The Psychological Record , 1998, 48, 591-600 AGGRESSIVE RESPONDING IN THE LABORATORY MAINTAINED BY THE INITIATION OF A PROVOCATION-FREE INTERVAL DONALD M. DOUGHERTY, DON R. CHEREK, and SCOTT D. LANE University of Texas-Houston Health Science Center Four adults participated in an experiment designed to study how the duration of an interval free from provocation , following aggressive responding, affected the maintenance of such responding . In testing sessions, two response options were made available on separate buttons labeled "A" and "B." Pressing Button A 100 times incremented a point accumulation counter, and these pOints were later exchanged for money (one point = 10 cents). Pressing Button B 10 times subtracted a pOint from a fictitious other subject. These B button responses were defined as aggressive because they ostensibly presented an aversive stimulus (i.e., point subtraction) to another person. Subjects were provoked by subtracting pOints from their pOint counter at random intervals (ranging from 6 s to 120 s) , and instructions attributed these point subtractions to the responding of the other subject. Button B responding could initiate periods free from provocation. Subjects were exposed to two provocation-free interval durations (5 sand 62 .5 s) in order to determine what role the provocationfree interval had in maintaining aggressive responding. We found that brief (5 s) provocation-free intervals (a) can initially engender high rates of aggressive responding ; but (b) cannot maintain aggressive responding during extended periods of exposure. These findings demonstrate the importance of the consequences of aggressive responding in determining the probability of an aggressive response following provocation . I n both humans and nonhumans aggressive and/or escape responses occur at increased probability follow ing aversive stimulus presentation (Azrin , Hutchinson , & Hake, 1967; Cherek & Dougherty, 1997). Furthermore, responding occasioned by the presentation of aversive stimuli typically occurs at a rate that is directly related to the frequency of aversive stimulus presentation. Earlier studies with nonhuman subjects have shown that the frequency of fighting among This research was supported by Grant DA-03166 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse . Reprint requests can be obtained from Don R. Cherek, Dept. of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, University of Texas-Houston , Health Science Center, 1300 Moursund Street, Houston, TX 77030. 592 DOUGHERTY ET AL. pairs of rats (Ulrich & Azrin, 1962) and aggressive biting in squirrel monkeys (Azrin, Hutchinson, & McLaughlin, 1965) was directly related to the frequency of electric shock presentation. Our recent study with human subjects (Cherek & Dougherty, 1997) has shown that the frequency of aggressive responding made toward a fictitious subject was directly related to the frequency of provocation (i.e., point subtractions) by a fictitious opponent. Together, these studies have demonstrated that the frequency of provocation is an important determinant of aggressive response rates. Although provocation is sufficient to generate aggressive responding, the probability that aggressive responding is maintained is largely dependent upon the consequences. A number of previous human laboratory studies have examined the role in which various contingencies affect aggressive responses. For example, previous studies have shown that aggressive responses decrease when the response requirement for an aggressive response is increased (Cherek, Spiga, & Egli, 1992), and higher rates of aggressive responding are maintained by an avoidance contingency rather than an escape contingency (Cherek, Spiga, Steinberg, & Kelly, 1990). In the present study, using the © Point Subtraction Aggression Paradigm (Cherek, 1981), we examined the role a contingency attached to aggressive responding, a temporary escape (or time-out) from provocation, plays in the maintenance of aggressive responding. In a recent study (Cherek & Dougherty, 1997), while examining the effect of a wide range of provocation frequencies (point subtractions) on rates of aggressive responding, we found aggressive responding varied as a function of provocation frequency. Interestingly, this relationship held true even during conditions of high provocation, when aggressive responding initiated only brief periods of escape from further provocation. Here we examined how the duration of the provocation-free interval (PFI), initiated by the subject's aggressive responding, affected rates of aggressive responding during extended exposure to a particular PFI value (5 s or 62.5 s). The PFI duration was manipulated in order to determine the importance of the PFI contingency in the maintenance of aggressive responding. Method Subjects Four subjects, three males and one female, between the ages of 20 and 34 participated after informed consent was obtained. One subject (S-1204) had previously