the beginning and the end of the shift,
arousal levels would generally decrease. This is because after a 12hour work period, one’s state of alertness is expected to decline
(nurses would not be as alert as they were at the beginning of the
shift); and (3) Between the beginning and the end of the shift,
acute stress levels would generally go up. Stressful work in healthcare settings would raise the acute stress level in nursing staff. The
results of the data analysis are reported in the subsequent sections.
is logical because hospitals are stressful environments, and a rise in
acute stress level could be expected for people working for 12 continuous hours in such an environment. Even exposure to a nature
view (with a hypothesized positive influence) may not completely
compensate for the stressors hospital nurses are exposed to.
What impact does view have on nurses working in hospitals? With
confidence in the construct validity of our instruments resulting
from the paired-sample comparisons, we proceeded to examine the
relationship between duration of exposure to exterior views and
acute stress and arousal.
Relationship Between View Duration, Stress, and Arousal
We developed multivariate OLS regression models to identify the
impact of view duration on acute stress and arousal levels. Before
finalizing the models and running the regression routines, however, we made several manipulations to the data set by combining
variables to create index measures. The purpose of creating index
variables was two-fold. First, some of the variables were highly correlated. Moreover, the main objective of the study was to explore
the association between view content and duration, and stress and
arousal. The control variables were included only to account for
other factors that are known to induce stress. Thus, we were less interested in exploring the associations between each individual control variable and stress or arousal. This step also helped reduce the
number of parameters being tested, considering the sample size.
Mean Stress and Arousal Before and After Shift
The paired sample comparisons focused on comparing the mean
levels of chronic and acute stress and arousal between the beginning and the end of 12-hour shifts. In this phase of analysis no
distinctions were made between the nurses based on view content
and duration. Table 2 summarizes the results of the paired sample
t-tests conducted for each of the dependent variables.
The results of the analyses coincided with our expectations. Mean
chronic stress levels remained the same. The difference in mean
chronic stress levels before and after the shift was not significant.
However, the differences in the mean arousal level and mean acute
stress level before and after the shift were significant. As expected,
the general level of arousal went down between the beginning and
the end of the shift, and the mean acute stress level went up for the
entire group between the beginning and the end of the shift. This
Workload index
The first index variable created was “workload,” which included
three measures: (1) hours worked since last day off, (2) number
of patients under direct care during the shift, and (3) percentage
of required tasks conducted by an aide. These three factors were
considered to contribute to perceived as well as actual workload,
and hence were aggregated.
AE index
The second index variable was created using four measures:
(1) age, (2) number of years worked as a nurse, (3) number of years
worked as a nurse in CHOA, and (4) hourly pay bracket. The four
variables were highly correlated with one another and formed a
meaningful cluster. We termed the new measure “AE Index” (to
stand for Age and Experience index).
Table 2. Results of the Paired Sample t-Tests for Chronic Stress, Acute Stress, and Arousal, Before and After a 12-Hour Shift
Dependent Measure
Mean Value Before Shift
Mean Value After Shift
Difference Between the
Means
Significance
Chronic stress
14.5953
13.6961
–0.89923
0.062
Acute stress
–3.9250
–1.8353
2.08969
0.000***
Arousal
7.9714
4.4551
–3.51634
0.000***
*** significant at 0.001
32
WWW.HERDJOURNAL.COM ISSN: 1937-5867
NWI-R score
Similarly, education was highly correlated with the NWI-R score.
There was no meaningful way to combine the two. Because education level is partly reflected in the AE Index, we decided to drop the
variable from the multivariate model.
Environmental Stress
Finally, we combined the four environmental stressors (that is,
lighting, auditory, thermal, and ergonomic) into one variable
termed “environmental stress.” Correlational analysis with the recomputed measures showed no major bivariate correlations among
the variables.
The multivariate models developed with the new set of variables
follow:
Arousal (after shift) = View (duration) + Arousal (before shift) + Environmental Stress + AE Index + Workload + Organizational Stress
Acute Stress (after Shift) = View (duration) + Acute Stress (before
shift) + Environmental Stress + AE Index + Workload + Organizational Stress
Table 3 summarizes the results of the regression analyses, showing
the association between arousal (after shift) and view (duration),
controlling for other hypothesized stressors. The model as a whole
is significant, and it explains 37.2% of the variability in arousal
(after shift). Among all the hypothesized factors bearing an impact
on arousal levels (other than before-shift arousal level), analysis
suggests that view duration is the second most influential factor.
The longer the view duration, the higher the response readiness of
nurses at the end of a 12-hour shift will be. The only other measure
that has a greater influence is organizational stress.
PA P E R S
There are other logical implications from the regression results: Organizational stress has a negative influence on alertness. The higher
the organizational stress, the lower the arousal level (alertness level)
will be at the end of the shift. In addition, both environmental
stress and workload have a negative association with arousal, suggesting that higher environmental stress, workload, or both will result in a lower level of alertness at the end of a shift. The only other
positive association is with the AE Index, implying that higher age
and experience are associated with higher arousal levels (alertness)
at the end of a shift, or with greater experience caregivers manage
to maintain relatively higher alertness throughout the shift.
Considering the relatively strong influence of view duration on
arousal level after shift, we ascertained the partial effect of view
duration on the latter. A joint partial F-test suggested that view
duration alone contributed 4.8% additional explanatory power
to the model. To understand the relationship in greater detail, we
grouped the respondents into two categories based on the difference between their alertness before and after their shifts, as follows:
(1) alertness remained the same or improved, and (2) alertness
deteriorated. We found that of all the nurses whose alertness remained the same or improved in the 12-hour period, almost 60%
had exposure to exterior views. All of these nurses had exposure to
a predominantly nature view. In contrast, of all nurses whose alertness deteriorated between the beginning and the end of the shift,
67% were exposed to no view or only to non-nature views.
We conducted a similar set of analyses to understand the relationship between view duration and acute stress. Table 4 summarizes
the results of the regression analyses, which show an association between acute stress (after shift) and view (duration), controlling for
other hypothesized stressors. As in the case of arousal (not counting
before-shift acute stress level), the view was the second most influen-
Table 3. Summary of Regression Analyses Showing an Association Between Arousal After Shift and View Duration
R
R2
R2 adjusted
F
Significance
0.624
0.389
0.372
22.103
0.000***
Estimate
Beta
T
Significance
7.006
0.000***
Parameters
Constant
View duration
13.28
0.1
0.273
4.109
0.000***
Arousal (before shift)
0.511
0.502
7.106
0.000***
Environmental stress
–0.46
–0.236
–3.693
0.000***
AE Index
0.47
0.159
2.464
0.015*
Workload
–0.867
–0.191
–3.119
0.002*
Organizational stress
–3.664
–0.307
–4.851
0.000***
*** significant at 0.001 ** significant at 0.01 * significant at 0.05
HERD Vol. 1, No. 2 WINTER 2008 • HEALTH ENVIRONMENTS RESEARCH & DESIGN JOURNAL
33
PA P E R S
Relationships between Exterior Views and Nurse Stress: An Exploratory Examination
PA P E R S
HERD Volume 1, Number 2, pp 27-38 Copyright ©2008 Vendome Group, LLC tial factor in explaining acute stress levels after shift. Environmental
stressors (that is, lighting, acoustics, thermal, and ergonomic) were
the most influential factors. Furthermore, workload demonstrated a
positive relationship with acute stress: A greater workload is associated with a higher level of acute stress after shift. On the other hand,
greater age and experience are associated with lower levels of acute
stress after shift. The association between organizational characteristics and acute stress was not significant, but positive. Additionally,
a partial F-test of the model suggested that view duration alone explained 6.4% of the variance in after-shift acute stress.
stress was conditional on the view content—nature or non-nature.
We hypothesized a similar model for arousal. However, partitioning
a relatively small sample into three subcategories and corresponding
interaction terms has its own analytical pitfalls. We approached the
issue by introducing a categorical variable on view content (which,
incidentally, has identical problems; sample sizes of the categories
were 14 with no view, 13 with predominantly nature views, and 5
with predominantly non-nature views). The results suggest that exposure to a nature view over a non-nature view, and a non-nature
view over no view have a beneficial impact on both acute stress and
arousal. (Given the small sample size, this conclusion should be treated as merely suggestive; full data are available upon request). Figures
3 and 4 illustrate the interaction effects. Note that we explored only
the differences in group means; there is a possibility that the slopes
might be different for the three groups, which we did not examine. This does suggest, however, that views (nature or non-nature)
have a positive effect on acute stress (Figure 3). Specifically, although
acute stress levels went up for the whole group during the shift, the
nurses exposed to a nature view had a lower level of stress than those
exposed to a non-nature view. The highest acute stress levels were
observed in nurses working in a no-view situation. Similarly, a nature
view (as opposed to a non-nature view and no view) has a positive
effect on alertness (Figure 4). Specifically, although alertness levels
went down for the whole group during the shift, nurses exposed to
a nature view were at a higher level of alertness than those exposed
to a non-nature view. The lowest levels of alertness were observed in
nurses working in a no-view situation.