participated in research in our laboratory and had been exposed to 155 sessions of the same experimental paradigm. Apparatus During experimental sessions, subjects sat in a 1.2-m by 1.8-m sound-attenuated chamber which was illuminated overhead by a 75-W light bulb. Masking noise was provided by a ventilation fan. Located HUMAN AGGRESSIVE RESPONDING 593 within the chamber were an Apple lie monochrome monitor and a customized response panel (10.0 cm x 43.0 cm x 25.0 cm). The response panel contained three push buttons (MicroSwitch) labeled "A," "B," and "C" (Button C was not used) mounted in a straight line 10.0 cm apart. Both the monitor and response panel were controlled by an Apple IIGS computer equipped with an Applied Engineering I/O 32 interfacing system located in an adjacent room. Procedure Recruitment and screenings. Potential subjects were recruited through advertisements for behavioral research placed in the classified employment section of local newspapers. Subjects were excluded if either a psychiatric disorder, including substance abuse, or medical illness was detected during screening exams. The psychiatric screening consisted of a mental status exam and the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia-Lifetime Version (SADS-L), a standardized psychiatric interview (Endicott & Spitzer, 1978). A medical questionnaire, administered by the interviewer was used to determine if any significant medical conditions existed that would preclude subject participation. Drug usage was monitored by collecting urine and expired air samples daily each morning shortly after each subject's arrival (8:00 a.m.). A urine drug screen (Enzyme Multiple Immunoassay Technique Drug Abuse Urine Assay by SYLVA Corporation, Palo Alto, CA), capable of detecting the presence of illicit and therapeutic compounds or their metabolites, was performed on daily samples. Recent alcohol intake was monitored by collecting an expired air sample using an Intoximeter Model 3000-111 (Intoximeter, Inc., St. Louis, MO). As part of the screening and recruitment process information regarding potential earnings, urine drug testing, breath alcohol testing, and the psychiatric and physical exams was provided. Subjects were told that they could earn approximately $4-$5 per session through point accumulation (paid daily) and would be paid a $10 per day bonus at the completion of their participation for maintaining drug-free urines while in the study. The study's purpose was described as a study of motor performance. Instructions. Subjects were shown a diagram of the testing apparatus and read a standard set of instructions describing the experimental paradigm prior to the subject's first session of participation. In these instructions the two response options were briefly described. These instructions appear verbatim elsewhere (Cherek & Dougherty, 1997) and informed subjects that they would be paired with another person who might take away points worth money during the course of the experimental sessions. Point Subtraction Aggression Paradigm (PSAP). During experimental sessions subjects were tested using the PSAP which makes two response options available to the subject: (1) Completing a fixed-ratio (FR) 100 on Button A was reinforced by adding one point (worth 10 cents) to the subject's point-accumulation counter, displayed 594 DOUGHERTY ET AL. on a computer monitor; and (2) completing a FR 10 on Button B ostensibly subtracted a point from a fictitious other subject. This responding was defined as aggressive because it ostensibly resulted in the presentation of an aversive stimulus to another person. Once either response button was chosen it was necessary to complete its ratio requirement before a different option could be chosen. Subjects were provoked by the PSAP program subtracting points from their counter at randomly selected intervals between 6 sand 120 s. These point subtractions were attributed to the fictitious partner in the instructions and occurred while the subject was in either response option. Besides ostensibly resulting in point subtractions, completing a FR 10 on Button B could also initiate an interval free from point subtractions (provocation-free intervals). A provocation-free interval (PFI) could be initiated only after at least one point had been subtracted from the subject's counter. Completing Button B's response requirement either before a point had been subtracted, or after a PFI had already been initiated, had no programmed consequence but these responses were included in the data analysis. In the absence of PFI initiation, points were subtracted at intervals ranging from 6 s to 120 s; all intervals were equally represented. Point subtractions were scheduled to occur 6 s to 120 s after a PFI had timed out. Because a PFI could be initiated only by the subject following at least one point loss, not all point subtractions could be avoided. However, the total number of point subtractions could be reduced by Button B