One nonconfirming aspect of the multivariate analysis was the
direction of the association between view duration and acute stress
after shift. We were expecting a negative association. However, the
analysis suggests a positive association: Longer view duration was
associated with higher post-shift acute stress, everything else remaining the same. To understand this relationship in greater detail,
we grouped the respondents into two categories based on the difference between their acute stress levels before and after shift, as
follows: (1) acute stress level remained the same or was reduced,
and (2) acute stress level increased. We found that of all nurses
whose acute stress condition remained the same or was reduced
between the beginning and the end of the shift, 64% were exposed
to views (71% of the 64% were exposed to a nature view). On the
other hand, of all the nurses whose acute stress levels increased
between the beginning and the end of the shift, 56% had no view
during the shift, or had only a non-nature view. This suggests a
beneficial effect of view on acute stress.
Discussion
Data analysis revealed that: (1) Chronic stress did not change over
the period of a 12-hour shift; (2) Acute stress for the whole group
went up between the beginning and the end of the shift; and (3)
Arousal levels (or alertness) for the whole group went down be-
Interaction Effects
The conflicting findings on the relationship between view and acute
stress prompted us to look for possible interaction effects. Our hypothesis was that the relationship between view duration and acute
Table 4. Summary of Regression Analyses Showing an Association between Acute Stress after Shift and View Duration
R
R2
R2 adjusted
F
Significance
0.669
0.447
0.431
28.063
0.000***
Parameters
Estimate
Beta
t
Significance
–6.348
0.000***
Constant
–13.223
View duration
0.117
0.266
4.956
0.000***
Acute stress (before shift)
0.499
0.5
9.616
0.000***
Environmental stress
0.847
0.362
6.453
0.000***
AE Index
–0.864
–0.244
–4.174
0.000***
Workload
0.599
0.11
1.998
0.047*
NWI-R
0.902
0.063
1.157
0.249
*** significant at 0.001 ** significant at 0.01 * significant at 0.05
34
WWW.HERDJOURNAL.COM ISSN: 1937-5867
PA P E R S
0
-0.5
Before Shift
After Shift
Acute Stress Scale
-1
-1.5
No View
-2
Non-Nature View
-2.5
Nature View
-3
-3.5
-4
-4.5
Figure 3. Figure shows the interaction effect of view types on acute stress, with nurses exposed to views at a lower level of acute stress.
(Note that the possibility of slope interaction was not explored.)
9
8
Arousal Scale
7
6
5
No View
4
Non-Nature View
Nature View
3
2
1
0
Before Shift
After Shift
Figure 4. Figure shows the interaction effect of view types on alertness, with nurses exposed to a nature view (as opposed to a non-nature
or no view) at a higher level of alertness. (Note that the possibility of slope interaction was not explored.)
tween the beginning and the end of the shift. These three findings contributed to the construct validity of our study instruments.
Because this study was designed as a preliminary examination, our
primary focus was on the directionality of the relationships rather
than on absolute values. Despite the small sample, findings were
consistent with our expectations.
Furthermore, the multivariate OLS regression analyses suggest that
exposure to view is an influential factor in explaining both arousal
and acute stress after shift. In fact, view explained as much as 5%
to 6% of the variance in arousal and acute stress after shift. More
notable are the findings that, among the variables considered in the
study, view is second only to stress associated with organizational
characteristics (in the case of alertness) and environmental stress
HERD Vol. 1, No. 2 WINTER 2008 • HEALTH ENVIRONMENTS RESEARCH & DESIGN JOURNAL
35
PA P E R S
Relationships between Exterior Views and Nurse Stress: An Exploratory Examination
PA P E R S
HERD Volume 1, Number 2, pp 27-38 Copyright ©2008 Vendome Group, LLC Organizational
Characteristics
Work Load
Personal Factors
Organizational
Characteristics
Work Load
Personal Factors
Staff Outcome
Physical Environment
Patient Outcome
Organizational Outcome
Figure 5. Model of the potential direct and indirect impact of care-giver view on the patient and the organization.
(in the case of acute stress) in its influence. Finally, the implication that the relationship between view duration and stress/arousal
could be conditional on view content is important to people in
both healthcare design and healthcare operations planning.
More specifically, the probability of the study’s findings conforming to a series of expected outcomes purely by chance is so low that
it partly offsets any shortcomings related to the nonexperimental
design and sample size of the study.