responding, because B responding immediately following each point loss would result in fewer total point subtractions in a given session. Provocation-free interval manipulations. Subjects participated in six sessions per day Monday through Friday. Twenty-five minute sessions were conducted at 9:00, 10:00 and 11 :00 a.m., and 1:00, 2:00, and 3:00 p.m., and lunch was provided at noon. In this experiment, we alternated duration of the PFI between two values. All subjects began in a condition where the PFI was equal to 62.5 s, and remained under this condition for a minimum of 10 sessions. After 10 sessions, or after B button responding stabilized, the PFI interval was decreased to 5 s. At the 5-s PFI essentially no time-out from point loss was provided. The reason for including 5 s instead of 0 s was to decrease the likelihood that Button B responses would be superstitiously suppressed (punished) by a point subtraction. Subjects remained in the 5-s PFI condition until responding either stabilized or extinction occurred. In the next phase, subjects were returned to the PFI 62.5-s condition. Then, in the final phase, the PFI 5-s condition was reinstated. Data were analyzed by measuring B responding across all sessions completed during the study. Evaluation of instructional deception. In order to assess whether or not the instructional deception regarding the other fictitious subjects had been established and maintained in these procedures subjects completed a questionnaire at the end of the day which asked them (1) to estimate the number of subjects they had been paired with that day, (2) HUMAN AGGRESSIVE RESPONDING 595 to describe these other subjects, and (3) to estimate whether they or the other subjects subtracted more points, Any report on the questionnaire indicating the subject did not believe that he or she had interacted with another individual would result in removal from the study, However, all 30 8-1204 25 PFI=62.5 s PFI=5 s PFI = 62.5 20 PFI=5 s 15 10 5 0 45 40 'S 35 ,~ 30 ~ 25 '- 20 Q) a.. 15 en 10 Q) en 5 c: 0 0 8-1267 PFI=62.5 Q) PFI=5 s PFI=62.5 s c.. ~ 30 a: 8-1269 PFI=62.5 s 25 PFI=5 s PFI=62.5 s PFI=5 s Q) > 'eenn Q) 'C') C') « 20 15 10 5 0 30 8-1287 25 f/) l!) 20 PFI=62.5 s II u:: PFI=62.5 s PFI=5 s a.. 15 10 5 0 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 Session Figure 1. Rates of aggressive responding under two PFI duration conditions: 62 .5 sand 5 s; each condition was repeated. 596 DOUGHERTY ET AL. subjects' reports endorsed interaction with other individuals during experimental sessions. Results All subjects reported having been paired with one or more subjects during each day of testing. None of the subjects were removed for alcohol or drug use. The dependent variable of primary interest was the rate of aggressive responding emitted on Button B. Rates of responding on this button under the two provocation conditions (PFI = 62.5 sand PFI = 5 s) are depicted in Figure 1. Initially, Button B responses were maintained by the 62.5-s PFI for all subjects, and when the 5-s PFI was introduced Button B responding extinguished. For three of the four subjects, the transition from the longer PFI (62.5 s) to the shorter PFI (5 s) resulted in an initial increment in aggressive responding. Except for Subject S-1267, baseline levels of Button B responding were recaptured after extinction when the PFI of 62 .5 s was reintroduced and then again extinguished when the PFI of 5 s was reintroduced. Subject S-1267's Button B responding never recovered when the PFI of 62.5 s was reintroduced, after the initial extinction. Subject S-1204, having the extensive history with this schedule, took considerably more sessions to extinguish than did any of the other subjects. Rates of responding on Button A (point-maintained responding) remained relatively stable across both PFI conditions. Rates of responding under the 62.5 condition averaged 4.53 (SEM = 0.03) and 4.62 (SEM = 0.03) responses per second in the 62.5 and 5 s PFI conditions, respectively. Discussion The purpose of this experiment was to examine the relationship between the provocation-free interval, which provides a period of escape from an aversive stimulus (point subtractions), and the rates of aggressive responding. The initiation of very brief periods of time-out from provocation (PFls) did not maintain aggressive (Button B) responding over several sessions. This finding is meaningful because it demonstrates that escape from provocation, or other aversive stimulation, is an important variable in maintaining aggressive behavior. Generally, the results of this experiment further the notion that both aversive stimulation (provocation) and escape from subsequent provocation (PFI) are important variables in the facilitation and maintenance of aggressive responding in the laboratory. The delivery of an aversive stimulus, for example, shock, loud noise, or point subtraction, is necessary for engendering aggressive responses; and a period free from provocation (an