Because long working hours, overtime, and sleep deprivation are
problems in healthcare operations, the suggestion that the physical design of clinical units could potentially improve the situation
significantly is of considerable importance. As mentioned earlier,
stress and alertness have hypothetical associations with safety issues. Moreover, these results offer a rather simple way to create
inspiring spaces that promote enthusiasm among nurses, enhance
job satisfaction and retention, and promote calm in an intense dayto-day work environment. This would necessitate a rethinking of
the way we approach healthcare facility design. Currently considerable emphasis is placed on nature views and natural light in patient
rooms. The issue, however, is less frequently discussed in the context of the caregiver’s environment, particularly those who work in
areas of the hospital that do not afford occasional views through
patient rooms. Access to a nature view or natural light or both for
care-giving staff could have a direct as well as an indirect effect on
patient outcomes. Figure 5 demonstrates this hypothesis, which
also suggests that these phenomena could impact organizational
objectives such as staff retention/turnover/recruitment, variable
costs, and market share.
Future studies should address one major confounding factor that
was not dealt with in this study. This confounding factor pertains
to view as opposed to natural light. Incidentally, this shortcoming
is also typical of the seminal studies mentioned in the introductory
section. In other words, it is not clear whether the positive influence of exposure to the exterior on patients and staff is associated
with the view, with natural light, or both.
Limitations of the Study
Despite the small sample in this study, the fact that a range of expected outcomes was confirmed by data analysis is notable, and it
develops a good foundation and confidence for subsequent studies.
36
WWW.HERDJOURNAL.COM ISSN: 1937-5867
This is especially important considering the impact natural light
bears on the neurobiological and physiological processes of human
beings. Recent studies are adding considerable weight to the associations between light wavelength, circadian rhythm, and alertness, among other outcomes. Lockley et al. (2005) designed a between-subject study involving 16 healthy adults at the Intensive
Physiological Monitoring Unit at Brigham and Women’s Hospital
in Boston. One group was exposed to monochromatic light wavelength of 460 nm (blue light), and the other group was exposed
to monochromatic light wavelength of 555 nm (green light), for
a period of 6.5 hours. They found that subjects exposed to 460
nm light had fewer attentional failures, decreased auditory reaction
time, and lower sleepiness ratings. The study was an extension of
an earlier study (Lockley, Brainard, & Czeisler, 2003) that articulated the variable impact of light wavelength on human circadian
rhythm, specifically the impact of blue light on human circadian
pacemakers. These studies begin to suggest that spectral qualities
of light (or natural light) may significantly impact the alertness of
nurses, irrespective of the availability of exterior view, with identifiable implications on patient safety parameters. Future studies need
to address this topic. Regardless of the outcome of such future studies, the findings will have a major impact on the way nursing units
are designed. In addition to the perception measures employed in
this study, objective measures of physiological correlates of stress
and duration of view in any future studies would begin to provide
information that could directly affect how hospitals are designed
for safety in the future.
Conclusions and Implications for Hospital Design
and Operation
Why is this study critical to hospital administrators, nurses, and
patients and their families? The study reveals that visual relief can
positively affect caregivers: It can, on a daily basis, improve alertness
and sharpen focus in an intense day-to-day work environment. On
a long-term basis, it might enhance job satisfaction and retention,
which consequently lowers operating costs. Additionally, higher response readiness should mean improved patient safety. Quality of
view—referring to visual and perhaps mental respite—is shown to
be a significant, possibly critical, factor in caregivers’ focus on their
subjects (patients) and their tasks (care giving). These findings warrant a change in facility planning priorities and the operational
policies of healthcare facilities. The message to trustees, administrators, and their planning and design consultants might be to:
1. Embrace the importance of view and visual relief—especially
for staff work areas—as capital planning and budgeting are
conducted.
2.
Require that the programming of space requirements include a
specific description of design intent (for a generous view) along
with the allocation of space to accommodate these elements of
design.
3.
Insist that the design of capital projects demonstrate attention
to the provision of views in all areas of the workplace created
for staff. This single goal, stated as a guiding principle for a
project, will have an immense impact on physical design.
4.
Mandate restorative breaks as an integral aspect of operational
policy. This could partly address the serious issue of patient
safety that healthcare organizations are struggling with today.
Insist that employees take breaks that include views—away
from their work environment, if necessary, if views are otherwise unavailable.
References
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. (2001). Making health care safer: A
critical analysis of patient safety practices. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. Retrieved April 20, 2006, from http://www.ahrq.gov/
Clinic/ptsafety/index.html
PA P E R S
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. (2005). Creating a culture of patient
safety through innovative hospital design. In Advances in Patient Safety, Vol.
2. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. Retrieved April
20, 2006, from http://www.ahrq.gov/downloads/pub/advances/vol2/Reiling.pdf
Aiken, L. H., & Patrician, P. A. (2000). Measuring organizational traits of hospitals:
The revised nursing work index. Nursing Research, 49(3), 146–153.
Baker, C. F. (1984). Sensory overload and noise in the ICU: Sources of environmental stress. Critical Care Quarterly, 6(4), 66–80.
Barach, P., & Weinger, M. (2007). Trauma team performance. In W. C. Wilson, C. M.
Grande, & D. B. Hoyt (Eds.), Trauma: Emergency resuscitation and preoperative
anesthesia management, Vol. 1. New York: Marcel Dekker, Inc.
Bashir, M. (2002). Avoiding muscular strain in patient-care activities. Nursing Journal of India, 93(4), 80–81.
Benyon, C., & Reilly, T. (2002). Epidemiology of musculoskeletal disorders in a
sample of British nurses and physiotherapists. In T. Reilly (Ed.), Musculoskeletal
disorder in health related occupations. Lancaster: IOS Press.
Boff, K. R., & Lincoln, J. E. (1988a). Engineering data compendium: Human perception and performance, Vol. 1. Wright-Patterson A.F.B., OH: Harry G. Armstrong Aerospace Medical Research Laboratory.
Boff, K. R., & Lincoln, J. E. (1988b). Engineering data compendium: Human perception and performance , Vol. 3. Wright-Patterson A.F.B., OH: Harry G. Armstrong Aerospace Medical Research Laboratory.
Carpman, J., Grant, M., & Simmons, D. (1984). No more mazes: Research about
design for wayfinding in hospitals. Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan
Hospitals.
Cohen, S. (1986). Contrasting the Hassle Scale and the Perceived Stress Scale.
American Psychologist, 41, 717–718.
Cohen, S., & Williamson, G. (1988). Perceived stress in a probability sample of the
United States. In S. Spacapan & S. Oskamp (Eds.), The social psychology of
health. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstien, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived
stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385–396.
Davis, P. M., Badii, M., & Yassi, A. (2004). Preventing disability from occupational
musculoskeletal injuries in an urban, acute and tertiary care hospital: Results
from a prevention and early active return-to-work safely program. Journal of
Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 46(12), 1253–1262.
Greco, P., Laschinger, H. K. S., & Wong, C. (2006). Leader empowering behaviours,
staff nurse empowerment and work engagement/burnout. Nursing Leadership,
19(4), 41–56.
Hinshaw, A. S., & Atwood, J. R. (1984). Nursing staff turnover, stress and satisfaction: Models, measures and management. In H. H. Werley & J. J. Fitzpatrick
(Eds.), Annual Review of Nursing Research: Vol 1 (pp. 133–153). New York:
Springer.
Houle, J. (2001). Health and safety survey. American Nurses Association. Retrieved
April 2, 2006, from http://nursingworld.org/MainMenuCategories/OccupationalandEnvironmental/occupationalhealth/HealthSafetySurvey.aspx
Keep, P. J., James, J., & Inman, M. (1980). Windows in the intensive therapy unit.
Anaesthesia, 35(3), 257–262.
King, M. G., Burrows, G. D., & Stanley, G. V. (1983). Measurement of stress and
arousal: Validation of the stress/arousal adjective checklist. British Journal of
Psychology, 74, 473–479.
Krosnick, J. A. (1999). Survey research. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 537–
567.
Landrigan, C., Rothschild, J., Cronin, J., Kaushal, R., Burdick, E., Katz, J., et al.
(2004). Effect of reducing interns’ work hours on serious medical errors in intensive care units. The New England Journal of Medicine, 351(18), 1838–1848.
Leather, P., Beale, D., Santos, A., Watts, J., & Lee, L. (2003). Outcomes of environmental appraisal of different hospital waiting areas. Environment & Behavior,
35(6), 842–869.
Lockley, S. W., Cronin, J., Evans, E., Cade, B., Lee, C., Landrigan, C., et al. (2004).
Effect of reduced interns’ weekly work hours on sleep and attentional failures.
The New England Journal of Medicine, 351(18), 1829–1837.