escape contingency) is necessary for maintaining aggressive responses. Subjects will not typically emit HUMAN AGGRESSIVE RESPONDING 597 aggressive responses in the absence of provocation (Azrin, Hutchinson, & McLaughlin, 1965; Kelly, Cherek, Steinberg, & Robinson, 1988), and escape from an aversive stimulus (e.g., provocation) appears to be a critical consequence responsible for maintaining aggressive responding in the laboratory (Dengerink & Covey, 1983). This is consistent with preliminary findings described in another study where, in the absence of periods of escape from further point subtractions, aggressive responding extinguished (Cherek, Spiga, Steinberg, & Kelly, 1990). This escape contingency between aggressive responding and provocation is similar to contingencies that exist in the natural environment. One of the most interesting findings was that for most subjects aggressive responding initially increased (before subsequently extinguishing) when transitions were made from the longer to the shorter PFI values. These increases were sometimes very large. Such increases are similar to the bursts of responding observed when conditions of extinction are introduced following an established reinforcement history, or when abrupt transitions are made from dense to sparse reinforcement schedules (Ferster & Skinner, 1957). In the vast literature on aggression, the consequences of aggressive behavior as a factor in maintaining and determining the rate of that behavior are seldom considered (see Baron & Richardson, 1994, cf. Cherek & Steinberg, 1987). Relatedly, most current experimental preparations with humans and nonhumans focus on the role of evocative or eliciting stimuli rather than the consequences of aggressive behavior (Miczek & Winslow, 1987; Taylor & Chermack, 1993). In such experiments, investigators often arrange environmental conditions that establish favorable consequences for aggressive responding. Typically, experiments carefully control the consequences of aggressive behavior, for example by using inanimate target objects, both with nonhuman and human subjects (Azrin, Hutchinson, & Sallery, 1964; Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963). Also, in the resident intruder paradigm (see Brain, Benton, Howell, & Jones, 1982) the intruder is commonly defeated. Thus, although not emphasized, the consequences for aggressive responding probably play an implicit but important role in many laboratory studies of aggression. Germane to the present experiment, data from several nonhuman studies support the importance of reinforcing consequences in maintaining aggressive responding (Cherek & Pickens, 1970; Hutchinson, Azrin, & Hunt, 1968; Scott, 1975). Our data are complementary to those nonhuman studies and support similar findings with human subjects (Dengerink & Covey, 1983). One could classify aggressive behavior in this context functionally as an escape behavior, idiosyncratic to laboratory conditions. However, it should be pointed out that laboratory paradigms of aggression have been externally validated in a number of studies that relate this behavior to aggressive behavior occurring outside the laboratory. These validations include positive correlations between aggressive responding and self-rated aggression and violence (Malamuth & Check, 1980; 598 DOUGHERTY ET AL. Shemberg , Leventhal , & Allman , 1968); peer ratings of aggression (Williams , Meyerson, Eron , & Semler, 1967); antisocial behavior (Hartmann, 1969); and criminal records (Wolfe & Baron, 1971). We have shown that response characteristics differ when instructions attribute the source of provocation to a machine rather than another individual (Cherek, Steinberg , Kelly, Robinson, & Spiga, 1990; Kelly & Cherek, 1993). And two recent studies in our lab have supported the external validity of the PSAP by showing that parolees with violent histories make significantly more aggressive responses than parolees with nonviolent histories (Cherek, Moeller, Schnapp , & Dougherty, 1997; Cherek, Schnapp, Moeller, & Dougherty, 1996). Collectively, the results of the current study and those noted above suggest that (a) both provocation and escape from subsequent provocation are necessary conditions for the occurrence and maintenance of aggressive behavior; and (b) both the rate of provocation and the duration of the escape period can affect the frequency of aggressive responding. Future studies of aggressive behavior should consider the important role of these variables. References AZRIN , N. H. , HAKE , D. F. , & HUTCHINSON , R. R. (1965). Elicitation of aggression by a physical blow. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 8, 55-57. AZRIN , N. H., HUTCHINSON , R. R. , & HAKE, D. F. (1967). Attack, avoidance and escape reactions to aversive shock. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 131 -148. AZRIN , N. H . , HUTCHINSON , R. R. , & MCLAUGHLIN, R. (1965). The opportunity for aggression as an operant reinforcer during aversive stimulation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 8, 171-180. AZRIN , N. H., HUTCHINSON, R. R, & SALLERY, R. D. (1964). Pain-aggression toward inanimate objects . Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 223-228. BANDURA, A. , ROSS, D., & ROSS , S. A. (1963). Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 3-11. BARON, R. A., & RICHARDSON , D. R. (1994). Human aggression. New York: Plenum Press. BRAIN, P F., BENTON , D. , HOWELL, P A. , & JONES, S. E. (1982). Resident rats' aggression toward intruders. Aggressive Behavior, 8, 371-383. CHEREK, D. R. (1981). Effects of smoking different doses of nicotine on human aggressive behavior. Psychopharmacology, 75, 339-349. CHEREK , D. R. , & DOUGHERTY, D. M. (1997) . The relationship between provocation frequency and human aggressive responding. The Psychological Record, 47, 357-370. CHEREK, D. R. , MOELLER, F. G., SCHNAPP, B. , & DOUGHERTY, D. M. (1997). Studies of violent and nonviolent male parolees: I. Laboratory and psychometric measure of aggression. Biological Psychiatry, 41 , 514-522. HUMAN AGGRESSIVE RESPONDING 599 CHEAEK, D. A., & PICKENS, A. (1970). Schedule-induced aggression as a function of fixed-ratio value. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 14,309-311. CHEAEK, D. A., SCHNAPP, W., MOELLEA, F. G., & DOUGHEATY, D. M. (1996). Laboratory measures of aggressive responding in male parolees with violent and nonviolent histories. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 27-36. CHEAEK, D. R., SPIGA, A., & EGLI, M. (1992). Effects of response requirement and alcohol on human aggressive responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 577-587. CHEAEK, D. A., SPIGA, A., STEINBEAG, J. L., & KELLY, T. H. (1990) . Human aggressive responses maintained by avoidance or escape from point loss. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 293-303. CHEAEK, D. A., & STEINBEAG, J. L. (1987). Effects of drugs on human aggressive behavior. In G. D. Burrows & J. S. Werry (Eds.), Advances in human psychopharmacology: A research annual, Vol. 4 (pp. 239-290). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. CHEAEK, D. A., STEINBEAG, J. L., KELLY, T. H., AOBINSON, D. E., & SPIGA, A. (1990). Effects of acute administration of diazepam and d-amphetamine on aggressive and escape responding of normal male subjects. Psychopharmacology, 100, 173-181. DENGEAINK, H. A., & COVEY, M. (1983). Implications of an escape-avoidance theory of aggressive responses to attack. In A. G. Geen & E. I. Donnerstein (Eds.), Aggression: Theoretical and methodological issues: Vol. 1 (pp. 163-188). New York: Academic Press. ENDICOTT, J., & SPITZEA, A. L. (1978). A diagnostic interview: The schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 837-851. FEASTEA, C. 8., & SKINNEA, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. HAATMANN, D. P. (1969). Influence of symbolically modeled instrumental aggression and pain cues on aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 280-288. HUTCHINSON, A. A., AZAIN, N. H., & HUNT, G. M. (1968). Attack produced by intermittent reinforcement of a concurrent operant response. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 489-495. KELLY, T. H., & CHEAEK, D. A. (1993). The effects of alcohol on free-operant aggressive behavior. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 11 (Supp), 40-52. KELLY, T. H., CHEAEK, D. A., STEINBEAG, J. L., & AOBINSON, D. (1988). Effects of provocation and alcohol on human aggressive behavior. Drug Alcohol Dependence, 21,105-112. MALAMUTH, N., & CHECK, J. V. (1980). The effect of mass media exposure on acceptance of violence against women: A field experiment. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Montreal, CN. MICZEK, K. A., & WINSLOW, J. T. (1987). Psychopharmacological research on aggressive behavior. In A. J. Greenshaw & C. T. Dourish (Eds .), Experimental pharmacology: Concepts and methods (pp. 27-114). Clifton, NJ: Humana Press. SCOTT, J. P. (1975). Aggression. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. SHEMBEAG, K. M., LEVENTHAL, D. B., & ALLMAN , L. (1968). Aggression machine performance and rated aggression. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 3,117-119. 600 DOUGHERTY ET AL. TAYLOR, S. P, & CHERMACK, S. T. (1993). Alcohol, drugs, and human physical aggression. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 11 (Supp), 78-88. ULRICH, R. E., & AZRIN, N. H. (1962). Reflexive fighting in pairs of rats. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 511-520. WILLIAMS, J. F., MEYERSON, L. J., ERON, L. D., & SEMLER, I. J. (1967). Peerrated aggression and aggressive responses elicited in an experimental situation. Child Development, 38, 181-190. WOLFE, B. M., & BARON, R. A. (1971). Laboratory aggression related to aggression in naturalistic social situations: Effects of an aggressive model on the behavior of college student and prisoner observers. Psychonomic Science, 24, 193-194.