HERD Vol. 1, No. 2 WINTER 2008 • HEALTH ENVIRONMENTS RESEARCH & DESIGN JOURNAL
37
PA P E R S
Relationships between Exterior Views and Nurse Stress: An Exploratory Examination
PA P E R S
HERD Volume 1, Number 2, pp 27-38 Copyright ©2008 Vendome Group, LLC Lockley, S. W., Brainard, G. C., & Czeisler, C. S. (2003). High sensitivity of the
human circadian melatonin rhythm to resetting by short wavelength light. The
Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 88(9), 4502–4505.
Lockley, S. W., Evans, E. E., Scheer, F., Brainard, G. C., Czeisler, C. A., & Aeschbach, D. (2005). Short-wavelength sensitivity for the direct effects of light on
alertness, vigilance, and the waking electroencephalogram in humans. Sleep,
29(2), 161–168.
Mackay, C., Cox, T., Burrows, G., & Lazzerini, T. (1978). An inventory of the measurement of self-reported stress and arousal. The British Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology, 17, 283–284.
Moore, M., Nguyen, D., Nolan, S., Robinson, S., Ryals, B., Imbrie, J., et al. (1998).
Interventions to reduce decibel levels on patient care units. American Surgeon,
64(9), 894–899.
Morrison, W. E., Haas, E. C., Shaffner, D. H., Garrett, E. S., & Fackler, J. C. (2003).
Noise, stress, and annoyance in a pediatric intensive care unit. Critical Care
Medicine, 31(1), 113–119.
Oklahoma Nurses Association. (2005-2006). Implications of fatigue on patient
safety. The Oklahoma Nurse, 50(4), 7–8.
Page, A. (2004). Keeping patients safe: Transforming the work environment of nurses. Washington, DC: The National Academy Press.
Paus, T., Zatorre, R., Hofle, N., Caramanos, Z., Gotman, J., Petrides, M., et al.
(1997). Time-related changes in neural systems underlying attention and arousal during the performance of an auditory vigilance task. The Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 9, 392–408.
Routhieaux, R. L., & Tansik, D. A. (1997). The benefits of music in hospital waiting
rooms. Health Care Supervisor, 16(2), 31–40.
Smedley, J., Egger, P., Cooper, C., & Coggon, D. (1995). Manual handling activities
and risk of low back pain in nurses. Occupational and Environmental Medicine,
52, 160–163.
Smedley, J., Inskip, H., Trevelyan, F., Buckle, P., Cooper, C., & Coggon, D. (2003).
Risk factors for incident neck and shoulder pain in hospital nurses. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 60, 864–869.
38
WWW.HERDJOURNAL.COM ISSN: 1937-5867
Tabone, S. (2004a). Data suggest nurse fatigue threatens patient safety. Silver
Spring, MD: Center for American Nurses.
Tabone, S. (2004b). Nurse fatigue: The human factor. Texas Nursing, June–July,
1–3.
Topf, M., & Dillon, E. (1988). Noise-induced stress as a predictor of burnout in critical care nurses. Heart Lung, 17(5), 567–574.
Topf, M., & Thompson, S. (2001). Interactive relationships between hospital patients’ noise-induced stress and other stress with sleep. Heart Lung, 30(4),
237–243.
Trinkoff, A. M., Lipscomb, J. A., Geiger-Brown, J., & Brady, B. (2002). Musculoskeletal problems of the neck, shoulder, and back and functional consequences in
nurses. American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 41(3), 170–178.
Ulrich, R. S. (1984). View through a window may influence recovery from surgery.
Science, 224(4647), 420–421.
Ulrich, R. S., & Gilpin, L. (2003). Healing arts: Nutrition for the soul. In S. B. Frampton, L. Gilpin, & P. Charmel (Eds.), Putting patients first: Designing and practicing patient centered care (pp. 117–146). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., & Zelson, M.
(1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 11(2), 201–230.
Verderber, S. (1986). Dimensions of person-window transactions in the hospital
environment. Environment & Behavior, 18(4), 450–466.
Verderber, S., & Reuman, D. (1987). Windows, views, and health status in hospital
therapeutic environments. Journal of Architectural & Planning Research, 4(2),
120–133.
Waters, T., Collins, J., Galinsky, T., & Caruso, C. (2006). NIOSH research efforts
to prevent musculoskeletal disorders in the healthcare industry. Orthopaedic
Nursing, 25(6), 380–389.
Wilson, L. M. (1972). Intensive care delirium: The effect of outside deprivation in a
windowless unit. Archives of Internal Medicine, 130(2), 225–226.
Download